Having, Holding, Being - Universität St.Gallen
Transcription
Having, Holding, Being - Universität St.Gallen
Having, Holding, Being The Relevance of Graspability for the Self-Extension Function of Symbolic Objects and Their Symbolized Meanings DISSERTATION of the University of St. Gallen, School of Management, Economics, Law, Social Sciences and International Affairs to obtain the title of Doctor of Philosophy in Management submitted by Philipp Scharfenberger from Germany Approved on the application of Prof. Dr. Torsten Tomczak and Prof. Dr. Andreas Herrmann Dissertation no. 4237 Rosch-Buch, Schesslitz 2013 The University of St. Gallen, School of Management, Economics, Law, Social Sciences and International Affairs hereby consents to the printing of the present dissertation, without hereby expressing any opinion on the views herein expressed. St. Gallen, October 21, 2013 The President: Prof. Dr. Thomas Bieger III Acknowledgments In your hands, you hold the graspable outcome of my work on this dissertation. It seems intriguing to me how this work began with just a few incidental thoughts and how it evolved from those through many days of reading, observing, thinking, and discussing; finally resulting in this particular sequence of words that, in their material state, to a certain extent, objectify this period of my life. Without exaggeration I can say that – at least knowingly – the last years of working on this thesis have been the most enlightening, formative, and intense years of my life so far. I have experienced it as a particular privilege to live in a surrounding and to have access to resources that have made it possible to work on this topic in the way that I have. There are certain people that I would like to thank in this regard. First and foremost, I would like to thank my supervisor Prof. Dr. Torsten Tomczak. He has been an extraordinarily supportive, as well as a critical and creative guide throughout the entire dissertation process. I am very thankful for his impressive capability to see at what times and steps I needed support and in which phases I could work autonomously. As a result, I had the possibility to work freely on this research while still being guided and never feeling lost. Without his thoughtful supervision, this dissertation would not have developed as it did. Likewise, I would like to thank my cosupervisor Prof. Dr. Andreas Herrmann for his support throughout recent years and for his helpful thoughts and suggestions, which enhanced this dissertation. Furthermore, I would like to thank Prof. Dr. Daniel Wentzel who has been – and still is – a very important mentor to me. His “painful” questions, in combination with his invaluable academic and personal advice, have been essential for this dissertation and my development. Similarly, I would like to thank Prof. Dr. Luk Warlop for his offer to cooperate and for his helpful input, especially on the conducted experiments. I would also like to use this opportunity to thank Prof. Dr. Christian Belz for his indefatigable efforts to motivate his students to develop good ideas and think differently. Thank you also to Prof. Dr. Sven Henkel for being such an openhearted, outspoken, and creative supporter and companion in recent years. In a similar vein, I would like to thank Dr. Kai Kruthoff for his amicable cooperation on many enthralling industry projects. Furthermore, I would like to express my gratitude to Veronika Hauser, Arlette Niedermann, and Dr. Klaus Edel for their support in all kinds of matters, and for the helpful steadiness that they brought to our prospering and therefore ever-changing department. IV I would like to thank Suleiman Aryobsei for being such a great “vis-à-vis” colleague and such a trusty and kindhearted pal throughout recent years. Also, I would like to thank Dr. Christian Purucker who became an important colleague and friend to me in the early phase of my dissertation and who will remain an inseparable part of my memories of that time. Similar is true for Dr. Klemens Knöferle and Dr. Benjamin von Walter, whom I would like to thank for all the great research related and non-research related discussions, which contributed to this dissertation. Thank you also to Dr. Christian Hildebrand – it is a great pleasure to work with someone who is so remarkably energetic, unconventional, and inspiring. Further, I would like to thank Dr. Dennis Vogt with whom I have cooperated in various industry projects, resulting in many valuable memories and experiences. In addition to these individuals, I would like to thank the entire team of the Center for Customer Insight for the pleasant and supportive working environment that they have created during my years in St. Gallen. Moreover, I would like to thank my family and my friends for their emotional as well as intellectual support during the last years. I am also very thankful for their understanding and courtesy with regard to my lack of time, particularly in the final phase of my dissertation. I would like to thank Sebastian Bebiolka, Judith Benner, Cem Erguel, Dr. Matthias Exner and Dr. Ralph Patocka, Tamara, Martin, and Dr. Andreas Fey, Max Gärtner, Verena Görtler, Stefanie Greb and Arndt Neckermann, Tim and Melissa Kaltenbach, Ute and Detlev Khatchikian, Dr. Christiana Merkl, Eva, Heiko, and Hannah Schaefer, Christina, Tobias, Jonas, and Adreana Scharfenberger, Prof. Dr. Christian Schmitz, Fred Schulz, Jens Schuster, Oskar Schwarz and Sophie Gaussiran-Racine, Oliver and Nadja Sequenz, Dr. Janice Spiess, Moritz Szelzki, Michael Wielan, Gözde Yalazi Özbek, as well as Anna, Judith, Samuel, and Daniel Zschätzsch. In addition, I would like to thank my grandparents, Gerhard and Margot Scharfenberger, as well as Mathilde and Dr. Walter Khatchikian. Finally, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my colleague and friend Dr. des. Miriam van Tilburg, to my sister Carolina, and my parents, Brigitte and Thomas Scharfenberger. They were always there for me at every stage of this journey and gave me the necessary emotional backing and stability to concentrate on this thesis. Furthermore, they managed to remind me that there are other important dimensions to life besides scientific work. For their tremendous support, in every sense, I thank them from the very bottom of my heart. St. Gallen, in December of 2013 Philipp Scharfenberger V Abstract Humans tend to own material objects that relate to certain self-defining meanings. They wear clothes that suit their personalities. They own furniture that embodies their lifestyles. They keep souvenirs of vacations they like to remember, and they attach to pictures of moments and people that compose essential parts of their selves. A vast amount of research has dealt with this self-extending nature of possessions. Little theoretical knowledge, however, exists concerning the question of how the physical graspability of possessions relates to their self-extension function. The essential aim of this dissertation is to approach this question. Regarding this goal, a review of previous research on the self-extending and symbolic meaning of objects is complemented by findings from construal-level theory. The combination of these perspectives allows for a precision of potential components and psychological processes that underlie the self-extension phenomenon. Based on this theoretical foundation, a framework is developed that relates subjects’ physical distance to an object to their perceived distance and self-extension towards that object’s symbolized meaning. Five experimental studies support the presumption that physical proximity to a symbolic object reduces subjects’ perceived distance and increases their perceived self-extension towards the object’s meaning. Furthermore, findings indicate that this affiliation also positively transfers into subjects’ behavior towards the symbolized meaning. Overall, the results support the notion that the physical graspability of economic goods exerts an essential self-defining use to consumers and a relevant impact on their behavior. This notion is particularly emphasized against a prevailing tendency of research and management to focus on immaterial components and utilities of economic offerings. Regarding management, this dissertation hence promotes an object-dominant logic that accentuates the relevance of the graspability of products and services. Concerning consumer theory, this dissertation proposes a revised understanding of the extended self concept that accounts for the graspability of possessions. In doing so, the author aims at providing management with a contracyclical and stimulating perspective on the management of products and services. Furthermore, the author intends to contribute to a more precise theoretical understanding of why humans possess and consequently why and how they consume. VI Zusammenfassung Der Besitz und das Selbstempfinden von Menschen sind eng miteinander verbunden. Menschen hüllen sich in Kleidung, die ihre Persönlichkeit widerspiegelt. Sie besitzen Einrichtungsgegenstände, die ihren Lebensstil verkörpern. Sie behalten Andenken von prägenden Reisen und umgeben sich mit Fotos von Momenten und Personen, die einen wichtigen Bestandteil ihres Selbst ausmachen. Diese selbsterweiternde Funktion von Besitz bildet einen grundlegenden Untersuchungsgegenstand der Konsum- und Marketingforschung. Welche Relevanz im Speziellen der physischen Greifbarkeit für diese Funktion von Besitzgegenständen zukommt, bleibt indes weitgehend unerklärt. Dieser Fragestellung widmet sich die vorliegende Dissertation. Aufbauend auf ihr wird eine Analyse bestehender Literatur zur selbsterweiternden und symbolischen Bedeutung von Besitz- und Konsumgegenständen durchgeführt. Die Erkenntnisse aus dieser Analyse werden mit Annahmen der Construal-Level Theorie verknüpft. Die Verbindung dieser Theoriestränge erlaubt eine Präzisierung von potentiellen Einflussfaktoren und psychologischen Prozessen, die der Selbsterweiterungsfunktion von Besitzgegenständen zugrunde liegen. Fünf Experimente stützen die Annahme, dass die physische Nähe zu einem greifbaren Objekt die empfundene Distanz zu dessen Bedeutungsgehalt reduziert sowie die empfundene Selbstverbundenheit zu ihm erhöht. Die Studien zeigen zudem, dass sich diese Selbsterweiterung positiv auf das Verhalten gegenüber dem Bedeutungsgehalt auswirken kann. Die Ergebnisse stützen die Annahme, dass die physische Greifbarkeit von ökonomischen Leistungen eine verhaltensrelevante Nutzendimension für Konsumenten darstellt. Besonders hervorgehoben wird dieser Standpunkt vor dem Hintergrund einer scheinbar abnehmenden Berücksichtigung dieser Nutzendimension in aktuellen Konsum- und Marketingkonzepten. Im Hinblick auf die Unternehmenspraxis entwickelt diese Arbeit daher eine Object-Dominant Logic, die die Relevanz der Greifbarkeit von Produkten und Dienstleistungen hervorhebt. Hinsichtlich der Theorieentwicklung schlägt diese Arbeit ein überarbeitetes Selbsterweiterungskonzept vor, das den spezifischen Einfluss der Greifbarkeit von Besitz berücksichtigt. Damit beabsichtigt der Autor einerseits, einen ideengebenden Perspektivwechsel im Management von Produkten und Dienstleistungen anzuregen. Andererseits intendiert die vorliegende Dissertation eine Erweiterung des theoretischen Verständnisses davon, warum Menschen besitzen und schliesslich warum und wie sie konsumieren. VII Table of Contents Abstract .......................................................................................................................... V Zusammenfassung......................................................................................................... VI Table of Contents ......................................................................................................... VII List of Figures ................................................................................................................ X List of Tables ................................................................................................................ XI List of Abbreviations ................................................................................................... XII 1 Introduction............................................................................................................. 1 2 Theoretical Foundation and Conceptual Framework ......................................... 6 2.1 Possessions and the Extended Self ..................................................................... 6 2.1.1 2.1.2 2.1.3 2.2 Objects as Graspable Symbols of Ungraspable Meaning ................................ 16 2.2.1 2.2.2 2.2.3 2.2.4 2.3 The Notion of Possessions as Self-Extensions............................................. 6 The Role of Materiality in Extended Self Research..................................... 8 A General Tendency Towards Immateriality? ........................................... 13 A Definition and Elaboration of Objects and their Properties ................... 16 Objects as Symbols .................................................................................... 22 The Relationship between Symbolic Objects and Their Meaning ............. 24 Symbolic Objects and Perceived Reality and Self ..................................... 27 Construal-Level Theory as an Approach to Conceptualize the Influence of Graspability on the Self-Extension Function of Symbolic Objects ............ 29 2.3.1 2.3.2 2.3.3 2.3.4 2.3.5 2.3.6 An Introduction to Construal-Level Theory .............................................. 29 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Perceived Distance to a Symbolized Meaning............................................................ 31 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Perceived Self-Extension towards a Symbolized Meaning ........................................ 33 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Behavior towards a Symbolized Meaning ................................................................. 33 The Moderating Influence of the Symbolic Connection between Object and Meaning ..................................................................... 34 Overall Conceptual Model ......................................................................... 36 VIII 3 Experimental Analyses ......................................................................................... 37 3.1 3.2 Overview of Empirical Approach .................................................................... 37 The Wedding Ring Experiment (Study 1) ........................................................ 41 3.2.1 3.2.2 3.2.3 3.2.4 3.2.5 3.2.6 3.2.7 3.3 The Mug Experiment (Study 2)........................................................................ 45 3.3.1 3.3.2 3.3.3 3.3.4 3.3.5 3.3.6 3.3.7 3.4 Overview .................................................................................................... 52 Participants and Procedure ......................................................................... 53 Manipulation of Independent Variables ..................................................... 55 Pretesting of Stimuli ................................................................................... 58 Operationalization of Dependent Variables ............................................... 60 Control Variables ....................................................................................... 61 Results ........................................................................................................ 61 Discussion .................................................................................................. 71 The Club Card Experiment (Study 4)............................................................... 74 3.5.1 3.5.2 3.5.3 3.5.4 3.5.5 3.5.6 3.5.7 3.6 Overview .................................................................................................... 45 Participants and Procedure ......................................................................... 46 Manipulation of Independent Variable ...................................................... 47 Operationalization of Dependent Variable................................................. 48 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables ............................................. 49 Results ........................................................................................................ 49 Discussion .................................................................................................. 50 The Tennis Ball Experiment (Study 3)............................................................. 52 3.4.1 3.4.2 3.4.3 3.4.4 3.4.5 3.4.6 3.4.7 3.4.8 3.5 Overview .................................................................................................... 41 Participants and Procedure ......................................................................... 41 Manipulation of Independent Variable ...................................................... 42 Operationalization of Dependent Variable................................................. 42 Control Variables ....................................................................................... 43 Results ........................................................................................................ 43 Discussion .................................................................................................. 44 Overview .................................................................................................... 74 Participants and Procedure ......................................................................... 75 Manipulation of Independent Variable ...................................................... 76 Operationalization of Dependent Variables ............................................... 79 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables ............................................. 81 Results ........................................................................................................ 81 Discussion .................................................................................................. 87 The Product- versus Service-Brands Experiment (Study 5) ............................ 89 3.6.1 3.6.2 Overview .................................................................................................... 89 Participants and Procedure ......................................................................... 90 IX 3.6.3 3.6.4 3.6.5 3.6.6 3.6.7 4 General Discussion................................................................................................ 96 4.1 4.2 Overall Findings ............................................................................................... 96 Theoretical Contribution .................................................................................. 99 4.2.1 4.2.2 4.2.3 4.3 4.4 4.5 Contribution to Extended Self Research .................................................... 99 Contribution to Research on Symbolic Consumption.............................. 101 Contribution to Construal-Level Theory .................................................. 102 Managerial Contribution ................................................................................ 103 4.3.1 4.3.2 4.3.3 (R)Evolving towards an Object-Dominant Logic .................................... 103 Exploiting the Self-Defining Meaning of Objects ................................... 104 Managing the Meaning of Objects ........................................................... 105 Limitations ...................................................................................................... 106 Future Research .............................................................................................. 107 4.5.1 4.5.2 4.5.3 5 Manipulation of Independent Variable ...................................................... 90 Operationalization of Dependent Variable................................................. 91 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables ............................................. 91 Results ........................................................................................................ 92 Discussion .................................................................................................. 93 Future Research on Objects...................................................................... 108 Future Research on Subjects .................................................................... 109 Future Research on Meanings .................................................................. 110 Conclusion ........................................................................................................... 112 References ................................................................................................................... 113 X List of Figures Figure 1-1: Figure 1-2: Figure 2-1: Figure 2-2: Figure 2-3: Figure 2-4: Figure 2-5: Figure 3-1: Figure 3-2: Figure 3-3: Figure 3-4: Figure 3-5: Figure 3-6: Figure 3-7: Figure 3-8: Figure 3-9: Figure 3-10: Figure 3-11: Figure 3-12: Figure 3-13: Figure 3-14: Figure 3-15: Figure 3-16: Figure 3-17: Figure 3-18: Figure 3-19: Figure 3-20: Figure 3-21: Figure 3-22: Figure 3-23: Figure 3-24: Figure 3-25: Figure 3-26: Illustration of Mind-Body Connection by Descartes (1662) .................... 1 Structure of Dissertation ........................................................................... 5 The Notion of Possessions as Self-Extensions ......................................... 7 Preliminary Study / Results / Sizes of Object Images ............................ 19 The Perceived Objectiveness of Objects as a Function of Object Size .. 20 The Reality- and Self-Construing Function of Symbolic Objects .......... 28 Conceptual Model of Dissertation .......................................................... 36 Study 1 / Design / Procedure of Experiment .......................................... 42 Study 1 / Results / Perceived Distance to Wedding................................ 43 Study 2 / Design / Picture of Utilized Mug............................................. 45 Study 2 / Design / Procedure of Experiment .......................................... 47 Study 2 / Results / Perceived Distance to Personal Experience ............. 50 Study 3 / Design / Procedure of Experiment .......................................... 54 Study 3 / Design / Depiction Wimbledon ............................................... 55 Study 3 / Design / Pictures of Utilized Tennis Balls .............................. 57 Study 3 / Pretest / Manipulation Check .................................................. 59 Study 3 / Results / Perceived Distance to Wimbledon ........................... 62 Study 3 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Wimbledon.................. 64 Study 3 / Results / Moderated Mediation Analysis ................................ 66 Study 3 / Results / Intention to Visit Wimbledon ................................... 68 Study 3 / Results / Serial Multiple Mediation Analyses ......................... 70 Study 4 / Design / Procedure of Experiment .......................................... 76 Study 4 / Design / Portrait Marketing Club ............................................ 77 Study 4 / Design / Picture of Utilized Membership Card ....................... 78 Study 4 / Design / Picture of Utilized Membership Voucher ................. 79 Study 4 / Design / Logo of Marketing Club St. Gallen .......................... 79 Study 4 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Marketing Club ........... 82 Study 4 / Results / Intention to Become a Club Member ....................... 83 Study 4 / Results / Mediation Analysis ................................................... 84 Study 4 / Results / Willingness to Pay for Membership ......................... 85 Study 4 / Results / Number of Specified Email Addresses..................... 86 Study 5 / Design / Procedure of Experiment .......................................... 90 Study 5 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Brands ......................... 93 XI List of Tables Table 2-1: Table 3-1: Table 3-2: Table 3-3: Literature Overview .................................................................................. 12 Overview of Conducted Experiments ....................................................... 40 Study 3 / Results / Indirect Effects – Moderated Mediation ..................... 67 Study 3 / Results / Indirect Effects – Serial Multiple Mediation .............. 71 XII List of Abbreviations ANOVA Analysis of variance ANCOVA Analysis of covariance BC Before Christ CI Confidence interval cm Centimeter e.g. Exempli gratia (for example) etc. Et cetera (and the rest) et al. Et alii (and others) f. Folio (following page) ff. Folio (following pages) H Hypothesis ibid Ibidem (in the same place) i.e. Id est (that is) LLCI Lower limit of confidence interval LSD Least significant difference t-test M Mean N Total sample size p Probability value p. Page pp. Pages r Pearson correlation coefficient SB Spearman-Brown coefficient SE Standard error UPCI Upper limit of confidence interval α Cronbach’s alpha ß Unstandardized regression coefficient 1 1 Introduction A unifying aim of the humanities lies in the basic goal to understand the essence of human being and behavior (Dilthey 1883). Disciplines within the humanities such as philosophy, sociology, psychology, economics, and finally consumer research can accordingly be understood as different perspectives of this goal. While the humanities hence focus on humans as beings with a consciousness that comprises a meta-physical understanding of existence, natural sciences focus on a more material conceptualization of humans and their surroundings. In contrast to the humanities, it is hence the goal of natural sciences to understand and explain the rules of this physical matter.1 A fundamental and lasting question that has arisen from this particular dualistic structuring of sciences is the question of how the physical body and surroundings of humans relate to their meta-physical understanding of being. This so-called mind-body problem has most famously been addresses by René Descartes (1641; 1662; see illustration below). It comprises the question of where within the human being consciousness and a sense of self exist and how they connect to the human body and its physical surrounding. Figure 1-1: Illustration of Mind-Body Connection by Descartes (1662) 1 The author uses this traditional and contrasting perspective of the humanities and natural sciences according to Wilhelm Dilthey’s (1883, p. 13 ff.; 1910, p. 5 ff.) conceptualization of the German term Geisteswissenschaften, knowing that there are possible other categorizations of the mentioned scientific disciplines. This dualistic comparison is chosen as it is essential for the formulation of the here described mind-body problem and reflects similar conceptualizations of various researchers who have approached this or related problems: see Descartes’ (1641) description of the mind-body problem above, Jasper’s (1954) idea of the “subject-object-split”, Cassam’s (1997) differentiation between object and subject, or Locke’s (1690) structuring of sciences as reported in Chapter 2.2. For a further discussion of this conceptualization and its relation to this dissertation, see Chapter 2.2. 2 A related question has concerned consumer research in the last four decades. It is the question of how belongings that consumers surround themselves with relate to their sense of self. How, for example, does a watch, a car, a house, a phone, a piece of art, or an heirloom that somebody owns influence his or her sense and perception of self? This question of which personal function possessions have for their owner’s identity and which relationship consumers accordingly create to their possessions is especially important for consumer research, as it sets a fundamental basis for understanding why humans possess and consequently why and how they consume (Belk 1988). According to this importance, a large number of researchers have examined the relationship between possessions and the self (see section 2.1.1). The majority of this research has approached this field from a sociological and anthropological perspective implementing mainly conceptual and qualitative research methodologies. This research, however, has disregarded the particular question of what importance the graspability of possessions has for their self-defining function. Literature in many cases suggests material characteristics of possessions by using expressions such as “material” or “physical” objects. At the same time, researchers use examples of possessions such as ideas, experiences, or relationships that undermine the actual relevance of a possession’s materiality for its self-extending function (see section 2.1.2). If possessions however actually have the function of manifesting a sense of self to their owners, it can be argued that concrete and directly experienceable possessions should serve this function better than possessions that themselves are just as abstract as their owners’ personalities. A glance into everyday life underlines and refines this notion: as mentioned above, humans tend to declare a wide range of things as their belongings that per se are intangible and abstract. They, for example, express ownership by describing personally important experiences as my vacation, my wedding, or my last concert visit. The same is true for other abstract possessions such as my religious belief, my job, my ideas, as well as relationships to my favorite band or my favorite soccer team. Interestingly, humans however are inclined to materialize these abstract belongings by objectifying them through graspable, symbolic objects – thereby transferring them from their immaterial, mental state into the “real” directly experienceable world. Vacations, for example, are tangibilized by bringing home souvenirs or pictures; relationships are sealed by wearing friendship bracelets during childhood and wedding rings during adulthood; a concert visit is materialized by retaining the entrance card; a religious belief by wearing a necklace with a cross; an abstract job through a simple business card; relationships to a band by owning a 12” record of their first album; 3 and relationships to favorite sports teams by wearing merchandising products such as jerseys and scarves. Further expanding this idea, even classic products such as sneakers or watches can be understood as symbolic objectifications of their brands and thus as vehicles to physically connect these brands to their owners. This everyday evidence suggests that the physical graspability of the things that someone uses to define his identity is crucial to the ability of these possessions to serve this function. Building upon this argumentation, the following research question is defined as a guideline for this dissertation thesis: Research Question: How does the graspability of symbolic objects relate to and influence human selfextension towards abstract meanings? In addressing this question, the present dissertation aims at two main goals: (1) the development of a revised conceptual understanding of the self-extension function of possessions that accounts for their graspability; (2) the creation and analysis of empirical data that is suitable to investigate the relevance of the graspability of symbolic objects for their self-extension function. Based on this focus, the present research is relevant for consumer research and marketing management in various ways: the project first contributes to the theoretical understanding of the relationship between possessions and consumer’s selves. It therefore aims at further developing this fundamental theoretical field within consumer research. Related to this, it thereby also refers to the broader theoretical question of the connection between mind, body, and physical surrounding and intends to gain further knowledge on their interdependence. Moreover, as previously outlined, a wide range of research in the concerned context is based on conceptual or qualitative research methods. The research at hand, in contrast, applies an experimental approach towards the observed phenomenon. By doing so, it not only fosters an alternative perspective of the matter of interest; it further aims at contributing to a stronger methodical diversity in its investigation and hence to a more thorough capturing of its nature (see section 3.1). In addition to these scientific contributions, the project aims at creating helpful knowledge and ideas for companies and their management of products and services. Recent years have shown socio-economic as well as technological developments that support a general dematerialization of products themselves as well as an emphasis of 4 immaterial product attributes by research and management (see section 2.1.3). Technological developments lead to a digitalization of entertainment and information media: records and movies as well as books or photo albums that previously were physically present in bookshelves are now abstractly stored on hard drives and servers. Simultaneously, socio-economic developments such as more flexible lifestyles support a dematerialization of products and services: in a time in which consumers constantly move from one job and one place to another, bulky possessions such as houses, cars, or heavy record collections are perceived as increasingly limiting (ibid). These current developments challenge companies to adequately react and exert influence where possible: should companies, for example, support flexible consumption habits such as renting or sharing? And if so, should they position themselves as providers of graspable products or less material services? Which of these two aspects should they emphasize in their communications? Should they dematerialize their offerings where possible? Or should they stick to graspable components in their portfolio; and if yes, which components? Economically as well as ecologically, it seems perfectly rational to dematerialize products and services – not only does this support a decrease of production costs; it also lives up to the necessity for a more careful usage of resources. But how does this dematerialization influence consumers’ incorporation of offerings into their self-concepts and thus their attachment to them? Gaining knowledge concerning these aspects is crucial from a managerial perspective. Not only does this dissertation aim at approaching these questions, it further aims at fostering their conscious and thorough discussion in research and management. Regarding these aims, the dissertation at hand is divided into five main chapters. This introductory chapter is followed by the theoretical approach to the outlined research question. The theoretical discussion is initiated with an overview of previous research on possessions and the extended self as well as related research fields. This literature overview particularly concentrates on the role of the materiality of possessions and economic goods in existing consumer and marketing research. The second section develops an understanding of graspable objects and their symbolic connection to meanings, which is essential for this dissertation project. Section three of the theoretical chapter introduces construal-level theory. As will be outlined, this theory provides a promising approach for the conceptualization of the influence of graspability onto the self-extension function of objects. Building upon this theoretical basis, concrete hypotheses are developed that are joined to the overall conceptual model of this dissertation. 5 In Chapter 3, five experimental analyses are documented and interpreted that were conducted to test the theoretically derived hypotheses. In Chapter 4, the results are consolidated and discussed against the theoretical framework of the thesis. Based on this interpretation of the results, practical and theoretical implications are formulated. Further, the limitations of the conducted studies are discussed. Based on these and the findings, future possible research directions are outlined. Chapter 5 finishes this dissertation with a summarizing conclusion. Chapter 1: Introduction Problem orientation of dissertation and formulation of research question (pp. 1-5) Chapter 2: Theoretical Foundation and Conceptual Framework Review of related research, development of conceptual framework, formulation of hypotheses (pp. 6-36) Chapter 3: Experimental Analyses Documentation and discussion of five experimental studies (pp. 37-95) Chapter 4: General Discussion Detailed discussion of the overall findings, implications, limitations, and directions for future research (pp. 96-111) Chapter 5: Conclusion Closing overview of research project (p. 112) Figure 1-2: Structure of Dissertation 6 2 Theoretical Foundation and Conceptual Framework This second chapter forms the theoretical foundation of this dissertation. Building upon a discussion of the relevant literature its aim is to create the conceptual framework for this research project. As regards this aim, the following section will discuss previous research on possessions and the extended self with a particular focus on the materiality of possessions (section 2.1). Subsequent, a foundational understanding of symbolic objects will be derived in section 2.2. Section 2.3 will introduce construal-level theory as a connecting element between the extended self concept and the developed understanding of symbolic objects. The sum of these considerations will finally flow into the development of concrete research hypotheses (see sections 2.3.2-2.3.6) that set the fundament for the empirical analyses that are outlined in Chapter 3. 2.1 Possessions and the Extended Self 2.1.1 The Notion of Possessions as Self-Extensions “Our fragile sense of self needs support, and this we get by having and possessing things because, to a large degree, we are what we have and possess.” Tuan 1980; citied by Belk 1988, p. 139 A large amount of research within consumer research and marketing has investigated the relationship between consumers and their possessions. Especially Russell Belk has influenced the idea of possessions as extensions of their owners’ selves in the last 40 years (see Belk 1978-2013). The relevance of this research field for consumer research as well as the importance of his fundamental article “Possessions and the Extended Self” (Belk 1988) is shown by the fact that this article is one of the most cited articles in consumer research2. A possible reason for this popularity is given by Belk himself, as he explains in the first sentence of this article: “We cannot hope to understand consumer behavior without first gaining some understanding of the meaning that consumers attach to possessions” (Belk 1988, p. 140). 2 The Journal of Consumer Research (JCR) that published this article from Russell Belk in 1988 is one of the most renowned journals within the field of consumer research. Today (2013), and hence 25 years after its publication, it still lists this article as the second most cited JCR articles of the last three years on its website. 7 To Belk possessions are a “major contributor to and a reflection of [their owners’] identities” (Belk 1988, p. 139). The self therefore not only comprises “that which is seen as ‘me’ but also that which is seen as ‘mine’” (ibid). Similar understandings of self-extending possessions are shared by researchers such as Prelinger (1959), Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Holton (1981), Wallendorf and Arnould (1988), Kleine, Kleine, and Allen (1995), Habermas (1999), Sirgy (1982), and many more. Wallendorf and Arnould (1988) highlight not only the self-orientated usage of possessions as extended self but also their social meaning: “We use objects to convey and extend our self-concepts to others as well as to demonstrate the self-concept to ourselves” (p. 531). Berger and Heath (2007) further emphasize this social (or as they term it, identity signaling) function of possessions and product domains (see also Richins 1994; Escalas and Bettman 2005; Derbaix, Decrop, and Cabossart 2002). Common between all these understandings of possessions is the idea that possessions symbolically condense a certain personal and social meaning that is of importance for the self-perception of a person as well as his desired perception within his social surrounding. Figure 2-1 visualizes this self-reflecting and manifesting function of possessions. Figure 2-1: The Notion of Possessions as Self-Extensions 8 Because of this self-extending function of possessions, individuals create an attachment to the possessions that manifest their personality (Belk 1989; Schultz, Kleine, and Kernan 1989). Attachment can thereby be understood as the “degree of linkage perceived by an individual between him / her self and a particular object” (Schultz et al. 1989, p. 360). This bond to a possession can become so strong that a loss of a possession is perceived as a loss of self (Wallendorf and Arnould 1988). 2.1.2 The Role of Materiality in Extended Self Research Despite an increasing interest in the relevance of possessions’ physical properties, research on the extended self has so far not precisely distinguished between material and immaterial possessions. This unclearness might be best displayed by two prominent quotes from Belk himself. On the one hand, Belk (1990, p. 669) explains, “The notion of the extended self suggests that we transcend the immediate confines of our bodies by incorporating into our identities, objects from our physical environment.” On the other hand, he states “We may summarize the major categories of extended self as body, internal processes, ideas, and experiences, and those persons, places, and things, to which one feels attached” (Belk 1988, p. 141; see also Belk 2013, p. 477 f.). According to Belk, the physical existence of possessions therefore seems to be of some relevance for their self-extending function (as depicted in the first quote). However, it seems to be of no deeper conceptual meaning, as possessions such as internal processes, ideas, and experiences are generally not material / physically graspable but attributed to the concept of self extension (in the second quote). This ambiguity in the perception of possessions permeates most of the influential work in this field of possessions and the self. Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton (1981) – although including mainly tangible possessions into their investigation of “the meaning of things” – define these things as “any bit of information that has a recognizable identity in consciousness, a pattern that has enough coherence, or internal order, to evoke a consistent image or label” (p. 14). In line with this definition, they point out that “In talking about objects, we shall not be concerned with their chemical composition, their mass, or their weight” (p. 13). Berger and Heath (2007) also extend their understanding of identity signaling products from very tangible product domains such as cars and stereos to more intangible domains such as hobbies, music genres, and sitcoms. A similar openness concerning the definition of possessions is found in the work of Richins (1994) and Mehta and Belk (1991). In contrast, Wallendorf and Arnould (1988) as well as Kleine et al. (1995) follow a very clear understanding of material and hence physically graspable possessions. Moreover, Belk, Wallendorf, and 9 Sherry (1989, p. 11) actually discuss tangible and intangible things as domains of sacred consumption. They further outline concepts such as objectification (p. 7), symbolic transformation of objects (p. 14), and tangibilized contamination (p. 30). However, just as the previously mentioned researchers, they neither address the particular issue and specific psychological meaning of the materiality and graspability of these things for self-extension processes nor have these considerations been incorporated in later revisions of the extended self concept (Belk 2013). A possible cause for this impreciseness seems to be that a major part of research on the relationship between possessions and consumers is strongly influenced by sociological and anthropological research streams. Therefore, the investigated research questions and implemented, mainly qualitative, methodologies aim at conceptualizing a more global understanding of the contemplated phenomenon – contrary to looking at particular physio-psychological aspects such as the concrete materiality of possessions. Another rather pragmatic reason seems to be that the distinction between material and immaterial possessions and components of the self is difficult and has therefore been avoided in past conceptualization. Concerning these difficulties and the prevailing unclearness, Belk just recently stated “Given the difficulties in separating mind and body in philosophies and psychologies of the self, objects in all of these categories [see the above mentioned categories] will be treated as parts of the extended self” (Belk 2013, p. 478). Despite this vague conceptualization of possessions, literature does reflect efforts to account for the spatial / physical references of self and the things that people account to their selves. Already the term extended self associates to the idea that the self is something that can be extended and thus has a physical reference. This closely connects to the conceptualization of the self by William James (1890), which is fundamental for a large proportion of extended self research (Belk 1988; Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981; Habermas 1999). James uses expressions such as the outline or the expansion of self in his discussion of the consciousness of self (James 1890, p. 312 f.; see also Habermas 1999, p. 60 f.). He even tries to identify the spatial center of the self and describes it to be located somewhere in the motions that he feels “in the head or between the head and the throat” (James 1890, p. 301) – a finding that however has been widely questioned (Habermas 1999, p. 60 f.). Prelinger (1959) further expands this search for the physical localization of the self. He has subjects sort 160 items according to their perception of the items as being part of the self. The items thereby reflect the following eight 10 categories of things3: body parts, psychological and physiological processes, personal characteristics, possessions, abstract ideas, other people, close objects, as well as the distant physical environment. Prelinger (1959) concludes that amongst other criteria physical proximity influences the perception of a thing’s belongingness to the self (see also Habermas 1999, p. 61; Belk 1988, p. 140). In line with this argumentation, Belk (1988, 1990) highlights the particular meaning of “contamination” for self-extension processes. Belk argues that bodily contact to an object may not only lead to an increased feeling of control and ownership towards the object (Belk 1988, p. 151). He further elaborates that objects that have been contaminated by a person also incorporate parts of that person – such as that secondhand clothing, for example, is associated with being connected to its previous owner (Dehling and Vernette 2013). Therefore, contamination not only connects objects to the touching person but also connects the person to the touched object. Grayson and Shulman (2000) further emphasize this notion of contamination or, in other words, the physical connection between an object and its meaning (see also Newman, Diesenbruck, and Bloom 2011; Grayson and Martinec 2004). They build upon Peirce’s semiotic theory to explain the process in which meaning is attached to possessions. According to them, possessions become particularly important to their owners when they are factually connected to a personally important experience and accordingly serve as evidence for that part of the self (Grayson and Shulman 2000, p. 19; see also section 2.2.3). This closely relates to Csikszentmihalyi and RochbergHalton’s (1981) as well as Belk’s notion of possessions’ functions “in constructing and maintaining a sense of past” (Belk 1990, p. 669 emphasis added4; see also Belk 1988; Love and Sheldon 1998). As mentioned, some of these approaches account for a physical connection of possessions to their symbolic meaning. However, none of these approaches knowingly addresses the particular relevance of a possession’s physical graspability for the self-extension process. Where the concrete materiality of possessions has been investigated it has been contemplated in a more sensory context such as affective reactions to touch or the quality-signaling relevance of haptic information (Peck and Childers 2003; Peck and Wiggins 2006; Krishna and Morrin 2008; Sonneveld and Schifferstein 2008). Peck and Shu (2009) have further been able to show that touching an object can increase 3 4 Relating to the results of Prelinger’s findings, the shown categories are sorted in decreasing order as being perceived as part of the self. The term constructing that Belk uses in this context relates to this dissertation’s reference to construal-level theory to explain the psychological processes that underlie the self-extension phenomenon (see Chapter 2.3). 11 perceived ownership and therefore fosters a personal connection to an object. Their findings hence support Belk’s notion of the contamination-effect and further refine findings in the field of the endowment effect (see also Kahneman, Knetch, and Thaler 1990; Pierce, Kostova, and Dirks 2003). Although accounting for the physical properties of possessions, these studies do not investigate the particular relevance of the materiality of possessions for their ability to self-extend. The relevance of the materiality of possessions for self-extension processes has however gained increasing attention in recent years apropos the growing amount of digital possessions such as digital music, books, photos, videos, and written documents (Belk 2013; Siddiqui and Turley 2006; Lehdonvirta 2010). These developments have led to a more sensitive view on the material aspects of self-extension and consumption in general. Siddiqui and Turley (2006) argue that a dematerialization of digital possessions leads to increased feelings of uncertainty as well as less emotional and more functional attachments to possessions. Magaudda (2011) denotes that digitalization does not actually result in dematerialization. According to her, current developments towards digital consumption just lead to a shift in relevant objects: objects such as CDs or tapes are replaced by objects such as iPods. Other objects such as vinyl records have “remained the central focus of interest of many music collectors” (Magaudda 2011, p. 28). However, in line with the previously mentioned authors, also these authors stay unclear in their particular understanding of material possessions. Above that, their findings stay fragmentary as well as vague regarding their contribution to consumer theory. The mentioned studies identify similarities and differences between tangible and intangible possessions (Siddiqui and Turley 2006; Belk 2013). A converging understanding of the relevance of the physical graspability for possessions’ self-extending function is however missing. This missing conceptual clarity is not least manifested in Belk’s recent revision of the “extended self in a digital world” and his summarizing finding that the “attachment to […] virtual possessions, [is] almost, but not quite the same” (Belk 2013, p. 478). This recent revision of Belk’s extended self concept (ibid) can also be seen in line with prevailing efforts in consumer and marketing research to refine current perspectives on consumption. In addition to the outlined digitalization of consumption, present trends such as product sharing as well as new and more flexible (consumption) lifestyles motivate researchers to develop new concepts that meet these changing, “postmodern” consumption patterns. Bardhi, Eckhardt, and Arnould (2012) recently introduced the concept of “liquid relationships to possessions”. Partially related to this concept, Vargo and Lusch introduced the “service-dominant logic” in 2004. These concepts are 12 of particular relevance for the here investigated research question because they share and facilitate the notion of a consumption dematerialization. Due to this approach, which to some extent opposes the here developed understanding of the relevance of graspable possessions, these currently emerging concepts will be closer discussed in the next section. Summarizing this section, five major fields within the broader context of extended self research can be differentiated that relate to the materiality of possessions and consumption: (1) research in the main field of possessions and the extended self; (2) research on symbolic consumption as regards the symbolic meaning of economic goods; (3) research on touch and ownership; (4) research on digital consumption; as well as (5) concepts on postmodern consumption. This last research field will be brought into closer context of this dissertation in the following section. Table 2-1 summarizes the highlighted five research streams. Research Field Research Focus and Reference to Materiality Methodology Exemplary Literature Possessions and Self ‐ Literature concentrates on broad conceptualizations of the relationships between consumers and their possessions Conceptual / Qualitative Belk (1985, 1988); Belk et al. (1989); Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton (1981); Kleine et al. (1995); Wallendorf and Arnould (1988); Habermas (1999) Experimental / Conceptual Grayson and Shulman (2000); Newman et al. (2011); Richins (1994); Berger and Heath (2007); Escalas and Bettmann (2005); Belk (1988) Experimental Peck and Childers (2003); Peck and Wiggins (2006); Peck and Wiggins (2011); Peck and Shu (2009); Krishna and Morrin (2008); Conceptual / Qualitative Belk (2013); Magaudda (2013); Siddiqui and Turley (2006); Styvén (2010); Hongladarom (2011) Conceptual / Qualitative Bardhi et al. (2012); Bardhi and Eckhardt (2012); Belk (2010); Vargo and Lusch (2004); Tuli et al. (2007) ‐ Implicitly this research builds upon a classic understanding of material possessions that are owned by consumers ‐ Explicitly this research however stays vague in its conceptualization of possessions and unspecific about the relevance of materiality for self-extension processes Symbolic Consumption ‐ Literature concentrates on the symbolic meaning and value of possessions and economic goods ‐ Implicitly this research stream builds upon a classic understanding of material goods ‐ Literature however stays inexplicit about the particular relevance of materiality for consumption processes Touch and Ownership ‐ Research investigates the effect of haptic properties of objects on consumers’ attitudes and behavior ‐ Research also looks at the effect of touch onto perceived ownership ‐ Research however does not investigate the particular relevance of the graspability of possessions for their selfextension function Digital Consumption ‐ Literature concentrates on consumption of digital products ‐ Although research particularly approaches the relevance of the materiality of products it stays vague in its definition of product materiality ‐ Results in this field stay fragmentary and disconnected Postmodern Consumption ‐ Literature develops new understandings of the relationships between consumers and economic goods ‐ This understanding is particularly influenced by new consumption practices and lifestyles such as digital consumption and sharing ‐ Research suggests a decrease of the importance of physical possessions and ownership Table 2-1: Literature Overview 13 2.1.3 A General Tendency Towards Immateriality? “Things are disappearing right before our eyes” (Belk 2013, p. 478). This notion that Belk formulates in his recent revision of the extended self concept can be understood not only literally as a description of a continuing dematerialization of digitalized possessions. Above that, it can also be understood symptomatically as a description of how consumer and marketing research presently changes its perspective of economic goods and services (Tuli, Kohli, and Bharadwaj 2007). In contrast to the classic understanding of material possessions and goods, recent consumer research tends to foster a more immaterial perspective on consumption practices. Research even goes so far as to emphasize the particular irrelevance of material possessions or even negative effects of material possessions for consumption practices. Bardhi et al. (2012) argue that in a time in which consumers increasingly engage in nomadic lifestyles “attachment to things becomes problematic because possessions tether one to particular locales both physically and symbolically” (p. 511). They accordingly show that well-situated individuals with flexible lifestyles tend to detach from material possessions and form so-called liquid relationships with their physical surroundings. This liquid understanding of the relationship that consumers have with possessions seems to be partially influenced by the continuing dematerialization of information media such as books, CDs, movies, pictures, etc. (Belk 2013; Siddiqui and Turley 2006; Magaudda 2011; Styvén 2010; Lehdonvirta 2010). More importantly, however, this notion seems to be related to an assumed detachment of consumers from their material possessions. Recent research on consumer behavior continuously emphasizes that consumers have an increasingly utilitarian perspective of goods that results in alternative consumption practices such as sharing or less solid and more liquid relationships to possessions. According to Bardhi and Eckhardt, “ownership is no longer the ultimate expression of consumer desire (…). Instead of buying and owning things, consumers want to access the goods and prefer to pay for the experience of temporarily accessing them” (2000, p. 881; emphasis added; see also Bardhi et al. 2012). This tendency to devalue the relevance of the physical and emotional bond between consumers and their possessions is not new. It is following a related but outdated discussion of marketing research apropos services. In her article “Breaking Free from Product Marketing”, G. Lynn Shostack already emphasized in 1977 (p. 73) the necessity of “making room for intangibility” in marketing management and research. 14 In a similar vein, Kotler notes that “the importance of physical products lies not so much in owning them as in obtaining the services they render” (1977, p. 8). These argumentations set the reference frame for the so-called service-dominant logic that Vargo and Lusch introduce in 2004 and which sets the present peak of a dematerialized view on economic offerings in marketing research. According to Vargo and Lusch, this approach creates a “reoriented philosophy” (2004, p. 2) that views all offerings, including material goods, as services that provide intangible uses to their owners. However, even this dematerialized notion of economic goods can be related to a much older and broader socio-economic discourse on dematerialization: relating to Belk’s citation in the beginning of this section, Marx and Engels already critically described capitalism in 1848 as a system in which “all that is solid melts into air” (Marx and Engels 1848, p. 476). Since this time their thoughts on the dematerialization of the economic system have set a fruitful breeding ground for a broad discussion of the socio-economic consequences of a continuing abstraction of the relationship between humans and the outcome of their labor (Bauman 2007; Weber 1920; Habermas 1985). A crucial and constitutional part of the above outlined discourse is the relationship between immateriality and uncertainty or, in other words, the interrelation between physical environment and psychological sentiment of people. Marx’s theory of alienation describes the idea of how human self-awareness (being) is disturbed when the output of human labor (doing) becomes increasingly abstract and detached from the worker (Marx 1844, p. 53 ff.; 1872, p. 402 ff.). In a similar vein, Bauman (2007) distinguishes current liquid times from the preceding solid modernity as an age of increasing mobility and flexibility as well as an “age of uncertainty”. In his related discussion of the culture of speed, Tomlinson states (2007, p. 91) that “immediacy […] is better understood as an essentially ambiguous condition rather than a pellucid narrative”. Likewise, a large proportion of research on services copes with the problem of uncertainty: the prevailing notion is that the immateriality of service results in increased feelings of risk and insecurity for service consumers (Murray and Schlacter 1990; Tarn 2005, p. 749). Similarly but from the opposite perspective, Rindfleisch, Burroughs, and Wong (2009) argue that materialistic consumers cope with uncertainty by attaching to material possessions (see also Belk 1985; Chang and Arkin 2002). Some of the above outlined anxieties might simply derive from a popular and exaggerated aversion against changes (Tomlinson 2007, p. 91 f.). Still, the lasting impression is that literature draws a link between materiality, solidity, and feelings of security as well as immateriality, unsteadiness, and feelings of insecurity. These 15 different perspectives of humans and their surroundings hence do not only emphasize negative consequences of missing material reference points for consumers’ sentiments. The discussion about the interconnection between materialism and insecurity further highlights the impreciseness of (consumer) research with regard to the general understanding of materiality / materialism: the term materialism in its original meaning clearly refers to the material state of goods. In its ontological meaning, it still refers to the centrality of physical matter for all worldly phenomena (Philosophie 1999; Oxford Dictionaries 2013). The economic understanding of materialism however has mainly detached from this physical reference and today more generally refers to an attitude and ideal in which consumption takes a central role in a person’s life (Rindfleisch et al. 2009, p. 2; Chang and Arkin 2002, p. 389; Belk 1985, p. 265). Therefore, similar to the unclear conceptualization of possessions (see section 2.1.2) and in line with the above outlined dematerialization process, the concept of materialism has also detached from its original reference to the physical world. It can accordingly be argued that parallel to the current process of focusing on immaterial aspects of consumption, research is also becoming less clear about the physical aspects of it. According to this unclearness, consumer and marketing research is thus running the risk of overlooking this possible driver of consumer behavior. The current trend in consumer research to emphasize immaterial aspects of consumption might accordingly derive either from an actual decline of the importance of physical aspects of consumption or it might result from a distorted perspective of and consequently a misled interpretation of current consumer behavior. At the beginning of this chapter, the importance of research in the field of possessions and the self was highlighted by stating that “we cannot hope to understand consumer behavior without first gaining some understanding of the meanings that consumers attach to possessions” (Belk 1988, p. 139). Referring to the discussion above, this statement shall be revised by adding that we cannot hope to understand the meaning that consumers attach to possessions without first understanding the relevance of their physical existence. In approach to this research gap and the underlying research question of this dissertation, section 2.2 will address a definition of objects as well as a clearer elaboration of their material properties. 16 2.2 Objects as Graspable Symbols of Ungraspable Meaning 2.2.1 A Definition and Elaboration of Objects and their Properties The word “object” originates from the Latin word objectum, which describes the idea of a “thing presented to the mind” (Oxford Dictionaries 2013). In its English meaning, it is today defined as “a material thing that can be seen and touched” or as “a thing external to the thinking mind or subject” (ibid). It hence creates a contrast to the word subject, which is understood as “a person or thing that is being discussed, described, or dealt with”; and in its philosophical understanding “as a thinking or feeling entity; the conscious mind; the ego; especially as opposed to anything external to the mind” (ibid). This dualistic relationship between object and subject closely corresponds to the previously outlined distinction between the humanities and natural sciences as well as the described mind-body problem in the introductory chapter of this dissertation: the subject describing a “thinking or feeling entity”, “a conscious mind”, which is mainly investigated by the humanities; opposed to something objective, often material outside of the mind, which is of particular interest to natural sciences. Obviously, the terms object and subject also relate to the scientific concepts of objectivity and subjectivity – the latter being understood as the personal- and perception-dependent notion of something in contrast to objectivity in its meaning as a personal- and perceptionindependent understanding of something that as this is “external to the thinking mind or subject”. Accordingly, the word object connects the notion of objectivity with its everyday meaning of “a material thing that can be seen and touched”. Concerning this connection, Csikszentmihalyi notes (cited from Belk 1988, p. 148, emphasis added) that “[…] the objects we possess and consume are […] wanted because […] they tell us things about ourselves that we need to hear in order to keep ourselves from falling apart. This information includes the social recognition that follows upon the display of status symbols, but it includes also the much more private feedback provided by special household objects that objectify a person’s past, present, and future, as well as his or her close relationships.” This considered quality of objects to objectify something abstract is linguistically also related to the meaning of the word “to grasp” – which, on the one hand, can be understood as physically encompassing something, as well as mentally comprehending 17 something, on the other hand5 (Oxford Dictionaries 2013). The connection between this physical and mental dimension of grasping is found and described in various contexts regarding human consciousness. Bowlby (1969) and White (1959) describe how the interaction with physical objects others than one’s own body facilitates an infant’s ability to perceive himself as something different from his surrounding and therefore leads to the creation of self-awareness. Reversely, as mentioned previously, Marx argues that the worker’s separation from the physical outcome of his labor leads to a social alienation and henceforth a loss of self-awareness (Marx 1844, p. 53 ff.; 1872, p. 402 ff.). Summarizing his philosophical discourse on the interrelation between bodily and mental self-awareness, Cassam (1997, p. 198) states: “The […] self-consciousness, and so the unity of consciousness, is intimately bound up with awareness of the subject ‘as an object’ – not as an ‘immaterial’ substance but a physical object in a world of physical objects.” Accordingly, there is some evidence for the relevance of objective, physical reference points for human’s development of a sense of self or, in other words, for human’s ability to grasp their selves. In line with this argumentation and contrary to the majority of existing research in the field of consumer research, the focus of the definition of an object (and of possessions) shall here lie on the perceived ability to physically grasp the object6. The perceived ability to grasp the object is thereby distinguished from the mere physical existence of an object or the ability to touch an object (Kaufman, Mareschal, and Johnson 2003, p. 517 f.). The following examples highlight the crucial difference: a molecule (e.g. an O2 molecule) has a physical existence and might be touched; however, due to its size it is not consciously graspable for humans. Hence, from a human perspective, it only restrictively fulfills the notion of an object. The same is true for the opposite – meaning very large entities. Belk gives examples of possessions such as a mountain that somebody climbs or a subway system that somebody knows well and therefore perceives as a part of his self (Belk 1988, p. 150). Both the mountain and the subway system have a physical existence and can be physically touched, but due to their size, it is hardly possible to encompass or, in other words, grasp them. This crucial differentiation between the notion of touching and grasping is similarly true for mental constructs: mentally touching a thought implies a contemplation of this thought; however, it also implies that the thought is not understood in its whole meaning. 5 6 The same is true for the German word Begreifen. The view on objects and their connection to the self is also controversially discussed in philosophy. As previously mentioned, this discourse closely relates to the mind-body problem and therefore to the question where the self exists and what importance the physical body and surrounding of a person play for its constitution. This interesting discussion that would overstrain and go beyond the scope of this dissertation is well documented in Cassam (1997). 18 Hence, just as the idea of grasping with regard to a mental object includes the notion of actually understanding its essence, the idea of grasping in a physical sense includes the idea of actually encompassing the object. As for the molecule example, as well as for the mountain or the subway system, the perception of something as an object, the perception of its “objectiveness”, accordingly seems to depend on the ability to grasp fully this entity (Csikszentmihalyi and Robinson 1990, p. 29). An O2 molecule can be touched but the inability to grasp it both physically and mentally restricts its perception as an object or, in other words, reduces its objectiveness. Only in the moment in which the air molecule is mentally imagined as a model in a graspable size does it becomes imagined as objective. Similarly, the earth can be touched. However, from a human perspective it only becomes graspable and therefore objective if it is mentally reduced to a graspable model (e.g. a globe). In a similar vein, Aristotle defines the limits of art appreciation in the size of objects and the relationship of that size to the body and perceptual abilities of humans: “Besides, a beautiful object, whether an animal or anything else with a structure of parts, should have not only its parts ordered but also an appropriate magnitude: beauty consists in magnitude and order, which is why there could not be a beautiful animal which was either minuscule (as contemplation of it, occurring is an almost imperceptible moment, has no distinctness) or gigantic (as contemplation of it has no cohesion, but those who contemplate it lose a sense of unity and wholeness) […].” (Aristotle 384-322 BC, p. 55) The objects that consumers specify when asked to name their most cherished possessions underline this importance of graspability (see Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981; Ahuvia 2005; Habermas 1999, p. 19). Often mentioned objects such as visual art, sculptures, musical instruments, TVs, stereos, radios, books, photos, plates, silverware, clocks, tools, sports equipment, trophies, cameras, toys, clothes, and jewelry can not only be grasped but many of them are actually created with the intention of being grasped. Further support for the notion of graspability as a crucial object property derives from the results of a preliminary study that was conducted in this context. The goal of this study was to estimate subjects’ perceived sizes of mental images of different objects. The hypothesis was that – detached from the original size of an object – its mental image would have a size that is easily physically and hence also mentally graspable. 96 participants (52% female; mean age: 34 years) in an online study estimated the size 19 of their mental image for one out of three different sized objects: an atom, a soccer ball, and earth.7 Figure 2-2 shows the mean values for estimated sizes of mental object images in each condition. Figure 2-2: Preliminary Study / Results / Sizes of Object Images Results revealed that the mean sizes of mental images varied between 7.27 cm for an atom, 19.31 cm for a soccer ball, and 25.08 cm for earth. 82% of the mentioned sizes were between 1 and 30 cm. Further, object type (atom, soccer ball, earth) was a significant predictor of indicated image sizes (F(2, 77) = 10.70, p < .01). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) revealed a significant difference between image sizes of an atom and a soccer ball (t(77) = 3.20, p < .01) as well as between an atom and the earth (t(77) = 4.43, p < .01). No significant difference was identified between image sizes for the soccer ball and the earth (t(77) = 1.41, p = .16). Results indicate that mental images can differ depending on the actual size of the imagined object. However, results further indicate that preferred image sizes mainly 7 Participants were asked to imagine the object and consider what it looks like (could look like in the atom condition). They were then asked to close their eyes and estimate the size of the object that appeared to them. Subjects then specified the estimated size in their own words. No scale or other reference point was given to subjects to limit the risk of biasing answers. A total of 16 participants was excluded from the analysis: seven participants were excluded because their description of object size was not interpretable; seven participants were excluded because their description of object size clearly referred to the actual size of the objects and not the size of their mental images; two participants were excluded because their answers strongly diverged from the answers of other participants. Subjects were retrieved from an online consumer panel and received $0.75 for participation in the study. 20 vary between 1 and 30 centimeters and hence in a range that is not only mentally but also physically easily graspable. The results accordingly support the notion that mental images of objects correspond to the perceptual abilities and therefore physical properties of humans. Tying the definition of objects to the subjectively perceived ability to physically grasp an object thus results in a prototypical size of an object, which is related to the bodily properties and perceptual abilities of humans – namely the size of the human body or, in other words, the size of an object that a person is able to physically and mentally grasp. Building upon the findings of the above reported study, this size is here defined to be in a range of approximately 1 to 30 centimeters. Further, as argued above, the perceived objectiveness of an entity should decrease with a change of this prototypical size towards an infinitely small or large size. Figure 2-3 depicts this relationship between an object’s physical size and its subjectively perceived objectiveness. Figure 2-3: The Perceived Objectiveness of Objects as a Function of Object Size 21 Summarizing the argumentation above, an object shall here be defined as an entity that is perceived to be physically graspable.8 This notion thereby highlights a further property of objects that is relevant for their conceptualization in this dissertation. The above stated definitions of objects all refer to an object as a thing. This implies that objects have “enough coherence, or internal order, to evoke a consistent image or label” (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981, p. 14). This again implies that objects can be differentiated from other objects, that they have a borderline that defines them as a stable unit (Cassam 1997, pp. 2-3; Kaufman et al. 2003, p. 518; Locke 1690, pp. 66-69). This further relates to the argumentation above in that the size of a unit and its graspability have an influence on the perceived objectiveness of an object. Very small entities such as the mentioned molecule fall below a human’s perceptual abilities and hence it is impossible to separate these entities from other things. Similarly, hiking on a large mountain reduces the perception of that mountain as an object because it becomes part of the surrounding environment and the lack of perceived borders makes it difficult to differentiate the mountain from other environmental fragments. Looking at the mountain from a more distant perspective, however, enables its perceiver to recognize its defining borders. This distant point of view accordingly enhances the subject’s perception of the mountain as an object. Therefore, the notion of the graspability of an object also highlights the idea of an object to have a clear, defining borderline that separates it from other objects or entities. In this understanding, the definition of an object shows some parallels to the semiotic understanding of symbols. These conceptual connections shall be discussed in the next section. 8 This here derived understanding of objects does not attempt to question the existence and relevance of material entities that are inexperienceable / ungraspable for humans. As regards the underlying research question of this dissertation and as argued above, it is however claimed that the here defined group of objects relates to entities that are perceived as particularly object-like / objective from human perspective. 22 2.2.2 Objects as Symbols “ALL that can fall within the compass of Humane Understanding, being either, First, The Nature of Things, as they are in themselves, their Relations, and their manner of Operation: Or, Secondly, That which Man himself ought to do, as a rational and voluntary Agent, for the Attainment of any End, especially Happiness: or, Thirdly, The ways and means, whereby the Knowledge of both the one and the other of these, are attained and communicated; I think, Science may be divided properly into these Three sorts.” Locke (1690, p. 462) Relating to the dichotomic description of the humanities and natural sciences in the introductory chapter of this dissertation, the above depicted citation expresses John Locke’s notion that semiotics can be understood as a third academic field that extends beyond both of the other fields and connects them. This idea has similarly been emphasized by researchers in the field of consumer behavior that have investigated the meaning of things (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981; Belk et al. 1989; Habermas 1999; Grayson and Shulman 2000). A relationship between the concepts object and symbol9 is not least found in the origin and developed meaning of both words. The word “symbol” derives from the Greek word sumbolon / symbolon, which can be translated as “mark” or “token” (Oxford Dictionaries 2013). It is derived from the Greek words sym (with) and ballein (to throw), which in this combination can be translated as “to throw together” or “to join” (ibid). According to Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton (1981, p. 40), this Greek meaning refers to the idea of a coin or another object that two friends break into two pieces when separating from each other. The joining of the two pieces when meeting again symbolizes the friendship between the two individuals. “Thus symbol originally meant that which brings people together” (ibid). Not only does the origin of the word symbol accordingly directly refer to a physical objectification of an 9 The term symbol is here consciously preferred over the term sign. This is done apropos the ambivalent and unclear differentiation of both terms in semiotics as well as due to the more common usage of the term symbol in consumer research and other non-semiotic disciplines (Nöth 2000, p. 178; Hjelmslev 1943, p. 113). Furthermore, as shown hereafter, the term symbol stronger emphasizes the here underlying idea of symbols as material objects. Finally, the term symbol (in its Peircian understanding; not in its Saussurian understanding) also better refers to the conceptual understanding of the mental connection between objects and their meaning as it is developed in this chapter. 23 intangible meaning per se, it further relates to the meaning of an object in such a way that an objectification leads to a more objective status of the signified (here the friendship) or, in other words, an interpersonal similar (objective) perception of the signified. It therefore, also in this sense “brings people together” (ibid). The English word “symbol” is defined as “a mark or character used as a conventional representation of an object, function, or process” or more generally as “a thing that represents or stands for something else, especially a material object representing something abstract” (Oxford Dictionaries 2013). Later definition actually highlights the material constitution of symbols. Both definitions however share the idea of a symbol as a sign of something signified (Nöth 2000, p. 178). Relating to the previous conceptualization of objects, the symbol therefore also makes something graspable that is difficult to grasp or handle per se (ibid, p. 180; Saussure 1916, p. 78; Peirce 1903, pp. 64-66). The symbolic word “dog”, for example, eases the communication about a dog, not only because it would be difficult to alternatively refer to a dog in this moment; but also because it represents a concept of a dog that is detached from one particular dog. Relating to this argument, Locke states: “MAN, though he have great variety of Thoughts, and such, from which others as well as himself might receive Profit and Delight; yet they are all within his own Breast, invisible and hidden from others, nor can of themselves be made to appear. […] The use then of Words, is to be sensible Marks of Ideas; and the Ideas they stand for, are their proper and immediate Signification.” (Locke 1690, p. 256) The symbol accordingly makes something directly accessible that is not accessible per se – perhaps because it is too far away, too small, too large, already past, still to come, or an abstract concept that is otherwise “invisible and hidden” in one’s mind (ibid). This quality of making something inaccessible directly accessible is perceived as essential for the understanding of symbols in this dissertation. A symbol however does not only have this representative quality. It also has a structuring impact (Eco 2002, p. 86; Hjelmslev 1943, p. 52 f.; Whorf 1956, p. 7 ff.): the meaning of the word “dog”, for example, is not only created by our experience of what a dog is, it also defines what we perceive as a dog and how we distinguish it from other animals and things. Transferred onto possessions, one can similarly argue that the meaning of a wedding ring is created, on the one hand, by the particular relationship between two married people and all the associated meanings and feelings 24 that connect to this relationship. On the other hand, the wedding ring thereby also becomes a constitutional part of the particular relationship type “marriage” and consequently is a defining part of its signified meaning. As such, symbols are a constituting and structuring element of our perception of reality (ibid; see also section 2.2.4). Finally, some semioticians (including Charles S. Peirce) go so far as to interpret all worldly phenomena as signs (Nöth 2000, p. 62; pp. 131-135): the ocean might be associated with freedom, rain with sadness, and the color red with danger or love. Not only does this broad perspective of semiotics further highlight the notion that the division of reality by signs into “graspable” units fundamentally influences society’s perception of reality (Eco 2002, p. 86; Hjelmslev 1943, p. 52 f.; Whorf 1956, p. 7 ff.), it moreover underlines the general human tendency to ascribe meanings to their physical surroundings. Based on this notion, it can be argued that by using physical phenomena and objects as symbols for meanings, humans manage to create an environment that satisfies their need for a meaningful reality, which nature itself does not provide (see also Belk et al. 1989).10 Building upon this understanding of symbols, symbolic objects shall henceforth be defined as graspable entities with an associated meaning. Symbolic objects hence comprise a meaning layer that is mentally added to their physical existence. The following section will further discuss the constitution of relationships between symbolic objects and their meanings. 2.2.3 The Relationship between Symbolic Objects and Their Meaning Two of the most influential semiotic models that have previously been introduced and utilized within consumer research to conceptualize the relationships between possessions and their meaning originate from Ferdinand de Saussure and Charles S. Peirce (Grayson and Shulman 2000; Grayson and Martinec 2004). As outlined by Grayson and Shulman (2000, p. 17 f.), these models provide a fruitful fundament for the analysis of the meaning of things. Building upon their argumentation, the next section will revise this transfer and subsequently develop its own understanding of the 10 This notion also connects to the differentiation between natural sciences, the humanities, and semiotics by Locke at the beginning of this section: natural sciences as the field that investigates the nature of things; the humanities as the field that investigates the human being and the attainment of any end, especially happiness; as well as semiotics as an instrument of communication that connects these two fields of human understanding. 25 relationship between symbolic objects and their meaning that is based on these two semiotic models. Saussure emphasizes that the connection between sign (signal) and its signified meaning (signification) is created entirely mentally11 (Saussure 1916, p. 76; Nöth 2000, p. 72 f.). According to him, there is no particular, predetermined relationship between the signal and the signification. Therefore, there is no reason to prefer one signal above another. For example, a dog might be called dog but it can also be called chien, perro, or hund and none of these signals is more suitable to signify the meant animal than the others do (Saussure 1916, p. 79). Similarly, a wedding ring is a wedding ring; however, societies could have also agreed on materializing marriages by a necklace or a tattoo. Consequently, to Saussure the meaning of signals is mainly arbitrary12. However, as Saussure also argues, once a sign has been conventionalized it cannot arbitrarily be alternated (Nöth 2000, p. 72). Contrary to Saussure, Pierce proposes a more differentiated concept of how signs and signified meanings relate to each other. Pierce determines three different qualities of these relationships. He distinguishes “iconic”, “symbolic”, and “indexical” relationships (Peirce 1903, p. 64 ff.; Nöth 2000, p. 62 ff.) Iconic signs relate to their signified meaning through a perceived sensual affinity. A small replica of the statue of liberty therefore signifies the original statue of liberty not only through a learned convention but also particularly through its figural similarity. Hence, the sensation that results from perceiving the signified is similar to the sensation one perceives from the sign (Peirce 1903, p. 64 f.; Nöth 2000, p. 65 f.; Grayson and Shulman 2000, p. 18). Peirce’s understanding of symbolic signs closely relates to Saussure’s understanding of signals. To Peirce symbolic signs do not provide a particular connection to their signified meaning. They are learned through personal experiences, rules and conventions (Peirce 1903, p. 65 f.; Nöth 2000, p. 65 f.; Grayson and Shulman 2000, p. 18). The above mentioned and explained example of the word “dog” reflects this type of sign. As illustrated, it is learned by language conventions. It has no particular 11 12 A problem that semioticians struggle with is the very inconsistent notation of concepts (see in this context Peirce’s argumentation about the ethics of terminology (Peirce 1903, p. 45)). Saussure understands signs as a superordinate concept and defines them as the combination of signal (here symbol) and its signification (here its meaning); see Saussure 1916, p.78 f. In this dissertation, however, the terms symbol, sign, and signal are used interchangeably. Saussure does concede that some signals have a particular predisposition to serve as signals for their signification. However, he argues that this aspect is inconsiderable (Saussure 1916, p. 80). 26 connection to its signified meaning, which is reflected by the fact that it varies between different language areas. Finally yet importantly, Peirce defines indexical signs as a third group of semiotic signs. Contrary to the previous described categories, the relationship between indices and their signified meaning is constituted by a factual connection between sign and signified (Peirce 1903, p. 65; Nöth 2000, p. 65 f.; Grayson and Shulman 2000, p. 18). According to Grayson and Shulman, the above-mentioned wedding ring is an example for such a sign. In line with their argumentation, the wedding ring is physically connected to the wedding that it represents. To its owner it signifies a factual evidence of the reality of this event. As such proof, it accordingly becomes irreplaceable to its owner – meaning that it cannot be replaced by another (even same looking) ring. Grayson and Shulman emphasize that the irreplaceability of these objects is due to “the factual, spatial connection with the special events and people they represent” (Grayson and Shulman 2000, p.19). Different to this implied assumption of Grayson and Shulman that indexical signs create a factual connection to their meaning, it is here argued that for indexical as well as for iconic and symbolic signs (and therefore for all signs) the relationship between the sign and the signified is wholly mental. This closely connects to Saussure’s understanding of signals (Saussure 1916, p. 76 f.). It further matches Peirce’s triadic idea of the relationship between signs, their signified meaning, and the “cognition produced in the mind” (Nöth 2000, p. 62; see also Peirce 1903, p. 64 ff.). Finally, it is also reflected in Grayson and Martinec’s later argumentation that “despite our belief that we perceive iconic or indexical signs ‘out there’ in the ‘real world,’ our perception of these signs are highly influenced by our personal predilections and perceptual imperfections” (Grayson and Martinec 2004, p. 299). Accordingly, the irreplaceability of a wedding ring is not a consequence of its factual, physical relationship to what it represents but a consequence of the fact that the owner projects this relationship onto the object. Hence, the owner would not care if the wedding ring were exchanged for an exact copy as long as he did not know. Therefore, similar to the Macintosh computer that symbolizes values such as creativity and differentness the wedding ring mentally symbolizes the relationship to a beloved person and all the events and feelings that are connected with this relationship. Further, it can be argued that the irreplaceability of the ring (or an object in general) is not a consequence of this perceived factual connection to its meaning per se. It can much more be ascribed to the uniqueness of this learned relationship between this particular physical symbol and this particular set of signified meanings (Newman and Bloom 2012, p. 560; Newman et al. 2011, 27 p. 216). Accordingly, should a couple exchange and wear four (instead of two) wedding rings for its wedding, not only would the personal importance of each wedding ring decrease; these rings would also be exchangeable between each other as they would symbolize the same set of meaning. Summarizing, it can be argued that the irreplaceability of an object is not a consequence of its particular physical connection to its signified meaning but a consequence of the uniqueness of the set of meanings that it symbolizes. Accordingly, in line with Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton (1981) as well as Habermas (1999), symbolic objects and possessions shall here be understood as symbols – that means, as objects that are mentally connected to their signified meaning. In line with Peirce, the differentiated semiotic relationships (iconic, symbolic, indexical)13 shall thereby be understood as different qualities of these mentally created relationships between object and meaning – however, neither of them shall be highlighted as particularly suitable to load symbolic objects with meaning. Further, as Peirce (1903, p. 64) and also Grayson and Shulman (2000, p. 18) highlight, these types of semiotic relationships are not conceptualized as mutually exclusive. It can even be argued that each of Peirce’s semiotic relations requires a predefined and therefore symbolic perception-system (Peirce 1903, p. 64; Eco 2002, p. 86; Hjelmslev 1943, p. 52 f.; Whorf 1956, p. 7 ff.). 2.2.4 Symbolic Objects and Perceived Reality and Self Independent of the controversial philosophical discussion of whether the symbolic objectification of an immaterial meaning is a part of reality14, it can be argued that from a subjective perspective this objectification clearly influences and hence is a part of reality. The popular placebo effect comprehensibly illustrates this notion: it is well known that simulated, medically ineffectual treatments can have psychological and social effects that again influence physiological processes (Hahn and Kleinman 1983). Burke interprets these bodily reactions as “a clear instance of the ways whereby the realm of symbolicity may affect the sheerly biologic motions of animality” (Burke 1966, p. 7). It can accordingly be argued that it is the mind that projects a certain immaterial meaning onto a material object and again it is the mind that conveys this immaterial 13 14 To differentiate between the overall understanding of symbols in this dissertation and the Peircian subcategory of symbols, the Peircian understanding will be denoted as “symbolic signs” from here on. See in this context, for example, Kant’s notion of the “thing-in-itself” or Plato’s “Allegory of the Cave”. 28 meaning into physical reactions and thus into the physical world (see also Saussure 1916, p. 14). Not only is the symbolic meaning of things hence a constitutional part of subjectively perceived reality. As such, it further becomes a potential antecedent of behavior and hence physical reality. Recapitulating section 2.2 and connecting it to the argumentation above, the following understanding of symbolic objects and their relation to subjective reality shall be retained: Humans tend to mentally attach meanings to the physical environment / objects that surround them. By doing so, they construct an environment of meaning. Symbolic objects are understood as entities that are perceived to be physically and hence mentally easy graspable and that are mentally associated with certain meanings. As such, symbolic objects do not only objectify certain meanings and make them directly accessible, they further become a structuring element of subjects’ mentally created reality. Refining figure 2-1 from section 2.1.1, figure 2-4 depicts this understanding of symbolic objects. Figure 2-4: The Reality- and Self-Construing Function of Symbolic Objects 29 Referring to the argumentation above, it can be further argued that by bodily relating to symbolic objects, subjects mentally relate their selves to the meaning that they project onto these objects. The relevance of the physical existence of symbolic objects is hence here not seen in their particular connections to a signified meaning (!) but in the perceived proximity that they create between their adherent, mentally created meaning and the body and mind of a subject (!). This proposition that the physical proximity (distance) of symbolic objects has an influence on subjects’ perceived distance and self-extension to the meaning of these objects constitutes the main hypothesis of this dissertation. By relating the argumentation of sections 2.1 and 2.2 to construal-level theory (and consequently to a psychologically based concept), this hypothesis shall further be substantiated in the following sections. 2.3 Construal-Level Theory as an Approach to Conceptualize the Influence of Graspability on the Self-Extension Function of Symbolic Objects 2.3.1 An Introduction to Construal-Level Theory The previous chapter outlined how symbolic objects objectify their abstract meanings. This next section will analyze the influence of this objectification process onto the subjective perception of intrinsically abstract meanings. The dissertation will therefore refer to construal-level theory. Construal-level theory not only shows a number of interesting parallels to the previous outlined concept of symbolic objects, it further offers a new perspective on objects and their meaning that, to the author’s knowledge, has not been investigated in the context of possessions and the extended self. Most importantly, construal-level theory provides a promising fundament for a more detailed psychological understanding of the meaning that graspability has for the selfextension function of possessions. The core idea of construal-level theory is to create a conceptual connection between the experienceability of entities, their mental construal, as well as the distance that subjects perceive to them. According to Liberman, Trope, and Stephan (2007), “people believe that they experience themselves and their immediate surrounding at the present moment” (Liberman et al. 2007, p. 353, emphasis added). Objects and events that are not present in the direct experience of reality are considered psychologically distant (ibid). As they cannot be experienced directly, they have to be mentally 30 construed and are hence thought of more abstractly (Trope, Liberman, and Wakslak 2007; Ross and Wilson 2002). Accordingly, things that are directly experienceable are mentally construed on a lower level than things that are not experienceable and therefore have to be imagined or, in other words, construed (Trope and Liberman 2010). Different to objective measures of distance, construal-level theory mainly refers to psychological / perceived distances. Perceived distance can be described as the subjective experience of the distance to an entity (Liberman et al. 2007, p. 353; Van Boven, Kane, McGraw, and Dale 2010, p. 872). Liberman et al. (2007, p. 353) differentiate between various forms of perceived distances: something might be perceived as distant because it is (or is believed to be) located in the past or the future (temporal distance), or at a remote location (spatial distance), because it refers to a foreign cultural surrounding (social distance), or because of its hypothetical or purely mental nature (hypotheticality). A large amount of research has shown influences of these types of perceived distances on mental construal. It has been shown, for example, that people think about events in the long-term future or long-term past more abstractly than if these events are scheduled in the close future or if these events have occurred in the close past (Semin and Smith 1999, Trope et al. 2007). Similarly, it has been illustrated that people think about other people in a more abstract manner if these people are believed to be remotely located than if they are believed to be at a close location (Fujita, Henderson, Eng, Trope, and Liberman 2006). Especially interesting for the current context is that studies referring to construal-level theory have shown that the influence of perceived distance on construal-level can be reversed: therefore, the level of construal of an entity also affects the perceived distance to that entity. Liberman, Trope, McCrea, and Sherman (2007), for example, show that low versus high level construals of a future event affect subjects’ perceived distance to that event: low level and consequently concrete descriptions of the event lead to perceptions of less distant enactment times of the event than high level construals. Accordingly, these studies generally indicate a relationship between the concreteness of the mental construction of an entity and its perceived distance. The concepts of construal-level and perceived distance hence closely relate to the previously outlined understanding of symbolic objects. In section 2.2 symbolic objects were defined as graspable entities that transfer their abstract, immaterial meaning into the concrete, physical reality. It was further accentuated that they therefore make this per se inaccessible meaning directly accessible (and hence experienceable). The combination of this previous developed understanding of symbolic objects and the 31 here outlined construal-level theory consequently support the development of various hypotheses that provide possible explanations for the particular influence of the graspability of objects for their self-extending function. These hypotheses shall be formulated in the following three sections. 2.3.2 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Perceived Distance to a Symbolized Meaning As discussed in section 2.2, it is argued that symbolic objects transfer their immaterial, abstract meaning into the direct experienceable, physical world. Referring to construallevel theory, it can further be argued that this objectification of a per se inexperienceable entity corresponds to a change in its mental construal and thus influences the perceived distance to the symbolized meaning. According to Liberman et al. (2007), a more concrete representation of an entity should hence reduce the perceived distance to that entity. Accordingly, the following hypothesis can be proposed: H1a: The perceived presence of a symbolic object reduces subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s symbolized abstract meaning (compared to a state in which no object is present). In section 2.2, it was further emphasized that symbolic objects are located physically in relation to the physical world and consequently create a physical relation between their adherent meaning and the body and mind of a subject. Building upon this argumentation, it can further be proposed that the perceived physical distance to a symbolic object influences the perceived distance to the object’s symbolized meaning. More specifically, a reduction of the physical distance between a subject and a symbolic object should also lead to a decrease of the subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s symbolized meaning. This proposition also defines a boundary condition of hypothesis 1a: symbolic objects therefore only reduce a subjects’ perceived distance to a symbolized meaning if they are located in physical proximity to the subject. If they are located distant to the subject, they lose their experienceability and become just as abstract and inexperienceable as their symbolized meaning. This argumentation also corresponds to given findings on subjects’ perception of loss of possessions. Findings indicate that the loss of possessions is in many cases also perceived as a loss of the symbolized meaning of that possession and “may bring about a diminishing sense of self” (Belk 1988, p. 142). Interestingly, losing a material possession, however, does not usually imply a disappearance of that object; it usually means that the object has 32 been displaced and cannot be found and / or reached anymore. It can accordingly be argued that the physical existence of the object has not changed – what has changed is the subject’s perceived distance to the object. Hence, the above-cited diminishing sense of self should not correspond to an analog dissolving of a physical possession, but to an increase of the perceived distance to the possession. In line with this argumentation, the following hypothesis can accordingly be formulated: H1b: Perceived physical proximity to a symbolic object reduces subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s symbolized abstract meaning (compared to a state in which the object is distant). Hypothesis 1b therefore not only redefines the effect of object presence onto the perceived distance to its meanings (H1a). By doing so, it also addresses a possible alternative explanation for this effect: referring to hypothesis 1a, it can alternatively be argued that perceived object presence might only evoke more vivid representations of its symbolic meaning independent of the physical existence of the object. A wedding ring, for example, might remind its owner of the exact moment in which he received the ring; a running shoe might evoke very concrete pictures of running; and a souvenir mug could remind the owner of the moment in which he was sitting in a café drinking coffee at a certain location during his vacation. Accordingly, the perceived reduction of distance to these associated meanings could also be caused by a more concrete representation of these meanings. Hypothesis 1b addresses this alternative explanation for hypothesis 1a as it relates the perceived distance of an abstract meaning to the physical distance to the object that symbolizes this meaning; for the outlined, alternative explanation the physical closeness to the object should be irrelevant. A confirmation of hypothesis 1b thus supports the notion that symbolic objects reduce the perceived distance to their meaning by physically transferring this meaning into the direct experienceable world; whereas a refusal of hypothesis 1b indicates an effect of symbolic objects solely through their property to evoke more vivid representations of their signified meaning. Hypotheses 1a and 1b are the main hypotheses of this dissertation. They propose influences of the physical presence / proximity of symbolic objects onto subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s symbolized meaning. This distance reducing influence of symbolic objects is perceived as foundational for the overall selfextending function of symbolic objects. The next section will more specifically relate the distance reducing function of objects to their self-extension function. 33 2.3.3 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Perceived Self-Extension towards a Symbolized Meaning Section 2.1.1 outlined how people use their possessions as extension of their selves or, in other words, how people incorporate their possessions into their sense of selves. As mentioned above, construal-level theory relates to this idea in that it suggests, “people believe that they experience themselves and their immediate surrounding at the present moment” (Liberman et al. 2007, p. 353, emphasis added). Hence, if people experience themselves in the present moment, then the things one surrounds himself with might be described as present reflections of the self or, in other words, as reference points for self-construal. It can accordingly be argued that symbolic objects do not only transfer abstract meanings into the direct experienceable world; it can further be proposed that through their physical proximity to a subject they bridge the subject’s perceived distance between the subject’s self and the meaning that is attached to the object. Building upon hypothesis 1b, it can accordingly be proposed that the reduction of perceived physical distance to a symbolic object leads to a decrease of a subject’s perceived distance to the symbolized meaning, which again leads to a stronger perceived self-extension (self-connection) of the subject towards the symbolized meaning. In other words, in can be postulated that the effect of an object’s physical proximity onto a subject’s perceived self-extension towards the object’s symbolic meaning is mediated by the perceived distance to the symbolized meaning (analog to H1b). Accordingly, hypotheses 2a and 2b are formulated: H2a: Perceived physical proximity to a symbolic object increases subjects’ perceived self-extension towards the object’s symbolized meaning (compared to a state in which the object is distant). H2b: The effect outlined in hypothesis 2a is mediated by the influence of perceived object proximity on subjects’ perceived distance to the symbolized meaning. 2.3.4 The Influence of Object Proximity on Subjects’ Behavior towards a Symbolized Meaning As mentioned in section 2.1.2, Peck and Shu (2009) have been able to show an effect of touching an object on subjects’ perceived ownership towards that object. They have further shown that this increase of perceived ownership can translate into higher valuations of the object. Their findings integrate into the broader theoretical concept of the endowment effect (Kahneman et al. 1990). Findings in the field of the endowment effect essentially show that legal as well as perceived ownership of an object leads to 34 stronger attachment to that object as well as higher loss aversions and higher valuations of that object (Peck and Shu 2009; Ariely, Huber, and Wertenbroch 2005). Peck and Wiggins (2006) have moreover shown that touching an object can influence behavioral intentions (such as the willingness to donate time or money) towards an institution that is symbolically connected to the object. However, they ascribe these effects to subjects’ affective reactions towards the object (ibid; see also Tom 2004). Referring to this literature stream, it is here argued that physical proximity to a symbolic object should enhance subjects’ behavior towards the object’s symbolized meaning. Different to Peck and Wiggins (2006), this effect is however accounted to the object’s ability to connect an abstract meaning to a subject’s self. More concrete, it is proposed that a decrease in physical distance to a symbolic object leads to a decrease in perceived distance to the symbolized meaning, which again strengthens subjects’ self-extension to that meaning, consequently enhancing behavior to that meaning. In other words, in can be postulated that the effect of an object’s physical proximity onto a subject’s behavior towards the object’s symbolic meaning is mediated by the perceived distance as well as self-extension towards the symbolized meaning (analog to H1b and H2b): H3a: Perceived physical proximity to a symbolic object increases subjects’ behavior (behavioral intentions) towards the object’s symbolized meaning (compared to a state in which the object is distant). H3b: The effect outlined in hypothesis 3a is mediated by the influence of perceived object proximity on subjects’ perceived distance and self-extension towards the symbolized meaning. 2.3.5 The Moderating Influence of the Symbolic Connection between Object and Meaning In the previous sections, it was argued that symbolic objects reduce the perceived distance to their symbolized meaning by transferring this abstract, inexperienceable meaning into the direct experienceable world. It was further argued that, by doing so, symbolic objects strengthen subjects’ perceived self-extension and therefore their behavior towards this symbolized meaning. According to section 2.2.4, this argumentation was developed from the premise that through its objectification the symbolic meaning becomes part of the subjective experienced reality and self. It can accordingly be argued that the above outlined effects of symbolic objects (H1-H3) should depend on the symbolic connection between an object and meaning. More 35 precisely, the reduction of the perceived distance to an abstract meaning through a symbolic object (H1a and H1b) should be stronger the stronger the object is symbolically associated to this meaning. In other words, the stronger an object is mentally associated with an abstract meaning the stronger it should transfer this meaning into the direct experienceable world, and hence the stronger should be its influence on the perceived distance to that meaning. Further, the symbolic connection between an object and a meaning should also influence the effect of the object on a subject’s perceived self-extension towards the meaning (H2a and H2b). Therefore, the postulated influence of object presence / proximity onto subjects’ perceived selfextension towards the object’s meaning should be stronger the stronger the object is mentally associated to that abstract meaning. Finally, this moderating effect should also account for the object’s effect on subjects’ behavior (behavioral intentions) towards the object’s meaning (H3a and H3b). Hence, the postulated influence of object presence / proximity on subjects’ behavior (behavioral intentions) towards the object’s meaning should be stronger the stronger the object is mentally associated with that abstract meaning. Refining the main hypotheses 1-3, the following hypotheses 4a, 4b, and 4c are formulated: H4a: An object’s influence on subjects’ perceived distance to an abstract meaning (H1a and H1b) is stronger the stronger the object’s symbolic connection is to the meaning. H4b: An object’s influence on subjects’ perceived self-extension towards an abstract meaning (H2a and H2b) is stronger the stronger the object’s symbolic connection is to the meaning. H4c: An object’s influence on subjects’ behavior (behavioral intentions) towards an abstract meaning (H3a and H3b) is stronger the stronger the object’s symbolic connection is to the meaning. Hypotheses 4a-4c are not only relevant for a more precise conceptualization of the previously outlined main effects. Moreover, they are crucial to rule out a further alternative explanation for these effects. Independent of the here outlined argumentation, a support for the main effects could also derive from a simple affective reaction of subjects to a close object per se (Peck and Wiggins 2006). Consequently, subjects may just feel closer to something they think of because of a close focal point that a proximal object provides them. Similarly, subjects’ perceived self-extension and behavior towards a certain meaning could be caused by such an affective reaction. Hypotheses 4a-4c are formulated to test and to control for these alternative 36 explanations of hypotheses 1-3. If the postulated effects of symbolic objects actually derive from their ability to transfer abstract meanings into the direct experienceable world then the symbolic connection between object and meaning should have an effect on this ability. The inclusion of the specified moderators therefore particularly serves a more precise testing of the here proposed conceptual framework. The previous sections defined the hypotheses that are essential for this thesis’ treatment of the defined research question. The next section will summarize the stated hypotheses and connect them to an overall conceptual model of this thesis. 2.3.6 Overall Conceptual Model In Chapter 1, the following research question was formulated as basis for this dissertation: How does the graspability of symbolic objects relate to and influence human self-extension towards abstract meanings? Building upon this question, Chapter 2 outlined the theoretical foundation and conceptual framework of this dissertation. Based on this fundament, a number of hypotheses were derived that provide possible conceptualizations of the investigated phenomenon. Figure 2-5 summarizes and visualizes these hypotheses (1-4) thereby depicting their overall interrelation. The following section of the dissertation will concentrate on the empirical investigation of this conceptual model. Figure 2-5: Conceptual Model of Dissertation 37 3 Experimental Analyses This chapter will outline the experimental analyses that were conducted to test the conceptual model of this dissertation. Five studies were conducted that will be drafted subsequently. The following section will give an introductory overview of the empirical approach and the studies. 3.1 Overview of Empirical Approach Based on the developed hypotheses, a sequence of five experiments was conducted. These experiments aimed to gather empirical evidence for the validity of the conceptual model of this dissertation. Experiments were chosen as the methodical approach of this dissertation for multiple purposes: first, experiments allow for testing of causality of postulated relationships (Field 2009, pp. 7-14). Secondly, the defined hypotheses are formulated specifically enough to be transformed into experimental research designs and thus allow for experimental testing. Thirdly, and most importantly, the psychological perspective of the investigated phenomenon supports an experimental testing of the hypotheses, as experiments are particularly popular in this discipline. This argument is reinforced through an aspect that was outlined in section 2.1.2: previous research on the meaning of possessions for self-extending processes has mostly been conducted from a sociological as well as anthropological perspective. Accordingly, this research has been strongly influenced by methodical approaches that are popular in these disciplines. Most of the research in this area has hence been qualitative and conceptual and has followed rather broad research questions and approaches of the investigated phenomenon (see table 2-1). Accordingly, and as highlighted in sections 2.1.2 and 2.1.3, the particular relevance of the graspability of possessions for self-extending processes has not been investigated as precisely as it is approached here. As experiments are particularly suitable to isolate and exhibit very specific effects that might be difficult or impossible to consciously articulate for individuals, they therefore constitute an appropriate methodical complement to these previous studies. The applied experimental approach hence not only relates to the underlying research question and theoretical fundament of this dissertation but also to the outlined research gap in the investigated research field. As mentioned above, the unifying aim of the conducted five experiments was to test the influence of the graspability of objects onto subjects’ perceived distance (H1a and H1b), self-extension (H2a and H2b), and behavior (H3a and H3b) towards the objects’ symbolized meanings. The studies were further designed to test the postulated 38 moderating effect of the symbolic connection between object and meaning (H4a, H4b, and H4c). Experiment 1 tested whether objects reduce the perceived distance to their symbolized, abstract meaning (H1a). It was the primary goal of study 1 to test this essential hypothesis under simple and realistic circumstances. Experiment 2 investigated this effect more elaborately by testing whether the physical distance to an object influences the perceived distance to its symbolized meaning in a controlled laboratory setting (H1b). Its main contribution was therefore its rigorous testing of this basic assumption. Experiment 3 tested whether the influence of object distance on the perceived distance to its symbolized meaning (H1b) is moderated by the strength of the symbolic connection between object and meaning (H4a). This study accordingly retested the effect of experiment 2, while including a further crucial factor into the study design, thus becoming more specific in its testing of the conceptual model. Further, experiment 3 also tested the proposed effects of symbolic objects onto subjects’ perceived self-extension (H2a) and behavioral intentions (H3a) towards the object’s symbolized meanings. It also enabled a testing of the postulated mediation and moderation effects concerning these dependent variables (H2b, H3b, H4b, and H4c). Hence, experiment 3 is the most comprehensive and complex experiment of this project as well as the most powerful in testing the overall model. Experiment 4 built upon the results of experiment 3 while returning to a simpler study design. The study tested whether objects with differently strong symbolic connections to a certain meaning differ in their influence on subjects’ self-extension (H2a and H4b) as well as behavioral intentions and actual behavior (H3a and H4c) towards the symbolized meaning. Experiment 4 thus shifted its aim from testing the precise psychological influences of symbolic objects towards a more realistic testing of their behavioral consequences and hence economic relevance. The main goal of experiment 5 was to generalize the findings of studies 1-4 in two ways: first, it examined whether the previous findings of the influence of symbolic objects on perceived self-extension towards an abstract meaning (H2a) could more generally be transferred onto graspable products and immaterial services and their associated brands. Secondly, by referring to a wide range of different products and service it transferred the analyses into a broader economic context. Table 3-1 summarizes the conducted experiments. To conduct the above outlined studies, it was necessary to find and choose objects and symbolized meanings that were suitable for experimental testing. It was decided to utilize different combinations of objects and symbolized meanings throughout the five studies to ensure that the measured effects were generalizable i.e. independent from 39 the used stimuli and study designs. The selection of objects was based on four criteria: first, the used objects had to match the conceptualizations of objects in section 2.2.1 – accordingly they had to be easily physically graspable with approximate sizes between 1 and 30 centimeters in diameter. Secondly, objects were chosen that were most likely well known to participating subjects – this was decided to prevent that physical proximity to the object would provide any not previously known information to subjects (see Peck and Shu 2009, p. 437). Thirdly, used objects had to be integrated easily and economically into experimental study designs. Fourthly, objects had to be suitable to being connected to a particular abstract meaning, which itself had to be suitable for being implemented in the studies. Vice versa, the selection of meanings was mainly based on the premise that people could refer to these meanings and that they could easily be connected to accessible objects. Table 3-1 shows the chosen pairs of used objects and symbolized meanings. The experiments used a mixture of laboratory, online, and survey designs. Laboratory experiments were particularly conducted to test the effects of object presence / proximity under as controlled circumstances as possible. Online and survey designs were applied complementary to test the assumed hypothesis under less artificial circumstances and therefore to generalize the investigated effects. Similar reasons determined sample characteristics of the conducted studies. Studies 2, 3, and 4 used student samples. As these studies mainly aimed at determining the psychological reactions and processes caused by symbolic objects they utilized homogenous samples in order to be more comparable regarding experimental manipulations (Calder, Phillips, and Tybout 1981, p. 200). Accordingly, and apropos their aim to generalize the tested effects, studies 1 and 5 utilized more heterogeneous consumer samples. Table 3-1 summarizes the designs of each of the five conducted experiments. The following sections will document the design and results of each of the studies in detail. 40 Overview of Conducted Experiments Study 1: The Wedding Ring Experiment Object-Meaning Pair Wedding Ring - Wedding Design 2 (Object Presence: Present / Not Present) Hypotheses Tested Hypothesis 1a Sample Sample of Married Individuals Pages 41-44 Study 2: The Mug Experiment Object-Meaning Pair Mug – Personal Experience Design 3 (Object Distance: Close and Touch / Close and No Touch / Distant and No Touch) Hypotheses Tested Hypothesis 1b Sample Student Sample Pages 45-52 Study 3: The Tennis Ball Experiment Object-Meaning Pair Design Tennis Ball – Tennis Tournament (Wimbledon) 2 (Object Distance: Close / Distant) x 3 (Symbolic Connection: No / Moderate / Strong) + 1 Control Condition (No Object) Hypotheses Tested Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 2a, 2b, 3a, 3b, 4a, 4b, 4c Sample Student Sample Pages 52-73 Study 4: The Club Card Experiment Object-Meaning Pair Membership Card – University Club Design 3 (Symbolic Connection: No Object / Weak / Strong) Hypotheses Tested Hypotheses 2a, 3a, 3b, 4b, 4c Sample Student Sample Pages 74-89 Study 5: The Product- versus Service-Brands Experiment Object-Meaning Pair Product / Services – Brands Design 2 (Object Presence: Present / Not Present) Hypotheses Tested Hypothesis 2a Sample Consumer Sample Pages 89-95 Table 3-1: Overview of Conducted Experiments 41 3.2 The Wedding Ring Experiment (Study 1) 3.2.1 Overview The purpose of study 1 was to test hypothesis 1a using a simple and realistic study design. The essential target was to create a study that referred to an object that subjects actually owned and which symbolized a meaning that subjects could relate to. Building upon this guideline, weddings were chosen as a self-defining but past event (i.e. one’s wedding is not directly experienceable anymore) and wedding rings as an object that is symbolically connected to that event. Specifically, it was assumed that participants would be able to refer to their own wedding and would be likely to carry their wedding ring with them, when participating in the study. Moreover, wedding rings fitted the understanding of graspable objects defined in this dissertation (see section 2.2.1). The study used a one-factorial design in which the extent to which the object (i.e. the ring) was consciously present was manipulated between subjects. It was predicted that participants would feel that their wedding was less distant when they were asked to look at their ring while thinking about their wedding compared to when they were just asked to think about their wedding. The experiment was conducted online for two reasons: first, online data collection eased reaching a wide variety of married individuals; secondly, the online setting enabled subjects to participate in anonymous and familiar surroundings. A conducted prestudy in which subjects were randomly approached in a shopping district of St. Gallen showed that many people felt uncomfortable to talk about their wedding and wedding ring with an unknown interviewer on the street. Accordingly, the online setting was preferred. 3.2.2 Participants and Procedure 107 married individuals (53% females; mean age: 51 years; mean marriage duration: 21 years) participated in the study. Subjects belonged to a German and Swiss consumer sample. Participation in the study was voluntary and participants did not receive any incentives. The mean participation time was less than 10 minutes. Participants were contacted via email and asked to participate in a study concerning consumer behavior. The email included a link that directed them to the study. After entering the online-survey, they were asked if they were married. If they were not married, they were advanced to a different study. If they were married, they were 42 randomly assigned to one of the two object conditions. After undergoing the manipulation of wedding ring presence, subjects were forwarded to the measurement of perceived distance to the wedding. Subsequently, subjects made further specifications about various control variables. Finally, subjects were thanked for participation. Figure 3-1 summarizes the overall procedure of experiment one. 1. Introduction to Online-Study Welcoming of participants and selection of married individuals 2. Manipulation of Object Presence Subjects are prompted / not prompted to pay attention to their wedding ring (see section 3.2.3) 3. Measurement of Dependent Variable Measurement of perceived distance to wedding (see section 3.2.4) 4. Measurement of Control Variables Check whether subjects wear wedding ring, year of marriage, age, gender (see section 3.2.5) Figure 3-1: Study 1 / Design / Procedure of Experiment 3.2.3 Manipulation of Independent Variable To manipulate the extent to which the object (wedding ring) symbolizing the event (wedding) was consciously present, participants in the no object condition were asked to take about 10 seconds and recall your wedding. In the object condition participants were asked to take about 10 seconds, look at your wedding ring, and recall your wedding. 3.2.4 Operationalization of Dependent Variable Perceived distance to the wedding was measured by asking participants to report how distant their wedding felt on two 7-point scales (feels very close (1), feels very distant (7); feels as if it was yesterday (1), feels as if it was long time ago (7)). This is a common measure of perceived distance (van Boven et al. 2010; Ross and Wilson 2002). Both scales were averaged to a single measure of perceived distance (r = .94; SB = .97)15 with low values indicating short perceived distances and high values indicating large perceived distances. 15 According to Eisinga, te Grotenhuis, and Pelzer (2012), Spearman-Brown coefficient (SB) is additionally used as reliability estimate for two-item constructs in this dissertation. 43 3.2.5 Control Variables Participants that were drawn into the object condition were asked whether they were wearing a wedding ring before proceeding to the manipulation section in which they were asked to look at their wedding ring and recall their wedding. If they indicated to not wear a ring, they were excluded from the study. Participants in the no object condition were asked at the end of the study whether they wore a wedding ring. Further, all subjects were asked to indicate the year of their wedding as well as their age and gender. 3.2.6 Results A one-factorial ANCOVA showed that participants felt that their wedding was less distant when they looked at their wedding ring (M = 2.89) relative to those participants that were not asked to look at their ring when thinking about their wedding (M = 4.00; F(1, 104) = 9.84, p < .01). The actual distance to the wedding (in years) was included as covariate in the analysis and showed a marginally significant influence on perceived distance to the wedding (F(1, 104) = 3.41, p = .07). The effect of object presence stayed significant when the variable actual distance to the wedding was excluded from the analysis (F(1, 105) = 11.10, p < .01). Figure 3-2 depicts the means and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ perceived distance to the wedding in both conditions. Figure 3-2: Study 1 / Results / Perceived Distance to Wedding 44 As participants in the object condition had to wear a wedding ring to participate in the study, whereas participants in the no object condition did not necessarily have to wear a ring, it was further controlled for a general effect of wearing a ring. A one-factorial ANOVA revealed no significant difference in perceived distance to the wedding for participants (in the no object condition) that did wear and those that did not wear a ring during the study (F(1, 48) = .19, p = .67). This finding indicates that this possible difference in sample characteristics between both conditions did not affect the dependent measure. 3.2.7 Discussion The main goal of experiment 1 was to test hypothesis 1a in a realistic study design. The results of experiment 1 support hypothesis 1a. Participants indicated a significantly smaller perceived distance to their wedding when paying attention to their physically close wedding ring than when just thinking about their wedding. Results further indicate that the presence of the ring explained a larger amount of variance in perceived distance to the wedding than the actual temporal distance to the wedding itself. Results of experiment 1 thus support the notion that possessions can decrease the distance to their self-defining, symbolized meanings. Experiment 1 supports this effect in a realistic field setting. However, this realistic study design comprises the disadvantage of a limited control as regards the experimental execution. Above that, the study design does not account for alternative explanations for the revealed effects. As previously outlined, it is possible that the decrease of perceived distance to the wedding is caused solely by more vivid memories of the wedding that are aroused by the presence of the wedding ring and not by the physical proximity of the ring (see section 2.3.2). Further, it is also possible that the decrease in perceived distance is caused by an affective reaction to the closeness of the ring, independent of its symbolic meaning (see section 2.3.5). The study design also made it difficult to control for other external influences and to check whether participants correctly followed the experimental instructions. These shortcomings of experiment 1 are approached in experiment 2 as well as the subsequent studies. 45 3.3 The Mug Experiment (Study 2) 3.3.1 Overview The purpose of study 2 was to test the proposition that physical proximity to an object decreases subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s symbolized meaning (H1b). In testing this hypothesis, the study aimed at finding more rigorous evidence for the revealed influence of symbolic objects in study 1. According to this aim and as regards the tested hypothesis, study 2 was designed as a laboratory experiment. A laboratory setting allowed for a stronger control of the correct execution of experimental tasks. It also allowed for a precise manipulation of the distance between a particular object and participant. In line with this controlled study setting, a standardized object was utilized in study 2. For various reasons, it was decided to use a standard white mug with the neutral imprint “remember” (see figure 3-3). Figure 3-3: Study 2 / Design / Picture of Utilized Mug Mugs were chosen as objects for study 2 as they have generally been identified as suitable objects for experiments since the majority of people knows and can relate to them (Peck and Shu 2009, p. 437; Kahneman et al. 1990). Like the previously used wedding rings, mugs were additionally utilized as they match the here underlying definition of objects since they have an easily graspable size. Above that, mugs were selected because they are often used as souvenirs and have accordingly been known to relate to certain personal meanings or experiences. Participants were asked to connect the mug to a personally relevant experience within a writing task. To facilitate this 46 connection, the above mentioned and depicted imprint “remember” was placed on the mug. The prediction was that the individually chosen past experience would be perceived as closer in situations in which the mug was close to the participant compared to situations in which it was distant. To test this premise, the study used a one-factorial design with three levels of spatial distance between the mug and the participant. In the short distance condition, the mug was placed directly in front of the participant (40-centimeter distance) and participants were allowed to touch the mug. In the distant condition, the mug was positioned at 4-meter distance to participants, clearly out of reach. In a third control condition the mug was positioned directly in front of the participant (40-centimeter distance) – here participants however were instructed not to touch the mug. This condition was included in the experimental design to control for a confounding effect of touch between the short and long distance conditions: it is possible that an observed difference between the short and long distance condition is not caused by the distance of the mug to the participant but by the ability of participants to touch the mug in the short distance condition. The close / no touch condition was included in the study design to control for this alternative explanation. 3.3.2 Participants and Procedure 126 students (51% females; mean age: 22 years) from the University of St. Gallen participated in the study. Four participants (3.17% of total participants) were excluded from analyses because they specified inappropriate experiences (see section 3.3.6). Participation in the study was voluntary and participants received a chocolate bar as incentive. The mean participation time was less than 10 minutes. Subjects were approached on university campus and asked to participate in a study concerning personal experiences. If agreeing to participate, subjects were led to the laboratory building where they were randomly assigned to one of the prepared rooms. Each room contained a large conference table. Before participants entered the room, the mug was placed at a short / long distance to a predefined seat depending on the tested condition. Participants received an introduction to the study by the student who had approached them on campus. In the course of this introduction, they received a paper and pen questionnaire. They were then left alone in the room. In the first part of the study participants were asked to think of a past, personally important experience. As a manipulation check, participants specified the experience in a few words and indicated its valence. After this first part of the experiment, 47 subjects’ attention was drawn to the mug, which was either located at a close or distant location on the table in front of them. They were asked to fulfill a writing task in which they mentally connected the mug to the previously named experience (see section 3.3.3). Subsequent to the writing task, participants rated their perceived distance to the experience and completed a series of control variables. Finally, subjects were thanked for participation and debriefed. Figure 3-4 summarizes the overall procedure of experiment 2. 1. Introduction to Study Participants receive information that the study is about personal experiences 2. Recall of Personal Experience Participants recall and specify a personal experience and indicate the valence of the experience (see section 3.3.3) 3. Manipulation of Object Proximity Participants are exposed to a mug that is positioned in short / long distance to participants (see section 3.3.3) 4. Writing Task to Connect Object to Specified Experience Participants symbolically connect the mug to the specified experience in a writing task (see section 3.3.3) 5. Measurement of Dependent Variable Participants indicate their perceived distance to the experience (see section 3.3.4) 6. Measurement of Manipulation Checks and Control Variables Participants indicate date of experience, difficulty to recall experience, difficulty to connect mug to experience, gender, age (see section 3.3.5) Figure 3-4: Study 2 / Design / Procedure of Experiment 3.3.3 Manipulation of Independent Variable As outlined in the previous section, participants began the experiment by thinking of a personally important experience. Specifically they were instructed to take some time and think of a personal experience of the previous year (2011) that you still like to remember and that is still personally relevant to you. The study was conducted in April 2012. It was specified to think of an experience in the year 2011 to ensure that the experience had been past for some time. At the same time, this reference frame was chosen to ensure that the actual temporal distance to the experience was possibly homogeneous between subjects while still offering participants a large enough timeframe to think freely of an experience. Participants specified the experience in a few words and answered a manipulation check concerning the valence of this experience (see section 3.3.5). 48 Participants’ attention was then drawn to the mug that was placed on the table in front of them. In the short distance conditions (with and without touch), the mug was placed at 40-centimeter distance and was therefore easy to reach. In the long distance condition, the mug was placed at 4-meter distance on the other side of the table, clearly out of reach. Depending on the condition, participants read the following instructions: Please look at the mug in front of you / on the other side of the table without touching it. There is an imprint on the mug saying “remember”. The mug shall remind you of the experience that you previously recalled. Please imagine that the mug symbolically reflects this experience. Try to mentally connect the mug to the experience. In all three conditions, participants were instructed not to touch the mug at this point of the study with the intention to focus their attention on the task of connecting the mug to the experience and to keep this process as similar as possible in all conditions. Participants were then asked to describe exactly what you think of, when mentally connecting the mug to the experience in a short writing task. The writing task was implemented to support participants in connecting the mug to the chosen experience. After stating their thoughts about the connection between the experience and the mug, participants read the following instructions: in the long and short distance condition in which the mug was not touched, participants were instructed to please take another look at the mug. In the short distance condition in which the mug was touched, participants were instructed to please take the mug into your hand and take another look at the mug. After undergoing this manipulation of mug distance, subjects continued to answer the questions regarding the dependent measure perceived distance to experience. 3.3.4 Operationalization of Dependent Variable Measurement of perceived distance was initiated with the following instruction: When looking at the mug and thinking about its symbolic connection to the experience: how close (distant) does the experience feel in this moment? Perceived distance to the experience was then measured using the same two 7-point scales as in study 1 (feels very close (1), feels very distant (7); feels as if it was yesterday (1), feels as if it was long time ago (7); see van Boven et al. 2010; Ross and Wilson 2002). Both scales were averaged to a single measure of perceived distance (r = .65; SB = .79) with low values indicating short and high values indicating large perceived distances. 49 3.3.5 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables In the beginning of the study, participants were asked to think of a personal experience from the year 2011. As outlined, the chosen experience served as a basis for the further experimental procedure. To control for the kind of experiences that people thought of, they were asked to describe the experience in a few words. Subsequently, participants indicated the perceived valence of the experience on a single scale reaching from positive (1) to negative (7). The scale was reversed so that high values indicated positive and low values negative valence. After indicating the perceived distance to the experience (dependent measure), participants further indicated how difficult it was for them to initially think of an experience (recall difficulty) and how difficult it felt for them to connect the experience to the mug (connection difficulty). Both variables were individually measured on single 7-point scales reaching from very easy (1) to very difficult (7). Recall difficulty was conducted to control for the possibility that process fluency influenced perceived distance to the experience. Connection difficulty was conducted to control for the possibility that mug proximity eased connecting the mug to the experience. The study further controlled for the actual date of the experience and ended with two questions on gender and age. 3.3.6 Results Manipulation and Confound Checks As previously documented, four participants were excluded from the analyses: Three participants specified very negative experiences. One participant specified a regularly repeating event that he had revisited just the previous day. No differences were found regarding the valence of the chosen experiences between the three conditions (F(2, 119) = 1.04, p = .36). At the same time, the overall mean for valence (M = 6.64) was significantly higher than the scale-mid-value (4.00), indicating that on average participants chose positive experiences (t(121) = 52.32, p < .01). A comparison of the specified experiences showed similar patterns for all three conditions: most participants considered experiences such as trips or vacations as well as particular successes in high school or college. No differences between conditions were found for recall difficulty (F(2, 119) = .65, p = .53) and connection difficulty (F(2, 119) = .28, p = .76). On average, the specified experiences had taken place eight months ago. This temporal distance did neither differ between conditions (F(2, 119) = .47, p = .63) nor influence perceived distance to the experience (β = .01, p = .78). 50 Testing of Hypothesis 1b Hypothesis 1b predicts that a reduction of spatial distance to an object leads to a reduction of the perceived distance to the symbolic meaning of the object. An ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of mug distance on perceived distance to the experience (F(2, 119) = 3.30, p = .04). In line with hypothesis 1b, planned contrasts (LSD16) indicated a significant mean difference between the long distance condition (M = 4.02) and the short distance conditions without touch (M = 3.38; t(119) = 2.02, p = .045) and with touch (M = 3.34; t(119) = 2.35, p = .02). No significant difference was found between the two short distance conditions (t(119) = .13, p = .89). Figure 3-5 depicts the means and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ perceived distance to the personal experience in each condition. Figure 3-5: Study 2 / Results / Perceived Distance to Personal Experience 3.3.7 Discussion The results of experiment 2 support hypothesis 1b. Hence, in conditions in which the mug was placed directly in front of participants the symbolized meaning of the mug was perceived as closer as in situations in which the mug was placed at a remote location. The results replicate the findings from experiment 1 while further refining 16 Least significant difference t-tests (LSD) were utilized for pairwise comparisons throughout this dissertation. Hence, it was not adjusted for multiple comparisons. This was decided as the comparisons were theoretically motivated and because the amount of comparisons per analysis did not exceed 3 (max. 6 in study 3). 51 them: the results support the assumption that the physical proximity to a symbolic object positively influences a subject’s perceived proximity to the object’s symbolized meaning. Findings further contradict a possible alternative explanation that was discussed apropos the findings of experiment 1 – that is, a symbolic object might solely intensify mental images of the symbolized meaning and therefore reduce its perceived distance. In experiment 2, participants in each condition connected the same mug to a certain self-selected experience. Consequently, it was controlled for a possible effect of the object on the vividness of mental images of the experience. The results therefore support the here proposed notion that symbolic objects transfer their adherent, abstract meaning into the direct experienceable world and therefore reduce the perceived distance to their meaning. Experiment 2 further controlled for the particular effect of touch. Touch did not significantly influence the perceived distance of the object’s symbolic meaning. Hence, perceived distances in conditions in which the mug was placed directly in front of subjects did not differ, irrespective of whether the mug was touched or not. This partially contradicts findings from Peck and Shu (2009, p. 437). Their findings indicate that subjects perceive stronger ownership towards a physically close object if the object is touched compared to when the object is not touched. A possible reason for this diverging result could be that the effect of touching an object onto the perception of owning it might not equally account for the here observed perceived distance to the object’s meaning. Whereas ownership is a relatively dichotomous status (to own or not own), distance is a clearly continuous variable (from infinite distant to infinite close). From this perspective, touching might be seen as one point in a continuous setting of distances reaching from “out of reach”, over “in reaching distance”, over “close to touch”, “slightly touching”, “touching”, to “intensively touching”. It can accordingly be argued that the experimental variation between a very close mug that is touched and a very close mug that is not touched might not be large enough to influence the here observed dependent variable. Most importantly, however, results indicate that the revealed effect is actually caused by the object’s spatial distance and not by a confounding effect of touch. At the same time, the result that both close mug conditions show significantly lower means for perceived distance to the experience than the distant mug condition further fortifies the influence of spatial distance. As the focus of this research lies in investigating this specific effect of spatial distance, the following experiments will not investigate closer the particular difference between distance and touch. 52 As outlined above, results of experiment 2 contradict the possible alternative explanation that objects lead to a more vivid mental construal of their symbolized meaning and therefore lead to a decrease in perceived distance to that meaning. A second alternative explanation that was outlined in the context of experiment 1, however, also partially accounts for the shown effects in experiment 2: as previously argued, it is possible that the shown effects of spatial distance to an object are due to an affective reaction to that object’s distance and are independent of the object’s symbolic connection to its meaning. Accordingly, it is possible that the revealed positive influence of mug proximity on perceived proximity to its meaning is solely caused by such an affective reaction to a close object. By further enhancing the utilized designs of studies 1 and 2, experiment 3 will account for this alternative explanation. 3.4 The Tennis Ball Experiment (Study 3) 3.4.1 Overview The purpose of study 3 was to test the overall conceptual model of this dissertation. Studies 1 and 2 focused on investigating the main effect of graspable, symbolic objects on the perceived distance of subjects to these objects’ meanings. Experiment 3 will retest these main effects (H1a and H1b) while including further dimensions in its study design. Experiment 3 is particularly designed to test the moderating effect of the symbolic connection between an object and a meaning on the previously tested main effect: it is argued that the influence of an object’s distance on the perceived distance to its symbolized meaning is stronger the stronger the object is symbolically connected to this meaning (H4a). Moreover, it is predicted that object proximity additionally leads to a stronger self-extension towards the object’s meaning and that this effect is mediated by perceived distance to the meaning (H2a and H2b). It is further predicted that this mediation is moderated by the symbolic connection between object and meaning (H4b). Above that, it is predicted that object proximity also results in stronger behavioral intentions towards the symbolized meaning and that this effect is mediated by perceived distance and self-extension towards the meaning (H3a and H3b). Moreover, this effect is predicted to be moderated by the symbolic connection between object and meaning (H4c). To test this hypothetical model, experiment 3 was conducted using a 2 (object proximity: close / distant) x 3 (symbolic connection: no / moderate / strong) design. 53 Additionally, one condition without an object was included in the study design to control for the general effect of object presence. As the study aimed at further and more precisely testing the conceptual model of this dissertation, it was designed as a laboratory experiment. Similar as for study 2, this setting was used to allow for a strong control of the correct execution of the experimental tasks. Further, this setting allowed for a precise manipulation of the spatial distance between objects and participants. A critical element of the development of study 3 was the determination of a suitable object and symbolized meaning to operationalize the outlined study design. To control the strength of the symbolic connection between an object and a meaning, it was decided to use objects and a meaning that stood in a predefined symbolic connection to each other. Hence, different to study 2, where the connection between the object and the personal experience was created by each subject individually, it was the goal of study 3 to use a generally known object and a generally known meaning that could easily be manipulated as regards their symbolic connection. Further, as in the previous studies, the chosen object was supposed to meet the requirements of the here defined properties of graspable objects (see section 2.2.1). Building upon these guidelines, tennis balls were chosen as objects and one of the most well-known tennis tournaments in the world, namely the ATP Wimbledon Tennis Championship, as a symbolically connected meaning. An informal prestudy conducted with 20 students from the University of St. Gallen showed that all participants, including subjects that did not play tennis, had previously touched a tennis ball and knew Wimbledon. The study was conducted in December 2012 and referred to the previously held Wimbledon Tournament of June to July 2012. Therefore, at the time of the experiment it was five months since the tournament had finished. Accordingly, it was distant enough to allow for the proposed distance reducing effect of a symbolically connected object. 3.4.2 Participants and Procedure 463 students (72% males; mean age: 22 years) from the University of St. Gallen participated in the study. The high proportion of male participants reflects the gender distribution at this particular university. 11 participants (2.38% of total participants) were excluded from the analyses because their answering patterns strongly diverged from the majority of participants. Answers of excluded participants differed more than two standard deviations from group means for at least one of the observed dependent 54 variables. Participation in the study was voluntary. Participants received a chocolate bar as incentive. The mean participation time was less than 10 minutes. The overall procedure of experiment 3 was similar to experiment 2. Subjects were approached on university campus and asked to participate in a study concerning sports. If agreeing to participate, subjects were led to the laboratory building where they were randomly assigned to one of the prepared rooms. Each room contained a large conference table. Before participants entered the room, one of three types of tennis balls (varying in their symbolic connections to Wimbledon) was placed in a short / long distance to a predefined seat depending on the tested condition. Participants received a short introduction to the study from the student who had approached them on campus. In the course of this introduction, subjects received a paper and pen questionnaire. They were then left alone in the room. In the first part of the study, participants were informed that the study concerned the Wimbledon tennis tournament. They subsequently received some basic information about the tournament. After that, their attention was drawn to the tennis ball on the table in front of them (this part was omitted for people in the no object condition). Subsequent to the tennis ball manipulation, participants rated their perceived distance to Wimbledon. Furthermore, they rated their perceived self-extension to Wimbledon as well as their intentions to visit the tournament in the future (behavioral intentions). The questionnaire ended with a series of control measures. Finally, subjects were thanked for participation and debriefed. Figure 3-6 depicts the procedure of experiment 3. 1. Introduction to Study Participants receive information that the study is about the Wimbledon tennis tournament 2. Exposure to Information about Event Participants read some basic information on Wimbledon (see section 3.4.3) 3. Manipulation of Object Proximity and Symbolic Connection of Object to Event Participants are exposed to a non- / moderate- / strong-symbolic tennis ball that is in a close / distant position. In the “no object” control condition, participants are not exposed to a tennis ball (see section 3.4.3). 4. Measurement of Dependent Variables Participants indicate their perceived distance, self-extension, behavioral intentions about Wimbledon (see section 3.4.5) 5. Measurement of Control Variables Participants indicate their knowledge about Wimbledon, interest in Wimbledon, gender, age (see section 3.4.6) Figure 3-6: Study 3 / Design / Procedure of Experiment 55 3.4.3 Manipulation of Independent Variables After being welcomed to the study, participants were informed that the study was about the Wimbledon tennis tournament. Subsequently, they received a short introduction to the tournament that included the following information: Wimbledon is one of the most traditional and distinguished tennis tournaments in the world. It is named after the Wimbledon district, an area in the city of London in England, where it is held. The tournament is the only one of the four major tennis tournaments (Grand-Slam-Tournaments) that is played on grass courts. It was first held in 1877. Wimbledon takes place each year for two weeks, beginning at the end of June. It was last conducted in the period from 25th June until 8th July 2012. Below you can see some impressions of the tournament: Figure 3-7: Study 3 / Design / Depiction Wimbledon 56 After receiving this information, participants’ attention was drawn to the tennis ball on the table in front of them. This part was omitted for participants in the no ball control condition. Spatial distance to the ball was manipulated analog to experiment 2. The ball was placed on the table before participants entered the room. In the short distance conditions, the ball was placed at 40-centimeter distance to the participant and was therefore in reaching distance. In the long distance condition, the ball was placed at 4-meter distance on the table, clearly out of reach. In the short distance conditions, participants were asked to please take the ball into your hands and look at it. In the long distance conditions, participants were asked to please look at the ball. To manipulate the symbolic connection between the ball and the tournament three different kinds of tennis ball were utilized. They were chosen based on Peirce’s differentiation between iconic, symbolic, and indexical signs (see section 2.2.3). However, these different kinds of relationships were not treated as factual relationships between the sign (the ball) and the signified (Wimbledon). In line with the theoretical argumentation in section 2.2.3, they were interpreted as different qualities of learned and thus purely symbolic relationships between the sign and its meaning. To manipulate the strength of the connection between the ball and the tournament, each higher factor level of symbolic connection comprised an additional semiotic meaning layer. In the non-symbolic ball condition, a simple leisure tennis ball was used as object. The ball was a plain tennis ball with no imprint (see figure 3-8). Therefore, this ball primarily had an iconic relationship to Wimbledon because it looked similar to the tennis balls that are used for the tournament. However, it had no direct symbolic connection to Wimbledon, as it did not particularly link to the tournament. Its only symbolic connection to Wimbledon was of indirect nature via the association “tennis”. To ensure that participants exposed to this ball had a homogenous understanding of its connection to the tournament they received the following information: This is a standard tennis ball that is used for leisure tennis matches. Tennis balls of this kind are not used at Wimbledon Championships. In the moderate-symbolic ball condition, the official competition ball of the Wimbledon tournament was used as object. This ball carried the imprint “Slazenger, WIMBLEDON ULTRA VIS” (see figure 3-8). Being the official tournament ball of Wimbledon, it hence symbolized a direct connection to the tournament. Whereas the non-symbolic ball only iconically related to Wimbledon, this ball related iconically 57 and symbolically to the tournament. To ensure that participants exposed to this ball had a homogenous understanding of its connection to the tournament they received the following information: This tennis ball is an official championship ball of the Wimbledon tennis tournament. Tennis balls such as this one are used for the matches at Wimbledon Championships. This ball has not been played. In the strong-symbolic ball condition, a played exemplar of the Wimbledon tournament ball was used.17 This ball also carried the imprint “Slazenger, WIMBLEDON ULTRA VIS” (see figure 3-8). Different to the moderate-symbolic ball, it had been used. Participants were told that the ball had been played at the previous Wimbledon Championship. Consequently, compared to the previously outlined condition a further layer was added to the connection between the ball and the tournament. Referring to Grayson and Shulman’s argumentation, participants learned that this ball had “a factual, spatial connection” to the tournament (Grayson and Shulman 2000, p. 19). Accordingly, in this condition the ball was iconically, symbolically, and indexically connected to Wimbledon. To ensure that participants exposed to this ball had a homogenous understanding of its connection to the tournament they received the following information: This tennis ball is an official championship ball of the Wimbledon tennis tournament. This ball was actually used at the last Wimbledon Championship. It was played in one of the matches during the tournament. Figure 3-8 depicts the three utilized tennis balls. Figure 3-8: Study 3 / Design / Pictures of Utilized Tennis Balls 17 The Wimbledon Championship was additionally chosen in this study opposed to the French Open, as it is played on grass courts. This was important for the experimental design, to keep differences in the outer appearance of the played and unused tennis balls as little as possible. A ball that had been played on a clay court would have stronger altered its outer appearance and hence enlarged the risk of confounding effects between this and the other two conditions. 58 Before the main study, these described and depicted tennis balls were tested as regards their symbolic connection to the Wimbledon tennis tournament. The next section outlines the procedure and results of this pretest. 3.4.4 Pretesting of Stimuli To ensure that the symbolic connection between the tennis balls and Wimbledon was manipulated correctly a prestudy was conducted in which the perceived symbolic and indexical connection between the three different tennis balls and Wimbledon were pretested. For efficiency reasons, the prestudy was conducted online. 90 subjects (62% male; mean age: 30 years) participated in the study. Participants were recruited from an online consumer panel. Subjects were invited to participate in a short study about tennis balls and the Wimbledon tennis tournament. They received $0.30 for participation. The mean participation time was less than five minutes. The study used a one-factorial between subjects design. Each subject was randomly assigned to one of three tennis ball conditions. After entering the study, participants read the following text: Below you see a tennis ball. Please look closely at the ball and read the information concerning the ball below. Participants then saw a standardized picture of one of three above depicted tennis balls (see figure 3-8). Furthermore, they read one of the above outlined descriptions referring to the shown ball (see section 3.4.3). After being exposed to the stimuli, subjects rated the ball on two dimensions: first, the general symbolic connection of the ball to the tournament; secondly, the indexical connection of the ball to the tournament. The symbolic connection between the ball and the tournament was measured using the following three items: How strongly does the tennis ball relate to the Wimbledon tournament? / How strong is the connection between the tennis ball and the Wimbledon tournament? / How strongly do you feel that the ball connects to the Wimbledon tournament? Scale endpoints were labeled very weak (1) and very strong (7). The three items were averaged to a single measure of symbolic connection (α = .98) with low values indicating a weak symbolic connection and high values indicating a strong symbolic connection. Indexical connection was measured using four items adapted from Grayson and Martinec (2004): There is a physical connection between the ball and the Wimbledon tournament / Being physically close to the ball would be like being physically close to the Wimbledon tournament / Looking at the ball makes me feel physically close to the Wimbledon tournament / Touching the ball would be like touching a part of the 59 Wimbledon tournament. Scale endpoints were labeled totally disagree (1) and totally agree (7). The four items were averaged to a single measure of indexical connection (α = .91) with low values indicating a weak indexical connection and high values indicating a strong indexical connection. A one-factorial ANOVA supported the appropriateness of the manipulation. Specifically, results revealed significantly different levels of symbolic connection between the three conditions (F(2, 87) = 56.00, p < .01). Ratings for the plain tennis ball with no symbolic connection (M = 2.81) were below ratings for the unplayed official Championship Ball (M = 5.60; t(87) = 7.57, p < .01). Ratings for the supposedly played official Championship Ball (M = 6.49) were above the ratings for the not played Championship Ball (t(87) = 2.48, p = .02). Similar results were found for the indexical connections of the balls to the tournament (F(2, 87) = 26.46, p < .01). Indexical connection for the plain tennis ball (M = 2.05) was perceived as lower than for the unplayed Championship Ball (M = 3.71; t(87) = 4.31, p < .01), which again was rated lower than the supposedly played Championship Ball (M = 4.77; t(87) = 2.83, p = .01). Figure 3-9 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ ratings of the symbolic and indexical connection for each of the three types of tennis balls. Figure 3-9: Study 3 / Pretest / Manipulation Check The results do not only support the intended manipulation; the indicated differences in ratings are moreover informative regarding the conceptual basis of this dissertation. The findings will further be discussed in section 3.4.8. 60 3.4.5 Operationalization of Dependent Variables After being exposed to and receiving the outlined information regarding one of the three types of tennis balls, measurement of perceived distance to Wimbledon was initiated with the following instruction: In the following section, we would like to ask you a few questions regarding the Wimbledon tournament: As previously mentioned, the Wimbledon tournament was last held in the period from 25th June until 8th July 2012. When looking at the ball and thinking about the Wimbledon tournament, how close (distant) does the tournament feel to you in this moment?18 Perceived distance to Wimbledon was then measured using the same two 7-point scales as in the previous studies (feels very close (1), feels very distant (7); feels as if it was yesterday (1), feels as if it was long time ago (7); see van Boven et al. 2010; Ross and Wilson 2002). Both scales were averaged to a single measure of perceived distance (r = .66; SB = .80) with low values indicating short and high values indicating large perceived distances. After rating the perceived distance to Wimbledon, subjects stated their agreement with several items used to operationalize their perceived self-extension to Wimbledon. The utilized scale was adapted from Escalas and Bettman’s self-brand connection scale (Escalas and Bettman 2003; 2005). It has previously been applied in similar contexts referring to self-construals of consumers (ibid). The scale consisted of the following seven, slightly adapted items (anchored by strongly agree (1) to strongly disagree (7)): The Wimbledon tournament reflects who I am / I can identify with the Wimbledon tournament / I feel a personal connection to the Wimbledon tournament / My interest in Wimbledon matches the image that I want to communicate to others / The Wimbledon tournament is a part of me / The Wimbledon tournament says something about who I am / The Wimbledon tournament suits me well. The items were averaged to a single measure of self-extension (α = .94) and values of this measure were reversed. Accordingly, low values indicated weak self-extension and high values indicated strong self-extension of subjects to Wimbledon. After measuring self-extension to Wimbledon, subjects’ behavioral intentions regarding visiting the tournament were inquired. The following two items were used to operationalize this construct (anchored by strongly agree (1) to strongly disagree (7)): 18 As previously outlined, participants in the “no ball” control condition were not exposed to the tennis ball and were just asked to think about the Wimbledon tennis tournament. 61 I would very much to like to visit the Wimbledon tournament at one point in the future / I can easily imagine visiting the Wimbledon tournament in the future. The items were averaged to a single measure of behavioral intentions (r = .82; SB = .90) and values of this measure were reversed. Accordingly, low values indicated weak intentions and high values indicated strong intentions to visit Wimbledon in the future. 3.4.6 Control Variables To control for possible confounding effects, further measures were included in the end section of the questionnaire. These contained two questions concerning subjects’ knowledge about Wimbledon prior to the study: How well did you know the Wimbledon tennis tournament before this survey? (very well (1), not at all (7)); How strongly were you interested in Wimbledon before this survey? (very strongly (1), not at all (7)). Furthermore, participants were asked to indicate: How intensively did you follow the last held Wimbledon Championship? (very intensively (1), not at all (7)). However, these control variables were excluded from the main analysis because they showed to be influenced by the conducted manipulations. Participants further indicated their gender and age. 3.4.7 Results The purpose of study 3 was to test jointly the overall conceptual model of this dissertation. Results of the analyses will be reported subsequently concerning the tested hypotheses. Testing of Hypotheses 1a, 1b, and 4a One purpose of study 3 was to retest the effect of object presence / proximity on perceived distance to the object’s meaning (H1a and H1b) while further testing the moderating influence of the symbolic connection strength between object and meaning (H4a). It was predicted that the presence of a tennis ball would only then reduce the perceived distance to Wimbledon if the ball was physically close and symbolically related to the tournament. Further, it was predicted that when the ball was close it would stronger reduce perceived distance to Wimbledon, the stronger it was symbolically connected to Wimbledon. A two-factorial ANOVA including two levels of ball distance (close / distant) and three levels of symbolic connection between the ball and Wimbledon (no / moderate / strong) was conducted. Additionally, paired comparisons between the control condition without ball and each of the conditions with tennis ball were conducted. 62 Figure 3-10 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for perceived distance to Wimbledon for each condition. Figure 3-10: Study 3 / Results / Perceived Distance to Wimbledon The conducted ANOVA19 revealed a significant interaction between ball proximity and symbolic connection (F(2, 352) = 3.47, p = .03). Specific contrasts (LSD) supported hypotheses 4a: physical proximity to the tennis ball only reduced the perceived distance to Wimbledon (compared to states in which the ball was distant) for tennis balls with a moderate connection to Wimbledon (M’s of 4.11 and 4.82; t(352) = 3.18, p < .01) and with a strong connection to Wimbledon (M’s of 3.78 and 4.61; t(352) = 3.34, p < .01). No difference was found for the non-symbolic ball (M’s of 4.78 and 4.80; t(352) = .06, p = .95). Further univariate tests revealed that there was no significant difference for perceived distance to Wimbledon between the different types of tennis balls in the distant ball condition (F(2, 352) = .36, p = .70). Perceived distance to Wimbledon did however differ between groups with different tennis balls within the close ball condition (F(2, 352) = 12.23, p < .01). Pairwise comparisons 19 Due to an initial focus of the study on the close / no ball conditions, participants are not equally distributed to the experimental cells. N for the cells are as follow: non-symbolic ball = 69 (close) / 47 (distant); moderatesymbolic ball = 82 (close) / 48 (distant); strong-symbolic ball = 75 (close) / 37 (distant); no ball = 94. Additional analyses were conducted with randomly drawn subsamples of the close ball conditions to match cell sizes. Results corresponded to the here reported analyses. The control condition without ball was excluded from the ANOVA because it did not match the factorial design. Pairwise comparisons were conducted to compare this baseline condition against the experimental conditions (see below). 63 (LSD) within the close ball condition revealed that compared to the non-symbolic ball group perceived distance to Wimbledon was significantly lower for the moderatesymbolic ball group (M’s of 4.11 and 4.78; t(352) = 3.33, p < .01) and for the strongsymbolic ball group (M’s of 3.78 and 4.78, t(352) = 4.87, p < .01). Perceived distance to Wimbledon was marginally lower for the strong-symbolic ball group compared to the moderate symbolic-ball group (M’s of 3.78 and 4.11; t(352) = 1.68, p = .096). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) between the no ball baseline condition (M = 4.84) and the ball conditions additionally supported the predicted outcome that only the close moderate-symbolic ball (M = 4.11; t(445) = 3.76, p < .01) and the close strongsymbolic ball (M = 3.78; t(445) = 5.32, p < .01) led to significantly lower perceived distances to Wimbledon. All other comparisons between the no ball condition and the ball conditions were insignificant, with p-values above .36. The outlined comparisons further support the predicted result that only physically close tennis balls with a direct symbolic connection to Wimbledon decrease participants’ perceived distance to Wimbledon. Testing of Hypotheses 2a, 2b, and 4b A further purpose of study 3 was to test hypotheses 2a, 2b, and 4b. It was predicted that physical proximity to a tennis ball would positively influence perceived selfextension to Wimbledon (H2a) via a reduction of perceived distance to Wimbledon (H2b) and that this mediation would be moderated by the symbolic connection between the tennis ball and Wimbledon (H4a). Before testing the proposed mediation, two analyses tested the direct effect of the utilized tennis balls on subjects’ perceived self-extension to Wimbledon. Analog to the above outlined procedure, first an ANOVA was conducted to test for the presumed interaction of object proximity and symbolic connection. Secondly, planned contrasts between the baseline condition without ball and the six ball conditions were conducted. Figure 3-11 depicts the means values and confidence intervals (95%) for perceived self-extension to Wimbledon in each condition. 64 Figure 3-11: Study 3 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Wimbledon The ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between tennis ball proximity and symbolic connection (F(2, 352) = 3.54, p = .03). Specific contrasts (LSD) revealed the predicted interaction pattern: physical proximity to the ball only strengthened perceived self-extension to Wimbledon (compared to states in which the ball was distant) for moderate-symbolic balls (M’s of 3.10 and 2.23; t(352) = 3.73, p < .01) and strong-symbolic balls (M’s of 3.06 and 2.40; t(352) = 2.60, p = .01). No difference was found for the ball without a direct symbolic connection to Wimbledon (M’s of 2.39 and 2.38; t(352) = .02, p = .98). Further univariate tests revealed that there was no significant difference for perceived self-extension to Wimbledon between the different types of balls in the distant ball condition (F(2, 352) = .23, p = .80). Perceived selfextension to Wimbledon did however differ between ball-types within the close ball condition (F(2, 352) = 7.11, p < .01). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) within the close ball condition revealed that, compared to the non-symbolic ball group, perceived selfextension to Wimbledon was significantly higher for the moderate-symbolic ball group (M’s of 3.10 and 2.39; t(352) = 3.43, p < .01) and for the strong-symbolic ball group (M’s of 3.06 and 2.39, t(352) = 3.16, p < .01). However, no difference in perceived self-extension was found between the groups that were exposed to close moderate- and close strong-symbolic balls (M’s of 3.10 and 3.06; t(352) = 0.20, p = .85). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) between the no ball baseline condition (M = 2.22) and the six ball conditions additionally supported the predicted outcome that only the close 65 moderate-symbolic ball (M = 3.10; t(445) = 4.55, p < .01) and the close strongsymbolic ball (M = 3.06; t(445) = 4.24, p < .01) led to significantly higher perceived self-extension to Wimbledon. All other comparisons between the no ball condition and the ball conditions were insignificant with p-values above .42. The outlined comparisons hence further support the predicted result that only physically close tennis balls with a direct symbolic connection to Wimbledon increase participants’ perceived self-extension to Wimbledon (H2a, H4b). Building upon these results, further analyses where conducted to test for the predicted moderated mediation. Specifically it was tested whether physical proximity to a tennis ball leads to perceived self-extension to Wimbledon and whether this effect is mediated by perceived distance to Wimbledon and moderated by the ball’s symbolic connection to Wimbledon. It was predicted that the mediation would only occur for the moderate and strong symbolic ball, not for the plain tennis ball without a direct connection to Wimbledon. As the previous analyses only revealed weak / no differences in the effects of the close moderate- and close strong-symbolic ball for the observed dependent variables, the comparison of these two conditions was excluded from the mediation analysis. Therefore, the non-symbolic ball condition served as a baseline condition. The moderate- and strong-symbolic ball conditions were contrasted against this condition. To test the predicted moderated mediation the independent variable “object proximity” was coded with the value “1” for short object distance and with the value “0” for large object distance. Further, dummy variables were created to differentiate the three levels of the moderator symbolic connection. The non-symbolic ball condition was determined as baseline condition. Two dummy variables were created to differentiate the moderate- and strong- symbolic ball conditions from the no ball condition and between each other. A stepwise mediation analysis was conducted following the procedure proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986). To estimate the indirect effects for each of the three tennis ball conditions the non-parametric bootstrap analysis provided by the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model 10) was applied (Hayes 2012; 2013). Figure 3-12 depicts the results of the moderated mediation analysis. 66 Figure 3-12: Study 3 / Results / Moderated Mediation Analysis As previously outlined, physical proximity to the tennis ball did not directly influence perceived self-extension to Wimbledon for the non-symbolic ball (βnon = .01, p = .98). In contrast to this ball, physical proximity to the moderate-symbolic ball had a significantly stronger influence on self-extension to Wimbledon (βmod = .86, p = .01). Similarly, compared to the non-symbolic ball the strong-symbolic ball led to a marginally stronger increase of perceived self-extension to Wimbledon (βstrong = .66, p = .06). A similar pattern was found for the influence of ball proximity on perceived distance to Wimbledon: no effect of physical proximity to the ball was found for the baseline condition with the non-symbolic ball (βnon = -.02, p = .95). In contrast to the plain tennis ball, physical proximity to the moderate-symbolic ball led to a significantly stronger decrease of perceived distance to Wimbledon (βmod = -.70, p = .03). Similarly, compared to the non-symbolic ball the strong-symbolic ball led to significantly stronger decrease of perceived distance to Wimbledon (βstrong = -.81, p = .02). As predicted, perceived distance to Wimbledon was moreover a significant predictor of perceived self-extension to Wimbledon (β = -.45, p < .01). Lastly, when both variables – object proximity and perceived distance to the symbolized meaning – were included in the overall regression model, perceived distance stayed a significant predictor of 67 perceived self-extension (β = -.41, p < .01), while the moderated impact of the independent variable was reduced for the moderate-symbolic object (βmod = .58, p = .06) and eliminated for the strong-symbolic object (βstrong = .33, p = .32). Based on these analyses, conditional indirect effects were estimated for each of the three tennis ball types. Using the 95% bootstrap confidence intervals provided by the deployed PROCESS macro, it was further checked whether these indirect effects differed significantly from zero (Hayes 2013). Table 3-2 shows the estimation results. Conditional Indirect Effects of Object Proximity on Self-Extension at Levels of the Moderator Level Moderator Effect Boot SE Boot LLCI Boot UPCI Non-Symbolic Ball .01 .09 -.17 .18 Moderate-Symbolic Ball .29 .11 .11 .52 Strong-Symbolic Ball .34 .11 .32 1.04 Table 3-2: Study 3 / Results / Indirect Effects – Moderated Mediation As predicted, the estimated confidence interval for the indirect effect of the nonsymbolic ball was not significantly different from the value zero (.01 with a CI of -.17 to .18). However, results revealed indirect effects for the moderate-symbolic ball (.29 with a CI of .11 to .52) and for the strong-symbolic ball (.34 with a CI of .32 to 1.04) that significantly differed from the value zero. The results of the moderated mediation analysis hence support hypothesis 2b and 4b. Testing of Hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 4c A further purpose of study 3 was to test hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 4c. It was predicted that physical proximity to a tennis ball would positively influence subjects’ behavioral intentions towards Wimbledon (H3a). It was further predicted that the direct effect of object proximity on these intentions would be mediated by subjects’ perceived distance and self-extension to Wimbledon (H3b). Moreover, it was predicted that this effect would be moderated by the symbolic connection between the tennis ball and Wimbledon (H4c). As reported in section 3.4.5, behavioral intentions were operationalized by asking subjects about their intentions to visit the tournament at some point in the future. Before testing the proposed mediation, two analyses tested the direct effect of object proximity for the three types of tennis balls on subjects’ intentions to visit Wimbledon. Analog to the previously outlined procedures, first an ANOVA was conducted to test for the predicted interaction of object proximity and symbolic connection between ball 68 and Wimbledon. Secondly, planned contrasts between the baseline condition without ball and the six ball conditions were conducted. Figure 3-13 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ intention to visit Wimbledon in each condition. Figure 3-13: Study 3 / Results / Intention to Visit Wimbledon The ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between ball proximity and symbolic connection (F(2, 352) = 3.03, p = .05). Specific contrasts (LSD) revealed the predicted interaction pattern: physical proximity to the tennis ball only strengthened behavioral intentions (compared to states in which the ball was distant) for the moderate-symbolic ball (M’s of 6.03 and 5.17; t(352) = 3.05, p < .01) and for the strong-symbolic ball (M’s of 5.81 and 5.16; t(352) = 2.08, p = .04). No difference was found for the nonsymbolic ball (M’s of 5.29 and 5.39; t(352) = .35, p = .73). Further univariate tests revealed that there was no significant difference in behavioral intentions between the different types of balls in the distant ball condition (F(2, 352) = .33, p = .72). However, behavioral intentions did differ between groups with different balls within the close ball condition (F(2, 352) = 4.39, p = .01). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) within the close ball conditions revealed that compared to the non-symbolic ball group intentions to visit Wimbledon were significantly higher in the moderate-symbolic ball group (M’s of 6.03 and 5.29; t(352) = 2.91, p < .01) and in the strong-symbolic ball group (M’s of 5.81 and 5.29, t(352) = 2.01, p = .05). However, no difference in 69 behavioral intentions was found between the groups that were exposed to close moderate- and close strong-symbolic balls (M’s of 6.03 and 5.81; t(352) = 0.87, p = .38). Further, pairwise comparisons (LSD) between the no ball baseline condition (M = 4.93) and the six ball conditions additionally supported the predicted outcome that particularly the close moderate-symbolic ball (M = 6.03; t(445) = 4.41, p < .01) and the close strong-symbolic ball (M = 5.81; t(445) = 3.45, p < .01) led to significantly stronger behavioral intentions. All other comparisons between the no ball condition and the experimental conditions were insignificant, with p-values above .12. The outlined comparisons further support the predicted result that only spatially close tennis balls with a direct symbolic connection to Wimbledon increase subjects’ behavioral intentions towards Wimbledon (H3a, H4c). Building upon these results, a mediation analysis was conducted. As outlined above, a serial multiple mediation of (1) object proximity on (2) perceived distance to the object’s meaning on (3) perceived self-extension to the object’s meaning on (4) behavioral intentions towards the object’s meaning was predicted. The conducted mediation analysis primarily aimed at testing the presumed serial constitution of the described mediation process. Due to this goal, it was decided to exclude the moderating influence of the symbolic connection between the object and its meaning from this analysis. The previous mediation analysis revealed that the mediation of object proximity on perceived self-extension to Wimbledon via perceived distance to Wimbledon only occurred for objects that were symbolically connected to Wimbledon. Accordingly, to test the predicted serial multiple mediation, one of the two symbolic ball conditions was chosen. As the estimated indirect effect of object proximity on perceived self-extension to Wimbledon had been strongest for the strongsymbolic ball conditions (.34), this subsample was chosen for the conducted serial multiple mediation. Figure 3-14 summarizes the results of the mediation analyses. 70 Figure 3-14: Study 3 / Results / Serial Multiple Mediation Analyses A regression analysis revealed that object proximity within the strong-symbolic ball condition was a significant predictor of behavioral intentions (βstrong = .65, p = .03). In line with the previous analyses, object proximity was further found to be a significant predictor of perceived distance to Wimbledon (βstrong = -.83, p < .01) and perceived self-extension to Wimbledon (βstrong = .66, p = .013). Further, perceived distance to Wimbledon was a significant predictor of self-extension to Wimbledon (βstrong = -.53, p < .01). Moreover, perceived distance (βstrong = -.44, p < .01) as well as self-extension to Wimbledon (βstrong = .68, p < .01) had a significant impact on subjects’ intentions to visit Wimbledon. When perceived self-extension to Wimbledon was regressed on object proximity as well as perceived distance to Wimbledon, the impact of perceived distance stayed significant (βstrong = -.50, p < .01) while the impact of object proximity was eliminated (βstrong = .25, p = .32). Finally, object proximity, perceived distance, as well as perceived self-extension to Wimbledon were included into the overall regression model predicting behavioral intentions. Only the impact of perceived selfextension stayed significant (βstrong = .63, p < .01) while the impact of object proximity (βstrong = .17, p = .53) and the impact of perceived distance to Wimbledon (βstrong = -.08, p = .46) were eliminated. 71 Based on these analyses, indirect effects for each of the possible mediation paths were estimated. Using the 95% bootstrap confidence intervals provided by the deployed PROCESS macro (Model = 6), it was further checked whether these indirect effects differed significantly from zero (Hayes 2013). Table 3-3 shows the estimation results. Indirect Effects of Object Proximity on Behavioral Intentions through Possible Mediation Paths Mediation Path Effect Boot SE Boot LLCI Boot UPCI Object Proximity -> Perceived Distance -> Behavioral Intentions .07 .10 -.14 .27 Object Proximity -> Self-Extension -> Behavioral Intentions .16 .15 -.15 .49 Object Proximity -> Perceived Distance -> Self-Extension -> Behavioral Intentions .26 .10 .11 .52 Table 3-3: Study 3 / Results / Indirect Effects – Serial Multiple Mediation As table 3-3 indicates, the conducted analysis revealed that only the indirect effect of the full mediation path from object proximity over perceived distance over perceived self-extension to behavioral intentions towards Wimbledon was significantly different from the value zero (.26 with a CI of .11 to .52). Indirect effects for the two other possible mediation paths were estimated to be not significantly different from zero. Further, as previously mentioned, the direct influence of object proximity was eliminated in the overall regression model. Results therefore support the postulated serial multiple mediation of object proximity on behavioral intentions for the strongsymbolic ball subsample (H3b). 3.4.8 Discussion The results of experiment 3 support hypotheses 1a, 1b, 2a, 2b, 3a, 3b, and partially support hypotheses 4a, 4b, and 4c. The results replicate the finding that physically close, symbolic objects (compared to distant objects) decrease the perceived distance to their abstract meaning (H1b). Above that, the results indicate that physical proximity to symbolic objects increases a subject’s perceived self-extension (H2a) and behavioral intentions (H3a) towards their meaning. Most importantly, results further support the hypotheses that these influences of objects only account for such objects that are symbolically connected to the concerned meaning (H4a, H4b, and H4c). 72 Further support for the interaction of physical proximity and symbolic connection derives from the conducted comparisons between the baseline condition without object and the experimental conditions with objects. Results revealed that the utilized tennis balls only affected the observed dependent variables when they were (1) symbolically connected to Wimbledon and (2) physically close to subjects. If one of these two properties was not given, the tennis balls were not found to exhibit an influence on the observed dependent variables that exceeded the baseline measurements without any object at all. The findings accordingly support the here underlying argumentation that symbolic objects actually transfer their immaterial meaning into the direct experienceable world and therefore reduce the distance to that meaning. At the same time, the findings oppose the previously discussed alternative explanation that the observed effects of object presence / proximity result from a solely affective reaction towards a physically close reference point. The results of experiment 3 further support the notion that an object’s influence on a subject’s self-extension towards its meaning is mediated by the subject’s perceived distance to that meaning (H2b). Findings further reveal that this mediation is moderated by the object’s symbolic connection to the meaning (H4b). Moreover, the results indicate that the object’s influence on behavioral intentions is mediated by a subject’s perceived distance and self-extension to that meaning (H3b). The findings do hence not only support a direct impact of an object on these self- and behavior-related constructs; they further support the predicted process-related, indirect causes that underlie this impact. Specifically the results support the notion that symbolic objects do not only reduce the perceived distance to a certain meaning. By doing so, they enhance a subject’s self-extension towards that meaning. This self-extension again influences a subject’s behavior / behavioral intentions towards that meaning. However, results did not completely support hypotheses 4a, 4b, and 4c. Physical proximity to the tennis ball with a strong symbolic connection to Wimbledon only led to marginally lower perceived distance to Wimbledon compared to the moderatesymbolic ball. Further, no significant differences between these two tennis balls were found concerning their effects on self-extension and behavioral intentions towards Wimbledon. One possible reason for this could be that the effect of objects on perceived distance and self-extension towards their meaning is limited. It is possible that an increase in connection strength between an object and meaning only influences the observed measures until a certain maximum level. In this case, the connection between the moderate-symbolic ball and Wimbledon might already have been too strong to observe an even larger influence of the strong-symbolic ball. A further 73 possibility is that this finding is a result of this particular manipulation of the symbolic connection between object and meaning. Although there is no evidence, it is possible that participants did not believe that the ball had actually been played at Wimbledon. Further, it is possible that the manipulation within the main experiment was not strong enough to have people actually realize that the ball had been played at Wimbledon and therefore was physically connected to Wimbledon. At the same time, the similar results for the moderate- and strong-symbolic ball also support a theoretically relevant argument of this dissertation (see section 2.2.3). As just outlined, the ball that was said to have been played at Wimbledon only led to marginally lower perceived distances towards Wimbledon (in the close ball condition) than the ball that was told to be an unplayed official Wimbledon ball. Simultaneously, the results of the pretest indicate that this unplayed official Wimbledon ball was perceived to have an indexical connection to the tournament. Hence, although participants knew that the ball was unplayed they drew a physical connection between the ball and Wimbledon: the purely symbolic connection between ball and Wimbledon was accordingly transferred into the perception that the ball was factually connected to its meaning. Regarding the conceptual foundation of this dissertation, this finding is relevant for the following reason: it supports the notion that the crucial quality of symbolic objects in serving a self-extending function is not their actual physical connection to the symbolized meaning but the mental projection of that connection onto the object. Building upon these findings, experiment 4 aims at further investigating the effect of the symbolic connection strength between an object and its meaning on subjects’ perceived self-extension and behavior towards that meaning. At the same time, study 4 is designed as a field experiment in a realistic survey setting. This is particularly done to test the generalizability of the previous findings to real-life circumstances. 74 3.5 The Club Card Experiment (Study 4) 3.5.1 Overview The purpose of study 4 was to retest the main findings of studies 1-3 in a more realistic field setting and to gain more reliable data on the behavioral and thus economic consequences of the previously identified effects. The study was particularly designed to test in which way a symbolic object affects subjects’ perceived self-extension (H2a) as well as behavior (H3a) towards an institution that is symbolically connected to that object. The study further retested in which way these effects depend on the symbolic connection between the object and that specific meaning (H4b and H4c). The underlying and previously supported effect of objects on perceived distances towards abstract meanings was disregarded in this study for multiple reasons: chiefly, this was decided as the main interest of this study advanced from understanding the psychological and process-related effects of symbolic objects to assessing their impact on self- and behavior-related outcomes. Further, it was excluded because the measure of perceived distance was difficult to apply to the particular study design of experiment 4 and might have irritated participants. Last, it was excluded because study 3 had supported the hypothesis that the effect of object proximity on the here investigated dependent measures was mediated by perceived distance to the object’s symbolic meaning. Hence, in favor of a more outcome orientated study design, it was decided to disregard this mediation effect in experiment 4. According to these goals, a crucial element of the development of experiment 4 was to find an appropriate, realistic study setting that allowed for the necessary experimental manipulations. The study was conducted in agreement with the Marketing Club of the University of St. Gallen that was planning a relaunch after it had been inactive for several years. The experiment was masked to be a survey among students that measured the general interest of students in such a club. The study used a one-factorial between-subjects design with three different object manipulations. In one third of the inquiry participants were exposed to a plastic membership card that was attached to the questionnaire (strong connection to club); in one third of the inquiry participants were exposed to an attached plastic membership voucher (weak connection to club); and in one third of the inquiry participants were solely exposed to a logo of the club that looked similar to the tested cards (no object baseline condition; see section 3.5.3 for a detailed discussion of the manipulation of the independent variable). 75 Plastic cards were used as objects because they are prevalent in everyday life and accordingly well known. They were further used as they closely match the here underlying definition of objects and because they could easily be integrated into the study design. According to hypotheses 4b and 4c and in line with the findings of study 3, it was assumed that compared to the baseline condition particularly the strong-symbolic card would exert a positive influence on the observed self- and behavior-related measures. The utilized membership card was not only easily graspable; it further objectified a club membership and therefore strongly related to the club. 3.5.2 Participants and Procedure 302 students (69% males; mean age: 22 years) from the University of St. Gallen participated in the study. The high proportion of male participants reflects the gender distribution at this particular university. Six participants (1.99% of total participants) were excluded from the analyses because their answering patterns strongly diverged from the majority of participants. Answers of excluded participants differed more than two standard deviations from group means for at least one of the observed dependent variables. Participation in the study was voluntary and participants did not receive any incentives. The mean participation time was less than 10 minutes. Bachelor students of a mandatory course were approached during group assignments for which they separated to various working rooms. Each working group randomly received paper and pen questionnaires of the same experimental condition. This procedure was chosen to ensure that participants did not notice that questionnaires differed as regards the object manipulation.20 Students were informed that the study was conducted to assess their interest in a planned marketing club. In the first part of the study, subjects received a short portrait of the club. After this description, they were exposed to one of the three stimuli. Thereafter, they rated their perceived self-extension to the club as well as their interest in becoming a member of the club. Additionally, they rated their willingness to pay for a membership and they had the opportunity to indicate their email address if they wished to be updated on the club. Information on whether subjects specified their email address was treated as a dependent measure. Subsequently, participants 20 Multilevel analysis that controlled for random intercepts of each working group showed no significant effects for any of the observed dependent variables. Estimated effect sizes for the random intercepts for each variable lay below .08 (p > .38). Therefore, the group information was excluded from the main analyses. 76 completed a series of manipulation checks as well as questions regarding their gender and age. Finally, subjects were thanked for participation. Figure 3-15 depicts the overall procedure of experiment 4. 1. Introduction to Study Participants receive information that the study is about the Marketing Club St. Gallen 2. Exposure to Portrait of the Club Participants read some basic information about the club (see section 3.5.3) 3. Object Manipulation Subsequent to the club portrait, participants are exposed to a membership card (strong-symbolic) / a membership voucher (weak-symbolic) / the club logo (no object baseline condition) (see section 3.5.3) 4. Measurement of Dependent Variables Participants indicate their perceived self-extension and behavioral intentions regarding the club (see section 3.5.4) 5. Measurement of Manipulation Checks and Control Variables Participants indicate their perception of the symbolic connection between the card and the club membership. They further indicate their gender and age (see section 3.5.5) Figure 3-15: Study 4 / Design / Procedure of Experiment 3.5.3 Manipulation of Independent Variable The introduction of the study informed students that its purpose was to assess their interest in a planned marketing club of the University of St. Gallen. Subjects subsequently read a short portrait of the planned club (see figure 3-16, p. 77). Following this portrait, subjects were exposed either to one of two symbolic objects or to a no object manipulation. The latter served as a baseline condition. As previously mentioned, plastic cards were chosen for the object manipulations. The main goal that underlay the design of the card manipulations was to vary the strength of the symbolic connection that was perceived between the card and the marketing club while at the same time keeping the visual and haptic information as similar as possible. Similarly, the baseline condition was designed to be possibly similar to the two card conditions while not including a graspable object. 77 Figure 3-16: Study 4 / Design / Portrait Marketing Club 78 Accordingly, in the strong-symbolic object condition the plastic card was framed as a membership card21 (see figure 3-17). It hence symbolized a membership within the club and was directly linked to being a part of the club. The plastic card was attached to the questionnaire. To emphasize the symbolic connection between the card and the club, the following text informed subjects about the card: Members of the club will receive the following membership card. The card will only be given to members and identifies them as part of the community. Owners of this card are part of the marketing club: Figure 3-17: Study 4 / Design / Picture of Utilized Membership Card In the weak-symbolic object condition, participants were exposed to the same plastic card, but in this condition it was described as a membership voucher22 (see figure 3-18). As a membership voucher, the card was also symbolically connected to the club. In contrast to the membership card that symbolized being a member of the club, the voucher, however, oppositely symbolized not yet being a member of the club. The plastic card was attached to the questionnaire. To emphasize this weak-symbolic connection between the card and the club, the following text informed subjects about the card: 21 22 As the study was conducted in German the card carried the imprint “Mitgliedskarte” (Membership Card) As the study was conducted in German the card carried the imprint “Gutschein” (Voucher) 79 The following membership voucher will be given to students at the beginning of their studies at the University of St. Gallen. Students can use this voucher to become a member of the marketing club: Figure 3-18: Study 4 / Design / Picture of Utilized Membership Voucher In the baseline condition participants were exposed to the brand logo of the club (see figure 3-19). The logo was depicted similarly to the alternatively attached plastic cards. It was printed in the same size and shown in the same position as the plastic cards. The logo was neutrally introduced with the following text: The following brand logo has been created for the Marketing Club St. Gallen: Figure 3-19: Study 4 / Design / Logo of Marketing Club St. Gallen 3.5.4 Operationalization of Dependent Variables Subsequent to the above outlined object manipulation, participants completed the dependent measures. The section containing the dependent measures was introduced by the following information: 80 In the following section, we would like to ask you a few questions regarding your opinion of the Marketing Club St. Gallen. Building upon your answers, we want to assess the general interest of students in such a club. In the first measurement section, subjects rated their perceived self-extension to the club. Perceived self-extension was measured with the same seven items used in study 3 (see Escalas and Bettman 2003; 2005). Items were slightly adapted to the study context: The Marketing Club St. Gallen reflects who I am / I can identify with the Marketing Club St. Gallen / I feel a personal connection to the Marketing Club St. Gallen / A membership in the Marketing Club St. Gallen would match the image that I want to communicate to others / A membership in the Marketing Club St. Gallen would help me become the person I want to be / The Marketing Club St. Gallen is similar to me / The Marketing Club St. Gallen suits me well. Responses were given on 7-point scales reaching from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). The items were averaged to a single measure of perceived self-extension (α = .94) with low values indicating weak self-extension and high values indicating strong self-extension to the Marketing Club. After measuring perceived self-extension to the Marketing Club, subjects’ interest and intention in becoming a member of the club was measured on four items (anchored with strongly disagree (1) and strongly agree (7)): I can easily imagine becoming a member of the Marketing Club St. Gallen / I would like to participate in the activities offered by the Marketing Club St. Gallen / I would like to become a member of the Marketing Club St. Gallen / I am very confident that I will become a member of the Marketing Club St. Gallen. The four items were averaged to a single measure of the intention to become a member (α = .95) with low values indicating weak intentions and high values indicating strong intentions. Additionally, subjects were asked to indicate how much they would be willing to pay for a one-year club-membership. As for the other measures, seven answer options were given that ranged from zero Swiss France (1) to more than 60 Swiss Francs (7) in incremental steps of 10 Swiss Francs. At the end of the study participants had the opportunity to indicate their email address if they wished to be informed about the club in the future. As previously mentioned, the information of whether subjects provided their email address or not was further utilized as a dependent measure in this study. 81 3.5.5 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables To test the intended manipulation of the symbolic connection between the card and the club, subjects were asked to indicate their agreement to the following statement at the end of the study (anchored with strongly disagree (1) and strongly agree (7)): The membership card / membership voucher symbolizes the membership of its owner in the Marketing Club St. Gallen. To control for characteristics of the utilized sample, subjects were further asked to indicate their gender and age. 3.5.6 Results Manipulation Check A univariate ANOVA indicated that the strong-symbolic membership card (M = 5.62) was perceived to be connected significantly stronger to a membership in the marketing club than the weak-symbolic membership voucher (M = 3.35; F(1, 195) = 86.07, p < .01). Further analyses revealed that the mean value for the strong-symbolic card was significantly above the mid-value (4) of the scale (t(95) = 10.57, p < .01), indicating that participants in general agreed to the statement that the membership card reflected being a member of the club. At the same time, the mean value for the weaksymbolic card was significantly below the mid-value (4) of the scale (t(100) = -3.46, p < .01), indicating that subjects in general disagreed to the statement that the membership voucher reflected being a member of the club. Therefore, not only did the two stimuli significantly differ in their associated connection to a membership in the club. The opposite orientation of both mean values to the two scale ends also indicates that participants recognized that the membership card symbolized a membership while the voucher actually symbolized the opposite, not being a member. Testing of Hypotheses 2a and 4b One purpose of study 4 was to test the influence of object presence on subjects’ perceived self-extension towards the object’s symbolized meaning (H2a). The study particularly aimed at testing the relevance of the strength of the symbolic connection between an object and meaning as regards the objects’ ability to foster feelings of selfextension towards its meaning (H4b). It was predicted that perceived self-extension to the marketing club would be higher in the strong-symbolic card condition than in the weak-symbolic card and no card condition. Further, it was predicted that there would be no or only weak difference between the observed measures for the weak-symbolic card and the no card condition. 82 An ANOVA supported the predicted outcome regarding perceived self-extension. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of the object manipulation (F(2, 293) = 7.46, p < .01). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) further revealed a significant difference between the strong-symbolic card (M = 4.23) and the no card condition (M = 3.53; t(293) = 3.86, p < .01) as well as between the strong-symbolic card (M = 4.23) and the weak-symbolic card condition (M = 3.87; t(293) = 2.00, p = .046). Further comparisons revealed a marginally significant difference between the weak-symbolic card (M = 3.87) and the no card condition (M = 3.53; t(293) = 1.89, p = .059). Figure 3-20 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for perceived selfextension to the marketing club for each condition. Figure 3-20: Study 4 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Marketing Club Testing of Hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 4c (Intention to Become Club Member) A further ANOVA supported the predicted outcome concerning subjects’ intention to become a member of the marketing club. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of the object manipulation (F(2, 293) = 8.81, p < .01). Pairwise comparisons (LSD) more specifically revealed a significant difference between the strong-symbolic card (M = 4.94) and the no card condition (M = 4.00; t(293) = 4.16, p < .01) as well as between the strong-symbolic card (M = 4.94) and the weak-symbolic card condition (M = 4.34; t(293) = 2.66, p < .01). No significant difference was found between the weak-symbolic card condition (M = 4.34) and the no card condition (M = 4.00; 83 t(293) = 1.53, p = .13). Figure 3-21 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ intention to become a club member in each condition. Figure 3-21: Study 4 / Results / Intention to Become a Club Member Further, regression analyses were conducted to test whether the effect of the object manipulation on intention to become a member was mediated by perceived selfextension to the club. The no object condition served as a baseline measure. Two dummy variables were created for the weak-symbolic and for the strong-symbolic card conditions to separately contrast the effect of their presence against the baseline condition (card not present = 0 / card present = 1; see Hayes and Preacher 2013). In line with the previously reported analysis, presence of the strong-symbolic card was found to be a significant predictor of the intention to become a member (βstrong = .94, p < .01) while presence of the weak-symbolic card had no significant impact on the intention to become a member (βweak = .34, p = .13). The analysis further revealed a significant effect for the strong-symbolic card (βstrong = .70, p < .01) and a marginally significant effect for the weak-symbolic card (βweak = .34, p = .06) on the perceived self-extension to the club. Perceived self-extension to the club was further found to be a significant predictor of the intention to become a club member (β = 1.04, p < .01). Lastly, when both independent dummy variables as well as perceived self-extension to the club were included in the regression model, the mediator remained a significant predictor of the intention to become a member (β = 1.02, p < .01), while the impact of 84 the strong-symbolic object was considerably reduced to being marginally significant (βstrong = .22, p = .10) and the impact of the weak-symbolic card stayed insignificant while being further lowered (-.01; p = .96). Based on these analyses, effect sizes were estimated that contrasted the indirect effects of the two object conditions against the no object baseline condition. Estimates were tested for reliability using the 95% bootstrap confidence intervals provided by the deployed SPSS PROCESS macro (Model 4; Hayes 2013; 2012; Hayes and Preacher 2013). Results revealed that the indirect effect of the strong symbolic card (.72 with a CI of .38 to 1.09) was significantly larger than the indirect effect in the baseline condition. In contrast, the determined indirect effect for the weak-symbolic card condition was found to be not reliable as the estimated confidence interval included the value zero (.35 with a CI of -.05 to .70). Figure 3-22 depicts the results of the mediation analysis. Figure 3-22: Study 4 / Results / Mediation Analysis Testing of Hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 4c (Willingness to Pay) Study 4 also tested whether the two utilized objects would predict subjects’ willingness to pay for a one-year club-membership. An ANOVA indicated a marginally insignificant overall effect of object presence (F(2, 293) = 2.27, p = .11). However, pairwise comparisons (LSD) indicated the predicted result patterns. The lowest willingness to pay was measured for the no object condition with a mean of 85 23.23 Swiss Francs. Subjects in the weak-symbolic card condition were willing to pay 24.65 Swiss Francs on average and subjects in the strong-symbolic card condition 28.23 Swiss Francs. A comparison between the strong-symbolic card and no card condition indicated a significant difference between the specified values (t(293) = 2.07, p = .04). All other comparisons were insignificant with p-values above .14. Figure 3-23 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ willingness to pay in each condition. Figure 3-23: Study 4 / Results / Willingness to Pay for Membership Analog to the above reported mediation analysis for intention to become a club member a further mediation analysis was conducted for willingness to pay. The results of both analyses revealed similar patterns. Again, the no card condition was chosen as the baseline condition. The weak-symbolic card condition showed no direct effect on willingness to pay (βweak = 1.42, p = .55). The strong-symbolic card however had a significant impact on willingness to pay (βstrong = 5.00, p = .04). Estimated effects of both card conditions on perceived self-extension to the club had the previously reported sizes and p-values (βstrong = .70, p < .01; βweak = .34, p = .06). Perceived selfextension to the club was further found to be a significant predictor of subjects’ willingness to pay (β = 6.34, p < .01). Lastly, when both dummy variables as well as perceived self-extension to the club were included in the regression model the mediator remained a significant predictor of willingness to pay (β = 6.30, p < .01), 86 while the impact of the strong-symbolic object was eliminated (βstrong = .58, p = .79) and the effect of the weak symbolic card remained insignificant (βweak = -.72, p = .73). The indirect effect for the strong-symbolic card condition was estimated to be significantly above zero (4.42 with a CI of 2.17 to 7.17). Results further revealed an insignificant indirect effect for the weak symbolic card condition (2.14 with a CI of -.15 to 4.45). Testing of Hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 4c (Email Address Specification) Study 4 utilized the information of whether participants specified their email address at the end of the study as a measure of actual behavior towards the club. Figure 3-24 compares the number of cases in which subjects specified and did not specify their email address for each group. In the no object baseline condition 26% (26/99) of subjects specified their email address; in the weak-symbolic card condition 40% (40/101) specified their address; and in the strong-symbolic card condition, 51% (49/96) of subjects specified their address. Figure 3-24: Study 4 / Results / Number of Specified Email Addresses Due to the dichotomous state of the measure email address specification (no email = 0 / email = 1), a logistic regression was applied to estimate the impact of the two card conditions on address specification. As in the previous analyses, the no object condition was used as baseline measure. 87 The omnibus test of the logistic regression model indicated that object presence was a significant predictor of the amount of specified email addresses (χ2(2) = 12.84, p < .01; Cox and Snell R2 = .04, Nagelkerke R2 = .06). Results further indicated that compared to the no object baseline condition, the weak-symbolic card (βweak = .61, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 3.98, p = .05) as well as the strong-symbolic card (βstrong = 1.07, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 12.29, p < .01) had a significantly positive impact on email address specification. A marginally insignificant difference was found between the weak- and strong-symbolic card conditions (β = .46, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 2.59, p = .11). Additionally estimated odds ratios indicated that the probability of email address specification was 1.84 times higher in the weak-symbolic card condition than in the no object condition. At the same time, the probability of email address specification was 2.93 times higher in the strong-symbolic card condition compared to the no object condition. Analog to the above reported mediation analyses it was further tested whether the influence of object presence on email address specification was mediated by subjects’ perceived self-extension to the club. Again, the no card condition served as baseline measure. The above reported logistic regressions supported a direct effect of both utilized cards on email address specification. A further analysis supported a significant influence of perceived self-extension to the club on email address specification (β = 1.19, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 63.80, p < .01). When both dummy variables as well as perceived self-extension were included into the regression model the mediator remained a significant predictor of email address specification (β = 1.15, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 60.02, p < .01), while the impact of the strong-symbolic object was reduced (βstrong = .69, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 3.71, p = .05) and the effect of the weak-symbolic card was eliminated (βweak = .43, Wald χ2(1, N = 296) = 1.43, p = .23). The indirect effects for the strong-symbolic card (.81 with CI of .34 to 1.34) as well as for the weak-symbolic card (.39 with CI of .02 to .87) were estimated to be significantly above zero. 3.5.7 Discussion The purpose of study 4 was to retest the main findings of studies 1-3 in a realistic field setting and to gain more reliable data on the behavioral and hence economic consequences of the previously identified effects. The study was particularly designed to test in which way a symbolic object affected subjects’ perceived self-extension (H2a) as well as behavior (H3a) towards an institution that was symbolically connected to that object. The study further tested in which way these effects depended 88 on the symbolic connection between the object and that specific meaning (H4b, H4c). Further, the study tested whether the object’s influence on behavior (behavioral intentions) was mediated by subjects’ perceived self-extension to the object’s meaning (H3b). The results of experiment 4 mainly support the tested hypotheses. Results indicate a significant impact of the strong-symbolic membership card on subjects’ perceived selfextension to the presented marketing club as well as on subjects’ behavioral intentions and actual behavior towards the club: subjects felt a stronger self-extension to the club when exposed to the membership card compared to when being exposed to no object. Subjects further showed higher intentions to become a member of the club and were willing to pay more for a club membership when the strong-symbolic card was presented to them compared to when no object was presented to them. Further, participants in the strong-symbolic card condition were more likely to specify their email address at the end of the study than participants in the no object condition were. Results further support stronger self- and behavior-related influences of the strongsymbolic card compared to the weak-symbolic card: subjects’ perceived self-extension to the club and their intention to become a club member was significantly higher when they were exposed to the strong-symbolic card compared to when they were exposed to the weak-symbolic card. Although not significant, comparisons between these groups showed similar trends for subjects’ willingness to pay and their likelihood to specify their email address. As predicted, less pronounced results were found for the weak-symbolic membership voucher. Results indicated a marginally positive impact of the weak-symbolic card on subjects’ perceived self-extension to the club (in contrast to the no object baseline condition). No significant impact of this card was found for subjects’ intention to become a club member and for their willingness to pay for a club membership. The weak-symbolic card manipulation did however result in a significantly higher likelihood of subjects to indicate their email addresses. Summarizing these findings, experiment 4 replicated the previously identified effects of symbolic objects on subjects’ perceived self-extension and behavior towards an abstract meaning in a realistic field setting. Moreover, results of experiment 4 support the notion that the impact of symbolic objects on these dependent measures increases with an upsurge in the strength of the symbolic connection between object and meaning. 89 Building upon these empirical findings, study 5 aimed at further generalizing the revealed effect of object presence on subjects’ self-extension to a symbolized meaning. This aim was approached by conducting an experiment that referred to a variety of products and services that participants actually consumed. 3.6 The Product- versus Service-Brands Experiment (Study 5) 3.6.1 Overview The main purpose of study 5 was to further generalize the findings of studies 1-4 in two ways. First, it was aimed at testing the previously supported effect of graspable objects on subjects’ perceived self-extension to a certain symbolized meaning using a range of various actual personal possessions. Secondly, by doing so, it was aimed at transferring the study design into a broader economic context. Regarding these goals, an online study was conducted that utilized two basic economic concepts that relate to the here investigated research question. First, the study built on the concept of brands as intangible meanings that are mentally connected by consumer to economic offerings (see e.g. Escalas and Bettman 2005 as well as Chapter 1). Secondly, the study built upon the concepts of products and services as a prominent differentiation between tangible and intangible economic goods (Zeithaml, Parasuraman, and Berry 1985, p. 33). Connecting these two concepts to the conceptual model of this dissertation, it was predicted that, on average, consumers would perceive stronger self-extensions to brands that were symbolically connected to graspable products that they owned compared to brands that were symbolically connected to ungraspable services that they regularly consumed (H2a). Study 5 aimed at testing this prediction. The study was conducted online for three reasons: first, the online setting enabled subjects to participate in anonymous and familiar surroundings; secondly, a computer aided survey system was better suited to realize the developed study design than a paper and pen setup; thirdly, online data collection eased reaching a wide variety of consumers. 90 3.6.2 Participants and Procedure 154 subjects (53% males; mean age: 40 years23) participated in the study. Subjects belonged to a German and Swiss consumer sample. 15 subjects (9.74% of total participants) were excluded from the analyses due to invalid responses (see section 3.6.6 for further information). Participation in the study was voluntary and participants did not receive any incentives. The mean participation time was less than 10 minutes. Subjects were contacted via email and asked to participate in a study concerning consumer behavior. The email included a link that directed them to the study. Having arrived at the survey site, participants were randomly assigned to a product or service condition. They were then asked to think of a product that they owned or a service that they regularly used. Subsequently, they were asked to specify the brand that was associated with that product (service). After indicating the brand name of the product (service), subjects were forwarded to the measurement of perceived self-extension towards that brand. Thereafter, subjects made further specifications about various manipulation checks and control variables. Finally, subjects were thanked for participation. Figure 3-25 depicts the overall procedure of study 5. 1. Introduction to Study Participants receive information that the study is about products / services 2. Manipulation of Object Presence Participants choose a product that they own / a service that they regularly use and specify the brand that relates to this product / service (see section 3.6.3) 3. Measurement of Dependent Variable Measurement of perceived self-extension to the product- / service-brand (see section 3.6.4) 4. Measurement of Manipulation Checks and Control Variables Participants indicate the perceived graspability of the product / service, the amount of usage of the product / service, and whether they have a special relationship to the brand of the product / service. They further indicate their gender and age (see section 3.6.5) Figure 3-25: Study 5 / Design / Procedure of Experiment 3.6.3 Manipulation of Independent Variable Participants were randomly assigned to the product or service condition when arriving at the survey site. Depending on which condition they were drawn to, they were either informed that the study was about products that they owned or services that they 23 Age was not surveyed in this study; it was estimated based on information from studies that had previously been conducted with similar consumer samples. 91 regularly used. Participants in the product condition read the following instruction: We would like you to think of a product that you own and regularly use. Once you have thought of a product, please specify the name of the company that produces this product in the following text box. Alternatively, participants in the service condition read the following instruction: We would like you to think of a service that you regularly access from the same company. Once you have thought of a service, please specify the name of the company that provides this service in the following text box. Participants were explicitly asked to specify the name of the company and not the brand as a pretest had revealed that subjects were insecure when asked to name the brand or corporate brand of the product (service). A check of the specified company names however indicated that they corresponded to the names of the affiliated brands. Participants subsequently advanced to the measurement of self-extension to the brand. 3.6.4 Operationalization of Dependent Variable The measurement of self-extension to the brand was introduced by the following instruction (the company name that subjects had specified previously was inserted into this instruction as well as into the measurement items below): Please take another moment to think about the product (service) of ___ as well as the brand ___. To what extent would you agree or disagree with the following statements about the brand ___? Subjects then rated their perceived self-extension to the brand of the product (service). Perceived self-extension was measured with the same seven items used in studies 3 and 4 (see Escalas and Bettman 2003; 2005). Items were slightly adapted to the study context: Brand ___ reflects who I am / I can identify with brand ___ / I feel a personal connection to brand ___ / Brand ___ matches the image that I want to communicate to others / Brand ___ is an important part of me / Brand ___ is similar to me / Brand ___ suits me well. Responses were given on 7-point scales reaching from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). The items were averaged to a single measure of perceived self-extension (α = .92) with low values indicating weak and high values indicating strong self-extension to the brand. 3.6.5 Manipulation Checks and Control Variables To control whether the named products and services actually differed regarding their graspability, subjects were asked to respond to the following item at the end of the study: How graspable / tangible do you perceive the product (service) to be? (not 92 graspable at all (1), very graspable (7)). Further, participants were asked to indicate how often they used the product (service) on a 7-point scale ranging from rarely (1) to very often (7). To further control for how often subjects were physically close to the product (service), they were also asked to indicate how often they touched the product / used the service on average. Answers were given on a 7-point scale using the following intervals: less than once a month (1) / once a month (2) / every second week (3) / once a week (4) / twice a week (5) / every second day (6) / every day (7). Furthermore, subjects were asked whether they had a special relationship to the company / brand they had named that exceeded the usual relationship of a consumer. If yes, subjects were asked to describe briefly the relationship. Finally, subjects indicated their gender and age. 3.6.6 Results Manipulation and Confound Checks When asked to specify the company name associated with the thought of products (services), subjects particularly named product companies such as electronics-, car-, watch-, jewelry-, and apparel-manufacturers. Named service companies referred to transportation (especially airlines)-, telecommunication-, media-, finance-, logistics-, retail-, hair and cosmetics-, as well as sports (a dancing school)-, and internet-services. Four subjects were excluded from the analysis due to invalid company specifications. An additional 11 subjects were excluded as they indicated to have relationships to the named companies that exceeded usual consumption relationships.24 A univariate ANOVA indicated that the considered products were perceived as significantly more graspable (M = 6.73) than the considered services (M = 5.21; F(1, 137) = 36.58, p < .01). Further, participants indicated to use the specified products (M = 6.51) more often than the specified services (M = 5.50; F(1, 137) = 26.06, p < .01). On average, respondents indicated to touch the named products about every day to every second day. The thought of services were indicated to be used once to twice a week on average. Accordingly, participants not only perceived the products to be more graspable than the services; they also perceived to use and be close to them more often. Results hence indicate that the thought of products were (at least consciously) stronger connected to subjects’ everyday lives than the thought of services. 24 Specified relationships were: I have friends who work at the company (4x) / I have worked for the company (2x) / My spouse works for the company (2x) / I have worked for the company and still have friends who work there / I know the family well who owns the company / Friends of mine play soccer for the team that is run by the company. 93 Testing of Hypothesis 2a The purpose of study 5 was to test how strongly subjects connected to brands (intangible meanings) that were symbolically connected to either graspable economic goods (products) or less graspable economic goods (services) that they consumed on a regular basis. According to hypothesis 2a, it was assumed that perceived self-extension to an associated brand would be stronger for products than for services. Results support the predicted outcome. A conducted ANOVA revealed a significantly higher perceived self-extension to the specified product brands (M = 3.02) compared to the specified service brands (M = 2.47; F(1, 137) = 4.57, p = .03). Figure 3-26 depicts the mean values and confidence intervals (95%) for subjects’ perceived selfextension to the named product- versus service-brands. Figure 3-26: Study 5 / Results / Perceived Self-Extension to Brands 3.6.7 Discussion The main purpose of experiment 5 was to further generalize the findings of the previous experiments by transferring its study design to a broader economic context. Particularly, the study aimed at testing whether the previously observed influence of objects on consumers’ perceived self-extension to an abstract meaning (H2a) could be replicated using a wide range of graspable and less graspable economic goods that consumers related to. Specifically, it tested whether subjects perceived a stronger self- 94 extension to brands of products that they owned than to brand of services they regularly used. The results support the predicted outcome. Subjects indicated to perceive stronger selfextension towards brands of the specified products than to brands of the specified services. An interesting aspect of this finding is that a substantial proportion of the named services are services that usually accompany a personal interaction or even personal relationship to the involved service provider. This accounts for relatively anonymous services such as the named transportation (especially airline) services or the specified financial (banking) services. However, it particularly accounts for somewhat personal services such as hair stylists, massage or cosmetic studios as well as for the dancing school that one subject specified. Although service literature argues that personal contact between employees and consumers is suitable for supporting consumers’ relationships to companies and their brands, the results of this study indicate a stronger connection for consumers to the brands of impersonal products they are surrounded with. An important constraint of study 5 is the vague operationalization of the graspability of economic goods by an approximate comparison of products and services. Overall differences between products and services offer many confounding causes for the here observed findings. Already the conducted control measures show that it was not only the graspability that subjects perceived to be different between products and services. Results further indicate that products were also perceived to be used more often and to be closer to everyday life than the specified services. However, as the study aimed at generalizing the results of the previous experimental findings, this shortcoming is accepted because of this particular study aim. These possible confounding effects have to be kept in mind though when interpreting the results of this particular study. At the same time, the just mentioned control measures offer further informative insights into possible differences between products and services and hence into potential differences between graspable and less graspable goods. As mentioned, products were not only perceived to be more graspable than services; they were also perceived to be used more often and to be closer to subjects’ everyday lives. This particular result emphasizes the notion that the graspability of a possession might not solely affect self-extension to a certain meaning because it objectifies this meaning. The results further indicate that through this objectification and its ownership the possession and as such its meaning become a more prominent component of everyday life and therefore physically and psychologically move closer to the owner. More concrete, a shoe of a certain brand might not only increase the perceived self-extension 95 of its owner to that brand because the shoe objectifies this relationship. It might further increase this self-extension because by being objective and being worn it becomes a more prominent material part of this person’s everyday surrounding. This notion closely relates to the here investigated hypotheses 1a and 1b. While hypothesis 1a conceptualizes the basic effect of an object’s presence, namely the existence of that object, hypothesis 1b conceptualizes the particular relevance of the physical distance of that object. Conceptually, these two aspects have been contemplated separately in the previous studies: as such, the previous studies manipulated object presence and further refined object presence through object proximity / distance. The findings of study 5, however, indicate that both aspects closely correspond. Hence, owning an object seems not only to influence the objective constitution of that object’s symbolized meaning (H1a); by owning the object it further becomes a more prominent (and therefore closer) part of everyday life (H1b). 96 4 General Discussion The following research question was defined in the introductory chapter of this dissertation and served as its guideline: How does the graspability of symbolic objects relate to and influence human selfextension towards abstract meanings? In addressing this question, this dissertation aimed particularly at two goals: (1) the development of a revised conceptual understanding of the self-extension function of possessions that accounts for their graspability; (2) the creation and analysis of empirical data that is suitable to investigate the relevance of the graspability of symbolic objects for their self-extension function. This chapter will recap and densify the theoretical and empirical findings of this dissertation with particular regard to this question and these aims. In the first section, a consolidating overview of the main findings will be given. That section will be followed by an outline of the theoretical and managerial contributions that derive from these findings. Limitations of the overall project and the empirical studies will be discussed subsequently. This report of limitations will be followed by an outline of possible future research directions that arise from the work on and findings of this project. 4.1 Overall Findings According to the above highlighted research question and research aims, the theoretical chapter of this dissertation approached a revised conceptual understanding of the self-extension function of possessions. The basis of its development was the proposition to tie the definition of possessions and objects to the bodily and perceptual properties of humans (section 2.2.1). An object was accordingly defined as an entity that is perceived to be physically graspable. Building upon this understanding, it was further argued that humans use such objects as symbols for abstract meanings (sections 2.2.2 and 2.2.3). Furthermore, it was postulated that humans utilize these symbolic objects to physically relate their immaterial meaning to their body and accordingly to their perception of self (sections 2.2.4). Building upon construal-level theory, it was argued that the proximity of a symbolic object results in feelings of increased proximity to its meaning (section 2.3.2). It was further argued that this perceived proximity results in stronger self-extension and increased behavioral intentions of subjects towards that meaning (sections 2.3.3 and 2.3.4). 97 By connecting previous research on possessions and their symbolic meaning to construal-level theory, this dissertation therefore developed a refined approach towards understanding the psychological mechanisms that underlie the self-extension phenomenon. Unlike existing approaches that particularly focus on the connection between objects and their meanings, this revised perspective concentrates on the physical connection between symbolic objects and humans. Accordingly, the importance of the physical existence of objective symbols is here not only seen in their particular connection to the signified meaning but in the perceived proximity that they create between their adherent, mentally created meaning and the body and mind of a subject. Building upon this understanding of symbolic objects, the third chapter of this dissertation outlined five empirical studies that were conducted to test this conceptual framework. Accordingly, these studies further served the second specified goal of this dissertation: to investigate empirically the relevance of the graspability of objects for their self-extension function. Study 1 used a realistic experimental study design to test the basic hypothesis that the presence of an object reduces subjects’ perceived distance to its symbolized meaning. Results of study 1 revealed that subjects felt significantly closer to a personal experience when paying attention to a physically close object that was connected to this experience than when solely thinking of the experience (H1a). Results of study 1 hence support this essential notion of the developed framework. Study 2 further refined this finding in a laboratory context. Subjects mentally connected a personal experience to a graspable object. In two conditions, the object was placed directly in front of subjects and they were either allowed to or not allowed to touch it. In a third condition, the object was placed at 4-meter distance to subjects and was therefore out of reach. Results revealed that subjects’ spatial distance to the object corresponded to their perceived distance to the object’s symbolized meaning (H1b). Further, the results countered two possible alternative explanations for this finding. First, results indicated that the effect was independent of a more vivid representation of the abstract meaning caused by the object, because subjects saw the object in all three conditions. Secondly, results indicated that the found effect of physical proximity of the object did not confound with touching the object. Study 3 indicated that the physical proximity of an object only affected perceived distance to the object’s meaning if object and meaning were symbolically connected (H1a, H1b, and H4a). Weak support was found for the assumption that the effect of 98 object proximity on the perceived closeness of its meaning was more pronounced for strong than for moderate object-meaning connections (H4a). Both findings are important as regards the conceptual framework of this dissertation. They support the notion that an object’s effect is not simply caused by an affective reaction of subjects to its proximity but by a perceived transfer of its abstract meaning into the experienceable world. Study 3 further supports the notion that physically close symbolic objects not only reduce the perceived distance to their abstract meaning but by doing so also increase subjects’ perceived self-extension (H2a, H2b, and H4b) and behavioral intentions towards this meaning (H3a, H3b, and H4c). This finding is particularly important concerning the underlying framework because it supports the notion that a reduction of subjects’ perceived distance to an abstract meaning transfers to an increase of subjects’ perceived self-extension towards that meaning. This relationship between perceived distance and perceived self-extension supports the here proposed association between construal-level theory and the extended self concept. Further informative evidence results from the prestudy that was conducted prior to study 3. This prestudy tested subjects’ perceptions of symbolic and indexical connections between object-meaning pairs that were utilized in study 3. Different from expected, subjects indicated a perceived indexical (factual) connection between an object that was only symbolically (and hence mentally) connected to a meaning. This finding is crucial concerning the theoretical argumentation in this dissertation: it supports the here proposed notion that symbolic objects and their meaning are primarily connected mentally and that even strictly symbolic connections between an object and its meaning can be perceived as factual bonds (see sections 2.2.3 and 2.2.4). It consequently supports the notion that physical closeness to a purely symbolic object can actually create a perceived factual connection to its meaning. This has relevant implications for marketing theory as well as management, which will be discussed in sections 4.2.2 and 4.3.3. Study 4 replicated the previously found effects in a more realistic field-experiment and therefore supports their validity outside of controlled laboratory settings. Results indicate that physical proximity to symbolic objects can increase subjects’ selfextension towards an institution that is symbolically connected to the object (H2a and H4b). Moreover, results indicate that this effect further transfers on subjects’ behavioral intentions and actual behavior towards that institution (H3a, H3b, and H4c). Specifically, subjects indicated stronger intentions to become part of a club, indicated higher willingness to pay for a membership, and showed higher chances to indicate their email address for further information about the club if they were exposed to an 99 object that was symbolically connected to that club. Interestingly, these effects occurred stronger when subjects were exposed to a membership card (strong-symbolic connection to membership) than when they were exposed to a membership voucher (weak-symbolic connection to membership). Possible implications of this finding will be discussed in section 4.3.2, regarding its managerial contribution. Study 5 particularly aimed at generalizing the previously supported impact of objects on subjects’ perceived self-extension to an abstract meaning towards a broader economic context. Specifically, it was tested whether subjects’ perceived stronger selfextension to brands of products that they owned than to brands of services they regularly used. In line with the essential notion of this dissertation, it was argued that physical products should ease subjects’ self-extension to the product brands (H1a). Results of study 5 support this predicted outcome. Further informative evidence resulted from the manipulation and control measures that were conducted in this study. Subjects indicated that they did not only perceive the products as more graspable than the specified services. The products were also perceived to be used more often and to be closer to subjects’ everyday life than the named services. This result emphasizes the notion that the graspability of goods might not solely affect self-extension to a meaning because they transfer this meaning into the directly experienceable world (H1a). The results further indicate that through this objectification, goods and accordingly their meaning become a more prominent component of everyday life and therefore physically and psychologically move closer to their owners and their owners’ selves (H1b). 4.2 Theoretical Contribution 4.2.1 Contribution to Extended Self Research By connecting extended self research and construal-level theory, this dissertation attempts to elaborate a more detailed and structured perspective of the broad extended self concept – it therefore aims at making the concept itself more “graspable”. The extended self concept has been criticized to be too comprehensive and too indistinct (Cohen 1989). The here proposed conceptual framework hence aims at meeting these objections. The proposed framework particularly contributes to a more detailed understanding of possessions and of the psychological processes that underlie the self-extension phenomenon. This understanding not only supports a more distinct conceptualization 100 of the self-extension function of possessions per se; it thereby also supports the applicability of the extended self concept to various related research phenomena and research domains. Further, the proposed definition of objects provides a discussible notion of how to distinguish actual possessions that consumers own from meanings that these consumers and these possessions relate to. This specification of the very broadly defined term “possession” – as it has been cultivated by Belk and related researchers – naturally comes with the price of making the concept of possessions less global. At the same time, it aims at distinguishing more distinctively between things that consumers actually possess and those that they relate to. A stone (object) that a mountaineer returns from an overmastered mountain (meaning) would be conceptually indistinguishable from its meaning according to the prevailing definition of possessions (see section 2.1.2). The here proposed distinction between graspable objects and their meaning aims at clarifying this relationship between subjects, their possessions, and the meaning that is attached to these possessions. By doing so, the developed framework also aims at accentuating a quality of possessions (and symbolic objects in general) that is perceived to be fundamental for human’s relationships to these entities: this is the perceived ability of symbolic objects to make an intrinsically inaccessible meaning physically and mentally graspable. Building upon this argumentation, this dissertation carves out a possible human (consumer) motive that constitutes a promising starting point for further inquiries of the extended self concept: this is the need of humans to be physically close to objects (and surroundings) that symbolize self-defining meanings (see Chapter 2.2) – or in other words, the need of humans to be close to and to experience their selves. By elaborating the idea that objects reduce subjects’ perceived distance and thereby strengthen their perceived self-extension to certain self-related meanings, the developed concept also supports the comparison of different kinds of objects and possessions. Particularly regarding recent trends such as digital consumption or sharing, the construct of perceived distance offers a potential dimension to compare these various consumption-types concerning their self-extending ability. In this context, it would be interesting to investigate, for example, whether consumers’ perceived distance to digital or shared goods is different to comparable physical possessions and how such possible differences transfer to consumers’ perceived selfextension to the symbolic meaning of these goods. Finally yet importantly, by stating the outlined influences of object graspability and proximity, this dissertation follows the goal of increasing the awareness of the 101 relevance of physical object properties for consumer behavior. By doing so, it emphasizes a more comprehensive view of economic goods that more carefully contemplates the particular relevance of the physical properties of these offerings. This return to a more material perspective of economic goods is particularly proposed with regard to the outlined trend of consumer and marketing research towards an immaterial perspective of the things that humans consume (see section 2.1.3). The research at hand hence opposes the presently cultivated notion that material aspects of consumption practices generally lose relevance. Alternatively, it follows Magaudda’s argumentation that changes in technological or socio-economic circumstances result in consumers’ adaptations towards the objects that they relate and attach to (Maggaudda 2011). Technological changes might result, for example, in a decreasing relevance of graspable possessions such as books, photos, or CDs. At the same time, they seem to increase consumers’ attachment to the particular objects that arise from these changes, such as mobile phones, laptops, and tablet PCs. Similarly, social changes towards more flexible lifestyles might result in lower attachments towards classic, inflexible possessions such as houses, cars, or furniture. At the same time, they might cultivate attachment to more mobile possessions such as clothing, wristwatches, headphones, as well as bags and suitcases. As such, ascribing the graspability of possessions a constitutional relevance for the self-definition of humans fosters the notion that these extended parts of humans cannot simply be diminished but underlie a constant change that derives from the ever-changing environment. 4.2.2 Contribution to Research on Symbolic Consumption Extensive research that refers to the self-extension function of economic goods concentrates on the symbolic meaning of these goods. A crucial interest of this research is the question of how goods connect to their meanings and how consumers react to these meanings. As previously outlined, particularly Kent Grayson and his co-authors have advanced the understanding of consumer research concerning how the meaning of things is created and how consumers react to these meanings (Grayson and Shulman 2000; Grayson and Martinec 2004; see also Belk 1988; Belk et al. 1989). Building upon the semiotic concept of Charles Pierce (1903), Grayson and Shulman (2000) have contributed to a more thorough understanding of how goods relate to their meaning. Closely relating to the concept of contamination, these researchers have emphasized the particular relevance of indexical (in other words, factual) connections between personal possessions and their meanings. This perspective has shown to be particularly 102 fruitful in understanding the relationship that consumers create to very specific, personal things. However, it runs the risk of undermining the powerful impact that purely symbolic (or mental) connections between economic goods and their meanings can have. A typical Apple product very unlikely actually touches an Apple designer or engineer, and never even comes close to Apple’s Headquarter. It is produced somewhere in China by people that consumers are unaware of (Kremp 2012). Still, touching and owning the product for many consumers activates feelings of an authentic connection to meanings that the Apple brand evokes. By emphasizing the purely mental connection between objects and their meaning (as is particularly accentuated by Saussure (1916)), this dissertation aims at further increasing the consciousness for the impact that this non-factual connection between objects and their meanings can have. Study 3 supports the notion that even purely symbolic objects can evoke feelings of factual connections to their meaning. The here proposed framework therefore also contributes to research regarding the transfer of semiotic theories into consumer research. It thereby fosters a Saussurian perspective of the meaning of things. 4.2.3 Contribution to Construal-Level Theory In transferring construal-level theory into extended self research, this dissertation not only contributes to a more distinct perspective on the self-extending function of possessions and the symbolic meaning of things; it simultaneously emphasizes a further application field of construal-level theory that so far has been widely neglected. The utilization of construal-level theory to understand human reactions to symbolic objects (or signs and emotional reaction to them, in a more general sense) is a fruitful application field of this concept for future research – this particularly, as the concepts of semiotics and construal-level theory show various parallels (see section 2.3). Nira Liberman, who has been one of the main contributors to the development of construallevel theory, states together with Yaacov Trope and Elena Stephan in their article on construal-level and psychological distance (2007, p. 353): “People believe that they directly experience themselves and their immediate surroundings at the present moment. Anything that is not present is distal. It may be thought of, constructed, or reconstructed, but it cannot be experienced directly.” According to this statement, the proposed consolidation of research on signs (semiotics) and construal-level theory promises a wide range of informative insights into why and how humans react towards symbols that concretize and hence make something experienceable that by itself is inexperienceable. 103 4.3 Managerial Contribution 4.3.1 (R)Evolving towards an Object-Dominant Logic As outlined in the introduction and theoretical part of this dissertation, marketing research as well as marketing management show various evidence of an ongoing development towards an immaterial perspective of economic goods (see section 2.1.3). Developments such as digital consumption as well as sharing practices closely correspond to concepts such as liquid possessions and the service-dominant logic that particularly emphasize immaterial facets of consumption. Especially the management orientated service-dominant logic supports a persisting change of marketing management towards understanding their products and services from the perspective of their immaterial uses and benefits. The here developed framework can be perceived as an antithesis to this prevailing development (see also Lindberg and Nordin 2008). Building upon the findings of the research at hand, it is argued that the graspability of objects itself bears an essential use to consumers that is neglected in contemporary utility-focused perspectives of economic goods. Accordingly, management should pay careful attention to an effective implementation and communication of graspable components of their offerings. The proposed perspective can therefore be understood as a partial return to a marketing logic that was prevalent in the early 20th century and that emphasized the product nature of economic offerings (Shostack 1977; Vargo and Lusch 2004). Different to the view of marketing scholars and practitioners from that time, the here developed understanding however is less concerned with the plain functional properties of offered physical goods. It rather emphasizes a return to the experienceable objectification of abstract services and meanings that companies offer and stand for. On a strategic level, this logic goes as far as to elicit the question of whether companies should position themselves as providers of graspable products or immaterial services and how prominently they should communicate these two facets of their offerings. It further breaks down to the question of which proportion of available resources management should allocate to the development and realization of products versus services. Should companies such as Hilti and Apple highlight the service aspects of their offerings or their graspable products? Should IBM continue to concentrate on consulting-services or should it partially return to the development of graspable products? Should Google market its offerings as digital products or as digital 104 services? Should Amazon highlight the product nature of their e-reader Kindle or the services that are offered for this product? These are just a few examples of questions that the here developed framework aims to evoke. On a more operative level, the object-dominant logic fosters the consideration of where within its portfolio a company should include graspable objects to support consumers in their self-extension. Examples of simple graspable components of intrinsically abstract offerings illustrate how easy and effective such objectifications can be implemented: credit card companies use the particular design of their credit cards (such as the black American Express Card) to objectify a self-defining meaning that is important to their customers. Clubs such as the Rotary Club use small insignia to objectify self-extending memberships within these clubs. In a similar vein, Lufthansa German Airlines offers its frequent flyers suitcase batches that are colored depending on their customer status; being easily visible on luggage pieces, they prominently objectify this self-extending status to their owners as well as to their owners’ social surroundings. Similar is true for wristbands that visitors of festivals receive and often continue to wear for months after the event. Campaigns such as the anti-Aids campaign connect to simple insignia such as the red ribbon, thereby stimulating their self-extending value; similar occurs for the Nike Livestrong Wristband that objectifies consumers’ engagement in the Livestrong Foundation. Not only have some of these simple objects gained cult status; they have further become inseparable symbols of the meanings that they stand for. These are just a few examples that highlight how simple objectifications can be implemented and what potential lies within a thoughtful objectification of abstract meanings. The here proposed object-dominant logic not only fosters management to consider the objectivation of self-relevant components of their offerings; moreover, it encourages management to pay careful attention to an appropriate selection and marketing of these objects. 4.3.2 Exploiting the Self-Defining Meaning of Objects The previously outlined findings and examples highlight the notion that objects can become relevant components of economic offerings if they actually create a physical connection between consumers and a certain self-extending meaning. Study 4 indicated that being exposed to a card that symbolized membership within a club had a stronger impact on subjects in becoming a member of that club than a membership voucher. This result, in combination with the above mentioned examples (credit cards, 105 luggage badges, insignia, and wristbands), stresses the notion that symbolic objects are particularly effective if they symbolize consumers’ affiliation to a certain selfextending group or meaning. A further example that outlines this notion is a temporary frequent flyer card that Lufthansa customers instantly receive when signing up for the airline’s frequent flyer program. The card is attached to the inflight magazine, which contains a form that customers can use to sign up for the program. When using this form, customers can directly detach a temporary membership card that is later exchanged for a regular membership plastic card. This temporary membership card hence not only instantly objectifies the membership to the frequent flyer program; it also facilitates customers’ awareness and prospect of receiving the actual membership card. This example further highlights the notion that creating and accentuating objects that symbolize customers’ affiliation to self-extending group or meaning can be an effective alternative to creating materials (such as vouchers) that solicit customers’ engagement to an economic offering. 4.3.3 Managing the Meaning of Objects The conceptual framework and empirical findings of this dissertation emphasize the relevance of symbolic (and thus purely mentally created) connections between objects and their meaning. Results from study 3 indicate that even purely symbolic connections between objects and their meaning can be perceived as factual relationships between these two entities. Accordingly, as previously outlined, an Apple product does not need to have a physical connection to an Apple employee to be perceived as factually related to the Apple brand. Similarly, a Marlboro cigarette does not need to be developed, manufactured, and marketed by a US-American Cowboy to be perceived as factually and authentically connected to the brand Marlboro and its particular image. The results of this research emphasize the notion that the connection between symbolic objects and their meaning is of a primarily mental nature and that this mental connection is itself already affective in connecting consumers to a certain self-defining meaning. Following the argumentation of Saussure (1916), the meaning of an object is therefore arbitrary until it is individually learned or even socially conventionalized. From this perspective, the crucial challenge for management in symbolically loading objects lies accordingly not in actually creating a factual connection between the object and its meaning; it lies much more in providing subjects and markets with the 106 necessary information to create and conventionalize a relevant self-extending meaning of that object. 4.4 Limitations The outlined conceptual argumentations and empirical studies include limitations that shall be pointed out and discussed in this section. By connecting the understanding of possessions to the here defined notion of graspable objects, this dissertation strongly narrows down previously outlined understandings of possessions. As shown by Belk (1988) as well as Pierce et al. (2003), possessions can include other ungraspable entities such as ideas, beliefs, experiences, rights, or virtual goods. This dissertation consciously separates these ungraspable possessions from the here defined graspable objects. Accordingly, the outlined findings have to be limited to these graspable or, in other words, material kinds of possessions. However, as the findings support a relevance of the graspability of possessions for consumer behavior, it is argued that a more careful distinction between graspable and ungraspable possessions should be considered concerning future research in the field of possessions and the extended self. The here developed understanding of graspable objects sets a foundation for a possible differentiation between graspable and ungraspable possessions. However, future research will have to further elaborate whether this conceptual differentiation has a strong enough explanatory power to be integrated into the general concept of the extended self. A further limitation refers to the definition of graspable objects and its transfer into the overall framework and empirical analyses of this dissertation. An object has here been defined as an entity that is perceived to be physically graspable. This definition derives from an analysis of the development of the word “object” as well as associated terms. Further, the definition is based on objects that consumers typically name when asked to specify personally important objects. Finally, this definition is based on a preliminary study that was conducted in this context. Based on these sources, this definition is proposed for future conceptualizations of objects and material possessions within consumer research. However, further research is needed to test the applicability and explanatory power of this definition. Above that, the here conducted empirical analyses support the defined hypotheses for this specified kind of graspable objects. However, further research will be needed to test whether the defined object properties (such as size) actually influence the self-extending functions of symbolic objects. 107 In addition to these conceptual limitations, further limiting aspects must be outlined regarding the conducted analyses. Three of the five reported experiments were conducted using student samples. The question of whether student samples should be used in consumer research has long been discussed. It however has been considered as adequate and is widely practiced for the specific use of the development and testing of new theories (Calder et al. 1981). As the main aim of this dissertation lies in advancing consumer theory, the utilization of student samples is hence regarded as acceptable. However, to meet further this limitation, studies 1 and 5, which particularly aimed at identifying and generalizing the predicted effects in real-world situations, were conducted with samples of German and Swiss adults. The results of these studies showed similar patterns as the studies conducted with students. This similarity further indicates that the results were not biased by specific characteristics of students. Finally, the exclusive utilization of experimental study designs and the extensive reliance on scale-measurements must be commented upon as limiting factors of the conducted analyses. As outlined, experiments were consciously used complementarily to the qualitative and conceptual research history in the field of possessions and the extended self. To still account for the external validity of the revealed effects, the conducted studies aimed at being as realistic ( and non-hypothetical) as possible. Study 1 referred to actual possessions and experiences of subjects. Study 4 was conducted in cooperation with an existing organization and measured actual behavior such as the specification of email addresses. Finally, study 5 also referred to real possessions and services that participants own respectively regularly use. As such, the conducted experiments aimed at utilizing realistic study designs to ensure a large external validity of the measured effects. Further studies that rely on actual company activities as well as on actual consumer behavior would however be sensible complements to the here reported experiments. 4.5 Future Research The conceptual framework of this dissertation proposes a revised understanding of the popular extended self concept. By altering the perspective of this prominent concept, this dissertation presents a variety of opportunities for novel approaches towards existing and new research questions. These opportunities for future research are outlined in the following sections. Building upon the three main components of the 108 here proposed framework, the suggestions for further inquiries are structured regarding their reference to objects, subjects, and meanings. 4.5.1 Future Research on Objects Future Research on Physical Object Properties An object has here been defined as an entity that is perceived to be physically graspable. As outlined in the limitations of this dissertation, a further investigation of the explanatory power of this differentiation of objects from other entities constitutes a promising field for future research. Future research could investigate the influence of object properties that relate to graspability on the ability of these objects to fulfill a self-extending function: a particularly interesting property for testing in future studies would be object size. Based on the conceptualization of objects in section 2.2.1, it is assumed that particularly small or large objects are less effective in reducing perceived distance and in increasing self-extension towards a certain meaning. Fragileness, slipperiness, or perishableness could be further possible object properties – amongst others – that negatively influence objects’ self-extending ability (see in this context also Locke’s thoughts on the solidity of objects (Locke 1690, pp. 66-69)). Future Research on the Particular Effect of Touch This research did not identify a particular effect of touching an object on subjects’ perceived distance to the object’s meaning. Future research could more intensively investigate the particular relevance of touch in this context. An interesting field for further inquiries is spanned by the question of whether the effectiveness of touching an object increases if the object has an indexical and hence particularly factual connection to its symbolized meaning. Therefore, future research could further investigate the interaction between touching an object and the quality of the symbolic connection between an object and its meaning. Future Research on Digital Objects As outlined, a growing proportion of possessions are of a digital nature. Particularly books, music, and other information media are increasingly owned virtually. Regarding this development, a promising field for future research could derive from the question whether the visual display of these documents influences the perceived distance that consumers feel to these digital entities and hence towards their meaning. Intentionally or not, Apple has repeatedly transferred the logic of graspable objects into their depiction of virtual properties. The desktop, for example, which was 109 developed by Xerox but marketed to a broad audience by Apple, can be understood as a translation of the physical desktop into a virtual working space. Following this logic further, the mouse can be understood as an extended hand that enables users to grasp objects that are placed on this desktop or in virtual folders. Still today, Apple implements representations of virtual objects that closely relate to their appearance in non-virtual environments: in the cover-flow option, iTunes users can scroll through their music library as if it was a collection of vinyl-records and the digital bookshelf on the iPad displays virtually owned books as if they were sorted on a wooden bookshelf. A promising field for future research is whether these kinds of depictions enforce their perceived graspability and thus support their self-extending function. 4.5.2 Future Research on Subjects Future Research on Subjects’ Insecurity This research did not broach the issue of how consumer characteristics moderate the proposed influence of object presence / proximity on subjects’ perceived distance towards an object’s meaning. Research has repeatedly emphasized the connection between consumer insecurity and materialism. The here developed framework provides a potential approach to a more thorough and coherent understanding of this connection. It is thinkable, for example, that secure consumers feel less distant per se to self-defining meanings than insecure consumers do. Therefore, it can be proposed that the potential reduction of distance to an abstract meaning by an object is larger for insecure consumers than it is for more secure consumers. Further, it could be interesting to investigate whether insecurity regarding certain selfextending domains increases the relevance of possessions in these specific domains. A consumer, for example, that begins a new hobby might be more prone to objectify this self-extending hobby by certain objects (equipment) than a consumer who has been carrying out this hobby for a long time. A possible explanation for such discrepancy could be that the more experienced person (compared to the inexperienced person) has already reduced his perceived distance to that abstract meaning and is hence less receptive to objects that provide a further distance reducing and therefore selfconnecting function to that meaning. Future Research on Subjects’ Object Attachment The research at hand concentrated on the impact that symbolic objects have on subjects’ perceived distance and self-extension towards a meaning that is connected to that object. Concentrating on this focus, this project did not pay attention to the 110 attachment that subjects create towards an object. The here proposed framework however contributes a possible approach for future research in this field: further inquiries in this direction could test whether the ability of an object to reduce the distance to a personally relevant meaning drives consumers’ attachment to that object. Future Research on Motivating and Demotivating Consequences of Object Proximity The here outlined framework and reported findings support the notion that object proximity enforces perceived self-extension and behavior towards the objects’ symbolized meaning. However, if objects actually reduce the perceived distance to a certain self-extending meaning then their proximity could also have demotivating consequences. This argumentation consequently refers to a compensatory influence of objects: in reducing the perceived distance to an intended part of the self and therefore providing a perceived accomplishment, a symbolic object might also demotivate a person from actually behaving in line with that intended self. Future research on this boundary effect of objects could be promising and an informative approach towards explaining negative effects of materialism and consumption in general. 4.5.3 Future Research on Meanings Future Research on the Personal Relevance of the Meanings of Objects This research did not investigate the particular effect of the personal relevance of a certain meaning on the self-extending function of objects. However, it indicates that the distance reducing function of objects does account for personally important meanings (such as weddings (study 1) or other personally important experiences (study 2)) as well as for less essential meanings (such as Wimbledon (study 3) and the Marketing Club St. Gallen (study 4)). Future research should pay closer attention to the particular role of the relevance of a certain meaning for the here outlined selfextension process (Dodson 1996; Tom 2004). Previously mentioned examples such as credit cards, frequent flyer luggage badges, festival wristbands, or the above mentioned hobby equipment emphasize the notion that objects are particularly effective if they are connected to personally relevant meaning. Future research could test how the personal relevance of a meaning affects an object’s influence on the subject’s perceived distance to that meaning. Moreover, an inquiry of the emotional consequences of such reduction of perceived distance depending on the personal relevance of a particular meaning would be a promising field for further research. 111 Further Research on Experienceable and Inexperienceable Meanings As previously outlined, there is an ongoing debate concerning the timeliness of material possessions and the idea of ownership. In many argumentations, this debate closely relates to the question of whether material possessions or experiences are more important and valuable to customers and their personal well-being (Van Boven and Gilovich 2003; Caprariello and Reis 2013). Interestingly, this discussion seems to be partially predetermined by certain stand- and viewpoints of the involved researchers. Some researchers approach the question from the perspective of experiences (ibid). Others researchers approach the question from the direction of material possessions (see section 2.1.1). Material possessions usually have an inferior starting point in this comparison as they are often (as it seems culturally) associated as being more superficial and shallow as actual experiences. Building upon this foundation, most research hence discusses these two consumption practices against each other. Little research however has tried to identify the complementary nature of experiences and material possessions. The derived framework offers an approach to consolidate these research streams on experiences and material possessions. Building upon the here developed proposition that customers constantly try to reduce the perceived distance to meanings that define their self, it can be argued that both – experiences and material possessions – serve this need. Personally relevant experiences can therefore be perceived as very direct experiences of the self. On the other hand, personal possessions make self-extending meanings accessible that intrinsically are not directly experienceable. Continuing this notion, the value of a particular possession should hence vary with regard to the experienceability of its meaning: the self-extending value of a material concert ticket, for example, should be low in a situation in which a person is actually experiencing directly a personally important concert. Once the concert is over and therefore only a past memory, the self-extending value of the ticket should however increase, as it becomes the most directly experienceable symbol of this personally important experience. Personal possessions, from this perspective, can accordingly be perceived as manifestations and stabilizations of these experiences and other elusive parts of the self (Belk 1990). Research that further analyses these complementary functions of experiences and possessions could be an additional interesting field for the further application and development of the here proposed framework. 112 5 Conclusion The essential aim of this dissertation was to approach the question of how the physical graspability of symbolic objects relates to their self-extension function. A review of previous research on the self-extending and symbolic meaning of objects not only provided a meaningful basis for the investigation of this question; it also carved out the tendency of present research to devalue the relevance of the materiality of economic goods. Moreover, this review highlighted the ambiguity of present research regarding the conceptualization of constructs such as possessions and materiality. Building upon these findings, an understanding of objects was developed that considers their graspability as a constitutional property. It was argued that an important quality of physical possessions and objects is their ability to provide a perceived proximity between their symbolic meaning and the body and mind of a subject. Construal-level theory served as a promising approach to conceptualize the psychological processes that underlie this function of symbolic objects. The conducted studies supported the presumption that physical proximity to a symbolic object reduces perceived distance and strengthens perceived self-extension of subjects towards the object’s symbolized meaning. Findings further supported the notion that this affiliation positively transfers into subjects’ behavior towards the symbolized meaning. Based on these empirical findings and the developed conceptual framework, a revised understanding of the current extended self concept was proposed. Furthermore, a possible consumer motive was outlined as an explanation for the human tendency to utilize material possessions as extended parts of the self. This motive was described as the need of humans to be physically close to objects (and surroundings) that symbolize self-defining meanings – or in other words, the need of humans to be close to and to experience their selves. By outlining and proposing this use of graspable objects, this dissertation not only emphasized a revised managerial and theoretical perspective of economic goods; it also contributed to variety of new approaches for future inquiries of the investigated as well as related research fields. Referring to these findings, the author intends to contribute to a more precise understanding of why humans possess and therefore why and how they consume. Consequently and relating to the very first sentence of this dissertation, the author finally wishes to contribute to a deeper understanding of human being and behavior. 113 References Ahuvia, Aaron C. (2005), “Beyond the Extended Self: Loved Objects and Consumers' Identity Narratives,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (1), 172-184. Ariely, Dan, Joel Huber, and Klaus Wertenbroch (2005), “When do Losses Loom Larger Than Gains?” Journal of Marketing Research, 42 (2), 134-138. Aristotle (384-322 BC / 2005), Poetics, ed. and trans. Stephan Halliwell, Harvard: Loeb Classic Library. Bauman, Zygmunt (2007), Liquid Times: Living in an Age of Uncertainty, Cambridge: Polity Press. Bardhi, Fleura and Giana M. Eckhardt (2012), “Access-Based Consumption: The Case of Car Sharing,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (4), 881-898. Bardhi, Fleura, Giana M. Eckhardt, and Eric J. Arnould (2012), “Liquid Relationship to Possessions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (3), 510-529. Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173-1182. Belk, Russell W. (1978), “Assessing the Effects of Visible Consumption on Impression Formation,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Ann Arbor: Association for Consumer Research, 39-47. Belk, Russell W. (1985), “Materialism: Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World,” Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (3), 265-280. Belk, Russell W. (1988), “Possessions and the Extended Self,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 139-168. Belk, Russell W. (1989), “Extended Self and Extending Paradigmatic Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (1), 129-132. Belk, Russell W. (1990), “The Role of Possessions in Constructing and Maintaining a Sense of Past,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 17, eds. Marvin E. Goldberg, Gerald Gorn, and Richard W. Pollay, Provo: Association of Consumer Research, 669-676. Belk, Russell W. (2010), “Sharing,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (5), 715-734. 114 Belk, Russell W. (2013), “Extended Self in a Digital World,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40 (3), 477-500. Belk, Russell W., Melanie Wallendorf, and John F. Sherry, JR. (1989), “The Sacred and the Profane in Consumer Behavior: Theodicy on the Odyssey,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (1), 1-38. Berger, Jonah and Chip Heath (2007), “Where Consumers Diverge from Others: Identity Signaling and Product Domains,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (2), 121-134. Bowlby, John (1969 / 1997), Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1, London: Pimlico. Burke, Kenneth (1966), Language as Symbolic Action: Essays on Life, Literature, and Method, Berkeley: University of California Press. Caprariello, Peter A. and Harry T. Reis (2013), “To Do, to Have, or to Share? Valuing Experiences Over Material Possessions Depends on the Involvement of Others,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104 (2), 199-215. Calder, Bobby J., Lynn W. Phillips, and Alice M. Tybout (1981), “Designing Research for Application,” Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (2), 197-207. Cassam, Quassim (1997), Self and World, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chang, LinChiat and Robert M. Arkin (2002), “Materialism as an Attempt to Cope with Uncertainty,” Psychology & Marketing, 19 (5), 389-406. Cohen, Joel B. (1989), “An Over-Extended Self?” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (1), 125-128. Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg-Halton, E. (1981), The Meaning of Things: Domestic Symbols and the Self, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly and Rick E. Robinson (1990), The Art of Seeing: An Interpretation of the Aesthetic Encounter, Los Angeles: Getty Publications. Dehling, Aurelie and Eric Vernette (2013), “Making Someone Else’s Object One’s Own: The Matter of Second-Hand Item Appropriation,” EMAC, 42nd Annual Conference. Derbaix, Christian, Alain Decrop, and Olivier Cabossart (2002), “Colors and Scarves: The Symbolic Consumption of Material Possessions by Soccer Fans,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 29, eds. Susan M. Broniarczyk and Kent Nakamoto, Valdosta: Association for Consumer Research, 511-518. 115 Descartes, René (1641 / 2011), Meditationen, ed. Andreas Schmidt, Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht. Descartes, René (1662 / 2003), Treatise of Man, trans. Thomas Steele Hall, New York: Prometheus Books. Dilthey, Wilhelm (1883 / 2013), Einleitung in die Geisteswissenschaften: Versuch einer Grundlegung für das Studium der Gesellschaft und der Geschichte, Berlin: Holzinger. Dilthey, Wilhelm (1910 / 2013), Der Aufbau der geschichtlichen Welt in den Geisteswissenschaften, Berlin: Holzinger. Dodson, Kimberly J. (1996), “Peak Experiences and Mountain Biking: Incorporating the Bike into the Extended Self,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 23, eds. Kim P. Corfman and John G. Lynch Jr., Provo: Association of Consumer Research, 317-322. Eco, Umberto (2002), Einführung in die Semiotik, ed. and trans. Jürgen Trabant, Paderborn: Wilhelm Fink. Eisinga, Rob, Manfred te Grotenhuis, and Ben Pelzer (2012), “The Reliability of a Two-Item Scale: Pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown?” International Journal of Public Health, 58 (4), 1-6. Escalas, Jennifer E. and James R. Bettman (2003), “You Are What They Eat: The Influence of Reference Groups on Consumers’ Connections to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 339-348. Escalas, Jennifer E. and James R. Bettman (2005), “Self-Construal, Reference Groups, and Brand Meaning,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 378-389. Field, Andy (2009), Discovering statistics using SPSS, London: Sage. Fujita, Kentaro, Marlone D. Henderson, Juliana Eng, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2006), “Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events,” Psychological Science, 17 (4), 278-282. Grayson, Kent and Radan Martinec (2004), “Consumer Perception of Iconicity and Indexicality and Their Influence on Assessments of Authentic Market Offerings,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 296-312. Grayson, Kent and David Shulman (2000), “Indexicality and the Verification Function of Irreplaceable Possessions: A Semiotic Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Research, 27 (1), 17-30. 116 Habermas, Jürgen (1985), „Dialektik der Rationalisierung,“ in: Die Neue Unübersichtlichkeit, ed. Jürgen Habermas, Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp, 167–208. Habermas, Tilman (1999), Geliebte Objekte: Symbole und Instrumente der Identitätsbildung, Berlin: Suhrkamp. Hahn, Robert A. and Arthur Kleinman (1983), “Perspectives of the Placebo Phenomenon: Belief as Pathogen, Belief as Medicine: ‘Voodoo Death’ and the ‘Placebo Phenomenon’ in Anthropological Perspective,” Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 14 (4), 3–19. Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling, White Paper, http://www.afhayes.com/ Hayes, Andrew F. (2013), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, New York: The Guilford Press. Hayes, Andrew F. and Kristopher J. Preacher (2013), Statistical Mediation Analysis with a Multicategorical Independent Variable, White Paper, http://www.afhayes.com/ Hongladarom, Soraj (2011), “Personal Identity and the Self in the Online and Offline World,” Minds and Machines, 21 (4), 533-548. Hjelmslev, Louis (1943 / 1961), Prolegomena to a Theory of Language, Madison: University of Wisconsin Press. James, William (1890 / 1950), The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1, New York: Dover. Jaspers, Karl (1954 / 1989), Psychologie der Weltanschauungen, Berlin: SpringerVerlag. Kahneman, Daniel, Jack L. Knetch, and Richard H. Thaler (1990), “Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem,” Journal of Political Economy, 98 (6), 1325-1348. Kaufman, Jordy, Denis Mareschal, and Mark H. Johnson (2003), “Graspability and Object Processing in Infants,” Infant Behavior & Development, 26 (4), 516-528. 117 Kleine, Susan Schultz, Robert E. Kleine, III, and Chris T. Allen (1995), “How Is a Possession "Me" or "Not Me"? Characterizing Types and an Antecedent of Material Possession Attachment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (3), 327343. Kotler, Philip (1977), Marketing Management: Analysis, Planning, Implementation, and Control, Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall. Kremp, Matthias (2012): “Umstrittene Tablet-Produktion: So werden in China Apples iPads gebaut,“ Spiegel Online, http://www.spiegel.de/netzwelt/gadgets/0,1518, 827063,00.html Krishna, Aradhna and Maureen Morrin (2008), “Does Touch Affect Taste? The Perceptual Transfer of Product Container Haptic Cues,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (6), 807-818. Lehdonvirta, Vili (2010), “Online Spaces have Material Culture: Goodbye to Digital Post-Materialism and Hello to Virtual Consumption,” Media, Culture & Society, 32 (5), 883-889. Liberman, Nira, Yaacov Trope, Sean M. McCrea, and Steven J. Sherman (2007), “The effect of level of construal on the temporal distance of activity enactment,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43 (1), 143-149. Liberman, Nira, Yaacov Trope, and Elena Stephan (2007), “Psychological Distance,” in: Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, eds. Arie W. Kruglanski and E. Tory Higgins, New York: The Guilford Press, 353-383. Lindberg, Nina and Fredrik Nordin (2008), “From Products to Services and Back Again: Towards a New Service Procurement Logic,” Industrial Marketing Management, 37 (3), 292-300. Locke, John (1690 / 2008), An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, ed. Pauline Phemister, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Love, Lisa L. and Peter S. Sheldon (1998), “Souvenirs: Messengers of Meaning,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 25, eds. Joseph W. Alba and J. Wesley Hutchinson, Provo: Association for Consumer Research, 170-175. Magaudda, Paolo (2011), “When Materiality ‘Bites Back’: Digital Music Consumption Practices in the Age of Materialization,” Journal of Consumer Culture, 11 (1), 15-36. 118 Marx, Karl (1844 / 2011), Ökonomisch-Philosophische Manuskripte aus dem Jahre 1844, Raleigh: Lulu. Marx, Karl (1872 / 2009), Das Kapital, Cologne: Anaconda. Marx, Karl and Frederick Engels (1848 / 1978), “Manifesto of the Communist Party,” in: The Marx-Engels Reader, ed. Robert C. Tucker, New York: W. W. Norton & Company. Mehta, Raj and Russell W. Belk (1991), “Artifacts, Identity, and Transition: Favorite Possessions of Indians and Indian Immigrants to the United States,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (4), 398-411. Murray, Keith B. and John L. Schlacter (1990), “The Impact of Services versus Goods on Consumers’ Assessment of Perceived Risk and Variability,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 18 (1), 51-65. Newman, George E. and Paul Bloom (2012), “Art and Authenticity: The Importance of Originals in Judgments of Value,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141 (3), 558-569. Newman, George E., Gil Diesendruck, and Paul Bloom (2011), “Celebrity Contagion and the Value of Objects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (2), 215-228. Nöth, Winfried (2000), Handbuch der Semiotik, Stuttgart: Metzler. Oxford Dictionaries (2013), Online Oxford Dictionaries, http://oxforddictionaries.com Peck, Joann and Terry L. Childers (2003), “To Have and To Hold: The Influence of Haptic Information on Product Judgments,” Journal of Marketing, 67 (2), 35-48. Peck, Joann and Suzanne B. Shu (2009), “The Effect of Mere Touch on Perceived Ownership,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (3), 434-447. Peck, Joann and Jennifer Wiggins (2006), “It Just Feels Good: Customers' Affective Response to Touch and Its Influence on Persuasion,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (4), 56-69. Peck, Joann and Jennifer Wiggins Johnson (2011), “Autotelic Need for Touch, Haptics, and Persuasion: The Role of Involvement,” Psychology & Marketing, 28 (3), 222-239. Peirce, Charles S. (1903 / 1983), Phänomen und Logik der Zeichen, ed. and trans. Helmut Pape, Frankfurt: Suhrkamp Verlag. 119 Pierce, Jon L., Tatiana Kostova and Kurt T. Dirks (2003), “The State of Psychological Ownership: Integrating and Extending a Century of Research,” Review of General Psychology, 7 (1), 84-107. Philosophie (1999), Philosophie Dtv-Atlas, Munich: Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag. Prelinger, Ernst (1959), “Extension and Structure of the Self,” Journal of Psychology, 47 (1), 13-23. Richins, Marsha L. (1994), “Valuing Things: The Public and Private Meanings of Possessions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (3), 504-521. Rindfleisch, Aric, James E. Burroughs, and Nancy Wong (2009), “The Safety of Objects: Materialism, Existential Insecurity, and Brand Connection,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (1), 1-16. Ross, Michael and Anne E. Wilson (2002), “It Feels like Yesterday: Self-Esteem Valence of Personal Past Experiences, and Judgments of Subjective Distance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82 (5), 792-803. Saussure, Ferdinand de (1916 / 2001), Grundfragen der allgemeinen Sprachwissenschaften, ed. Charles Bally and Albert Sechehaye, trans. Herman Lommel, Berlin: de Gruyter. Schultz, Susan E., Robert E. Kleine, III, and Jerome B. Kernan (1989), “These Are a Few of My Favorite Things: Toward an Explication of Attachment as a Consumer Behavior Construct,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 16, ed. Thomas K. Srull, Provo: Association for Consumer Research, 359-366. Semin, Gün R. and Eliot R. Smith (1999), “Revisiting the Past and Back to the Future: Memory Systems and the Linguistic Representation of Social Events,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (6), 877-892. Shostack, G. Lynn (1977), “Breaking Free from Product Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 41 (2), 73-80. Siddiqui, Shakeel and Darach Turley (2006), “Extending the Self in a Virtual World,” Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 33, eds. Connie Pechmann and Linda Price, Duluth: Association for Consumer Research, 647-648. Sirgy, M. Joseph (1982), “Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (3), 287-300. 120 Sonneveld, Marieke H. and Hendrik N. J. Schifferstein (2008), “The Tactual Experience of Objects,” in: Product Experience, eds. Hendrik N. J. Schifferstein and Paul Hekkert, San Diego: Elsevier, 41-67. Styvén, Maria Ek (2010), “The Need to Touch: Exploring the Link between Music Involvement and Tangibility Preference,” Journal of Business Research, 63 (9), 1088-1094. Tarn, David D. C. (2005), “Marketing-Based Tangibilisation for Services,” The Service Industries Journal, 25 (6), 747-772. Tom, Gail (2004), “The Endowment-Institutional Affinity Effect,” The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 138 (2), 160-170. Tomlinson, John (2007), The Culture of Speed: The Coming of Immediacy, London: Sage. Trope, Yaacov and Nira Liberman (2010), “Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance,” Psychological Review, 117 (2), 440-463. Trope, Yaacov, Nira Liberman, and Cheryl Wakslak (2007), “Construal Levels and Psychological Distance: Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation, and Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17 (2), 83-95. Tuli, Kapil R., Ajay K. Kohli, and Sundar G. Bharadwaj (2007), “Rethinking Customer Solutions: From Product Bundles to Relational Processes,” Journal of Marketing, 71 (3), 1-17. Van Boven, Leaf and Thomas Gilovich (2003), “To Do or to Have? That Is the Question,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 (6), 1193-1202. Van Boven, Leaf, Joanne Kane, A. Peter McGraw, Jeannette Dale (2010), “Feeling Close: Emotional Intensity Reduces Perceived Psychological Distance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (6), 872-885. Vargo, Stephan L. and Robert F. Lusch (2004), “Evolving to a New Dominant Logic for Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 68 (1), 1-17. Wallendorf, Melanie and Eric J. Arnould (1988), “My Favorite Things: A CrossCultural Inquiry into Object Attachment, Possessiveness, and Social Linkage,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (4), 531-547. Weber, Max (1920 / 2013), Die protestantische Ethik und der Geist des Kapitalismus, ed. Dirk Kaesler, München: Beck. 121 White, Robert W. (1959), “Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of Competence,” Psychological Review, 66 (5), 297-333. Whorf, Benjamin L. (1956 / 2008), Sprache – Denken – Wirklichkeit: Beiträge zur Metalinguistik und Sprachphilosophie, ed. and trans. Peter Krausser, Reinbek bei Hamburg: Rowohlt. Zeithaml, Valerie A., A. Parasuraman, and Leonard L. Berry (1985), “Problems and Strategies in Services Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 49 (2), 33-46. Curriculum Vitae Name Philipp Scharfenberger Date of Birth 26. August 1982 Place of Birth Cologne, Germany Education 2009-2013 University of St. Gallen, Switzerland Doctoral Studies in Business Administration / Consumer Behavior University of Essex, United Kingdom Summer Program in Quantitative Methods of Social Research 2003-2008 Johannes Gutenberg-University, Mainz, Germany Diploma Studies in Media Economics 1994-2001 Prälat-Diehl-Schule, Gross-Gerau, Germany Abitur Work Experience 2009-2013 Center for Customer Insight, University of St. Gallen, Switzerland Research Associate, Consumer Research 2004-2009 Localmotion Communication, Mainz, Germany Entrepreneur, Media Design 2002-2009 Lufthansa German Airlines, Frankfurt (a. M.), Germany Employee, Airline Services 2006-2007 J. Walter Thompson, Frankfurt (a. M.), Germany Internship, Strategic Brand and Marketing Management 2005-2006 Springer & Jacoby, Hamburg, Germany Internship, Advertisement