from univ-amu.fr - Laboratoire de psychologie cognitive
Transcription
from univ-amu.fr - Laboratoire de psychologie cognitive
Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Veterinary Behavior journal homepage: www.journalvetbehavior.com Canine Review Canis sensitivus: Affiliation and dogs’ sensitivity to others’ behavior as the basis for synchronization with humans? Charlotte Duranton a, b, *, Florence Gaunet a a b Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, UMR 7290, Marseille, France AVA Association, Cuy-Saint-Fiacre, France a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Received 30 January 2015 Received in revised form 5 August 2015 Accepted 18 August 2015 Available online 5 September 2015 One widely studied phenomenon in behavioral sciences is the ability of 2 individuals to behave in a synchronous way, which has links with affiliative relationships and an adaptive role in increasing fitness. Behavioral synchrony is found in various social species, but has been very little studied at the interspecific level. The unique relationship of humans and dogs deserves to be studied from this perspective. This article reviews present knowledge about dogs’ sensitivity to other dogs’ and humans’ behaviors. The conclusions are that (1) dogs are sensitive to other dogs’ and humans’ direct behavior, and adjust their own behaviors accordingly; (2) dogs use some information from third-party interactions, and adjust their behavioral response according to the outcome of the interactions; and (3) some recent data suggest that dogs are sensitive to humans’ emotional cues, that they show social referencing toward humans, and emphasize the strong affiliative bond between dogs and their owners. This finding suggests that dogs can be considered as an appropriate biological model to study interspecies synchronization with humans. We propose that dogs could mirror at least their owners during day-to-day situations and encourage further research in this field. Ó 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: domestic dog behavioral synchronization behavioral mimicry dog-human synchronization dog-human affiliation Introduction Being synchronized1 with other individuals can be defined by 3 complementary characteristics, 3 types of synchrony. First, the temporal aspect of synchronization (also called temporal synchrony), is defined as switching actions at the same time (Dostálková and Spinka, 2007; Lakin et al., 2003). The actions/activities/events can be identical or different (i.e., if all individuals in a group switch from inactivity to activity, even if they are performing different actionsdone switching from resting to scratching itself, another from resting to yawning, and the third from resting to standing up and moving), but the important feature is the timing. * Address for reprint requests and correspondence: Duranton Charlotte, Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, UMR 7290, 3 place Victor Hugo, CS 80249, Bât. 9, Case D, 13331 Marseille Cedex 03, France, Tel: þ33 4 13 55 09 60; Fax: þ33 4 13 55 09 65. E-mail address: charlotte.duranton@cegetel.net (C. Duranton). 1 In this review, we chose to focus only on nonconscious synchronization. Indeed, conscious synchronization is a full field of study, leading to more complex phenomena such as cooperation, but we did not review it. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.08.008 1558-7878/Ó 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Second, the behavioral aspect of synchronization, often called activity synchrony, is defined as performing the same behaviors as others in a short lap of time. This type of synchronization is also often called behavioral matching, allelomimicry or nonconscious behavioral mimicry in the literature (Chartrand and Barg, 1999; Chartrand and Lakin, 2013; Gautrais et al., 2007; Lakin et al., 2003; Louwerse et al., 2012). Third, the local aspect of synchronization, named local synchrony, is defined as being in the same place at the same time (Bertram, 1980; King and Cowlishaw, 2009). These aspects are often combined together and/or cited under the name of synchronization in a broad sense (for each temporal/behavioral/ local synchrony alone or combined together), as suggested by Louwerse et al. (2012), and we chose to do the same in the following parts of the present review. Synchronization is widespread among live beings and has various adaptive values. Indeed, being synchronized with others increases the fitness of offspring by decreasing the pressure of predation (Yasaka et al., 2003), increases the chances of survival of each member by avoiding predators (Conradt and Roper, 2000; Fernandez et al., 2003), increases foraging efficiency (King and Cowlishaw, 2009; Stone et al., 2003), and increases social 514 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 cohesion among individuals (Gautrais et al., 2007; Rook and Penning, 1991). Animals in pairs also benefit from behavioring in a synchronous way. Synchrony helps to maintain pair bonds between partners (Lakin et al., 2003), which has direct benefits on the pairs’ behaviors, for example, by increasing the efficiency of antipredation strategies such as vigilance in pademelons (Pays et al., 2007; 2008), of a huddle in penguins (Ancel et al., 2009), and of arrival in migratory birds (Gunnarsson et al., 2004). Does synchronization also occur between 2 individuals from different species? Surprisingly, this question has been very little studied. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the existence of interspecific synchronization as a main topic, focusing on activity synchrony between humans and capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus), with the latter interacting more with people who did behaviorally synchronize with them (Paukner et al., 2009). The lack of studies on activity synchrony between individuals from different species could be due to methodological and observational issues. Indeed, it is possible that interspecific synchronization mainly occurs between stable group members, or at least needs the 2 protagonists to regularly interact. These situations are not very common. One situation in which individuals from different species form a stable group does exist and is easily observable: humans sharing their daily life with dogs (Canis familiaris). The 2 main hypotheses proposed to explain the special relationship between domestic dogs and their human caregivers are the effects of a highly ancient domestication (Hare et al., 2002; 2010; Vilá et al., 1997) and the effects of dogs’ history of life (Wynne et al., 2008), the 2 being not mutually exclusive (Udell and Wynne, 2010). Anecdotes report that dogs sleep when humans sleep and are active when they are active. Pet (Hart, 1995; Serpell, 1995, 1996; White et al., 2010) or working (Gaunet and Milliet, 2010) dogs are known to form strong affiliative bonds with humans. In humans, it is known that synchronization and affiliation are positively correlated (Cheng and Chartrand, 2003; Lakin et al., 2003), but the ability of dogs to synchronize their behavior with human behavior has not been studied to date. The aim of this review is to discuss some prerequisites in dogs leading to synchronization with humans: that is attention or sensitivity to others’ behaviors and the ability to adjust their own behavior accordingly to what they perceive (e.g., as found in humans and fish, Chartrand and Bargh, 1999; Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg, 1998; Pitcher, 1979). We do so by examining dogs’ behavioral reactions to their conspecifics’ direct behavior, to humans’ direct behavior, to dogs’ and humans’ indirect behaviors, and finally by focusing on more direct evidence supporting our hypothesis. Behaving in congruence with each other is yet an essential aspect of human-dog interactions and relationships. We review the following data about why dogs would show high levels of synchronization with humans (see Figure 1 for a schematic overview), more specifically with their owners. Synchronization with humans could arise from dogs’ sensitivity to conspecifics’ direct behaviors Evidence of the ability of dogs to adjust their behavior to that of other dogs The first step in this review is to consider the ability of dogs to observe and use the behaviors of their conspecifics. In various observed spontaneous dyadic interactions, dogs “adjust their communicative behavior according to the behaviors of other individuals,” that is, they change their behavior so that it fits to another individual’s behavior. For example, if a dog receives agonistic behaviors from another dog, it is likely to display visual communicative signals that will lead to a decrease in the other’s agonistic signals (Gazzano et al., 2010). Dogs also adjust their behavior according to their familiarity with the congener encountered: they emit twice as many communicative signals, such as freezing and looking elsewhere, when meeting an unfamiliar dog than a familiar one (Mariti et al., 2010). Sensitivity to another dog’s behavior also leads to more complex phenomena such as “social learning” (i.e., learning from conspecifics). For instance, “observational learning” is the ability of an individual to learn to display a behavior faster if that individual has previously seen a skilled individual (i.e., a skilled demonstrator) performing it, than if there was no demonstrator, or after seeing a “naïve” demonstrator (not skilled at performing the task) (Slabbert and Rasa, 1997). This is highly adaptive, since it helps an individual to acquire a strategy to achieve a goal or to decrease the costs of nonsocial learning such as trial-and-error (Kubinyi et al., 2009). Dogs learn from observing conspecifics from puppyhood. Indeed, German shepherd pups, from observing their skilled mother, learned to locate and retrieve a bag of narcotics faster than puppies that did not observe this behavior (Slabbert and Rasa, 1997). This ability is also found in adult dogs, and has been tested in various situations. For instance, dogs learn to find hidden food by observing a conspecific: they performed faster in finding a hidden treat after seeing a skilled demonstrator dog going to the location (Heberlein and Turner, 2009; Range and Virányi, 2013). They further adjusted their behavior on the information they obtained after interacting with the demonstrator conspecific (Heberlein and Turner, 2009): they searched for food with more motivation and perseverance if the demonstrator dog had found food than if it did not. This last point thus shows that dogs are not only sensitive to their conspecifics’ behaviors they also adjust their own behaviors accordingly. The ability to adjust to other dogs’ behaviors is also evidenced by “sensitivity to the attentional state of conspecifics.” Dogs were found to be sensitive to the direction of visual attention of other dogs during dyadic play between dog partners (Horowitz, 2009). Indeed, if a dog was not looking at its partner, the other dog moved and changed its behavior to attract its attention (see Figures 2 and 3). In addition, they displayed more play signals when the other dog was pausing in the play session. Dogs thus produce communicative cues in a flexible way when interacting with a partner (Horowitz, 2009). Dogs also adjust their behavior to others in more artificial settings. For example, a dog’s ability to coordinate with a partner (i.e., to spatially and temporally adjust their behaviors) was studied during attempts to solve a complex cognitive task and obtain a food reward. If their partner was delayed, the dogs waited and inhibited their own behavior until the partner was about to start the task (Ostoji c and Clayton, 2013). This is striking evidence that dogs tune their own behavior to that of others in a flexible way. Evidence of the ability of dogs to synchronize with other dogs This topic has not been widely studied in recent canine studies. However, dogs have been identified as an allelomimetic species for some time. Vogel et al. (1950) wrote: “it is fairly obvious that this [mutual mimicry] must occur in some of the highly coordinated group activities of vertebrates” (p.121), “in a highly allelomimetic species such as dog” (p.123). This statement shows that the extent to which dogs synchronize their behaviors with others has never been directly tested, as the ability of dogs to synchronize with others was merely assumed to exist. We think that this assumption and the late development of the study of dog cognition to which belongs dog-others synchronization, may be an explanation for the lack of recent studies in this field. Synchronization of behaviors in dogs was mentioned by Scott and Fuller (1965, p.63), and synchronized switches of activities was assumed: the authors described puppies howling together, and both puppies and adult dogs sleeping, getting up, sitting and lying down, walking, and C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 515 Figure 1. Dogs’ sensitivity to behaviors of other dogs as the basis for synchronization with humans. Arguments for considering that dogs show high levels of synchronization with humans and would be a relevant biological model to study interspecific behavioral synchronization. running at the same time (see Figure 4). More precisely, they described (p.106) synchronized behaviors as emerging very early in various breeds such as Basenji, beagle, cocker spaniel, Sheltie, and fox terrier: “At first the members of a litter are quite independent in their activities [.]. Between 3 and 4 weeks of age they begin to follow each other around, and by 5 weeks we often see puppies rushing toward the gate of their pen as a group. [.] By 7 weeks the puppies have begun the group attacks [.] which are a combination of allelomimetic and agonistic behavior”. Scott and Fuller (1965, p.74) indeed stated “allelomimetic behavior is a basic part of the social life of dogs.” The authors proposed that the main function of behavioral synchronization in dogs is to provide safety, because all group members can act together in emergencies (Scott and Fuller, 1965, p.75). To behave in a synchronous way, dogs maintain close contact with one another (Scott and McGray, 1967) not only using vision but also hearing and touch (Scott and Fuller, 1965, p74). In a task consisting of running in pairs, without competition between the individuals, mutual mimicry was observed in 2 different studies with a tendency to synchronize the pace of running between the 2 dogs (Scott and Marston, 1950a; Scott and McGray, 1967), but no effect of the familiarity of the congener was evidenced (Scott and Marston, 1950b). In a recent study, dogs have been observed to synchronize their behavior according to their familiarity. Indeed, free-ranging dogs were locally synchronized: follower dogs presented more local proximity with the usual leaders than with other followers (Bonanni et al., 2010). The authors proposed that dogs showed more synchrony with individuals they considered as their favorite social partners. Thus, it appears that dogs show more local synchrony with congeners with which they have a higher degree of affiliation. Finally, even if more research in this field is needed, dogs 516 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 Figure 3. In play context, dogs adjust their production of play signals (e.g., play bow, “chase me,” open mouth, bowed head) according to their partner’s direction of attention. Dogs performed more play signals toward attentive recipients (black component of bar) than towards inattentive ones (gray component). Adapted from Horowitz et al. (2009). Figure 2. Dogs adjust their behaviors to other dogs. During a dyadic play session, when a dog stops looking at its playmate (A), the other one reorients itself (B), produces attention-getting signals such as play-signal behaviors (C) to incite its partner to go back to play. Adapted from Horowitz (2009). do appear to show behavioral synchronization with others dogs. We thus propose that their skills for intraspecific synchronization could be one of the phenomena underlying their ability to synchronize their behavior with that of human beings. This is the first prerequisite showing that dogs are a good candidate species for interspecific synchronization with humans. Synchronization with humans could arise from dogs’ sensitivity to humans’ direct behaviors Dogs are sensitive to communicative cues from humans Dogs are one of the very first species to live in such a close cohabitation with humans (Vilá et al., 1997; Virányi et al., 2004). First, dogs are sensitive to human behavior and use it to develop new behaviors via direct observation and learning (Kubinyi et al., 2003a). For example, when alone, dogs were not good at detouring a V-shaped fence to obtain a toy or food reward, but they learned to go around the fence by watching a human demonstrator, using the information gained by observation (Pongrácz et al., 2001). Kubinyi et al. (2003a) also found that dogs used a new path to come back home after seeing their owner using it, and they managed to anticipate it when walking with their owner more than 50% of the time. Dogs are also capable of learning from a human demonstrator in manipulative tasks. When confronted with a box to open to obtain a ball, they performed better after seeing their owner performing the task, and used the same strategy (Kubinyi et al., 2003b). Second, dogs are skilled at using or understanding human communicative cues. The ability of dogs to follow human deictic cues (e.g., arm pointing) to find hidden food has been widely studied using the so-called 2-way object-choice task. The dog is facing a human demonstrator who is standing between 2 containers (one baited with food, the other not). The demonstrator shows a container to the dog by pointing at it, and the dog is then allowed to go and choose one container. Dogs have been found to succeed (i.e., choosing the container indicated) in this task with proximal or distal pointing or even cross-pointing and pointing when more than 2 containers were present (Lakatos et al., 2012; Miklósi and Soproni, 2006; Soproni et al., 2002). Moreover, dogs also follow human gaze (head plus eyes) with good scores and better performances if the gaze cue is given in repetitive consecutive manner compared to single continuous manner (Agnetta et al., 2000; Kaminski et al., 2012; Téglás et al., 2012). There are indications that this sensitivity is learned in everyday life or in trials, because a barely visible gesture, such as eye direction only, is poorly used (McKinley and Sambrook, 2000). In working dogs, Lit et al. (2011) evaluated the effect of humans’ beliefs and nonconscious gestures on working dog outcomes. Lit et al. tested drug and explosive detection dog-handler teams in different conditions, including some in which the owner was misled with false information about the location of the marker: dogs made more mistakes (i.e., indicating the wrong place) when the handler believed the marker was in that place, even if he did not talk to the dog or intentionally communicate with it (Lit et al., 2011). Thus, dogs appear to be sensitive to slight communicative cues from humans, even when these are unconsciously produced. C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 517 Figure 4. Example of behavioral synchronization among adult dogs. Barking and howling together (A) or lying together (B). Adapted from Scott and Fuller (1965). Dogs adjust their behaviors according to human behaviors Dogs are sensitive to humans’ body postures: they behave in different ways depending on them. First, they are sensitive to the direct behavior of a person toward them. For instance, Millot (1994) used childlike dummies and presented them to the dogs. The dummy was either in an affiliative posture (kneeling, with the head tilted toward the shoulder, right hand extended with the palm upward) or in an agonistic posture (kneeling, trunk and head forward, and right hand in a threatening position). The dogs’ reactions were more positive (e.g., affiliative behavior such as tail wagging, showing interest in the dummy) in the first condition than in the second. Reactions of dogs to positive or friendly and negative or threatening postures have also been investigated when they are confronted with adult humans, in the so-called approach paradigm (see Figure 5). In one study, an unfamiliar human approached a dog in different ways, switching from one attitude to another, while the }ri dog’s behavioral response was recorded. Barrera et al. (2010), Gyo et al. (2010), and Vas et al. (2005) found that the dogs’ behavioral reactions varied according to the threatening or friendly approaching human. Dogs were more likely to avoid the person, avert their gaze, and vocalize, and less likely to exhibit contactseeking behaviors with the threatening approach than with the friendly approach. Finally, the dogs adjusted their behavior from passive or friendly to aggressive or avoiding, and the reverse, according to switches in the human’s behavior (see also Gácsi et al., 2013; Vas et al., 2008). Dogs also flexibly change their behaviors according to a human’s behavior when facing an unsolvable problem-solving task. One study reported that they looked back more often at their owners if he/she had previously encouraged them than if the owner remained silent (Horn et al., 2012). Second, it appears that dogs discriminate the direction of visual attention in humans, that is, body direction, and the orientation of the head and eyes (i.e., gaze). When asked by their owners to lie down, dogs behaved in different ways depending on context (Schwab and Huber, 2006). When the dogs were being watched by the owner, they stayed in the commanded position more often and for longer than in other situations (e.g., owner reading a book, watching TV, and so forth). In another obedience context, dogs were confronted with a visible piece of food, and were told to leave it by their owners (Call et al., 2003). The dogs approached food in a more indirect way and took less food when the experimenter was facing them, compared with when she was turning her back or absorbed in some activity. When the dogs were facing two humans, one turning away from them and the other facing them and were then allowed to beg for food, they preferentially begged for food from the person facing them (Gácsi et al., 2004; Virányi et al., 2004). In another context using play (object- fetching game), dogs had to bring the toy back to the front of the human: they reoriented their own position if the human was turning his back to the dog (Gácsi et al., 2004). Dogs also discriminate their owner talking to a third party and not paying attention to them (i.e., they obey their owner less) compared with directly talking to them or talking away from the dog toward an empty space (Virányi et al., 2004). Furthermore, when dogs were involved in a referential communicative task toward their owners, they positioned themselves in the optimal location to attract their owners’ attention, taking into account their line of gaze (Gaunet and Deputte, 2011; cf. also Kaminski et al., 2009 for sensitivity to the visual perspective of humans, but see Gaunet and El Massioui, 2014). These results show the flexibility of dogs’ behaviors and their ability to adjust them to the direction of attention in humans (see also Savalli et al., 2014). However, there are limits to this sensitivity. Indeed, Gaunet (2008, 2010) found that whereas guide dogs performed more audible mouth licks toward their blind owners than pet dogs toward their sighted owners to obtain food, Figure 5. Dogs adjust their behavior to direct human cues. In the so-called approach paradigm, dogs adjust their behavior from passive or friendly to aggressive or avoiding according to the switch in human behavior from friendly (A) to threatening (B) approach. Adapted from Vas et al. (2005). 518 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 the 2 groups did not differ when they asked for a toy. These results suggest that dogs’ ability to adjust a toy-directed behavior might be tuned to early experience with humans bcause it is likely to be sighted families that raise guide dogs. Finally, this sensitivity to human’s communicative cues in dogs extends to another area: sensitivity to emotional cues in humans. This sensitivity is essential in interactions between individuals, enabling them to behave in the appropriate way in accordance with the visible emotional cues the partner is displaying (Ekman and Frisen, 1975). Even if this is a new research field in dog science, there are studies with evidence that dogs are sensitive to cues signalling emotions in humans. Dogs are sensitive to emotional global body posture and adjust their behavior accordingly (Vas et al., 2005; Custance and Mayer, 2012). They are also sensitive to different emotional tones of voice, such as gentle (high pitched) versus harsh (low pitched) voice (Ruffman and MorrisTrainor, 2011; Scheider et al., 2011). Finally, they are also able to focus on the face, and discriminate different human expressions, such as smiling faces from blank expressions (Nagasawa et al., 2011), sadness versus happiness (Morisaki et al., 2009), happiness versus disgust (Buttelmann and Tomasello, 2013), or happiness versus anger (Müller et al., 2015). According to these authors, this ability to discriminate human facial expressions is adaptive, because it helps dogs to adjust to humans. In another study, dogs were exposed to human faces expressing joy, fear, anger, or disgust (Deputte and Doll, 2011). Adult dogs, but not younger ones, responded more to anger (by avoiding the face) and fear (by paying more attention to the face). The dogs could have learned to adjust their behavior according to human emotions by repeated exposure to them. These data add consistent evidence that dogs are sensitive to direct behaviors in humans, which is one of the prerequisites leading to dogs’ ability to synchronize with humans beings. Synchronization with humans could arise from dog’s sensitivity to third-party interactions Dogs are efficient at adjusting their behaviors to the direct behaviors of both conspecifics and humans. We also must consider the sensitivity of dogs to indirect behaviors, that is, third-party interactions, as an essential prerequisite to dog-human synchronization. Sensitivity to indirect behaviors is defined as the ability to get information from the interaction of 2 individuals (thirdparty interaction), and then to behave with the protagonists according to the outcome of the interaction (also known as eavesdropping). Sensitivity of dogs to human third-party interactions The issue of the sensitivity of dogs to indirect human behavioral cues during third-party interactions has been explored in the foraging context. In a first experiment, after dogs were informed that 1 of 2 pots could contain food, they were faced with 2 humans interacting together: one called the other and only showed him/her 1 of 2 containers (the baited one) by pointing or gazing at it (Kaminski et al., 2012dexperiment 3). Then the dogs were released. They did not choose the indicated baited container more than the nonbaited one. Thus, in this study, dogs did not gain information from a third-party interaction between humans about a food location, or at least did not use it or behave accordingly. In other studies, Marshall-Pescini et al. (2011) and Kundey et al. (2011) tested dogs’ ability to eavesdrop in a food-sharing task. An observer dog watched 2 humans (the donors) interacting with a third (the demonstrator). The demonstrator requested food from the donors. The mean donor did not allow the requester to have the food (e.g., by removing the food, or saying “no” in a firm voice and flicked her hand away), and the friendly donor gave the food to the requester. The dogs were then allowed to go and choose a donor: they first approached and interacted more with the friendly donor rather than with the mean one in both studies (see Figures 6 and 7). The authors concluded that dogs distinguished the roles of the participants, and then used the information to behave accordingly. When Nitzschner et al. (2014) replicated this protocol, the results were not convergent. Moreover, the authors added a condition: after the dog observed the interactions, the friendly and mean donors switched their places (without the dog seeing it). Surprisingly, the dogs did not choose the friendly donor, but preferred the side where the positive interaction occurred. The authors thus concluded that there is no support for eavesdropping in dogs when observing humans sharing food. Interpreted more parsimoniously, dogs would have chosen the side emphasized during the positive interaction. In a last study, dogs observed 2 donors behaving in the same way (i.e., giving food to the person asking) but with the requester reacting in a positive manner to one (eating the food and speaking in a positive tone) and in negative manner to the other (not eating the food and speaking in a negative tone) (Freidin et al., 2013). The dogs more often chose the donor obtaining the positive interaction. The authors concluded that the dogs were able to eavesdrop from the interaction. Thus, given the variability of the protocols and results presented previously, replications and deeper analyses of the tasks and processes involved will clarify the conditions of fulfillment of dogs’ sensitivity to human third-party interactions. Sensitivity of dogs to interspecific third-party interactions Sensitivity of dogs to conspecific third-party interactions Dogs are sensitive to the behavior of conspecifics from early ages, and third-party interactions are found among littermates. Indeed, when 2 puppies play-fight together, a third often intervenes in the interaction, that is, approaches the dyad and physically interferes, by targeting (e.g., jumping on, biting at, or mounting) the loser (Ward et al., 2009). The authors of this study proposed that puppies behave in a specific way toward the loser by doing what the winning puppy is doing. Sensitivity to third-party interactions is also found in adult dogs. During a conflict between 2 individuals, dogs display reconciliation behaviors toward the loser, that is, exchanges of friendly behaviors shortly after the aggressive conflict (Cools et al., 2008). This shows that dogs recognize the outcome and individual roles of conspecifics in various third-party situations (e.g., play, contest), and adjust their own behavior in an appropriate way, even if more studies in this field are clearly needed. To our knowledge, only 3 studies have investigated the ability of dogs to evaluate indirect behaviors in interspecific interactions. In one study, dogs observed 2 experimenters interacting with a demonstrator dog (Nitzschner et al., 2012). One behaved in a friendly way with the dog (e.g., displaying play signals, talking nicely to the dog, petting the dog), and the other ignored the dog (e.g., walked by without looking at or talking to it). First, the dogs vocalized, scratched, and jumped on the separation barrier more often during the observation phase of the friendly interaction than during the other interaction. Then, when allowed to interact with the experimenters, they did not show any behavioral difference toward one or the other. The authors concluded that the dogs distinguished the different types of dog-human interactions and gained information but did not then use it for their own behavior. They proposed 2 hypotheses to explain this failure to use the information. First, the ignoring situation could have been interpreted C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 519 Figure 7. Eavesdropping in dogs. Dogs observed a third-party interaction (experimental group: 1 human requester, and 2 human donors; control group: 2 donors, no requester). In the experimental group, the dogs spent more time interacting with the generous donor (45%) than with the selfish one (2%); in the control group, no effect was found. The dogs spent significantly more time interacting with the friendly donor in the experimental group than in the control group. *P < 0.05. Adapted from MarshallPescini et al. (2011). Figure 6. Dogs gain information from observing third-party interactions. After observing a human demonstrator asking for food and (A) receiving it from the friendly donor (left) but (B) not receiving it from the selfish donor (right), the dog preferred to interact with the friendly donor (C). Adapted from Marshall-Pescini et al. (2011) and Kundey et al. (2011). by the observing dog as a walking activity and considered to be as positive as the friendly interaction. Second, the situation was quite neutral, as no play, contest, or foraging was involved, and it could be that the dogs did not consider the information gained or perceived as relevant for themselves. This logic is in agreement with another study where, in a more natural setting, dogs gained information from interactions between humans and dogs (Rooney and Bradshaw, 2006). The dogs were spectators of interspecific competitions between a human and a dog. The 2 protagonists were competing for an object, either in a playful way (the human presented play signals before the interaction) or in a contesting way (the human did not produce play signals before the interaction). Then the spectator dog was allowed to go and interact with the 2 protagonists. The dogs more often and more readily approached the winner, whether the human or the dog won the tug-play game. Conversely, the dogs approached both participants more slowly when the interaction was similar to a real contest. Thus, the dogs not only distinguish between the behaviors of 2 interacting partners, even when they were not involved in the interaction, they also discriminated between different contexts (play/contest) and tuned their behaviors according to the outcome of the interspecific interaction. Finally, Range et al (2009) showed that dogs were sensitive to third-party interspecific interactions in an obedience context. Indeed, 2 dogs (the subject and a partner) had to perform a command (give the paw). If the subject received no food, the authors found that the dog would stop obeying the command earlier if he could see the partner dog receiving food, than he would have when alone. The authors concluded that dogs were sensitive to the outcome of the interspecific interaction and behaved accordingly. Dogs are thus sensitive to third-party interactions between others dogs, other humans, and even between dogs and humans. These are consistent findings, enabling us to propose that dogs have one more prerequisite leading to the ability to synchronize their behaviors with those of humans. Direct evidence supporting the existence of dog-human synchronization This review provides evidence for dogs’ sensitivity to human behavioral cues and shows how skilled they are at using human behaviors. They are able to distinguish human behaviors when they are directed toward them, and also when directed toward other humans. We hypothesize that these skills may underlie dogs’ ability 520 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 to adjust their behavior to human behavior (Ostoji c and Clayton, 2013). It is well known that dogs are strongly affiliated with humans and even more strongly with their owners (Horn et al., 2013). In addition, synchronization between human individuals is positively correlated with rapport (Chartrand and van Baaren, 2009). Indeed, high rapport and liking between individuals have been found to be linked to a greater tendency to behavioral synchronization (Chartrand and van Baaren, 2009). In one study, when a person interacted with another human presented as a peer, he/she more often exhibited behavioral synchronization (such as tapping a foot, or touching the face) than with a confederate presented as a supervisor (Cheng and Chartrand, 2003). In another study, if 2 human individuals had a common affiliation goal, an increase in synchronization between them was observed, compared to when there was none (Lakin and Chartrand, 2003). This phenomenon is also found in many other species. Indeed, the more 2 wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) engaged in affiliative behaviors, the more they exhibited synchronous pair-swimming behaviors (e.g., type of activity, direction of swim, breathing episodes; Sakai et al., 2010). A similar facilitating role of affiliation on synchronized breathing behavior was also found in long-finned pilot whales (Senigaglia et al., 2012). In Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana), affiliative relationships play an essential role in collective movements: indeed, it was observed that an individual joined the movement of collective departure depending on the departure of its strongly affiliated mates (Sueur et al., 2009). At group level, dogs more readily follow a leader toward whom they show affiliative behavior than other individuals (Bonanni et al., 2010). Dogs and their owners develop strong affiliative bonds (Horn et al. 2013; White et al., 2010). Because dogs are sensitive to human behavioral cues, and adjust their behavior in an appropriate manner, it appears plausible that a phenomenon such as synchronization could exist between dogs and humans. Very few studies have investigated this question in dogs, and it was not the main focus of the studies involved. For example, Millot et al. (1988) studied the factors influencing dog-child interactions. They found that when the child presented more affiliative behaviors, it increased the tendency among the dogs to present affiliative behaviors too. But the authors did not investigate whether there was actual behavioral synchronization between the 2 partners. To our knowledge, only 1 recent study has investigated synchronization between human and dogs, observing interactions in terms of timing patterns (Kerepesi et al., 2005). These authors analyzed a cooperative situation between an owner and his/her dog (the owner asked the dog to help him to build a tower). The results showed that the behaviors of both dog and owner were organized into temporal patterns, with communicative behavior units and action units. The authors proposed that dog-human interactions could be much more organized in time than previously thought (Kerepesi et al., 2005). Gaunet et al. (2014) also provided evidence that dogowner dyads in the street presented an adaptive synchronized and silent walk, indicating synchronous behaviors between the members of dyads and highlighting a specific bond (Hart, 1995). Thus, we propose that behavioral synchronization could indeed exist between dogs and humans during daily interactions. Two further fields of study in dogs may support our hypothesis. First, the existence of dog-human yawning contagion, that is, yawning in dogs triggered by seeing a human yawning (JolyMascheroni et al., 2008). It is proposed to be linked to the capacity for empathy of a given species (Romero et al., 2013; Silva and De Sousa, 2011), but a more parsimonious consideration of yawning contagion as a case of activity synchrony between humans and dogs has been suggested, whereby dogs are considered as “social chameleons” (Yoon and Tennie, 2010). Moreover, it has been proposed that dog-human contagious yawning depends on the emotional connection between individuals (Joly-Mascheroni et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2012), and dogs were observed to be more sensitive to their owner’s yawns compared to those of an unfamiliar human (Romero et al., 2013; Silva et al., 2012). These authors proposed that this reflects the existence of an affiliation-dependent activity synchrony between dogs and humans. Yet recent studies have shown that an actual mimicry of human yawns and the processes at plays are not so clear-cut (Harr et al., 2009; O’Hara and Reeve, 2011). If confirmed, the existence of dog-human contagious yawning would thus be a clear case of dog-human synchronization and is essential to take into account. Finally, recent studies have shown that dogs were sensitive to their owners’ emotional cues. When the owner was stressed, the behaviors of dogs were affected in a cognitive task, suggesting an emotional contagion (Sümegi et al., 2014). When listening to a human infant crying, dogs reacted in the same way as humans: their cortisol level increased, and their behaviors were affected (Yong and Ruffman, 2014). For the authors, these findings evidence the potential existence of dog-human emotional contagion. Custance and Mayer (2012) also found that when confronted with a human crying, dogs interacted more with him/her than if the person was just talking or humming. The authors proposed a parsimonious interpretation of emotional contagion between human and dogs, coupled with a learning history with dogs rewarded for approaching their sad owners. Even if still not fully explained and demonstrated, these observations of dog-human yawning and emotional contagion could add important evidence supporting our hypothesis. The second field of study potentially supporting our hypotheses is led by Merola and colleagues and focuses on social referencing in dogs. “Social referencing” is the process by which an individual, when confronted with an unknown stimulus or event, seeks and gains information from another individual and uses it to guide its own behavior (Merola et al., 2012a, b). Social referencing involves 2 different components: the subject alternating gaze from the individual to the object or event, and the subject’s behavioral adjustment based on the information received from the informant (see Merola et al., 2012a for more details). In a recent study, the authors found that dogs chose an object according to the cues of their owner’s visible emotional reaction (Merola et al., 2013b). When confronted with two identical toys, the dogs preferentially chose the toy eliciting a positive visible emotional reaction in their owner. In their other studies, the authors confronted dogs with an unfamiliar stimulus (a fan with green plastic ribbons on it). Then they observed the dog’s behaviors according to its owner’s reaction (Merola et al., 2012a, b) or to a stranger’s reaction (Merola et al., 2012b). The authors found that whatever the degree of familiarity with the person, when dogs were faced with the novel object, they cast gaze alternations between the object and the human. The humans were then told to behave either in a positive way towards the object (happy voice and facial expression, then approaching the object), or in a negative way (fearful voice and facial expression, then moving away from the object). When the human informant was the owner, dogs in the negative group showed more static behaviors than dogs from the positive group, and they used the space in opposite manners, mirroring their owner’s movements. The dogs in the positive group indeed moved closer to the fan, whereas in the negative group, they spent more time as far as possible from the fan (Merola et al., 2012a) (see Figure 8). These dogs where thus strongly influenced by their owners’ behaviors toward the new object, because they mirrored their owners’ reactions. When the informant human was a stranger there were fewer differences between the negative and positive groups, suggesting that the relational bond was essential for the dog to synchronize its behavior on that of the human (Merola et al., 2012b). C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 521 Figure 8. Social referencing paradigm between dogs and their owners. When confronted with a strange object, the behavioral responses of the dogs [approaching (A) or avoiding (B) the new object] varied according to their owners’ reactions [curious (A) or fearful (B)]. Adapted from Merola et al. (2012a; b). But in another study, the authors found that pet dogsdbut not water rescue-trained dogsdused strangers as informants in a social referencing context (Merola et al., 2013a). All these results show that, even if still not yet extensively studied in interspecific context, social referencing occurs between dogs and humans, and dogs synchronize their behavioral reaction with humans’ one in an adaptive manner, at least when confronted with an unknown object. Finally, a study recently found that when confronted with a problem-solving task in which dogs were released to open a box after watching their owner doing itdthe owner staying silentdinterestingly, the dogs exhibited an automatic tendency to imitate their owners, even when it was counterproductive (Range et al., 2010) (see Figure 9 for more details about the proceduredA, B, C, and resultsdD). This one study adds a further argument in favor of our hypothesis that dogs synchronize their activity with their owners’ one. To get additional insight into possible behavioral synchronization by dogs, we propose to consider another natural situation experienced by dog-owner dyads: encounters with unfamiliar persons, for example, when going to the vet, or when walking dogs in public areas. Indeed, owners need to manage their dogs in a safe way when they meet very different types of unfamiliar people: children, elderly persons, or people afraid of dogs (Gaunet, 2012). These situations are known to cause agonistic reactions in dogs toward humans, and most bites in public areas involve people who are unfamiliar to the dogs (Cornelissen and Hopster, 2010). This is why dogs’ reactions toward an unfamiliar person have been extensively studied, with the so-called approach paradigm (described above). But so far, to our knowledge, only the effect of various behaviors by strangers has been investigated (e.g., Vas, }ri et al., 2010). In natet al., 2005; 2008; Ortolani et al., 2009; Gyo ural circumstances, when owners encounter a stranger, it is not easy to ask the stranger to behave in a specific way to avoid agonistic reactions from the dog. As a consequence, owners are often seen calling back their dogs, changing their path, or trying to hold their dogs back to prevent them from reacting toward the Figure 9. Automatic imitation is present in dogs in a problem-solving task. The procedure is detailed on apparatus (A), pre-training (B), no command test (C). Results are displayed in (D): the dogs more readily adopted the same action as that demonstrated by their owner. Adapted from Range et al. (2010). stranger. In these circumstances, owners are often stressed, and/or behave in a quite harsh way toward their dogs. This could induce negative associations for the dogs, creating a vicious circle leading to agonistic reactions of the dogs toward the unfamiliar person. Thus, to be more realistic, and considering the research reviewed here, we propose that further research studies should focus on the effect of the owner’s behavior, and on the way it could influence dogs’ behavioral responses when faced with an unfamiliar person, by way of observational learning, emotional contagion, and/or behavioral synchronization. What appears essential is to establish the extent to which the owners’ reactions during such encounters influence their dogs’ behavioral responses. In other words, does a stressed, avoiding reaction by the owner, or conversely a happy, relaxed reaction, affect the dog’s behavior? To our knowledge, only one study has discussed this potential influence. Neessen (2013) tested the influence of a supportive owner versus a passive owner when the dogs met a new person. The author found that when the 522 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 owners gave active support to their dogs (i.e., by talking calmly to them and petting them), it reduced the agonistic behaviors toward an approaching unfamiliar person, but these results have not yet been published (Neessen, 2013). Thus, we recommend that further studies should investigate these aspects as a paradigm for studying synchronization between human and dogs. From the current review, we can suggest that owners’ reactions do influence dogs’ behaviors toward an unfamiliar human. We hypothesize that dogs synchronize their behavior with their owners’ behavior toward the stranger. We also hypothesize that the type of relationship between the dog and its handler could play a role in this behavioral synchronization (e.g., caretakers with shelter dogs; different members of the family with pet dogs; various dog professionals with pet and working dogs). A key step will thus be to design experiments testing these specific hypotheses in an approach paradigm, varying owner behaviors to observe the consequences on response in the dogs. Further research in this field will give more understanding about dog-human interactions. From a practical viewpoint, it will provide key information on how the owner can influence and manage his/her dog in daily situations such as meeting an unknown person who wants to chat or to pet the dog, or who is wearing unusual clothes or behaving unusually. Ultimately, if behavioral synchronization is present between the owner and his/her dog, if dogs do indeed mirror their owners, it could enable us to provide scientific data for dog trainers and dog owners, and to propose advice on how to behave to reduce stress and agonistic behaviors in dogs in these situations, by considering or acting on the owner’s nonverbal behavior. Acknowledgments The authors gratefully acknowledge Marion Montaigne for her drawings. We thank Prescott Breeden, Séverine Belkhir, and Dr. Thierry Bedossa for reading the article and their helpful suggestions on it. This work was funded by Association Nationale de la Recherche et de la Technologie, Association Aide aux Vieux Animaux, the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, and AixMarseille Université. The idea for the article was conceived by Dr. Florence Gaunet and Charlotte Duranton (PhD). The review was written, by both Dr. Florence Gaunet and Charlotte Duranton. Conflict of interest The authors declare no conflict of interest. References Agnetta, B., Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2000. Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use. Anim. Cogn. 3, 107e112. Ancel, A., Beaulieu, M., Le Maho, Y., Gilbert, C., 2009. Emperor penguin mates: Keeping together in the crowd. Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 2163e2169. Barrera, G., Jakovcevic, A., Elgier, A.M., Mustaca, A., Bentosela, M., 2010. Responses of shelter and pet dogs to an unknown human. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 5, 339e344. Bertram, B.C.R., 1980. Vigilance and group size in ostriches. Anim. Behav. 28, 278e286. Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Natoli, E., 2010. Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim. Behav. 79, 981e991. Buttelman, D., Tomasello, M., 2013. Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expression to locate hidden food? Anim. Cogn. 16, 137e145. Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Tomasello, M., 2003. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 257e263. Chartrand, T.L., Bargh, J.A., 1999. The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psyc. 76 (6), 893e910. Chartrand, T.L., Lakin, J.L., 2013. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Ann. Rev. Psy. 64, 285e308. Chartrand, T.L., van Baaren, R., 2009. Human mimicry. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 219e274. Cheng, C.M., Chartrand, T.L., 2003. Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 1170e1179. Conradt, L., Roper, T.J., 2000. Activity synchrony and social cohesion: A fissionfusion model. Proc. Biol. Sci. 267, 2213e2218. Cools, A.K.A., van Hout, A.J.M., Nelissen, M.H.J., 2008. Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: Do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates? Ethology 114, 53e63. Cornelissen, J.M.R., Hopster, H., 2010. Dog bites in The Netherlands: a study of victims, injuries, circumstances, and aggressors to support evaluation of breed specific legislation. Vet. J. 186, 292e298. Custance, D., Mayer, J., 2012. Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: And exploratory study. Anim. Cogn. 15 (5), 851e859. Deputte, B.L., Doll, A., 2011. Do dogs understand human facial expressions? J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 6, 78e79. Dijksterhuis, A., van Knippenberg, A., 1998. The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of trivial pursuit. J. Pers. Soc. Psyc. 74 (4), 865e877. Dostálková, I., Spinka, M., 2007. Synchronization of behaviour in pairs: the role of communication and consequences in timing. Anim. Behav. 74, 1735e1742. Ekman, P., Frisen, W.V., 1975. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotion from Facial Clues. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New-Jersey. Fernandez, G.J., Capurro, A.F., Reboreda, J.C., 2003. Effect of group size on individual and collective vigilance in Greater Rheas. Ethology 109, 413e425. Freidin, E., Putrino, N., D’Orazio, M., Bentosela, M., 2013. Dogs’ Eavesdropping from people’s reactions in third party interactions. PLoS One 8, e79198. Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Varga, O., Topál, J., Csányi, V., 2004. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention. Anim. Cogn. 7, 144e153. Gácsi, M., Vas, J., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., 2013. Wolves do not join the dance: Sophisticated aggression control by adjusting to human social signals in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 145, 109e122. Gaunet, F., 2008. How do guide dogs of blind owners and pet dogs of sighted owners (Canis familiaris) ask their owners for food? Anim. Cogn. 11, 475e483. Gaunet, F., 2010. How do guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) ask their owners for their toy and for playing? Anim. Cogn. 13, 311e323. Gaunet, F., 2012. Sensitivity and adjustments of domestic dogs to social agents: Knowledge from the scientific field. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 7, e6. Gaunet, F., Deputte, B.L., 2011. Functionally referential and intentional communication in the domestic dog: Effects of spatial and social contexts. Anim. Cogn. 14, 849e860. Gaunet, F., El Massioui, F., 2014. Marked referential communicative behaviours, but no differentiation of the “knowledge state” of humans in untrained pet dogs vs 1-year-old infants. Anim. Cogn. 17 (5), 1137e1147. Gaunet, F., Milliet, J., 2010. The relationship of visually impaired people with the guide dog: how could the use of dogs be further developped in France. Europ. J. Disab. Res. 4, 116e133. Gaunet, F., Pari-Perrin, E., Bernardin, G., 2014. Description of dogs and owners in outdoor built-up areas and their more-than-human issues. Environ. Manag. 54 (3), 383e401. Gautrais, J., Michelena, P., Sibbald, A., Bon, R., Deneubourg, J.L., 2007. Allelomimetic synchronization in Merino sheep. Anim. Behav. 74, 1443e1454. Gazzano, A., Mariti, C., Papi, F., Falaschi, C., Foti, S., Ducci, M., 2010. Are domestic dogs able to calm conspecifics by using visual communication? J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 5, 28e29. Gunnarsson, T.G., Gill, J.A., Sigurbjörnsson, T., Sutherland, W.J., 2004. Arrival synchrony in migratory birds. Nature 431, 646. } ri, B., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., 2010. Friend or foe: Context dependent sensitivity to Gyo human behaviour in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 128, 69e77. Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., Tomasello, M., 2002. The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298, 1634e1636. Hare, B., Rosati, A., Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., Tomasello, M., 2010. The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: A response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne, et al. (2008). Anim. Behav. 79, e1ee6. Harr, A.L., Gilbert, V.R., Phillips, K.A., 2009. Do dogs (Canis familiaris) show contagious yawning? Anim. Cogn. 12, 833e837. Hart, L.Y., 1995. Dogs as human companions: A review of the relationship, 161-178. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Heberlein, M., Turner, D.C., 2009. Dogs, Canis familiaris, find hidden food by observing and interacting with a conspecific. Anim. Behav. 78, 385e391. Horn, L., Virányi, Z., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., Range, F., 2012. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation. Anim. Cogn. 15, 57e71. Horn, L., Range, F., Huber, L., 2013. Dogs’ attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Anim. Cogn. 16, 435e443. Horowitz, A., 2009. Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Anim. Cogn. 12, 107e118. Joly-Mascheroni, R.M., Senju, A., Shepherd, A.J., 2008. Dogs catch human yawns. Biol. Lett. 4, 446e448. Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., Tomasello, M., 2009. Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human’s perspective. Behaviour 146, 979e998. C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., Tomasello, M., 2012. How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Dev. Sci. 15, 222e232. Kerepesi, A., Jonsson, G.K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., Csányi, V., Magnusson, M.S., 2005. Detection of temporal patterns in dog-human interaction. Behav. Processes 70, 69e79. King, A.J., Cowlishaw, G., 2009. All together now: behavioural synchrony in baboons. Anim. Behav. 78, 1381e1387. Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., Csányi, V., 2003a. Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in dogs: preliminary results. Anim. Cogn. 6, 57e63. Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V., 2003b. Dogs (Canis familiaris) learn from their owners via observation in a manipulation task. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 156e165. Kubinyi, E., Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., 2009. Dog as a model for studying conspecific and heterospecific social learning. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 4, 31e41. Kundey, S.M.A., De los Reyes, A., Royer, E., Molina, S., Monnier, B., German, R., Coshun, A., 2011. Reputation-like inference in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 14, 291e302. Lakatos, G., Gácsi, M., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., 2012. Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog-human communication in relatively complex situations. Anim. Cogn. 15, 201e213. Lakin, J.L., Chartrand, T.L., 2003. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol. Sci. J. Am. Psychol. Soc. 14, 334e339. Lakin, J.L., Jefferis, V.E., Cheng, C.M., Chartrand, T.L., 2003. The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J. Nonverbal Behav. 27, 145e162. Lit, L., Schweitzer, J.B., Oberbauer, A.M., 2011. Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes. Anim. Cogn. 14, 387e394. Louwerse, M.M., Dale, R., Bard, E.G., Jeuniaux, P., 2012. Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cogn. Sci. 36, 1e23. Mariti, C., Papi, F., Ducci, M., Sighieri, C., Martelli, F., Gazzano, A., 2010. Domestic dogs display calming signals more frequently towards unfamiliar rather than familiar dogs. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 5, 62e63. Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Ferrario, A., Valsecchi, P., Prato-Previde, E., 2011. Social eavesdropping in the domestic dog. Anim. Behav. 81, 1177e1183. McKinley, J., Sambrook, T.D., 2000. Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 3, 13e22. Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Marshall-Pescini, S., 2012a. Social referencing in dogowner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 15, 175e185. Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Marshall-Pescini, S., 2012b. Dogs’ social referencing towards owners and strangers. PLoS One 7, e47653. Merola, I., Marshall-Pescini, S., D’Aniello, B., Prato-Previde, E., 2013a. Social referencing: Water rescue trained dogs are less affected than pet dogs by the stranger’s message. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 147, 132e138. Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., Lazzaroni, M., Marshall-Peschini, S., 2013b. Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Anim. Cogn. 17 (2), 373e385. Miklósi, Á., Soproni, K., 2006. A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim. Cogn. 9, 81e93. Millot, J.L., 1994. Olfactory and visual cues in the interaction systems between dogs and children. Behav. Proc. 33, 177e188. Millot, J.L., Filiatre, J.C., Gagnon, A.C., Gray, R. De, Cedex, B., Eckerlin, A., 1988. Children and their pet dogs: How they communicate. Behav. Proc. 17, 1e15. Morisaki, A., Takaoka, A., Fujita, K., 2009. Are dogs sensitive to the emotional state of humans? J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 4 (2), 49. Müller, C.A., Schmitt, K., Barber, A.L.A., Huber, L., 2015. Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Biol. 25, 601e605. Nagasawa, M., Murai, K., Mogi, K., Kikusui, T., 2011. Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions. Anim. Cogn. 14, 525e533. Neessen, P., 2013. Human-canine Interaction Active Support versus Passive Support: The Influence of Type of Support Given by the Owner on the Behaviour of the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris) in an Approach Test. Master Thesis Report. Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences. Nitzschner, M., Melis, A.P., Kaminski, J., Tomasello, M., 2012. Dogs (Canis familiaris) evaluate humans on the basis of direct experiences only. PLoS One 7, e46880. Nitzschner, M., Kaminski, J., Melis, A., Tomasello, M., 2014. Side matters: Potential mechanisms underlying dogs’ performance in a social eavesdropping paradigm. Anim. Behav. 90, 263e271. O’Hara, S.J., Reeve, A.V., 2011. A test of the yawning contagion and emotional connectedness hypothesis in dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim. Behav. 81, 335e340. Ortolani, A., Vernooij, H., Coppinger, R., 2009. Ethiopian village dogs: Behavioural responses to a stranger’s approach. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 119, 210e218. Ostoji c, L., Clayton, N.S., 2013. Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner. Anim. Cogn. 17 (2), 445e459. Paukner, A., Suomi, S.J., Visalberghi, E., Ferrari, P.F., 2009. Capuchin monkeys display affiliation towards humans who imitate them. Science 325 (5942), 880e883. Pays, O., Renaud, P.C., Loisel, P., Petit, M., Gerard, J.F., Jarman, P.J., 2007. Prey synchronize their vigilant behaviour with other group members. Proc. Biol. Sci. 274, 1287e1291. Pays, O., Dubot, A.L., Jarman, P.J., Loisel, P., Goldizen, A.W., 2008. Vigilance and its complex synchrony in the red-necked pademelon, Thylogale thetis. Behav. Ecol. 20, 22e29. Pitcher, T., 1979. Sensory information and the organization of behaviour in a shoaling cyprinid fish. Anim. Behav. 27 (1), 126e149. 523 Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Gurobi, K., Topál, J., Csányi, V., 2001. Social learning in dogs: The effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a detour task. Anim. Behav. 62, 1109e1117. Range, F., Horn, L., Viranyi, Z., Huber, L., 2009. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. PNAS 106 (1), 340e345. Range, F., Huber, L., Heyes, C., 2010. Automatic imitation in dogs. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 1e7. Range, F., Virányi, Z., 2013. Social learning from humans or conspecifics: Differences and similarities between wolves and dogs. Front. Psychol. 4, 868. Romero, T., Konno, A., Hasegawa, T., 2013. Familiarity bias and physiological responses in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy. PLoS One 8 (8), e71365. Rook, A.J., Penning, P.D., 1991. Synchronisation of eating, ruminating and idling activity by grazing sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 32, 157e166. Rooney, N.J., Bradshaw, J.W.S., 2006. Social cognition in the domestic dog: Behaviour of spectators towards participants in interspecific games. Anim. Behav. 72, 343e352. Ruffman, T., Morris-Trainor, Z., 2011. Do dogs understand human emotional expressions? J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 6 (1), 97e98. Sakai, M., Morisaka, T., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., Kohshima, S., 2010. Fine-scale analysis of synchronous breathing in wild Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Behav. Proc. 83, 48e53. Savalli, C., Adés, C., Gaunet, F., 2014. Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way? PLoS One 9 (9), e108003. Scheider, L., Grassmann, S., Kaminski, J., Tomasello, M., 2011. Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when following a human pointing gesture. PLoS One 6 (7), e21676. Schwab, C., Huber, L., 2006. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. J. Comp. Psychol. 120, 169e175. Scott, J.P., Marston, M.V., 1950a. Social facilitation and allelomimetic behavior in dogs. I. Social facilitation in a non-competitive situation. Behaviour 2, 121e134. Scott, J.P., Marston, M.V., 1950b. Social facilitation and allelomimetic behavior in dogs. II. The effects of unfamiliarity. Behaviour 2, 135e143. Scott, J.P., Fuller, J.L., 1965. Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago and London. Scott, J.P., McGray, C., 1967. Allelomimetic behavior in dogs: Negative effects of competition on social facilitation. J. Comp. Physiol. 63, 316e319. Senigaglia, V., de Stephanis, R., Verborgh, P., Lusseau, D., 2012. The role of synchronized swimming as affiliative and anti-predatory behavior in long-finned pilot whales. Behav. Proc. 91, 8e14. Serpell, J.A., 1995. Introduction. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Serpell, J.A., 1996. In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-animal Relationships. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Silva, K., De Sousa, L., 2011. Canis empathicus? A proposal on dogs’ capacity to empathize with humans. Biol. Lett. 7, 489e492. Silva, K., Bessa, J., de Sousa, L., 2012. Auditory contagious yawning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): First evidence for social modulation. Anim. Cogn. 15, 721e724. Slabbert, J.M., Rasa, A.E., 1997. Observational learning of an acquired maternal behaviour pattern by working dog pups: An alternative training method? App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 53, 309e316. Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., Csányi, V., 2002. Dogs’ (Canis familaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J. Comp. Psychol. 116, 27e34. Stone, L., He, D., Becker, K., Fishelson, L., 2003. Unusual synchronization of Red Sea fish energy expenditures. Ecol. Lett. 6, 83e86. Sueur, C., Petit, O., Deneubourg, J.L., 2009. Selective mimetism at departure in collective movements of Macaca tonkeana: An experimental and theoretical approach. Anim. Behav. 78, 1087e1095. Sümegi, Z., Oláh, K., Topál, J., 2014. Emotional contagion in dogs as measured by change in cognitive task performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 160, 106e115. Téglás, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., 2012. Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Curr. Biol. 22, 209e212. Udell, M.A.R., Wynne, C.D.L., 2010. Ontogeny and phylogeny: Both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim. Behav. 79, e9ee14. Vas, J., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V., 2005. A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 94, 99e115. Vas, J., Topál, J., Györi, B., Miklósi, Á., 2008. Consistency of dogs’ reactions to threatening cues of an unfamiliar person. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 112, 331e344. Vilà, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonado, J.E., Amorim, I.R., Rice, J.E., Honeycutt, R.L., Crandall, K.A., Lundeberg, J., Wayne, R.K., 1997. Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science 276, 1687e1689. Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Csányi, V., 2004. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus. Behav. Proc. 66, 161e172. Vogel Jr., H.H., Scott, J.P., Marston, M.V., 1950. Social facilitation and allelomimetic behavior in dogs. I. Social facilitation in a non-competitive situation. Behaviour 2, 121e134. 524 C. Duranton, F. Gaunet / Journal of Veterinary Behavior 10 (2015) 513e524 Ward, C., Trisko, R., Smuts, B.B., 2009. Third-party interventions in dyadic play between littermates of domestic dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Anim. Behav. 78, 1153e1160. White, J.M., McBride, A.E., Redhead, E., 2010. Relationship between dog owner behavior and dog attachment security in the strange situation. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 5, 47. Wynne, C.D.L., Udell, M.A.R., Lord, K.A., 2008. Ontogeny’s impacts on human-dog communication. Anim. Behav. 76, e1ee4. Yasaka, M., Terazawa, K., Koyama, H., Kon, H., 2003. Masting behavior of Fagus crenata in northern Japan: Spatial synchrony and pre-dispersal seed predation. For. Ecol. Man. 184, 277e284. Yong, M.H., Ruffman, R., 2014. Emotional contagion: Dogs and humans show a similar physiological response to human infant crying. Behav. Proc. 108, 155e165. Yoon, J.M.D., Tennie, C., 2010. Contagious yawning: a reflection of empathy, mimicry, or contagion? Anim. Behav. 79, e1ee3.