IPA/USA E-Journal 2013
Transcription
IPA/USA E-Journal 2013
Fall 2013 www.Ipausa.org president's message Thank you for your interest in this issue of the IPA/USA e-journal. We are committed to supporting the child’s right to play and to serving as an advocacy organization via publications, conferences, and networking. You can make a difference by joining IPA/USA. Simply download a membership application at our website, www.ipausa.org. Membership in the IPA/USA affiliate automatically enrolls you in our parent organization, the International Play Association, and includes international publications and discounts for international conferences. IPA/USA officers look forward to working together to make this a vibrant organization to support the child’s right to play. The Executive Board includes: Dorothy Sluss, President; Ladonna Adkins, Vice-President; Marcy Guddemi, Treasurer; Danielle Marshall, Secretary; Joanne Cemore, Member at Large; and Joyce Hemphill, Member at Large. We are especially indebted to Georgiana Duarte, who serves as Editor in Chief of the e-journal. We also extend special thanks to Cynthia Gentry who has served as webmaster for the past two years. If you are interested in taking a more active role in IPA/USA services and activities, please contact one of the board members. An exciting new initiative is the establishment of a partnership with the US Coalition for Play. We are excited to collaborate with them during their 2014 Conference on the Value of Play which will be held on February 15-19 at Clemson University, South Carolina. This collaboration will create meaningful outcomes for participants by encouraging exchange of ideas through discussion and conversation. Keep on playing, Dorothy Sluss Dorothy Sluss President, International Play Association, USA slussdj@jmu.edu 2 editor's message Dear IPA/USA Members: This spring, our journal was delayed for a number of important changes in schedule and submissions. It is with great joy we are back on schedule to share some very reflective and thoughtful articles. These articles illustrate the challenges of play, the language and cognitive benefits, personal reflections on the joys and constructs of play, and a research study detailing how children learn and build foundations for later learning. The authors in this issue have contributed from a research lens, a personal reflective perspective of history and emotion to that of strong voices of advocacy of parents, children and adults. These articles are professional and personal as they reveal the many voices of research and advocacy in play. The first article in this issue carefully examines current kindergarten expectations, IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 and the role of play in curriculum. Guddemi, Fite and Selva have systematically investigated the Gesell Development Observation-Revised, the psychometric results and implications for tasks and subtasks on the GDO-R, developmental and academic criteria for success, and how children learn through play. The extensive results inform and support that the GDO-R is a reliable and valid measurement tool, but that the child’s developmental level clearly contributes to his other success for learning. Equally important, they assert that many children are not developmentally ready for rigorous academic work, but rather a learning environment that embraces play, relevant curriculum strategies, and not rote memorization. They argue that children could have been engaged in meaningful play and building a foundation for the academic core. The second article written by Matilde Sarmiento, a teacher/ parent conducts a case study of her four year old in a preschool in Barcelona, Spain. Sarmiento documents the frustration of play being challenged and not understood by families, community, and public policy as not valuable in the child’s development. She affirms the research of many researchers and parents in asking the important questions about the importance play and its role in appropriate and meaningful curriculum. Sarmiento speaks and moves between the worlds of teacher and parent as she struggles with observations of inappropriate classrooms where academics are stressed, and the multitude of decisions that she must make to ensure that her child has the right to play, and has a quality setting “to play” where the teachers also understand the importance of play. The third fascinating article speaks to the need for children re-connecting with nature in her article entitled, “Artful Play at the Backyard Easel”. Geneser details how children talk, share, describe, and dramatize 3 while they are painting, and how this discourse is shaped by their emotional and physical environments. She illustrates through scenarios of children engaging in art outside the importance of careful listening and observing a child’s play. She argues that outdoor environments matter as she describes the importance of express language, artistic endeavors and symbolic play. Through the play of Sophie, she points out the need to carefully listen and learn from their play. The author explains the numerous ways that outdoor play support, extend and enrich language development. After reading this article, you may be eager to go and observe children dig in the worm bin! The fourth article of the journal is a resurrection of play by Laurel Moldawsky Silber and Barbara Moldawsky Stern. They revive the importance of play through their lens of memories as sisters in play, but also as researchers who study play. They cite research and current studies that examine the value of play, and eloquently point out that children are stressed out. They reflected upon their early experiences, learning, joys, challenges of play and reflect upon how play contributed to their own development and learning. They as they struggle with the existing challenges of the neglect and absence of play, they examine their own personal journey as children. The article is both personal and professional as they call for awareness, commitment and activism on behalf of play. As you read this article, and complete the journal, you will find yourself drifting to early memories of play, examining your own research and advocacy and wondering and affirming why play is so very important. And finally, there is a brief history of IPA and IPA/USA that you will find fascinating. Our roots go back almost 100 years! Warm regards. Georgianna Duarte duartefree@aol.com Design and layout: Marcy Guddemi IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 4 CONTENTS Chapter 1 Title Page Where Is the Play? Current Kindergarten Expectations Unsubstantiated: Findings from Gesell Institute’s Study of Preschool Children Marcy Guddemi, Kathleen Fite, and Gitta Selva 6 2 Her Favorite Four Letter Word – Play Matilde A. Sarmiento-Arribalzaga ………………………………. 21 3 Artful Play at the Backyard Easel: Thinking, Talking and Painting Outdoors Vivien Geneser…………………………………………………… 30 4 Resurrecting Play for the Next Generation Laurel Moldawsky Silber and Barbara Moldawsky Stern 5 37 International Play Association (IPA) and the American Affiliate IPA/USA: A Brief History Marcy Guddemi and Joe Frost…………………………………… 46 IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 5 Where Is the Play? Current Kindergarten Expectations Unsubstantiated: Findings from Gesell Institute’s Study of Preschool Children Marcy Guddemi, Kathleen Fite, and Gitta Selva abstract Note: The authors would like to acknowledge the work of Andrea Sambrook, research project coordinator, and Bruce Randel, psychometrician, for this study. Educators and parents are increasingly concerned about the effects of high-stakes testing on children who may not be developmentally ready to perform tasks according to the rigorous standards of today’s kindergarten curriculum (Copple & Bredekamp, 2009; Almon & Miller, 2011). In response to this issue, and to provide renewed validity evidence for the ©2007 Gesell Developmental Observation (GDO), Gesell Institute of Child Development conducted a nationwide study to collect new data with almost 1,300 children ages 3 to 6 years. New data is presented for 17 of the 19 original tasks on the GDO, and additional measures of overt behavior and social/emotional/adaptive skills. Results confirm that children are able to perform developmental items within tasks according to a sequential trajectory of increasing difficulty, relative to their chronological age in six month intervals, and that performance mastery on items does not occur at the same time for all children of the same age. In addition, results support the continued validity and reliability of the revised GDO, now named the Gesell Developmental Observation-Revised (GDO-R), as an instrument to determine a child’s developmental level along a continuous path of growth and learning. Also discussed are the importance of establishing effective and appropriate academic goals and the role of play on a child’s developmental assessment results. Introduction Cultivating expectations that are not appropriately aligned with the child’s development creates unrealistic levels for achievement of young children. As the problematic No Child Left Behind legislation saw many children indeed left “behind,” American educators and policy makers advocated for more consistent and rigorous standardized benchmarks for learning for younger and younger children, resulting in the Common Core State Standards in 2010 for Grade Kindergarten to 12 in Math and Literacy. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 While these standards represent a widespread initiative to try to close the achievement gap and provide accountability through test scores, many early childhood advocates question their appropriateness and even whether or not they are achievable. One of the most alarming aspects is that most Common Core advocates eliminate the use of play as a learning medium. Many policy and decision makers appear to be obsessed with the educational idea that sooner is better than later when it comes to learning core knowledge. Jean 6 Piaget (1896-1980), an influential Swiss child psychologist, referred to the American fixation that children should learn “sooner and faster” as “the American disease” (Guddemi and Zigler, 2011). Drilling children to memorize academic facts at earlier ages will not result in greater achievement for the vast majority of children, due to the developmental trajectory of child development and individual differences among children (Almond & Miller, 2011). Also, increased testing intervals will not help or hasten the process. In fact, standardized testing is very unreliable with young children under the age of seven. Experts have determined that there is only a 40% correlation between results of intelligence tests administered prior to kindergarten and results on achievement tests in third grade (Kim & Suen, 2003). Informed parents, educators, and advocates of young children seek effective ways to establish academic goals for young children which correspond appropriately to developmental level, including social, emotional, and adaptive capacities. Arnold Gesell, PhD and MD, developed the first assessment of human development, identifying the ages and stages of child development based on his maturationist theory (Gesell, 1925). He published the original Gesell assessment, known today as the Gesell Developmental Observation (GDO). It was updated in 1940 and 1965. In 1979, Ames, Gillespie, Haines, and Ilg published Gesell Institute’s The Child from One to Six: Evaluating the Behavior of the Preschool Child with updated technical data for the GDO. This was the last time a study was conducted using the GDO solely for the purpose of collecting scientific data. A handful of other small-scale studies and dissertation research projects were conducted in the last 30 years for a variety IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 of purposes, mostly surrounding issues of school readiness and transition classrooms. In 2007, minor edits were made to the GDO by the Institute, and in 2011 the newly revalidated and revised Gesell Developmental ObservationRevised (GDO-R) was published. The (GDO-R) evaluates a child’s performance on a series of developmental and academic tasks in relation to the sequential ages and stages of typical child development in the cognitive, motor, language, and social/emotional/adaptive domains. The results of the GDO-R reveal a child’s Developmental Age and Performance Level Ratings in each of five domains at a point in time. A Developmental Age may differ from the child’s chronological age, being lower or higher, or be the same. Knowing each child’s Developmental Age enables educators to customize developmentally appropriate academic experiences based on play and curricula to best meet the learning needs of every child. This paper presents some of the psychometric results and implications for tasks and subtasks on the GDO-R, indicating the typical ages at which specific developmental and academic tasks needed for realistic, developmentally appropriate success in kindergarten are mastered. Also discussed is the role of play as a learning medium as it relates to child development. Literature Review Experts have stressed the significance of the kindergarten year as it relates to the child’s development and the child’s ability to succeed within the school environment. Kindergarten sets the tone for learning and future school success (Black, 2008; Guddemi & Zigler, 2011). Embarking on new learning creates numerous opportunities for the development of the child not only in areas of cognition, but 7 also in social, emotional, and physical growth, as well as in his/her growth as an individual within a community. Lifelong, vital skills are acquired through the appropriate play-based learning opportunities presented within the kindergarten environment (Galinsky, 2010). With an increased and emphasis placed on rigorous academic new standards and accountability, educators and parents are faced with new challenges relating to a new definition of school readiness and the academic curriculum in kindergarten.. Schools play an important role in readiness; however, various schools have different expectations regarding readiness. A child may be considered prepared for one school environment and not prepared for another based on that particular school’s expectations for readiness (Maxwell & Clifford, 2004). Because play has been eliminated from most kindergartens, parents and educators are concerned as the pressures and demands within early learning environments have increased. Kagan and Lowenstein (2004, pg. 59) proposed that many view play as an “oxymoron to school readiness.” Today’s kindergarten academic expectations are set essentially at the achievement levels of first grades 20 years ago (Miller & Almon, 2009). Although there is much discussion relating to the readiness of incoming Kindergarten children, it is a school’s responsibility to educate children who are legally of age to attend school. Most states require children to attend school by a certain age regardless of their readiness or skill level. On the flip-side there is also a need for schools to be ready for the child. It is widely accepted that school readiness is multidimensional (National Education Goals Panel, 1997; Ewing Marion Kauffman Foundation, 2002) and IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 encompasses the following areas: physical well-being and motor development, social and emotional development, language development, approaches to learning, and cognition and general knowledge (National Education Goals Panel, 1997). Therefore, for a school to be ready for the child all of these areas must be addressed by the school--and a play-based curriculum is the best way to do so (Guddemi & Zigler, 2011). Furthermore, it is essential that schools, communities, and families acknowledge gaps in each child’s educational abilities that can occur based not only on individual differences in normal development, but also on such factors as birth weight, nutrition, television viewing, parent-child ratio, children’s exposure to language and literacy, and parental involvement and participation in the child’s well-being. Despite a national focus on early childhood education, current research indicates that educational gaps continue to exist, and that achievement gaps occur prior to the beginning of elementary school (Langham, 2009). It has been suggested that high quality early education that aligns with high quality kindergarten through third grade programs plays a critical role in attempting to close educational gaps, and potentially contributes to enhancing the child’s development, school readiness, and future school success. Long-term effects of a quality pre-kindergarten experience can affect grade retention, placement, special education, and school dropout rates (Mead, 2008; Barnett, 1993; Campbell et al., 2002). Unfortunately, not all children have the opportunity to participate in a high quality play-based pre-kindergarten program. A consistent characteristic of high quality preK and K programs is the reliance on developmentally appropriate practices for each child based on each 8 child’s needs. Developmentally appropriate practice (DAP) is defined by the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC). DAP is a means to meet a child’s educational/instructional needs by first understanding where a child is developmentally and then providing both challenging and achievable experiences based on the child’s stage of development. The organization supports educators in “promoting young children’s optimal learning and development” (Copple & Bredekamp, 2009, p.16). NAEYC’s DAP encourages educators to provide learning opportunities that will enhance all areas of a child’s development and to understand that a child’s development follows a welldocumented, sequential order. DAP is based on being aware that each child develops at his/her own unique rate and that learning opportunities need to be challenging, but within the child’s ability. Understanding a child’s development are keys to setting expectations that are appropriate and to planning curriculum that meets the child’s needs and abilities. Early educators need to adhere to methods and practices of teaching that foster a child’s development with learning being concentrated in all areas of development – cognitive, social, emotional, language, and physical (Kagan & Reid, 2009). Developmentally appropriate practice also means using play as a medium for learning. “Play is the work of the child” is frequently heard as one reason that children need to learn through active, hands-on interactions with their environment and with others. There is much research to show that “play-filled” classrooms have better outcomes than direct instruction classroom on reading and math scores (Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, & Singer, 2006; Bodrova & Leong, 2007). Of particular interest is the research on mature, sustained, pretend play with IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 others and the increased development of prefrontal cortex of the brain that is responsible for Executive Functioning, namely skills such as self-control, flexibility, and working memory. Unfortunately, many adults view play as frivolous and meaningless, or the opposite of work (Almon & Miller, 2011). In order to plan developmentally appropriate practice, assessing the readiness or where a child is on the path of development is essential in determining what play experiences a child is ready for. This use of readiness assessments should never exclude children from learning opportunities, but rather help determine how and what educational and learning experiences should be developed and/or modified to meet the child’s developmental level (Gullo, 2005). Through documentation and assessment, an educator is better able to understand the child. These instruments offer insight into a child’s development and his ability to learn, making learning visible to the educator (Seitz, 2008). Since learning is multi-dimensional, it is important for an assessment to be used as a tool to help educators better understand children, their development, and how they learn (Tomlinson, 2008). Research on the GDO-R The purpose of Gesell Institute’s GDO Study was to provide updated technical data, validity, and reliability evidence for 17 of the 19 original tasks on the ©2007 GDO with three to six year old children (see Table 1 in the Appendix). Two tasks, which were intended for assessing older children, were omitted. Another purpose of the GDO Study was to include Overt Behavior (Task 20) and to strengthen the social/emotional/adaptive domain by use of the new Parent/Guardian Questionnaire and Teacher Questionnaire (Task 21). The criterion that shaped this study, and 9 ultimately the scoring rubrics in this revision, was based on three sources of information: 1. Scientific data collected on a nation-wide sample of 1287 children ages three to six years old. This technical data provides information about how children across the U.S. performed on GDO-R tasks, and it can be used to compare a child’s performance to that of typically developing agematched peers. 2. Knowledge and experience of professionals who teach and work with children in each age band. A panel of nationally recognized experts with extensive experience in the field of child development agreed upon the GDO-R Performance Level Definitions as a means for examiners to confirm a child’s overall results on the GDO-R. 3. Well-established research findings and theoretical frameworks. Children grow and mature through a series of predictable stages in a sequential order. Their development is dynamic, continuous, and reflects a pace unique to each child. The sample included a diverse group of sites (N=53) in terms of the type of school, region of the country (23 states), size, and population served (i.e., ethnicity and percentage eligible for free and reduced lunch). Refer to Figure 1 for sample distribution by state and Tables 2 to 4 for demographics of participating sites. Sites volunteered to be part of the study. Almost all sites were current users of the GDO and thus should be considered a sample of convenience. (See complete Technical Report for more details.) Results IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 Presented here are five tables to demonstrate the points made in the following implications. Please refer to Table 5 (Cubes), Table 6 (Copy Forms), Table 7 (Identifying Letters), Table 8 (One-to-One Correspondence and Conservation), and Table 9 (Calculations) in the Appendix. Complete psychometric data can be found in the Technical Report (Gesell, 2012). Overall results confirm that children are able to perform developmental items (subparts of the task) according to a sequential trajectory of increasing difficulty, relative to their chronological age in six month intervals, and that performance mastery on items does not occur at the same time for all children of the same age. The tables, even at a glance, show the developmental trajectory by following the color-coding. Dark grey means that 70% or more of the children at that age band could complete the task. Solid Performance Expectations are foreseeable for that age group. The light grey shading means that 50-69% of all children at that age band could complete the task; thus for this group the outcome is a Qualified Performance Expectation. White means fewer than 50% of all children could perform the task; thus this group has No Performance Expectation or Not Yet. The data presented here and in the complete Technical Report confirm that not all children arrive at the same time at important developmental milestones. The data helps us understand when most children might arrive at these milestones, and when we can have solid, qualified, or no expectation yet. Implications The most valuable implication of Gesell Institute’s research is that the GDO-R has renewed reliability and validity evidence to support its continued use as a 10 developmental instrument to evaluate growth and development of children age 3-6 years, and inform instruction for developmentally appropriate activities. The results from this study also support the original findings for developmental tasks as originally published by Arnold Gesell. Children are developing and reaching the major developmental milestones at about the same time as they did when Dr. Gesell first started collecting date over a century ago (Gesell, 1925). A few of the important implications of the research for educators nationwide include the following: 1. Perceiving oblique lines is a prerequisite to letter formation and writing two essential expectations in the kindergarten curriculum of today. Building the Gate (Task 1 - Cubes) and copying the Triangle (Task 4 - Copy Forms) require that the child not only perceive the oblique angle of the cube or the form, but is able to reproduce the structure in 3-D or on paper. The GDO study documents that this developmental capacity is solid only by age 5 (Task 1 - Cubes - Gate) and 5.5 (Task 4 - Copy Forms - Triangle). Educators must be alert to both chronological variations in age and developmental level in order to properly balance the pace and sequence of daily learning activities for each child. 2. Children correctly identify letters in the alphabet in a graduated process that is affected by age, experience and exposure to the printed word. As such, the average 4.5 year old can successfully identify approximately 12 letters of the alphabet, and by a year later, at 5.5, they can identify 21-22 letters. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 Educators who attempt to teach writing letters before the age of 5.5 (when most children can perceive and execute oblique lines of letters) are doing their young students a disservice, which may possibly result in a child internalizing failed attempts at writing before his or her developmental capacity for the task exists. Taking the time to understand how developmental level can be leveraged for teaching will benefit both children and teachers. 3. Educators who are able to recognize when a child is beginning to conserve 10 or more items will likely find that the child can also begin to succeed at simple calculations which have final answers less than 5 (beginning around 5.5 years and solid expectation by 6). Until a child can conserve item sets of 13 -20, his or her success at calculations will likely remain the product of memorization or chance, as opposed to concepts of true numeracy. Conclusion The results of this study, based on a culturally and socio-economically diverse sample of children three to six years of age in seven age bands, provide evidence that children’s performance on developmental and academic tasks, as measured by the Gesell Developmental Observation-Revised, occurs in a sequential progression of mastery that increases with age. In addition, the results provide evidence that not all children of the same chronological age arrive at each developmental level for the same tasks at the same time. Thus, there exists variation in performance on 11 developmental and academic tasks between children of the same age. Future research should include a more intensive analysis of the data by weighting variables such as child ethnicity, geography, and socio-economic level to pursue solidity in the findings. It is essential that educators, policy makers, and parents understand the significance of developmental level in setting standards and planning appropriate curriculum (content and instructional strategy) for children. By using robust observational methods coupled with comprehensive developmental assessment tools, educators can utilize each child’s unique developmental profile to plan curriculum that respects the developmental level and potential of the child. Utilizing standardized, performance-based instruments to understand an individual developmental level, cultural and social influences, and individual interests and needs of each child allows for consistency of expectations, appropriate goals for learning, and proper accountability in the educational system. The results of the GDO Study presented here strongly support the GDOR as a reliable and valid developmental measurement tool, and confirm the essential role that a child’s developmental level contributes to his or her success for learning today and for preparation as a citizen of tomorrow. The study also confirms that many children are not developmentally ready for the rigorous academic work mandated by the Common Core. Young children learn through playbased curricula. Memorizing content that the child is not developmentally ready to master is a waste of time in two ways. One, the child, through rote memorization, is not really learning; and IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 two, the child could have been playing and truly learning and building the foundation that will support the more rigorous common core when the child is ready to understand. Authors: Marcy Guddemi, PhD, MBA As Executive Director of Gesell Institute of Child Development, Marcy leads her team “promoting the principles of child development in all decision-making for young children.” She is widely recognized as an expert in early education, learning though play, and developmental assessment. mguddemi@gesellinstitute.org Kathleen Fite, EdD As Distinguished Alumni and professor of education at Texas State University, Kathleen is an advocate for children worldwide, has worked as a writer, consultant, researcher, educator, and leader at many levels. She is a Gesell International Ambassador and serves on the Gesell Institute Advisory Council. She also serves on the Executive Board for the Association of Childhood Education International. She has taught public school and undergraduate through doctoral level university classes. Gitta Selva is the Director of Programs and Editorial Services at Gesell Institute of Child Development. She previously worked at Queensland University of Technology’s School of Learning and Professional Studies in Brisbane, Australia, and Yale University’s Center in Child Development and Social Policy. She co-authored several scholarly articles on teacher education and service learning. gitta@gesellinstitute.org Gesell Institute of Child Development 310 Prospect St. New Haven, CT 06511 203-777-3481 office@gesellinstitute.org 12 References Almon, J., & Miller, E. (2011). The crisis in early education: A research-based case for more play and less pressure. College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood. Barnett, W. S. (1993). Benefit-cost analysis of preschool education: Findings from a 25-year follow up. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63(4), 25-50. Black, S. (2008). Early education, later success. American School Board Journal, 196(9), 61-63. Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian approach to early childhood education (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice Hall. Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E. P., Sparling, J., & Miller-Johnson, S. (2002). Early childhood education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian project. Applied Developmental Science, 6, 42-57. Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2009). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs: Serving children from birth through age 8. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Ewing Marion Kauffman Foundation (2002). Set for success: Building a strong foundation for school based on the social-emotional development of young children. Kansas City, MO: Author. Galinsky, E. (2010). Mind in the making. New York City: Harper-Collins.. Gesell, A. (1925). The mental growth of the pre-school child: A psychological outline of normal development from birth to the sixth year, including a system of developmental diagnosis. Macmillan. Gesell Institute of Child Development (2012). Gesell developmental observation-revised and Gesell early screener technical report ages 3-6. New Haven, CT: Author. Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Singer, D. (2006). Play=learning: A challenge for parents and educators. In D. Singer, R. M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds). Play=learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and socialemotional growth. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Guddemi, M., & Zigler, E. (2011). Community early childhood LEADership e-kit [CDROM]. New Haven, CT: Gesell Institute of Child Development. Gullo, D. (2005). Understanding assessment and evaluation in early childhood education. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Kagan, S. L., & Lowenstein, A. E. (2004). School readiness and children’s play: Contemporary oxymoron or compatible option? In E. Zigler, D. Singer, & S. BishopJosef (Eds). Children’s play: The roots of reading. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press. Kagan S. L., & Reid, J. (2009). Invest in early childhood education. Phi Delta Kappan, 90(8), 572-576. Kim, J., & Suen, H. K. (2003). Predicting children’s academic achievement from early assessment scores: A validity generalization study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18(4), 547-566. Langham, B. A. (2009). The achievement gap: What early childhood educators need to know. Texas Child Care Quarterly. Retrieved November 21, 2012 from http://www.childcarequarterly.com/fall09_story2a.html Maxwell, K., & Clifford, R. (2004). Research in review: School readiness assessment. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 13 Young Children & Assessment. Washington, D.C.: NAEYC. Mead, S. (2008). Find success in early childhood education. American School Board Journal, 195(11), 25-29. Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Crisis in the kindergarten: Why children need to play in school. College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood. National Education Goals Panel. (1997). The national education goals report: Building a nation of learners. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Seitz, H. (2008). The power of documentation in the early childhood classroom. Young Children, 63(2), 88-92. Tomlinson, C. A. (2008). Learning to love assessment. Educational Leadership, 65(4), 8-13. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 14 Appendix Figure 1 Sample Distribution by State Note. Participating states (N=23) are indicated by a star. Table 1 GDO-R Tasks Task # 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Task Name Cubes Interview Name and Numbers Copy Forms Incomplete Man Right and Left Visual I Visual III Naming Animals Interests Prepositions Digit Repetition Comprehension Questions Color Forms Three-Hole Form Board Action Agents IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 GDO©2007 X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X GDO-R X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 15 17 18 19 20 21 Identifying Letters and Numbers Numeracy Motor (Fine and Gross) Overt Behavior Social, Emotional, and Adaptive X X X X X X X X Table 2 Descriptive Statistics for Participating Sites (N=53): School Type School Type # Sites #GDO-R Assessments Private Public 33 (62%) 584 (45%) 20 (38%) 703 (55%) South West Table 3 Descriptive Statistics for Participating Sites: U.S. Region Region Participating States # Sites # GDO-R Assessments Northeast CT, MA, ME, NY, PA 20 (38%) 672 (52%) Midwest KS, MI, MN, ND, OH, SD 9 (17%) 201 (16%) AL, FL, GA, KY, LA, NC, SC, TN, TX 18 (34%) 288 (22%) AZ, CA, CO 6 (11%) 126 (10%) Table 4 Descriptive Statistics for Participating Sites: Ethnicity and SES Ethnicity Mean percent African-American 14.8 (34.1) American Indian 2.9 (.6) Asian American 4.5 (1.0) Caucasian not Hispanic 60.7 (42.8) Hispanic 15.3 (28.1) Other 1.8 (1.2) % Eligible for Free/Reduced Lunch 28.2 (35.64) Enrollment N Mean # children enrolled 168 Note. Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations. Eligibility for Free/Reduced Lunch program is used as a representative variable in the sample for lower socioeconomic level. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 16 Table 5 P-values for Cubes Task by Age Band (polytomous items) 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5 6.0 (n=53) (n=130) (n=186) (n=264) (n=278) (n=221) (n=152) Tower 0.86 0.91 0.93 0.94 0.97 0.99 1.00 Train 0.61 0.68 0.83 0.91 0.93 0.96 0.94 Bridge 0.51 0.66 0.87 0.94 0.98 0.99 1.00 Gate 0.07 0.21 0.32 0.64 0.80 0.89 0.93 Steps (6) 0.06 0.05 0.14 0.32 0.49 0.65 0.81 Steps (10) 0.02 0.02 0.05 0.16 0.35 0.52 0.74 Note. In Cubes, examiners were instructed to administer each item, and provide a DEMO if required. Each item was scored using three categories: Successfully completed without DEMO (2), Successfully completed with DEMO (1), Unsuccessful (0). Table 6 P-values for Copy Forms Task by Age Band 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5 6.0 (n=53) (n=131) (n=185) (n=261) (n=278) (n=220) (n=153) Scribble 0.81 0.87 0.90 0.91 0.95 0.91 0.79 Stroke – Vertical 0.75 0.87 0.85 0.92 0.95 0.93 0.82 Stroke – Horizontal 0.74 0.80 0.83 0.91 0.95 0.92 0.83 Circle 0.74 0.85 0.94 0.98 0.99 1.00 0.99 Cross 0.30 0.53 0.67 0.87 0.94 0.97 1.00 Square 0.23 0.25 0.36 0.71 0.83 0.93 0.98 Triangle 0.08 0.15 0.12 0.35 0.55 0.73 0.90 Divided Rectangle 0.02 0.04 0.06 0.12 0.26 0.41 0.65 Diamond – Horizontal 0.02 0.04 0.05 0.05 0.19 0.34 0.48 Diamond – Vertical 0.02 0.04 0.05 0.07 0.19 0.34 0.58 3-D Cylinder 0.00 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.07 3-D Cube Faceon 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.01 3-D Cube Pointon 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.01 Note. In Copy Forms, examiners were instructed to administer each item, and to administer demonstrations (DEMOS) if required. However, for the purpose of the study, each item was scored (0, 1) regardless of any DEMO required. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 17 Table 7 P-values for Identifying Letters Task by Age Band 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 (n=50) (n=125) (n=172) (n=229) (n=232) A 0.12 0.15 0.34 0.60 0.80 B 0.06 0.14 0.30 0.54 0.69 C 0.06 0.14 0.31 0.50 0.71 D 0.04 0.14 0.20 0.47 0.66 E 0.06 0.10 0.24 0.48 0.69 F 0.02 0.13 0.20 0.45 0.59 G 0.06 0.12 0.20 0.42 0.59 H 0.02 0.10 0.20 0.48 0.62 I 0.00 0.10 0.13 0.34 0.52 J 0.04 0.14 0.20 0.47 0.65 K 0.06 0.20 0.21 0.49 0.63 L 0.04 0.11 0.19 0.45 0.66 M 0.06 0.16 0.24 0.46 0.66 N 0.04 0.12 0.19 0.47 0.63 O 0.06 0.15 0.34 0.57 0.75 P 0.06 0.12 0.23 0.47 0.66 Q 0.02 0.16 0.22 0.42 0.60 R 0.02 0.13 0.22 0.46 0.65 S 0.08 0.11 0.21 0.51 0.70 T 0.04 0.14 0.22 0.48 0.66 U 0.04 0.07 0.17 0.38 0.56 V 0.02 0.08 0.13 0.36 0.56 W 0.06 0.16 0.25 0.42 0.64 X 0.02 0.17 0.33 0.54 0.72 Y 0.04 0.14 0.23 0.46 0.60 Z 0.04 0.14 0.23 0.48 0.65 Mean p value .05 .13 .23 .46 .65 Mean number of 1.18 3.41 5.9 12.04 16.83 letters identified (3.10) (6.74) (8.25) (10.23) (9.67) IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 5.5 (n=184) 0.89 0.86 0.87 0.82 0.83 0.84 0.79 0.84 0.64 0.83 0.82 0.82 0.82 0.81 0.90 0.88 0.83 0.85 0.89 0.85 0.80 0.74 0.83 0.91 0.82 0.83 .83 21.6 (7.38) 6.0 (n=139) 0.96 0.94 0.92 0.92 0.94 0.93 0.93 0.94 0.81 0.93 0.94 0.94 0.93 0.95 0.94 0.94 0.90 0.91 0.93 0.93 0.91 0.89 0.91 0.97 0.93 0.96 .93 24.07 (4.86) 18 Table 8 P-values for One-to-One Correspondence and Conservation Tasks by Age Band 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5 6.0 (n=53) (n=128) (n=171) (n=231) (n=219) (n=179) (n=135) 4 pennies, count them 0.43 0.55 0.74 0.85 0.94 0.99 0.99 Altogether 0.23 0.25 0.38 0.62 0.79 0.85 0.93 10 pennies, count them 0.13 0.27 0.43 0.59 0.80 0.87 0.87 Altogether 0.08 0.08 0.26 0.46 0.72 0.80 0.85 13 pennies, count them 0.06 0.14 0.18 0.45 0.60 0.80 0.86 Altogether 0.02 0.04 0.10 0.34 0.54 0.74 0.82 20 pennies, count them 0.02 0.06 0.08 0.27 0.45 0.67 0.81 Altogether 0.02 0.02 0.06 0.24 0.43 0.64 0.76 Table 9 P-values for Calculations Task by Age Band 3.0 3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5 6.0 (n=44) (n=107) (n=147) (n=208) (n=214) (n=179) (n=137) 2+2 0.08 0.17 0.25 0.44 0.64 0.77 0.87 2+3 0.04 0.13 0.17 0.39 0.53 0.68 0.84 5-2 0.07 0.13 0.21 0.35 0.50 0.63 0.77 7+3 0.01 0.07 0.08 0.19 0.35 0.48 0.70 6-4 0.08 0.11 0.21 0.27 0.34 0.51 0.66 14+3 0.00 0.02 0.03 0.08 0.18 0.30 0.55 16-4 0.00 0.02 0.06 0.09 0.15 0.25 0.41 Note. Children were scored using three categories; Successfully completed without pennies (2), Successfully completed with pennies (1), or Unsuccessful (0). Descriptions of Tasks Task 1: Cubes This set of items requires the child to reproduce block structures built by the examiner: The Tower, the Train, the Bridge, the Gate, Steps with 6 cubes, and Steps with 10 cubes. The ability to reproduce the structures successfully and the approach to the item used by the child provides information about horizontal and visual perception, fine motor coordination, attention span, spatial judgment, and short term memory. Children are able to successfully build the Tower (10 cubes) by 3.0 years, the Train (4 cubes) by 4.0 years, the Bridge (3 cubes) by 4.0 years, the IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 Gate (5 cubes) by 5.0 years, the Steps (6 cubes) by 6.0 years, and finally the Steps (10 cubes) at 6.0 years. Task 4: Copy Forms The child is asked to copy a Circle, Cross, Square, Triangle, Divided Rectangle, Vertical and Horizontal Diamonds, and ultimately 3-dimensional shapes (cube and cylinder) according to his/her age and demonstrated ability. Some age 6 children were not administered the first three items (Scribble, Horizontal and Vertical Stroke) when the examiner believed the items were too easy for these children. In this case, the children received a missing score that was 19 treated as incorrect for the analyses, leading to the lower p-values for these items for age 6 children. Children are able to copy the Circle by 3.0 years, the Cross and the Square by 4.5 years, the Triangle by 5.5 years, and the Divided Rectangle only after 6.0 years of age. Task 17: Identifying Letters and Numbers a) Identifying Letters This task requires a child to identify random capital letters. This task is dependent on prior exposure and knowledge of the alphabet. Children are able to identify a mean of 17 letters by the time they are 5.0 years and eligible to enter Kindergarten. b) Identifying Numbers This task requires a child to identify numerals 1-12 by name. This task is dependent on exposure to and knowledge of numbers. Children are able to identify a mean of 8 numbers by the time they are 5.0 years and enter Kindergarten. Task 18: Numeracy (Counting, One-to-One Correspondence, Conservation, and Calculations) a) Counting Counting reveals the child’s experience with and ability to remember numbers in a sequence. Children were allowed to count up to 40. As age increases, children’s ability to count to higher numbers increases as well. By age 4.0, children can count correctly to approximately 10. By age IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 5.0, they count to about 24, and by age 6.0, to 34. b) and c) One-to-One Correspondence and Conservation One-to-One Correspondence evaluates the child’s understanding that each item is represented only once by a number name. Children learn to count with one-toone correspondence before they learn to conserve the same number of items; e.g., the child counts one-to-one by pointing to each of 10 pennies, but when asked, “how many altogether?” the young child often tries to recount, or push them physically together with his or her hands. By age 4.5, children are able to count 4 pennies, but may not be able to conserve 4 pennies. By age 5.0, children can both count to 10 and conserve 10 items. They begin to have some success at counting to and conserving 13 pennies, but it is not until the age of 5.5 when they can both count and conserve 13, but not likely 20 items until the age of 6.0. d) Calculations The calculation task demonstrates a child’s ability to compute simple mathematical problems without counting. Beginning at age 5.0, children show emerging ability to successfully complete calculations with digits less than 10 where the answer is less than 5 (with or without using pennies.) 20 Her Favorite Four Letter Word – Play abstract Matilde A. Sarmiento-Arribalzaga The overall purpose of this single case study is to describe the experiences of a preschool child participating in an early childhood program that uses play as its premise to develop curriculum and the child. Although “play” is just a four letter word, in today’s educational environment the word is viewed negatively. These preliminary findings support the need to advocate and begin thinking about a new preschool pedagogy that takes into account the similarities of play and learning from the child’s perspective. Play is so important that its significance in children’s lives is recognized by the United Nations in Article 31 (Leisure, play and culture) as a specific right in addition to, and distinct from, a child’s right to recreation and leisure. "Children have the right to relax and play, and to join in a wide range of cultural, artistic, and other recreational activities."(http://www.unicef.org/crc/). Unfortunately, over the last several decades the physical and social environments in which preschool children develop has changed significantly. In traditional early childhood (EC) classrooms, preschool children rarely have long uninterrupted blocks of time to play indoors or outdoors, by themselves or with their peers. Instead we see a growing number of young children spending substantial time in settings that are strictly structured for educational and assessment purposes (Almon, 2003; Bodrova & Leong, 2005; Christensen & Kelly, 2003; Stipek, 2006). Despite warnings from as early as 1987 from the National Association for the Education of Young Children IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 (NAEYC), in 2003 the U.S. federal government began using standardized tests in programs such as Head Start to measure achievement in literacy and number skills (Almon, 2003, Rothstein, 2004). This coupled with most states already using standardized testing in the kindergarten level for readiness and screening tests given before entering and graduating from kindergarten, the preschool educational experience is no longer just about the sandbox, dramatic play, blocks and finger-painting. Instead preschoolers find themselves being drilled on letters, dividing words into syllables and spelling (Almon, 2003; Christensen & Kelly, 2003; Clements & Fiorentino, 2004). As elementary schools in the United States become more test driven there is a direct impact on what preschoolers are expected to learn in an EC setting. This coupled with the limited hours that preschoolers are in school and the pressure for them to be ready for elementary, play in the EC classroom is usually replaced by structured and teacher-led activities that are believed to 21 help them be successful academically. In today’s test driven educational systems, many including parents and EC teachers have forgotten that learning through play during EC is basis for later critical thinking and thus paving the way for academic success (Ashiabi, 2007; Bredekamp, 2005; Carlsson-Paige, 2001). In spring 2002, legislation was introduced by the Senate H.E.L.P. Committee to make more funds available to EC programs in all 50 states. The committee estimated that a total of one billion dollars per year would be allocated towards helping the Early Care and Education Act address the importance of physical, social, and emotional development as well as early literacy in preschoolers. In theory this was a positive development but in practice, the legislation allowed for bonuses to be given to states that can could show gains in children’s school preparedness; it has become the driving force to pressure preschoolers to do better and to learn faster (Johnson, 2006; Olfman, 2003; Oliver & Klugman, 2007). Rather than allow children to engage in play, teachers under pressure to prepare students for standardized tests are turning to planned lessons and even lectures. All this mad rush to learn being guided by a mistaken belief that an early emphasis on academics will lead to creating better students. There is also pressure from parents, who believe that their children benefit from direct instruction and early formal academic lessons. As a result, parents and teachers have begun to teach preschool and kindergarten children as if they were in a more advanced grade (Buchanan & Cooney, 2000, Elkind, 2007; Fisher, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, & Glick, 2008, Oliver & Klugman, 2004, Stipek, 2006). Although “play” is just a four letter word in today’s educational environment, IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 the word is viewed negatively and can spark lively discussions. Consequently EC programs have begun to take the word out of their curriculum to placate parents and legislation. Many parents misguidedly prefer that their young children focus on academics and prefer classrooms that are more academically oriented. They believe that knowing the letters of the alphabet, being able to count to 20 or more, and using pencils and paint brushes are essential for a preschooler to be successful in elementary (Buchanan & Cooney, 2000; Fisher, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Glick, 2008, Heidemann, 2010; Oliver & Klugman, 2004). Sadly this all indicates a true lack of understanding of "play" as the learning opportunity instead of "play" as something children do between learning activities/lessons or as a reward for having done their work quickly (Blakemore, 2003; Elkind, 2003; Garvey, 1993; Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, Eyer, 2004). This lack of understanding is why during the fall of 2009 with the experience and knowledge of a former EC teacher and Department of Education university professor in the United States, a parent made the decision to relocate to Spain in order to 1) provide her toddler a different preschool education experience, 2) provide the toddler a trilingual education opportunity and 3) document toddler's language development over the period of 5 years. For the purpose of this article, the focus will be on the toddler's preschool education experience and the role “play” has had on her social, intellectual, and physical development (Buchanan & Cooney, 2000; Casby, 2003; Garvey, 1993; Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, & Eyer, 2004; Iverson, 2010; Vygostsky, 1976, 1978). The parents' experience with the conditions of public preschools in the United States over a period of 15 years 22 caused her great distress and concern as the child approached preschool age. The parent realized that there was the possibility of sending the child to a preschool where the toddler would find an EC setting featuring scripted teaching, computerized learning, standardized assessment and possible elimination of physical education and recess of being eliminated or replaced by Accelerated Reader, Voyager, etc… depending on the school, district and program they were part of. While many of the EC programs attempt to provide a “quality preschool education,” they actually undermining a child’s natural ability to learn by pressuring them with their "academic readiness" agenda (Almon, 2003; Fisher, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Glick, 2008; Olfman, 2003; Oliver & Klugman, 2006). Phrases such as “bridging the preKelementary school divide” and “creating seamless transitions from kindergarten to elementary school” which are common discussion topics in terms of preschool curriculum are in reality detrimental to EC programs (Oliver & Klugman, 2007; ). They tend to diminish the importance and the reality of how children develop socially, cognitively, and emotionally and that they do so very differently from older children in elementary school (Bar-Haim & Bart, 2006). The purpose of this article is not to put forth a negative critique of all EC programs in the United States, but mainly to advocate for the need to begin thinking about a new preschool pedagogy that takes into account the similarities of play and learning from the child’s perspective. As an educator but more importantly as a parent this individual wanted the child to have the environment needed to allow this child to learn and expand her mind. The mother firmly believes the child can only achieve this by having access to quality EC education that allows/fosters IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 independence and nurtures a child's ability to discover things on their own. She is a child after all and her favorite four letter word is PLAY (Carson, 2001; Doctoroff, 2001; Olfman, 2005; Piek, Dawson, Smith & Gasson, 2008). What I Know About Play as a Parent and a Teacher As a parent the most important thing she has done is support healthy play by giving the child space, time to play and having readily available simple play materials that were not store bought. For many months the family's pots, pans, spoons, sheets and pillowcases were exclusive domain of the child’s imagination and play activities. The child was allowed to be part of physical work that was done around the house such as sorting laundry, putting away groceries, taking care of a garden, hammering nails, measuring both with rulers and measuring cups during baking. Park time was a daily routine and t.v. viewing time was limited. At the age of 3 the child's daily life of play and work held no distinctions- if asked what she was doing when assisting with sorting laundry she would answer “playing.” The child like all other children was born with an urge to learn and continually develop new skills (Ashiabi, 2007; Bodrova & Leong, 2005; Buchanan & Cooney, 2000; Vygotsky, 1976; 1978). 23 Having worked extremely hard at home to ensure that the child's development was priority, the parent felt she had to find the ideal EC classroom setting that would respect the child's innate drive to learn and not strip her of her confidence in directing her own learning. The parent wanted an EC classroom that would not be hurrying the child along and burning her out. Piaget, whose research explains how children advance cognitively when they are engaged in a naturalistic hands-on exploration of the real world, informed the parents' expectations. As a side note, American educators often asked Piaget how the stages of cognitive development could be accelerated. Piaget called this “the American question” (Duckworth, 1979, p.303). Many studies today claim that children create knowledge when they play (Belka, 2004; Bodrova & Leong, 2005; Buchanan & Cooney, 2000; Oliver & Klugman, 2004; NAECS, 2001). Play gives children opportunities to be in control of what is happening and what they know. It is also linked to growth in memory, self-regulation, oral language, and recognizing symbols (Casby, 2003; Elkind, 2003, 2007; Garvey, 1993; Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek & Eyer, 2004). It has been linked to higher levels of school adjustment and increased social development. Play can also develop literacy skills and impact other areas of academic learning (a view held by Piagetian and Vygotskian theories of child development) (Ashiabi, 2007; Bredekamp, 2004; Clements & Fiorentino, 2004; Vygotsky, 1976, 1978). The preschool Upon the family's arrival in Barcelona, Spain the search for a preschool began. School visits, school administration interviews, and word of mouth led the IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 parent to a PK-12th grade consortium (a school that is funded 50% by the government and the remaining 50% is paid by parents as tuition) school that had “play” as part of their EC curriculum. After initial meetings with the school administrator and the EC director (interesting to note that the director herself teaches in the EC department) the parent was allowed to meet and interview the EC teachers that would be responsible for the child’s day to day activities. The EC director and teachers demonstrated a true understanding of play as a natural, essential part of every healthy child's social, emotional and intellectual development. A typical day On any given day a stroll through the child’s preschool and you will see children digging in dirt, building with sticks and rocks, planting seeds, hanging from trees, playing hide-and-seek, and exploring their natural surroundings. The child's preschool embraces play as premise for existence and expect children to run, jump, dig, explore, talk, build, paint, and sing. This school’s central feature focuses on a child-centered approach where children are allowed to free play from the moment they walk into classroom at 9 AM. The school day by U.S. standards is extremely long- 9 AM - 5:30 p.m. Initially 24 this was of great concern to the parent, but after discussing the daily schedule and having the preschool director explain the rationale behind the 5:30 p.m. dismissal time; it made sense. By the time the children are dismissed at 5:30p.m., they have had time allocated to do 30 minutes free play time block as soon as the children arrive (rationale- free play to get them moving and ready for their day at work/play), a 45 minute free play time block after a 30 minute lunch, a 30 minute nap/relaxation time block, snack time, and a 45 minutes physical activity block (swimming, tennis, or ballet). At the end of the day parents are expected to pick up children and just go about their daily lives without the pressure of having to take them to after school activities (parks are plentiful and full of children running, playing and enjoying themselves between 5:30 p.m. - 7 PM) (Bar-Haim & Bart, 2006; Belka, 2004; Bredekamp, 2004; Carlsson-Paige, 2008; Casby, 2003; Clements & Fiorentino, 2004). The classroom and playground by U.S. standards also have confined space and limited equipment. In contrast to a U.S. preschool classroom, where children are bombarded with an overabundance of toys and other play objects, as well as by an overload of sense impressions, this child's IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 classroom has limited toys and only child made art projects, stories, and drawings are on display. There is no massive overstimulation just a fairly simple but pleasing environment that is childcentered and owned (Doctoroff, 2001; Heidemann & Hewitt, 2010; Kowalski, Wyver, Masselos & deLacey, 2005). The playground consists of 4 large trees that provide shade. There is no swing set, no slide, no playground equipment just a few soccer balls that many times go unnoticed and ignored because children are so engrossed in playing, running, chasing each other, playing tag, digging in the dirt, collecting sticks, leaves, rocks and anything else they might find. With the limited play materials children seem to become inventive often times making or finding what they needed to complete their selfdirected and made up games (Bar-Haim & Bart, 2006; Blakemore, 2003; Carson, 2001; Doctoroff, 2001; Iverson, 2010; Olfman, 2005; Pate, Pfeiffer, Trost, Ziegler & Dowda, 2004; Payne & Issacs, 2008). Typically within the first 15 minutes of the children's arrival to the classroom, they change into their smocks and the learning environment is completely transformed with play material made available. A group of 24 children can easily create six or seven play areas for themselves because the classroom itself has no designated earning centers since they function under the premise that the entire school and its grounds are for learning. There is always a fair amount of negotiation that takes place as the children sort out who is going to use what and in which areas. When a child engages in meaningful activities such as play, their imagination is stimulated and this leads them to create and use methods that are unconventional – something that cannot be taught (Ashiabi, 25 2007; Bar-Haim & Bart, 2006; Belka, 2004; Carlsson-Paige, 2008; Doctoroff, 2001; Elkind, 2007; Iverson, 2010). During the 2012-2013 academic year, the child was completing year 2 of the EC program. The child was in P-4 and next academic year (2013-2014) will be in P-5, which is equivalent to the U.S. kindergarten. The child has never brought homework home but is asked to bring one favorite book to school on a daily basis. She speaks three languages (Catalan, Castellano and English), swims like a fish, dances like a ballerina, kicks like a soccer player, milks a cow like a farmer, loves to garden, slays dragons and can be cutest princess when not covered with band-aids. The parent measures the child's EC program effectiveness and academic rigor by using the premise that evidence of the curriculum is visible in the way the child plays. When observing the child playing, one will see that she is having fun, but take a closer look and one sees much more learning happening. The child knows her numbers, letters, can retell all the most popular fairytales with different endings depending on what occurred at school that day. Evidence of learning is also clearly present in the child's drawings. After a field trip to the Picasso Museum, the child produced a selfportrait and with much excitment explained to the parent how she was able to create it "using Picasso's painting style" - these were the child's words. The child's play is dynamic, multisensory, interactive, creative, and imaginative. Clearly developing skills (problem solving, basic literacy, math and science) that will lay the foundation for academic succeed in the future. The child loves schools, paper, pencils, colors, paint, rocks, sticks, mud, water, and truly knows how to PLAY! Final thoughts The last two years of collecting work samples, observing the child on the IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 playground, and playing with the child have yielded preliminary findings that clearly supports the premise that children learn naturally from, but teachers and parents must have an active role in ensuring that children have enough time and opportunities to play. It is thus important for parents to look for a preschool that adopts and promotes play to develop their curriculum. Preschools that incorporate play as a means to fill rather than promote learning and development is a waste of time and resources. Preschoolers that are pushed too early to accomplish tasks that their brains are not able to handle yet may experience frustration, stress and anxiety. Without a doubt a child that is continually expected to do work that he/she is not academically ready for, will become averse to it and in the long run, we run the risk that the child will be turned off to learning. There is still a great need for research on the relationship between quality play and development of specific academic skills; however, perhaps because the typical EC school environment does not allow sufficient time for children to engage in extended play, this line of research has not been extensive (Almon, 2003; Belka, 2004; Bodrova & Leong, 2005; Carlsson-Paige, 2008; Christensen & Kelly, 2003; Clements & Fiorentino, 2004; Elkind, 26 2003, 2007; Fisher, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, & Glick, 2008; Johnson, 2006; Olfman, 2003). Thus, one major challenge of this type of EC education for all is to be able to articulate to policy makers how children's development of the types of cognitive skills that are demonstrated in play are important (or even more important) for academic readiness and later school success than memorizing the standard set of information officially targeted as early childhood competencies. Even if play has not yet been demonstrated to be the cause of long-term school success, the evidence is clear that it is an integrated coexisting component of young children's developmental progress (Vygotsky, 1976, 1978). Unfortunately, most of the present research evidence has come from smallscale cross-sectional studies that may seem irrelevant to educators and policy makers; therefore the other challenge to researchers is to mount some more extensive and practice-oriented studies (preferably longitudinal) to investigate play/cognition relationships in diverse early childhood settings. Educators should resist policies that reduce time for play experiences in preschool and work to increase funding for research on play/cognition relationships in early IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 childhood. We must stop politicizing education and instead focus on the question of what children need for their long-term emotional, social and intellectual development (Almon, 2003; Bodrova & Leong, 2005; Buchanan & Cooney, 2000; Christensen & Kelly, 2003; Elkind, 2003; Heinemann & Hewitt, 2010; Johnson, 2006). To reverse the significant deterioration play is undergoing, parents and educators need to work together to bring it back to its core in early childhood programs. In the current climate of school readiness- workbooks, standardized tests and forcing children to learn things before they are developmentally ready will not improve the educational system. Play in EC helps stimulate physical, social, emotional, and cognitive learning and needs to be given the status it deserves in EC curriculum. When play becomes part of the curriculum, then it will improve the quality and scope of play. After all PLAY is just a four letter word! Author Dr. Matilde A. Sarmiento-Arribalzaga is an independent Education Consultant that has served as an Assistant Professor for the University of Texas System in the Department of Education specializing in Bilingual Education. She is currently conducting research in Barcelona, Spain. 27 References Almon, J. (2003). All work and no play...How educational reforms are harming our preschoolers. In S. Olfman (Ed.), The vital role of play in early childhood Education (pp. 17-42). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group. Ashiabi, G.S. (2007). Play in the preschool classroom: Its socioemotional significance and the teacher's role in play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 35(2), 199-207. Bar-Haim, Y., & Bart, O. (2006). Motor function and social participation in kindergarten children. Social Development, 15(2), 296-310. Belka, D. (2004). Substituting skill learning for traditional games in early childhood. Teaching Elementary Physical Education, 15(3), 25-27. Blakemore, C. (2003). Movement is essential to learning. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 74(9), 22-25, 41. Bodrova, E., & Leong D.J. (2005). High quality preschool programs: What would Vygotsky say? Early Education and Development, 16(4), 435-444. Bredekamp, S. (2004). Play and school readiness. In E. Zigler, D. Singer, & J. BishopJosef (Eds.), Children’s play: The roots of reading. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press. Buchanan, M., & Cooney, M. (2000). Play at home, play in the classroom: Parent/ profess ional partnerships in supporting child play. Young Exceptional Children, 3(4), 9-15. Carlsson-Paige, N. (2008). Reclaiming play: Helping children learn and thrive in school. Exchange, 180, 44-48. Carson, L. M. (2001). The “I Am Learning” curriculum: Developing a movement awareness in young children. Teaching Elementary Physical Education, 12(5), 9-13. Casby, M. W. (2003). The development of play in infants, toddlers, and young children. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 24(4), 163-174. Christensen, A. & Kelly, K. (2003). No time for play: Throwing the baby out with the bath water. Reading Teacher, 56(6) , 528-530. Clements, R., & Fiorentino, L. (Eds.) (2004). The child’s right to play: A global approach. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. Doctoroff, S. (2001). Adapting the physical environment to meet the needs of all young children for play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 29(2), 105-109. Duckworth, E. (1979). Either we're too early and they can't learn it or we're too late and they know it already: The dilemma of applying Piaget. Harvard Educational Review, 49(3), 297-313. Elkind D. (2003). Thanks for the memory: The lasting value of true Play. Young Children 58(3), 46-50. Elkind D. (2007). The power of play: How spontaneous, imaginative activities lead to happier, healthier children. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press, Inc. Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. & Glick, R. (2008). Conceptual split? Parents’ and experts’ perception of play in the 21st century. Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 305 – 316. Garvey, C. (1993). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Eyer, D. (2004). Einstein never used flashcards: How our children really learn- and why they need to play more and memorize less. New York: Rodale Books. Heidemann, S., & Hewitt, D. (2010). Play: The pathway from theory to practice. St. IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 28 Paul, MN: Redleaf Press. Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229-261. Johnson, J. (2006). Play and the development of the young child in USA today. In M. Levin, D.E., & Carlsson-Paige, N. (2006/1987). The war play dilemma: Everything parents and teachers need to know (2nd ed.). NY: Teachers College Press. Kowalski, H., Wyver, S., Masselos, G., & deLacey, P. (2005). The long-day childcare context: Implications for toddlers' pretend play. Early Years An International Journal of Research and Development, 25(1), 55-65. National Association of Early Childhood Specialists in State Departments of Education. (2001). Recess and the importance of play: A position statement on young children and recess. Washington, DC: Author. Olfman, S. (2003). All work and no play … How educational reforms are harming our preschoolers. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood. Olfman, S. (2005). Where do the children play? In S. Olfman (Ed.), Childhood lost: How American culture is failing our kids (pp. 203–216). Westport, CT: Praeger. Oliver S.J. & Klugman, E. (2004). Speaking out for play-based learning: Becoming an effective advocate for play in the early childhood classroom. Child Care Information Exchange, 155, 22-25. Oliver, S.J., & Klugman, E. (2006). Play and standards-driven curricula: Can they work together in preschool? Exchange, 170, 12-14. Oliver, S. J., & Klugman, E. (2007). Building a play research agenda: What do we know about play? What new questions do we need to ask? Exchange 173, 4-17. Pate, R .R., Pfeiffer, K. A., Trost, S. G., Ziegler, P., & Dowda, M. (2004). Physical activity among children attending preschools. Pediatrics, 114, 1258–1263. Payne, V. G., & Isaacs, L. D. (2008). Chapter 12: Fundamental locomotion skills of childhood. Human motor development: A lifespan approach (7th ed.) (pp. 299-327). New York: McGraw Hill. Piek, J., Dawson, L., Smith, L., & Gasson, N. (2008). The role of early fine and gross motor development on later motor and cognitive ability. Human Movement Science, 2(5), 668-684. Rothstein, R. (2004). Class and schools: Using social, economic, and educational reform to close the black-white achievement gap. New York: Teachers College Press. Stipek, D. (2006). No Child Left Behind come to preschool. The Elementary School Journal, 106, 455-467. United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child. Article 31. Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 536-552). New York: Basic Books. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. IPA/USA e-journal, Fall 2013 29 Artful Play at the Backyard Easel: Thinking, Talking and Painting Outdoors abstract Vivien Geneser Children often talk while painting and this discourse is influenced by their physical and emotional environment. By listening carefully and offering reflective feedback, an astute teacher can effectively facilitate the young artist’s thought processes and thus impact the corresponding language experience and art product. In this example, the researcher encouraged the artist to share her thoughts during a painting session at a backyard easel. In a quest to portray how the outdoor environment influenced the artist’s narrative, and how this factor contributed to her experience, the transcript from the session was evaluated for evidence of the correlations between her expressive language, artistic endeavors, and symbolic play. The goal of this study was to provide insight into the ways that children reveal their inner worlds while participating in guided art activities, and to show how educators can facilitate language development and share in the wonder and enjoyment of an outdoor classroom. Easel art talk “Open it please. Thank you. Add more. It is mine. Uh, oh!” (a spill) “It is so pretty! It is pink! It is you! It is brand new.” (fresh paper) “Can you hear that? It is my brand new brush. It is so messy in the brush. Sorry. I want green. I want green now. Can I have green please?” Twenty-seven month old Sophie stood on a box next to the easel under a tree in the backyard and painted to her heart’s content, which was-on this dayapproximately one hour and fifteen minutes. Sophie had arrived earlier with eager anticipation of her time at the easel. As usual, she began by greeting the cats, birds, dogs, and her friend, Wilson, before announcing her readiness for painting. Sophie brought her enthusiasm, artistic skills, cultural literacy, and emerging symbolic concepts of representation to the IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 session. The researcher escorted her to an easel in the back yard and provided brushes, paints, and white paper. According to Vygotsky (1962, 1978), people learn to think and to talk based on their early social circumstances and language experiences. Language serves as a tool to guide the process and contributes to who we are and who we become as thinkers and doers. In the following vignette, an assessment of Sophie’s lively narrative during the painting session provides insight into the overall development of a precocious young learner. “Ooh. Wow. It is so pretty. Did you hear that?” (Sophie made a pounding noise with the brush.) “I’m painting. Do you want some there? What is that? There you go. Orange, Vivien, orange!” 30 Sophie could easily articulate the terminology for the painting activity: paint, paper, brush, and paint jar. She was also able to correctly identify all of the paint colors. The words and terms were part of her daily vocabulary; which are also known as her “everyday concepts” (Bodrova & Leong, 1996, p. 58). Although Sophie had chronologically progressed beyond the infamous toddler “language explosion” stage (Woolfolk & Perry, 2012, p. 158), she continued to impress her friends and family members with regular additions to her expressive vocabulary. She demonstrated her metalinguistic awareness by using numerous nouns and adjectives in her daily speech. For example, in the first quote, she identified the paint as “pretty”, and the paper and brush as “brand new”. Later, the brush got “messy”. Her awareness of social rules was also keen. Every segment of the narrative includes evidence (please, thank you, sorry) of her desire to assimilate into the prevailing social milieu (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). Symbolic play “Open, please open. It’s mine. It is orange. Draw a triangle please.” (The reseaacher declined.) “OK.” (Sophie drew the triangle herself and added eyes.) “She can sing. Do you want me to make her sing?” (She poured paint on the paper.) “Shapey, shapey, shapey!” At first, Sophie delighted in the sensation of the color on white paper, and then she personalized the effort. Her triangle became a character with “eyes” to see, and then a “mouth” to sing. With a simple flick of the wrist, she transformed her sketch into a character. Evidence of abstract thought emerged from the motorific exertion as she translated the marks into meaning, a uniquely human achievement (Sheridan, 2006). Next, by naming her creation “Shapey”, she highlighted the pervasive use of symbols IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 in human culture (Gardner, 1982). Symbols (such as geometric shapes) are an indicator of the child’s ability to recognize that something can represent something else, but the meaning can be elastic (Vygotsky, 1978). Ideally, the early learning environments and experiences of children will collaborate in tandem to prepare them for a future world filled with a vast array of symbols. “Shapey” emerged as a new pal. Sophie offered the paintbrush to the researcher and asked her to complete the composition. Instead, the researcher encouraged her to continue painting independently, and invited both Shapey and Sophie to participate as sing-along partners with “Little White Duck”, a familiar tune. Sophie began to sing, and then completed her portrait of “Shapey”, who “sang” along, too. In this moment, Sophie entered into Vygotsky’s infamous “Zone of Proximal Development” (Bodrova & Leong, 1996), as she demonstrated that she could increase her artistic ability with an appropriate amount of guidance. Sophie extended her grasp slightly beyond the familiar as she became more comfortable with a task that was previously too challenging. As Sophie transfered orange liquid from the jar on the easel tray-outside her brain-she also perceived the color-within her brain-and, in her consciousness, she accomplished the feat of internalization (Vygotsky, 1962, 1978). Then she applied color to the paper on the easel with an expressive gesture, and correlated it to an image from her memory, as she transitioned from imagination to experimentation to representation and back again; completing the cycle of concept formation. The act of painting provides a method for sorting visual stimuli, and connecting the work of the hands with speech trains the brain to pay attention and facilitates meaning making efforts (Sheridan, 2006). 31 “Oh!” Sophie babbled and hummed while painting. “It is like soap.” She switched to white paint, then continued to hum and sing while she painted with the soap-like substance. Sophie picked up the white paint, shook it, and was disappointed by the result: “I got it all o’er me!” Since she was already covered in paint, she attempted finger painting with the tempura paints for a few minutes. Later, she painted with both hands, using two paintbrushes, one that was dipped in red paint, and the other one in blue paint. Once again, Sophie demonstrated that she was able to conceptualize comparisons when she described the white paint as being like soap (the Ivory soap in the bathroom) (Smith, 1993). By using white paint to symbolize soap, a nonexperiential depiction, she demonstrated that she had moved beyond painting for “motoric pleasure” and had transitioned into the stage of representation while engaging in artistic efforts (Kellogg, 1967). She was also engaging in a bilateral activity by using both hands simultaneously, an activity which serves an important function by connecting the two hemispheres of the young brain (Sheridan, 2006). The artist as rebel “Draw a big M, please.” (Again, the researcher declined, but offered verbal encouragement: “You can do it.”) Sophie opened the brown paint. “A blue, blue, a green? Oh, no, brown. It is green; GREEN! Green like the grass! It is yummy.” Sophie attempted to empty the jar on the paper and laughed. “It is like chocolate. It is like chocolate pudding. It is like pizza. Out of the way, Mommy!” The mood changed as Sophie teased the researcher. She tested her limits by threatening to pour brown paint on the paper. She recognized the color as brown but called it blue. As she said blue, she IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 looked up with a mischievous expression. Then, she argued that it was green and laughed aloud. Finally, she acknowledged that it was brown-brown like chocolate pudding- and poured the entire jar on the paper. When the researcher intervened, she called her “Mommy”. The teasing antics were indicators of an emerging self concept (Bodrova & Leong, 1996) as she challenged the status quo. Sophie observed that the brown paint had the qualities of pudding, so she pretended that it was chocolate pudding, and entered into the realm of play (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). In this example, Sophie began her pretend dialogue at a point when she was faced with an unrealizable situation (the jar of paint was not edible), so she initiated the mode of play to gratify her desire to relieve the tension. She entered into “an imaginary, illusory world in which the unrealizable desires can be realized and this world is what we call play” (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 93). Sophie invited her assistant to enter into this world of play but the researcher signaled her decline by cleaning up the glob of paint dripping from the bottom of the easel. So, Sophie communicated a change of perspective by code switching. She changed her label for the researcher from her first name; “Vivien”, to a nomenclature; “Mommy”. The name change indicated her rebellion. It signified a change in the relationship, as if “Vivien” was a painting pal, but “Mommy” was an authority figure. Nevertheless, the researcher appreciated that she managed to control her impulse to spill more paint (Bodrova & Leong, 1996) Artful play “This is green.” Sophie began to sing. “Now, blue. Sky blue! Blue-blue-blue!” She sang with clicks and clacks and beat her paintbrush to the rhythm of her tune. 32 Sophie used her paintbrush in two ways; as a tool for art and as an instrument for rhythm. She alternated between the two purposes and then combined the playful gestures (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). As she sang, she swayed, hummed to herself, and directed her storysong to a bird at the feeder, expressing the spontaneous thoughts that formed while she engaged in her work. Throughout the entire activity, Sophie’s movements represented an outward expression of her inner language as her tools were repurposed. The paintbrush became a drumstick, and was transformed from an instrument for expressing color and form to an instrument for expressing rhythm. Next, it became a pivot for another activity as she reentered her inner world of private thought (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). With the paintbrush as pivot, Sophie was able to transcend her environment. She entered into an imaginary world of make-believe, into a place where she could make music with drums (Vygotsky, 1978). Yet, here in the backyard, under a canopy of trees, she also needed to use it while painting, so she switched back to the process of painting on paper. On this day, Sophie’s creative expression encompassed art, music, and dance, and utilized multiple talents. Activities that facilitate graphic representation such as drawing and painting serve multiple functions. The internal mechanisms of private speech were helping Sophie process new ways to think about the activity, the tools, the people, and the various roles that people play in her life. Sophie’s inner dialogue fostered her imagination as she explored various perspectives, ideas and concepts in her mind (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). Social and emotional skills Wilson appeared from inside the house. As he approached Sophie and greeted her, he said: “Hi, Sophie!”, but her only IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 response was to smile. She watched him pet the dog, Bonnie, before retreating into the house. Although Sophie watched Wilson intently, she didn’t answer his salutation and, as soon as he was out of sight, she looked back at the paper and resumed painting. Then, in an apparent effort to refocus on the task at hand, she requested a new color: red. Sophie and Wilson were fast friends but, in this encounter, she did not speak to him. It was an unusual occurrence, which seemed to indicate that she felt self conscious. If so, this marked a new phase in her social development. As to Wilson (age 17), he seemed surprised, yet nonchalant. Later, she engaged in conversation with him but, during the painting session, she was reticent. Perhaps she preferred to stay on task with the painting project (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). “No, it is my turn. Open please. Thank you. It is my magic word.” (“Is thank you a magic word?” asked the researcher.) “Yes. Do a triangle please. Do a circle please.” Sophie demonstrated familiarity with the norms of socialization by recognizing “please” and “thank you” as magic words. By representing “thank you” as a magic concept, she was demonstrating a new language acquisition, which also reflected her recent social interactions. As Sophie’s range of community expanded, she acquired more rules about proper, or polite communication. The self-control that Sophie exhibited during this painting session involved the complexity of the mental work of thinking about the creative process such as making meaning from splashes of paint, and it also was connected with the physical exertion of managing the artistic mediums and tools with little hands (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). Woven into this scenario were the socially internalized rules that Sophie 33 demonstrated when she stopped short of pouring the whole jar of paint on the paper. Each successful painting session and each productive experience will contribute to Sophie’s ability to see herself as a competent doer. On this particular day, she exhibited behavior that illuminated her budding self-regulatory mechanisms, which are essential elements for success in the social world. Each mannerly effort reflected some aspect of her social conditioning (Vygotsky, 1978). “En plein air” Consistent with the artistic tradition known as “en plein air”, Sophie drew inspiration from the outdoor environment. “En plein air” is a term that refers to the philosophy of nineteenth century Impressionist painters such as Claude Monet, Camille Pissarro, and Auguste Renoir, who advocated painting outdoors in the open air for the sake of feeling the fresh air, utilizing natural light, and capturing the essence of the sky as it changed colors throughout the day. More recently, painter and art educator, Ev Hale has promoted the practice of painting “en plein air” as a way for artists to enhance their awareness of nature. She believes that the artists’ creative energies are replenished by the elements of an outdoor environment. Furthermore, Hale asserts that artists who paint “en plein air” are able to develop new world views, while also improving their artistic development (Hales, 2012). Educator as “artist apprentice” Sophie likewise preferred the experience of an outdoor easel. She happily led the endeavor as she explored every color of paint. She painted prolifically and, throughout the session, Sophie shifted from expressive art to artful play. The researcher facilitated her artistic efforts by serving as a supportive listener and also by providing materials and verbal IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 support as needed. In this capacity, one in which the adult’s role is less didactic and more collaborative, the contribution of the adult who is assisting can be classified as an “artist apprentice” (Van Hoorn, et al, 2007, p. 116). Despite this apparently minimal involvement, the researcher’s presence was essential to Sophie’s artistic process because she contributed subtle observational comments regarding colors and technique, and assisted with the logistics (Sedgwick, 2002). By gently providing support for Sophie’s artistic efforts, she also served as a reflective partner in the creative process, thus supporting the philosophy of art therapist Judith Rubin, who believes that “the adult offering art must provide a framework or structure within which the child can be free to move and to think and to fantasize, not a structure which imposes, controls, and makes a child dependent” (Rubin, 1978, p. 30). Art is basic Art is a branch of learning that appeals to the imagination, as well as a means of creative expression that reflects the inner world of the individual who is producing the representation. John Dewey claimed that art is the human’s primary way of engaging in the world (Dewey, 1934). If you observe a group of chattering youngsters on the verge of painting, you will witness how they become solemn and intent when they are presented with a colorful tray of paints (Sheridan, 2006). They seem to be enchanted by the effort, as they engage in the manipulation of the colors and the creation of a composition. Often, it appears to be purely a sensational experience, as when they paint with feathers, yet the opportunity for expression also lends itself to the creation of a composition in which meaning is conferred (Bodrova & Leong, 1996; 34 Sheridan, 2006; Smith, 1993). For all artists, but especially young children, painting is “an adventure with color and paper” (Smith, 1993, p. 3). Making sense of paint as a medium can be a daunting task, yet it is a very desirable pursuit. The act of painting functions as a treat of the senses that facilitates one’s ability to focus (Sheridan, 2006). When children maintain a cognitive focus and resist distractions, they are developing their attentional skills. Furthermore, the child’s thoughts and observations are stimulated by the visual tactile experience such as the colors of paint and the novelty of various art supplies. Art experiences offer “novel, challenging learning that’s usually coherent and relevant” (Jensen, 2006, p.226). Art is basic to the preschool curriculum because of the role it plays in fostering the child’s imagination and visual skills. Thus, it is also one of the crucial building blocks for future literacy. Furthermore, it is a deeply satifying experience. Painting with preschool-aged children satisfies their need to communicate through visual means, to feel competent with art materials and to make sense of their world (Smith, 1993). Participating in art activities helps children build their social skills, language skills, attention skills and observation skills. When children produce art they are using a visual form to express their encounter with the world (Fineberg, 2001) and this helps them to see the world in new ways. The experience of drawing or painting facilitates the child’s visual acuity. As the young artist transfers interpretations into visual forms, the motorific efforts can be manipulated to represent people, experiences and observations (Sheridan, 2006). Art activities also provide important opportunities for students to represent IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 their understanding of symbols. Scholars have long explored the connection between art and cognitive development in early childhood. Teachers who study child art develop insight into the child’s inner world as they begin to understand the cognitive mechanisms of the creative process, which will ultimately benefit their teaching practice (Bodrova & Leong, 1996; Dewey, 1934; Gardner, 1982; Jensen, 2006; Kellogg, 1967; Lowenfeld, 1964; Rubin, 1978; Smith, 1993; Sheridan, 2006; Vygotsky, 1962, 1978). Conclusion: Art experiences provide a necessary balance between intellect and emotions and serve to facilitate language development in young children (Lowenfeld, 1964). Outdoor artistic projects can be as whimsical as summer “pudding paintings” or as meaningful as acrylic paintings that are framed for posterity. Thus, through a variety of activities and mediums, teachers and parents can facilitate children’s language development with outdoor art activities that promote private speech, interactive communication, attention skills, and help children connect their inner thoughts with marks on paper. Sophie’s painting session illuminated the important connection between art and learning. By providing guided painting sessions outdoors, educators can optimize the experience for young artists because the child’s sense of wonder is enhanced by painting “en plein air”. Finally, with Sophie’s parting words: “Let’s go look in the worm bin”, we concluded our painting session at the backyard outdoor easel. 35 Author Vivien Geneser, PhD is an Assistant Professor, Early Childhood at Texas A&M University-San Antonio. She has taught Head Start, private preschools, and public school first grade. She is interested in creativity and arts and play. vivien.geneser@tamusa.tamus.edu References Bodrova, E. & Leong, D. J. (1996). Tools of the mind, The Vygotskian approach to early childhood education. Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. Berkeley Publishing: New York, NY Fineberg, J. (2001). Discovering child art: Essays on childhood, primitivism, and modernism. Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ Gardner, H. (1982). Art, mind and brain: A cognitive approach to creativity. Basic Books: Cambridge, MA Hales, E. (2012). Painting en plein air: Teaching -techniques, tactics, tips. Kindle Edition. Amazon Digital Services, Inc. Van Hoorn, J., Nourot, P., Scales, B. (2011). Play at the center of the curriculum Pearson: Upper Saddle River, NJ Jensen, E. (2006). Enriching the brain: How to maximize every learner’s potential. JosseyBass Education: Hobeken, NJ Kellogg, R. (1967). The psychology of children’s art. Random House: New York, NY Lowenfeld, V. (1964). Creative and mental growth. MacMillan: New York, NY Rubin, J. (1978). Child art therapy. Van Nostrund Reinhold Co.: New York, NY Sedgwick, F. (2002). Enabling children’s learning through drawing. David Fulton Publishers: Great Britain, UK Sheridan, S. (2006). How marks change minds. Drawing/Writing Publications: Addison, ME Smith, N. R. (1993). Experience and art: Teaching children to paint. Teachers College Press: New York: NY Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA Vygotsky, L. S. (1978) Mind in society. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA Woolfolk, A. and Perry, N. (2012). Child and adolescent development. Pearson: Upper Saddle River: NJ IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 36 Resurrecting Play for the Next Generation abstract Laurel Moldawsky Silber and Barbara Moldawsky Stern As a child psychologist and elementary school Principal, the co-authors have had a bird’s eye view of the misguided cultural shift in attitudes undermining access to free play. Through a memoir of their own childhood play as sisters occurring during the 50’s and 60’s, the authors share how critical play had been to their own self-development. Through a cross-disciplinary professional and personal interweaving of the multiple strands that inform our thinking on play this narrative offers a rich illustration of the many benefits of play for children and the adults they will become. This article contributes to raising awareness regarding the detrimental effects of the play deficit. It concludes with a call for activism on the part of adults to correct for the misguided path we have taken in contemporary society and to resurrect play. As educator, child psychologist, mothers and sisters we have deepened our appreciation of the fact that self-defining experiences and core memories that reflect who we are today are significantly connected to the way we played as children. This recognition, coming through a retrospective lens, startles us when we note the decrease in children’s play in our current times (Elkind, 2001). It is well documented in the child development literature that play for young children is important for their development in the cognitive, language, creative, social, neurobiological, physical and emotional domains. It is one of those common sense findings that are, in fact, IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 increasingly substantiated by multidisciplined research (Elkind, 2007, Brown, 2009, Ginsburg, 2007, Linn, 2008, Siegel, 1999, Singer & Singer, 2005, Hirsh-Pasek, 2009, Zigler, 2004). As an elementary school principal (Stern) and a child psychologist (Silber), we consider this a serious problem. How do young people find meaning in their lives and integrate experience without access to play? How will they learn to regulate their minds and bodies and come to know who they are, what they are capable of, what they feel passionate about, and how to solve the world’s problems? Moreover, are they having any fun? 37 The grim statistics about childhood depression, anxiety disorders (CDC, 2013), attention deficit disorders (Froehlich, et.al 2007), childhood obesity (Wang & Beyhoun, 2007) and the newly identified “nature deficit disorder” (Louv, 2005) all suggest we have become deeply out of step with children’s developmental needs. By encroaching on children’s right to play we have compromised many aspects of their self-development. They are bombarded with external stimulation without adequate opportunity to build the internal scaffolding for processing their experience. Without play they are stressed out. In sharing our own play experiences here, we offer a case study of what was happening when we played together as sisters, and we also consider our professional, parenting, and adulthood experiences to reflect on the longitudinal value of our childhood play. We are thus joining a chorus of voices—Alliance for Childhood, US Play Coalition, IPA/USA, National Institute for Play, Ultimate Block Party, KaBOOM, the Philadelphia Declaration of Play, The Gesell Institute of Child Development and others— advocating for the reintroduction of play into children’s lives. Our Play We grew up along with two younger brothers in an intact middle-class suburban family during the 50s and 60s. Our family settled into a northeastern U.S. community and stayed put, so we came to know our environment well. We played together and apart in our downstairs playroom, our shared bedroom, backyard IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 playhouse and tree house, and the hidden places of our home, our street, the woods and brook at the bottom of our steep hill, on the forbidden rooftop (we could reach it by crawling out of our bedroom window), and later at a cottage on a lake. Our play often included our brothers and neighborhood kids of all ages. It didn’t include our parents. We knew, however, that our parents valued play because they implicitly conveyed this message: “You are responsible to create your own fun.” Consequently, fun was registered as a legitimate pursuit, and we proceeded to construct our own space to play. That space was not monitored, filmed, tested, or supervised. Our free play had no coaches, teachers, scout leaders, or facilitators, nor the related prizes and trophies to evaluate our progress and support our self-esteem. We created elaborate games and the rules to govern our play completely on our own. If a problem occurred and we went to our parents, the response was often, ”I guess you’ll have to figure that out.” From our current vantage point as professionals and parents, we see the significance of our generationally bounded childhood play, and we want to differentiate the multiple aspects of that play to articulate its value. The whole is, of course, more than the sum of the parts, but we will explore what play is made of and how it benefitted us. Finding voice Who gave me (Stern), at age 10, the notion to write “The Timber Hill News” (named for the street we grew up on)? The 38 idea did not arrive from the outside in a prepackaged kit; I thought it up myself. I was preoccupied at the time with Lois Lane, or more likely with the fact that Superman loved Lois Lane, and I decided to try out the feel of being a journalist. With pads in hand, my friend Wendy and I went house to house ringing doorbells and asking for news. “The Timber Hill News” had a lifespan of only four editions in six weeks, as our interest ran its natural course and we moved on, but the effect was powerful. Grown-ups gave us their news and read our paper. They didn’t laugh at us. We all learned at an early age that we had the right to be spokespersons, to write down our thoughts, and to share them with our community. Juxtapose this with the experience of a child who receives a newspaper-making kit, complete with stories to print! Such a well-meaning gift allows little chance to define oneself or to find one’s voice. My experience, on the other hand, helped shape my abilities and contributed to the formation of my identity as a school principal. Inspired by Lois Lane, I turned off the television set and actively imitated her character and behavior. I involved my friends, my sister, and our neighbors and played the role of a woman in charge of communicating important information. I was figuring out how the world works and IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 exploring my place in it as I externalized my ideas into play. Besides having fun, I gained the kind of self-confidence that helps regulate anxiety. Had my parents asked me to type, practice spelling, write stories, and count money as exercises in skill development, what would have happened to the fun, to the ability to make meaning, and to the integrative possibilities for developing my identity? Taking risks Many of the games we played and the places we explored involved some risk. We were free to roam the neighborhood until the 6:00 whistle blew from the town firehouse, signaling that dinner would be on the table and we had better be sitting down ready to eat it. Aside from that somewhat dated expectation, we experienced less anxiety and judgment about our behaving independently. What did we do in that time away? Here are a few examples. Hellerinkel’s was a neighborhood candy store that could be reached by walking up two hills, crossing a very busy road, and then cutting behind several stores on an even busier road. It was an important rite of passage when we were deemed “old enough to go to Hellerinkel’s”—that we could be counted on to walk there and back by ourselves within an agreed-upon time frame. Our 39 physical world expanded and with that came a sense of trust and possibility. In our everyday life there were many risks. We walked about a mile to get home from school and had to cross a bridge over a brook. We often got sidetracked by the brook and would end up playing there, sometimes getting “soakers” from wading or building dams. Once there was a group of older boys on the bridge who wouldn’t let me (Stern) pass unless I paid a toll. In a panic, I scooted around the bridge and waded through the icy water in my school shoes and ran all the way home. Despite my fear, I figured out a way to outsmart them. Along with neighborhood friends, we both enjoyed exploring the new houses under construction in our suburban development. It was intoxicating to most suburban children of this generation, as our towns were exploding with growth. On the weekends when the workers weren’t there, we would steal pieces of lumber and drag them home to make things. We would also play house in the houses. Everybody played their parts— father, mother, teenagers (always those mysterious teenagers!), and little ones. For what seemed like hours and hours we would have pretend family lives in the various unfinished rooms. The only time we got into trouble was when our brother Brian jumped off an open deck and went tumbling down a steep hill. He didn’t break a bone, but our parents got clued in about where we were playing, which became a problem. Once the adults were aware of our play world, it became less tantalizing. Interestingly, the low level of parental participation and higher level of risk in our activities could be viewed as neglectful in today’s terms. Attitudes have changed and safety has become paramount, but wasn’t there value in having some risk in our play? We gained a sense of personal power. We felt IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 ownership over our environment and entitled to explore it. The opportunity for a child to experience mastery and become more confident is lost if all risk is avoided and all activities are supervised. Children then internalize the attitude of fear and the notion that they could not possibly be trusted to problem-solve. In our excursions to Hellerinkel’s and in our construction-site play we learned something about negotiating in the outside world on our own terms. We were not visitors to our parents’ world, extensions of their plan, but rather people in our own right. These days we see a hidden paradox in our efforts as parents to protect children: we have set them up to enter the world without an ability to assess risk. In 2003, as a parent of a12-yearold, I (Silber) was invited by other parents to join a “walk pool”—a group of parents who took turns meeting children after school once weekly to walk them across the street to Hebrew School. I thought to myself, “A walk pool? How would I have felt if my parents assessed the risk as too great and not allowed me to go to Hellerinkel’s?” Feeling unpopular, and risking the shaming judgment of being an inattentive parent, I declined the invitation and said my son could walk across the street without an adult’s presence. He found a friend, and together they found their way. How did such risk become defined as too great—so great that the chance for independent exploration was lost? Attachment researchers Bowlby (1969), Ainsworth (1978), and Main (1995) have observed that children who feel securely attached move off a literal or figurative parent’s lap into exploratory play and then return. The findings underscore the importance of having both an attachment and access to independent space for a child’s self-development. My sister increased her sense of security, for example, as she put into action her idea of 40 a newspaper and as she found her own way home. My son had his parents’ blessings for walking to and from school and felt that he could be trusted. Such experiences lay down an internal foundational resource that supports autonomous functioning and a feeling of security. That sense of security is a feeling not easily quantifiable, and yet when children enter an unknown space, what else do they fall back on? Managing time and space Back when family lives were less hectic, we experienced the total glory of waking up on a Saturday and knowing we had the whole day in front of us. We had some chores to do, but in our minds there was an expansive feeling of endless hours in which we could “make it up.” We rolled over in bed, got a pad of paper and a pencil, and started planning/negotiating over the “Seven Games” we would play that day. The planning part could take more time than the playing part, and that was fine. On the list would go things, such as Monopoly, Barbies, cards (War, Fish, or Spit), riding bikes, building a fort, baking something, even getting around the room without touching the floor. We might agree to take breaks for breakfast and lunch and many snacks, or we could incorporate food into the games. For example, relay race instructions might be to run across the room to a snack tray, eat crackers and peanut butter, sing, and then run back. Sometimes Wendy and Sally, the sisters two doors down, joined in negotiating the choices and in playing the Seven Games. We’re not sure how old we were—somewhere in middle childhood before pre-adolescence. The point is, we believed that the day was ours to plan. We didn’t need anyone to drive us anywhere. Whether or not we played all the games was irrelevant. There were times when we got pulled away by chores or neighborhood IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 games or books we were in the middle of. At other times, the Seven Games stretched into the night and Sunday. To children such extended free time is a luxury. But to today’s parents it may seem like time wasted, opportunities lost for future enhancement. Our own children did not experience the uninterrupted days of play that we remember from our childhood. We both worked outside our homes, and our children could not be there alone. Like our contemporaries, we struggled to find balance and to decide how our children should use their time, given the cultural expectations and realities of modern family life. Our children often did afterschool sports, lessons and other structured activities, and we did our share of parental driving. We know firsthand that creating more unscheduled time for children to play goes against prevailing cultural attitudes and realities. But we believe that the lack of unscheduled time is itself hugely consequential. Just as children feel grounded with a sense of place, they also directly benefit from having a sense of time that is their own to construct. In the way a dream functions for the sleeper, allowing the mind to sort out experience while the physical self sleeps, free play allows children (now awake) a chance to reflect on experience, to make sense of it, to map it out and come to know and represent it. By attributing meaning to experience and organizing it, children are literally developing their minds and the neural pathways of the brain. Dream research shows that dreams are critical to neuro-bio-social-psychological development, and we are discovering through research that the same holds true for play (Cartwright, 2010). Like Max, the protagonist in Maurice Sendak’s Where the Wild Things Are, children use dreams and play to sort things out. They are constructing the inner scaffolding of the 41 mind through the recontextualizing of experience with memory. It is this kind of mental organizing that allows for abstraction, greater flexibility, and problem solving. When children build a foundation for organizing experience, they are in a better position to tolerate paradox and to see and hold multiple perspectives, skills so necessary to adult development. The strong foundation gained through free play also stimulates experimentation, role modeling, and creativity. As a child, I (Silber) recall the pleasure of making mud pies with berries, leaves, and pebbles from the yard. I used picnic benches to affect the look of bakery shelves for my pies to be displayed. I never cared what they tasted like; I knew no one would eat them. Later, as a parent, I enjoyed hearing my then eight-year-old daughter instruct her imaginary students in the pretend schoolroom she set up in our basement. Her free play seems to have been an organizing experience then and now, as she is currently taking education classes in college. The absence of real consequences in pretend play reduces risk about making mistakes. One gets to experiment with the possibilities and use whatever is at hand to create something new. It is also in these moments that children get the chance to try out the language and roles of the adults they observe. They play out the problems in their lives that preoccupy them, scare or confuse them. They bring what excites them into dramatic play to try to make sense of it. The mastery they experience in the play lays down a foundational sense of trust for using autonomous thought so necessary to functioning in the real world. In our school and therapeutic playroom we have the privilege to be with children in the midst of this pretend play and it is a joy to hear them working out solutions to problems, negotiating with their peers and trying on different roles with their own ideas. We increasingly also see children IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 unfamiliar with and reluctant to enter this play universe. Without the chance to actively integrate real and imagined experiences and to find coherence through play, children feel a sense of disconnection when they are bombarded with stimuli and have no time to attribute meaning and put their own stamp on what is happening. Hyperactivity, distractibility, and impulsivity associated with attention deficit disorders may reflect, in part, this insufficiency of attaching meaning to experience. Our minds continually organize and reorganize experience for mastery and self-development. Without time to act on experience, to play with it, we find difficulty seeing it and referencing it, and we feel disorganized because internally, we are. Paradoxically, we require a slower childhood to keep pace with development in a faster world. Solving problems and building community The kids on Timber Hill seemed to like to play at our house best. This was probably because our mother didn’t mind the mess associated with play. She was unconcerned about the elaborate forts, castles, stores, and teepees that we built in our playroom with stuff we gathered around the house. Periodically she would simply rake it all into a big pile in the middle of the room and either ask us to put it all away or put it away herself. We would then begin again with the great new space created in the middle of the room. Just as the space and materials were redesigned in the physical realm, our minds were given over to reimagining other uses for the toys and materials. For example, what began as a bunch of blocks and towels became a fort and later a ship, illustrating that the process underlying play is metaphor and abstract thought—finding other ways to look at things. We let new constructions 42 emerge; from nothing comes something, and then something else. Players often had different ideas for how to use the same materials, but it was also comforting to return to the same construction or game over and over again. As a principal is wont to do, I (Stern) was fond of organizing everyone and directing elaborate productions— plays, dances, and musicals. One example was “The Runaway Rebels,” a conglomeration of Parent Trap, Circus Boy, and Swiss Family Robinson (movies of the day) all rolled into one. We hung a blanket in the middle of the garage from the two supporting poles and set up seating on the other side of the “curtain.” All along the way we solved problems and made decisions about the tickets, advertising, and refreshments. In the time that was unscripted we created our own script: instead of running away, we pretended, worked it out together, and wrote a script about—what else?— running away. Childhood offered many chances for rehearsals of all kinds. Community games, like kickball, took place at the top of our hill. The street that ran perpendicular to Timber Hill was relatively flat, perfect for kickball and dodgeball games. In the spring, summer, and fall, when it was still warm and light enough to play outside, right after dinner around the same time every night kids would gather at the top of the hill for a pickup game. We never heard kids say they couldn’t play because they had to do homework. If there was any homework in those days, we do not remember it having much significance. Moreover, we always seemed to haveenough players for two teams. We were all ages, brothers and sisters, and everyone got on a team. When cars came we would jump to the side, and then we would chant, “Car, Car, C-A-R, Stick your head in a jelly jar.” There were scuffles over rules and calls, but somehow IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 we would settle them so the game could go on. If not, sometimes a fight broke out and the game ended and kids scattered. We worked at creating cohesiveness and inclusiveness in the community because we were all commonly invested in continuing the play. We wanted to come back the next night because it was fun. We needed the other players. When I (Silber) first rode my two-wheeler, my father let go of the back of the bike, and it was past these kids that I rode. It meant something to me that they witnessed my achievement. Shifting cultural expectations Over the past few years, we have presented workshops on the topic of play, and we ask participants to share memories of important play experiences they had as children. Typically within seconds, everyone starts sharing memories with the persons sitting next to them. The room buzzes with a felt excitement. People hold these memories dear, like buried treasures. Next we ask participants how these play experiences contributed to shaping their adult selves? We hear anecdotes about gaining confidence, learning how to relate to others, feeling pleasure, taking risks, and being inventive. We also hear great stories about humorous antics and memorable experiences unique to various cultures and socioeconomic levels. What would it take to give children in our current world the opportunity for more play? It would take the combined efforts of parents, teachers, community leaders, policy makers, and business executives to adjust the balance of work and family, to reduce homework requirements, and to give over neighborhood and park spaces for the purpose of children creating their own play. It would require a seismic attitudinal shift for adults to stop assuming that children are “wasting” time if they are not 43 learning a skill or completing an assignment. Educators and psychologists, as arbiters of what children need to grow, would have to educate society about the importance of play. If it can’t happen in neighborhoods, can we allow play to inhabit more of the school day in our kindergartens and preschools? Can afterschool programs adjust their curricula and provide safe spaces for children to manage their own time, choose their own activities, organize their own games, and maybe even just have quiet time to reflect and think? Can the training for educators and psychologists emphasize curriculum that has play as the centerpiece for young children? Can school administrators adjust policies to build in play? Perhaps in the recent decades, with the explosion of information about parenting, we have effectively stirred parental anxiety to such a pitch—scared parents out of their minds, so to speak— that we’ve lost a connection to what we innately know to be true (Warner, 2005). Play brings us joy throughout our lifetime and is the basis of discovery, human advances, and creativity. It is the very foundation of our adult selves. As for the two of us, in this work we are drawing on our best play skills and our adult sense of responsibility to try to make a difference for children. We rehearsed for this moment in the way we played, and our play continues to be a vital resource and spur to action. We mine the gold of that hopeful time and combine it with our professional skills as we face the huge challenge of transforming cultural attitudes that undermine child development. Like the old days when we were working on the “The Timber Hill News,” we are reporting on what we see. We need the community. You in? IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 Acknowledgements: The authors wish to acknowledge their parents, Drs. Stanley and Patricia Moldawsky, who were great champions of play for their four children beginning in early childhood and throughout the lifespan. They always demonstrated a great respect for play, and both encouraged and allowed their children to fully explore their neighborhood and community, pursue their interests, and take the time needed to reflect on and integrate their experiences. Secondly the authors wish to acknowledge their cousin and playmate Mary Russell Curran, who graciously shared her editing skills in reading several drafts of this paper and offering her insights. Authors: Laurel Moldawsky Silber, Psy. D., Child Psychologist in Private Practice, Adjunct Assistant Professor at Institute for Graduate Clinical Psychology at Widener University, Chester, PA., faculty, founding member of the Philadelphia Declaration of Play and Ambassador for the Decade of Childhood Laurelsilber@gmail.com Barbara Moldawsky Stern, MBA, MS, 6th Year in Educational Leadership, Retired Principal of Darcey School Early Childhood Center, Cheshire, CT, current President, Board of Directors, Gesell Institute of Child Development, New Haven, CT 41 Marvel Road New Haven, CT 06515 203-387-0056 Babstern@gmail.com 44 References Ainsworth, M., M. Blehar, E. Waters, and S. Wall. ( 1978) Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum, Bowlby, J. (1969)Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Brown, S., and C. Vaugh. (2009) Play; How it Shapes the Brain, Opens the Imagination, and Invigorates the soul. New York: Penguin Group Inc. Cartwright, R. The Twenty- Four Hour Mind: The Role of Sleep and Dreaming on Our Emotional Lives.(2010) New York: Oxford University Press Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Mental Health Surveillance Among ChildrenUnited States, (2013) 2005-2011. MMWR/ 62(02); 1-35,. Elkind, D. The Hurried Child: Growing Up Too Fast Too Soon. Cambridge, MA: Perseus, 2001. Elkind, D. (2007) The Power of Play. Cambridge, MA: Perseus. Froehlich, T., B. Lamphear, J. Epstein, W. Barbaresi, S. Katusic, and R. Kahn.(2007) "Prevalence, Recognition, and Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a National Sample of US Children." Arch Pediatric Adolescent Medicine 161 (857864. Ginsburg, K. (2007)"The Importance of Play in Promoting Healthy Child Development and Maintaining Strong Parent-Child Bonds." Pediatrics 119.1 : 182-91. Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Berk, L., and Singer, D. (2209) A Mandate for Playful Learning in Preschool: Presenting the Evidence. New York: Oxford University Press,. Linn, S. (2008) The Case for Make Believe; Saving Play in a Commercialized World. New York: W. W. Norton &Co, Inc. Louv, R. (2005)Last Child in the Woods; Saving Our Children from Nature-Deficit Disorder. Chapel Hill: Algonquin Books. Main, M. (1995)Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental and Clinical Perspectives in Goldberg, et.al. Recent Studies in Attachment. New York: Analytic.. Siegel, D. (1999)The Developing Mind. New York: Guilford. Singer, D., and J. Singer. (2005)Imagination and Play in the Electronic Age. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP. Wang, Y., and M. Beyhoun. (2007)"The Obesity Epidemic in the United States-Gender, Age, Socioeconomic, Racial/Ethnic, and Geographic Characteristics: A Systematic Review and Meta-Regression Analysis." Epidemiologic Reviews 29. Warner, J. (2005)Perfect Madness; Motherhood in the Age of Anxiety. New York: Riverhead Books. Zigler, E., Singer, D., and Bishop-Josef, S.(2004) Children's Play: The Roots of Reading. Washington, DC: Zero To Three Press. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 45 International Play Association (IPA) and the American Affiliate IPA/USA: A Brief History Marcy Guddemi and Joe Frost The origin of the International Play Association and its affiliates in nearly 50 countries is close to 100 years old. In 1923, Save the Children founder Eglantyne Jebb drafted The Declaration of the Rights of the Child. Jebb believed that the rights of a child should be especially protected and enforced. These ideas were adopted by the International Save the Children Union, in Geneva, on February 23, 1923 and endorsed by the League of Nations General Assembly on November 26, 1924 as the World Child Welfare Charter. These proclamations, however, were not enforceable by international law, but rather only guidelines for countries to follow. Consequently, on November 20, 1959 the United Nations General Assembly adopted a much expanded version as its own Declaration of the Rights of the Child, adding ten principles in place of the original five. Principal Nine of the 1959 Declaration of Rights addresses the child’s right for “full opportunity to play and recreation…” November 20th has been adopted as the Universal Children's Day as ongoing celebration of that event. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 Interest in providing quality play opportunities for children had been gradually increasing over the decades before the signing of the Declaration of the Rights of the Child and in 1955 a major seminar on playgrounds was held in Europe. The need for international action was evident. In 1961 IPA was born in Scandinavia and held its first conference in Copenhagen that year. IPA has held eighteen (18) International Conferences 1961– Copenhagen 1964 – Zurich 1967 – London/Liverpool: Recreation and Play 1969 – Paris: Creative Play 1972 – Vienna: Play and Creativity 1975 – Milan: Adventure Playgrounds and Children’s Creativity 1978 – Ottawa: Play in Human Settlements 1981 – Rotterdam: Growing up in an Adult World – Beyond Play & Recreation 1984 – Ljubljana: Innovation – Participation – Action 46 1987 – Stockholm: Creativity through Play 1990 – Tokyo: Play and Education 1993 – Melbourne: World Play Summit 1996 – Espoo: Dimensions of Play 1999 – Lisbon: The Community of Play 2002 – Sao Paulo: Culture and Play in Urban Spaces 2005 – Berlin: Play: Learning for Life 2008 – Hong Kong: Play in a Changing World 2011 – Cardiff: Playing into the Future – Surviving and Thriving 2014 -- Istanbul The United Nation’s International Year of the Child in 1979 injected IPA with new energy. Though IPA began its life with an emphasis on adventure playgrounds it gradually changed its focus to play itself and to the child’s right to play. In the 1980’s IPA was effective in establishing the word “play” in article 31 of the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child Thus, in 1989, the Convention on the Rights of the Child, which strengthened the Declaration of Rights of 1959, was adopted by UN General Assembly. On September 2, 1990 it became international law with one notable exception: the US signed the Charter but has not ratified it. The Convention consists of 54 articles that address the basic human rights to children everywhere are entitled. Article 31 specifically addresses the child’s right play. The American Association for the Child’s Right to Play (IPA/USA) was formed in Philadelphia in 1973 as the USA affiliate of the International. The beginning of IPA/USA started with playground creator and author Paul Hogan’s 1967 trip to Europe. Hogan was checking out adventure playgrounds in London with Lady Allen of Hurtwood when he first learned about IPA and began attending their meetings. Actually, the United States had IPA members before the affiliate was formed by Hogan. Mrs. Thomas Hess of Greenwich, Connecticut, was the first American member and Pacific Oaks College and Children’s School in Pasadena, California, was the first American organization member, having joined in 1969. By the early 1970’s IPA/USA membership had grown to the point that a national representatives was selected to send to IPA World Council meetings which are held at the triennial IPA meetings. The first USA representative was Paul Hogan who attended the 1975 Milan meeting two years after IPA/USA was formed. The first president of IPA/USA was not “elected.” While attending that 1975 conference, Hogan met Muriel Otter, the IPA secretary. She “cornered him and made him president of IPA/USA.” He consequently started PlayPlans magazine and began to secure subscribers and more members for IPA/USA. Elaine Ostroff followed Paul as USA Representative to International. Donna Seline attended the Ottawa IPA conference in 1978 and was appointed USA national representative by IPA President, Polly Hill of Canada. Seline organized a regional IPA/USA conference in 1980 at the University of Minnesota with keynote speakers Polly Hill and Brian Sutton-Smith. A subsequent IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 47 national IPA/USA conference was held in 1983 at the University of Minnesota with keynote speakers Janet McLean and Joe Frost. In June 1983, The International Conference on Play and Play Environments; sponsored by IPA/USA and twelve other state, national, and international organizations; was held at the University of Texas. This was reputed to be the largest gathering of play scholars ever convened, with more than 500 participants from twelve nations. At this conference, plans were initiated for a more formal organization of IPA/USA and for the 1986 national conference in Cincinnati. In July, 1985, Donna Seline submitted her resignation as national representative, effective April, 1986. In February 1986, Donna Seline initiated calls for nominations for a formal Board of Directors for IPA/USA and a mail election was held. Elected for two-year terms were Joe Frost (President and U.S. Representative), Sue Wortham (Treasurer and Membership Coordinator), Marcy Guddemi (Newsletter Editor), and Board members Jay Beckwith, Harris Forusz, Roger Hart, Robin Moore, and Barbara Sampson. An USA/IPA national conference was organized by Harris Forusz in 1986 and was hosted by Adventure Playgrounds, Inc. and the University of Cincinnati. Formal resolutions were agreed upon, including a pronouncement of support for the IPA 1977 Declaration of the Child’s Right to Play adopted in Malta in 1977 and revised in Vienna in 1982. Additional details for modifying Incorporation and Bylaws documents were addressed. Robin Moore and Joe Frost participated in the 1986 World Council IPA meeting and special International Year of Peace seminar in Birmingham, UK. During this IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 period a number of adventure playgrounds were developed in the U.S. By 1987, IPA included members from fifty countries. Thirty-five Americans attended the 1987 World Congress in Stockholm. By 1988 IPA/USA membership had grown to 165. In 1988 Tom Jambor was elected president and in that year the national conference was held in Washington, D. C., directed by Barbara Sampson, Robin Moore, and Donna Seline. Board members were Tom Jambor (President), Helge Stapel (Treasurer), Marcy Guddemi (Newsletter Editor), and Board Members Duraid Da’as, Robin Epstein, Harris Forusz, Paul Hogan, Lulu King, Mary Lillie, Ruth Morrison, Jack Pentes, Donna Seline, Randy Smith, Frank Rudloff, and William Weisz. In January 1989, this expanded Board met in Cincinnati and made plans for expanding revenue, newsletter, membership, and creating a clearinghouse on information. At the Tokyo conference in 1990, Robin Moore of IPA/USA, already an IPA international officer, was the first American to be elected IPA president. With these patterns in place, IPA/USA programs continued to expand to the present time, including community PlayDays, annual play advocacy presentations at national conferences, semi-annual national IPA/USA conferences, promoting the international IPA conference, preparing and distributing materials for parents and school administrators promoting advocacy and reinstatement for school recess, media campaigns for recess and outdoor play, and multiple efforts to support and gain awareness of the value of play and the child’s right to play. National conferences were held jointly with The Association for the Study of Play conferences at the Strong Museum of Play in 2007 and at Georgia State University in 2010. In 2010, 48 Olga Jarrett was the outgoing President and Tom Reed was the newly elected president. Play Resources o o o o o Book Reviews IPA Brochures IPA Working Paper on Children’s Play PlayRights Magazine Resources and Links IPA/USA is the American Affiliate of the International Play Association: Promoting the Child's Right to Play. The purpose of this organization is to protect, preserve, and promote play as a fundamental right for all humans. Membership is open to any individual, group, or organization which endorses the right of children to play, stated in the United Nations' Declaration of the Rights of the Child (1959), Article 7, paragraph 3: "The child shall have full opportunity for play and recreation which should be directed to the same purposes as education; society and the public authorities shall endeavor to promote the enjoyment of the right...” and in the Convention on the Rights of the Child (1989), Article 31: 1. States Parties recognize the right of the child to rest and leisure, to engage in play and recreational activities appropriate to the age of the child and to participate freely in cultural life and the arts. 2. States Parties shall respect and promote the right of the child to participate fully in cultural and artistic life and shall encourage the provision of appropriate and equal opportunities for cultural, artistic, recreational and leisure activity. IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 IPA/USA holds a national conference every three years and sponsors or cosponsors workshops and symposia from time to time. A quarterly newsletter, available on the website, features articles on play environments, safety, toys and materials, play leadership, and the role of play in child development. IPA International publishes an international journal called PlayRights and hosts a World Conference every three years. IF YOU BELIEVE THAT: Play, along with the basic needs of nutrition, health, shelter, and education, is vital for the development of the potential of all children, Family participation needs to be strengthened by support systems such as a play leadership, and Environments and programs to meet children's real needs should be created. IF YOU ARE CONCERNED THAT: Society is indifferent to the importance of play, Schools are indifferent to the importance of play, Children are increasingly living with inadequate provision for survival and development, Children are increasingly living in inappropriate housing settings with less spaces to play, Children are neglected in environmental planning, Cultural traditions are deteriorating and that children are Increasingly exploited commercially, Children are constantly exposed to war, violence, and destruction, "Winning at all costs” dominates children's sports and play, 49 THEN PLEASE CONSIDER BECOMING A MEMBER OF IPA/USA and become a part of an international, interdisciplinary organization that supports and works for the Child's Right to Play. IPA is an international nongovernmental organization founded in 1961. It provides a forum for exchange and action across disciplines and across sectors. University of Texas at Austin, where he taught for 34 years. He has authored or co-authored 18 books, several having been translated into other languages, as well as six volumes of original papers, reports and articles. Dr. Frost has also served as editor for many texts. He is known all over the world for his more than 30 years of work on early childhood and children’s play environments. He has also served as a consultant for playgrounds worldwide and is a past president of both the Association for Childhood Education International and International Play Association/USA IPA’s purpose is to protect, preserve and promote the child’s right to play as a fundamental human right. ~Article 31 of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child “That every child has the right to rest and leisure, to engage in play and recreational activities appropriate to the age of the child and to participate freely in cultural life and the arts. That member governments shall respect and promote the right of the child to participate fully in cultural and artistic life and shall encourage the provision of appropriate and equal opportunities for cultural, artistic, recreational and leisure activity.” Authors Marcy Guddemi, PhD, MBA, is the Executive Director Gesell Institute of Child Development. She is the current Treasurer of IPA/USA, Past President of IPA/USA, and Past Secretary of IPA. She has also served as Newsletter Editor and Secretary of IPA/USA. Joe Frost, EdD., L.H.D., is the Parker Centennial Professor Emeritus at the IPA/USA e-journal Fall 2013 50