Gender Differences in Motivational Attitudes in
Transcription
Gender Differences in Motivational Attitudes in
ISSN 1392-3110 (Print) ISSN 2351-6712 (Online) Socialiniai tyrimai /Social Research. 2014. Nr. 2 (35). 5-19 Gender Differences in Motivational Attitudes in Academic Studies: The Context of Lithuanian Students' Socialization Aušra Turčinskaitė-Balčiūnienė 1 , Gediminas Merkys 2 University of Applied Sciences, Didlaukio str. 49, LT-08303 Vilnius, Lithuania ausrat@yahoo.com. 2 Kaunas University of Technology, K.Donelaicio str. 73, LT-44029 Kaunas, Lithuania gediminas.merkys@ktu.lt. women vary across time, ethnic group, and social situation, but the opportunity to behave as manly men or womanly women is ubiquitous". When gender inequality is not conceptualized, the main discussion about gender differences is more or less explicitly related to the concept of gender identity. As noticed by Brinkman, Rabenstein, Rosen, and Zimmerman (2012), traditional approaches to conceptualizing the development of gender identity can be categorized into three general types of theories: 1) essentialist theories (arguing that gender is predetermined and directly tied to the biological categories of male and female as determined by genetics and hormones; suggesting that gender is dichotomous such that all males are inherently masculine and all females are inherently feminine; describing gender identity as being not necessarily something that develops, but simply unfolds over time); 2) developmental theories (arguing that gender identity develops over time in predictable and 'normative' stages such as those suggested by Piaget or Erikson; asserting that as children get older, they internalize the gender expectations they have learned and many of them endorse rigid gender rules); and 3) socialization theories. Because both the essentialist and developmental approaches overgeneralize gender identity development and both do not assume certain individual differences (for example pathologizing gender atypical children), as well as interpreting children as being mostly passive participants in their identity development process, our approach builds on the socialization theories. As literature review indicates, in the structures of gender identity, mainly developed through socialization in the family context, academic achievement motivation is formed as a result of family expectations and value valences. While in the case of science studies and science carrier choices gender differences are quiet often discussed (Boiche et al., 2013; Buday et al., 2012; Chouinard & Roy, 2008; Koveriene & Zickiene, 2008; Novelskaite, Abstract This article addresses the problem of genderbiased differences in the motivational attitudes toward academic studies and self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts as a result of socialization. Recently in Lithuania academic motivation issues have been considered to be of great importance and actuality by several researchers; nevertheless, there was no evidence provided on gender differences in the motivational attitudes in academic studies and gender effects on active academic improvementoriented efforts. In this paper the results of the Lithuanian higher education students' survey are analyzed and discussed. Keywords: socialization, gender differences, motivational attitudes. Introduction Different gender related issues are widely discussed in contemporary scientific literature. Thousands of studies have analysed various genderrelated patterns in almost every domain of human activity and the further questioning is still relevant and of a great actuality. From the moment of birth, gender is one of the most salient features that distinguish human beings. This gender label affects almost every aspect of a person's social life, from birth throughout childhood and the adult years. Across all cultural groups, there is perhaps no other variable that has such a sustained, critical influence throughout the lifespan as does gender. Gender is ever present and has both serious as well as trivial consequences for behaviour, determining such aspects as career opportunities or the colour of clothing one wears. Gender roles create social expectations, shape behaviours, and amplify or minimize gender differences that result from biology (Best, 2010). The complexity of the issue is reflected by Deutsch (2007): gender is continually socially reconstructed according to the 'normative conceptions' of gender. "People act with the awareness that they will be judged according to what is deemed appropriate feminine or masculine behaviour. These normative conceptions of men and 5 (Spirmen-Brown Formula or Cronbach's alpha, when appropriate; corrected item-total correlation and factor analysis); comparative analysis of statistical data, applying non-parametric statistical procedures (Mann-Whitney Test). 2008; Purvaneckiene, 2004; Stanisauskiene, 2008; Taljunaite, 2005; Urboniene, 2008; Zvinkliene, 2003 and 2005), there is no evidence if similar differences exist in overall motivational attitudes toward academic studies and whether those differences affect active improvement-oriented efforts. Whereas academic motivation issues were considered to be of great importance and actuality in several recent surveys conducted in Lithuanian higher education by Leonavičius and Rutkiene (2010), as well as by Matulionis et al. (2010), we must highlight the regrettable fact that gender effects on academic motivation were not discussed. Therefore, in this paper we focus on gender-biased differences in the motivational attitudes toward higher education studies and self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts as a result of socialization. Gender Differences in Motivational Attitudes toward Studies and Study-Related Behaviours In its most common and general usage, the term 'socialization' refers to the process of social interaction through which an individual acquires the norms, values, beliefs, attitudes, and language characteristic of his or her group. During that process, the individual self and personality are created and shaped (Gecas, 2000). Hence, socialization describes the accumulation of gender identity as a process that occurs over time due to the influence of others (Brinkman et al, 2012). Perhaps the most important insight regarding gender development is the critical role of socialization experiences. These differential social pressures operate throughout the life course and can maximize, minimize, or even eliminate gender differences in children's behaviours. The social settings, the social partners with whom children are engaged, the tasks they are assigned, and the way that parents and other socialization agents interact with children, influence the development of gender-related behaviours (Best, 2010). Within sociology, there are two main orientations toward socialization: 1) the structural-functionalist perspective views socialization primarily as the learning of social roles, considering that individuals become integrated members of society by learning and internalizing the relevant roles and statuses; and 2) the symbolic interactionist perspective views socialization mainly as self-concept formation; therefore, the core of socialization is considered to be the development of self and identity in the context of intimate and reciprocal relations (Gecas, 2000). Structural-functionalist socialization theory is often applied in empirical research, analysing gender differences (Carson & Knoester, 2011; Hadjar et al., 2008; Schwartz & Rubel-Lifschitz, 2009), but it is also criticized for certain limitations. First of all, it sees gender roles as learned by children from their parents, that is, primarily within the family, suggesting that one function of the family is to teach children the correct behaviours associated with their gender. Hence, gender is interpreted as a learned phenomenon and children are assumed to acquire the correct role through interaction with parents. This becomes problematic for those people who are not correctly socialized, since many people, irrespective of sexual orientation, don't embody or adopt gender stereotypes (Hicks, 2008). The second critical Scientific problem of this article is formulated by the following question: what gender differences in the motivational attitudes toward studies, resulting from socialization, may be observed in the context of Lithuanian higher education? The aim of the paper is to seize the problem of gender-biased differences in the motivational attitudes toward higher education studies and selfrecorded improvement-oriented efforts as a result of socialization. Research objectives: 1. To provide scientific literature analysis on gender identity development through socialization, including socializing family context and value socialization oriented to academic achievement and academic study aspiration and motivation issues. 2. To choose appropriate research methods for the assessment of gender-biased differences in motivational attitudes toward higher education studies and self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts. 3. To ensure the methodological quality of survey instrument, analysing such psychometric characteristics as reliability and construct validity. 4. To produce statistical data analysis, providing conclusions and discussion on gender-biased differences in motivational attitudes toward higher education studies and self-recorded improvementoriented efforts as a result of socialization. Research methods: scientific literature analysis on gender identity development through socialization, including socializing family context and value socialization oriented to academic achievement and academic study aspiration and motivation issues; assessment of the methodological quality of survey instrument, analysing such psychometric characteristics as reliability and construct validity 6 observation belongs to Deutsch (2007), arguing that gender is an ongoing emergent aspect of social interaction. Whereas structural-functionalist theories assume that individuals internalize the gendered norms that were salient when they were growing up, the symbolic interactionist perspective assumes that people respond to changing contemporary norms. This is an important implication of the idea that if gender is constructed, then it can be deconstructed. Gendered institutions can be changed, and the social interactions that support them can be undone. As noticed by Brinkman et al. (2012), children are in fact active agents who recognize the pressures to conform and adjust their behaviour accordingly and are able to play a more active role in their development and make conscious decisions regarding conformity. In this perspective children are social actors and they are not only shaped by their environment but also interact with and affect the environment by their behaviour. Therefore, our work builds on the symbolic interactionist perspective interpretation of gender socialization issues, where socialization as gender "identity formation occurs through a number of more specific processes associated with selfconcept development: reflected appraisals, social comparisons, self-attributions, and identification" (Gecas, 2000). & Roy, 2008) or gender differences in learning and study strategies as well as studying behaviours (Marrs & Sigler, 2011), while others draw attention on the family context factors such as family expectations (Boiche et all., 2013). Eccles (2011) discuss both academic achievement motivation and social family context factors. Spera (2006) analyses both academic achievement motivation and social family context factors. Hadjar et al. (2008), analyse both social family context and value socialization-related issues. Some gender differences discussed in psychology are quiet relevant to our study in sociology. For example, Sanches-Lopez et al. (2012), observed gender differences in personality structure on such dimensions like: a) Motivating Styles, b) Thinking Styles, and c) Behaving Styles. The authors explain these differences by the social learning of gender, operationalized as the level of conformity to gender norms, that play an important role in personality styles. This would mean that a greater or lower compliance to a given set of gender norms leads to differences in personality, and that, regardless of sex, the degree of conformity to these norms establishes differences between people, even within the same sex. Thus, Sanches-Lopez et al. (2012), observed that according to their data, gender socialization plays an important role in personality differences between men and women because, when the degree of adherence to gender norms in women is taken into account, the differences with respect to men decrease. On the other hand, Molina et al. (2013), found that being male is negatively associated with the level of cooperation as a personality style. The abovementioned gender difference was also obtained after netting out this effect from other socio-demographic and gender differences in risk, social and competitive preferences. The fact that they obtained similar results while using alternative subsamples and econometric techniques indicates the validity of the conclusions. The authors claim that their results point toward a gender difference in the level of cooperation that may be attributed to a genetic factor. Genetic perspective explaining gender differences in this case doesn't seem appropriate to our understanding. We are willing to explain these differences similarly to Sanches-Lopez et al.'s interpretation: we think that gender differences in personality styles and structures depend much more on socialization than on genetic factors. Social family context factors as important indicators for gender socialization differences are analysed by a number of scientists, drawing attention to such factors as parenting style (Carlson & Knoester, 2011; Hadjar et al., 2008), socialization style (Buday et al., 2012; Portfeli et al., 2012; Spera, 2006) and parents' expectations and attitudes (Boiche According to scientific literature review, genderbiased differences, interpreted as an outcome of gender socialization, most often are analysed within psychological and sociological perspectives focusing on various social and psychological factors. In the context of our research, aiming to address a particular field of interest, many aspects of gender differences discussed in contemporary scientific literature were consciously excluded from our literature review (for example: gender differences according to race; ethnicity; social class; religiosity and spirituality; anxiety; anger; bullying; giftedness; disabilities; substance use; delinquency; violence; various sexual issues, including homosexuality and sexual harassment; cohabitation and marriage; language use particularities; health issues; political issues; vocational socialization; the use of technologies; females in masculine settings; urban and rural gender differences; gender inequality, etc.). We focused on gender identity development through socialization, including socializing family context and value socialization oriented to academic achievement and academic study aspiration and motivation issues (see Table 1). Some authors analysing gendered achievementrelated educational and occupational choices according to Expectancy Value Model focus on academic achievement motivation factors (Chuinard 7 Table 1 Gender Differences as a Result of Gender Socialization within Psychology and Sociology Publications Socialization related issues Gender identity development Publications Within Psychology Journals 1) Cognitive perspectives on gender development (Martin & Ruble, 2011); 2) Gender differences in personality styles and structures (Molina, Gimenez-Nadal, Cuesta, Gracia-Lazaro, Moreno & Sanchez, 2013; Sanchez-Lopez, Cuellar-Flores, Liminana & Corbalan, 2012). 1) Role ofgender within parental socialization to academic work and academic achievements (Portfeli, Ferrari, & Nota, 2012); 2) Parental socialization style influence on academic Family context achievement and motivation (Spera, 2006); 3) Gendered achievement-related educational and occupational choices based on Expectancy Value Model (Boiche, Plaza, Chalabaev, Guillet-Descas, & Sarrazin, 2013; Eccles, 2011) Values Publications Within Sociology Journals Symbolic interactionist perspectives on gender identity development (Brinkman et al., 2012; Deutsch, 2007). 1) Gender role attitudes as a result of family expectations (Raty & Kasanen, 2007); 2) Value socialization in the family through gender-specific parental styles (Hadjar, Baier & Boehnke, 2008); 1) Value socialization in the family through gender-specific parental Gender related differences in value structures and aspirations styles (Hadjar et al., 2008). (Morgan & Robinson, 2013; Schwartz & Rubel-Lifschitz, 2009) 2) Intergenerational transmission ofgender ideology (Carlson & Knoester, 2011) 1) Gendered achievement-related educational and occupational choices based on Expectancy Value Model (Chouinard & Roy, Gender and 2008; Eccles, 2011; Marrs & Sigler, 2011); science carrier, 2) Parental socialization style influence on academic academic achievement and motivation (Spera, 2006); achievement 3) Higher intrinsic motivation ofmales is related to higher motivation level ofclass performance (Cortright, Lujan, Blumberg, Cox & DiCarlo, 2013) et al., 2013; Chouinard & Roy, 2008; Eccles, 2011; Marrs & Sigler; Morgan & Robinson, 2013; Raty & Kasanen, 2007; Schwartz & Rubel-Lifschitz, 2009). Parents play a unique role as socializing agents transmitting their educational values, goals, and aspirations to their children. Parental values represent the importance parents place on their children's educational achievement. Researchers studying parental socialization of school achievement have found that aspects of parenting are strong predictors of adolescent school achievement and motivation; they observed that parents' beliefs (e.g., values, aspirations, goals) about their child's education are significant predictors of adolescents' grade point average; they discovered a relationship between parenting styles and adolescent self-regulation (Spera, 2006). In analysing socialization, Hadjar et al. (2008), conducted a study concentrating on authoritarian and achievement-focused parenting, as well as structural and ideological predictors thereof. According to the results, there are direct paths between parents' values/ attitudes and adolescents' values/attitudes reflecting 1) Gender and science carrier choice as a result of social support (Buday, Stake & Peterson, 2012); 2) Gendered institutional structures, cultures, and practices posing barriers to women advancement and acceptance as professionals scientists (Rhoton, 2011). the influence of social-structural variables and parenting modes. This relationship is comparatively low. This could be an indication that there are other socialization agents (for example, school, peers, massmedia) that — at least additionally — also shape the adolescents' value and attitude systems. The 14-yearold adolescents in the study are probably already more peer-centred than younger children for whom parents are the primary source of value socialization. Nevertheless, the authors share the conviction that structural variables (such as profession, prestige, income) influence parenting styles, and that parenting styles play a distinct role in the socialization of values. According to Raty & Kasanen (2007), parents' perceptions of their children's abilities can be regarded as naturally occurring social cognitions. Examining these cognitions is an efficient way to clarify the interplay between categorical and individuating information in parents' interpretations of their children's schooling. According to our culturally prevailing representation of intelligence, boys are expected to surpass girls in the cognitive 8 domain, mathematics in particular, whereas girls are expected to surpass boys in the verbal and social domains. Mathematics is generally regarded as a domain in which parents' gender-bound expectations are the strongest, and this tendency shows a measure of cross-cultural generalizability. The authors observed that in Finland, despite girls' and boys' equal school performance in mathematics, the parents' assessments in favour of boys do not seem to change, at least during the first few years of school. Moreover, there is evidence that parents' underestimation of girls' mathematical capacity is also manifested in their inclination to explain girls' mathematical accomplishments with reference to hard work and boys' mathematical accomplishments with reference to natural ability. To sum up, with the findings of Raty & Kasanen (2007), it is evident that parents' assessments of their children's competence in both mathematics and native language are clearly influenced by a cultural expectation that the two genders possess different abilities, and this expectation is further moderated by the parents' attitude toward it. Parents' endorsement of the gender stereotype seems to lower the competencies they attribute to their child, i.e., mathematical competence in the case of girls and verbal competence in the case of boys. Because endorsement of the gender stereotype predicted the parents' later assessments of competence, the findings lend support to the argument that it really is the gender-related stereotype that affects parental competence evaluations, not the other way around. The results from the study of Spera (2006) suggest that the educational goals and values parents hold for their children are related to the practices they enact to socialize their children. Eccles (2011) summarizes a theoretical model of the social, cultural, and psychological influences on achievement-related choices and outlines how this model explains gendered educational and occupational choices. He argues that both gender differences and individual differences within each gender in educational and occupational choices are linked to differences in the individuals' expectations for success and subjective task values (for example, females are less likely to enter mathematics and physical sciences in particular than males both because they have less confidence in their mathematics and physical sciences abilities and because they place less subjective value on these fields than they place on other possible occupational niches). Furthermore, gendered socialization practices at home, in the schools, and among peers play a major role in shaping these individual differences in self-perceptions and subjective task values. Similar conclusions are drawn by Portfeli et al. (2012), after having asserted a theoretical model of academic and work socialization within the family setting. The authors discovered that children's perceptions of parents mediate the relationship between parents' and children's selfreported work valences, and that children's work valences are in turn associated with academic interest and achievement. In other words, parents serve as role models in the construction of their children's conceptions of (academic) working/work. Boiche et al. (2013), supports Eccles's theoretical model as well. Researchers observed that gender differences in sport are often perceived as resulting from natural biological factors; however, these gender differences can also be explained by social processes. Their work indicates that adolescents tend to endorse gender beliefs related to sport competence and value that are related to the beliefs they perceive in the cultural milieu and in particular their parents and that parents could transmit such beliefs in an implicit way. The authors bring empirical support to such a hypothesis in the sport context, which is still conceived as a male domain. The results indicate that this social transmission might operate both through explicit processes and unconsciously. Boiche et al., also point to the mechanisms through which the adolescents' endorsement of such beliefs may in turn lead to lower self-perceptions and behavioural disengagement from organized sport. Buday et al. (2012), analyse the problem of women being severely underrepresented in science careers. There is no evidence that underrepresentation is due to gender differences in intrinsic aptitude or achievement. Studies have found no biological differences between men and women that can explain why there are so few women faculty in academic science and math departments and other positions of science leadership. Scientists with traditionally masculine qualities are believed to be more competent, but women who possess these qualities are viewed as being aggressive. Men and women both hold these implicit biases that favour men and disadvantage women in science careers. According to the authors, these findings suggest that the root of the underrepresentation of women in science careers is social and psychological in nature. The results of the Buday et al. study suggest that increasing social and environmental support for a science career could result in both 1) increased self-confidence regarding a career in science, and 2) improved interest in and motivation for a science career. Chouinard & Roy (2008) examined high-school students' motivation in mathematics over time, particularly in terms of competence beliefs, utility value and achievement goals, aiming to provide a clearer picture of this evolution related to gender. The results support the 9 theory of gender convergence in mathematics rather than gender differentiation (decrease in positive attitudes towards mathematics affects girls more than boys at a certain moment, but the gap between boys and girls decrease over time). Gender differences in study motivation and studies-related behaviour observed by Marrs & Sigler (2011) were noted as follows: female students are more deliberate in their studying in order to be prepared for examinations by applying strategic studying. A significant gender difference was also found for deep learning (understanding). In relation to study strategies, the authors observed significant differences on the general motivation to do well, self-testing (reviewing material), study aids (creating diagrams, charts, using other memory aids), and time management (the ability to self-regulate the use of time so that sufficient time is devoted to studying). According to Marrs & Sigler, "these findings imply that female college students are more motivated academically and display more self-discipline related to actual studying behaviours". The authors discuss the differences in study skills as possibly related to gender roles regarding appropriate male studying behaviour. Students who score higher on femininity tend to rank studying behaviours as more important than students who score higher on masculinity. It looks like achievement without much effort may be valued by males more than achievement that requires hard work. Achievement motivation and academic choices in college may be related to the perceived gender roles and expectations that men have concerning their perceived masculinity or femininity. Summing up scientific literature analysis on gender identity development through socialization, we consider gender identity formation a result of reflected appraisals, social comparisons, selfattributions, and identifications, primarily affected by social environment and family context. Starting within the family context and influenced by other socialization agents, value socialization forms academic achievement, academic study aspiration, and motivation patterns. Various gender socialization outcomes are discussed in science publications: gender differences in personality structures, concerning motivation, thinking style and behaviour, as well as cooperation; the importance of parents as socializing agents transmitting their educational values, goals and aspirations is highlighted; gender biased differences in studying, learning, and time management practices are discussed, implying gender differences in academic motivation and self-discipline related to actual studying behaviour areas. With reference to Marrs & Sigler's (2011) study within psychology, we design our sociological survey in the same approach, aiming to analyse gender differences in the motivational attitudes toward general higher education studies and self-recorded improvementoriented efforts as a result of socialization. Method The motivational attitudes toward academic studies and self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts were assessed using certain scales from the QUISS II1 survey methodology, elaborated by the scholars team at KonztanzUniversity (Germany) in 1983, asit was used repeatedly in surveys in Germany and systematically improved by the authors (Bargel, Schmidt & Bargel, 2009). In 2009 the latest and improved version of the QUISS II was used in a students survey in Lithuania. Lithuanian State Studies Foundation financed the scientists' group project "Academic Studies Quality and Social Context Survey", directed by Professor Gediminas Merkys in which QUISS II was translated and culturally adapted (Merkys, Lekavičiene, Saparniene, Mikutaviciene, & Turcinskaite-Andujar, 2009; Turcinskaite-Balciuniene & Merkys, 2010). The questionnaire qualitative translation assessment was ensured by expert panel review. There were 3 versions of the questionnaire: English, German (original) and French. Each version of the questionnaire was translated separately and then the three translations were confronted in order to elaborate the best possible translation by the panel of three experts assessing and ensuring translation quality. Two of three experts were researchers. Once the experts validated the translation, the questionnaire was reviewed by another independent trilingual expert (Lithuanian, German and English) and returned to the expert panel review for finalisation of the questionnaire translation procedure. After the final expert panel translation quality review, the final Lithuanian questionnaire version was approved to be of a good quality and suitable for applying it as a survey instrument (Pauliukaite, TurcinskaiteBalciuniene and Balčiūnas, 2011). Then in 2011 the translated, culturally adapted and validated instrument (Pauliukaite, Turcinskaite-Balciuniene & Merkys, 2011) was used in another students survey in Lithuania. Hence, the results discussed in this article are drawn from the data collected in two separate surveys by paper-pencil type questionnaire. In this paper we analyse data collected by 52 items distributed in 10 Likert-type scales and subscales of different levels (from 3 to 7), with central categories (see Table 2). The motivational attitudes to studies are assessed by 36 items, regrouped in 4 scales; two measure the perceived importance or usefulness of 1 10 Qualitâtsverbesserung in Schulen und Schulsysthemen II. various factors for personal growth and professional perspectives, one scale measures the willingness to engage in different extracurricular activities and the last scale measures personal advancement perception as a motivational force for further studying efforts. Self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts are assessed by 16 items regrouped in 2 scales representing different aspects. The Personal Initiative and Extracurricular Activities scales describe more personal initiative demanding efforts that are not required by the institution and teaching staff. On the contrary, the Studying Efforts and Applying Academic Skills in Curricular Activities scale represents items related to the efforts that are more or less explicitly required by the institution and teaching staff. Evaluating the psychometric characteristics (reliability and construct validity) of the measurement instruments, the SPSS 13.0 statistical package for Windows was used by computing different statistics: 1) the Spirmen-Brown formula and Cronbach's alpha, when appropriate, in order to assess the internal consistency of the items in the scales; 2) the corrected item-total correlation analysis in order to determine how well one item's score is internally consistent with composite scores from all other items that remain; 3) and a factor analysis to determine the dimensionality of the scales (see Table 3). Using Likert-type scales is imperative to calculating Cronbach's a (scale length from 6 items to 12) or the Spirmen-Brown p;when the scale is relatively short, it is important to relate psychometric reliability to test length. Most of the scales are relatively short, including less than 7 items; therefore, Spirmen-Brown p was computed by forecasting a scale length of 12 items. The scale consisting of 11 items (Improvement in general transferable skills) records Table 2 Instrument for Studies' Motivational Attitudes and Active Improvement-Oriented Efforts Assessment Scales H S a vo m .<u •3 S m isi« o* "O s « s « > is a 2 .a s o a -A vo B yS 2 §lg S Factors for personal growth 8 items Factors for professional perspectives 8 items Practical skills 5 items How important/useful are these factors for your personal growth? Practice in participation in research project. Successful studies 3 items How important/useful are these factors for your personal growth? The best possible results of examinations. Practical skills 5 items How important/useful are these factors for your professional perspectives? Foreign language learning. Successful studies 3 items How important/useful are these factors for your professional perspectives? After BA studies, enter MA studies. Willingness to engage in improvementoriented extracurricular activities 6 items Personal advancement perception 14 items Personal initiative and extracurricular activities 11 items Item example Subscales Would you like to make use ofthe opportunities to improve your qualification in extracurricular activities? Attending open lectures. Improvement in specific skills, related to the main studies field 3 items How much self-improvement could you record in these areas? Practical skills, related to your studiesfield. Improvement in general transferable skills 11 items How much self-improvement could you record in these areas? Analytical and scientificproblems solving skills. Engagement in improvement-oriented extracurricular activities 6 items Are you attending any of the extracurricular opportunities listed below to improve your qualification? Foreign language courses. Active improvement oriented efforts by personal initiative 5 items How often in your studies did it happen to you? Reading some more studies related literature in addition to what was recommended by your teacher. Studying efforts and applying academic skills in curricular activities 5 items Do these statements correspond to your situation? You are able to learn quiet easily new material, facts and details. 11 an excellent reliability score. According to George and Mallery (2003), lower than 0,50 Cronbach's a scores are unacceptable; from 0,50 to 0,60 scores are poor; from 0,60 to 0,70 scores are questionable; from 0,70 to 0,80 scores are acceptable; from 0,80 to 0,90 scores are good; and higher than 0,90 scores are excellent. A Cronbach's a of 0,80 is considered to be a reasonable goal. The corrected item-total correlation analysis for each of the scale's items was computed. It is the correlation between a given item and the sum score of the other scale's items. The last column in Table 3 displays for each scale the range of its items corrected item-total correlations. This is a way to assess how much one item's score is internally consistent with composite scores from all other items that remain. This correlation is considered weak when the score is less than 0,30 (De Vaus, 2004); in our case all correlations are higher. Exploratory factor analysis involving the principle component analysis extraction and varimax rotation is commonly used to assess the construct validity. Factor analysis is based on the correlation matrix of the variables involved, and the correlations usually need a large sample size before they stabilize. The sample size is expected to be at least 300, with 500 being better and 1000 or more excellent. As a rule, a bare minimum of 10 observations per variable is necessary to avoid computational difficulties. As our sample size is from 1211 to 1517, factor analysis can be applied for dimensionality analysis ofthe scales. Before conducting a factor analysis for each scale, the Kaiser-MeyerOlkin measure of sampling adequacy needs to be run: Table 3 Internal Consistency and Construct Validity Statistics Scales (N varies from 1211 to 1517) KMO Explained variance (%) Factor lodging SpirmenBrown p Cronbach's a Corrected item-total correlation Studies ' Motivational Attitudes 36 items Factors for personal growth Practical skills 5 items 0,73 44,4 0,75-0,62 0,95* - 0,37-0,53 Successful studies 3 items 0.62 59,9 0,83-0,67 0,97* - 0,37-0,55 Factors for professional perspectives (2 subscales) Practical skills 5 items 0,74 41,0 0.73-0.59 0,94* - 0,34-0,47 Successful studies 3 items 0,64 60,6 0.71-0,82 0,97* - 0,41-0,54 Willingness to engage in improvement-oriented extracurricular activities 6 items 0,82 47,6 0,76-0,63 0,97* 0,77 0,42-0,59 Improvement in specific skills, related to the main studies field 3 items 0,70 70,7 0,82-0,87 0,98* - 0,60-0,68 Improvement in general transferable skills 11 items 0,96 55,8 0,68-0,79 0,86 0,92 0,60-0,73 Personal advancement perception Active Improvement-Oriented Efforts 16 items Personal initiative and extracurricular activities (11 items, two-dimensional scale) Engagement in improvementoriented extracurricular activities 6 items 0,84 48,0 0,63-0,73 0,98* 0,79 0,48-0,57 Active improvement oriented efforts by personal initiative 5 items 0,76 53,4 0,65-0,80 0,97* - 0,49-0,64 Studying efforts and applying academic skills in curricular activities 5 items 0,73 49,8 0,66-0,80 0,97* - 0,47-0,62 * Spirmen-Brown p computed forecasting scales length of 12 items. 12 a value lower than 0,50 is unacceptable (Cekanavicius ir Murauskas, 2004). According to the Kaiser-MeyerOlkin measure of sampling adequacy, none of the scales records unacceptable sampling adequacy. For all scales factor analysis can be applied - none of the correlation matrices is an identity matrix (Bartlett's tests of sphericity are significant with probabilities less than 0.05). According to the results, both validity and reliability analyses of the instrument produced reasonably good results; therefore, scales can be concluded and declared to have acceptable psychometric properties and further analysis of the data can be produced. Sample In 2009 the questionnaire was completed by 991 respondents and in 2011 by 555 respondents; from Kaunas (in total 442 respondents), Klaipeda (in total 292 respondents), Siauliai (in total 409 respondents) and Vilnius (in total 403 respondents); from universities (1142 respondents constituted of 616 females, 380 males and 146 not specified) and colleges (404 respondents constituted of 201 female, 138 male and 65 not specified), in their second and third study years of Bachelor degree studies in different programmes. Characteristics of the sample are displayed in Table 4. The paper-pencil questionnaire was completed by higher education students in their studies Table 4 Sample Date University College BA Study Year 2nd University 3rd 2009 November Total 991 2nd College 3rd 2nd University 2011 April 3rd 2nd College 3rd Female Total 817 78 18 38 96 230 71 22 79 49 221 2 1 41 Town Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Kaunas Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Klaipėda Šiauliai Vilnius Total Klaipėda Šiauliai Total 44 26 14 19 1 60 37 33 8 18 96 11 30 14 14 69 33 20 27 80 13 4 17 13 Male Total 616 64 2 13 32 111 25 17 31 15 88 6 1 30 9 46 8 11 14 33 54 28 10 29 121 12 5 9 34 60 2 12 16 30 17 12 29 Not specified Total 211 19 1 11 31 62 12 6 19 10 47 1 1 11 1 14 4 24 5 2 35 19 4 3 5 31 1 2 3 6 4 6 10 6 6 Total 161 21 62 159 403 108 45 129 74 356 9 3 82 10 104 30 46 35 17 128 110 65 21 52 248 24 37 26 48 135 39 38 43 120 36 16 52 institutions. In average it took about 75 minutes to fill in the questionnaire. In further analysis of gender socialization differences in the motivational attitudes to studies and active improvement-oriented efforts, "not specified" gender cases are removed from the data. Results Prior to starting the analysis of research results, psychometric properties (item reliability and construct validity) of the measurement instrument were assessed. According to the results, research instrument scales were concluded to have acceptable psychometric properties, necessary for any further analysis of the data (see Table 3). Following an exploratory factor analysis, factor scores may be computed and used in subsequent analyses. Factor scores are composite variables that provide information about an individual's placement on the factors. For the scales of Motivational attitudes toward studies and Active improvement-oriented attitudes a least squares regression approach was applied to predict factor scores. The procedure of the least squares regression is a multivariate procedure, which takes into account not only the correlation between the factors and between factors and observed variables (via item loadings), but also the correlation among observed variables, as well as the correlation among oblique factors. Under this process, the computed factor scores are standardized to a mean of zero; however, the standard deviation of the distribution of factor scores (by factor) will be 1 if principal components methods are used and will be the squared multiple correlation between factors and variables (typically used as the communality estimate) if principal axis methods are used (DiStefano, Zhu and Mindrila, 2009). This procedure was applied for the scales maximizing validity of estimates and producing Table 5 Mann-Whitney Test Statistics Scales Factors for personal growth: subscale Practical skills T3 S on T3 « o£ Factors for personal growth: subscale Successful studies Factors for professional perspectives: subscale Practical skills "O s Factors for professional perspectives: subscale Successful studies s o a Willingness to engage in improvementoriented extracurricular activities § Personal advancement perception: subscale Improvement in specific skills, related to the main studies field Personal advancement perception: subscale Improvement in general transferrable skills Personal initiative and extracurricular activities: Engagement in improvement• ^ S £ . oriented extracurricular activities U (Ö Personal initiative and extracurricular s w U >M ^ activities: Active improvement oriented ou ® efforts by personal initiative & =s . IS -E o Studying efforts and applying academic Gender N Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total Female Male Total 746 478 1224 761 490 1251 719 458 1177 738 462 1200 629 404 1033 799 495 1294 799 496 1295 588 406 994 804 499 1303 803 507 1310 14 Mean Rank Mean Diff. MannWhitney U Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed) 663,06 533,59 0,33 140576,5 ,000 686,75 531,65 0,43 140214,0 ,000 640,08 508,81 0,33 127923,5 ,000 653,53 515,79 0,40 131342,5 ,000 556,28 455,84 0,36 102348,5 ,000 667,25 615,62 0,14 181974,0 ,016 689,87 580,55 0,30 164697,0 ,000 500,57 493,05 0,04 117559,0 ,681 641,66 668,66 0,08 192284,0 ,208 701,73 582,29 0,31 166441,0 ,000 a standardized summarizing scale variable for each of the scales. Then the normality of the distributions was verified and after it was discovered that none of the variables was normally distributed, non-parametric tests were chosen to assess the gender differences in the motivational attitudes toward academic studies and self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts (see Table 5). In the Table 5 a significant difference in the two samples is indicated by the difference between the two group means which is higher than 0,20. The highest mean difference is recorded in the scales of the motivational attitudes toward academic studies, when male and female assess the importance of successful studies factors for their personal growth and professional perspectives. The smallest mean differences are obtained in the scales of self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts: Personal Initiative and Extracurricular Activities. A relatively small mean difference is recorded in one scale of the motivational attitudes toward academic studies, when personal advancement in skills is related to the main studies field (see Table 5). According to the results, females score higher in almost all scales except the two scales of self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts with one of the smallest mean differences: in the Extracurricular Activities scale the male records relatively higher scores than females even though this difference is not statistically significant. For gender differences assessment using the MannWhitney Test, it appears that the distribution functions in male and female groups differ with respect to the median in all cases of the motivational attitudes to studies and in the case of one scale, measuring selfrecorded improvement-oriented efforts: Studying Efforts and Applying Academic Skills in Curricular Activities scale. According to the data displayed in Table 5, in all cases except one, the significance level a=0.00 in the case of Personal advancement Perception: the subscale, expressing improvement in specific skills, related to the main studies field a=0.016. In the case ofthe other two scales, measuring self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts: Personal Initiative scale and Extracurricular Activities scale, the distribution functions in male and female groups have identical distribution functions (see Table 5). Our results confirm the gender differences observed by Marrs & Sigler (2011). Females record statistically significant higher scores in all the scales of academic studies' motivational attitudes. Similar results are obtained in the self-recorded improvementoriented efforts' scale which concerns Studying Efforts and Applying Academic Skills in Curricular Activities. When focusing on the two scales of selfrecorded improvement-oriented efforts, expressing personal initiative and involvement in extracurricular activities, there is no statistically significant gender difference observed. Conclusions and Discussion According to the literature review, we consider gender identity formation a result of reflected appraisals, social comparisons, self-attributions, and identifications strongly affected by social environment and primarily the family context. Starting within the family context and influenced by other socialization agents, value socialization forms academic achievement, academic study aspiration, and motivation patterns, resulting in gendered studies behaviours. Motivational attitudes toward studies and selfrecorded improvement-oriented efforts were assessed using certain scales from the QUISS II survey methodology, translated, culturally adapted and validated. According to the results, both validity and reliability analyses, the instrument was concluded to have acceptable psychometric properties for data analysis. Our results confirmed and complemented the gender differences observed by Marrs & Sigler (2011): 1. Females record statistically significant higher scores in all the scales of motivational attitudes toward academic studies. 2. Females record statistically significant higher scores in the self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts scale which concerns studying efforts and applying academic skills in curricular activities. 3. There is no statistically significant difference between men and women in the two scales of self-recorded improvement-oriented efforts, expressing personal initiative and involvement in extracurricular activities. The results obtained in the survey indicate gender socialization differences that reveal females are being socialized as either more academically motivated or more submissive and agreeing to academic requirements (recording higher scores in self-reported efforts and academic skills required by the curricular activities). It could be explained by Sanches-Lopez et al. (2012), gender behaving styles differences: submissiveness and cooperation/agreeing being characteristics of women, while independence and unconventionality being characteristic of men. This result may reflect a gender role regarding appropriate behaviour which is a direct outcome of socialization. Similar observations are made by Raty & Kasanen (2007), describing social stereotype by which at least in the case of sciences females are supposed to work hard while males refer to natural ability; or 15 by Marrs & Sigler (2011), finding that masculinity is related with academic achievement without much work. On the other hand, although females are more motivated and assumingly work harder in curricular activities than males, they somehow do not record higher levels of personal initiative and involvement in extracurricular activities levels. This may also be related to the gender socialization issues. Further research would be helpful for developing a greater understanding of gender differences in the motivational attitudes toward academic studies and behaviours as a result of socialization. Another important direction for future research is to explore how social class relates to possible gender differences in the motivational behaviours and attitudes toward academic studies. References 1. Bargel, T., Schmidt, M., Bargel, H. (2009). Quality and equity in higher education - international experiences and comparisons. International Workshop November 2008. Konstanz University. 2. Best, D., L. (2010). The Contributions of the Whitings to the Study of the Socialization of Gender. Journal of Cross-CulturalPsychology, 41 (4), 534-545. 3. Boiche, J., Plaza, M., Chalabaev, A., Guillet-Descas, E., Sarrazin, P. (2013). Social Antecedents and Consequences of Gender-Sport Stereotypes During Adolescence. Psychology of Women Quarterly, Sage, 1-16. 4. Brinkman, B.,G., Rabenstein, K.,L., Rosen, L., A., Zimmerman, T., S. (2012). Children's Gender Identity Development: The Dynamic Negotiation Process Between Conformity and Authenticity. Youth and Society, Sage, 1-18. 5. Buday, S., K., Stake, J., E., Peterson, Z., D. (2012). Gender and the Choice of a Science Carrier: The Impact of Social Support and Possible Selves. Sex Roles, 66, 197-209. 6. Carlson, D., L., Knoester, C. (2011). Family Structure and the Intergenerational Transmission of Gender Ideology. Journal of Family Issues, 32 (6), 709-734. 7. Chuinard, R., Roy, N. (2008). Changes in High-School Students'Competence Beliefs, Utility Value and Achievement Goals in Mathematics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 31-50. 8. Cortright, R., N., Lujan, H., L., Blumberg, A., J., Cox, J., H., DiCarlo, S., E. (2013). Higher Levels of Intrinsic Motivation are Related to Higher Levels of Class Performance for Male but Not Female Students. Advance in Physiology Education, 37 (3), 221-232. 9. Cekanavicius, V., Murauskas, G. (2004). Statistika ir jos taikymo metodai II. Vilnius: TEV. 10. Deutsch, F., M. (2007). Undoing Gender. Gender and Society, 21, 106-127. 11. DiStefano, C., Zhu, M., Mindrila, D. (2009). Understanding and Using Factor Scores: Considerations for the Applied Researcher. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation, 14 (20), 1-11. 12. De Vaus, D. (2004). Surveys in Social Research. Routledge. 13. Eccles, J. (2011). Gendered Educational and Occupational Choices: Applying the Eccles et al. Model of Achievement-Related Choices. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35 (3), 195-201. 14. Gečas, V. (2000). Socialization. In Borgatta, E. & Montgomery, R. (Eds.), Gale Encyclopedia of Sociology (2855-2864). New York: Macmillan Reference USA. 15. Hadjar, A., Baier, D., Boehnke, K. (2008). The Socialization of Hierarchic Self-interest: Value Socialization in the Family. Young: Nordic Journal of Youth Research, 16 (3), 279-301. 16. Hicks, S. (2008). Gender Role Models... Who Needs 'Em?! Qualitative Social Work, 7(1), 43-59. 17. Koverienė, A., & Žičkienė, S. (2008). Support Programmes for Women in Science in Lithuania and EU Countries. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 5, 109-114. 18. Leonavičius, V., & Rutkienė, A. (2010). Aukštojo mokslo sociologija: studijų pasitenkinimas ir vertinimas. Kaunas: Vytauto Didžiojo universitetas. 19. Marrs, H., Sigler, E., A. (2011). Male Academic Performance in College: The Possible Role of Study Strategies. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 13 (2), 227-241. 20. Martin, C., L., Ruble, D. (2011). Children's Search for Gender Cues: Cognitive Perspectives on Gender Development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67-70. 21. Matulionis, A., Maniukaitė, G., Trinkūnienė, I., Krukauskienė, E., & Liutikas, D. (2010). Lietuvos studentijos socialinio portreto bruožai. Vilnius: Lietuvos socialinių tyrimų centras. 22. Merkys, G., Lekavičienė, R., Saparnienė, D., Mikutavičienė, I., Turčinskaitė-Andujar, A. (2009). Akademinių studijų kokybė ir socialinis kontekstas. Lietuvos studentų tarpkultūrinis palyginamasis tyrimas (ataskaita). Kaunas. 23. Merkys, G., Pauliukaitė, Ž. (2010). Apklausos instrumentų tarpkultūrinio perkėlimo metodologinės patirtys: Lietuvos kontekstas. Filosofija. Sociologija, 21 (4), 340-356. 24. Molina, J.,A., Gimenez-Nadal, J., I., Cuesta, J.,A., GraciaLazaro, C., Moreno, Y., Sanchez, A. (2013). Gender Differences in Cooperation: Experimental Evidence on High School Students. PLOS ONE, 8 (12), 1-10. 25. Morgan, J., Robinson, O. (2013). IntrinsicAspirations and Personal Meaning Across Adulthood: Conceptual Interrelations and Age/Sex Differences. Developmental Psychology, 49 (5), 999-1010. 26. Novelskaitė, A. (2008). Scientific Aspirations of Lithuanian Bachelor Level Students in Informatics, Physics and Mechanics. Gender, Field of Study, adn Year of Study Related Factors. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 5, 120-127. 27. Pauliukaitė, Ž. (2009). Apklausos instrumentų kalbinė adaptacija tarpkultūriniuose tyrimuose. Tarpdisciplininis diskursas socialiniuose moksluose 2: Socialinių mokslų doktorantų irjaunųjų mokslininkų konferencijos straipsnių rinkinys, 85-91. Kaunas: Kauno technologijos universitetas. 16 28. Pauliukaitė, Ž., Turčinskaitė-Balčiūnienė,A., Balčiūnas, J. (2011). Translation Quality of Survey Instruments: Assessment of Questionnaire on Motivation and Academic Studies Quality. Social Research, 22 (1), 52-64. 29. Pauliukaitė, Z., Turčinskaitė-Balčiūnienė, A., Merkys, G. (2011). Cross-Cultural Validation of QUISS II Questionnaire: The Case of Lithuania. 4th International Conference: Changes in Social and Business Environment, issue: 04, (155-164). Panevėžys: Kauno technologijos universitetas, on www.ceeol.com. 30. Portfeli, E., Ferrari, L., Nota, L. (2012). Work Valence as a Predictor of Academic Achievement in the Family Context. Journal of Career Development, 40 (5), 371-389. 31. Purvaneckienė, G. (2004). Gender Dimension in the Process of Social Change. Socialiniai mokslai, 1(43), 11-19. 32. Raty, H., Kasanen, K. (2007). Gendered Views of Ability in Parents' Perceptions of Their Children's Academic Competencies. Sex Roles, 56, 117-124. 33. Rhoton, L., A. (2011). Distancing as a Gendered Barrier: Understanding Women Scientists' Gender Practices. Gender and Sociaty, 25 (6), 696-716. 34. Sanchez-Lopez, M., Cuellar-Flores, I., Liminana, R., Corbalan, J. (2012). Differential Personality Styles in Men and Women: The Modulating Effect of Gender Conformity. SAGE Open, April-June, 1-14. 35. Schwartz, S., H., Rubel-Lifschitz, T. (2009). CrossNational Variation in the Size of Sex Differences in Values: Effects of Gender Equality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (1), 171-185. 36. Spera, C. (2006). Adolescents'Perceptions of Parental Goals, Practices, and Styles in Relation to Their Motivation and Achievement. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 26 (4), 456-490. 37. Stanišauskienė, V. (2008). Karjeros sprendimų pagrįstumas lyčių aspektu. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 5, 143-149. 38. Taljūnaitė, M. (2005). Atstovavimo principas kaip lyčių lygybės aspekto integravimo priemonė. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 1, 5-8. 39. Turčinskaitė-Balčiūnienė, A., Merkys, G. (2010). Assessing Higher Education Quality in the Context of Conceptual Confusion and Methodological Eclecticism. Social Research, 4(21), 70-81. 40. Urbonienė, A. (2008). Prielaidos merginų profesinio identiteto vystymuisi technologijos ir fizinių mokslų studijose. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 5, 84-90. 41. Žvinklienė, A. (2003). Moters kelias į akademiją: istorija ir dabartis. Sociologija. Mintis ir veiksmas, 1, 50-70. 42. Žvinklienė, A. (2005). Situacija apibrėžta, kas toliau? Mokslininkių skatinimo perspektyvos Lietuvoje ir Europos Sąjungoje. Lyčių studijos ir tyrimai, 1, 9-16. Turčinskaitė-Balčiunienė, A., Merkys, G. Lyčių skirtumai ir studijų motyvacinės nuostatos: Lietuvos studentų socializacijos kontekstas Santrauka pagal trejopas teorijas: 1) esencialistines / biologines teorijas; 2) raidos teorijas; 3) socializacijos teorijas. Remiantis Deutsch (2007), Hicks (2008), Brinkman ir kt. (2012) rekomendacijomis, pagal simbolinio interakcionizmo perspektyvos suformuluotą lyčių socializacijos teoriją lyties identiteto formavimas suprantamas kaip procesas, vykstantis daugiau ar mažiau specifiniuose savęs suvokimų formavimosi procesuose: atspindėtuose įvertinimuose, socialiniuose palyginimuose, apibūdinant save ir identifikuojantis sąveikoje su kitais reikšmingais asmenimis, visų pirma tėvais. Siame straipsnyje pristatomi tyrimo, kuriuo siekta išsiaiškinti socializacijos nulemtus su lytimi susijusius akademinių studijų motyvacinių nuostatų ir akademinės elgsenos skirtumus, rezultatai. Tyrimas grindžiamas įvairių mokslininkų darbais, kuriuose atspindimi socializacijos nulemti lyčių skirtumai asmenybės struktūrose (Sanches-Lopez ir kt., 2012) ir asmenybės stiliuose (Molina ir kt., 2013), taip pat nagrinėjama įvairių šeimos konteksto veiksnių svarba lyčių socializacijos procesams (Boiche ir kt., 2013; Eccles, 2011; Hadjar irkt., 2008; Portfeli irkt., 2012; Raty ir Kasanen, 2007; Spera, 2006), sukeliantiems akademinių studijų motyvacijos ir akademinės elgsenos skirtumus (Buday ir kt., 2012; Chouinard ir Roy, 2008; Cortrightirkt., 2013; Eccles, 2011; Marrs ir Sigler, 2011; Rhoton, 2011; Spera,2006). Teoriniai tyrimo pagrindai. Mokslinėje literatūroje nagrinėjami įvairūs su lyčių skirtumų analize susiję probleminiai klausimai. Lyčių skirtumai reiškiasi beveik kiekvienoje žmogaus veiklos srityje. Best (2010) nuomone, tikriausiai nėra kito tokio socialinio kintamojo, kuris per visą gyvenimą būtų toks aktyvus kaip lytis. Deutsch (2007) pažymi, kad lyties samprata yra nuolat socialiai perkonstruojama, remiantis norminėmis lyties koncepcijomis, kurios bėgant laikui kinta, priklausomai nuo socialinės situacijos pokyčių. Todėl lyčių skirtumų problematika niekuomet nepraranda aktualumo, joje visuomet galima rasti naujų, nepakankamai išnagrinėtų aspektų. Nors Lietuvoje analizuota lyčių nelygybės tematika akademinėje srityje (Koverienė, Žičkienė, 2008; Novelskaitė, 2008; Purvaneckienė, 2004; Stanišauskienė, 2008; Taljūnaitė, 2005; Urbonienė, 2008; Žvinklienė, 2003 ir 2005), tačiau akademinės motyvacijos tema šiame kontekste negvildenta. Taip pat lyčių poveikis neanalizuotas pastaruoju metu Leonavičiaus ir Rutkienės (2010) bei Matulionio ir kt. (2010) atliktuose akademinės motyvacijos tyrimuose. Kai lyčių nelygybės klausimas nėra pagrindinė nagrinėjamos mokslinės problemos ašis, lyčių skirtumai siejami su lyties identiteto samprata. Brinkman ir kt. (2012) pažymėjo, kad lyties identiteto formavimas nagrinėjamas 17 Straipsnyje analizuojama mokslinė problema formuluojama klausimu: kokie socializacijos nulemti lyčių skirtumai pasireiškia akademinių studijų motyvacinėse nuostatose, nagrinėjamose Lietuvos aukštojo mokslo kontekste? Šio straipsnio tikslas - apčiuopti socializacijos nulemtus lyčių skirtumus aukštojo mokslo studijų motyvacinėse nuostatose ir respondentų pateiktuose atsakymuose į tai, kaip dažnai jie įsitraukia į asmeninės iniciatyvos reikalaujančią akademinę veiklą. Tyrimo metodika ir imtis. Akademinių studijų motyvacinių nuostatų ir tobulinimosi pastangų intensyvumo vertinimo skalės paimtos iš QUISSII apklausos instrumento, kurį sudarė ir ištobulino mokslininkų grupė iš Konstancos universiteto Vokietijoje (Bargel ir kt., 2009). Paskutinė QUISS II versija išversta į lietuvių kalbą ir naudota Lietuvos studentų apklausoje įgyvendinant mokslininkų grupės projektą: „Akademinių studijų kokybės ir socialinio konteksto tyrimas", per kurį atlikta klausimyno kultūrinė adaptacija (Pauliukaitė ir kt., 2011). Projektą rėmė Lietuvos valstybinis mokslo ir studijų fondas, projekto vadovas profesorius Gediminas Merkys (Merkys ir kt., 2009; Turčinskaitė-Balčiūnienė ir Merkys, 2010). 2011 m. išverstas ir adaptuotas instrumentas Lietuvos aukštųjų mokyklų studentų tyrime panaudotas antrą kartą. Popieriaus ir pieštuko klausimynas sudarytas iš psichometrinių Likerto tipo skalių, turinčių nuo trijų iki septynių padalų. Studijų motyvacinės nuostatos matuotos 7 skalėmis, išreiškiančiomis 1) praktinių įgūdžių svarbos asmeniniam tobulėjimui įvertinimą (5 testo žingsniai); 2) sėkmingų studijų svarbos asmeniniam tobulėjimui įvertinimą (3 testo žingsniai); 3) praktinių įgūdžių svarbos profesinėms perspektyvoms įvertinimą (5 testo žingsniai); 4) sėkmingų studijų svarbos profesinėms perspektyvoms įvertinimą (3 testo žingsniai); 5) pageidavimą tobulintis ir kelti kvalifikaciją veikloje, nenumatytoje specialybės studijų programoje (6 testo žingsniai); 6) specifinių įgūdžių, susijusių su studijuojamu dalyku, pažangos įžvelgimas (3 testo žingsniai); 7) bendrųjų ir perkeliamųjų įgūdžių pažangos įžvelgimas (11 testo žingsnių). Respondentų nurodytas tobulinimosi pastangų dažnumas matuotas pagal 3 skales, išreiškiančias: 1) asmeninį iniciatyvumą imantis tobulinimosi veiklos, nenumatytos specialybės studijų programoje (6 testo žingsniai); 2) asmeninę iniciatyvą savarankiškai papildomai gilinantis į dalykus, susijusius su studijomis (5 testo žingsniai); 3) aktyvias studijas ir akademinio darbo įgūdžius (5 testo žingsniai). Išanalizuoti tyrime naudotų skalių psichometriniai rodikliai: 1) skalių vidinis nuoseklumas apskaičiuojant Kronbacho alfa bendruosius koeficientus; 2) skalės testo žingsnių pastovumas įvertinant skalės ir jos testo žingsnio koreliaciją; 3) skalių komponentiškumas atliekant faktorinę analizę. Pagal visus analizuotus psichometrinius rodiklius instrumento skalių kokybė pripažinta pakankamai gera. Siame straipsnyje analizuojami duomenys, surinkti Lietuvos aukštųjų universitetinių ir neuniversitetinių mokyklų studentų apklausose 2009 m. (991 respondentas) ir 2011 m. (555 respondentai) iš Kauno (442 respondentai), Klaipėdos (292 respondentai), Šiaulių (409 respondentai) ir Vilniaus (403 respondentai). Apklausta 817 merginų ir 616 vaikinų, studijuojančių antrame ir trečiame bakalauro studijų kursuose iš universitetų (1142 respondentai) ir kolegijų (404 respondentai). Tyrimo rezultatai. Atlikus tiriamąją faktorinę analizę, apskaičiuotos standartinės skalių faktorinės reikšmės, išreiškiančios apibendrintus skalių kintamuosius, naudotus duomenų analizėje. Kadangi skalių apibendrintų reikšmių standartiniai kintamieji netenkino normalaus skirstinio sąlygos, lyčių skirtumams įvertinti buvo pasirinktas neparametrinis Mann-Whitney testas. Pirmiausia išanalizuoti vidurkių skirtumai vyrų ir moterų imtyse. Didžiausias vidurkių skirtumas gautas akademinių studijų motyvacinių nuostatų skalėse, pagal kurias vertinama sėkmingų studijų svarba asmeniniam tobulėjimui ir profesinėms perspektyvoms. Mažiausi vidurkių skirtumai gauti dviejose tobulinimosi pastangų skalėse, pagal kurias vertinamas asmeninis iniciatyvumas imantis tobulinimosi veiklos, nenumatytos specialybės studijų programoje, ir asmeninė iniciatyva savarankiškai papildomai gilinantis į dalykus, susijusius su studijomis. Remiantis Mann-Whitney testo rezultatais, merginų įverčiai beveik visose skalėse yra statistiškai reikšmingai didesni už vaikinų. Statistiškai reikšmingo skirtumo nėra tik dviejose tobulinimosi pastangų skalėse, išreiškiančiose: 1) asmeninį iniciatyvumą imantis tobulinimosi veiklos, nenumatytos specialybės studijų programoje, ir 2) asmeninę iniciatyvą savarankiškai papildomai gilinantis į dalykus, susijusius su studijomis. Išvados, diskusija. Remiantis mokslinės literatūros apžvalga, lyties identiteto formavimą suprantame kaip procesą, vykstantį daugiau ar mažiau specifiniuose savęs suvokinių formavimosi procesuose: atspindėtuose įvertinimuose, socialiniuose palyginimuose, apibūdinant save ir identifikuojantis sąveikose su kitais reikšmingais asmenimis, visų pirma tėvais. Šeimos kontekste vykstanti socializacija perteikia šeimos vertybes, suformuojančias tam tikrą akademinių studijų motyvaciją ir elgseną. Šiame straipsnyje nagrinėjamo tyrimo rezultatai iš esmės patvirtina ir papildo Marrs ir Sigler (2011) gautus rezultatus: 1. Merginos pasižymi didesne motyvacija pagal visas 7 akademinių studijų motyvacinių nuostatų skales, naudotas tyrime (išvada statistiškai reikšminga). 2. Pagal respondentų atsakymus, merginos įdeda daugiau pastangų studijuodamos ir sugeba geriau pasinaudoti savo akademiniais įgūdžiais nei vaikinai (išvada statistiškai reikšminga). 3. Nagrinėjant respondentų pateiktus atsakymus į tai, kaip dažnai jie iš asmeninės iniciatyvos įsitraukia į studijų kontekste nereikalaujamą veiklą, merginų ir vaikinų rezultatai nesiskiria. Gauti rezultatai rodo, kad dėl socializacijos merginos greičiausiai yra labiau motyvuotos akademinei veiklai arba labiau linkusios paklusti akademiniams reikalavimams (Sanches-Lopez ir kt., 2012, nustatė, kad dominuojantys merginų elgsenos stiliaus bruožai yra bendradarbiavimas ir paklusnumas, o vaikinų elgsenos stiliaus - savarankiškumas ir nekonvencionalumas). Gautus rezultatus galima bandyti paaiškinti ir lyčių vaidmenis atitinkančios elgsenos demonstravimu (Raty ir Kasanen, 2007, pagal galiojančius visuomenės stereotipus tiksliųjų mokslų atveju merginų 18 pasiekti rezultatai aiškinami darbštumu ir įdėtomis pastangomis, o vaikinų rezultatai - įgimtais gabumais; panašios nuomonės laikosi Marrs ir Sigler, 2011, teigiantys, kad pati vyriškumo samprata siejasi su akademiniais pasiekimais neįdedant daug pastangų, nes daug studijuoti esą nevyriška). Taip pat įdomi socializacijos pasekmė yra tai, kad merginos, nors labiau motyvuotos ir įdeda daugiau pastangų studijuodamos, nėra linkusios dažniau nei vaikinai užsiimti asmeninės iniciatyvos reikalaujančia tobulinimosi veikla. Plėtojant lyčių skirtumų studijas, kuriose nagrinėjama motyvacinių nuostatų ir akademinės elgsenos tematika, rekomenduotina tęsti tyrimus, tikslinti įvairius gautus rezultatus. Itin svarbus, tačiau šiame straipsnyje neanalizuotas klausimas yra lyties ir socialinio sluoksnio sąryšiai, paveikiantys akademinių studijų motyvacinių nuostatų ir akademinės elgsenos skirtumus. Pagrindiniai žodžiai: socializacija, lyčių skirtumai, motyvacinės nuostatos. The article has been reviewed. Received in 29 May 2014, accepted in 03 November 2014 19