Author`s personal copy
Transcription
Author`s personal copy
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit: http://www.elsevier.com/copyright Author's personal copy Sexologies (2009) 18, 44—50 ORIGINAL ARTICLE / ARTICLE ORIGINAL An exploratory study on male homosexual erotic imagery Une étude exploratoire de l’imagerie érotique des homosexuels Un estudio exploratorio del imaginaroio erótico del varón homosexual M. Cacioppo (PhD) a,∗, V. Vizzari b, F. Corica b, V. Maestri b, C. Simonelli (PhD) a a Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Rome ‘‘Sapienza’’, Via dei Marsi, 78, 00185, Roma, Italy b Istituto di Sessuologia Clinica, Via Savoia, 78, 00198, Rome, Italy Available online 10 March 2008 KEYWORDS Sexual; Fantasies; Gay man; Differences ∗ Summary Having sexual fantasies is considered functional to sexual health. The literature focuses mainly on heterosexual subjects and studies of the erotic imagery of homosexuals are rather scarce. This study aims at investigating the erotic imagery of male homosexuals today. Fifty male homosexuals were interviewed; mean age of the subjects is 26.36 years (range 20 to 36). Their level of education is quite high. By means of a questionnaire we studied the following aspects: number of actual and imaginary partners, motivations for sexual intercourse, situations that trigger erotic fantasies, incidence of erotic fantasies during sexual activity, during nonsexual activity and during masturbation. Finally, we discussed the content of sexual imagery by listing the top five fantasies in order of frequency of occurrence. Main results show that the erotic fantasies in male homosexuals are principally triggered by visual stimuli. Compared to past results, we found more recognition of physical, emotional and relational aspects. In comparison with the past, there is a large use of erotic imaginary, especially during masturbation. We note the absence in our sample of fantasies referring to the opposite sex and of fantasies of forced intercourse, which, on the contrary, were found in previous studies. We discuss our results to describe the differences with previous research and we try to provide a picture that takes into account sociocultural changes. © 2008 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés. Corresponding author. E-mail address: marco.cacioppo@uniroma1.it (M. Cacioppo). 1158-1360/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés. doi:10.1016/j.sexol.2007.12.007 Author's personal copy An exploratory study on male homosexual erotic imagery MOTS CLÉS Sexuel ; Fantaisies ; Homosexuel ; Différences PALABRAS CLAVE Sexual; Fantasías; Varón homosexual; Diferencias 45 Résumé. — On considère que la présence de fantaisies sexuelles imaginatives est un indicateur fonctionnel de la santé sexuelle. La littérature est principalement focalisée sur la population hétérosexuelle et les études sur l’imaginaire érotique de l’homosexualité sont insuffisantes. L’objectif de ce travail est d’investiguer l’imaginaire érotique des homosexuels masculins d’aujourd’hui. Cinquante personnes ont été interrogées ; l’age moyen est de 26,36 ans (de 20 à 36) ; le niveau scolaire est assez élevé. Nous avons utilisé un questionnaire pour étudier ces aspects : le nombre de partenaires réels ou imaginaires ; les motivations aux rapports sexuels ; les situations qui suscitent les fantaisies érotiques ; la fréquence de l’imaginaire érotique pendant l’activité sexuelle ; la fréquence de l’imaginaire érotique en dehors de l’activité sexuelle ; la fréquence de l’imaginaire érotique pendant la masturbation. À la fin, nous avons dressé une liste des cinq fantaisies sexuelles les plus courantes par ordre de fréquence. Les principaux résultats montrent que les situations qui suscitent principalement les fantaisies érotiques chez l’homme homosexuel concernent principalement les stimuli visuels. Par rapport au passé, il y a une forte utilisation de l’imaginaire érotique, particulièrement pendant la masturbation et une plus grande prise en compte des aspects physiques, émotionnels et relationnels. Nous avons noté dans notre échantillon, l’absence de fantaisies imaginatives portant sur le sexe opposé ou sur des rapports sexuels imposés par la force, fantaisies qu’on avait retrouvé dans des recherches précédentes. Ces résultats ont été discutés pour décrire les différences trouvées par rapport aux recherches précédentes, en cherchant à donner un tableau qui prenne en compte les changements socioculturels. © 2008 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés. Resumen Se considera que la presencia de fantasías sexuales es un indicador de funcionalidad de la salud sexual. La literatura se ha focalizado principalmente sobre la población heterosexual y los estudios sobre el imaginario erótico de la homexualidad son insuficientes. El objetivo de este trabajo es investigar el imaginario erótico de los homosexuales masculinos en la actualidad. Cincuenta personas han sido interrogadas ; la edad media es de 26,36 años (de 20 a 36) ; el nivel medio de estudios es bastante elevado. Hemos utilizado un cuestionario para estudiar los siguientes aspectos : el número de parejas reales o imaginaries ; las motivaciones en las relaciones sexuales ; la frecuencia del imaginario erótico fuera de la actividad sexual ; la frecuencia del imaginario erótico durante la masturbación. Al final hemos propuesto una lista de las cinco fantasías sexuales más corrientes por orden de frecuencia. Los principales resultados muestran que las situaciones que suscitan la mayoría de las fantasías eróticas en el varón homosexual están actualmente dirigidas sobre estímulos visuales. Hay una fuerte utilización del imaginario erótico con respecto al pasado, particularmente durante la masturbación y mayor reconocimiento de las emociones físicas y de los aspectos relacionales. Hemos observado en nuestra muestra, la ausencia de fantasías imaginativas sobre el otro sexo o sobre relaciones sexuales impuestas por la fuerza, fantasías que se habían encontrado en investigaciones precedentes. Estos resultados han sido comentados para describir diferencias encontradas en nuestras investigaciones precedentes, buscando dar una explicación que tenga en cuenta los cambios socioculturales. © 2008 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés. Version abrégée On considère qu’une fantaisie érotique est une représentation mentale plus ou moins organisée des désirs sexuels. Elle peut se réaliser soit pendant les rapports sexuels, soit pendant l’activité masturbatoire, soit en dehors de toute sexualité. On considère que c’est un indicateur de santé sexuelle et les études les plus récentes montrent que la majeure partie des personnes affirme en avoir. Crépault, 1981 faisait remarquer que les sujets homosexuels ont plus de fantaisies érotiques que les hétérosexuels soit durant les rapports sexuels, soit durant la masturbation et qu’ils ont une activité fantasmatique majeure surtout dans la phase préorgasmique. La littérature sur l’imaginaire érotique homosexuel, toutefois, est aujourd’hui plutôt limitée et pas toujours systématique et le but de cette étude est d’examiner l’imaginaire érotique d’un groupe d’hommes homosexuels italiens et de comparer les résultats avec ceux de recherches précédentes. À cet effet on a utilisé un questionnaire de Crépault, 1981 dont on a analysé les questions relatives aux domaines suivants : informations sociodémographiques ; nombre de partenaires réels et imagines ; motivations poussant à avoir des rapports sexuels ; situations provoquant les fantaisies érotiques ; importance de l’imaginaire érotique en dehors de l’activité sexuelle ; importance de l’imaginaire érotique à l’extérieur de l’activité sexuelle ; imaginaire érotique à l’intérieur de la masturbation. Nous avons analysé 50 questionnaires distribués dans des cercles gay des alentours de Rome et remplis par des hommes qui se déclarent homosexuels d’âge moyen de 26, 36 ans. Les résultats ont été comparés aux données obtenues en 1993 par la même méthodologie sur un échantillon Author's personal copy 46 similaire, pour fournir une description qui tient compte de la relation entre imaginaire érotique et changements socioculturels avérés dans le cours des années. On a en outre listé les cinq premières fantaisies classées en ordre de fréquence de survenue. Les premiers résultats concernent le nombre de partenaires réels et imaginaires : le nombre de partenaires imaginés est très grand par rapport aux études précédentes tandis que le nombre de partenaires réels semble extraordinairement réduit. Concernant les facteurs qui motivent les rapports sexuels on observe une augmentation, comparativement au passé, des valeurs relatives aux aspects corporels par rapport aux aspects émotionnels, bien que ceux-ci augmentent aussi. Concernant les situations qui favorisent les fantaisies érotiques, on observe que le canal qui a la majeure influence est le canal visuel ; un résultat similaire avait déjà été trouvé dans le passé bien que les valeurs en pourcentage étaient inférieures aux valeurs actuelles. Cette donnée pourrait indiquer une moindre inhibition, par rapport au passé, à déclarer une préférence pour les fantaisies de voyeurisme. Quant à l’importance des fantaisies on assiste à leur augmentation comparativement aux données de 1993 par rapport aux quelles, on fait un grand usage de l’imaginaire érotique durant la masturbation. Si on considère les cinq premières fantaisies on observe l’absence dans notre échantillon de fantaisies qui font référence au sexe opposé et de fantaisies de rapports forcés, qui, au contraire, avaient été trouvées par les études précédentes. Ces différences dérivent des changements culturels qui, grâce aussi à une majeure diffusion par la presse, ont contribué à l’émergence d’une sexualité plus libre et accessible et qui ont probablement offert aux hommes comme aux femmes de plus larges possibilités d’explorer ce qu’ils préfèrent et ce qu’ils ne préfèrent pas, dépassant ainsi les stéréotypes sexuels. Full version Introduction At least occasionally, most people engage in erotic fantasies (Hsu et al., 1994) and this is considered functional to sexual health (Person et al., 1992). It is now considered a sign of pathology not to have sexual fantasies rather than to have them. For example, infrequent sexual fantasy is one of the defining criteria for the sexual disorder ‘‘inhibited sexual desire’’ described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). ‘‘Erotic imagery can be defined as the faculty that humans have to produce mental representations of their erotic desires, through the fantasies. The erotic fantasy is a mental picture, sometimes structured, sometimes less so’’ (Crépault and Couture, 1980, p. 565). Erotic fantasies, which can occur both with and without sexual intercourse, as well as during masturbation, contribute to the arousal and maintenance of sexual excitement and represent an indispensable element for the diagnosis and often for the therapeutic strategy of many sexual disorders (Crépault, 1981). Hicks and Leitenberg (2001) observe that most people claim to have sexual fantasies, specifying that M. Cacioppo et al. for members of married or de facto couples, a large amount of fantasies are about extramarital intercourse. Crépault (1981) highlights the fact that compared to heterosexuals, homosexuals engage in erotic fantasies more frequently, both during sexual intercourse and during masturbation, and they have a higher fantasmatic activity, especially during the preorgasmic phase. Masters and Johnson (1979) state that the most frequently reported erotic fantasy in a group of male homosexuals was the representation of male body parts, but that the real partner was rarely the object of desire. In spite of Leitenberg and Henning’s (1995) review of erotic imaginary highlighting that studies on homosexual fantasies are rather scarce and not systematic, we can state that research carried out so far has shown that all individuals have a tendency to produce erotic mental images that are used during both sexual and nonsexual activities (Allen et al., 2005). The present study aims at studying the erotic imagery of a group of Italian male homosexuals and at discussing the differences found compared to the results of previous studies, thus providing a picture that takes into account the relationship between erotic imagery and sociocultural changes, which have occurred over the years. Method The present study used Crépault’s questionnaire (1981), which had already been utilized in an exploratory study on male homosexual erotic imagery (Palaia and Petruccelli, 1993). The instrument designed by Crépault consists of 49 questions, some open-ended and some closed that inquire into sexual behaviour and the erotic imagery of subjects during sexual intercourse, during masturbation and during nonerotic activities. This study analyses only items pertaining to the following: sociodemographic data, number of actual and imaginary partners, motivations for sexual intercourse, situations that trigger erotic fantasies, incidence of erotic imagery during sexual and nonsexual activity, incidence of sexual imagery during masturbation. Comparisons with the results of previous studies also utilising this same questionnaire are made descriptively. Finally, we chose to discuss the content of sexual imagery by listing the top five fantasies and comparing them with the ones described by Masters and Johnson (1979) and Price et al. (1985). Procedure and subjects Participants in the present study were chosen by distributing 100 questionnaires at homosexual clubs and meeting-points in Rome; in order to guarantee anonymity, each questionnaire was supplied with a stamped addressed envelope; so, after filling out the questionnaire, the subject could seal it and post it to the Rome Istituto di Sessuologia Clinica. The institute received 50 correctly completed questionnaires from subjects declaring to be homosexual and whose mean age was 26 (age range 20 to 36 years). The 50 subjects of the previous study by Palaia and Petruccelli (1993) were recruited in the same way and had a similar mean age (mean age 27; age range 18 to 39 years). Subjects’ characteristics are shown in Table 1. Author's personal copy An exploratory study on male homosexual erotic imagery Table 1 Characteristics of subjects interviewed. Caractéristiques des sujets interviewés. Year of the study 1993 Number of subjects Mean age (years) Age range (years) 2006 50 27 18—39 50 26,36 20—36 Table 2 Number of sexual partners. Nombre de partenaires sexuels. Year of the study 1993 Partners Real (%) Over 100 23,71 From 50 to 100 8 From 20 to 50 19,56 From 10 to 20 13,04 From 5 to 10 27 Fewer than 5 8,69 2006 Imaginary (%) Real Imaginary (%) 46 18 16 10 6 4 0 0 8 44 46 2 48 30 2 16 2 2 Results The percentages of answer scores to questionnaires were analysed using SPSS base 8.0 in order to compare the results of the two studies. First results shown refer to numbers of real and imaginary partners (Table 2); as in the previous study, the number of imaginary partners is very high (48% of the sample of the present study and 46% of the previous study claimed to have had more than 100 imaginary partners) while the number of real partners is dramatically reduced: in the past study 23.71% of the sample declared to have had more than 100, while at present no subject declared to have had sex with more than 50 partners. As to the motivations for sexual intercourse (Fig. 1), we note that subjects of the present study stated that a factor that ‘‘often’’ leads to sexuality was mostly the physical one Figure 1 Motivations for sexual intercourse. Motivations des rapports sexuels. 47 (73.4%), as well as the need for affection (65.6%), need for communication (14.2%), need for reassurance (46.4%) and the desire to satisfy one’s partner (31.5%). On the other hand, the Palaia and Petruccelli’s (1993) sample stated that sexuality was mainly sought for affective needs (58.8%) and, to a lesser extent, for physical needs (49%); furthermore, in the past, the ‘‘need for reassurance’’ and ‘‘to satisfy one’s partner’’ were indicated as a motivation for sexual intercourse by only about half the percentage of our study; with regard to the need for communication, the percentage was 17.6%. The main difference with the previous study is the increase of values concerning physical aspects compared to values regarding emotional aspects; however, even the latter increased (except for the ‘‘need for communication’’ which showed a slight decrease). Considering those situations which favour the emergence of erotic fantasies (Fig. 2) we find that the visual channel is the most influential: the most stimulating situations are ‘‘watching an erotic movie’’ (77.6%) and ‘‘watching an attractive man’’ (73.2%), followed by ‘‘reading an erotic book’’ (51.4%). Values of the previous study were much lower: ‘‘watching an erotic movie’’ (39%), ‘‘watching an attractive man’’ (45.1%), and ‘‘reading an erotic book’’ (35.3%). This finding might indicate less inhibition than in the past in revealing one’s preference for voyeuristic fantasies. As to the incidence of fantasies (Fig. 3), we find that 11.7% of subjects interviewed claim ‘‘always’’ to have sexual fantasies during sexual intercourse, 39.6% ‘‘almost always’’, 18.6% ‘‘very often’’, 23.3% ‘‘often’’ and ‘‘sometimes’’ 6.8%. It is noteworthy that no subject states ‘‘never’’ having erotic fantasies during intercourse. In this set of results we find an increase in fantasies compared to the previous study by Palaia and Petruccelli (1993), where 5.9% of sample stated ‘‘always’’ engaging in fantasies during intercourse, 3.9% ‘‘almost always’’, 5.9% ‘‘very often’’, 13.7% ‘‘often’’, 55% ‘‘sometimes’’ and 15.6% ‘‘never’’. As to fantasies during masturbation (Fig. 4), we find that 61.4% of subjects declare ‘‘always’’ engaging in fantasies while masturbating and 36.6% ‘‘almost always’’. Only 2% of subjects state they fantasize ‘‘very often’’ and none stated they engaged in fantasies ‘‘often’’, ‘‘sometimes’’ Author's personal copy 48 or ‘‘never’’. Again we note an important difference with the previous study, when results were: ‘‘always’’ 31.2%, ‘‘almost always’’ 18.7%, ‘‘very often’’ 4.2%, ‘‘often’’ 12.5%; ‘‘sometimes’’ 29.2% and ‘‘never’’ 4.2%. M. Cacioppo et al. Results regarding the incidence of erotic fantasies outside sexual intercourse (Fig. 5) show that they are not at all marginal in the life of the homosexuals interviewed. In fact, no subject declares ‘‘never’’ or ‘‘a few times a Figure 2 Situations that favour the emergence of erotic fantasies. Situations favorisant l’émergence de l’imaginaire érotique. Figure 3 Incidence of erotic fantasies during sexual intercourse . Incidence des imageries érotiques pendant les rapports sexuels. Figure 4 Incidence of erotic fantasies during masturbation. Incidence des imageries érotiques pendant la masturbation. Author's personal copy An exploratory study on male homosexual erotic imagery 49 Figure 5 Incidence of erotic fantasies outside sexual intercourse. Incidence des imageries érotiques en dehors des rapports sexuels. year’’ engaging in fantasies, whereas Palaia and Petruccelli (1993) found that 4% did not have any erotic imagery outside intercourse. In the present sample, 52.6 and 31.5% declared they engaged in fantasies outside intercourse ‘‘a few times a day’’ and ‘‘a few times a week’’ respectively, whereas ‘‘several times a day’’ 9.7% and only 6.2% ‘‘a few times a month’’. Formerly, instead, values obtained from the subjects interviewed were as follows: ‘‘several times a day’’ 3.9%, ‘‘a few times a day’’ 33.3%, ‘‘a few times a week’’ 29.4%, ‘‘a few times a month’’ 13.7%, ‘‘a few times a year’’ 15.7%. On the whole, these data seem to indicate that today’s male homosexual is less embarrassed in expressing his sexuality, probably also thanks to a wider social acceptance of sex in general and of homosexuality in particular. Qualitative data Some information regarding the content of sexual imagery can be obtained by listing the top five fantasies ranked in order of frequency of occurrence (Leitenberg and Henning, 1995). Sometimes these data are obtained from studies in which checklists of different fantasies are provided to participants but unfortunately, the lists are usually not the same across studies. In other instances, participants are asked to write down their favourite, most arousing or most frequently recurring fantasies. Despite the different methodologies and samples used in these studies, some findings are in common, apparent and consistent among them. Masters and Johnson (1979) reported a list with: imagery of sexual anatomy; forced sexual encounters with men and women (forcing or being forced or both), sexual encounters with a woman, idyllic sexual encounters with unknown men and group sexual activities. For Price et al. (1985) the top five fantasies for male homosexuals were: unspecified sexual activity with another man, performing oral sex, having a partner perform oral sex, participating in anal sex, having sex with another man the subject had not been previously involved with. Palaia and Petruccelli (1993) reported a list with: a scene from an erotic film, performing oral sex, caressing a man’s genitals, performing anal sex and watching a man take off his clothes. Our results show that the five most common fantasies in order of frequency of occurrence for men are: group sexual activities, a scene from an erotic film, caressing a man’s genitals, imagery of sexual anatomy and having a partner perform oral sex. We note that the main differences refer to encounters with the opposite sex and forced sexual encounters which were not found in our sample. In our results there is a striking similarity between the fantasies of male heterosexuals (Price et al., 1985) and those of male homosexuals, apart from the prevalence of sex of the imaginary partner. Discussion and conclusion In recent research there is a growing interest in gender differences in erotic fantasies (Mussweiler and Förster, 2000; Zurbriggen and Yost, 2004). In this regard, an interesting point emerging from our results is the tendency towards psychological androgyny: on the one hand, today situations that stimulate erotic fantasies in the male homosexual are mainly visual and therefore more bound to male gender stereotypes as already demonstrated by several studies (Gold and Gold, 1991; Leitenberg and Henning, 1995; Ellis and Symons, 1990; Kelley, 1984; Follingstad and Kimbrell, 1986; Gil, 1990). On the other hand, some needs traditionally ascribed to complementary gender identity are now present, in that male homosexuals apparently let themselves express and recognize purely physical aspects and emotional and relational ones, reaching a greater balance than in the past. Furthermore, feeling freer to express their sexual orientation both to themselves and to others, male homosexuals participating in our study use their erotic imagery much more frequently than in the past, mainly during masturbation, which, moreover, is more frequent in the present sample. Another interesting finding is the total absence in our sample of erotic fantasies referring to the opposite sex and of fantasies of forced intercourse as found by Masters and Johnson (1979); in such fantasies the homosexual individual often played an active or passive role in a rape and victims were generally immobilized and forced into engaging in sexual activity. Such differences are the consequences of cultural changes which, also thanks to an increasing media coverage, have contributed to making sexuality freer and Author's personal copy 50 more accessible and that have probably given both men and women wider possibilities of exploring their own likes and dislikes, thus overcoming gender stereotypes. However, due to the small number of protocols, which were identical and comparable between the two Roman studies but however not validated, considering also the specific geographical location, our explanatory hypotheses are to be verified on larger populations and in different contexts. References Allen ES, Atkins DC, Baucom DH, Snyder DK, Coop Gordon C, Glass SP. Intrapersonal, interpersonal and contextual factors in engaging and responding to extramarital involvement. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 2005;12(2):101—30. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (TR — fourth ed.), Washington DC, 2000. Crépault C, Couture M. Men’s erotic fantasies. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1980;9:565—81. Crépault C. L’imaginaire érotique et ses secrets. Sillery: Université du Québéc; 1981. Ellis BJ, Symons D. Sex differences in sexual fantasy: an evolutionary psychological approach. Journal of Sex Research 1990;27:527—55. Follingstad DR, Kimbrell CD. Sex fantasies revisited: an expansion and further clarification of variables affecting sex fantasy production. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1986;15: 475—86. Gil VE. Sexual fantasy experiences and guilt among conservative Christians: an exploratory study. Journal of Sex Research 1990;27:629—30. M. Cacioppo et al. Gold SR, Gold RG. Gender differences in first sexual fantasies. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy 1991;17:207—16. Hicks TV, Leitenberg H. Sexual fantasies about one’s partners versus someone else: gender differences in incidence and frequency. The Journal of Sex Research 2001;38:43—50. Hsu B, Kling A, Kessler C, Kruipke K, Diefenbach P, Elias J. Gender differences in sexual fantasy and behavior in a college population: a ten-year replication. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy 1994;20:103—18. Kelley K. Sexual fantasy and attitudes as functions of sex of subject and content of erotica. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 1994; 1984;4:339—47. Leitenberg H, Henning K. Sexual fantasy. Psychological Bulletin 1995;117(3):469—96. Masters WH, Johnson VE. Homosexuality in perspective. Boston: Little Brown; 1979. Mussweiler T, Förster J. The sex->aggression link: a perceptionbehavior dissociation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2000;79:507—20. Palaia B, Petruccelli F. L’immaginario erotico nell’omosessualità. In: Baldaro Verde I, Gennazzani AR, Marrama P, editors. L’identità sessuale e le sue vicissitudini. La sessualità quale integrazione tra psicologico e biologico. Roma: CIC; 1993. p. 607—11. Person ES, Terestman N, Myers WA, Goldberg E, Borenstein M. Associations between sexual experiences and fantasies in a nonpatient population: a preliminary study. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis 1992;20:75—90. Price JH, Allensworth DD, Hillman K. Comparison of sexual fantasies of homosexuals and heterosexuals. Psychological Reports 1985;57:871—7. Zurbriggen EL, Yost MR. Power, desire and pleasure in sexual fantasies. The Journal of Sex Research 2004;41(3):288—300.
Similar documents
Nima Shaykhian
Président de l'Université : Professeur Jean-Pierre FINANCE Doyen de la Faculté de Médecine : Professeur Henry COUDANE Vice Doyen Mission « sillon lorrain » : Professeur Annick BARBAUD Vice Doyen Mi...
More information