Transcription
PDF
Chapter 3 Mood and Stereotyping: Affective States and the Use of General Knowledge Structures Herbert Bless Universitiit Heidelberg Norbert Schwarz University of Michigan, Ann Arbor Markus Kemmelmeier Universitiit Heidelberg ABSTRACT We review how affective states moderate the impact of stereotypes in social judgment. The reported evidence suggests that stereotypes have more impact on judgments formed by individuals in a happy mood rather than sad mood, whereas the reverse holds for individuating information. We trace these effects to mood-dependent differences in processing style. Extending previous theorizing, we suggest that individuals are more likely to process new information in a bottom-up fashion when the situation is perceived as problematic, which is partially a function of being in a sad mood. In contrast, individuals are more likely to rely on pre-existing general knowledge structures when the situation is perceived as safe, which is partially a function of being in a happy mood. The available evidence indicates that the differential impact of general knowledge structures is not mediated by mood effects on processing capacity or motivation, in contrast to what previous theorizing suggested. European Review of Social Psychology, Volume 7. Edited by Wolfgang Stroebe and Miles Hewstone. © 1996 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 64 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER INTRODUCTION Social scientists have long assumed a close link between affective states on the one hand and stereotyping and prejudice on the other hand. This link may take two different forms. On the one hand, the stereotyped group itself (or some contextual information associated with the group) may elicit im affective reaction. Bodenhausen (1993) has referred to such affective reactions as "integral" affect. On the other hand, individuals may be in some affective state, e.g., a happy or sad mood, when they are exposed to a stereotyped target. In this case, the perceiver's affective state is not a reaction to the stereotyped group and has hence been termed "incidental" (Bodenhausen, 1993). While the potential impact of incidental affect has only recently received theoretical and empirical attention (see Bodenhausen, 1993), integral affect has played a central role in numerous approaches that understood prejudice as resulting from negative affective states. At the beginning of this century, the influence of Freud's (1933) psychoanalysis prompted researchers to focus predominantly on intrapsychic causes of stereotyping and prejudice. Thus, research into the authoritarian personality (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950) portrayed prejudice as a product of the individual's defense mechanisms which serve to shield him- or herself from aversive emotional impulses. Sharing this focus on negative affective states, the frustration-aggression hypothesis conceptualized negative affect towards outgroups as a channeled projection of aggressive impulses resulting from deprivation experiences (Dollard et ai., 1939; Miller & Bugelski, 1948). This notion is also captured in the "scapegoat theory" that views prejudice as an outlet of displaced hostility (Bettelheim & Janowitz, 1949). Allport (1954) discussed a variety of negative affective states, including pr9jected guilt and anxiety, as antecedents of stereotyping and prejudice. Lippmann (1922), who introduced the concept of a "stereotype", saw stereotypes as devices to avoid the aversive states of high cognitive complexity and subjective uncertainty. Combining cognitive and motivational aspects in social identity theory, Tajfel and Turner have conceptualized individuals' affect towards themselves and their group as driving forces for ingroup favoritism and outgroup discrimination (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner, 1987). In line with social identity theory, inducing negative affect by threatening individuals' selfesteem has been found to increase ingroup favoritism (Meindl & Lerner, 1984), and vice versa, the expression of ingroup favoritism increased reported self-esteem (Oakes & Turner, 1980; Lemyre & Smith, 1985; see also Messick & Mackie, 1989). The above approaches share the assumption that negative affect becomes associated with the stereotyped group, rendering it an integral part of the process of stereotyping. While these approaches conceptualize stereotyping and prejudice as part of the individual's intrapsychic self-regulation, other MOOD AND STEREOTYPING approaches have conceptualized stereotyping as emanating from real tergroup conflict over power and resources (e.g. Allport, 1954; Blau Both lines of reasoning, however, converge in suggesting that stereot most likely to occur when individuals experience negative emotiona The focal role of affect in different theories of prejudice suggests that 1 of intergroup relations may be ideal for investigating the impact of a states on social judgment. As Hamilton (1981, p. 347) noted, "If ther domain of human interaction that history tells us is laden with stror passionate feelings, it is the area of intergroup relations". Nevertheless, affective states have received little attention as resea stereotyping progressed. With the rise of the information processir digm, most researchers seemed to lose interest in the affective comp( prejudice and the focus shifted towards the cognitive bases of stere (Hamilton, 1981; see also Stroebe & Insko, 1989). This shift went alon decreased interest in emotion in general, reflecting that affective stat difficult to capture in the computer metaphor that dominated infor processing theories (see Hilgard 1980; Zajonc, 1980). However, as pI by some researchers (e.g., Tomkins, 1981), affective states have 1:: discovered as a research topic in the 1980s and the interplay of afl cognition became a central area of research in social cognition. This I resulted in the integration of affective processes in models of info processing (see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Forgas, 1992; Sch Clore, in press, for reviews) and focused on the impact of moods on t1 of information stored in memory ·(e.g., Bower, 1991~ , the use of mOl basis of judgment (e.g., Schwarz & Clore, 1983), and the impact of m the style of information-processing (e.g., Isen, 1987; Fiedler, 1991; S 1990). More recently, researchers have drawn on these lines of theoriz have expanded the analysis of the role of affective states in stereoty focusing attention on the role of incidental rather than integral aff( present article reviews this research, focusing on how incidental ( states may facilitate or inhibit reliance on stereotypic knowledge stru( social judgment. Reflecting the nature of current research, our revi phasizes the role of global happy and sad moods, rather than the specific, and more intense, emotions. Moreover, we concentrate moods influence cognitive processes involved in stereotyping and do dress how stereotyping, in turn, may influence affective states. In eSSf ask if being in a happy or sad mood makes individuals differentially I rely on stereo typic knowledge structures, and if so, what are the p: that may mediate such an effect? The focus of the present chapter is on the interplay of moods, steI and individuating information in social judgment. Following a short n dual-processing models that conceptualize the role of categori, 66 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER individuating information in the judgment process, we address different assumptions bearing on how moods may influence information-processing and derive competing predictions for the impact of categorical and individuating information in social judgment. Subsequently, we review the available evidence in light of these hypotheses. This review will indicate that being in a happy mood increases reliance on stereo typic knowledge structures, whereas being in a sad mood increases reliance on individuating information at the expense of stereotypic knowledge structures. Unless the individuating information is blatantly inconsistent with the implications of the stereotype, these processes result in increased stereotyping under incidental happy moods and decreased stereotyping under incidental sad moods, in contrast to what previous theorizing on the role of integral affect would suggest. Following this review, we offer some general conclusions and outline issues for future research. MOODS, STEREOTYPES AND EVALUATIVE JUDGMENTS The Impact of Categorical and Individuating Information in Person Perception Stereotypes are often referred to as the cognitive component of prejudice and discrimination and are assumed to consist of a structured set of beliefs about the attributes shared by members of a particular group (e.g., Hamilton, 1979; Hamilton & Trolier, 1986; Lippmann, 1922). Thus, a stereotype may be considered a group schema, i.e., a general knowledge structure about a group. Independent of whether these knowledge structures are accurate or not, they often have a strong impact on how individuals form judgments (e.g. Bodenhausen & Wyer, 1985), on how they seek (e.g., Kunda, 1990; Snyder & Swann, 1978) and interpret (e.g., Darley & Gross, 1983; Sagar & Schofield, 1980) information, on attribution processes (e.g., Macrae & Shepherd, 1989), and on how they react towards a member of the group (e.g., Gaertner & Dovido, 1977; Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977; for an overview see Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). Of course, the degree to which evaluative judgments are influenced by stereotypes may vary across situations. To capture these differences, recent models of person perception distinguish between two different processing strategies involved in impression formation (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). On the one hand, judgments may be primarily based on the implications of a target person's category membership. In this case, judgments reflect the perceiver's general knowledge about the category to which the target is assigned, i.e. the implications of the stereotype. On the other hand, judgments MOOD AND STEREOTYPING may be primarily based on the available individuating information abc specific target. In this case, the impact of category information is dimini: One of the key assumptions holds that judgments based on category men ship information require less processing capacity and/or motivation than ments' based on individuating information (see Fiske & Neuberg, . Kruglanski, 1989). For the present purpose, it is important to note that the impact of a st. type may be mediated by different processes. On the one hand, indivi. may primarily rely on general knowledge structures pertaining to the tal category membership at the expense of individuating information, essen using the category membership information as a peripheral cue that simj: the judgmental task. If so, specific individuating information about the t should have little impact. Moreover, any inconsistencies between the viduating information and the implications of the target's category men ship are likely to go unnoticed. On the other hand, individuals may rely on general knowledge structm encoding the specific individuating information, thus relating both soure information to one another, similar to event-schemas supporting the enc( of social situations (e.g. Abelson, 1981; Graesser, Gordon, & Sawyer, 197 so, individuating information that is consistent with the implications 0 target's category membership can be processed rather parsimoniously. mechanism, however, does not necessarily imply less processing in all ! . tions. In particular, inconsistent information will require additional proce attention (for overviews see Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Srull & Wyer, 1989). C that this additional processing attention is provided, inconsistent inform is likely to receive particular weight in forming an impression (Ma Hewstone, & Griffiths, 1993; Stangor & Duan, 1991; for meta-analyses 0 role of inconsistent information, see Rojahn & Pettigrew, 1992; Stang McMillan, 1992). The specific role of inconsistent information will be. particularly important in the following sections and will be addressed: extensively below. Mood and Different Styles of Information Processing Research into the interplay of affect and cognition documents that n differences in affective states may have a pronounced impact on cogr processes (for overviews, see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Fiedler, Forgas, 1992; Isen, 1987; Schwarz, 1990; Schwarz & Bless, 1991; Schwa Clore, in press). The empirical evidence suggests that the processing of viduals in elated moods can be characterized as more heuristic, and a! systematic and detail-oriented than the processing of individuals in ne moods. In contrast, individuals in mildly depressed moods seem particl likely to engage in detail-oriented, analytic processing of new informatic 68 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER These conclusions have been most consistently supported in research on mood and persuasion (e.g., Bless et at., 1990; Bless, Mackie, & Schwarz, 1992; Bohner et at., 1993; Mackie & Worth, 1989; Worth & Mackie, 1987; see Schwarz, Bless, & Bohner, 1991, for a review). Specifically, individuals in an elated mood have been found to be more influenced by the presence of peripheral cues associated with the message and less influenced by the quality of the presented arguments than individuals in a mildly depressed mood, with individuals in a neutral mood falling in between. Similarly, in the domain of person perception, individuals in an elated mood have been found to be less influenced by detailed person descriptions, and to be more likely to show halo effects, than individuals in a depressed mood (see Sinclair & Mark, 1992, for a review). While there is consensus about the general conclusion that individuals' affective states influence their style of information-processing, different assumptions have been offered regarding the mediating processes, as reviewed below. Mood and Processing Capacity One assumption holds that being in a good mood limits processing capacity. This assumption rests on the notion that more positive than negative material is stored in memory and that positive material is more interconnected. Indirect evidence for this "Pollyanna Principle" results from the observation that positive material is more easily recalled than negative material (Boucher & Osgood, 1969; Matlin & Stang, 1979). If being in a positive mood increases the accessibility of pos~tively valenced material, while being in a negative mood increases the accessibility of negatively valenced material (Bower, 1981; Isen, 1984), more material will be potentially activated when individuals are in a positive affective state. Accordingly, the activation of a large amount of interconnected material may decrease processing capacity for the task at hand (e.g., Mackie & Worth, 1989). Mackie and Worth (1989) report empirical evidence for the assumption of reduced capacity under happy mood in the persuasion domain. In general, attitudes of subjects in a happy mood failed to reflect the quality of a persuasive message, while attitudes of subjects in a neutral mood showed differential effects after exposure to strong or to weak arguments. Happy subjects did, however, differentiate between strong and weak arguments when they were encouraged to take as much time as they wanted for reading the message. Mackie and Worth concluded that the extra processing time provided to subjects eliminated the capacity deficits of happy subjects, resulting in the observed differential impact of strong and weak arguments (for an alternative explanation of these results see Schwarz, Bless, & Bohner, 1991). If being in a happy mood decreases processing capacity, we may expect that happy individuals are more likely to rely on stereotypes, which generally serve MOOD AND STEREOTYPING as energy saving devices in social judgment (Bodenhausen, 1990; Gilb Hixon, 1991; Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994). Moreover, the pr, ing con~traints presumably experienced by happy individuals should r the likelihood that happy individuals elaborate on detailed individuati formation about a specific target. It has also been argued that negative affective states reduce proc. capacity because they activate (negative) material that is irrelevant fe task at hand (Ellis & Ashbrook, 1988). Supporting empirical evie however, has primarily been obtained in studies that assessed memo: relatively content-free materials. Studies that used more meaningful materials, on the other hand, did not obtain supportive evidence (f overview see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994). Mood and Processing Motivation An alternative approach suggests that the impact of moods on proc( style reflects the informative function of affective states (Schwarz, Schwarz & Bless, 1991). According to this approach, being in a sad I signals that the environment poses a problem, whereas being in a happy I signals that the environment is benign. As a result, individuals in a sad : are more motivated to engage in detail-oriented systematic proc( strategies, which are typically adaptive in handling problematic situatio. contrast, individuals in a good mood may see little reason to spontane engage in effortful processing strategies, unless this is called for by other (see Schwarz, 1990, for a more detailed discussion). Evidence consistent with this assumption has been reported in a vari. domains (see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Schwarz & Clore, in pre~ a review). In the persuasion domain, for example, sad subjects are persuaded by strong than by weak arguments, whereas happy subjeci equally persuaded by both, as noted above. If this difference is due t. ferences in processing motivation, it should be reduced by manipulation increase the processing motivation of happy subjects. Supporting this ass tion, Bless et at. (1990) observed that happy subjects differentiated bet strong and weak arguments when they were instructed to pay attenti, argument quality. Further supporting the informational functions approach to mood ane cessing style, Sinclair, Mark, and Clore (1994) observed that the imp, happy and sad moods on the processing of persuasive communication: eliminated when the informational implications of the mood were callee question. Relying on good vs. bad weather as a mood induction, Sin Mark, and Clore (1994) replicated that sad but not happy subjects wer. ferentially affected by strong vs. weak arguments. However, when the j mational value of subjects' mood was discredited by drawing sub 70 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER attention to the weather as a potential cause for their momentary feelings (see also Schwarz & Clore, 1983), mood no longer played a role and a main effect of message quality emerged. In combination these findings cast doubt on a cognitive capacity account of mood effects on ~rocessing style. If being in a happy mood cluttered subjects' minds with irrelevant thoughts that reduce processing capacity, instructing subjects to pay attention to argument quality or inducing them to attribute their mood to the weather should not eliminate the impact of the presuma.bly automatic process of mood-congruent recall. Hence, these findings point to .' the impact of moods on processing motivation. Given that a decreased processing motivation usually results m an mcreased impact of stereotypes (for examples, see Kruglanski, 1989), one would expect a stronger impact of stereotypes on the judgm~nts for~ed. by happy rather than sad individuals. Moreover, reduced processmg motivatIOn should also decrease the likelihood that happy individuals elaborate on the individuating information, unless this is required by other goals. A Mood-and-general-knowledge Assumption Finally, Bless (1994; see also Bless et al., in press) proposed an alternative r.n 0del that accounts for the heuristic processing observed under happy moods without assuming that different moods result in different amounts of processing. Extending the mood-as-information approach (Schwarz, 1990; Schwarz & Bl~ss, 1991), this model again assumes that positive affective state~ inform the subJe~t about a benign situation, whereas negative affective states Signal a problematic situation. When the current situation is perceived as unproblematic and safe, as signalled by being in a positive affective state, indi~iduals may not, .or not o~ly, feel less motivated to engage in effortful processmg. Rather, the Information provided by happy moods may imply that it is possible to rely on o~e's gen~ral knowledge structures, which usually serve one well. In contrast, the mformatlOn provided by sad moods may imply that reliance on general knowledge .structures is risky, reflecting that problematic situations usually require a~te~tlOn to their specific characteristics. If so, happy individuals may feel more mclmed to rely on their general knowledge structures than sad individuals. This mood-and-general-knowledge assumption is compatible with much of the available evidence. If we consider heuristic processing strategies as involving the application of general knowledge structures to specific information (Nisbett & Ross, 1980), we may view heuristics as a variant of a larger set of general knowledge representations including schemata, scripts, or stereotypes. From such a perspective, the increased reliance on heuristics under happy moods (see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Isen, 1987) would fit nicely with the assumption that happy individuals are more likely to rely on general knowledge structures than sad individuals. Similarly, in the MOOD AND STEREOTYPING persuasion domain, happy subjects are more likely than sad subjects to rely a previously activated global representation of a persuasive message (Bh Mackie, & Schwarz, 1992). While the available evidence clearly indicates a more pronounced impaci heuristics and other general knowledge structures under happy moods, . specific mediating mechanism is open to debate. As noted above, the ( served effects are usually attributed to motivational or capacity deficits, wh presumably limit the amount of processing. The actual amount of processi: however, has rarely been directly assessed. Rather, it has typically been ferred that given an increased impact of general knowledge structures, thl must be some deficit in either processing capacity or processing motivation. contrast, the general knowledge structure approach does not assume that 1 impact of happy moods is mediated by reduced processing capacity motivation. Directly addressing this assumption in a dual-task paradigm, Bless et at. press) had subjects work on a secondary task while they were exposed information to which a previously activated script was applicable. As I pected, happy subjects' encoding of the presented information showed stronger impact of the activated script than sad subjects' encoding. When this finding, by itself, may reflect any of the processes discussed above, t data bearing on the secondary task are diagnostic of the underlying process. happy subjects are more likely to rely on general knowledge structures, ~ coding information to which an accessible script is applicable should requ: fewer attentional resources, thus allowing happy subjects to perform better the secondary task. If being in a happy mood generally decreases processi motivation or processing capacity, however, happy subjects should perf01 poorly at the secondary task. Consistent with the former prediction, hap subjects outperformed sad subjects on the secondary task, rendering it u likely that they suffered from a general deficit in processing motivation capacity. This interpretation is further supported by the finding that subjec mood did not affect their performance on the "secondary" task if this task w presented as the only task. Bless and Fiedler (1995) reported converging evidence in another domai Specific judgments about a target person were more strongly influenced . preceding general trait judgments about the target when subjects were in happy rather than a neutral or a sad mood. Analyses of response latenci strongly indicated that the increased impact of global trait judgments I specific judgments observed under happy mood was not mediated by a I duced amount of processing but rather by happy subjects' reliance on genel knowledge structures. As stereotypes are another form of general knowledge structures, tl mood-and-general-knowledge assumption again predicts a stronger impact stereotypes when individuals are in a happy rather than a sad mood. This is, 72 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER course, the same general prediction that has been derived from the reduced capacity or reduced motivation assumptions. These approaches differ, however, in their predictions pertaining to the interplay of stereo typic and individuating information. If happy individuals are less likely to engage in extensive processing (either due to limited capacity or limited motivation), individuating information should receive little attention and the judgment should primarily be based on the implications of the stereotype. Hence, these approaches trace the increased impact of stereotypes under happy moods to the neglect of individuating information. In contrast, no processing constraints are postulated by the mood-and-general-knowledge hypothesis, which assumes that happy individuals process individuating information to the same extent as sad individuals. Reflecting their increased reliance on general knowledge structures, however, happy individuals are expected to encode the individuating information in terms of the stereotype. This should result in more stereotypic judgments under good mood, provided that the individuating information is not inconsistent with the stereotype. If the individuating information is inconsistent with the stereotype, no stereo typic judgment should be obtained. In fact, the judgment may be contrasted to the stereotype under this condition. Hence, the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption traces the increased impact of stereotypes under happy moods to stereotypic encodings of the individuating information rather than to a neglect of individuating information. These different mechanisms can be distinguished by testing the impact of moods under conditions where the individuating information is either consistent or inconsistent with the implications of the stereotype, as we shall discuss in more detail below. Empirical Evidence The different conceptualizations of the impact of moods on processing strategies converge on the prediction that stereotypes should have a stronger impact on judgments formed by individuals in a happy rather than sad mood, at least under conditions where the individuating information is not clearly inconsistent with the stereotype. Several studies support this general prediction. For example, Bodenhausen, Kramer, and SUsser (1994) presented subjects in different mood states with descriptions of an alleged student misconduct and asked subjects to determine the target's guilt. Subjects in a happy mood rated the offender as more guilty when he was identified as a member of a group that is stereo typically associated with the described offense than when this was not the case. This impact of the stereotype, however, was not observed for subjects in a neutral mood. Similarly, Bodenhausen, Sheppard, and Kramer (1994) observed that sad subjects were less affected by an applicable stereotype than neutral mood subjects. Moreover, Edwards and Weary (1993) reported converging evidence based on naturally depressed MOOD AND STEREOTYPING m?o~s. Non-~epressed subjects were more likely to rely on category mem!; ship mformatton than depressed subjeCts, who seemed to engage in a ffio eff~rtful an~lysis of the individuating information provided to them. Thus, available eVidence suggests that stereotypes have more impact on individuah a happ~ rather than a sad or neutral mood. This conclusion is compatible \\ the notIOns that happy mood reduces processing capacity (Mackie & WOl 1989), that h~ppy mood reduces processing motivation (Schwarz, 1990), or t happ~ mo~d mcreases r~liance on general knowledge structures (Bless, 199, U~~g different mampulations, Bodenhausen, Kramer, and SUsser (19' expl.lcltly c~ntr~sted the re~uced capacity hypothesis and the reduced p cessmg mottvatlOn hypothesIs. To reduce the likelihood that the mood man ulation taxes subjects' cognitive capacity, they selected mood manipulati< that were unlikely to drain cognitive resources (smells facial feedback man ulations) . ~ince thes 7 non-taxing mood inductions r~plicated the previou observed mcreased Impact of stereotypes for happy subjects, the auth, concluded that the cognitive capacity approach cannot account for their fi! ings. Moreover, increasing subjects' processing motivation with an accou ability manipulation (TetJock, 1983) eliminated the differential impact stereotyp.es on hap~y vs. neutral mood subjects. Again this finding sugge that the mcreased Impact of stereotypes under happy moods is not due ~educed proce.ssin.g capacity. At least, any processing constraints that may md~ced by ?emg m a happy mood cannot be very severe, given that they; eaSily overndden by accountability manipulations (Bodenhausen, Kramer. SUsser, 1994) or processing instructions (as observed in the persuasion; main, Bless et al., 1990). Wher7as t.hi~ evidenc7 does not support the reduced processing capac assumption, It IS compatible with the reduced motivation as well as with 1 mood-and-general-knowledge assumption. As mentioned above however 1 latter t-:v0 mechanisms differ with respect to their predictions 'regardin~ 1 processmg of stereotype consistent and inconsistent individuating informati< The Impact of Inconsistent Information If th~ increa~ed imp~ct o~ stereotypes on judgments formed by happy indiv ual~ IS mediated p~lm~nly by ~appy individuals using the stereotype a~ penpheral cue, speCific mformatton about the target is less likely to be elat rated. As a consequence, judgments should be independent of whetl stereotype-consistent or stereotype-inconsistent information is provided. In 1 case ~f inc~nsistent inf~rmation, happy individuals may either not detect may SlIll~ly Ignore ~he discrepancy due to their reduced processing motivati< If th~ mcreased Impact of stereotypes on happy individuals is mediated happ.y mdivi~uals' reliance on general knowledge structures in processi new mformatton, however, specific individuating information is unlikely to 74 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER unnoticed. In this case, their encoding of new individuating information should be guided by the activated stereotype. As long as the individuating information is consistent with the implications of the stereotype, the two sources of information have the same implications for evaluative judgments. As a consequence, the "peripheral cue" and the "guided encoding" mechanisms cannot be distinguished on the basis of individuals' judgments if the individuating information is consistent with the stereotype, as was the case in all previously reported studies. In contrast, individuating information that is inconsistent with the implications of the stereotype should strongly affect the obtained judgments. This requires, however, that the inconsistency is noticed in the first place. According to the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption, this should be more likely when individuals are in a happy mood, and hence more likely to rely on stereotypes, than when they are in a sad mood, and hence unlikely to draw on the stereotype to begin with. Accordingly, the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption predicts that stereotypeinconsistent information receives more weight in judgment formation under happy than under sad moods. Note, however, that dealing with stereotypeinconsistent information requires considerable cognitive resources (e.g. Macrae, Hewstone, & Griffiths, 1993; Stangor & Duan, 1991; see also Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Srull & Wyer, 1989). Thus, observing a more pronounced impact of stereotype-inconsistent information under happy rather than sad mood conditions would argue against the assumption that being in a happy mood reduces processing capacity or processing motivation. To summarize, the assumption of reduced processing under happy mood (either due to a lack of capacity or a lack of motivation) predicts a more pronounced impact of stereotypes when individuals are in a happy rather than in a sad mood. Happy individuals are assumed to rely on the target's category membership as a peripheral cue and to neglect individuating information, thus rendering it irrelevant if the individuating information is consistent or inconsistent with the stereotype. As a result, reduced processing approaches predict a main effect of mood on stereotyping. In contrast, the mood-and-generalknowledge assumption predicts increased stereotyping under happy mood only for conditions under which the individuating information is consistent with the stereotype. According to this assumption, happy individuals are more likely to rely on general knowledge structures and will therefore encode the individuating information in terms of stereo typic concepts. This results in stereotypic judgments when the individuating information is consistent with the stereotype, but not when it is inconsistent. Sad individuals, on the other hand, are expected to process individuating information without drawing on stereotypic knowledge structures, rendering the consistency of the individuating information and the stereotype irrelevant. Accordingly, this approach predicts an interaction effect of mood and the stereotype consistency of the individuating information. MOOD AND STEREOTYPING Investigating the Relative Impact of Categorical and Individuating Information !n ~ s.eries o.f st~dies, Bless, Wieland, and Schwarz (in press) investigated · ~ndlVlduals III different mood states are influenced by category member I~for~ation, by individuating information, and, most importantly, by the) tlOnshlp of the two. In these studies, subjects were provided with categOi and individuating information of different valence about a target person. , result of this orthogonal manipulation, the implications of the stereotype v either consistent or obviously inconsistent with the implications of the tar{ specific behaviors. As the stimuli materials were basically identical in all studies repo below, we first describe these materials. Subjects were told that they woul. listening to a tape-recorded description of a target person and would I answer some questions about it. The tape-recorded description provided categorical as well as the individuating information, which both resulted f extensive pretesting, and which both pertained to the same traits descri below. In the positive category condition the target was presented as worl full-time as a Greenpeace representative, while in the negative category co tion the target was described as a manager of BASF, a large chemical c pany. In all descriptions the category label was mentioned five times, twic the beginning, once in the middle, and twice at the end. In addition, subj were presented with specific behaviors about the target that were combine form a person description. In the positive individuating information condi subjects were presented with six positive and two negative behaviors, whil the negative individuating information condition subjects were presented, six negativ~ and two positive behaviors. Information that had been prete: as unambIguously neutral was added in order to provide cohe: descriptions. After listening to the tape-recording, which took about 3 minutes, subj were asked to evaluate the target along the four dimensions that had emer fr?m the pretest results. Subjects were asked to evaluate the target 's car, ~md~?ness, his consumer orientation, his ecological responsibility, and likabIlIty on four nine-point rating scales. According to pretest data, ecol ca.l responsibility and likability constituted positive attributes, whereas car, mmdedness and consumer orientation constituted negative attributes for subjects. Treating the four judgments as a within-subjects factor did not re in any significant interactions of this factor with the experimental ma: ulations, and a summary score was computed by substracting the rating! the two negative dimensions from the ratings on the two positive dimensi and divid!ng the difference by four. This summary score (Cronbach's alph. 0.74 or hIgher) could range from -4, indicating negative evaluations to indicating positive evaluations. ' 76 HERBERT BLESS, N ORBERT SCHWA RZ A ND MA RKUS KEMMELMEIER MOOD AND STEREOTYPING Experiment 1: The Relation of Categorical and Individuating Information In Experiment 1, subjects in different affective states were exposed to the four different target descriptions outlined above. To induce a happy or sad mood, subjects were asked to report either a positive or negative li~e-event, p~rpor tedly to construct a Life-Event Inventory. Neutral mood subjects descnbed a normal day in their life. An assessment of subjects' mood revealed a reliable effect of these manipulations. Subjects then listened to the tape-recorded descriptions of the target person and evaluated the target al~ng. the fo~ dime~sions. The pattern of results shown in Figure 3.1 reflects a signIficant mteractlOn of mood, valence of category, and valence of individuating information, sugges~ ing that subjects in different mood states made differential use of the ~ateg.on cal and the individuating information. First, it can be seen that subjects m a sad mood were unaffected by the valence of the category, replicating the findings reported by Bodenhausen, Sheppar?, a~d ~amer (199~). ~~reov.er, sad subjects' judgments only reflected the ImplicatIOns of the mdlVlduatmg information, consistent with previous findings reported by Edwards and Weary (1993): the target was evaluated significantly more positively when the majority of the behaviors was positive rather than negative. Like sad subjects, neutral mood subjects were significantly influenced by the individuating information. In addition, however, they were also influenced by the category label and evaluated a member of a positively evaluated category more positively than a member of a negatively evaluated category. The absence of an interaction between the two sources of information suggests that neutral mood subjects used them additively in forming a judgment. For happy mood subjects, no main effect of the categorical information was obtained, although a significant interaction of individuating and categorical information clearly indicates an impact of the category label. However, the specific influence of the category label depended on the nature of the individuating information. If provided with positive individuating information, happy subjects rated the target more favorably when it was a member of the positively rather than the negatively evaluated category. This positive impact of being a member of a positively rather than a negatively evaluated category replicates the findings obtained by Bodenhausen and colleagues (Bodenhausen, Kramer, & SUsser, 1994). This effect reversed, however, when negative individuating information was provided. In this case, the target was rated less favorably when described as a member of a positive category performing negative behaviors rather than as a member of a negative category performing these behaviors. In summary, the results obtained under conditions where the implications of the individuating information were consistent with the implications of the category membership information replicated previous findings by indicating a stronger impact of stereotypes on judgments formed by individuals in a happy than in a sad mood (Bodenhausen, Kramer, & SUsser, 1994; Bodenhausen, 2 Sad mood \ 1 c: 0 ii::l Happy mood Neutral mood 0 'jij Indlvldu Informa' !' > OJ a; +poall ···nega e'·1 {! ' .. • ·2 ri ------' ·3 po.lliv. negallv. pOIIII'll nagatlve poaltlv. nagatlv. Category Information Figure 3.1 Target evaluation as a function of mood, valence of category informa and valence of individuating information (from Bless, Wieland, & Schwarz, in p Experiment 1) Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994; Edwards & Weary, 1993). Findings of this 1 have typically been attributed to a neglect of individuating information UI happy mood conditions, in line with reduced processing assumptions. Howe the results obtained under conditions where the implications of the individ ing information were inconsistent with the implications of the stereotype this conclusion into question. Under these conditions, happy individuals ev ated the target most negatively when the target was described as a member positive group engaging in negative behaviors. If happy subjects had sin relied on the stereotype as a peripheral cue, as predicted by reduced proces assumptions, a member of a positively evaluated group should have been ev ated more favorably than a member of a negatively evaluated group, indel dent of the individuating information. In contrast, the obtained interac effect indicates that happy subjects processed the individuating information elaborated on the relationship between both sources of information. 1 however, implies that happy subjects had sufficient cognitive resources were motivated to bring them to bear on the task, as implied by the mood-, 78 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER general-knowledge assumption. Thus, the pattern of findings obtained under happy mood conditions renders it very unlikely that being in a happy mood reduced subjects' processing capacity or motivation. In general, the observed interaction pattern is compatible with the assumption that the category information guided the encoding of the individuating information. However, the interaction was asymmetrical, as shown in Figure 3.1. Whereas negative behaviors shown by a member of a positive category resulted in a particularly negative evaluation, positive behaviors shown by a member of a negative category did not result in a particularly positive evaluation. Hence, the increase in the impact of inconsistent information predicted by the " guided encoding" assumption was only observed under positive category membership conditions. Why, however, was this pattern not observed when the implications of a negative behavior contradicted the implications of positively evaluated category? This issue was addressed in a subsequent study. MOOD AND STEREOTYPING 8 Individuating Information • positive III negative 6 2 Experiment 2: The Diagnosticity of Negative and Positive Information If the increased impact of inconsistent information under happy mood results from its discrepancy to the categorical information, then the asymmetry could be due to a differential degree of inconsistency in the stimuli materials. In fact, previous research demonstrated that negative behaviors are generally considered more diagnostic for morality related judgments than positive behaviors (Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989; see also Rothbart & Park, 1986). Although bad persons may occasionally do good things, doing one bad thing implies one isn't really a good person. Thus, environmentally irresponsible behaviors may be perceived as being more discrepant with the category label "Greenpeace activist" than are environmentally responsible behaviors with the category label "manager of a chemical company". This differential degree of inconsistency may underlie the asymmetric nature of the interaction of category membership and individuating information observed under the happy mood conditions of Experiment 1. The plausibility of this explanation was explored by presenting subjects in a neutral mood with the same target descriptions as in Experiment 1. Instead of forming evaluating judgments, however, subjects rated the discrepancy between the implications of the category label and the described behaviors. The results presented in Figure 3.2 clearly supported the assumption that the combination of a positive category with negative behaviors reflected a higher degree discrepancy than the reversed combination. Presumably, this pattern is not specific to the present materials, but reflects that negative behaviors are generally judged as more diagnostic for morality-related judgments than positive behaviors (Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989). These findings have direct implications for the interpretation of Experiment 1. Specifically, we suggested that as a result of happy subjects processing o . negative positive Category Information Figure 3.2 Perceived discrepancy as a function of valence of category informa and valence of individuating information (from Bless, Wieland, & Schwarz, in pI Experiment 2) the behavioral information in terms of their general knowledge about target's category, the obviously inconsistent information received additic attention and strongly affected subjects' judgments. If, however, only negative behaviors are perceived as inconsistent with the positive categ( the impact of inconsistent behavioral information should be restricted to . condition-as was the case in Experiment 1. Experiment 3: The Functional Equivalence of Explicit Instructions and Different Mood States The results of Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that happy subjects' encoding of individuating information was guided by their knowledge about the targ category membership. As this impact was not observed in the form of a rr effect of the category information, the findings render it unlikely that hal subjects merely used the category information as a peripheral cue. Rather, results that happy subjects related the specific behavioral information to implications of the target's category membership. If so, explicitly instruct neutral mood subjects to process the behavioral information in relation to 80 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER target's category membership should result in a similar pattern as inducing happy mood. Similarly, the reported evidence suggests that sad subjects did not rely on the category information but focused solely on the behavioral information. If so, explicitly instructing neutral mood subjects to focus on the behavioral information should result in a similar pattern as inducing a sad mood. To test these considerations, subjects in a third experiment were provided with the four target descriptions above and were either instructed to focus " especially on the relation between the target's profession and the individual behaviors" (an instruction supposed to elicit the strategy presumably observed under happy mood conditions) or "to focus especially on the individual behaviors" (an instruction supposed to elicit the strategy presumably observed under sad mood conditions). These manipulations resulted in a significant interaction of instruction, valence of category, and vaJence of individuating information, as shown in Figure 3.3. A comparison of Figure 3.1 and Figure 3.3 illustrates the similarity of the patterns obtained in both experiments. Specifically, subjects who were instructed to focus on the individuating information reported a pattern of judgments similar to the one obtained for sad subjects in Experiment 1. These subjects rated the target more favorably if positive rather than negative behaviors were presented. In contrast, subjects who were instructed to focus on the relation between the individuating and the categorical information reported a pattern of judgments similar to the one obtained for happy subjects in Experiment 1, as indicated by the same significant simple interaction of category and individuating information. When provided with positive individuating information, these subjects rated the target more favorably when it was a member of a positively rather than a negatively evaluated category. As in Experiment 1, the impact of positive category membership information was again eliminated, however, when negative individuating information was provided and the target was rated more unfavourably when described as a member of a positively rather than a negatively evaluated category. In combination, these results and the findings of Experiment 2 suggest that happy individuals are more likely to relate individuating information to relevant category information than are sad individuals. Of course a similar effect pattern does not necessarily imply the same underlying mechanism. However, the similarity of the results suggests a functional equivalence of (a) explicitly instructing subjects to relate individuating behaviors to the category label and being in a happy mood, and (b) explicitly instructing subjects to focus on the individuating information and being in a sad mood. Experiment 4: The Timing of the Category Information The findings reviewed so far suggest that happy individuals are more strongly influenced by category membership information than sad individuals. On a MOOD AND STEREOTYPING 2 - Focus on relation Focus on individuating information 1 ~" 0;------------------------------------- .. " > . ftj .. Individuating Ir + positive -• . negative .' . -2 -3~-----------------------------positive negatlvB positive negative Category Information Figure 3.3 Target evaluation as a function of instructional set, valence of cate information, and valence of individuating information (from Bless, Wielanc Schwarz, in press; Experiment 3) general level this conclusion converges with previous research Bodenhausen and colleagues (e.g., Bodenhausen, Kramer & Susser, 1994) emphasized above, however, the present findings do not reflect a main el of mood but an interaction effect of mood and the stereotype consistenc the individuating information. This interaction could not be detected in vious research because the stimulus materials did only include stereoi consistent or stereotype irrelevant information. While the obtained inte tion is incompatible with a reduced processing assumption, it follows dire from the mood-and-general-knowledge hypothesis. In addition to their differential ability to account for the observed inte tion, the competing theoretical assumptions differ in their predictions reg; ing the processing stage at which category membership information may e: its influence. The mood-and-general-knowledge hypothesis holds that ha subjects relate the individuating information to the category member: information. This obviously requires that the target's category membershi 82 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER known when subjects are exposed to the behavioral description. Acquiring the category membership information after the behavioral information is encoded should therefore greatly reduce the impact of category information. Thus, the pattern obtained for happy subjects in Experiment 1 should be restricted to conditions under which subjects know the target's category membership when they are exposed to the behavioral description. In contrast, the reduced processing assumption holds that subjects use the category membership information as a peripheral cue to simplify th.e j~dgment task. I~ s~, category membership information may also exert Its mftuence when It IS presented after the individuating information. To test these competing predictions, the person descriptions were modified so that the category membership information either preceded or followed the individuating information. Specifically, the target's category membership was mentioned three times, either at the beginning or at the end of the descriptions (in the materials used in the preceding experiments the category membership information was presented five times, twice in the beginning, once in the middle and twice at the end). Prior to the presentation of the descriptions, happy or sad mood was again successfully induced by having subjects report either a positive or negative life-event. As expected, the results obtained in Experiment 1 replicated when the category label preceded the individuating information, but not when it followed the individuating information. When the category label was presented at the beginning of the description, the pattern of results was very similar to Experiment 1, resulting in the same significant three-way interaction, as shown in Figure 3.4. Under these conditions, sad subjects were again unaffected by the category membership information and evaluated the target more positively when positive rather than negative behaviors were presented. For happy subjects a significant interaction of individuating and category membership information in form of the already known pattern was obtained. Again, judgments were congruent with the implications of the category when the individuating information was positive. As in Experiment 1, negative individuating information eliminated this pattern, due to its particularly strong impact when it was inconsistent with the implications of category membership. Thus, the findings of Experiment 1 were replicated when the category membership information preceded the individuating information. However, a completely different pattern emerged when the category information was presented after the individuating information. In this case, subjects evaluated the target more positively when positive rather than negative behaviors were presented, and when positive rather than negative category information was provided. However, there were no indications that subjects in different mood states made differential use of the category membership or of the individuating information (all interactions involving mood were nonsignificant). MOOD AND STEREOTYPING Sad mood Happy mood § O·· t-----------------------__________~-;;; Indlvlduatln! Information I positive " ~ • negative lii e> .1 - {! ·2 ·3 -· ~-------- positive ___________________ negative positive negative Category Information Figure 3:4 Target evaluatio~ as. ~ fun~tion of instructional set, valence of categ informatIOn, and. va.lence of Indtvtduatlng when the category information was I sented at the beginning (from Bless, Wieland, & Schwarz, in press; Experiment 4) The reliability of the pattern obtained in Experiment 1 under conditi< where the catego:y information preceded the individuating information, c t~e ab~en~e of thIS pattern when the category information followed the in ~Iduatmg mformati?n, supports the assumption that happy subjects are m. lIkely to rely on theIr general knowledge structures than sad subjects. Presu abl~, happy subjects' encoding of the individuating information was guided theIr .knowled~e about the category-which was only possible when the ca gory mformatIOn preceded the behavioral information. WHAT MEDIATES THE IMPACT OF CATEGORICAL INFORMATION ON HAPPY INDIVIDUALS? In combi~ation, th~ reported studies support previous findings suggesting tJ category mformatIOn has a stronger impact on judgment formation wh individuals are in a happy rather than a sad mood (e.g., Bodenhaus. Kramer, & Susser, 1994; Bodenhausen, Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994; EdwaJ 84 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER & Weary, 1993). By manipulating the valence of the individuating informa- tion the present research provides a better understanding of (a) how the impact of the category information affects judgments, and of (b) what causes happy individuals to rely on category membership information. Most obviously, the specific nature of the impact of the category information, in particular the increased impact of inconsi~tent negative in~ivi~~ating information, is not consistent with the assumptIOn that happy mdlVlduals resort to a target's category membership as a peripheral cue that allows them to form a judgment without paying much attention to the individuating information. If happy subjects simply used the target's category membership as a peripheral cue, a main effect of the category membership information should have been obtained under happy mood conditions (Experiments 1 and 4); moreover, such a main effect should also have been obtained when the category membership information was introduced after the individuating. infor~a tion (Experiment 4)-neither of which was the case. In fact, subjects m a happy mood were most likely to use category membership and in?ivid~ating information in the present studies, and elaborated on the relatIOnshIp between the two. This elaboration, however, requires a considerable amount of processing capacity and motivation (e.g. Macrae, Hewstone, & Griffiths, 1993; Stangor & Duan, 1991; see also Fiske & Taylor, 1991). This additional processing should not have been observed if being in a happy mood reduced processing capacity (e.g., Mackie & Worth, 1989) or processing motivation (e.g., Schwarz, 1990).1 Accordingly, the present results render it unlikely that the happy subjects were either unable or unwilling to process the individuating information provided to them. Rather, the pattern of results is most consistently accounted for by the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption, which does not imply any capacity constraints. This perspective holds that happy individuals are more likely to rely on general knowledge structures and to process specific information in light of them. As a consequence, inconsistent information is more likely to be noticed and elaborated on and results in a stronger impact on judgments. This conclusion is further supported by the finding that explicitly instructing neutral mood subjects to relate individuating and categorical information to one another resulted in a similar pattern as inducing a happy mood. Moreover, the crucial pattern of results was only observed when the categorical information I Onc may try to reconcilc the obtaincd findings with the assumption that being in a happy mood decreases the likelihood of systematic processing, as other goals or relevant fcatures of the situation may elicit additional processing (Schwarz, 1990). Thus, one would assume that the inconsistencies instigated more cffortful processing of the detailed information. Note, however, that only judgments of happy subjects showed an !nteraction of individ~ ating and categorical information, while judgments of neutral mood subjects showed an additive Impact of the two sources of information. This suggests that the inconsistency by itself was not sufficient for the interaction effect observed under happy mood conditions. MOOD AND STEREOTYPING preceded the individuating information, thus allowing subjects to process behaviors in light of category membership information, but not when categorical information followed the individuating information. Finally, the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption can account previous findings which seemed to support the notion that being in a ha mood reduces processing motivation. As reviewed above, Bodenhausen his colleagues (e.g., Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser, 1994) observed a I nounced impact of category membership information under happy but under sad mood. They attributed this finding to reduced processing moi tion under happy mood, after having ruled out an account in terms 01 duced processing capacity. However, their conclusion was based on stu in which the target's category membership was either consistent with implications of the target's behavior or irrelevant to these behaviors. Ur these specific conditions, the mood-and-general-knowledge assump leads to the same predictions and the respective conditions of the pre: experiments replicated Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser (1994) findi The resulting main effect pattern, however, was not observed when inca stent individuating information was provided. Under this condition, ar teraction of category and individuating information emerged, sugges that happy subjects used the category information as a processing guide the encoding of the individuating information and did not rely on the c gory information as a peripheral cue at the expense of individuating in mation. Thus, the independent manipulation of the evaluative implicat: of individuating and categorical information allowed us to investigate I happy subjects use category membership information in arriving ( judgment-and the observed use is inconsistent with the redw processing-motivation assumption we favored in our own previous rese, (e.g., Schwarz & Bless, 1991). In sum, the reported findings provide substantial evidence for the m( and-general-knowledge model introduced by Bless (1994; Bless et al. press). Building on earlier theorizing (Schwarz, 1990; Schwarz & Bless, 19 this model assumes that affective states inform the individual about the na of current situation, with happy moods signalling a benign situation and moods signalling a problematic situation. The information provided by ha moods presumably implies that it is possible to rely on one's general knl edge structures, which usually serve us well. In contrast, the informa provided by sad moods presumably implies that reliance on general knl edge structures is risky, given the nature of the situation, which may req attention to specifics. If so, happy individuals may feel more confident to on their general knowledge than sad individuals. As discussed above, sUi processing style should have resulted in a main effect of category member: information, without much impact of the more detailed individuating in mation, which should have received little attention. In contrast, the me 86 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER and-general-knowledge structures assumption accounts for the observed interaction effect of both sources of information under happy mood conditions. As emphasized above, this interaction indicates that happy subjects not only paid attention to the individuating information, but processed it in terms of the implications of their general knowledge about the target's categ~ry membership-resulting in an increased impact of stereotypes and an mcreased impact of stereotype inconsistent information. . Importantly, in the domain of stereotyping, the assumed stereotype gUIded encoding of individuating information can only be distinguished from the sheer reliance on category membership as a peripheral cue when the implications of the individuating behavior contradict the implications of category membership. In contrast, when the implications of the individuating information are consistent with, or unrelated to, the target's category membership, stereotype-guided encoding and reliance on category membership as a peripheral cue result in the same pattern of judgments. Overriding Effects and the Impact of Specific Emotions Whereas the mood-and-general-knowledge assumption generally implies that positive affective states should increase, and negative affective states should decrease, reliance on general knowledge structures, the effects elicited by the valence of mood can be overriden by other factors. For example, increasing individuals' processing motivation or increasing processing capacity will generally decrease reliance on general knowledge structures (see Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Kruglanski, ·1989, for overviews). Depending on the relative contributions of the affective state and the processing motivation and capacity, mood effects might not be observable. This has been demonstrated in studies which, in addition to mood, manipulated subjects' processing motivation or processing capacity. As expected, happy subjects' reliance on heuristics and stereotypes decreased as processing motivation (Bless et ai., 1990; Bodenhausen, Kramer, and Susser, 1994) or processing capacity (Mackie & Worth, 1989) increased. Similarly, an increased reliance on general knowledge structures can also result from individuals' arousal levels being either very low (e.g. Bodenhausen, 1990) or very high (Kim & Baron, 1988; Paulhus, Martin, & Murphy, 1992; see also Broadbent, 1971). As affective states may differ with respect to the associated level of arousal, the valence of the affective state may not be the only determinant of the use of general knowledge structures. If so, negative affective states associated with a high level of arousal may increase the impact of general knowledge structures. Some evidence for this assumption has been reported by Bodenhausen, Sheppard, and Kramer (1994). Investigating the impact of different negative affective states, the authors observed that sad subjects were less influenced by stereotypes than MOOD AND STEREOTYPING neutral mood subjects. In contrast, however, inducing an angry m, presumably associated with a higher level of arousal-elicited an incre stereotyping. Similarly, the emotional state of anxiety has been fou facilitate stereotypic judgments (Wilder, 1993). CONCLUSIGNS The available evidence on the relation between individuals' mood an !mpact of stereotypes suggests that positive affective states may increa~ Impact of stereotypes, while negative affective states may decrease the il of s~ereotyp~~ on evaluative judgments. Given that most people report be a mildly pOSItIve mood most of the time (e.g., Diener et ai., 1985), this seE spell trouble. Depending on the assumptions made about the underlyinl cess, however, there may be a silver lining at the horizon. As the rev. eviden.ce indicates, the pronounced impact of stereotypes under happy mmost likely due to happy individuals' tendency to process new informat light of existing general knowledge structures. If so, happy individuals ShOi more likely to apply a stereotype. As a result of drawing on the stere. however, they should also be more likely to detect discrepancies between ~tereoty~e and new, stereotype-inconsistent information. As relating th, mf~rmatIOn to the stereotype seems essential for changing the stereotyp vanous models see Hewstone, 1989, 1994; Rothbart, 1981; Weber & en 1983), we may therefore speculate that being in a happy mood may fac stereotype change. In contrast, sad individuals have been found to be less to rely on stereotypes in forming a judgment. Rather, sad individuals primarily on individuating information. Although this lack of stereotypir der s~d mood may seem desirable in many ways, it has negative implica Mos~ lffiportantly, it implies that sad individuals are unlikely to notice inc tencies betwe~n individuating information and the stereotype they hold. r.esult, they w.Ill n.ot experience a need to revise their stereotypes, rendeJ hkel~ that ?emg m a sad mood inhibits stereotype change. These possit proVIde an Important avenue for future research. S~milarly, future ~esearch could also benefit from relating to the pI findmgs to research mto the impact of moods on stereotype-related cog pr.ocesses that are .not directly involved in forming evaluative judgmen thIS respect, affectIve states have been found to influence the percepti outgroup hom~geneity (Stroessner & Mackie, 1992), the phenomena lusory. correlatIOns (Stroessner, Hamilton, & Mackie, 1992), and, pe most Importantly, the amount of ingroup favoritism (Forgas & Fiedl press). In .additio~, future r~search needs to extend current experiments b dressmg the Impact of mtegral affect, that is, affect elicited by (or at MOOD AND STEREOTYPING 88 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER attributed to) the target (Bodenhausen, 1993). So far, research into mood and stereotyping has focused on incidental affect, such as mood states induced in a manner that is unrelated to the target. Whether this is an advantage or disadvantage depends on one's primary research goal. If one's primary goal is to understand the role of affective states in social judgment, relying on incidental affect inductions has the advantage that it allows researchers to isolate the role of affective states. This is impossible to accomplish when affect is elicited by the target itself, as this strategy would require comparisons between targets that elicit different affect (in which case the targets are bound to differ on many other dimensions) or comparisons between individuals who hold different stereotypes about the same target (in which case the individuals' knowledge structures are bound to differ in many ways). On the other hand, reliance on incidental affect inductions limits the implications of the obtained results for situations in which affect is an integral part of the stereotype. We trust, however, that future research into the role of integral affect will benefit from an understanding of the impact of incidental affect, which will help in disentangling some of the unavoidable confounds that this research will have to tackle. Bless,d H. G .,'Schwarz, N.' Golisano , V ., Rabe ' C ., & Wo-lk , M. (.In press ) M c h , Clore,f an t el~se 0 scnp.ts: Does happy mood make people really mindless? lou'rnal Persona Ity and SOCial Psychology. Bless, H., & Fiedler, K (1995). Affective states and the influence of activated ene g knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 7766-78 Bless, H., Mackie, ~. M., & S~hwarz, N. (1992). Mood ~ffe~ts on en~oding and 'u( mental processes In persuasIOn. lournal of Personality and Social PsychologyJ . lli~. ' Bless, H ., Wiela~d,.~. & S.ch~arz, N. (in press). Mood and stereotyping: The impact category and mdlViduatlng mformation. European lournal of Social Psychology Bo~enhau~e~, G. ~19~0): St~reotypes as judgmental heuristics. Evidence of ci~, dian vanatlOns m dlscnmlnatlOn. Psychological Science, 1, 319-22. B<1e~ha~sen, G. V. (1993). Emotions, arousal, and stereotype-based discriminati, eunshc model ~~ affect and stereotyping. In D. M. Mackie & D . L. Hamilt «EdS), Affect, Cog~ztlOn, and Stereotyping: Interactive processes in group percepti pp. 13-35). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Bod~nh.aus~n, G .. V ..' Kramer, G. P., & SUsser, K (1994). Happiness and stereoty In SOCial Judgment. lournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, y. ~~I.nklng 6~ Bode~h~usen, G. V ., Sh~ppard, . L. .A ., & Kramer, G . P. (1994). Negative affect a Judgment: The dlfferenhallmpact of anger and sadness. European lournal SOCial Psychology, 24, 45-62. Boden~ausen, V., &. Wyer, R. S., Jr (1985). Effects of stereotypes on decisi( making and informatIOn-processing strategies. lournal of Personality and Soc Psychology, 48, 267-82. Bohner, G., Crow, K, E:rb, H .-P., & Schwarz, N. (1993). Affect and persuasion: Mo effe~tls on the processmg of message content and context cues. European lournal occa Psychology, 22,511-30. BOLucher~ J., & Osgood, C E. (1969). The Pollyanna hypothesis. lournal of Veri earmng and Verbal Behavior, 8, 1-8. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and me.mory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-48. Bower, G ..H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgment. In J. Forgas (Ed) Emoti ., and Soccal ludgments (pp. 31-53). Oxford: Pergamon. Brewer, M. A. (1988). A .d ual model of impression formation. In T. K Srull & R. Wyer (Eds), Advances zn Social Cognition , 1, 1-35. Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and Stress. London: Academic Press Clore, <? L. Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Cognitive causes and c~nsequences ~~~hpon. 32n3~'17s), wH~lelrd&1 TN ' K . Srull (Eds), Handbook of Social Cognition (2: ,p. -. . I S a e, J : Erlbaum. Dalrley, J. M ., & Gros~, P. H . (19.83). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effec . ourna l 0 f PersonallCY and Soccal Psychology, 44, 20-33. Dle~er, E ..' Larsen, R. ~., Levi~~, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and fre uen, ~Iml enslOns underlymg positive and negative affect. lournal of Personality dnd S cia Psychology, 48, 1253-65. Do!lard, J., Doob, ~. W., Miller, M. E., Mowrer, O . H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustl tlOn an d AggreSSIOn. New Haven: Yale University Press. Ed,:"ards,.J . .A., & Weary, G: (1993) . Depression and the impression-formation cc tlnuum. Piecemeal processmg despite the availability of category infonnation lot n,al of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 636-45. . EllJS, H . C, & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects .epressed m~od states on memory. In K Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds), Affect, Cogl Clon, and Soccal BehaVior (pp. 25-43). Toronto: Hogrefe International. socI~1 ? ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The reported research was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Bl 286/5 to H . Bless, N. Schwarz, & M. Wanke; Schw 278/5 to N. Schwarz, H. Bless, and G. Bohner; and a DAAD (German Academic Exchange Service) fellowship to M . Kemmelmeier). Thanks are due to Galen V. Bodenhausen, Gerd Bohner, and Klaus Fiedler for stimulating discussions and comments on a previous draft. REFERENCES Abelson, R. P. (1981). The psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36,715-29. Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D . J., & Sanford, R . N. (1950). The Authoritarian Personality. New York: Harper. Allport, G . W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bettelheim, G ., & Janowitz, M. (1949). Ethnic tolerance: A function of social and personal control. American lournal of Sociology, 55, 137-45. Blau, P. (1977). A macrosociological theory of social structure. American lournal of Sociology, 83, 26-54. Bless, H. (1994). Stimmung und die Nutzung allgemeiner Wissensstrukturen: Ein Modell zum Einflu~ von Stimmungen auf Denkprozesse [Mood and the use of general knowledge structures]. Habilitationsschrift, Universitat Heidelberg. Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16,331-45. S r 90 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER Fiedler, K (1991). On the task, the measures and the mood in research on affect and cognition. In: J. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and Social Judgments (pp. 83-104). Oxford: Pergamon. . Fiske, S. T. (1988). Compare and contrast: Brewer's dual process model and Fiske .et al.'s continuum model. In T . K Srull & R S. Wyer (Eds.) Advances m Social Cognition , 1, 65-67. . Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E . (1991). Social Cognition. New 'york: ~cGraw-HI.ll . Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of ImpreSSIOn .formatlOn f~om category-based to individuating processing: Influences of mformatlo~ and m~tlva tion on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Adv~nces lit Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1-74). Orlando, Fl:-: .Academlc P~ess. Forgas, J . P. (1992). Affect in social judgments and decIsIOns: A mUlti-process model. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. . . .. . 227-75). San Diego: Academic Press. Forgas, J. P., & Fiedler, K (in pr~ss). M?~d effects on mtergroup d.lscnmmatl~n: The role of affect in reward allocatIOn decIsIOns. Journal of Personailly and Social Psychology. . Freud, S. (1933). New Introductory Lectures on PsychoanalysIS: New 'york: Morton. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovido, J. F. (1977). The subtlety of white raCism, arousal and . helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 691-708. Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and applIcation of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509-17. Graesser, A c., Gordon, S. E . & Sawyer, J. D . (1979). Memory for typic~l and atypical actions in scripted activities: Test of a script pointer + tag hypotheSIS. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior, 18, 319-32. Hamilton, D. L. (1979). A cognitive-attributional analysis of stereotyping. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, (Vol. 12, pp. 53-84). New York: Academic Press. Hamilton, D . L. (1981). Stereotyping and intergroup behavior: Some thoughts.on the cognitive approach. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive Processes in Stereotypmg and Intergroup Behavior (pp. 333-53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hamilton, D . L., & Sherman, J. W. (1994). Stereotypes. In R S. Wyer & T. K Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (2nd Edn, Vol. 1). Hills~ale, NJ: Erlba.um. Hamilton, D . L. & Trolier, T. (1986). Stereotypes and stereotypmg: An .oven:le~ o~ the cognitive approach. In J. F. Dovido & S. L. Gaert~er (Eds), PreJudIce, Dlscnmlnation, and Racism (pp. 127-63). New York: ~cademlc. Press. . Hilgard, E . R (1980). A trilogy of mind: CognitIOn, affectIon, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16, 1~7-1? .. . Hewstone, M. (1989). Changing stereotypes with dlsconfirmmg mformatlOn. In D . BarTal, C. F. Graumann, A W. Kruglanski, & W. Stroebe (Eds), Stereotypes and Prejudice (pp. 207-23). New York: Springer. . . Hewstone, M. (1994). Revising and change of stereotyplc belIefs: In search ~f the elusive sUbtyping model. In W. Stroebe & ~. Hewsto~e (Eds), European ReVIew of Social Psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 69-109). Chichester: Wiley: .. Isen, A M (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect m cogmtlOn. In R ~. Wyer & T . K Srull (Eds), Handbook of Social Cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203-53). San Diego: Academic Press. Kim, H .-S., & Baron, R S. (1988). Exercise and illusory correlation: Does arousal heighten stereo typic processing? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 366-80. MOOD AND STEREOTYPING Kruglanski, A W. (1989). The psychology of being "right": On the problem curacy in social perception and cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 395-40' Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 101 98. Lemyre, L., & Smith, P. M. (1985). Intergroup discrimination and self-esteem . minimal group paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 6 Lippmann, W. (1922). Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace. Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. (1989). Cognitive deficits and the mediation of p' affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27-40. Macrae, C. N., Hewstone, M., & Griffiths, R J. (1993). Processing load and m for stereotype-based information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, Macrae, C. N., Milne, A . B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as e saving devices: A peek inside the toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Ps ogy, 66, 37-47. Macrae, C. N., & Shepherd, J. W. (1989). Stereotypes and social judgments . . Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 319-25. Martin, L. M., Ward, D . W., Achee, J. W., & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: I have to interpret the motiva tional implications of their moods. Journal of P ality and Social Psychology, 64, 317-26. Matlin, M. W. , & Stang, D . J. (1979). The Pollyanna Principle. Cambridge Schenkman. Meindl, J. R , & Lerner, M. (1984). Exacerbation of extreme responses to a group. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 71-84. Messick, D . M. & Mackie, D. M. (1989). Intergroup relations. In M. R Rosenz~ L. W. Porter (Eds), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 45-81). Pale CA: Annual Reviews. Miller, N., & Bugelski, R . (1948). Minor studies in aggression: The influence of f: tions imposed by the in-group on attitudes expressed by the out-group. JOll! Psychology, 25, 437-42. Nisbett, R E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and shortcomi social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Oakes, P. 1. & Turner, J. C. (1980). Social categorization and intergroup beh, Does minimal intergroup discrimination make social identity more positive? pean Journal of Social Psychology, 10,295-301. Paulhus, D. L., Martin, C. T ., & Murphy, G . K (1992). Some effects of arousal stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18,325-30. Reeder, G. D. & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of dispositional attri in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 86,61-79. Rojahn, K, & Pettigrew, T . F. (1992). Memory for schema-relevant informat meta-analytic resolution. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31,81-110. Rothbart, M. (1981). Memory processes and social beliefs. In D. L. Hamilton Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior (pp. 145-82). Hi! NJ: Erlba um. Rothbart, M., & Park, B. (1986). On the confirmability and disconfirmability ( concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 131-42. Sagar, H. A, & Schofield, J. W. (1980). Racial and behavioral cues in black anc children's perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. Journal of Personali Social Psychology, 39,590-98. Schwarz, N., (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational fur of affective states. In R M. Sorrentino & E. T . Higgins, Handbook of Motl and Cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527-61) .. New Guilford. 92 HERBERT BLESS, NORBERT SCHWARZ AND MARKUS KEMMELMEIER Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). H appy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. 1. In J. Forgas (Ed.): Emotion and Social Judgments (pp. 55-71). Oxford: Pergamon. Schwarz, N., Bless, H., & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion: Affective states influence the processing of persuasive communications. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in ExperimentaL SociaL PsychoLogy (Vol. 24, pp. 161- 97). New York: Academic Press. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-23. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (in press). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds), Social Psychology: A handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford. Sinclair, R. c., Mark, M. M., & Clore, G. L. (1994). Mood-related persuasion depends on misattributions. Social Cognition, 12, 309-26. Sinclair, R. c., & Mark, M. M. (1992). The influence of mood state on judgment and action: Effects on persuasion, categorization, social justice, person perception, and judgmental accuracy. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds), The Construction of Social Judgment (pp. 165-93). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E . (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 131-42. Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 36, 1202-12. Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D ., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656-66. Stangor, C., & Duan, C. (1991). Effects of multiple task demands upon memory for information about social groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 357-78. Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social develop mental literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42-61. Stroebe, W., & Insko, C. A. (1989). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination: Changing conceptions in theory and research. In D. Bar-Tal, C. F. Graumann, A. W. Kruglanski, & W. Stroebe (Eds), Stereotypes and Prejudice (pp. 3- 36). New York: Springer. Stroessner, S. J., Hamilton, D . L. & Mackie, D. M. (1992). Affect and stereotyping: The effect of induced mood on distinctiveness-bass illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,564-76. Stroessner, S. J., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). The impact of induced affect on the perception of variability in social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 546-54. Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological Review, 96, 58-83. Tajfel, H. (Ed.) (1982) Social Identity and Intergoup Relations. London: Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In W. G. Austin & W. Worchel (Eds), The Psychology of Intergroup Relations (2nd Edn, pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 74-83. Tomkins, S. S. (1981). The quest for primary motives: Biography and autobiography of an idea. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,306-29. MOOD AND STEREOTYPING Turner-, J. C. (1987). Rediscovering the social group. A Self-categorization theory_ ford: Basil Blackwell. Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of stereo beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 961-77. Wilder, D. A. (1993). The role of anxiety in facilitating stereo typic judgmen outgroup behavior. In D. Mackie, & D. L. Hamilton (Eds), Affect, Cognition, Stereotyping (pp. 87-109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Worth, L. T., & Mackie, D . M. (1987). Cognitive mediation of positive affe persuasion. Social Cognition,S, 76-94. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking. Preferences need no inferences. Ame. _ Psychologist, 35, 151-75.