EDRA 42 - Environmental Design Research Association

Transcription

EDRA 42 - Environmental Design Research Association
Make No Little Plans
Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the
Environmental Design Research Association
Chicago, Illinois
May 25-28, 2011
Editors: Daniel Mittleman, PhD and Deborah A. Middleton, PhD
EDRA42Chicago Conference Co-Chairs
Sally Augustin
Danny Mittleman
Roberta Feldman, Chair Emerita
EDRA42Chicago Conference Committee
Saif Haq
Vibhavari Jani
Yusuke Kita
Byoung-Suk Kweon
Janet Loebach
Deborah Middleton
Kyriakos Pontkis
Lubomir Popov
April Allen
Susanna Alves
Cherif Amor
Mallika Bose
Arza Churchman
Meredith Dobbie
Mohammad Gharipour
Tasoulla Hadjiyanni
Robert Ryan
April Spivak
Thea Standerski
Zhe Wang
Nick Watkins
Jeremy Wells
HG (Helena) Yoon
EDRA42Chicago Conference Sponsors
Sustainable Design Sponsors
The Hulda B. and Maurice L. Rothschild Foundation
HOK, Inc.
Intensive Lunch Sponsor
SAGE
Supporting Sponsor
Archeworks
EDRA Board of Directors
Lynne Dearborn, Chair
Nicholas Watkins, Chair-Elect
Mallika Bose, Secretary
Shauna Mallory-Hill, Treasurer
Robert Ryan, Past Chair
Victoria Chanse
Virginia Kupritz
Byoung-Suk Kweon
Janet Loebach
Atiya Mahmood
Kate O’Donnell, Executive Director
(ex officio)
EDRA Placemakers
EDRA Wayfinders
Keith Diaz Moore
Karen Franck
Nicholas Watkins
Sherry Ahrentzen
Lyn Geboy
Danny Mittleman
Wiley Clifton Montague
Jack Nasar
Lynn Paxson
Thierry Rosenheck
Copyright © 2011, The Environmental Design Research Association (EDRA) www.edra.org
1760 Old Meadow Road, Suite 500 • McLean, VA 22102
ISBN 978-1-257-76543-0
All rights reserved. No part of this book covered by copyright rein may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any
means – graphic, photocopy, recording, taping, or digital – without written permission of the publisher.
Table of Contents
Keynote Address
Creating a Children’s Hospital by Engaging the City of Chicago...........................................................................1
Plenary Sessions
Wishful Thinking or Welcome Change? Scrutinizing New Workplace Beliefs.....................................................2
Big Plans to Restore the Rustbelt: The Calumet Region of Indiana and Illinois ....................................................2
Design Practitioner Intensives
Sustainable Charrette: Fully Integrated Thinking....................................................................................................3
Lost in Translation: The Design Challenges Faced by Multinational Corporations...............................................4
Implementing Person-Centered Design in Healthcare: Building Connections........................................................4
The Research Funders Speak: Support for Applied and Theoretical Research.......................................................5
Design Practitioner Professional Development: Writing and Administering Surveys...........................................5
Show Me the Data: The Essential Role of Research in Making the Business Case for Building a
Better Hospital....................................................................................................................................................5
Watching Closely: Environmental Design Insights Courtesy of the Lincoln Park Zoo Gorillas............................6
Design Practitioner Professional Development: Post-Occupancy Evaluations in the Trenches.............................6
Design Practitioner Professional Development: Ask the Research Experts............................................................6
Career Award Winner
Galen Cranz, PhD, Professor, University of California, Berkeley..........................................................................7
Service Award Winner
Janice Bissell, PhD, Principal, The Practitioner’s Resource...................................................................................8
Refereed Full Papers
How the Logic of Research Misunderstands the Logic of Design........................................................................11
Public Perception of Walkability: Assessing Perception Dependency on Scene Attributes and Respondent
Characteristics .................................................................................................................................................18
Participatory Design in Positive Aging: Products of the First Cycle of an Environmental Design
Action Research Project...................................................................................................................................32
Grandma’s World: Mapping Dancers in Beijing...................................................................................................38
The Morphological Evolution of the ‘Bazaar Streets’: A Configurational Analysis of the Urban Layout
of Dhaka City...................................................................................................................................................50
Refereed Full Papers (continued)
Lessons From The Road: Meaning and Community Identity Examined Through the Lens of the
Roadside Attraction..........................................................................................................................................62
Children’s Conception of Natural Environments and Associated Play Activities in Santiago, Chile ..................72
Enhanced Photo-Elicitation for Accessing Alzheimer’s Perceptions....................................................................82
The Perception of Place in Planning for Urban Change: A Small World Paradigm.............................................91
Effects Of Garden Design On Quality Of Life ...................................................................................................102
Integration of Ecology, Sociology and Aesthetics in Urban Design: Fantasy or Reality? A Study through
the Lens of Landscape Architecture ..............................................................................................................114
Positive Environments: Emotional Relief through Evidence-Based Design of a Neonatal
Intensive Care Unit.........................................................................................................................................123
A Survey on the Social Activity of the Demented Elderly by RFID in the Special Care Unit...........................132
Emerging Learning Commons in Japanese Academic Libraries: Behavioral Aspects of Actual Usage
and Communication Patterns .........................................................................................................................145
The Role of Built Heritage on Aesthetic Evaluation of Urban Landscape..........................................................154
Evaluating the Impact of Urban Form on Older Adults’ Social Activity and Public Health .............................166
Threshold In School – As Event and Place for the Autism Spectrum Disorder Pupil.........................................174
A Systemic Theory for Historic Preservation: Land Use Management Strategies as Preservation Policy.........181
Valuation of Cultural Heritage: Toward a Conceptual Model and Potential Evaluation Strategies...................189
Conceptual Foundations for Modeling the Sociospatial Structures of Activity in Facilities Programming.......197
The Architecture and Urban Design Project Viewed as a Catalyst of Identity: .................................................203
Analysis of Recent Contributions to ‘Montrealness’...........................................................................................203
Environmental Values and Their Relationship to Ecological Services...............................................................212
Rethinking Architectural Research......................................................................................................................218
Therapeutically Enhanced School Design for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD):
A Comparative Study of the United States and the United Kingdom ...........................................................225
Residential Experience of Rural Immigrants: Understanding Migrant Enclaves in Urban China......................236
Abstracts.................................................................................................................................................... 249-370
Posters........................................................................................................................................................ 373-467
Author Index............................................................................................................................................. 469-472
Author Index
Author Index
A
Adams, Roxanne 301
Adams, Vera 373
Adhya, Anirban 279, 288
Aeschbacher, Peter 281, 334
Afshar, Maryam 249
Ahn, Changhoun 295
Ahn, You-Kyong 365
Ahrentzen, Sherry 287
Akagi, Tetsuya 374
Al Slik, Ghada Musa Rzouki 365
Alawadhi, Ahmed 268
Alden, Andrew 324
Allen, April 270, 365
Altinbasak, Ece 249
Alves, Susana 291, 359
Alward, Sarah 250
Amam, Willow Lung 272
Amedeo, Douglas 250
Amor, Cherif 252
An, So Mi 375
Anderson, Nadia 253
Andrews, Clinton 366
Angrisano, Neal 339
Anthony, Kathryn H. 362
Ashida, Haruka 376
Asojo, Abimbola 254, 296
Aspinall, Peter 342, 359
Augustin, Sally 2, 5, 6, 254, 281
Awwad-Rafferty, Rula 297, 312
B
Badenhope, Julia 254
Balakrishnar, Bimal 268
Baran, Perver 287
Barker, Greg Allen 6, 342
Bartlett, M’Lis 255
Bartos, Ethan 345
Beacham, Cindy 255
Bergdall, Terry 256
Bernasconi, Claudia 18
Bestetti, Maria Luisa Trindade 257
Biavatti, Camila 341
Bissell, Janice 8
Blake, Sheri 317
Bloom, Michael F. 5
Boeck, David 32
Bose, Mallika 263
Bradley, Gordon 300
Brand, Jay 2
Brigman, Jonee Kulman 257
Brisby, Traci 258
Brösamle, Martin 11
Brown, Barbara 259
Budd, Christopher 2
Buechner, Simon 288
Bulone, Phil 285
Byrne, J. Kevin 259
Byun, Kyoungmee 377
C
Calkins, Margaret 4, 312
Campo, Michael 324
Cannon, Molly Boeka 250
Capozzi, Christopher 82, 265, 266
Carlson, Laura 288
Cayton, Jennifer 363
Chandrasekera, Tilanka 260, 268
Chang, Chun-Yen 261, 355
Chang, Hyejung 250, 261
Chang, Yuan-Yu 261
Chapagain, Neel Kamal 348
Chaudhury, Habib 324
Chen, Caroline 38
Chen, Chan 331
Cheow, Puei Kuen 262
Chiang, Yen-Cheng 262
Choi, Jaepil 313
Choi, SeonMi 257, 378, 379
Choi, Sohee 380
Choi, Young-Seon 263
Chou, Wan Yu 381
Colman, Jennifer 258
Cook, Jessica 263
Corriente, Marina Mellado 348
Cosme, Sílvia 341
Coulombe, Simon 382
Cranz, Galen 7, 264, 265, 266, 279,
281
Crawford, Pat 307
D
D’Souza, Newton 260, 268
da Cunha, Regina Tristão 341
Dalton, Ruth 288
Davis, Lee 270
Day, Adam M.B. 270
Dearborn, Lynne 287
Després, Carole 271
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Devlin, Ann 6
Diamond, Beth 250, 271, 278, 336, 343
Dieterlen, Susan 272, 279
Dobbie, Meredith 273
Dominski, Jason 6
Doyle, Michael 271, 273
Doyle, Philip C. 270
Drucker, Susan 5, 321
Duffey, Melanie 282
Dulaney Jr., Ron 274
Duncil, Barry 278
Durnbaugh, Aaron N. 2, 366
E
Egan, Kate 324
Elabd, Aliaa 274, 275
Erickson, Doug 4
Ericson, Jonathan 288
F
Feldman, Roberta 276
Fell, David 276
Ferdous, Farhana 50
Fernando, Nisha A. 277
Figueira, Kristen 383
Finlay, Karen 277
Flanagan, Robert 298
Flanders, Ben 256
Floyd, Myron 353
Foen, Fai 278
Fonseca, Ernesto 287
Fowlow, Loraine 62
Fox, Andrew 278
Franck, Karen 279, 281, 298
Fratinardo, Marlise 281
Frisius, Daniela Casanello 72
G
Gadoury, Colin 82, 265, 266
Gaines, Kristi 225
Gale, Amanda 282
Ganis, Mary 91
Geboy, Lyn 324
Ghobad, Ladan 282
Gillem, Mark 283
Gilliam, Alex 283
Gilliland, Jason 284, 315
Ginsburg, Rebecca 300
Gobes-Ryan, Sheila 285, 321
469
Author Index
Gobster, Paul 259, 353
Goetcheus, Cari 365
Goto, Seiko 102
Green, Raymond James 72
Greene, Margarita 285
Grese, Robert E. 300
Gross, Broc 297
Guerin, Denise A. 257
Guilford, Emily 252
Guindon-Bronsard, Valérie 271
Gulwadi, Gowri Betrabet 291, 297
Gumpert, Gary 5, 321
H
Ha, Ji-Min 384
Hadjiyanni, Tasoulla 286, 287
Hajrasouliha, Amir Hossein 114
Hallowell, George 275, 287
Han, Jung-Ho 385
Haq, Saif 288
Harbison, Rachel 386
Harland, Joel S. 297
Harvey, Thomas E. 335
Harwood, Pamela 387, 388
Hasirci, Deniz 304
Hathorn, Kathy 325
Hayashida, Daisaku 328
Hayata, Naohiko 389
Heggernes, Laurel J. 297
Henderson, Phyllis J. 390
Heroux, Elizabeth 123
Hikita, Atsushi 327
Hillman-Healey, Christy 62
Hipp, Aaron 291
Hirata, Sonomi 293
Hoback, Alan 18
Hodulak, Martin 4, 317
Holleran, Michael 365
Hölscher, Christoph 11, 288
Honda, Tomotsune 391
Hornor, Jim 6
Horrigan, Paula 294
Host-Jablonski, Lou 287
HoVan, Ngoc 392
Hsu, Tan-Kui 132
Hu, Jianxin 282
Huang, Te-Sheng 298
Hunt, Jon 368
Hwang, EunJu 368
Hwang, Ji Hye 295, 311, 312, 393, 394
Hwang, Yao-Rong 132
470 I
Ierodiaconou, Daniel 355
Iguchi, Aya 395
Imon, Sharif Shams 348
Irvine, Aliah 301
Ishikawa, Makoto 293
Itoh, Shunsuke 328
J
Jaddo, Lahib 296
Jang, Miseon 396
Jani, Vibhavari 254, 296
Jazwinski, Christine H. 297
Jensen, Sara 297
John, Naiana Maura 397
Joplin, Amber 298
Jorn, Myounghee 398, 399
Juhasz, Joseph 298, 368
Jung, Eun Jung 400
K
Kader, Sharmin 324
Kaller, Gregor 288
Kamada, Mitsutoshi 401
Kamel, Ehab 348
Kanakri, Shireen 402
Kaplan, Abram W. 300
Kaplan, Rachel 300
Kaplan, Stephen 300
Kapp, Paul 365
Kato, Akikazu 4, 145, 403
Kaufman, Andrew 301
Keable, Ellen 2, 321
Keddy, Karen 302
Kee, Chan 4
Kemper, Rebecca 302
Kepez, Orcun 249, 303
Keul, Alexander 366
Khasawneh, Fahed A. 145
Kilic, Didem Kan 304
Kim, Hye-Young 257
Kim, Jun 404
Kim, Jusuck 405
Kim, Min Kyoung 406, 407
Kim, Minseok 313
Kim, Suk-Kyung 304
Kim, Sun Young 408
Kita, Yusuke 305
Kivutha, Kwekwe 409
Kline, Rhonda 311, 410
Kocher, Sara 365
Kocs, Elizabeth A. 306
Komiske, Bruce 1
Kong, Chuijing 411
Koo, Jayoung 306
Kornakova, Maria 307
Kozak, Robert 276
Koziol, Christopher 365
Kramer, Carrie 338
Kremer, Peter 355
Kubala, Tom 337, 345
Kuo, Frances (Ming) 291, 308
Kuo, Nai-Wen 362
Kupritz, Virginia 321
Kweon, Byoung-Suk 308
Kwon, Jain 309
Kwon, Young-Gull 309
L
Labbe, Delphene 412
Lake, Jordan 413
Langhorst, Joern 309
Lay, Maria Cristina Dias 154
Lee, Dongjoo 311, 312
Lee, Eunsil 310
Lee, Jae Hwa 414
Lee, Jaechoon 304
Lee, Jongmin 415, 416
Lee, Sangmi 301
Lee, Seunghae 311
Lee, Yeunsook 295, 311, 312, 417, 418,
419, 420, 421, 422, 423
Leslie, Eva 355
Levi, Daniel 365
Lewis, Kristin 350
Lim, Lisa 313
Lim, Soohyun 424
Limb, Eugene 6
Lin, Yu Fong 268
Lindsay, Georgia 264, 265, 266
Link, Felipe 285
Linn, Colleen 425
Lloyd, Richard 314
Loebach, Janet 284, 315
Lottrup, Lene 316
Luo, Yang 316
M
Macedo, Silvio 336
Machemer, Patricia 307
Madani, Mehran 166
Maguire, Barry 174
Mahmood, Atiya 317
Mahon, Bonnie 4
Main, Debbi 263
Author Index
Malkin, Jain 4
Mallory-Hill, Shauna 317
Marans, Robert 366
Marberry, Sara O. 5
Marmurek, Harvey 277
Marshall, Wesley 263
Masters, Jane M. 352
Mateo-Babiano, Derlie 91
Mavridou, Magda 11
Mayer, Robert 4
Mazumdar, Sanjoy 293, 328
McAllister, Keith 174, 426
McBain, Marsha 333
McCoy, Janetta 254
McKenzie, Kristyn 338
McLaren, Coralee 288
Mehaffy, Michael 337
Messaoud, Meriem Belhaj 271
Miklus, Melissa 278
Milota, Cynthia 427
Minner, Jennifer 365
Minnery, John 91
Mittleman, Daniel 2, 256, 321, 322, 323,
328
Modi, Sonal Mithal 348
Mohai, Paul 308
Mojtahedi, Amin 323
Monnai, Teruyuki 305
Montague, Kim 4
Moore, Gary T. 352
Moore, Keith Diaz 324, 365
Mora, Rodrigo 285
Morckel, Victoria 325
Morefield, Leslie 278
Morhayim, Lusi 264, 265, 266
Mori, Shiho 145, 428
Motoyama, Yui 429
Moxley, David 32
N
Nanda, Upali 325
Narayan, Debarati Majumdar 326
Nasar, Jack 281
Newman, Galen 181, 327
Nippert-Eng, Christina 6
Nishide, Kazuhiko 262
Noh, Taejin 430, 431
O
O’Connor, Zena 189
Oakley, Christine 166
Oh, Chanohk 434
Oh, Jiyoung 432, 433
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Ohno, Ryuzo 327, 328
Okely, Anthony D. 352
Opreane, Danielle 268
Orthel, Bryan 330, 365
Ou, Sheng-Jung 330
Owens, Patsy Eubanks 332
Oygur, Isil 332
P
Pable, Jill 252, 333
Padmanabha, Aarthi 301
Papanikolopoulos, Nikos 286
Park, Changbae 333
Park, Jin Gyu “Phillip” 333
Park, Jin Kyoung 435
Park, Kyung-nan 436
Park, Nam-Kyu 310
Park, Soim 437
Patel, Kush 334
Pati, Debajyoti 252, 335
Pavlides, Eleftherios 265, 266
Paxson, Lynn 298, 368
Pearson, David 327
Pentecost, Ray 4
Pereira, Maria Lourdes 341
Perez, Yael Valerie 264, 335
Petit, Dana 336
Petzoldt, Natalie 363
Pillemer, Karl 365
Pippi, Luis 336
Place, Wayne 282
Ponsart, Carline 271
Pontikis, Kyriakos 337, 345
Popov, Lubomir 197, 285, 322, 338, 342
Popova, Margarita 322, 338
Prince, Ashley 338
Prochazka, Alena 203
Proffitt, Mark 438
Pukszta, Michael 363
Purcell, Terry 262
Q
Quillien, Jenny 337, 345
R
Rabig, Jude 4
Radja, Abdul Mufti 328
Rapoport, Amos 328
Rashid, Mahbub 339, 340
Rego, Isabel Estrela 341
Reis, Antonio Tarcisio 341
Ren, Lanbin 283
Rice, Arthur 336
Robinson, Julia 279
Rodrigues, Mirian 154
Roe, Jenny 342, 357, 359
Rofè, Yodan Y. 337, 369
Rogers, Carl 368
Roh, Dae-gun 439
Rohde, Jane 4
Rohlfing, Colin 3
Rollings, Kimberly A. 287
Rosenheck, Thierry 342
Rosenthal, Cindy Simon 32
Ross, Laurel 2, 366
Rowley, Joshua 440
Runge, Christian 343
Ryan, Robert 300
S
Sagan, Hans 343
Schneekloth, Lynda 279
Schnobrich, Corey 344
Schramm, Ulrich 317
Schroeder, Herbert W. 212
Schwarz, Benyamin 218
Schweitzer, Will 259
Scott, Angelita 257
Seamon, David 337, 345
Seeger, Chris 254
SengKee, Chan 441
Senick, Jennifer 366
Seo, Hyun 442
Seo, Hyun-Bo 347
Shabha, Ghasson 225
Shambaugh, Neal 255
Shateh, Ali-Hadi 340
Shehayb, Dina K. 348
Shibayama, Yoriko 145, 443
Shibley, Robert 279
Shih, Chia-Jung 444
Shin, Jung-hye 347
Shitnaka, Akitomo 445
Shores, Kindal 353
Showers, Leslie 256
Sickler, Stephanie 446
Sieweke, Jorg 447
Silva, Kapila D. 348
Silverberg, Dana 448
Singh, Punya 449
Sinha, Amita 348
Skorupka, Aga 288
Sleegers, Frank 349
Smith, Erin 324
Socha, Chris 345
471
Author Index
Son, Da-jung 450
Song, Jihyun 350
Song, Seung Eon 451
Spivack, April 4
Spooner, David 350
Spreckelmeyer, Kent 339
Springer, Tim 2
Stallmeyer, John 351
Stefanovic, Ingrid 345
Steggell, Carmen 317
Steimel, Allie 338
Sterne, Rita Hansen 351
Stigsdotter, Ulrika K. 316, 357
Su, Shiuan-Ru 352
Sue, Mayu 452
Sugiyama, Takemi 352, 353, 355
Sullivan, William 300, 355
Suntikul, Wantanee 348
Surratt, Melissa 356
Suzuki, Takeshi 328
Swanson, John 2
Sykes, Kathy 365
T
Takehara, Misato 453
Taniguchi, Gen 145
Taylor, Zachary 82
Terzano, Kathryn 356
Tesar, Paul 250, 279
Thering, Susan 287, 357
Thompson, Catharine Ward 357, 359
Thompson, Jo Ann Asher 332
Torgrude, Sue 288
Touboku, Hiroki 454
472 Tsuchida, Hiroshi 455
Tural, Elif 360
Tzeng, Seungeon 456
Wolfe, Julie 367
Wong, Sally 6
Wonnett, Robert 279
V
X
van Oel, Clarine J. 361
Vandermark, Elizabeth 361
Vanucchi, Jamie 294
Varma, Annie 348
Villagra-Islas, Paula 272
Vo, Ngoc 268
Vujkov, Aleksander 334
Xu, Fang 459
Xu, Huiyan 460
W
Wachter, Hans-Peter 32
Wakasugi, Sae 457
Walden, Rotraut 317
Walker, Verrick 252
Wampler, Janet 362
Wang, Chia-Hui 362
Wang, Zhe 363
Warners, Roemer W. 361
Watkins, Nick 6, 324
Waxman, Lisa 333, 364
Weidemann, Sue 321
Wells, Jeremy C. 345, 365
Wells, Nancy M. 287, 300, 356, 357,
365
Wener, Richard 366
Westlund, Anna 458
Westphal, Lynne 2, 300, 366
Whitlock, Stephanie 5
Wiener, Jan 288
Willow, Diane 286
Y
Yamamoto, Rika 461
Yocca, David 367
Yokoyama, Yurika 328
Yoon, Hyegyung 311, 312
Yoon, So-Yeon 260, 268
Yoshinaga, Norio 462
Yost, Bambi 368
Yousefi, Maryam 463
Yun, Hae-Young 368
Z
Zarchin, Inbal F. 369
Zervas, Deborah 288
Zhang, Bo 369
Zhang, Guixian 464, 465
Zhang, Yuben 466
Zheng, Hua 467
Zhu, Yushu 236
Ziebarth, Ann 368
Zimring, Craig 263
Zou, Jun 370
Zuin, Affonso 359
Chicago is the intersection of people and place. The
history of Chicago is marked by seminal events intertwining people and place—from the rebuilding after
the great fire of 1871, to Pullman’s company town, to
a diaspora of Southern rural Blacks in the 1930s, to the
riots of 1968. Interspersed among these events were
ambitious inventions including Olmstead’s design of
Riverside: the first planned suburb, the floating foundation that enabled the first steel skyscraper, the first hotel
elevators, and the first Ferris wheel. Design history
continues to be made in Chicago—from the sophisticated tools used by Studio Gang on the Aqua Tower, to
the floating roof on the Modern Wing of the Art Institute of Chicago (Renzo Piano) that floods the structure
with sunlight, to Mayor Daley’s programs for public
greening and bicycle paths, to the innovative public
sculptures in Millennium Park that encourage user
interaction..
Today Chicago is the home of great museums; significant civic and neighborhood parks; public and art
and sculpture. It is a city of ethnic neighborhoods, wide
boulevards, botanical gardens and forest preserves. It is
a city that has protected almost all of its shoreline for
public use.
Chicago is the incubator of great plans and urban
invention. And for this reason, EDRA borrowed Daniel
Burnham’s charge for its 2011 conference theme: Make
No Little Plans.
EDRA42Chicago is a new generation of EDRA
conference, fully integrating its theme. We’ve made no
little plans here. EDRA42Chicago retains the association’s traditional focus on research and practice that
enhance human experience, both inside and outside
structures, in spaces that are man-made and those that
bear only the lightest human imprint. But this conference also grows EDRA’s missional commitments in
several directions.
• The paper submission and refereeing process has
been revamped for EDRA42Chicago toward the
delivering the highest level of conference quality
possible. Delivery on this goal has been supported
by receiving and processing over 450 program
submissions, a 33 percent increase over the DC
conference.
• EDRA42Chicago has extended the track of Design
Practitioner sessions begun last year in DC. We
offered nine sessions explicitly designed to transfer
research knowledge to design practice.
• Mobile sessions, created this year, are an extension
of the traditional EDRA tours with an intentional
focus of providing in situ academic content.
• This year, for the first time, Display Posters images
appear in the proceedings. This provides you with a
post conference view of these visual presentations.
This EDRA program is large, varied, and robust. It
represents the broad array of environment and behavior
interests research and practiced by our members. It
includes topics that have been grist for EDRA conversation for over four decades, as well as cutting
edge—cyberspace—issues, non-existent a decade ago.
EDRA members’ diverse interests and their dedication
to enhancing the lives of others are EDRA’s greatest
strengths. As you read through the pages that follow
you will see that the mission of EDRA’s founders, first
articulated 42 years ago, retains its vigor.
EDRA42Chicago Proceedings Reviewers
Erin Adams, Morteza Adib, Mariela Alfonzo, April Allen, Sarah Alward, Cherif Amor, Abimbola Asojo, Sally Augustin,
Greg Barker, M’lis Bartlett, Lisa Bates, Cindy Beacham, Julio Bermudez, Mallika Bose, Angeal Bourne, Traci Brisby, J.
Kevin Byrne, Ashley Calabria, Maggie Calkins, Tilanka Chandrasekera, Chin-Wei Chang, Chun-Yen Chang, Meldrena
Chapin, Nancy Chapman, Habib Chaudhury, Chingwen Cheng, Youngseon Choi, Ji Young Choi, Jessica Cook, Anne Dahl
Refshauge, Adam Day, Evrim Demir Mishenko, Ann Devlin, Beth Diamond, Keith Diaz Moore, Susan Dieterlen, Suining
Ding, Meredith Dobbie, Michael Doyle, Ron Dulaney Jr., Jason Duvall, Aliaa Elabd, A.Sameh Elkharbawy, Jenny Ernawati,
Andrew Fox, Marlise Fratinardo, Amanda Gale, Lyn Geboy, Hodjat Ghadimi, Mohammad Gharipour, Rokhshid Ghaziani,
Ladan Ghobad, Alex Gilliam, Jason Gilliland, Sheila Gobes-Ryan, Denise Guerin, Gowri Gulwadi, Tasoulla Hadjiyanni,
George Hallowell, Rita Hansen Sterne, Sonomi Hirata, Heidi Hohmann, Paula Horrigan, John Hu, Misun Hur, Lahib Jaddo,
Vibhavari Jani, Christine Jazwinski, Sara Jensen, Didem Kan Kilic, Mihyun Kang, Shubhra Kannan, Stephen Kaplan, Ellen
Keable, Suk-Kyung Kim, Nana Kirk, Jayoung Koo, Maria Kornakova, Jain Kwon, Christiana Lafazani, Joern Langhorst,
Jessu Lara, Lisa Lim, Georgia Lindsay, Richard Lloyd, Janet Loebach, Lene Lottrup, Gabriela Luna, Yang Luo, Victoria
Linn Lygum, Debarati Majumdar Narayan, Gesine Marquardt, Sanjoy Mazumdar, Janetta Mccoy, Lindsay Mccunn, Deborah
Middleton, Danny Mittleman, Hiroko Mizumura, Victoria Morckel, Galen Newman, Crispino Ochieng, Eva Oliveira, Eric
Orlando Jiménez Rosas, Patricia Ortega-Andeane, Bryan Orthel, Sheng-Jung Ou, Isil Oygur, Samira Pasha, Lynn Paxson,
Yael Valerie Perez, Illène Pevec, Luis Pippi, Jena Ponti, Kyriakos Pontkis, Lubomir Popov, Isabel Rego, Jenny Roe, Yodan
Rofe`, Christian Runge, Robert Ryan, Hans Sagan, Henry Sanoff, Lynda Schneekloth, Corey Schnobrich, Ulrich Schramm,
David Seamon, Hyun-Bo Seo, Archana Sharma, Jung-Hye Shin, Frank Sleegers, Jihyun Song, April Spivack, David Spooner,
Thea Standerski, Wei-Chia Su, Takemi Sugiyama, Lolly Tai, Jackie Teed, Kristin Thorleifsdottir, Zeynep Toker, Maria Luisa
Trindade Bestetti, Elif Tural, Clarine Van Oel, Elizabeth Vandermark, Janet Wampler, Zhe Wang, Chia-Hui Wang, Nicholas
Watkins, Lisa Waxman, Nanci Weinberger, Jeremy Wells, Kyle Whitfield, Julie Wolfe, Helena Yoon, Hae-Young Yun, and
Bo Zhang
EDRA42Chicago Proceedings Network Participants
Mallika Bose (Active Living by Design)
Barbara Brown (Active Living by Design)
Darcy Varney (Children & Youth Environments)
Yusuki Kita (Cities & Globalization)
April Allen (Cultural Aspects of Design)
Sanjoy Mazumdar (Cultural Aspects of Design)
Saif Haq (EDRAMove)
Sue Torgrude (EDRAMove)
Jeremy Wells (Historic Preservation)
Cherif Amor (Interior Design)
Mohammad Gharipour (International Housing)
Susana Alves (Nature & Ecology)
Aaron Hipp (Nature & Ecology)
Lubomir Popov (POE/Programming)
Tasoulla Hadjiyanni (Residential Environments)
Janet Loebach (Student Network)
April Spivak (Work Environments)
Invited Papers
and Special
Presentations
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Keynote Address
Creating a Children’s Hospital by Engaging the
City of Chicago
Thursday, May 26, 10:30am
Bruce Komiske, MHA, FACHE
Bruce Komiske, Chief of New Hospital Design and Construction, Children’s Memorial
Hospital, Chicago, will focus on the importance and impact of engaging the Chicago community and cultural groups in a participatory process to fund and build the tallest children’s
hospital in the world, the Ann & Robert H. Lurie Children’s Hospital of Chicago.
Komiske’s address cross cuts interest in healthcare design, workplace design, children and
youth environments, and participatory design, by discussing the development of this children’s hospital design program that integrated deep programmatic participation from over a
half dozen Chicago area museums, each of which developed, integrated, and curated museum
artifacts into the design of the hospital.
Bruce Komiske is considered a “world expert” on all facets of hospital operations, planning, design and construction. He is an experienced healthcare executive who has had the
opportunity to plan, build, operate and raise the philanthropic support for eight new hospitals,
each more innovative than the previous, all from the owner’s perspective. In addition, he is
a frequent speaker, author and consultant on the inclusive planning process, healing environments, engaging the community in a healthcare project, all facets of children’s hospitals,
healthcare design and healthcare philanthropy for hospitals around the world.
He is the editor and creator of Children’s Hospitals – the World’s Best Designed (Images
Publishing), Children’s Hospitals – The Future of Healing Environments (Images Publishing),
Maria’s Wish, the Story of the Maria Fareri Children’s Hospital (Images Publishing), and
Family Partnership in Hospital Care (Springer Publishing).
Prior to his position at Children’s Memorial Hospital, Komiske led the pre construction
planning for the University of California San Francisco, (UCSF), Mission Bay Project, a
$1.5 billion, Children’s, Women’s and Cancer Hospital. He received his Masters in Hospital
Administration from Duke University and is a Fellow in the American College of Healthcare
Executives.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
1
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Plenary Sessions
Wishful Thinking or Welcome
Change? Scrutinizing New
Workplace Beliefs
Big Plans to Restore the
Rustbelt: The Calumet Region
of Indiana and Illinois
Friday, May 27, 10:30am
Saturday, May 28, 10:30am
Ellen Keable, Jacobs Global Buildings
Consulting; Tim Springer, HERO-inc; Sally
Augustin, Design With Science; Christopher
Budd, STUDIOS architects; Daniel Mittleman,
DePaul University; and Jay Brand, Haworth
Lynne Westphal, U.S. Forest Service, Northern
Research Station; Laurel Ross, Chicago
Wilderness; John Swanson, Northwestern
Indiana Regional Planning Commission; and
Aaron N. Durnbaugh, Chicago Department of
Environment’s Natural Resources and Water
Quality Division
Workplace innovation draws on assumptions about
changes in the nature of work, people, business, and
technology. Projected benefits of new workplace are
based on beliefs about the effects of places on human
performance, satisfaction, and health. They seem true –
but is there evidence? How much is wishful thinking?
How much is visioning for the future? Does experience
prove these beliefs are the right road to success?
This session uses a Pecha Kucha format to discuss
some of the most common assumptions behind models
of “The Workplace of the Future”. These short, provocative presentations will draw on relevant research,
organizational values, and practitioners’ own experience
on the following common beliefs:
1. Collaborative Work Is More Important Than
Individual Work.
2. Seeing and Hearing Work Activities Keeps People
Connected and Collaborative.
3. People Can Work Anywhere.
4. Change Management Eases Acceptance of
Workplace Innovation
5. Older Generations Should Adopt Newer
Generations’ Ways Of Working
2 The Calumet region hugs the shore line of Lake
Michigan from southeast Chicago through northwest Indiana, including the Indiana Dunes National Lakeshore.
Long before the Calumet Region became the largest
area of heavy industry in the world, it was part of the
most extensive network of wetlands in the Midwest area
of the United States.
Today, two decades after the decline of the steel
industry, Calumet bears a legacy of both environmental
riches and its industrial heritage. On a short drive, you
might see bird-filled marshes, active industry and abandoned factories, slag heaps, popular riverfront fishing
spots, landfill “hills,” and patches of forestland. While
the region’s residents struggle with limited economic
opportunities and share serious concerns about pollution, they are also enormously proud of the remaining
natural areas and the local industrial heritage.
The conference theme “Make No Little Plans” is a
good fit for Calumet. Calumet has always been big –
from the steel plants and infrastructure, to the passion
and efforts people bring to caring for the region, to the
flurry of plans to bring Calumet to its next phase. These
plans include the Chicago Wilderness’s Green Infrastructure Vision, the Marquette Plan for northwest Indiana, and the Calumet Area Land Use Plan for Chicago.
This presentation will discuss these plans, the partnerships that brought the plans into being, the research that
informed them, and the future steps needed to make the
plans a reality.
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Design Practitioner Intensives
Sustainable Charrette: Fully
Integrated Thinking
Wednesday, May 25, 8:30 am – 5:00 pm,
HOK Chicago, 60 E Van Buren St., Chicago, IL
Organized by Colin Rohlfing, Senior Associate,
Sustainable Design Leader, HOK
“Using a living systems thinking tool to bring about
innovative design solutions and social change.”
The creation of the built environment will someday
revolve around the idea of Fully Integrated Thinking
(FIT)—a living systems thinking tool created using
the Biomimicry Guild’s ‘Life’s Principles’. It aligns
and interconnects currently disconnected systems and
decision-making processes through rethinking and
repositioning a city as one system or ecosystem. By
rethinking of the city as an ecosystem, we are able to
align with both the wisdom and genius of the place
(the natural environment) and how we have settled that
place. It helps us fit in by modeling nature’s local best
practices and leveraging those latent strategies, functions, and opportunities. This tool allows us to be able
to look to the local/regional biomes and ask, “What
are the regional realities that we can look to, and then
relate to what we are doing?” FIT is the starting point.
Species in place are naturalized—they do something
beneficial for the system they are a part of, and the
system becomes generative over time. How do human
beings become naturalized to our place, and thus by
default, become generative? To be naturalized, you fit
in. Over time, an ecosystem will create more diversity,
more resilience, more… life. In thinking of our legacy,
how do we want to leave our cities for our children and
grandchildren? Nature leaves a place better than it was.
How do we become a naturalized, generative species?
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
This charrette will focus on systems that have a correlation to the entire ecosystem of a city. Participants will
engage select city sites on varying levels through various means of education and learning and will be challenged to answer the question, “How do human beings
become naturalized to our place, and thus by default,
become generative?” Participants will be equipped
with a FIT matrix and will have to utilize triple bottom
line thinking and Life’s Principles to create connections
between the city sites, both visited and nonvisited. The
ultimate goal of the charrette is to establish goals, strategies, and connections for the FIT systems listed below
that will benefit multiple sites around the city, whether
built or vacant.
FIT Systems (how an ecosystem/city functions):
Environmental
Ecology, Water, Atmosphere, Materials, Energy, Food
Social
Community, Culture, Health, Education, Governance,
Transport, Shelter
Economic
Commerce, Value
Life’s Principles (how nature solves problems using its
own “technology”):
• Use Benign Manufacturing
• Leverage Interdependence
• Optimize Rather than Maximize
• Resiliency
• Integrate a Cyclic Process
• Locally Attuned and Responsive
.
3
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Lost in Translation: The
Design Challenges Faced by
Multinational Corporations
Wednesday, May 25, 8:30 am – 5:00 pm
April Spivack, University of North Carolina
at Charlotte; Martin Hodulak, Top Office
Management; Akikazu Kato, Mie University;
Chan Kee, Design-Environment Group
Architects LLP
For many corporations, business on a global scale
has become a necessity in order to expand or maintain
their competitiveness. In order to achieve the necessary
global presence, executives of these corporations are
challenged to establish and maintain international divisions outside of their own country.
These executives are faced with the paradox of maintaining their original corporate identity, while remaining
sensitive to the new cultural context in which they now
operate. However, this paradox is not always recognized
or respected by decision-makers, which can lead to a
cookie-cutter approach to physical space design across
national boundaries. Creating identical offices using
the same processes in each locale ignores the important
ways that national culture should influence the placedesign process and the ultimate physical form of office
environments.
The purpose of this intensive is to explore this
paradox of flexible consistency from the perspective of
the design of these international sites. To that end, we
will invite a series of speakers with a range of related
experiences to share their viewpoints. Participants will
include:
- Executives within multinational corporations, who
have to meet local employees’ expectations while
maintaining corporate values and routines.
- Practitioners working with these organizations who
have to adapt or find solutions to global models
when the “one design fits all” approach may not
work as planned.
- International or national consultancy firms who,
out of their own scope of work, have developed
approaches and methodologies to resolve national
culture-based conflicts.
4 In order to explore the most central issues, speakers
will represent different professional perspectives and
national cultures.
Presentation:
Design Challenges for Multinational Corporations,
Speaking from Asian Perspectives
When comparing offices for local organizations and
multinational corporations, the situation is quite similar
in Japan and Singapore. For local companies, the
average floor area is smaller, and fewer provisions for
privacy are provided. Along with discussing photos and
floor layout plans, three Mie University master’s degree
students will discuss their experiences in the United
States and Singapore during an internship abroad program in summer 2010.
Implementing Person-Centered
Design in Healthcare: Building
Connections
Wednesday, May 25, 8:30 am – 5:00 pm
Speakers include: Margaret Calkins, PhD, CAPS,
EDAC; Doug Erickson, FASHE, CHFM, HFDP;
Bonnie Mahon, RN, BSN, MSM; Jain Malkin,
CID, AAHID, EDAC; Robert Mayer, PhD; Kim
Montague, AIA, LEED AP; Ray Pentecost, DrPH,
FAIA, FACHA, LEED AP; Jude Rabig, RN, PhD;
and Jane Rohde, AIA, FIIDA, ACHA, AAHID
There has been a dramatic shift over the past several decades to include patients/residents/families as
active partners in care, across the full continuum of
care. However, insufficient attention has been directed
at the important role and positive impact of patient/
family/person-centered care, especially with respect to
the design and built environment of healthcare settings.
This session will highlight some of the ways in which
patient/family/person-centered care is being translated
throughout the creative design process across acute and
long-term healthcare settings to create very positive
outcomes. This intensive is made possible by the generous support of the Hulda B. and Maurice L. Rothschild
Foundation. Lunch will be provided for all attendees
and is sponsored by SAGE.
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
The Research Funders Speak:
Support for Applied and
Theoretical Research
Thursday, May 26, 9:00 am – 10:00 am
Speakers include: Stephanie Whitlock, Program
Officer, Graham Foundation; Gary Gumpert
PhD, President, Urban Communication
Foundation; Susan Drucker, PhD, Treasurer,
Urban Communication Foundation and Professor,
Department of Journalism/ Media Studies, School
of Communication, Hofstra University; Michael
F. Bloom, Sustainability and Green Buildings
Program Advisor, Office of Federal HighPerformance Green Buildings (MG), U.S. General
Services Administration
It would be wonderful to live in a world where all
worthy research projects are fully funded. Unfortunately, we don’t. Speakers at this session will share information and insights on obtaining funding for applied
and more theoretical research.
Design Practitioner
Professional Development:
Writing and Administering
Surveys
Friday, May 27, 8:00 am – 9:00 am
Speaker: Sally Augustin, PhD, Principal, Design
With Science
Show Me the Data: The
Essential Role of Research in
Making the Business Case for
Building a Better Hospital
Friday, May 27, 9:00 am – 10:00 am
Speaker: Sara O. Marberry, EVP and COO,
The Center for Health Design
At a time of enormous uncertainty and pressure on
healthcare leaders, an often overlooked opportunity is
the connection between the built environment of healthcare and its impact on safety and quality. The Center for
Health Design (CHD) has identified more than 1,200
studies that describe how the built environment can
reduce infections, falls, medication errors, unnecessary in-hospital transfers, and staff injuries. In addition,
economics is often a major barrier to incorporating cost
effective building design innovations in healthcare settings. Many healthcare leaders do not fully understand
the importance of balancing one time capital/construction costs with the multi-year operating cost savings
of a facility. They need and want data to support the
decisions they are making.
In this presentation, Sara Marberry will show how
data can be used to present the business case for better
building design. She will describe the Fable Hospital®,
a hypothetical facility that incorporated proven design
innovations based on actual research and achieved
remarkable results. Marberry will also share results
and examples from CHD’s Pebble Project® research
initiative, in which several pioneering hospitals have
used an evidence-based design process to incorporate
many design innovations into their projects. She’ll present several strategies for encouraging clients to fund
research and examples of creative funding approaches.
Design practitioners need a range of different information to do their jobs well, and often collect at least
some of it using surveys. Attend for realistic insights
and recommendations for writing and administering
those surveys.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
5
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Watching Closely:
Environmental Design Insights
Courtesy of the Lincoln Park
Zoo Gorillas
Friday, May 27, 1:30 pm – 3:00 pm
Speakers include: Christina Nippert-Eng PhD,
Associate Professor of Sociology, Acting Chair
Department of Social Sciences, Illinois Institute
of Technology; Sally Wong; Jim Hornor; Jason
Dominski; and Eugene Limb
We can learn a lot about places in which humans
thrive by studying the spaces in which gorillas flourish.
Sound far-fetched? You’ll be a believer by the end of
this session. Nippert-Eng will also discuss observational
research in a whole new way – observing gorillas leads
to loads of insights for improving how observational
research is done.
Design Practitioner Professional
Development: Post-Occupancy
Evaluations in the Trenches
Friday, May 27, 4:00 pm – 5:30 pm
Speaker: Greg Allen Barker, AIA
Post occupancy evaluation was initially recognized
as a formal process using the structure and methods of
environmental design research. This approach to POE
has become increasingly familiar to and accepted by
major institutions with both individual projects and
major building programs, such as the United States
Postal Service, The San Francisco Public Library, and
US General Services Administration. However, the
scope of effort to conduct such POEs tends to be more
costly than most capital development programs are able
to accommodate. This workshop reexamines the fundamental structure and methods used for POEs, taking
the United States Postal Service Level One POE as an
6 example of modest scope POEs, and seeks to define a
basic structure and set of research methods that can be
used by project managers and/or design professionals
without advanced research training for useful facility
evaluations at a level of effort commensurate with most
capital development projects.
Design Practitioner
Professional Development:
Ask the Research Experts
Saturday, May 28, 1:30 pm – 3:00 pm
Experts available include: Sally Augustin, PhD,
Principal, Design With Science; Ann Devlin, PhD,
Professor, Connecticut College; and Nick Watkins,
PhD, Director of Research, HOK
Are you a design practitioner facing a research challenge? If you are, bring your questions to this session
and ask the research experts for suggestions on resolving them.
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
CAREER AWARD WINNER
Galen Cranz, PhD, Professor, University of California,
Berkeley, is the 2011 recipient of the EDRA Career Award.
The accomplishments of Galen Cranz, PhD, in teaching, research, and design have inspired a generation
of students and professionals. Her ability to transcend
the boundaries of discipline, to cross from meticulous
intellectual work to applied research and design, and her
passion, integrity, and deep concern for the rightness of
all things and places we use embody what is best about
EDRA.
Her decades of teaching and lectures at U.C. Berkeley and at numerous other national and international
institutions and conferences have exposed individuals from many fields to the effect of the environment
on people. In over three decades of teaching, she has
served as a mentor and role model for students. Her survey classes in social factors in architecture at Berkeley
have touched thousands of students while her graduate
courses and advising have nurtured many others. Many
of these students are currently practicing informed design, teaching, and carrying out research across the U.S.
and internationally.
Dr. Cranz’s research stretches in scale from the human body to large urban parks, and her work has also
made social factors in design accessible to a wider
audience beyond the academy. Her book The Politics of
Park Design, A History of Urban Parks in America is a
staple reading for landscape architecture students across
the country.
More recently, her book The Chair won the 2004
EDRA Achievement Award and gained the attention
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
of the international media. For this book, Galen was
interviewed on NPR’s Fresh Air by Terry Gross as well
as on Australian and British radio. As the result of this
research, she also consulted in ergonomics with furniture and automobile manufacturers.
Additionally, although not formally trained as a designer, Dr. Cranz has transcended the role of researcher
and academic by participating in and winning design
competitions for urban parks. Perhaps most notable is
her collaboration with architect Bernard Tschumi which
was the winning entry for the competition for the design
of Parc de la Villette in Paris.
Dr. Cranz has played a significant role in advocating for assessment of designed spaces. For many years,
students in her Social Factors in Design class worked in
teams to conduct post-occupancy evaluations of buildings on the campus and in the community. This process
not only taught design students about the difference between design intention and actual use, but also specific
methods for collecting information: interviews, surveys,
as-built drawings, etc. At the 2010 EDRA conference,
she presented an assessment of this work and proposed a
very critical need to shift from evaluation to assessment.
It is due to this type of long-range view of the evolving nature of environmental design research as well as
her impressive accomplishments in teaching, research,
and applied design that we award Dr. Cranz the 2011
EDRA Career Award.
7
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
SERVICE AWARD WINNER
Janice Bissell, PhD, Principal, The Practitioner’s Resource,
is the 2011 recipient of the EDRA Service Award.
Janice Bissell, PhD has been a member of EDRA
since 2002 and served the organization as a board member from August 2007 through July of 2010. Janice’s
efforts go far beyond the service expected of board
members. During the past five years Janice has served
the organization through her tireless volunteer efforts to
organize, improve and professionalize EDRA’s annual
conference.
Janice served as Conference Chair of EDRA 38 in
Sacramento. As the single individual acting as conference chair, she secured conference sponsors, schedules,
tours, plenary speakers, worked with the hotel to ensure
that all details were addressed, and generally gave the
conference a professional atmosphere. During the next
year, Janice continued to work on conference related
issues and became the liaison between the EDRA office
and the Veracruz Conference Chair and committee for
EDRA39. In preparation for the EDRA 40 conference,
Janice continued to act as liaison between the EDRA
office and the Conference Committee, working on
improving conference quality. It is likely that EDRA
would not have been able to successfully carry on with
the EDRA 41 conference in Washington DC without
Janice’s critical involvement.
It is a testament to Janice’s commitment and service
to EDRA that over the past five years she acted in her
role as conference liaison/event organizer in a volunteer
capacity. She committed to increasing the professional-
8 ism of EDRA’s main public event. She stepped in to assist local conference chairs and depending on the abilities of each did whatever was needed to ensure that the
annual conference was worry-free for EDRA members
Additionally, Janice’s presence on the board strengthened design practitioner representation within EDRA.
This representation led to new and revised EDRA
policies and increased revenues from additional sponsorships and membership. Efforts with the NAED and
other initiatives demonstrated a clear and unwavering
vision to bridge gulfs among research, practice, and
policy.
Much of Janice’s efforts were clearly driven by a sincere love for EDRA and what it represents. Such devotion was evident when she assumed responsibility over
the EDRA business office following Janet Singer’s passing. Janice shouldered several early morning and late
nights. During these odd hours she not only assumed
conference organization responsibilities, but reviewed
boxes of paperwork to prepare EDRA for a transition to
its current Washington, D.C., office.
Janice’s stellar track record in conference organization and management and her passion for EDRA as
an organization makes her a unique and outstanding
member of this organization. Janice’s commitment to
EDRA and its mission makes her the recipient of the
2011 EDRA Service Award.
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
Refereed
Full Papers
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
9
Invited Papers & Special Presentations
10 Refereed Full-Papers
How the Logic of Research
Misunderstands the Logic
of Design
Martin Brösamle (University of Freiburg,
Germany), Magda Mavridou (University
College London, UK), Christoph Hölscher
(University of Freiburg, Germany)
This paper was presented at EDRA41 in Washington, D.C.
Abstract
Evidence-based design aims to optimize design solutions based on empirical evidence, scientific research,
and other available information. Candidate solutions
are explored and evaluated according to criteria and
principles derived from various fields of research and
scientific investigation.
We present the optimization of a conference centre with respect to wayfinding complexity: Based on
empirical evidence (wayfinding experiments), formal
analysis (Space Syntax), and architectural expertise,
the existing building is systematically modified. The
variations will later be built in a virtual version of the
conference centre such that the effectiveness of the new
solutions can be tested empirically.
The authors analyze their own research process with
respect to the dependencies between the three ostensibly independent components: analysis of the status-quo,
development of design interventions, and finally the
empirical evaluation of the resulting design variants.
Having a closer look at the re-design process itself
and how it draws on the previous scientific results
reveals that the preceding analyses strongly influence
the exploration of new design solutions. While this is in
line with the idea of evidence-based design, nevertheless it indicates an inter-dependency between research
methods and design. In the light of the restricted-ness of
available research methods this demonstrates a narrowing factor in the production of design solutions.
More striking is the finding that the availability of
evaluation methods projects back on the design generation; naively, the evaluation follows the design generation and should therefore not influence it. The interwoven-ness of the three components in the evidence-based
design process have strong methodological implications: The methods of analysis as well as evaluation
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
techniques have a strong impact on resulting design
solutions. It is therefore necessary to carefully coordinate research methods and design intention.
1 Introduction
Wayfinding and navigation issues have been discussed in different disciplinary fields such as Architecture, Environmental Design as well as Behavioral and
Cognitive Science. (Evans & McCoy 1998; Passini
1996)
Weisman (1981) is one of the first to formally address environmental factors influencing the navigability of built environments. Our own research aims at
the systematic investigation of architectural properties
that shape human wayfinding cognition in architecture: Hölscher, Meilinger, Brösamle and Knauff (2006)
asked participants to find six locations in a complex,
multi-level conference centre, located in Günne am
Möhnesee, Germany. (Fig 1) Based on wayfinding
FIGURE 1: The Conference Centre in Günne,
Lake Möhne, Germany.
11
Refereed Full-Papers
behaviour, debriefing interviews, and informal architectural analysis Hölscher et al. (2006) identify a number
of architectural properties they consider responsible
for the navigation difficulties. Brösamle, Vrachliotis
and Hölscher (2007) provide a formal analysis of the
architecture of the conference centre based on Space
Syntax methods (Bafna 2003). They relate characteristic configurational properties to each of the previously
identified usability issues.
The present paper discusses a number of options
for re-designing the existing conference centre, with
an emphasis on usability optimization. Following an
evidence-based design scheme, development of design
variations will consider empirical results and architectural analysis. Candidate solutions will then be evaluated in order to put the intended improvements under
empirical test. From an idealistic scientific point of
view, we are looking for a universal test criterion, which
distinguishes between good and bad design solutions –
similar to a litmus test. Furthermore, we not only intend
to recognize good design solutions, but we also want to
understand how architectural design succeeds or fails
to produce them: Which design principles are capable
of producing good designs? Knowing which approach
leads to good (or bad) design potentially reveals crucial
ingredients of good architecture.
As it turns out, the ideal of a universal evaluation
criterion for all candidate designs contradicts the intrinsic properties of design: A litmus-test type of evaluation is based on a simple test based on the same (few)
variables. By contrast, design problems typically are
characterized by their manifold conceptions resulting
in different and large sets of variables. (Goel & Pirolli
1992) Different design solutions follow from different
conceptions of the matter, which in turn influences the
criteria of evaluation.
But even in laboratory science, where well-defined
criteria appear to be possible, established concepts,
procedures, and instruments implicitly influence and
constrain scientific investigation. Ludwik Fleck (1980,
orig. 1935)1 identifies implicit constraints in the laboratory-centred development of the pathological entity
syphilis. His argument draws on social mechanisms in
groups of researchers: Scientific procedures, such as
laboratory tests in immunology, co-evolve with pre-
dominant thought styles (‘Denkstil’) in a though collective (‘Denkkollektiv’).
The present paper makes two points: First, design
variations of the Günne conference centre and the
underlying design rationale will be presented. Second,
the context of their production and evaluation will be
considered as a factor influencing the design outcome
as well as the overall scientific process. The production
and investigation of design variations is constrained
by considered analyses and empirical data as well as
available evaluation methods; just as the conception
of syphilis was implicitly constrained by procedures,
instruments, and collective conceptions. We will show
a ‘backward’ influence from evaluation-related methodological arguments on the actual productive design
decisions.
2 Methods
First, a brief review of the architectural analyses of
the existing conference centre and wayfinding-related
empirical evidence will be given. Second, the corresponding systematic design interventions developed by
the three authors are presented. A set of systematic and
theoretically motivated variations of the existing conference centre was to be developed. These design variants should then be evaluated in order to reveal design
principles capable of producing well-navigable design
solutions.
In order to show the implications of scientific methods on the design outcome, we make our own research
activity to an object of investigation itself. The present
study re-constructs the course of arguments between
the three authors while developing the design variants
and the evaluation study. Sessions were structured as
expert interviews – one Architect taking the role of an
interviewed expert while two Cognitive Scientists acted
as interviewers. While the Architect was responsible for
actually producing design modifications, critical questions from the interviewers ensured the explication of
underlying design rationale. At a later stage, the scheme
was shifted from the strict interviewer-informer-type
of dialogue towards a collaborative discussion. All sessions where video recorded such that verbal utterances
could be transcribed as well as gestures and drawing
marks be reconstructed. The data would allows for
1
Thomas Kuhn would later mention the works of Ludwig Fleck as an important influence on his own ‘Structure of Scientific Revolutions’. He
would give the concept thought style a much stronger twist and call it a ‘paradigm’.
2
Double quotes “ ” indicate direct citation whereas single quotes ‘ ’ indicate special words, informal expressions or the like.
12 Refereed Full-Papers
further, more formal analyses (Suwa, Purcell & Gero
1998; Brösamle & Hölscher 2008) which is however
not necessary for the point to be made here.
Discourse is reconstructed along the interview
transcript, where direct citations represent crucial turns
in the argumentation between Architecture (A) and Science (S or S1/S2).2
3 Development of Design Variants
Review of Status Quo Analsyes:
There are two main studies dealing with the conference centre and its usability issues in the present form.
As already mentioned, Hölscher et al. (2006) address
wayfinding difficulties directly by asking participants to
find targets in the building and to explain their impressions of the building in debriefing interviews. Brösamle
et al. (2007) provide a formal account of the architectural structure by quantifying configurational aspects and
making visibility relations explicit. The key components
of these two studies is user behaviour (and cognition)
on the one hand and spatial structure on the other. Most
usability issues are comprehensible under the concepts
provided by these studies, such as the lack of visibility
of navigation options or staircases, dead ends in circulation relevant areas and the like.
Less obvious is the phenomenon that the building
seems to fail to communicate a clear main staircase.
Related evidence comes from the debriefing interviews
of the 2006 study, where users complained about the
absence of a clear main staircase. In the 2007 study this
finding is reflected in the results from Visibility Graph
Analyses (Turner et al. 2001) which analyse each floor
separately. (Fig 2) In the lower floors the foyer stairs
(left in the figure) is more integrated 3 than the stairs
close to the living quarters (right in the figure). By contrast, in the first and second floor the pattern switches
such that the living quarter stairs is the more integrated
one. Brösamle et al. (2007) conclude that the role of the
staircase is different in different floors, namely, for the
upper floors the living quarter stairs is most important
while the foyer stairs is most important in the two lower
floors. This makes plausible that users have difficulties
in deciding for one main staircase.
4 Exploration of design solutions
Layout-centred aspects:
The design interventions discussed in the very begin-
3
ning of the sessions all follow most directly from the
previous analyses, namely to improve the visibility
in the entrance area and make the adjoining staircase
directly visible from the entrance hall. Other early solutions are based on the non-congruence of the different floors. They either intent to make the ground floor
similar to the basement such that the mental integration
between floors can be improved (Soeda, Kushiyama,
and Ohno 1997) or try to reduce the basement to a minimum of complexity. (cf. Brösamle & Hölscher 2007)
FIGURE 2: Integration Values of two Staircases. The
Values are based on separate analyses for each
floor. While in the lower floor the staircase in the
Foyer is more integrated whereas in the upper floors
the pattern switches such that the Stairs in the Living
Quarters is more integrated.
‘Integration’ is a Space Syntax measure reflecting the centrality of a space in relation to the overall system (building, city). (cf. Bafna, 2003)
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
13
Refereed Full-Papers
Independent of how complex or simple these layout
oriented interventions are, they can be characterised
as closely related to the spatial structure of the building. The underlying design rationale is based on layout,
configuration and visibility. An other instance of such
layout-driven interventions widens corridors, moves
staircases towards more visible locations, straightens
zig-zag-turns and so on. The underlying reasoning is
always about making crucial parts easily visible from
key points, and to provide overview in important areas.
(Fig 4)
A much more “invasive” solution, which directly
addresses the main staircase issue, puts an atrium in the
foyer. (Fig 5) The staircase would become easily recognizable as the central staircase. At the same time, the
upper floors are well connected with the ground floor
entrance area.
An alternative to the atrium makes the staircases easily recognizable by straightening the corridor in the first
and second floor. Similarly, the corridor could be reorganized into an L-shape instead of the zigzag-turns.
The recognition of each stairs would be facilitated by
increasing overall visibility in the corridor layout.
While the latter three solutions are the more elaborate instances in the layout-based category, there is an
other intervention which is, in principle, the most direct
FIGURE 3: The design variant ‚congruent floors’
makes the floorplan layout match between different
levels.
FIGURE 4: This design variant is primarily based on
layout-based, ‘syntactical’ interventions. E.g. corridors
are widened and staircase locations are changed,
such that they improve visual access.
14 Refereed Full-Papers
cure of the identified integration pattern in the separate
floor analysis (Fig 2). The difference in the integration
pattern between the two upper floors and the two lower
floors was seen as resulting from an “in-balance” in the
building. To re-balance the upper floors, a connection
between the lecture room part and the living quarters in
the first floor was suggested (Fig 3):
S: “So one of the reasons why we naively thought of
making this connection upstairs on the first floor was
to give it more balance.”
It is not surprising, that this solution was suggested
by the Scientists party (S), which also made the related
analysis.
Functional aspects:
S: “You were not so fond of the idea of making a
connection upstairs?”
A: “No. I think with the existing layout, it wouldn’t
help that much. It would help, but not that it would
make a big difference. It would make more sense
and much more difference if the functions were
shifted, especially if the offices were upstairs and
the lecture rooms downstairs, so there’s no need of
connection because this [the offices] would work
totally independently, function-wise.”
As we can see, the Architecture party (A) suggested
to re-arrange functional areas instead of making connections between “two spaces [that] have different
function“. The underlying design rationale is no longer
primarily layout driven but emphasizes function as a
second aspect:
A: “It’s a relation of the function and the layout of the
building. It should not be looked at only as a layout,
but because it has a function as well, the function
should be taken into account rather than – the
function can help the mapping of the layout.”
The previous analyses were based on Space Syntax
methods and Visibility Graph Analysis (VGA) and thus
triggered a spatial and configuration-centred approach
to the problem. The problem was identified syntactically and the attempt to solve it was also syntactical.
What is expressed in the above citation is an approach
which re-conceptualizes the problem as optimizing the
correspondence between spatial and functional characteristics.
With respect to usability, the previous idea of “how
[…] [to] assign specific functions to layout” has a cognitive component to it:
A: “[People ] are not all equal as users […] it’s even
good to have spaces which are not equal and are
not grasped by everyone the same way, so you want
to have different understandings of the building by
different users.”
The concept of “visitability” nicely condenses the
troika of functional, spatial and user group aspects. The
function-related solution of the main-staircase-issue is
clearly distinguished from the layout-related solutions:
it needs no changes of layout at all.
FIGURE 5: The design variant introduces an atrium
into the intrance area.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
5 Evaluate Design Solutions
Following from the overall challenge – to produce
15
Refereed Full-Papers
navigation-friendly designs – a reasonable criterion of
evaluation would be whether or not people are able
to grasp how the circulation works for their purposes.
This criterion is universal in the sense that it can be
applied to every existing building design by asking the
users. However this is not a practicable solution for
planning, which takes place before there are users who
could be asked. Ideally, solutions could be tested in
beforehand.
For the floor plan equalizing solution both parties
agreed that it was a much better solution than the
original – under a pure layout point of view. Also, there
was little doubt that the improvement would easily be
demonstrated by controlled wayfinding experiments in
virtual reality.
S: “If you think of […] the hotspots that we have tried
to describe in our previous paper [Hölscher et al.
(2006)], none of them have anything to do with
semantics. They were all very much about the
geometry of surveyed places, and staircases and
connectivity and stuff. […] I mean, all that would
be interventions that should reflect well in a way
finding experiment […]”
A: “I think it would be important to differentiate
the tasks […] based on the layout and based on
function. […] or you can simply say that we’re
testing only the layout and we don’t care about the
function […] You do solutions which improve the
layout.”
S: “But we cannot really test this. I mean, okay, we
can test it without giving any semantic tasks to the
people.”
At the same time, layout interventions would have to
be tested with tasks that do not refer to functions in the
building. This is especially true for the labels of target
locations, as was expressed in the former interview
passage. Functional and semantic aspects are degraded
as disturbing factors, which would have to be controlled
for (rather than to be investigated). For the testing of
pure layout variations, the necessity to “control for
function” by using “artificial arbitrary landmarks for
marking destination points” or even to “strip off” the
“functionality of the building” is raised.
By contrast, an attempt to develop an experiment
to test solutions with respect to functional criteria was
only sketched but never developed in detail.
16 A: “[…] you would assign your functions according to
the level of visitability you wanted to have.”
S: “Which means we cannot easily test this in VR
[virtual reality].”
A: “You can test the layout, but if there are no
functions, then you don’t –”
S: “So we don’t test the actual building, how it is
working in place, but we are only testing parts of
it?”
A:“Yes.”
While the syntactic solutions were considered as easily testable, by contrast, the functional aspects appear to
be hard to test under laboratory conditions. This marks
the aspectual bifurcation not only in the rationale of
experimental testing but also in the design rationale. In
other words, layout-based design interventions reveal
their strengths under different testing conditions than
function-oriented design interventions. Here, the interdependency of evaluation methods and design production – which contradicts the analytic idea of a universal
‘litmus test evaluation’ – becomes most obvious.
To elaborate on this a little further, it is at odds
with the very fundamental idea of analytic science:
To find simple elementary explanations, processes or
mechanisms to account for complex phenomena. This
thinking-style (‘Denkstil’, Fleck 1980) has a hard time
in analyzing the essence of design, which is holistic by
nature. Architecture emphasizes the inseparability of the
functional aspects from the theme of navigability:
A: “navigation and way-finding is not simply about
layout or simply about function, but it’s about both
these things.”
This last argument points to the difficulties which
may arise when investigating a phenomenon as complex as Architecture whilst founding this investigation
on a theory as reductionist as the symbol processing
model of Cognitive Science. Goel (1995) expresses a
similar concern regarding the application of classical
processing models in design problem solving research.
The incompatibilities between research paradigms
finally propagate back to the design process under
investigation: Methodological arguments on empirical testability eventually influence the final decision in
favour of layout-based design variations, but against
functionally motivated variations. The logic of the laboratory has re-directed design activity and scientific in-
Refereed Full-Papers
vestigation: ‘What constitutes a good design solution?’
was – at least partly – re-formulated as ‘Which aspects
of good design can be investigated under controlled
conditions?’.
6 Discussion and Outlook
The self-investigation presented here shows that the
introduction of scientific methods into the design process may result in un-anticipated constraints or biases.
Environmental design research as well as evidencebased design has to consider the formative influence
that analytic techniques, scientific concepts, and evaluation methods may have on the overall process.
The conflict between analytic science and productive, “holistic” design points to the question how design
solutions should be put under investigation. Especially,
it is an open question whether laboratories of empirical
sciences are the right tool to inform practical design.
The impulse to reduce a whole design solution solely to
layout illustrates the inherent difficulties in the methodology. For example, in controlled navigation studies, it
is common practice to use arbitrary landmarks (Janzen
2006), which was introduced in the above discussion to
“strip off” the semantics of a building.
To conclude, available tools, research methodology,
problem conception, thought style (Fleck), and choice
of evaluation criteria have a strong impact on the course
of scientific and design processes and thus influence the
overall outcome.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by the SFB/TR8 Spatial
Cognition of the German Research Council (DFG).
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
References
Bafna, Sonit. 2003. “Space syntax: A brief introduction to its
logic and analytical techniques.” Environment and Behavior
35(1), 17-29.
Brösamle, Martin, Christoph Hölscher, and Georg Vrachliotis.
2007. “Multi-Level complexity in terms of Space Syntax.”
In Proceedings, 6th International Space Syntax Symposium,
Istanbul: Istanbul Technical University.
Brösamle, Martin and Christoph Hölscher. 2008. “The Architects’
Understanding of Human Navigation.” In Movement and
Orientation in Built Environments: Evaluating Design
Rationale and User Cognition, Report Series of the
Transregional Collaborative Research Center SFB/TR 8
Spatial Cognition, edited by Saif Haq, Christoph Hölscher,
and Sue Torgrude. Bremen/Freiburg, Germany: University of
Bremen / University of Freiburg.
Brösamle, Martin, and Christoph Hölscher. 2007. “Architects
seeing through the eyes of building users.” In Spatial
cognition in architectural design, edited by T. Barkowsky, Z.
Bilda, C. Hölscher, and G. Vrachliotis, Melbourne, Australia.
http://www.sfbtr8.spatial-cognition.de/SCAD/.
Evans, G.W., and J.M. McCoy. 1998. When buildings don’t
work: The role of architecture in human health. Journal of
Environmental Psychology 18, 85-94.
Fleck, Ludwik. 1980. Entstehung und Entwicklung einer
wissenschaftlichen Tatsache. Frankfurt, Suhrkamp. Orig.
Benno & Schwabe, 1935.
Goel, V and P. Pirolli. 1992. The structure of design problem
space. Cognitive Science 16, 395-429.
Goel Vinod. 1995. Sketches of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Hölscher, Christoph, Tobials Meilinger, Georg Vrachliotis,
Martin Brösamle, and Markus Knauff. 2006. Up the Down
Staircase: Wayfinding Strategies and Multi-Level Buildings.
Journal of Environmental Psychology 26(4), 284-299.
Janzen, G. 2006. Memory for object location and route direction
in virtual large-scale space. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology 59, 493-508.
Passini, R, 1996. Wayfinding design: Logic, application and
some thoughts on universality. Design Studies 17, 319-331.
Turner, A., M. Doxa, D. O’Sullivan, A. Penn. 2001. From
Isovists to Visibility Graphs: A Methodology for the Analysis
of Architectural Space. Environment & Planning B: Planning
and Design 28, 103-121.
Soeda, M., N. Kushiyama, and R. Ohno. 1997. “Wayfinding in
cases with vertical Motion.” In Proceedings of MERA 97:
Intern. Conference on Environment-Behavior Studies, 559564.
Suwa, M., T. Purcell, and J. Gero J. 1998. Macroscopic analysis
of design processes based on a scheme for coding designer’s
cognitive actions. Design Studies 19(4), October 1998.
Weisman, J. 1981. Evaluating architectureal legibility.
Environment and Behavior 13 (2), 189-204.
17
Refereed Full-Papers
Public Perception of
Walkability: Assessing
Perception Dependency on
Scene Attributes and
Respondent Characteristics
Claudia Bernasconi (University of Detroit
Mercy), Alan Hoback (University of Detroit
Mercy)
Abstract
This study investigated public perception of a walking environment in Detroit, Michigan, utilizing visual
evaluation techniques in order to establish correlations
between perceptions of the general public and characteristics of the street environment. To accomplish this
goal, we asked the public to rate pictures portraying
existing urban scenes through a five-category descriptor
of walkability (aesthetic quality, orientation, comfort,
safety, and security). We employed landscape evaluation techniques shifting the target from the natural landscape to the urban landscape. Results indicated which
attributes of the walking environment affected walkability perception. Vegetation, enclosure, rhythm, texture
diversity, and sidewalk quality were positively correlated with walkability perception, while amount of sky
was negatively correlated with the walkability index
(WI). This study suggests the existence of a dichotomy
between urban perception modes and natural landscape
ones, where the first gravitate around enclosure perception and the latter around perception of openness. While
assessing which respondents’ characteristics significantly influenced walkability perception, we detected
familiarity and education levels as significant factors.
Our findings also indicate that both site familiarity
and walking ease affect ranking of the five walkability
characteristics, with familiar people less concerned
with safety and people with lower walking ease more
concerned with comfort. Results do not reflect the complete range of influences of attributes on preferences,
and limit our understanding of which particular attribute
contributes the most to the WI of each scene. Such limitations are to be expected given the diverse multilayered
character of the urban environment.
18 Introduction
Increased dependency on vehicular transportation
has given rise to a wide range of issues in the domain
of social interaction and human health, affecting whole
communities in the great majority of American towns
and suburban areas. Cities do not always provide inhabitants with adequate pedestrian facilities or adequate
pedestrian links to local attractions and businesses.
The acknowledgement of a general low quality level of
the walking environment has led to increased research
in this field. The linkages between physical environment and walking habits have been investigated from a
variety of perspectives for several decades (e.g., Cullen,
1969, Lynch, 1960, Kaplan, 1985; Furin, J. 1971). The
abundant research in the planning, design, behavioral,
and health fields confirms the crucial role of pedestrian
transportation research in contemporary automobiledependent societies. Extensive research has been conducted striving toward the definition of levels of service
(LOS) of pedestrian facilities (e.g., Gallin, N. 2001;
Landis, et al., 2001), with a primary focus on safety
assessment. Less research has been directed towards the
understanding of the walking environment as a comprehensive space constructed by a variety of interrelated
factors, which go beyond safety, security, and continuity
of the paved sidewalk (e.g., Reid, 2008; Sarkar, 1993;
Spooner, 2007). Most recent research has emphasized a
connection between health factors and planning considerations through the employment of interdisciplinary
approaches (e.g., Chanam and Moudon, 2004; Saelens,
et al., 2003).
This study investigated public perception of a walking environment in Detroit, Michigan, utilizing visual
evaluation techniques in order to establish correlations between perceptions of the general public and
characteristics of the street environment. Our goal in
this study was to understand the perceived walkability
of pedestrian transportation along a main urban corridor, with particular attention to walkability perception
dependency on visible elements of the urban scene. We
sought to separate and measure the various components
that determine the cumulative experience and understand which elements had a greater influence on perception. To accomplish this goal, we asked the public to
rate pictures portraying existing urban scenes through a
five-category descriptor of walkability. We employed
landscape evaluation techniques shifting the target
from the natural landscape to the urban landscape. As
indicated by Palmer (2003), the broadening of the field
Refereed Full-Papers
of landscape studies is desirable for the advancement of
landscape perception research.
The overarching goal of this research project was to
develop and test new approaches to walkability assessment based on landscape evaluation techniques.
More specifically, goals of this investigation were to: 1)
understand public perception of walkability for urban
scenes along a main transportation corridor, 2) assess
scene attributes (environmental design attributes and
picture attributes) that have significant relationship with
public perception of walkability, 3) assess significant
relationships between respondents’ characteristics and
perception of walkability, and 4) offer suggestions for
future planning of walking environments.
Background
Our study referred to the concept of walkability, intended as the ability of the built environment to promote
and support walking (Jacobs, 1995; Spooner, 2007).
Walkability has been investigated through a variety of
Figure 1. Initial Study of the Area
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
19
Refereed Full-Papers
approaches. Several scholars studied walking environments through a multidisciplinary perspective linking
health, behavior, and physical environment (e.g., King
et al., 2002; Frank and Engelke, 2001). A Levels of
Service approach (LOS) has often been utilized in
transportation research (e.g., Sarkar, 1995b). As seen
in Ferreira and Sanchez (1998), this method includes
on-site evaluations and is aimed to capture ranking and
user perception of safety, security, comfort, continuity,
and visual and psychological attractiveness of walking
environments. Other studies referred to a broader range
of walkability descriptors, including safety, security,
convenience and comfort, continuity, system coherence,
and attractiveness (Sarkar 1993).
We were interested in defining and testing a new
walkability assessment methodology that could be
administered to a large population sample off-site, and
would combine the transportation research approach
(e.g., LOS) with a visual aesthetic understanding of
the overall urban environment. Design theory suggests
that all considerations related to the design of any built
structure are highly dependent on the location and the
context, as well as the specific shape of the structure
itself (Appleyard et al., 1964; Holgate, 1992; Simonds
and Starke, 2006). In addition, numerous authors have
indicated the need for evaluating design in connection
with people’s experience and perception of the overall
urban environment (e.g., Lynch, 1960; Cullen, 1961;
Kaplan, 1985; Bosselman, 1998; Kido, 2005).
Our study follows the perception-based approaches,
which emphasize the human dimension of values
connected to the natural and urban landscape (Dan-
iel, 2001). Extensive research has been conducted to
improve the understanding of the aesthetic qualities of
landscapes (Litton, 1968; McKenzie and McKenzie,
1980; Schuttleworth, 1980; Palmer, 1983; Smardon et
al., 1986; Coeterier, 1996; Priestley and Evans, 1996;
Schmid, 2001; Arriaza et al., 2004; Wong, 2005).
Most assessment tools and techniques have examined
preferences for natural landscapes more often than
urban landscapes and man-made structures. Numerous
studies support the idea that specific characteristics of
an urban environment can significantly impact perception of walking environments and affect behavior
(Hieronymus, et. al, 2009; Naderi, 2003; Zacharias,
2001, Lavery et al., 1996). Scholars advocate a better
integration of quantitative and qualitative elements in
the assessment of the walking environment (Reid, 2008)
and indicate that subjective perceptions of the walking
experience should be included in urban design studies
(Brown et al., 2007). While scholars have investigated
relationships between environmental attributes of the
urban environment and behavior (e.g., Naderi, 2003;
Wolf, 2004), to the knowledge of the authors there is no
comprehensive study on walkability that attempted to
correlate urban environment characteristics to walkability perception from an environmental aesthetic perspective with the use of landscape evaluation techniques.
New landscape evaluation tools and techniques have
been developed and tested during the last 20 years (e.g.,
Hull and Revell, 1989; Buhyoff et al., 1994;Wherrett,
2000; Perez, 2002; Arriaza et al., 2004; Wong and
Domroes, 2005; Rogge et al., 2007). There is a vast and
fragmented pool of options when approaching environ-
Table 1. Attributes and Picture Categorization
20 Refereed Full-Papers
Table 2.
Figure 2. Survey Panel
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
21
Refereed Full-Papers
mental aesthetics. Landscape evaluation techniques can
be divided into two main approaches: one based upon
an objectivist or physical paradigm, the other based
upon a subjectivist or psychological paradigm (Lothian, 1999; Daniel, 2001). The first group relies on the
opinions and judgments of experts, considering quality as an inherent characteristic of the object viewed
and evaluating the landscape through abstract design
parameters (Daniel, 2001). The second group of studies
relies on perception-based assessments or ratings from
the general public gathered through written and photographic surveys. This group includes the psychophysical, the cognitive or psychological approach, and the
experiential or phenomenological approach.
The method employed in this study for the assessment of the urban landscape quality is referred to as
psychophysical. This method achieves a compromise
between expert-based and perception-based methods,
combining public perception surveys and statistical
analysis of the data gathered in order to identify visual
components determining the public perception (Schafer
et al., 1969; Daniel, 2001; Arriaza et al., 2004). Surveys
are utilized to gather public preferences for scenes portrayed in photographs.
In our study, we combined the identification of
walkability descriptors (which we called walkability
characteristics), as suggested by transportation research
Table 3a. Respondent Characteristics
22 approaches (e.g., Ferreira and Sanchez, 1998), and the
landscape evaluation approach, in which subjective
preferences for scenes are connected to specific visible elements of the selected scenes (e.g., Arriaza, et.al,
2004). The use of photographs to elicit scene evaluations has been successfully accomplished and tested
by numerous studies, which include the comparison
of results of photo-based and on-site ratings (Schafer
and Brush, 1977; Schuttleworth, 1980; Law and Zube,
1983; Stewart et al., 1984; Hull and Stewart, 1992;
Stamps, 1990; Bernaldez et al., 1998).
Study Area
Our study focused on the walking environment along
a primary urban corridor, Woodward Avenue, also
called M1, in Detroit, Michigan. The corridor connects
the heart of downtown Detroit to major city districts,
such as Midtown and New Center, and to a series of
suburban centers sprawling north from the city. The
Woodward corridor is characterized by a variety of
urban landscapes with different degrees of urban or
suburban vitality, ranging from high-rise developments
and commercial and business districts to ample vacant
or abandoned lots and small single-family residential
units. A proposed light rail system along Woodward
Avenue is close to implementation. The implications for
the revitalization of all decaying urban and suburban
Refereed Full-Papers
areas, in the light of this new infrastructural design, are
great and spur investigations on use, perception, and
environmental characteristics of the corridor. Furthermore, recent research has indicated that use of light rail
is more likely to happen in areas that are felt as more
walkable (Werner et al., 2010). In our investigation,
we concentrated on the New Center area, a major city
district in proximity to the existing Amtrak rail station,
where a new intermodal station will be built to connect
the existing rail system and new light rail transit.
Methodology
The methodological framework consisted of the:
1) study and mapping of the walking environment, 2)
picture acquisition, 3) categorization of pictures for environmental design and picture attributes, 4) definition
of walkability characteristics, 5) preference solicitation,
6) statistical analysis, and 7) results and implications.
In order to refer to the psychophysical method, we
mapped typologies of walking environments in the
area, based on overall spatial structure (scale, vertical
relationships, openness), the materiality and textures
(types of building materials, maintenance levels) and
entourage (people, vegetation, vehicles). Because the
psychophysical method requires that the vantage points
(i.e., picture-taking locations and viewsheds) be representative of conditions found in the area (Schroeder and
Daniel, 1981; Hull et al., 1987; Hull and Revell, 1989)
we derived the site selection for picture sampling from
the study of area and the larger urban context. We did
not consider utilizing a grid cell or a route sampling
method, as the use of randomly determined distances
has been found to often result into meaningless pictures
(Rogge et al., 2007), while flexibility in view selection
has been suggested to better represent how the observer
experiences and views the landscape (Buhyoff et al.,
1986). We used some flexibility in choosing the exact
spot to take the picture to avoid obstructing elements,
such as trucks, light poles, and other elements in the
foreground, as suggested in Rogge et al. (2007). We
captured the pictures in a landscape format consistently
with previous studies (Hull and Stewart, 1992; Wherrett, 2000; Rogge et al., 2007). While little is known of
the influence of picture format on perceptions, authors
suggest that the landscape view better represents the
human visual process, given that the binocular human
visual field can be approximated as a horizontal oval
shape (Webb, 1964; Sheppard, 1989; Franz, 2005). All
pictures were taken at eye level with consistent orientation and without any vertical tilting of the camera. The
photographic survey was conducted over two days in
June with very similar light conditions. A schematic
representation of the initial study is shown in Figure 1.
We selected nine scenes to portray the range of walking
environment typologies in the study area.
We utilized both visible physical attributes of scenes
and formal features (such as cognitive-related attributes), as suggested by expert and perception-based
assessments (Daniel, 2001). The elements present in
each image were identified as environmental design
attributes (i.e., design characteristics of the walking environment) and picture attributes (i.e., general aesthetic
environmental components of the scenes mainly dependent on the time and location of picture taking) (Table
1). Environmental design attributes included: spatial
enclosure (buildings on the two sides of the street,
building on one side of the street, no buildings along
the street); horizontal plane characteristics (hard surface
or mixed hard surface and grass); rhythm (repetition of
vertical elements on vertical planes in the foreground);
Table 4. Walkability Index (average score)
Table 5. Best and worst scenes
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
23
Refereed Full-Papers
color; texture diversity; sidewalk quality; sidewalk materiality; street furniture; vegetation (amount and quality); and proximity of crossroads/intersections (based
on visibility in the scene). Picture attributes included:
amount of people; amount of sky; color of sky; street
orientation (street on right or left of the scene); amount
of vehicles; and amount of shade (casted shade).
The variables chosen were based on the study of the
area and on a review of studies of natural landscapes
(McKenzie and McKenzie, 1980; Hammitt et al., 1994;
Arriaza et al., 2004; Rogge et al., 2007), and urban
landscapes (Appleyard, 1981; Anderson and Schroeder,
1983; Horton and Reynolds, 1971; Wong and Domroes,
2005; Spooner, 2007). The visual components were
evaluated using categorical codes as shown in Table
1. We used categories to capture major distinctions for
each attribute. For example, we noted no vegetation
or minimal amount (value = 0), areas containing some
grass or few trees (value = 1), or areas with grass and
trees/shrubs or extensive grass (value = 2), as suggested
by Arriaza et al. (2004). We used horizontal plan type
to measure the amount of impermeable and permeable surface, as suggested by Anderson and Schroeder
(1983). We chose to include amount of vegetation as
indicated by several scholars (e.g., Rogge et al., 2007;
Hertzog et al., 2000; Coeterier, 1996), but we combined amount and quality, as overgrown vegetation on
a pedestrian pathway might counteract positive effects
of vegetation confirmed by several scholars (e.g., Wolf,
2004). We chose to include the amount and color of
sky, as both determine a very diverse appearance of the
scenes, as suggested by several studies (e.g., Wong and
Domroes, 2005; Hammitt et al., 1994; Anderson and
Schroeder, 1983). This attribute is highly dependent on
ephemeral factors such as atmospheric conditions and
on the relationship of the viewshed with the direction
of the sunrays. The shadows attribute is strongly connected to the urban character of the scenes, and takes
into account the percentage of dark areas in the picture.
Landis et al., 2001, lists shade as one of the elements
shaping pedestrians’ experience of street environments.
The level of vehicular congestion (i.e., number of cars)
was also included. Appleyard (1981) suggests higher
traffic levels contribute to fewer on-street activities.
The number of buildings was also included and was
measured through the overall texture variability in the
scene (i.e., buildings in the different grounds of the
image were treated as fields of texture). Several studies have linked buildings and vehicles with differences
in levels of preference for scenes (e.g., Anderson and
24 Table 6. Correlation between attributes and
Walkability Index
Schroeder, 1983). Amount of people in the scene was
included as an attribute, as suggested by Zaccharias
(1997b). Van den Berg et al., 1998 and Rogge et al,
2007 indicate that maintenance is a significant factor in
perception; therefore sidewalk quality was included. We
also included two attributes that do not refer to specific
physical elements of the scene, but rather the relationships that occur among more elements: enclosure and
rhythm. Several studies indicate that iterative-dynamic
perception, depth perception, and serial vision are key
components of perception of urban environments (e.g.,
Smardon et al., 1986; Cullen, 1961). The attribute
rhythm captures the emphasis on depth in the image
that occurs with the repetition of vertical elements, such
as posts, shop window partitions, etc. Rhythm, as well
as texture, is related to the level of detail in architecture,
as its perception requires visible architectural elements
dividing and characterizing a surface. Several scholars
indicate the importance of detail in the perception of
street environments (e.g., Loidl, et al., 2003; Hedman,
1984; Curran, 1983; Dee, 2001). Finally, enclosure has
also been identified as an important component of urban
environments (e.g., Cullen, 1961; Schultz, 1980; Jacobs, 2005; Moughtin, 2003) and more recently investigated in connection to walkability perception (Spooner,
2007).
The study of the urban context and literature review
on levels of service for pedestrian transportation (e.g.,
Landis et al, 2001; Ferreira and Sanchez, 1998; Sarkar,
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 7. Correlation among walkability characteristics
1993; Sarkar, 1995b; Furin, 1971) was utilized to define
walkability predictors, i.e., walkability characteristics.
We opted for a five-category descriptor for walkability including the following characteristics: comfort,
orientation, aesthetics, safety, and security and provided
the public with a concise definition of each characteristic (Table 2). We utilized a semantic approach with
five sets of adjectives: comfortable – uncomfortable;
orientating – disorienting; attractive – unattractive;
safe – unsafe; secure – unsecure. We asked the public
to indicate which adjective would better describe their
perception of each presented scene by specifying where
their perception lied in a five-point scale (as suggested
by Hattori and Naka, 2010).
A 11x17” panel with color scenes was utilized (Figure 2), together with a scoring sheet and a written questionnaire. The written questionnaire was used to gather
respondents’ socio-demographic characteristics and
included questions on site familiarity, walking habits,
and walking ease, as well as on ranking of walkability
characteristics. Since studies have found perception to
be connected to socio-demographic factors (Lothian,
1999; Dramstad et al., 2006; Rogge et al., 2007; Elke et
al., 2007), we were interested in assessing if walkability
perceptions differed by group. Studies have also indicated that site familiarity may affect preference scores
(Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Dramstad et al., 2006);
therefore we included questions on residency and
frequency of visits to the area. We also administered
the survey in both urban and suburban areas to capture
differences among local community and commuters
or visitors. Several studies have found that landscape
perception varies with age (e.g., Yamashita, 2002). In
particular, Ferreira and Sanches (1998) investigate ranking of quality indexes for pedestrian levels of service
(i.e., visual attractiveness, comfort, continuity, safety,
and security) and found significant changes among
age groups. For example, in their study safety was
predominantly the priority for respondents under 16,
while comfort was the priority for groups from 30 to 60
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
years old. Interestingly, attractiveness was found to be
the top priority for respondents over 60. The elderly are
reported to be less concerned with safety and more with
aesthetics. We were interested in testing dependency of
ranking of walkability characteristics on respondents’
age, site familiarity, and walking habits and ease.
A preliminary survey was administered during the
month of June. The analysis of preliminary results led
to the simplification of the written questionnaire and the
modification of the visual survey. For examples, some
images were changed to ensure the representation of all
features of the walking environment, and the description of characteristics was simplified to aid respondents
in their interpretation of survey directions. The final
survey was administered during the months of August
and September. Surveys were administered in areas
along the Woodward corridor in three locations: the
New Center area, the downtown area, and Birmingham,
a suburban center north of the study area.
Overall, 299 visual surveys and written questionnaires were administered, of which 282 visual surveys
where successfully completed and 262 written questionnaires. Question 14 asked respondents to rank the five
walkability characteristics on a five-point scale, but
about 20 percent of respondents used their own method
to reply to question 14, assigning the same value to
more than one characteristic. For example, some respondents decided to give fives to up to five characteristics. Rather than discarding those questionnaires when
analyzing importance, we took the respondents’ answers
as given. A brief description of the population sample is
included in Table 3a and Table 3b.
Results
Respondents’ ratings were used to calculate an average walkability index (WI) per scene, comprehensive of
the five characteristics ratings (Table 4). When comparing the best rated and the worst rated scene (scenes 7
and 8), there is some agreement in ratings, but we also
can detect some variability in answers (Table 5). In
25
Refereed Full-Papers
order to understand which attributes affected walkability perception, we referred to WI for testing correlation
between scene attributes and public perception.
Correlation analysis was first utilized to test significant correlation among explanatory variables (i.e.,
environmental design and picture attributes). When
there is a strong relationship between explanatory variables (multicollinearity), the variances of the parameter
estimates increase (Ramanathan, 1992) and may result
in lack of statistical significance of individual explanatory variables when a sample size is small. When the
coefficients between two variables are higher than .75,
then the variables are noted to have strong correlation.
This is a common threshold to evaluate the amount of
correlation between variables, as noted by Lind et al.
(2008). In addition, when the p value is .1, we accept
that the correlation is statistically significant. Three
variables were dropped due to significant correlation:
street furniture (correlated with people), sidewalk materiality (correlated with sidewalk quality), and vehicles
(correlated with enclosure).
The correlation test also revealed which variables are
correlated with WI (Table 6). The explanatory variables
that are positive and statistically significant (p=0.10) for
the environmental design attributes are vegetation, enclosure, rhythm, texture diversity, and sidewalk quality.
Among picture attributes, one variable, amount of sky,
was statistically significant and had a negative relationship with the walkability index. Horizontal plane type
and proximity of intersecting streets negatively affected
the walkability index, but were not statistically significant. Similarly, color and sidewalk quality positively affected the walkability index, though not in a statistically
significant manner. Among picture attributes, amount
of shade had a negative effect on the walkability index,
while color of sky and people positively influenced the
walkability index (p>0.1). These findings confirm what
was observed in best and worst scenes, as scene 7 appears open and bare, with no detail and with low-quality
sidewalk, while scene 8 displays a higher degree of enclosure, people, vegetation, and shop window partitions
(i.e., rhythm), and good quality sidewalk. Implications
of these findings and comparison with findings from
other studies will be discussed in more detail in the
discussion/conclusion section of this paper.
As part of the analysis, we were interested in understanding correlation between each walkability characteristic (comfort, orientation, aesthetic, safety, and
security) and scene attributes. This was not possible, as
we encountered a significant single-mindedness in rat-
26 ings of the five characteristics. Overall, all of the walkability descriptors were highly correlated to each other.
We conducted a correlation test among characteristics
and found significant correlation (Table 7). A probability test shows that there was no difference in the rating
of aesthetics versus safety (p<0.01). This suggested
that respondents had one mental image of a scene and
based all ratings on that image. Furthermore, we found
Table 8a. Familiarity & WI
Table 8b. Familiarity & safety
Table 8c. Familiarity & safety ranking
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 9. Walking ease & safety ranking
reduced variability in answers as the number of rated
scenes increased. Correlations were performed between
our measure of single-mindedness and general information about the respondent. We wanted to know if there
were any groups that we could exclude from the analysis. We looked at age, education, residency, number of
walking trips, and familiarity with the area. We could
not detect significant differences in single-mindedness
for any group. Therefore, it was concluded that ratings
of comfort versus security only were different measures of the single image that a respondent had about a
scene. The implications of single-mindedness for our
study and in general for transportation research will be
discussed in the discussion section of this paper.
One of our research objectives was to assess significant relationships between respondents’ characteristics and perception of walkability. We were interested
in understanding effects of age, education, and site
familiarity, walking trips, and walking ease on the walkability index. We did not detect significant effects of
age, walking trips, and walkability ease on walkability
perception. When testing effects of familiarity of the
area on walkability perception, we found that people
familiar with the area rated the scenes with a higher
average score (WI), as shown in Table 8a. A probability
test resulted in p < 0.01, therefore the difference was
significant. In addition, we were able to measure effect
of familiarity with perception of safety. As seen in
Table 8b, people familiar with the area view the area
as safer (p = 0.06). When testing education effects on
WI, we found that more educated people tended to rate
scenes lower. We also detected a number of intersecting trends. County people (vs. people from the urban
area) were more educated, less familiar with the area,
and rated scenes lower, while familiar people were less
educated and rated scenes more highly.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
We were also interested in understanding ranking of walkability characteristics and influences of
respondents’ characteristics on ranking. Overall most
respondents said that safety was their primary concern
in rating walkability. However, many people familiar
with the area felt safety was less important for walkability compared to other issues (p=0.1) (Table 8c). We
also looked at influences of walking ease on ranking of
walkability characteristics. We found that people with
trouble walking rated comfort as more important than
safety, as shown in Table 9. Orientation also had greater
importance for this group. At the same time, walkability
ease did not appear to generate differences in overall
rating of the scenes.
Discussion/Conclusions
The evaluation techniques used in this study allowed
us to understand the overall perception of walkability
(WI) for urban scenes that represented the wider urban
context of the Woodward Avenue corridor. We were
able to assess which scene attributes (environmental
design attributes and picture attributes) had significant
relationship with public perceptions of walkability. The
discussion of findings allows us to offer suggestions for
future planning of walking environments.
Vegetation, enclosure, rhythm, texture diversity, and
sidewalk quality were positively correlated with walkability perception, while amount of sky was negatively
correlated with WI. The positive impact on walkability
of the first attribute, vegetation, confirms preference for
a greater amount of vegetation or for a greater naturalness of scenes, which is consistent with other studies
(Rogge et al., 2007;Wolf, 2004; Herzog et al., 2000;
Coeterier, 1996; Kaplan and Kaplan, 1983; Thelen,
1996; Purcell and Lamb, 1984) and also confirms the
importance of natural elements in the perception of
transportation systems and urban environments. In
terms of enclosure, it must be noted that in our case,
scenes with low levels of enclosure also resulted in
lower levels of sidewalk quality and rhythm (due to the
absence of buildings in the foreground). Nevertheless,
this finding aligns with what found by Spooner, 2007.
Interestingly, while several studies have employed
openness as a predictor for landscape preferences (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Coeterier, 1996; Herzog et al.,
2000) and found it positively correlated with landscape
appearance, enclosure, not openness, appears to be the
key predictor for studying the urban walking environment. In fact, the negative impact of amount of visible
sky on perceived walkability suggests the existence of a
27
Refereed Full-Papers
dichotomy between urban perception modes and natural
landscape ones, where the first gravitates around enclosure perception and the latter around openness. This
result spurs reflections on interpretations of the urban
space as an interior one, rather than an exterior. Studies by Smardon et al., 1986; Cullen, 1961, and Schultz,
1980 provide interpretations of urban places in this
direction. The importance of enclosure confirms findings of other scholars (e.g., Jacobs, 1995; Mounghtin,
2003) as well as much earlier studies by Roman and
Renaissance theorists and architects (Spooner, 2007).
The texture diversity and the rhythm attributes impacted
positively on walkability perception. We can interpret
these attributes as qualitative details of the urban environment connected to scale perception (Hedman, 1984).
Other scholars (e.g., Curran, 1983, Loidl, 2003, Dee,
2003,) also reported the importance of architectural
detail in their studies and highlighted the connection
between surface characteristics (nature of enclosing
elements) and experience of the environment. Finally,
the high correlation of sidewalk quality with walkability
perceptions appears consistent with traditional transportation research studies, which typically include sidewalk maintenance and continuity as key variables.
While assessing which respondents’ characteristics
significantly influenced walkability perception, we
detected familiarity and education levels as significant
factors. These findings align with results from other
studies, which did not specifically treat walkability
but dealt with environment, perception, and behavior
(e.g., Lothian, 1999; Dramstad et al., 2006; Rogge et
al., 2007). In particular Elke et al., 2007 reported that
preferences greatly vary among groups with different
interests due to the attachment of different levels of importance to the same landscape features. Furthermore,
Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989 and Dramstad et al., 2006
have indicated that site familiarity may affect preferences for environments. In our case, we found that familiar
people rated scenes more highly. This finding, in light
of the fact that familiar people also were less concerned
with safety, seems to point at important psychological
components of perception.
A crucial component of our study was the understanding of how people responded to our survey. The
detected single-mindedness spurs us to question studies
on walkability that attempt to separate components
of the walking experience, in particular traditional
transportation research (e.g., LOS approaches). The
great complexity of interrelated factors calls for further
research in order to test the appropriateness of utiliz-
28 ing distinct characteristics (e.g., perception of safety,
or security, level of comfort, attractiveness of the street
environment) as a measure of the overall holistic experience. The unwillingness of people to rank characteristics, as 20 percent of respondents choose to assign the
same value to more than one characteristic, seems to
confirm the difficulty in separating components of the
walking experience.
In Ferreira and Sanchez (1998), ranking of visual and
psychological attraction, comfort, continuity, safety, and
security varied with age. Our findings indicate that both
site familiarity and walking ease affect ranking of walkability characteristics, with familiar people less concerned with safety and people with lower walking ease
more concerned with comfort. The better understanding
of ranking of different characteristics is a first step towards understanding possible approaches to walkability
perception.
This study is connected to both perceptual and
transportation/planning research. The results from this
analysis can help identify future design considerations
for comparable urban environments and advance research in this field. There are, at the same time, several
limitations of this study. Though our model enabled us
to measure the most significant variables for perception and connections with respondents’ characteristics,
the brevity of the visual survey and the high number
of variables limit the extent and accuracy of results.
Further research and potentially innovative survey administration techniques are needed in order to overcome
study limitations. It must be noted that when studying
an existing environmental setting, correlation among
variables can often emerge and limit the interpretability
of results, as noted by Hull et al. (1987). In our study
several correlated variables were dropped in order
to avoid multicollinearity. Results do not reflect the
complete range of influences of attributes on preferences, and limit our understanding of which particular
attribute contributes the most to the WI of each scene.
Such limitations are to be expected given the diverse
multilayered character of the urban environment. In addition, it is important to note that seasonal changes can
affect appreciation of scenes (Palmer and Sena, 1993).
Additional research is needed to capture preferences for
scenes during the various seasons.
Acknowledgements
This study was made possible through an internal
university grant. We would like to thank Veronica Allen
and Blake Hill for their help in designing and adminis-
Refereed Full-Papers
tering surveys and for the graphics, and Professor Utpal
Dutta and Amy Chesterton for materials and suggestions for the development of this study.
References
Anderson, L.M., Schroeder, H.W., 1983. Application of
wildland scenic assessment methods to the urban landscape.
Landscape Urban Plan. 10, 219–237.
Arriaza, M., Canas-Ortega, J.F., Canas-Madueno, J.A., RuizAviles, P., 2004. Assessing the visual quality of rural
landscapes. Landscape Urban Plan. 69, 115–125.
Appleyard, D., 1981. Livable Streets. Berkley: University fo
Claifornia Press.
Appleyard, D., Lynch, K., Myer, J.R., 1964. The View From the
Road. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Bernaldez, F.G., Ruiz, J.P., Benayas, J., Abello, R.P., 1998. Real
landscapes versus photographed landscapes: preference
dimensions. Landscape Res. 13, 10–11.
Bosselman, P., 1998. Representation of Places: Reality and
Realism in City Design. University of California Press,
Berkley.
Buhyoff, G.J., Hull, R.B., Lien, J.N., Cordell, H.K., 1986.
Prediction of scenic quality for urban forests using vegetation
measurements. For. Sci. 30, 71–82.
Buhyoff, G.J., Miller, P.A., Roach, J.W., Zhou, D., Fuller, L.G.,
1994. An AI methodology for landscape visual assessments.
AI Appl. 8, 1–13.
Brown, B.B., Werner, C.M., Amburgey, J.W., Szalay, C. 2007.
Walkable route perceptions and physical features. Env
Behavior, 39:34-61.
Chanam, L., Moudon, A.V., 2004. Physical activity and
environment research in the health field: implications for
urban and transportation planning practice and research. J
Plan Lit, 19, 147-181
Coeterier, J.F., 1996. Dominant attributes in the perception and
evaluation of the Dutch landscape. Landscape Urban Plan.
34, 27–44.
Cullen, G., 1961. The Concise Townscape. Van Nostrand
Reinhold Company, London, UK.
Curran, Ramond J. (1983). Architecture and the Urban
Experience. NewYork: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Daniel, T.C., 2001.Whiter scenic beauty? Visual landscape
quality assessment in the 21st Century. Landscape Urban
Plan. 54, 267–281.
Dee. C. (2001). Form and Fabric in Landscape Architecture: A
Visual Introduction. London: Spon Press.
Dramstad, W.E., Sundli, M.T., Fjellstad, W.J., Fry, G.L.A., 2006.
Relationship between visual landscape preferences and mapbased indicators of landscape structures. Landscape Urban
Plan. 78, 465–474.
Elke, R., Frank, N., Hubert, G., 2007. Perceptions of rural
landscapes in Flanders: looking beyond aesthetics.
Landscape Urban Plan. 82, 159–174.
Ferreira, M.A.G., Sanchez, S., 1998. Evaluating the quality of
pedestrian facilities. Conference proceedings at the 68th
Annual Meeting of the Institute of Transportation Engineers,
Toronto, Canada.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Frank, L.D., Engelke, P.O. 2001. The built environment and
human activity patterns: Exploring the ipacts of urban form
on public health. J Plan Lit 16:202-218.
Franz, G., 2005. An empirical approach to the experience of
architectural space. Dissertation at the Max Planck Institute
for Biological Cybernetics, Tubingen and the Bauhaus
University,Weimar.
Furin, J., 1971. Pedestrian: Planning and Design. Metropolitan
Association of Urban Designers and Environmental Planners,
Inc., New York, 1991.
Hammitt,W.E., Patterson, M.E.,Noe, F.P., 1994. Identifying and
predicting visual preferences of southern Appalachian forest
recreation vistas. Landscape Plan. 29, 171–183.
Hattori, K., Naka, M. 2010. The study of the visual value of open
space on a university campus. Paper presented at the 41st
Annual Conference of the Environmental Design Research
Association, Washington, DC.
Hedman, R. and Jaszewski, A. (1984). Fundamentals of Urban
Design. Washington D.C.: Planners Press.
Herzog, T.R., Herbert, E.J., Kaplan, R., Crooks, C.L., 2000.
Cultural and developmental comparison of landscape
perceptions and preferences. Environ. Behav. 32 (3),
323–346.
Holgate, A., 1992. Aesthetics of Built Form. Oxford Press, NY.
Horton, F.E., Reynolds, D.R., 1971. Effects of urban spatial
structure on individual behavior. Econ. Geogr. 47 (1), 36–48.
Gallin, N. 2001. Quantifying pedestrian friendliness-guidelines
for assessing pedestrian level of service. Conference
Proceedings Road and Transport Research: Walking the 21st
Century, 20-21 February 2001, Perth, Australia,
Hieronymus, C.B.,de Vries, S.I., Graham, J. M., van Dongen,
J.E., Bakker, I., Miedema, H.M.,2009. Influence of
environmental street characteristics on wlaking route choice
of elderly people. J. Environ Psychology, 29:477-484
Hull, R.B., Buhyoff, G.L., Cordell, H.K., 1987. Psychophysical
models: an example with scenic beauty perceptions of
roadside pine forests. Landscape J. 6 (2), 113–122.
Hull, R.B., Revell, G.R.B., 1989. Issues in sampling landscapes
for visual quality assessments. Landscape Urban Plan. 17,
323–330.
Hull, R.B., Stewart, W.P., 1992. Validity of photo-based scenic
beauty judgments. J. Environ. Psychol. 12, 101–114.
Jacobs, A. (1995). Great Streets. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Kaplan, R., 1985. The analysis of perception via preference: a
strategy for studying how the environment is experienced.
Landscape Plan. 12, 161–176.
Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., 1989. The Experience of Nature. A
Psychological Perspective. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge, MA.
Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., 1983. Cognition and Environment.
Functioning in an Uncertain World. Ulrichs Bookstore, Ann
Arbor, MI.
Kido, E.M., 2005. Aesthetic aspects of railway stations in
Japan and Europe, as a part of “Context sensitive design for
railways”. J. East Asian Society Transp. Stud. 6, 4381–4396.
29
Refereed Full-Papers
King, A.C., Stokols, D., Talen, E.G., Brassington, S.,
Killingsworth, R. 2002. Theoretical approaches to the
promotion of physical activity: Forging a transdisciplinary
paradigm. American J Preventive Medecine 23 (2, Suppl):1525.
Landis, B.W., Vattikuti, V.R., Ottenberg, R.M., McLeod, D.S.,
Guttenplan, M., 2001. Modeling the roadside walking
environment. Pedestrian Level of Service. Transportation
Research Board 1773, Paper No. 01-0511
Lavery, I., Davey, S., Woodside, A., Ewart, K. 1996. The vital
role of street design and management in reducing barriers to
older people’s mobility. Landscape Urban Plan 35:181-92.
Law, C.S., Zube, E.H., 1983. Effects of photographic
composition on landscape perception. Landscape Res. 8,
22–23.
Lind, D.A., Marchal,W.G.,Wathen, S.A., 2008. Statistical
Techniques in Business and Economics, 13th ed. McGrawHill/Irwin Companies, New York, NY.
Litton, R.B., 1968. Forest Landscape Description and
Inventories—A Basis for Land Planning and Design.
Berkley, CA., Pacific SW. Forest and Range Exp. Sta. 64 pp.,
illus. (U.S.D.A Forest Serv. Res. Paper PSW-49).
Loidl, Hans and Stefan Bernard. (2003). Opening Spaces: Design
as Landscape Architecture. Boston: Birkhauser.
Lothian, A., 1999. Landscape and the philosophy of aesthetics:
is landscape quality inherent in the landscape or in the eye of
the beholder. Landscape Urban Plan. 44, 177–198.
Lynch, K., 1960. The Image of the City. MIT Press, Cambridge,
MA.
McKenzie, R.L., McKenzie, R., 1980. The Pinelands Scenic
Study: Summary Report. [S.l.]: HCRS. BM Owns.
Burlington County Library System,Westampton, NJ.
Moughtin, C, et. al. (1995). Urban Design Ornamentation and
Decoration. Boston: Butterworth Architecture.
Naderi, J.R., 2003. Landscape Design in Clear Zone: Effect of
Landscape Variables on Pedestrian Health and Driver Safety.
Transportation Research Board 1851, pp 119-130.
Palmer, J.F., 2003. Research agenda for landscape perception.
In: Buhmann/Ervin (Ed.), Conference Proceedings at
Anhalt University of Applied Sciences, 4th Conference
Trends in Landscape Modeling. Herbert Wichtmann Verlag,
Heidelberg.
Palmer, J.F., 1983. Visual quality and visual impact assessment.
In: Finsterbusch, K., Llewellyn, L.G., Wolf, C.P. (Eds.),
Social Impact Assessment Methods. Sage Publications,
London, pp. 268–283.
Palmer, J.F., Sena, K.D., 1993. Seasonal scenic value and forest
structure in northeastern Hardwood stands. In: Vander Stoep,
G.A. (Ed.), Proc. 1992 Northeastern Recreation Researches
Symposium. Gen. Tech. Rep. NE-176. Northeastern Forest
Experiment Station, Radnor, PA, pp. 115–121.
Perez, J.G., 2002. Ascertaining landscape perceptions and
preferences with pair-wise photographs: planning rural
tourism in Extremadura, Spain. Landscape Res. 27, 297–308.
Priestley, T., Evans,G.W., 1996.Residents perception of a nearby
electric transmission line. J. Environ. Psychol. 1, 65–74.
Purcell, A.T., Lamb, R.J., 1984. The prediction of scenic beauty
fromlandscape content and composition. J. Environ. Psychol.
4, 7–26.
30 Reid, S., 2008. Fit for purpose: evaluating walkability.
Engineering Sustainability, 161-2, 105-112.
Ramanathan, R., 1992. Introductory Econometrics with
Applications, second ed. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.,
Ontario, FL.
Rogge, E., Nevens, F., Gulinck, H., 2007. Perception of
rural landscapes in Flanders: looking beyond aesthetics.
Landscape Plan. 82, 159–174.
Saelens, B.E., Sallis, J.F., Frank, L.D., 2003. Environmental
correlates of walking and cycling: Findings from the
transportation, urban design, and planning literature. Ann
Behav Med 25, 80-91.
Sampei Yamashita, S., 2002. Perception and evaluation of water
in landscape: use of Photo-Projective Method to compare
child and adult residents’ perceptions of a Japanese river
environment. Landscape and Urban Planning, Vol 62:1, 3-17
Sarkar, S., 1993. Determination of Service Levels for
Pedestrians, with European Examples. Transp Res Board
1405, pp 32-45.
Sarkar, S. 1995b. Evaluation of safety for pedestrians at macro
and micro levels in urban areas. Transp Res Records 1405,
35-42.
Schafer, E.L., Hamilton, J.F., Schmidt, E.A., 1969. Natural
landscape preferences: a predictive model. J. Leisure Res. 1,
1–19.
Schafer Jr., E.L., Brush, R.O., 1977. How to measure preferences
for photographs of natural landscapes. Landscape Plan. 4,
237–256.
Schmid, W.A., 2001. The emerging role of visual resource
assessment and visualization in landscape planning in
Switzerland. Landscape Urban Plan. 54, 213–221.
Schultz, C.N. 1980. Genius Loci. Towards a Phenomenology of
Architecture. Rizzoli International Pubblications, NY.
Schuttleworth, S., 1980. The evaluation of landscape quality.
Landscape Res. 5, 14–20.
Smardon, R.C., Felleman, J.P., Palmer, J.F., 1986. Foundations
for Visual Project Analysis. John Wiley and Sons, New York,
NY.
Schroeder, H.W., Daniel, T.C., 1981. Progress in predicting the
perceived scenic beauty of forest landscapes. For. Sci. 27,
71–80.
Simonds, J.O., Starke, B.W., 2006. Landscape Architecture: A
Manual of Environmental Planning and Design, fourth ed.
McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
Spooner, D.D. 2007. Enclosure and walkability: An Italian street
study. Paper presented at the 38h Annual Conference of the
Environmental Design Research Association, Sacramento.
Stamps, A.E., 1990. Use of photographs to simulate
environments. A meta-analysis. Percept. Mot. Skills 71,
907–913.
Stewart, T.R., Middleton, P., Downton, M., Ely, D., 1984.
Judgment of photographs vs. field observation in studies of
perception and judgment of visual environment. J. Environ.
Psychol. 4, 283–302.
Thelen, L..A. 1996. Correlates of preferences for urban parks and
plazas. Paper presented at the 27th annual conference of the
Environmental Design Research Association, Salt Lake City,
UT.
Refereed Full-Papers
Van den Berg, A.E., Vlek, C.A.J., Coeterier, J.F., 1998. Group
differences in the aesthetic evaluation of nature development
plans: a multilevel approach.
J. Environ. Psychol. 18, 141–157.
Webb, P., 1964. (Ed.). Bioastronautics Data Book. NASA,
Washington, DC. Last consulted on the web at http://vision.
arc.nasa.gov/personnel/al/papers/ 64vision/17.htm on 1 April
2008.
Werner, C.M., Brown, B.B., Gallimore, J. 2010. Light rail use
is more likey on walkable bloks: Further support for using
micro-level environmental audit measures. J. Environ.
Pshychol. 30, 206-214.
Wherrett, J.R., 2000. Creating landscape preference models
using internet survey techniques. Landscape Res. 25, 76–96.
Wolf, K.L., 2004. Trees and business district preferences: A case
study of Athens, Georgia, U.S. J. Arboricult. 30, 336–346.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Wong, K., Domroes, M., 2005. The visual quality of urban park
scenes in Kowloon Park, Hong-Kong: likeability, affective
appraisal, and cross-cultural perspective. Environ. Plan. B,
Plan. Des. 32, 617–632.
Yamashita, S., 2002. Perception and evaluation of water in
landscape: use of Photo-Projective Method to compare
child and adult residents’ perceptions of a Japanese river
environment. Landscape Urban Plan. 62, 3–17.
Zacharias, J. 2001. Pedestrian behavior and perception in urban
walking environments. J Plan Lit. 12, 3-18.
Zacharias, J. 1997b. The impact of layout and visual stimuli
on the itineraries and perceptions of pedestrians in a public
market. Environ. Plan. B: Planning and Design 23, 23-35.
31
Refereed Full-Papers
Participatory Design in Positive
Aging: Products of the First
Cycle of an Environmental
Design Action Research Project
David Boeck (University of Oklahoma), David
Moxley (University of Oklahoma), Hans-Peter
Wachter (University of Oklahoma), Cindy
Simon Rosenthal (University of Oklahoma)
Abstract
The authors, an interdisciplinary team of researchers, consider their first year in environmental design
action research, which focuses on the documentation of
environmental conditions within a small city in south
central Oklahoma in the United States, as well as action to support positive aging within the city through
environmental design. In addition to describing the
parent action research project, the authors present key
conceptual frameworks that were products of the first
cycle of inquiry, document their efforts to advance
community participation, and outline their strategy for
transforming the project from action research in which
the researchers are dominant to a participatory model
in which community members have equal standing in
implementing design-relevant research activities.
Introduction
The intentionality with which environmental designers approach their work is inherently creative since it
brings new ways of thinking about how environments
function and bring about satisfaction for their users.
In his book, The Hidden Connections, Capra (2002)
amplifies the importance of novelty to the innovation
process inherent in ecological systems. As human beings enter such systems for the purposes of intentional
design of environments (or for the purposes of restoration, rebuilding, or repurposing) their novelty and
creative processes form critical actions in relationship
to the adaptation of environments to the current needs
of people and populations, and to the anticipation of
needs relative to future quality of life and sustainability.
Given climate change, intentional design of environments will become increasingly important as human
beings seek to preserve or restore nature (Babbitt, 2005;
McKibben, 2007), and adapt to the realities of new
32 climate patterns (McKibben, 2010).
Capra underscores how eco-literacy and eco-design
influence intentionality in environmental design. Multiple ways of action are relevant here: first, the designer
must anticipate sustainability, defined here as both the
careful application of the human footprint on the environment by considering the consequences and impact of
design as well as the preservation of resources for future
generations. Second, the designer must contemplate
how human diversity itself influences the competence
of environments to meet a range of human needs. False
dichotomies recede within good design, offering multiple forms of mobility, reducing the distinction between
able and disabled, and equipping people with ways of
reading their environments so they can make informed
choices about fulfilling their needs. Third, the designer
must consider the centrality of environmental quality as
the wellspring of human activity. Environmental degradation reduces healthy activity. Erosion of a natural
and civic aesthetic reduces cognitive variation, which
can reduce motivation, engagement, and enjoyment of
users.
Thus, fourth, design is culture. It interconnects
society’s fundamental belief system about how people
function well with how people within contexts come to
fulfill their needs. For whom is design for? Who does
design favor? What people does design bring into the
centrality of community life, and what people does it
leave out or otherwise omit? Design ethos and ethics
are important here—ethos as belief system, and ethic as
doing good are fundamental to achieve a higher plane of
community for all.
Here the contemporary movement towards universal design constitutes an ethic in itself. That universal
design makes communities function for all is realized,
particularly in its efforts to enable people to control
their environment (Gregory & Newell, 2002). In addition, universal design is instrumental—the control of
environment and the ensuing participation it fosters can
enhance a person’s satisfaction and so influences a person’s sense of quality of life (Carr et al., 2001). For the
Norman Aging Needs Assessment Project (NANAP), a
key assumption is that a person’s sense of quality of environment influences his or her perceived quality of life.
The First Cycle of Environmental Design
Action Research
The authors, who are linking environmental design
to positive aging, focus on the first principal cycle of
Refereed Full-Papers
action research (defined within this paper as the search
for knowledge or active learning from the engagement
of a particular phenomenon or object through systematic means) because it is within this first year that they
have sought to lay the foundation of practice useful
in both team development and community involvement for positive aging. Two dimensions characterize
the authors’ action research—what they have learned
about what constitutes good design (theory), and what
they have learned about their own process for advancing design to support positive aging (action). It is the
intent of NANAP to link theory and action in a manner
consistent with the best traditions of action research.
For the authors, positive aging does not involve an
idealized form of health and functioning. It does not
mean maintaining youth or gaining powers people once
had at hand when they were younger. The concept of
positive aging is both functional and instrumental since
it involves a process of becoming older in an environment that fosters people’s capacities to achieve those
things they value, and in which they find worth. Diversity in valuing reminds us that people can vary in what
is important to them within a given cultural context.
While people may share mutual aspirations for independence and good health, they may nonetheless express
these values differently. For one person, this may mean
sustaining his or her work in the arts, while for another,
it may involve parenting grandchildren. Even though
people face ill health as they age, good design can
compensate for decline. Good environmental design
anticipates such support for roles, functioning, competence, health or ill health, and ultimately satisfaction.
The Challenge Of Action Research And Its
Participatory Variant
As action research evolves into a participatory form,
it can break down the traditional barriers that separate
researcher and subject. However, it can also raise ethical considerations pertaining to the control of research
direction and ownership of knowledge (Minkler, 2004;
Whitehead, 1993). Academic researches may yield to
the expertise of community members, which means
that a norm of equality is sought among those who
would be normally excluded from meaningful roles
in the research and program development (Wergin,
2000; Zambrana, 1996). The elevation of those who
were once thought of as only subjects to the status of
involved participants is a principal quality of participatory action research (Carolo & Travers, 2005, Harper
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
& Carver, 1999, Travers, Leaver, & McClelland, 2002),
and requires sharing power and knowledge across
groups that normally may stand apart (Ochocka et al.,
2002). Partnerships of diverse members coming from
different locations within a community imbues participatory forms of action research with considerable
distinctiveness (Butterfoss et al., 1993, 1996) bringing
members of marginalized groups into central roles in
knowledge production (Hall, 1992) and strengthening
validity of research as a result (Christens & Perkins,
2008). Therefore, collaboration across diverse people is
an essential ingredient of participatory forms of action
research (Roussos & Fawcett, 2000).
Diversifying the qualities of those who participate in
environmental design research can expand perspectivetaking and strengthen understanding of the dynamics of
social issues (Israel et al., 2001) through engagement of
multiple stakeholders in complementary activity (Moxley, 2004; Nyden et al., 1997). The exercise of control
over research by those who have been historically omitted from key research roles is an essential outcome of
participatory research (Schnarch, 2004). The formation
of partnerships, the orchestration of diversity brought
into a project by virtue of the introduction of different
perspectives, and the empowerment of the perspectives
of those individuals or groups who were once left out of
inquiry ascend in their importance as central practices
within participatory action research (Minkler & Wallerstein, 2003, Minkler, 2005).
Norman Aging Needs Assessment Project (NANAP)
And Its Initial Participatory Qualities
Brought together by a mutual interest in intentional
design to support people’s well-being, functioning, and
aging, the core research team—the members of which
represent the disciplines of architecture, interior design,
social work, and political science—has spent a full year
in mutual commerce finding a central focus for complementary research activity. Environmental design as a
construct offers a mutual focus on responsive environments, ones that can support diversity through flexible
qualities.
The first cycle of inquiry has unfolded in several
phases, including the development of a team configuration necessitating the building of trust, mutual understanding of the disciplines the authors have brought to
the project, and creating norms supportive of collaboration (Cargo & Mercer, 2008; Hardy & Philips, 1998).
Taking approximately six months of dialogue and visits
33
Refereed Full-Papers
in which the authors engaged in open-ended conversation concerning concepts of aging, positive aging,
and design, strong interpersonal bonds emerged, ones
indicative of sound interdisciplinary process. For the
next three months, the authors laid out the actual research enterprise, educating themselves about methods
of inquiry, research ideas each of their disciplines found
useful, and action research relative to environmental
design. Alternative methods took a prominent place in
the work of the team, involving the use of photography,
photovoice, and quality methods as documentary and
illuminatory strategies useful for understanding intentional and unintentional design from the perspective of
people who often face design inequities that limit their
range of functioning and satisfaction.
The authors have accelerated their efforts to expand
the participatory scope of NANAP across the expanse
of the research process (research purpose, design, methods, data collection, analysis, interpretation, dissemination, utilization, and quality management). Participation
in this context meant for the authors that through the
involvement of people who have firsthand experience
with environment and aging, new insights could enrich
the relevance of the project and set the stage for utilization. NANAP’s research should advance environmental
policy that fosters positive aging within a small city
or town environment, a logical end of sound action
research. Good action research can lead to actionable
knowledge to make an impact for the better on how an
object functions within the context in which it is situated.
The enactment of NANAP within Norman, a town
located in south central Oklahoma, is a relevant context
for linking environmental design and positive aging.
Like much of Oklahoma, Norman is a desirable retirement location given its climate, economic infrastructure, health services, cultural activities, and community
ambience. Over the next twenty years, the city of
Norman expects a substantial increase in its population of older citizens far exceeding the general increase
in population for the city as a whole. Like the nation,
Norman expects significant increases in populations of
the young old, old, and old-old subgroups and is not
prepared for this substantive change in the character of
its population.
Inviting older citizens into the project as participants
is a salient strategy for shifting the project from an interdisciplinary action research context to a participatory
form. This means NANAP must create diverse roles
for participation, not only to address the preferences of
34 participants, but also to ensure that participation cuts
across all important phases of the parent research project. While the project is only now experimenting with
role innovation, promising practices already are emerging involving the use of continuing education for seniors as a way of obtaining their input into the principal
knowledge development activities of NANAP, involving participants directly in documentary data collection
activities they control (such as photovoice) so they can
tell their own stories about good and poor environmental qualities, and involving elders directly in the process
of making sense of the data the project captures. While
the project has much progress to make in achieving its
participatory ends, these ends are very much part of the
ethos and identity of NANAP as a whole.
Early Products
The Value-Added Sequence of Environmental Design
Within their own action research practice, the authors
have dialogued considerably about the value-added
process of environmental design. For the purposes
of NANAP, they conceive of the following sequence
inspired by the San Francisco Department of Public
Health HDMT checklist.
Quality of Environment
Infrastructure Development
Promotion of Social Cohesion
Functioning to Fulfill Needs
Satisfaction
Here environment and infrastructure form a holding
space for the fulfillment of human needs. It is the quality of environment that makes possible sound infrastructure development, a form of development in which
sustainability takes on important meaning and reality.
Social cohesion is a product of people who are able to
influence decisions shaping environment and quality of
life. Important here is broad-based citizen involvement
in social decision-making in what political scientists
Refereed Full-Papers
call co-production of policy (Bovaird, 2007; Brudney
& England, 1983). It is here citizens can influence the
design of policy, action, service, and environments
strengthening their stake in implementation and sustainability (Needham, 2009). To further enhance social
cohesion, public infrastructure is vital, involving public,
nonprofit, and for-profit local enterprises that support
the fulfillment of human needs. Quality of environment,
infrastructure development, and social cohesion offer
opportunities for people to meet their needs, particularly those they invest with some priority, and function
in ways they find meaningful.
Aims of Environmental Design
From this value-added sequence of environmental
design, the authors have developed eight domains in
which environmental design is undertaken for the pur-
poses of creating sustainable and competence-enhancing environments for people of diverse backgrounds.
These eight domains involve: (1) Environmental stewardship with the objective of fostering a shallow footprint and sustainable human activity; (2) Low impact of
diverse modes of mobility; (3) Ease of way finding; (4)
Involvement in decision-making; (5) Systems for social
cohesion and social support; (6) Quality of permanent
housing options; (7) Quality of economic and productive life; (8) Aesthetics of daily life.
Conclusion: The Participatory Process
What follows is a roadmap guiding participatory
aims. Broad-based participation within action research
projects can include involvement of participants in governance and advisement roles, specification of research
purpose, objectives, and questions, design activities,
Stage of Participation
Engagement
Core Elements
Mutual Respect Understanding
aging and positive
aging and the kind
of knowledge
participants seek
through NANAP
Trust
Consistent
involvement in
partnership and
community
Formalization
Action
Sustainability
Formalizing roles
within a coordinating
structure within the
project
Consulting with
one another before
taking action;
consensual action
Socializing
newcomers into
the practices of the
participation and
collaboration
Adhering to agreed
upon norms of
interaction and agreed
upon road map
Adhering to
the action plan
until there is
change based
on consensual
decisions
regarding design
priorities
Expanding mutually
beneficial outcomes
Relationship
Formation
Getting to know
Consistent positive
one and accept one
interaction among
another as individuals members within the
project structure
Community
members and
university
personnel
working together
collaboratively
Succession planning
and socialization of
leaders and members
into critical roles
Commitment
to Action
Agreeing on
purpose, aims,
and vision
Mutual assessment
and evaluation of
action
Continuous
strategic planning
for sustaining
collaboration
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Formulating a joint
work plan
35
Refereed Full-Papers
implementation, analysis and interpretation, and dissemination and utilization (Cargo & Mercer, 2008).
Within their framework, the authors populate the cells
with key activities for the development of participation
within NANAP. Note that the framework conceives
of partnership in phases—that it extends from initial
engagement through formation into action and then into
sustainability. The core elements of mutual respect,
trust building, relationship formation, and commitment
to policy or design action (e.g., co-production) evolve
across the stages, taking on considerable value as the
process and products of participatory action research
emerges as a defining property of NANAP. So the
realization of participatory aims is integral to action
research in environmental design.
References
Babbitt, B. (2005). Cities in the wilderness: A new vision of
land use in America. Washington: Island Press.
Bovaird, Tony. 2007. Beyond engagement and participation –
user and community co-production of public services, Public
Administration Review, 67 (5): 846-860 (2007).
Brudney, J. and England, R. 1983. Towards a definition of the
co-production concept. Public Administration Review, 43
(10), 59-65.
Butterfoss, F. D., Goodman, R. M., & Wandersman, A. (1993).
Community coalitions for prevention and health promotion.
Health Education Research, 8, 315-330.
Butterfoss, F. D., Goodman, R. M., & Wandersman, A. (1996).
Community coalitions for prevention and health promotion:
Predicting satisfaction, participation, and planning. Health
Education Quarterly, 23, 65-79.
Capra, F. (2002). The hidden connections: Integrating the
biological, cognitive and social dimensions of life into a
science of sustainability. New York City: Doubleday.
Cargo, M. & Mercer, S. (2008). The value and challenges of
participatory research: Strengthening its practice. Annual
Review of Public Health, 29, 325-350.
Carolo, H., & Travers, R. (2005). Challenges, complexities and
solutions: A unique HIV research partnership in Toronto.
Journal of Urban Health, 82, ii42.
Carr J. a., Gibson, B., Robinson G. P. (2001), Is quality of life
determined by expectations or experience? BMJ Helping
Doctors making better decisions, 19 May 2001 Volume 322,
Issue 7296
Christens, B., & Perkins, D. D. (2008). Transdisciplinary
multilevel action research to enhance ecological and
psychopolitical validity. Journal of Community Psychology,
36(2), 214-231.
Gregory, P., & Newell, A.F. (2002), Design for older and
disabled people– where do we go from here? Univ Access Inf
Soc (2002) 2: 3–7 / Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/
s10209-002-0031-9 Department of Applied Computing,
The University of Dundee, Nethergate, Dundee, DD1 4HN,
United Kingdom, http://www.computing.dundee.ac.uk,
Published online: 6 November 2002
36 Hall, B. (1992). From margins to center? The development and
purpose of participatory research. American Sociologist, 23,
15-28.
Hardy, C., & Phillips, N. (1998). Strategies of engagement:
Lessons from the critical examination of collaboration and
conflict in the interorganizational domain. Organization
Science, 9, 217-229.
Harper, G., & Carver, L. (1999). Out of the mainstream youth
as partners in collaborative research: Exploring the benefits
and challenges. Health Education and Behavior, 26, 250-265.
Israel, B., Schulz, A., Parker, E., & Becker, A. (2001).
Community-based participatory research: Policy
recommendations for promoting a partnership approach in
health research. Education for Health, 14, 182-197.
McKibben, B. (2007). Hope, human and wild: True stories of
living lightly on the earth.
Boston: Little Brown.
McKibben, B. (2010). Earth: Making a life on a tough new
planet. New York City: Times Books.
Minkler, M. (2004). Ethical challenges for the “outside”
researcher in community-based participatory research. Health
Education and Behavior, 31, 684-697.
Minkler, M. (2005). Community-based research partnerships:
Challenges and opportunities. Journal of Urban Health, 82,
ii2-ii12.
Minkler, M., & Wallerstein, N. (2003). Community-based
participatory research for health. San Francisco: JosseyBass.
Moxley, D. (2004). Engaged research in higher education
and civic responsibility reconsidered. In T. M. Soska &
A. K. Johnson (eds.), University-community partnerships:
Universities in civic engagement. (pp. 235-242).
Binghamton, NY: Haworth.
Needham, C. 2009. Co-production: an emerging evidence base
for adult social care transformation. SCIE Research Briefing
31. London: Social Care Institute for Excellence.
Nyden, P., Figert, A., Shibley, M., & Burrows, D. (1997).
Building community: Social science in action. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Ochocka, J., Jansen, R., & Nelson, G. (2002). Sharing power
and knowledge: Professional and mental health consumer/
survivor researchers working together in a participatory
action research project. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal,
25, 379-387.
Pinto, R., McKay, M., & Escobar, C. (2008). “You’ve gotta know
the community”: Minority women make recommendations
about community focused health research. Women & Health,
47(1), 83-104.
Roussos, S. T., & Fawcett, s. B. (2000). A review of collaborative
partnerships as a strategy for improving community health.
Annual Review of Public Health, 21, 369-402.
Seamon, D. (2000). Concretizing Heidegger’s Notion of
Dwelling: The Contributions of Thomas Thiis-Evensen and
Christopher Alexander. Building and Dwelling [Bauen und
Wohnen], Waxmann: 189-202.
Schnarch, B. (2004). Ownership, control, access, and possession
or self-determination applied to research: A critical analysis
of contemporary First Nations research and some options for
First Nations communities. Ottawa: First Nations Centre.
Refereed Full-Papers
Travers, R., Leaver, C., & McClelland, A. (2002), Assessing HIV
vulnerability among lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and
transsexual and two spirited youth who migrate to Toronto.
Canadian Journal of Infectious Diseases, 13, Supplement A,
Abstract #408.
Wergin, J. Elements of effective community engagement. In S.
L. Percy, N. L. Zimpher, & M. J. Brukardt (eds), Creating
a new kind of university: Institutionalizing communityuniversity engagement. (pp.23-42). Bolton, MA: Anker.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Whitehead, M. (1993). The ownership of research. In J. Davies
& M. Kelly (eds.). Healthy cities: Research and practice.
(pp. 83-89). New York City: Routledge.
Zambrana, R. (1996). The role of Latino/Hispanic communities
in health services research: Strategies for meaningful
partnership. Journal of Medical Systems, 20, 325-336.
Zmudzinska-Nowak, M. (2003). “Searching for Legible City
Form: Kevin Lynch’s Theory in
Contemporary Perspective.” Journal of Urban Technology,
10(3): 19-39.
37
Refereed Full-Papers
Grandma’s World: Mapping
Dancers in Beijing
Caroline Chen (University of California,
Berkeley)
Abstract
This study employs cognitive mapping and surveys
to learn about the world of aging women in Beijing
who participate in daily dancing exercises. It was
carried out on a small sample of retired residents in
the Honglianbeili neighborhood of Beijing (N = 14)
to pursue two goals: (1) to learn the time and activity
structure of a typical day, and (2) to identify meaningful
landmarks and daily destinations in their neighborhood
that are meaningful to the respondents. This research
was responding to interview data gathered in 2007,
when dancers reported encountering dangerous spaces
while traveling to their dancing places. In an effort to
understand where these challenging spaces were located
and to gain a better sense of the overall territorial range
of older women in Beijing, this research was proposed.
This paper will focus primarily on the triangulation of
data from three instruments: (1) daily routine surveys
identified hour-by-hour activities and locations for one
waking day; (2) dancer-drawn cognitive maps showed
everyday spaces in the neighborhood; and (3) a general
survey posed follow-up questions to those asked in
2007 surveys.1 This paper will focus on the data gathered during the spring and summer of 2010 at Honglianbeili plaza. The results showed that food markets,
neighborhood activity centers, and urban plazas, as well
as roads, bridges, and intersections, were represented
in nearly all the cognitive maps. Respondents reported that they spend over two hours a day engaged in
directed physical activities such as taichi and dancing.
Residents also credited dancing as a source of making
new friends, relaxing, feeling physically stronger, and
experiencing joy. Respondents reported, however, a
sense of powerlessness in changing the constraints of
their dancing space and expressed a desire for design
improvements in their existing dance space.
Introduction
“The city should provide more space for the people
who like to do exercise, because the population of aging people is increasing. Life quality and health are precious.” – 53-year-old dancer in Beijing
Changes in the City and Urban Life
The urban landscape of Beijing in recent history has
dramatically changed since China’s shift from a planned
to a market economy in the 1980s. Mao Zedong’s ideal
vision of the city as an industrialist dream – “a forest of
smokestacks” – gave way to Le Corbusier’s Modernist
vision of “high buildings, set far apart from one another,
[to] … free the ground for broad verdant areas” (Le Corbusier, 1973, p. 65) as Mao’s successor, Deng Xiaoping,
instituted sweeping social and economic reforms. Architects such as Wu Liangyong who wished to preserve the
character of old neighborhoods in Beijing experimented
with projects such as Ju’er hutong, attempting to preserve traditional low-rise siheyuan courtyard structures
in the face of these changes (Wu, 1999). Other familiar
spatial forms such as the Soviet-styled workunits—
the danwei—also encountered change, although some
studies show that the cellular urban form in Beijing still
persists (Lu, 2003).
In this backdrop of social and spatial transformation,
aging women in Beijing continue daily patterns of life,
shopping for vegetables, preparing meals, visiting the
doctor, and dancing. Engagement with physical activity
is associated with successful aging and longevity (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, 1996), and
many elderly in Beijing choose dance as their preferred
form of exercise. Although there is no clear way to
determine exactly how many residents dance in Beijing,
the China Daily newspaper provided an estimate in 2004
of the number of Beijing residents that dance just one
type of dance—the yangge—alone: 60,000.2 Most of
the dancers are women and group sizes range from thirty
to over one hundred. Musical accompaniment may come
in the form of an MP3 or cassette player attached to an
amplifier on a small dolly, or in the form of a live band
of musicians, playing drums, cymbals, and flute.
In parks and in places that Leanne G. Rivlin calls
“found spaces” (2007: 38), dancing happens on a regular
schedule in the city: usually in the morning or in the
A longitudinal comparison between data from 2007 and 2010 between dance groups located at the same sites will be the subject of a
different paper.
2
The yangge is a traditional agricultural dance from the northern provinces in China. Farmers in the countryside celebrated the harvest
dancing yangge, but yangge is now danced in the city for physical fitness.
1
38 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 1: Musicians playing yangge music under the
Jingouhe freeway bridge in Beijing (Source: Caroline
Chen)
evening. Spaces such as underground parking garages,
spaces under freeway overpasses, sidewalks, and parking lots are routinely appropriated by nearby residents
for a few hours each day. These spaces are temporarily
pried apart from their original purpose, “loosened” for
a few hours to function as impromptu neighborhood
stages (Franck & Stevens, 2007).
Dancing in Beijing became very popular during
the 1990s (Graezer, 2004), an indirect response to the
outcome of the One-Child-Per-Couple policy instituted
in China in 1979. The policy set out to reduce pressure
on national resources by slowing China’s population
growth (Smil, 1993). But today, an unintended consequence of this successful family-planning initiative is
emerging: aging Chinese couples now have only one
adult child to depend upon for assistance and support in
their old age. With many adult children busy with their
own families, many aging Beijing residents spontaneously started organizing dancing groups as a way to
help themselves stay happy and healthy, so they would
not burden their children. Honglianbeili plaza is the
dance place of one such self-organized dancing group.
Locating Honglianbeili Community
Honglianbeili community is a small residential
community located west of Lotus River in the southern
Xuanwu district of Beijing. The south side of Beijing
has historically been a less developed, less affluent part
of the city (Elliott, 2001). Much of the development in
Beijing has been focused on the northern and western
parts of the city, where the embassies and business
districts are located.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 2: 2010 map of dance places in Beijing.
Honglianbeili is circled in the southwest side of the
city. (Map: Caroline Chen)
Figure 3: Arial view of the Honglianbeili plaza,
located between the Honglianbeili community on the
west, and Lotus River on the east. The Yuanjian
Mingyuan community is located across the river to
the east. (Source: Google Earth Pro)
While some areas of the “South City”—such as
the Qianmen Theater and Silk District—have been
razed and rebuilt, the Honglianbeili neighborhood has
remained relatively intact. The map in Figure 2 shows
the locations of all the dance places studied during the
spring and summer of 2010. This paper will focus on
one, Honglianbeili, the site situated between the third
and fourth ring road that is circled on the map.
Context and Physical Aspects of Honglianbeili Plaza
Urban Context
Honglianbeili plaza is located to the east of the
Honglianbeili community, a series of five-story walk-
39
Refereed Full-Papers
up apartment units, arranged in rows. Rows of mature
trees provide a deciduous canopy in the spaces between
buildings. Forming the western boundary of the plaza
is the Lotus River and a north-south running access
road called Lianhua River Westside Road. This access
road intersects the Maliandao South Road just to the
south of the Honglianbeili plaza.
In Figure 3 above, the study site is circled. To the
south of the site is the Honglian culture plaza, another
large hardscape space that is also a popular evening
dancing place. Though only a few feet away, the
Honglian culture plaza is the site of a very different
kind of dancing: nightly disco-dancing for adolescents
and young adults.3 This larger plaza, though not the
focus of this paper, shows up as a landmark in many
cognitive maps.
Honglianbeili Plaza
Approximately 60 by 80 feet, Honglianbeili plaza is
paved with concrete bricks. A large wooden pergola
with benches is built on the side of the plaza adjacent
to the restaurant. The plaza itself is open to the sky, but
protected from major traffic noise from the nearby Maliandao South Road because the plaza is set back from
this main thoroughfare.
Two large planters with evergreen trees are also
located in the plaza. The trees do not provide shade, but
demarcate the northern and southern boundaries of the
space. Low shrubs planted along the back of the pergola and lawn planted inside the tree planters soften the
impact of the concrete paving and brick wall that are
the two major defining features of the space. In Figure
4 above, the planter and tree on the northern boundary
of the space is visible. The researcher stood inside the
southernmost tree planter to take this photograph.
An abundance of small markets and stores are embedded in this old neighborhood, and the sidewalks are
crowded with many aging residents, walking or biking without hurry, toting colorful nylon shopping bags
filled with greens. The squeals and laughter of children
playing in the street add to the small-scale, intimate
feeling surrounding Honglianbeili plaza. According to
residents, this neighborhood has retained much of its
original character, unlike other districts in Beijing that
have experienced a great deal of demolition.
3
Figure 4: A view of the Honglianbeili riverside plaza
after an afternoon shower: view to the northwest.
(Source: Caroline Chen)
As night falls, the space fills with older women
carrying nylon bags, but this time the bags are filled
with dancing props instead of vegetables. They bring
folded fans, handkerchiefs, rubber balls connected to
elastic cords for dancing, but they also bring newspaper clippings and herbal health remedies to share with
fellow dancers. When the flood lamps that shine on the
restaurant billboard overlooking the plaza are turned
on, the reflected overhead lighting shines coldly on the
women who dance in rows, arms-length apart. When
the billboard lamps are off, the plaza is only illuminated
by small walkway lamps to the north of the site. Under
these lighting conditions, it is impossible to see the
features of each dancer’s face, so one must remember
what each person is wearing to tell the dancing bodies
apart. Still, silhouettes are visible in the semi-darkness,
and with the music so lively and gay, dancers are able
to somehow avoid colliding with one another in the
deepening darkness.
Dancer Profiles
The leader of the group is a sixty-nine year old woman who expressed her interest in participating in the
research to make their dancing plight known: the spaces
in the city for the elderly to dance are diminishing. The
total number of members of the dancing community at
Honglianbeili is estimated to be three hundred, although
In Figure 3, just south of the Honglianbeili plaza, a small white “X” is visible, marking the upper level of the Honglian Culture Plaza.
A darker “X” indicates the lower level of the Honglian Culture Plaza that lies to the west.
40 Refereed Full-Papers
at any one time, there are only one hundred women
dancing.
The profiles of the dancers who gather at Honglianbeili are very similar to those of other groups I have
studied in Beijing.4 Most of the participants were women and the members’ ages ranged from the late fifties to
their early eighties. The members of this generational
cohort were teenagers during the Cultural Revolution in
the 1960s, and are often referred to as the Lost Generation5 or the Sent-Down Youth (MacFarquhar & Fairbank
1991: 216). Because of the social upheaval of that
time, many were unable to complete their education,
affecting their later career prospects.
On a typical night, dancers gather at the pergola to
wait for their friends. Eventually, they organize themselves into rows, facing the restaurant wall. The dance
leader has organized a song list for the evening and
selects a song on her MP3 player to play through a
portable amplifier that she has brought with her from
home. The amplifier is attached to a light, foldable
rolling dolly – the kind that I have observed some older
women use for carrying vegetables. Dancing begins
promptly at 7:15pm and lasts until 9:30pm, seven days
a week: each dance session lasts for one hour, with a
fifteen-minute break between the two sessions.
During intermission, dancers rest on the benches,
fanning themselves. Bags, coats, and pet dogs are left
on the benches under the pergola. This is a safe place
to leave their belongings because, one dancer explained
to me, the space remains in full view throughout the
entire dance session. Bystanders who stop to watch the
group sometimes sit under the pergola, but more often
cluster around the perimeter of the dancing group.
Goals and methodology
As my study focuses on dancing by older people,
only older dancers in the group were encouraged to
participate.6 The study seeks to attain two goals: (1) to
learn the time and activity structure of a typical day for
an older person in Beijing who leads an active lifestyle,
and (2) to identify landmarks and daily destinations that
4
are meaningful to the respondents. Instruments used
to discover the contours of a day in the life of a dancer
included: (1) daily routine surveys to identify hour-byhour activities and locations for one waking day; (2)
dancer-drawn cognitive maps or yìngxiàngtú, translates
into “impression map”) to show everyday spaces in the
neighborhood; and (3) a general survey to ask follow-up
questions to those asked in 2007 surveys.
The methodology was devised so three different
types of data may be triangulated to construct an image
of a typical day in the life of an active older person
living in Beijing. The purpose of the cognitive maps
was to gain insight into the individual-centered world
of the respondent (R. Sommer & B. B. Sommer, 2002):
what are the landmarks and the places that one values,
and what are the paths worth noting. Use of cognitive
mapping was inspired by the work of Kevin Lynch and
many others who used drawing as a method for design
inquiry. In Image of the City (1960), Lynch provided
his respondents with very precise instructions for drawing a cognitive map:
a. Draw a quick sketch map of the area in question,
showing the most interesting and most important
features, and giving a stranger enough knowledge
to move about without too much difficulty.
b. Make a similar sketch of the route and events
along one or two imaginary trips, trips chosen to
expose the length and breadth of the area.
c. Make a written list of the parts of the city felt to
be most distinctive, the examiner explaining the
meaning of “parts” and “distinctive.”
d. Put down brief written answers to a few questions of the type: “Where is ___ located?”
(Lynch, 1960, p. 155)
A Fulbright grant enabled me to come to China in 2004 and I have been studying dancers in Beijing ever since.
5
“Lost” sometimes refers to loss of life opportunities that this generation experienced; many were not able to continue their education in the
wake of the Cultural Revolution. “Sent down” refers to the official program by the government that “sent” the youth “down to the countryside” to learn Communist values from farmers.
6
The retirement age for women in China is typically 50 years of age; state officials may work until 55 years of age. The retirement age for
men in China is 60 (Xinhua, 2011).
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
41
Refereed Full-Papers
While Lynch focused on how respondents viewed
the city, his emphasis lay in ways of seeing, on the
faculty of perception itself. Implications from his work
suggested ways that designers and planners may create
more legible spaces that enhance orientation.
This study seeks not to find generalizable rules for
how humans comprehend a city, but instead to find out
what elements of their surroundings were important
components to their daily lives. In this way, the cognitive mapping for my study draws from Randy Hester’s
Sacred Structure mapping, where residents of a small
town named Manteo in North Carolina were asked to
name places in town that were particularly important to
them as insiders (Hester, 1985).7 This method emphasized the perspective of the insider and is different from
Lynch’s emphasis on drawing a map for “a stranger”
(see above).
The method used for this study was purposefully
made open-ended because of some unknowns. Respondents were asked to: (1) draw a freehand map of their
everyday environment and (2) draw and label on the
maps places that they frequent in the course of a regular
day. These instructions were given for two reasons: (1)
to see whether Chinese residents would represent their
world differently from residents from the U.S., and (2)
to identify what the important everyday spaces are.
The daily routine survey and general survey were
collected to provide and time and narrative structure to
the cognitive maps. The first survey consisted of two
pages of basic demographic information and a table.
Respondents are asked to fill out the table, hour by hour,
listing the activities they were engaged in, where they
were, how they arrived at that location, and how long
they were in transit (and the mode of travel). The general survey consists of both multiple choice and openended questions. They contain questions that were used
previously in 2007.
7
8
Method
Overview of the Research
Fourteen dancers agreed to draw cognitive maps
and to fill out a daily routine survey, while many more
agreed to fill out general surveys. However, as mentioned above, the purpose of this paper is to examine the
cognitive maps, so only respondents who have completed all three instruments will be included in this study.
Of the participants, the youngest dancer was 51 and the
oldest was 83 years of age.
Procedure
The daily routine and general surveys were administered first. Participants were given two weeks to take
the materials home, complete them, and return them to
the researchers for a small honorarium.8 The cognitive
map drawing took place after surveys were completed,
over a four-hour period on one day at the urban plaza.
Participants made appointments to meet with the research team, and respondents waited their turn to draw
a map. The map drawing took place outdoors, while
sitting on the bench under the pergola on the side of the
plaza nearest the restaurant. The Chinese translation
of “cognitive map” we used was yìngxiàngtú, which
roughly translates to “impression map.” I provided 18
x 24 inch white drawing paper and a drawing surface,
along with pencils, markers, and pens.
Participants were given instructions to: (1) draw the
dancing place and the river as a point of reference, and
(2) draw any of the other places that are meaningful
for them in their own neighborhood. I had to explain
the difference between the cognitive maps and objective maps several times as some of the respondents felt
afraid they would “make a mistake.”
To help some dancers orient, the dance leader drew
the cardinal directions on the four edges of the paper
for two participants. This extra prompt seemed to help
respondents begin to draw, even if they did not orient
their drawing correctly to the cardinal directions. We
A recurrent theme that ran through the Hester study was a desire by the town of Manteo to preserve local needs and perspectives, defending against potential contamination from outsider desires or expectations: “The residents wanted these places protected. One newspaper
editor expressed his concern that the identification of these places in the survey meant that the designers were considering changing the
places to attract tourists. He carefully listed those places that must not be profaned for tourists, stating that they were “perfect jewels”
the way they were.” (Hester, 1985, p. 14) This sentiment to keep things as they are was not in evidence in the Beijing dancer study. On
the contrary, the dancers looked forward to modernization changes in their neighborhood that would help bolster China’s emerging global
presence. They just hoped that they would be able to continue their daily dancing as they did before, in the new landscape.
I recorded a DVD of one of their dancing sessions, and dancers who participated in drawing cognitive maps were given a copy of this DVD.
42 Refereed Full-Papers
also prompted some dancers to draw by asking them:
“where is your home?” But aside from asking them to
draw the dance space, river, directions, and for some,
their home for reference, we did not prompt the dancers
to include any other information on their drawings.
I recorded some comments by respondents in my
field notebook because some made interesting com-
ments while drawing, but the information was not
included in the drawing. For some of these comments
that refer to a specific location on the map, I included
the comments in italics. When respondents made more
general comments or global explanations while drawing, I recorded these comments as interview text and
did not add these comments to the map.
ID
Places appearing on Daily
Routine Surveys
Places appearing on Cognitive Maps
500
Home
work
Honglianbeili activity
center
Honglianbeili dance place
510
Work
Home
supermarket
morning market
drugstore
Honglian Culture Plaza
food market
bridge (3)
overpass
road
513
shopping at food market
home
514
home
hospital
life insurance company
dance place
walkway
road
gardens (2)
residential tower (home)
morning market 1 (comment: “outdoor – inexpensive”)
morning market 2 (comment: “indoor – expensive”)
supermarket
primary school
home
food market
Honglian Culture Plaza
518
bus stop (comment: [this
is where she practices
taichi])
food shopping
home
dance place
bus stop #80 (comment: she exercises on the sidewalk near
the bus stop)
Honglian Culture Plaza
food market
bridge
road (comment: “crooked”)
bridge
morning market (comment: “cheap”)
food market
morning market
home
middle-school
Figure 5: Each row contains responses provided by the same respondent. Destinations that appear on the Daily Routine
survey appear in the left column. Destinations that appear on the Cognitive Map appear on the right column.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
43
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 6: Drawing from the 69 year old leader of the Honglianbeili
dance group.
Results
Time/Activity Structure from Daily Routine surveys
Nearly all respondents reported arising at 6am and
going to bed at 10pm, although one respondent reported
arising at 5am. Half of the dancers take naps after
lunch in the afternoon for one hour; one respondent
who still works takes her afternoon nap in her office.
All respondents spend two hours in the evening dancing at Honglianbeili plaza. One respondent noted that
she spends 20 minutes biking to work, and another 20
minutes biking back home, but she did not include this
activity as exercise when calculating her total daily
exercise time for one day. All of the retired dancers do
housework in the morning and afternoon, and all of the
dancers either walk, bike, or more rarely, take the bus
to arrive at their destinations. None of the respondents
drive.
Comparison of destinations appearing on Daily
Routine surveys and Cognitive Maps by a selection of
respondents
Figure 5 shows how cognitive maps can gather
much more data about the context of the neighborhood
than an hourly inventory of destinations traveled by
a respondent for one day. Markets, roads, stores, and
schools appear with great frequency in the cognitive
maps. Respondents report several reasons for dancing:
for friendship, to find community, to feel happy, to find
44 relief from stress, to “strengthen the body,” and above
all, “not to burden their children” by falling ill.
Discussion
Daily Routine Surveys versus Cognitive Maps
A comparison of the Daily Routine surveys show
that many of the respondents have similar time/activity
structures throughout the day. A comparison between
the places that were visited in one day, compared to the
places named on the cognitive map showed that respondents are constrained by time to visit very few known
places in the course of one waking day. The research
implication is that if one seeks to understand the scope
of a subject’s world, cognitive maps can yield this kind
of global information. However, for research on vulnerable populations like the elderly that may be at risk of
falls and fractures, tracking individuals’ daily paths in
the form of activity diaries and GPS tracks may be useful to learn exactly where the difficult obstacles are in
the daily landscape (Shumway-Cook et al., 1997).
Cognitive Maps: Pride and Problems
The cognitive map below was drawn by the dance
leader. She spoke with pride about her neighborhood.
She explained that Honglianbeili has a very good primary school and all the dancers’ children attended this
school, so it is a great source of pride for them.
Refereed Full-Papers
Although she does not volunteer at the school or
have anything directly to do with the school any longer,
she prominently drew this place on her map and explained to me that everybody feels happy just knowing
the school is still there and still winning national attention for its excellence.
On her drawing, she also noted which markets had
better produce prices, and what obstacles presented
dancers with difficulties. She notes that the dancing
place is dark at night and the river has a bad odor. She
wishes the government would help: “At night, there
are no lights. We hope the government can solve this
problem” (see Figure 6, above).
Bad Lighting
She was not alone. In fact, nearly half of the participants provided suggestions for how their neighborhood
could be improved for dancing. Half of the comments
focused again on the need for reliable nighttime lighting.
Figure 7 below shows the large billboard advertisement for the neighboring restaurant that has been
providing Honglianbeili plaza with evening lighting for
several years. While the billboard lamps were a reliable
source of light in the past, it has not always been illuminated at night in recent years.
One respondent explained why: “It has been much
hotter this summer and the restaurant that we dance in
front of is using up lots of electricity because it has to
keep the air conditioner on the entire day. As a result of
Figure 7: Honglianbeili plaza at twilight, a quarter
hour before dancing begins.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
this, the restaurant did not want to leave [the outdoor]
lights [that illuminate their small dancing plaza] at
night. We need more lights for our dancing.” [3:11]
As an unintended consequence of energy conservation, the elderly are dancing in the dark. The billboard
is no longer a reliable source of light for the older
people.
Confusing Places
Other respondents described dangerous intersections
on the way to the dance place. One dancer focused her
drawing on a particularly confusing and inconvenient
area near her home: “There is a bus station with lots of
parking—this area is a mess. Many people are there
and the traffic is terrible. I think there should be a road
that cuts through this area so people don’t have to go
all the way around the parking area.” [6:1] In Figure 8
below, this respondent drew a circle around the area she
found “confusing”—the area around the West Railway
Station—and then told us that area near the SecondHand Goods Market was confusing as well.
The participant who drew this map recommended a
through street in this congested area so aging residents
like her can avoid circumnavigating the parking lot
before arriving at the bus station [6:2].
Figure 8: Living near one of Beijing’s main transit
hubs, a 60 year old dancer laments the confusing
road conditions.
45
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 9: Constructive criticism for city officials from a 61 year old dancer.
“The River is Dead”
Other respondents were concerned about the nearby
Lotus River. One-third of the participants remarked
that the river was in need of immediate remediation,
complaining of a bad odor:
One respondent acknowledged previous efforts by
city planners to improve the river’s water quality. The
efforts failed, however. In Figure 9 in the upper right
corner, one respondent wrote encouraging words to the
planners on her map: “Continue restoring the river, little
by little.” [7:1].
Sunlight and Seasonal Timing
This same respondent who was concerned about the
river also named another common concern in many
general surveys: how to avoid the sun. In Figure 9
above, she named the “Dancing Place in the Morning”
at the top of her drawing. She explained that this was
a particularly well-designed for dancing because “the
buildings cast a long shadow in the mornings” [7:1],
blocking the sunlight from the concrete area in front of
the building.9 According to her daily routine survey,
this sixty-one year old woman begins her day dancing
in front of this tall building with the long shadow, and
then she leaves for work.10 After work and before dinner, she plays hacky-sack for one hour with her friends.
After dinner, she dances two more hours at the Honglianbeili plaza with the other one hundred older women.
Every day of the week, she exercises 2.5 hours, not
including the forty minutes she spends each day commuting to work and back.
Another respondent mentioned that the position of
the sun during different seasons was a criterion for
where one could or could not dance. She provided an
example: in the wintertime mornings when temperatures are low, dancers search for places that are warmed
by the sun.
From the general survey, she identified Honglian
culture plaza as such a space. This is the plaza that is
directly to the south of Honglianbeili plaza. In Figure
10 above, this large plaza is simply labeled “plaza.”
Note that the map she drew is rotated so that the map’s
“north” is actually pointing west. This respondent
added that the warming rays of the sun serve two func-
9
Very generally, many Chinese women are afraid of becoming tan from the sun. Many wear large, floppy hats or carry umbrellas to avoid
the sun’s browning rays.
10
While the retirement age for women in China is 55 or 60 (for those who are officials), permission to work beyond retirement age is possible on a case-by-case basis. This respondent is 61 years old and still commutes by bicycle to her work each day.
46 Refereed Full-Papers
businesses such as restaurants are generally a good
match. Office buildings and schools do not pair well
with dancing places, as workers and school children inside may not be able to concentrate well when the older
people begin dancing.
Lacking large extended families and adequate retirement pensions to depend on for old age, the aging
dancers at Honglianbeili have crafted their own health
intervention for those in the same generational cohort to
help one another. Given their demonstrated desire and
motivation to dance, it appears that the task is now up
to physical planners, urban designers, and landscape architects to respond: to set aside space and design places
so this aging cohort may safely and comfortably dance
their way to health and long life.
Figure 10: A 54 year old dancer’s drawing of her residential
community.
tions: (1) to keep dancers comfortable, but also (2) to
melt the snow on the ground so dancers may have a
clear place to dance.
Conclusion
Implications for Design and Planning
From this analysis, designers and planners working
in China are in a good position to help design a danceable city. For safe evening dancing, night lighting for
dancing spaces and electrical plugs for amplifiers would
help reduce the risk of dancers colliding with one
another, falling down, or otherwise sustaining injuries
from dancing in the dark.
Aging residents are at pains to find flat urban plazas
that are warm in the winter and protected from sun in
the summer. Spaces designed for morning dancing do
not require lighting, but would ideally receive morning
sunlight. Spaces designed for evening dancing would
naturally not require this kind of solar access, but
should be located in a space that would dampen sound
so the dancing music will not disturb residential neighbors. Pairing dancing places next to large commercial
Limitations
There were two major sources of bias in this study:
(1) privileged representation of the opinions of bettereducated dancers, and (2) bias introduced by intermediaries who helped illiterate dancers fill out surveys.
Even though the surveys were written in Chinese, many
of the dancers possessed only an elementary or middleschool education and could not read the forms.11 As a
result, my survey data favors the participation of dancers who have the ability to read and write. There were
instances, however, of illiterate dancers who desired to
participate in the research and who were enterprising
enough to secure the help of friends or family members
to help them complete the survey. This, in turn, raised
questions of whether the helping agent who assisted
the respondent explained the question adequately, or
recorded the responses of the respondent accurately.
The original mapping study aimed to collect drawings from a much greater number of dancers from several different dancing groups. What was ultimately possible within the limits of time and funding was far more
modest (N=14). While many dance groups were willing
to draw cognitive maps and fill out the daily routine
and general surveys, few were willing to participate in
a different part of this study involving GPS.12 After five
weeks of searching in vain for volunteers, the dance
School in China was suspended for three years from 1966-1969 so officials could adapt educational materials to reflect contemporary
political ideology and so youth could be sent to the countryside to “learn from the peasants.” Four million high school and university students were affected by these social policies and most were unable to resume their studies after returning to their homes in the late 1970s.
This affected their later life prospects and accounted for the high illiteracy rate amongst the dancing retirees (MacFarquhar & Fairbank
1991: 215).
12
The data from the GPS portion of the fieldwork is not yet analyzed and not discussed in this paper.
11
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
47
Refereed Full-Papers
group at Honglianbeili agreed to take part for a nominal
fee.13 As the initial study was planned and budgeted
for six weeks in Beijing, it was not possible for me to
continue searching for other groups to participate. I
was, however, able to remain in Beijing for four additional months to achieve non-mapping related fieldwork
goals, including gathering follow-up surveys (N=200)
from members of dancing groups first surveyed in 2004
and 2007.14
Acknowledgements
I wish to thank Galen Cranz, Michael Southworth,
Malo André Hutson and the committee for the Roslyn
Lindheim Award from the School of Public Health at
the University of California at Berkeley for providing
me the means to return to Beijing over the spring and
summer of 2010 so I may conduct follow-up studies on
dancing groups that I met three years ago. I am also
grateful to Yuhuan (Janna) Huang, Xiuhui Zhong, Li
Yan, and Han (Susan) Yafei for providing me valuable
assistance setting up interviews, accompanying me
to site visits and entering data, and to the anonymous
reviewers at the Environmental Research Design Association for offering me very useful and specific advice
on how to restructure this paper. A special thanks to
Hyeon-Ju Rho and Alex Wang in Beijing, who opened
their home to me for five months so I could complete
the fieldwork.
REFERENCES
Berik, Günseli, Xiao-yuan Dong, & Summerfield, Gale. (2007).
China’s Transition and Feminist Economics. Feminist
Economics, 13 (3/4), 1-33.
Chen, Lanyan & Standing, Hilary. (2007). Gender equity in
transitional China’s healthcare policy reforms. Feminist
Economics, 13 (3/4), 189-212.
Elliott, Mark C. (2001). The Manchu Way; the Eight Banners
and Ethnic Identity in Late Imperial China. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Francis, Mark. (1988). Changing values for public spaces.
Landscape Architecture, 78, 54–59.
Franck, Karen A., & Stevens, Quentin. (2007). Tying Down
Loose Space. In K. A. Franck & Q. Stevens (Eds.), Loose
Space: Possibility and Diversity in Urban Life (pp. 1-51).
London: Routledge.
13
14
Xinhua. (2011, February 27). Chinese ministry to extensively
review women’s retirement age. GOV.cn: Chinese
Government’s Official Web Portal. Retrieved March
15, 2011, from http://www.gov.cn/english/2011-02/27/
content_1812306.htm
Graezer, Florence. (2004). Breathing New Life into Beijing
Culture: New “Traditional” Public Spaces and the Chaoyang
Neighborhood Yangge Associations. In S. Feuchtwang (Ed.),
(pp. 61-78). London: UCL.
Hester, Randolph T. J. (1985). Subconscious Landscapes of the
Heart. Places, 2(3), 10–22.
Jiang, Lin. (1995). Changing Kinship Structure and its
Implications for Old-Age Support in Urban and Rural China.
Population Studies: A Journal of Demography. 49 (1), 127.
Le Corbusier. (1973). The Athens Charter [by] Le Corbusier.
New York: Grossman.
Lindheim, Roslyn. (1974). Environments for the Elderly:
Future-Oriented Design for Living? Journal of Architectural
Education (1947-1974), 27 (2/3), 7-56.
Lindheim, Roslyn. (1985). New Design Parameters for Health
Places. Places, 2 (4).
Lindheim, Roslyn & Syme, S. Leonard. (1983). Environments,
people, and health. Annual review of public health, 4 (1),
335-359.
Liu, Jieyu. (2007). Gender dynamics and redundancy in urban
China. Feminist Economics, 13 (3/4), 125-158.
Lynch, Kevin. (1998). Good city form. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT
Press.
Lynch, Kevin. (1960). The Image of the City. The MIT Press.
MacFarquhar, Roderick, & Fairbank, John K. (1991). The
Cambridge History of China: Volume 15, The People’s
Republic, Part 2, Revolutions Within the Chinese Revolution,
1966-1982. The Cambridge History of China (Vol. 15).
Cambridge [England]: Cambridge University Press.
Rivlin, Leanne G. (2007). Found Spaces: Freedom of Choice
in Public Life. In K. A. Franck & Q. Stevens (Eds.), Loose
Space: Possibility and Diversity in Urban Life (pp. 38-53).
London: Routledge.
Shumway-Cook, Anne, Baldwin, Margaret, Polissar, Nayak L., &
Gruber, William. (1997). Predicting the Probability for Falls
in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Physical Therapy,
77(8), 812-819.
Sommer, Robert, & Sommer, Barbara B. (2002). A Practical
Guide to Behavioral Research: Tools and Techniques (5th
ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Smil, Vaclav. (1993). China’s Environmental Crisis: An Inquiry
into the Limits of National Development. Armonk, N.Y: M.E.
Sharpe.
Syme, S. Leonard. (1984). Social support and risk reduction.
Möbius: A Journal for Continuing Education Professionals in
Health Sciences, 4 (3), 44-54.
They needed funding to pay for dancing uniforms and I was willing to pay them for participating in the research.
The locations of these dancing groups are shown in Figure 2, but the comparison of data from general surveys from the three visits to
Beijing between 2004 and 2010 will not be examined in this paper.
48 Refereed Full-Papers
Tang, Kai (2004). Urban Planning System in China: Basic Facts
and Reform Progress. Paper presented at the International
Conference on China’s Planning System Reform. Retrieved
May 4, 2010, from http://www.adb.org/Documents/
Events/2004/PRC_Planning_System_Reform/tang4.pdf
Tomba, Luigi. “Creating an urban middle class: social
engineering in Beijing.” The China Journal (2004): 1–26.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996).
Physical Activity and Health: A Report of the Surgeon
General. Atlanta, Georgia: US Department of Health and
Human Services, Public Health Service, CDC, National
15
Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health
Promotion.
Wu, Liangyong (1999). Rehabilitating the Old City of Beijing
a Project in the Ju’er Hutong Neighbourhood. Vancouver:
UBC Press.
Lu, Xiang. (2003). Beijing: Yuan City - The structure of urban
space under the system of Danwei (thesis). Peking University,
Beijing.
APPENDIX15
The 2010 The Danceable City Daily Routine Survey and the 2010 The Danceable City General Survey are available upon request.
Please contact: carolinechen06@berkeley.edu or if after 2012, carolinechen@post.harvard.edu
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
49
Refereed Full-Papers
The Morphological Evolution
of the ‘Bazaar Streets’: A
Configurational Analysis of the
Urban Layout of Dhaka City
Farhana Ferdous (The University of Sydney,
Australia)
Abstract
The city of Dhaka developed throughout different
phases of a period spanning four hundred years. The
traditional spatial structure and hierarchy of its spaces
have become part of a cultural heritage that includes
historically famous and organically developed bazaar
streets and commercial interfaces. Urban development
of Dhaka city is well known for its spontaneous spatial
structure and indigenous morphology. A ‘space syntax’
technique is used to understand the structure, function,
hidden logic, morphological growth, and evolution of
urban systems. This paper focuses on both the spatial
dynamics of urban structure and on the spatial nature
of the bazaar streets (commercial networks) and aims
to explicate the notion of configurational analysis of
urban growth. By determining the syntactic pattern and
evolution of bazaar streets, this analysis can be usefully
applied to the design of future urban growth.
Introduction and Objectives
Dhaka, the capital and provincial city of Bangladesh
and one of the oldest cities in South Asia, (Figure 1) has
grown in size, scale, and extent throughout its historical and morphological evolution. Over time, the urban
function of Dhaka has changed according to political
and commercial considerations (Ahsan, 1991; Islam,
1996). Although this ancient city gained official recognition four hundred years ago as the capital of the Mughal eastern province of Bengal, its origins extend even
further back. Under the Mughal administration, and up
until 1765 when the British took over colonized India,
Dhaka was the centre of the region’s political, cultural,
and social life (Mamun, 1993). Within that context, the
market centre or bazaar street has remained the city
dwellers’ most important activity hub and still maintains its functionality as a site of commercial enterprise.
This paper provides an historical and morphological
overview of the different phases of the development
of Dhaka city. As a method of analysis, ‘space syntax’
theory is applied to determine whether these changes
have been arbitrary, or whether there has been a hidden
logic behind the pattern of growth over time. The objectives of this paper are as follows:
a. To reveal the structure and function of the urban
system of Dhaka city in the context of historical development
b. To identify the hidden logic and spatial configuration of the ‘Bazaar Streets’ in relation to changing
patterns of urban growth and function
Figure 1 Map of South Asia showing the location of Dhaka, the capital of Bangladesh. Adapted from
(Texas Austin, 2004)
50 Refereed Full-Papers
The analysis leads to the conclusion that other than
historical development, there is some structural and
configurational regularity in relation to the changing
pattern of urban growth. Through this morphological
analysis, the development pattern of the city’s urban
system and the local and global importance of the bazaar streets may be identified.
The Theory of Space Syntax: A Configurational
Analysis
Space syntax is a set of descriptive techniques for
representing, quantifying, and modeling spatial configuration in buildings and settlements. Space syntax theory
describes and measures quantitatively the relational
properties of urban space (Hillier, 1996; Hillier & Hanson, 1984). In this context, the theory of urban space is
described in terms of abstract properties of a topological nature rather than in terms of geometric regularities;
and space is conceived of as a relational pattern, which
can be explored and understood without being directly
visible in its entirety (Bafna, 2003; Penn, 2003). Above
all, space syntax builds a conceptual model to investigate social patterning in spatial content and spatial
patterning in the social content (Hillier, Penn, Hanson,
Grajewski, & Xu, 1993). A network of pedestrian paths
can be described as a hierarchy; that is, the more centrally located path segments; the more likely they are
to host high numbers of people. Thus, the socio-spatial
patterning of the perceived local areas seems most likely to be revealed by the spatial analysis of space syntax.
It assists the analysis of patterns of connection, differentiation, and centrality to characterize and explore
the relationships of urban systems. This paper will now
address the concepts of most important configurational
measurements, i.e., local integration, global integration,
and integration core, in order to provide a clear picture
of spatial measure.
Local and global integration: Among a number of
spatial measures, the most important is ‘integration’,
which is the relative depth or hollowness of any spatial
system seen from any particular point within it. The integration of a space is a function of the mean number of
lines and changes of direction that need to be taken to
travel from that space to all other spaces in the system
(Bafna, 2003). Integration of any line can be computed
in terms of access from all other lines (radius = n, called
global integration or Rn) or those lines that are accessible up to a given number of lines away (for example
radius = 3, called local integration or R3). Global inte-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
gration values are calculated based on all depths within
a global structure of a system, while local integration
indicates a more local structure (Baran, Rodriguez, &
Khattak, 2008).
Integration core: An axial map is a network of the
fewest and longest sets of intersecting lines derived by
drawing the least and longest axial lines of uninterrupted permeability (Baran, et al., 2008; Jiang & Liu, 2010;
Kim & Penn, 2004). The axial map as a distribution
of integration represents the set of the most integrated
axial lines that are collectively known as an ‘integration core’. This is one of the most important measures
treated as a representation of syntactic centrality. The
core of a system consists of 10 percent of the most
highly integrated lines applied uniformly in the interests
of comparability of the total number of spaces, which
take the strongest values (Hillier & Hanson, 1984). The
nature of the integration core, its shape, size, coverage, and so on, depends on the shape, connectedness,
and geometry of the urban system and on its mode of
growth. In most cities or urban areas, integration core
maps follow the main commercial or shopping areas as
a morphologically deep structure, whereas the least integration follows residential functions or comparatively
shallow structures within the overall urban system.
The Morphology of a Bazaar Street
Morphologically, two types of space exist in old
Dhaka, the street and the court. These are connected in a
continuous network of open and semi-open, and public
and semi-public spaces. Streets, when they meet, often
turn into chawks or nodes. Another important element
is a ‘Bazaar Street’, which signifies the commercial
interface or market centre of the city. Typically, in the
indigenous form of a city, the grid structure is muted to
a complex path system. This was the core concept of
generating Islamic Bazaar Streets, as these heterogeneous types of spaces generated human activity at the
pedestrian level. Rapoport depicted the transformation
of simple homogeneous streets into complex indigenous
bazaar streets (Figure 2). These types of bazaar streets
follow the neighborhood or residential area. They are
morphologically indigenous and sometimes of mixed
use in character.
Mahalla is another spatial archetype that characterizes the older part of Dhaka city. Usually, mixed
commercial-residential neighborhoods generated the
mahalla, which were distinct entities, with a homoge-
51
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 2 Transformation of Roman street into Islamic bazaar.
Adapted from (Rapoport, 1977, p. 219)
Figure 3 Mahalla morphology in historical Dhaka.
Adapted from (I. Khan, 1982)
neous population related through similar occupation,
religion, and caste (Siddique, 1991). Thus, mahalla
were simultaneously social, commercial, and spatial
units that helped to generate both morphological and
cultural homogeneity. In old Dhaka, a mahalla constituted a cluster of houses, usually arranged linearly
along the Bazaar Streets (Figure 3). Like typical organic
cities, the earliest communities of old Dhaka were created by mahalla located either along the bazaar streets
or encircled around courtyards. Khan (1982) argued that
this demonstrated a single pattern that gave rise to two
different forms: linear bazaars and circular mahalla, and
suggested that the linear or circular mahallah acquired
their branches concomitant with the later subdivision of
land.
52 Figure 4 Bazaar axis of Muslim city.
Adapted from (Rapoport, 1977)
Khan (1982) further maintained that ecologically
there is no separation between the location of the bazaars and the residential mahallah. Morphologically, the
access street with commercial value was the centre of
the mahalla and was not used to mark boundaries. This
differentiates the physical, regional, and spatial structure of Muslim cities from the indigenous, spatial pattern of old Dhaka. In a typical Muslim city, the bazaar
streets are the main sites of major urban activities (Figure 4). Seen in early Islamic civilizations between India
and Morocco, this would be part of the most important
civic centre of the town, formed and dominated by the
major congregation mosque, bazaar, palace, and/or
fort, and supported by the Muslim cultural (social and
religious) order. Gradually Muslims dominated Dhaka,
Refereed Full-Papers
who rather than being businesspersons or artisans were
either under royal patronage and/or were landowners.
The local morphology and spatial distribution of urban
spaces in indigenous old Dhaka are characterized by a
non-linear overlapping and combination of socio-spatial
structures of different social units and communities.
Historical Evolution of the Urban Layout of Dhaka
City
The historical evolution illustrates how the existing urban morphology transformed into the present
context and how the functional aspects implied by the
layout have changed accordingly. The historical and
morphological development in eight different stages is
highlighted by using syntactic maps from 1608 to 2007.
The bold lines signify the highest integration (R=n)
values that mainly pass through the commercial areas,
and different bands of color denote different ranges
of syntactic measures. The numerical and explanatory
interpretations of these syntactic values are discussed in
the following section.
The syntactic analysis of Figure 6 shows that in PreMughal Dhaka (map of 1608 AD), the global integration
core sits along the oldest market centre, and different
Figure 5 Demarcations between Pre-Mughal
and Mughal Dhaka. Adapted from (DAD, 2009)
Figure 6 Global integration map of Dhaka city in 1608 and 1764. Basic axial map adapted from
(Nilufar, 1997), syntactic analysis (Ferdous, 2010).
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
53
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 7 Global integration map of Dhaka city in 1859 and 1916. (Axial map drawn from the base map
of 1764 that was reconstructed on the basic information of 1859 and 1916).
bazaar streets show equal importance in the integration
analysis. Moreover, the analysis shows the elongated
integration core within the market centre leading in
a northerly direction towards Mughal Dhaka. In the
syntactic analysis of Mughal Dhaka (map of 1764
AD), this core again resides on the market centre of the
Pre-Mughal city and extends towards the northwestern
periphery of the city (Figures 5 and 6). The morphological pattern seems to correspond to the commercial
and administrative cores to a greater extent. The local
integration values suggest that most of the bazaar streets
are locally focused.
In 1859, the global integration core sat along the two
bazaar streets (Figure 7). It took the form of a T connecting the two commercial hubs of the city. During
this phase, the city’s Pre-Mughal Hindu Core comprised
densely inhabited areas that extended towards the
Muslim Mughal Core, i.e., the Chawk, and projected
outwards to the colonial city. This may be referred to as
the period of unification (Nilufar, 1997). Other than the
Mughal core, most of the areas, particularly the extended western part of Chawk Bazaar and the northern
part of the British addition, were loosely integrated. In
the axial map of 1859 Dhaka, it clearly shows that the
integration core largely coincided with the commercial
54 interface of the city, suggesting that the bazaar streets
of Dhaka constituted the city dwellers’ most important
commercial hub. In the map of 1916, the most integrated line connected with the Chawk Bazaar and extended
towards the north, forming a linear integration core
parallel to the river. From the syntactic analysis of 1916,
it can be clearly seen that since the Mughal period, the
bazaar streets along the Buriganga River have retained
their importance, both locally and globally.
As the city extended towards the north, the integration core also extended in a northern direction and concentrated on the geographical centre of the city. From
Figure 9, it is noticeable that in every stage of syntactic
analysis, the integration cores (thick red lines) follow
the bazaar streets or commercial interfaces of the city.
Revealing the historical review of urban development
shows that the evolution and changes of urban morphology clearly cluster in the central area rather than
the periphery. From the syntactic analysis of different
phases of the 19th century (Figure 9), it can be clearly
identified that since the Mughal period the integration
core included the bazaar streets that follow the geographic centre of the city. As Dhaka city extended in a
northerly direction, the integration core followed the
most connected and integrated commercial interface
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 8 Photographs of Bazaar Streets at old and new Dhaka. (Ferdous, 2009)
Figure 9 Global integration map of
Dhaka city in different phases from 1960
to 2001. Basic axial map adapted from
(N. Khan, 2008; Nilufar, 1997), syntactic
analysis (Ferdous, 2010).
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
55
Refereed Full-Papers
of the city. The integration values (Table 1) suggested
that both local and global function of the city expressed
steady growth to the inhabitant.
Analysis of the Spatial Configuration of Dhaka City
Pattern of Axial Segments
The axial structure of Dhaka to some extent looks
like a ‘deformed grid’ in which all the major lines extend by conserving lower degree line relations, allowing ninety-degree line relations to the longer lines. Like
some Western and Arab cities, the local sub-areas in
Dhaka are mostly fitted into the larger scale grid, using
a ninety-degree relation to join the internal streets of the
city’s local areas. Sometimes, this ninety-degree relation transformed into an obtuse or open angle as the internal streets move towards the heart of the local areas
and continue out in another direction (Hillier, 1996).
Hillier provided a theoretical overview of local spatial structure and suggested that when a local area with
a relatively small number of axial segments is considered, the lines which are wholly internal are likely to be
shorter than the lines which connect the interior to the
exterior (Hillier, 1996). This proposition seems apparent in the morphology of Dhaka. Old Dhaka is indicative of a morphology in which the lines that intersect at
open angles tend to be shorter and less differentiated in
length from those of the more localized streets. Conversely, new Dhaka morphology illustrates a pervasive
disposition for longer lines to be incident to others at
open angles, while the more localized shorter lines tend
to be adjacent or close to right angles. Further, it appears that in old Dhaka, lines that are internal to an area
rarely cross a perceived boundary and become part of
another local area. This is in opposition to new Dhaka,
where at least some lines which are part of the local
areas continue into neighboring areas. In Hillier’s terms,
old Dhaka resembles a ‘linearly discrete’ character like
some Arab cities, but new Dhaka resembles the organic
morphological pattern of some west European cities,
e.g., London. Hillier further suggests that the morphological differentiation that distinguishes old from new
Dhaka represents “an example of parametric differences expressing cultural variation in a fundamental
settlement process” within the perimeter of a single city
(Hillier, 1996, pp. 354-355). Although both old and new
Dhaka followed a similar, natural process of formation,
the underlying spatial differences are evident at the local as well as at the global level.
Global integration (Rn)
Figure 10 illustrates the distribution of global integration (Rn) across Dhaka city in 2007. According to the
map, the most integrated lines are located at the geographic centre of the city rather than at the periphery.
More specifically, the longest axial line that mainly
passes through the commercial hub of the city is the
most integrated. The higher Rn values are concentrated
within the more contemporary part of Dhaka. Physically, the most integrated lines, i.e., red in color, connect
the eastern and western parts of Dhaka city, indicating
Table 1 Global and local measures of Dhaka at different times (1608-2007)
56 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 10 Global integration (Rn) map of Dhaka,
2007. (Ferdous, 2010)
Figure 11 Local integration (R3) map of Dhaka,
2007. (Ferdous, 2010)
an extension of the core towards the north. The most
segregated lines are concentrated mainly on the periphery of the city (Figure 10). The southeastern and
northwestern edges are totally isolated from the centre
or integration hub of the city. In general, the global
integration values (Table 1) appear to be confined by
the major roads that tend to spread over a wide range
but do not necessarily traverse from the central to the
peripheral areas. The mean global integration value of
the total urban system (in 2007) was 0.539, with a minimum and a maximum range of 0.210 and 0.838 respectively, suggesting a comparatively medium to high level
of accessibility to the urban system.
integration map (Figure 11), a different distribution
pattern is found, in which the local integrators tend to
be dispersed over the whole plan layout or city rather
than concentrated upon a specific location. A comparative analysis of the local and global integration maps
shows that most of the roads that are globally important
may not necessarily have been integrated locally, and
vice versa. Among all axial lines, very few roads are
integrated both locally and globally. The highest and
lowest R3 values of the total urban system (Figure 11)
are 4.399 and 0.333, respectively. The mean R3 value
is 1.6055, implying that the configuration of Dhaka is
characterized by a comparatively lower level of accessibility with reference to local neighbors.
Local integration (R3)
In contrast to global integration (Rn), local integration (R3) indicates the ‘relative accessibility’ of
specific lines up to three turns away with reference
to their immediate neighbors. By observing the local
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Intelligibility
‘Intelligibility’ is one of the principal measures
related to the configuration of the spatial structure that
plays an important role in determining the legibility of
57
Refereed Full-Papers
any city. In syntactic terms, intelligibility is the theoretical interrelationship of the local and global spatial configuration (Tuncer, 2007). The value of intelligibility is
that it can reveal how the constituted local and global
parts are embedded within the urban system as a whole.
According to Kim, other than reflecting the local and
global relationships of the spatial configuration, intelligibility plays an important role in shaping the ways in
which people perceive and use space (Kim, 1999; Kim
& Penn, 2004). An intelligible system exhibits a smooth
relationship between the local and global measures and
the scale of movement (Hillier, 1996).
Analyses of the scattergrams of the urban layout of
Dhaka city (Figure 12) shows that the spatial configuration of Dhaka as a whole is of moderate intelligibility.
The gentle slope of the regression line further confirms
the perceived continuity between the local and global
configurations of Dhaka. The tangent of the slope or
value of R2 for the scattergram is (Rn R3 = 0.1673).
According to space syntax theory, in any unintelligible
system, individuals face difficulty when inferring the
overall structure and trying to orient themselves to
their immediate contexts. This can be explained by the
relatively low consistency of morphological structures
where the urban system comprises relatively segregated
or excessively locally oriented areas which fail to relate
to the whole. Therefore, this analysis suggested that the
spatial configuration of Dhaka is relatively discontinuous and partially fragmented.
Spatial nature of the integration core
The integration core of a city is mainly formed along
the highest integrated axial lines and conventionally
comprises 5 to 20 percent of the most integrated streets,
depending on the size of the axial map. By examining
the core features, the distribution of relative accessibility and the size, the shape, and location of highly
integrated streets can be identified, which can provide
an understanding of the configuration of the layout of
any city. The 5 to 20 percent most integrated streets
of the urban system are highlighted by thick lines in
Figures 13, 14, and 15 and show how the syntactic core
is clustered.
In the case of Dhaka, the highest integration is
clustered mostly around an arc of the perimeter of the
geographic centre. The curvilinear arc shape integration
core (5 percent highest integrated line) follows a pattern
of linear commercial interface (Figure 13). The form
of this spinal shape core shifts into a hollow, deformed
wheel shape with major outlines when considering
the 15 percent most integrated lines (Figure 14). The
integration core acts as a skeleton, shaping the spatial
structure of Dhaka. When the core analysis is concerned
with the 20 percent most integrated lines of the whole
system, the deformed core loses its hollowness and
concentrates densely in the centre and begins to extend
to the north (Figure 15). Therefore, the curvilinear
arc shape of the integration core forms a continuous
concentrated core at the geographical centre of Dhaka.
Figure 12 Relationship between global and local integration of Dhaka city.
Scattergram by (Ferdous, 2010)
58 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 13 The dark lines show the 5%
most integrated streets. (Ferdous, 2010)
This type of global integration divides the Dhaka city
into four major morphological ‘regions’ or sectors
towards its north, south, east, and west. The core is
mainly formed around the central area and the globally
important commercial road networks with comparatively larger axial segments. Newly developed localities
in Dhaka are more connected to their surroundings, and
the older counterparts are rather isolated. Therefore, in
comparison to the apparently densely knit spatial network of older areas, the contemporary settlements are
more permeable to their surroundings.
Discussion and Conclusion
The spatial structure of a growing city: Dhaka
has grown in size and scale over the last four hundred
years. However, the urban morphology of old and new
Dhaka are quite different. In old Dhaka, streets that are
internal to an area rarely cross a perceived boundary
and tend to be shorter and less differentiated in length
from some of the more localized streets. In new Dhaka,
at least some of the axial lines that are part of the local
areas continue into neighboring areas, illustrating a per-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 14 The dark lines show the 15%
most integrated streets. (Ferdous, 2010)
vasive disposition for longer streets. The more localized
shorter axial lines or streets tend to be incidental to or
close to right angles.
The shifting pattern of the syntactic heart of the
city: The curvilinear arc shape of the integration core
forms a continuous concentrated core at the geographical centre of Dhaka that passes mainly through the
commercial interface. The shifting position of the global
integration core or ‘syntactic heart’ of Dhaka city clearly
clusters in the central area that follows market centres
and different bazaar streets. As Dhaka city extends
toward the north, the integration core follows the most
connected and integrated commercial interface of the
city. Therefore, the core is mainly formed by a globally
important and connected commercial network with a
comparatively larger axial segment.
The spatial nature of integrated road networks:
The longest axial line possesses higher global integration (Rn) values and is concentrated within the area of
the later part of Dhaka. The most integrated line connects the eastern and western part of Dhaka city and
gives direction to extend the core towards the north. The
59
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 15 The dark lines are the 20% most
integrated streets. (Ferdous, 2010)
highly local integrators (R3) tend to be dispersed all
over the city rather than concentrating on a specific location. The most segregated lines are mainly distributed
on the periphery of the city. The average connectivity of
local systems in the central part is also higher than for
the peripheral counterparts. The maximum connected
roads exhibit the highest level of integration and are
sited within the geometric centre of Dhaka and therefore helped to form the global integration core. These
axial segments are more continuous and long, rather
than fragmented.
The spatial relationship between local and global
streets: The spatial configuration of Dhaka as a whole
is of low intelligibility. The lower slopes of the regression lines further confirm low continuity between the
local and global configuration of Dhaka. The spatial
configuration of Dhaka, which is relatively discontinuous and partially fragmented, forms a good, linear, tight
scattergram and crosses the main regression line at a
comparatively steeper angle.
Syntactic analysis allows for a focus on the urban
grid, building complexes, and overall urban structure
in order to identify the spatial dimensions of social,
60 morphological, and configurational conditions. In the
case of Dhaka city, syntactic analysis helps to identify
the pattern of axial structure, the evolution of its spatial
system, and the morphological growth of its urban system. This kind of spatial pattern where old and newly
developed axial segments are juxtaposed side by side
is very unique in character. Like most of the old cities,
the evolution of Dhaka is not planned. However, there
is a hidden logic in the configuration of urban layout.
The hidden logic of urban spatial structure that follows mostly the bazaar streets or commercial centers
has been revealed through the syntactic analysis. For
the case of Dhaka city, from the very early period, the
highest integration lines follow the bazaar streets, and
these commercial belts are still the most integrated lines
for contemporary Dhaka. By determining the pattern of
growth and the location of Dhaka’s bazaar streets, this
analysis explore the structure, function, and logic of the
spatial configuration of urban function for the growing
metropolitan area of Dhaka. The successful intervention
and use of space syntax as a design and decision-making tool is widely accepted in different Western cities.
This study shows that there could be benefits of using
space syntax for future design of cities. It is acknowledged that the results of the analysis may vary over time
with diverse and complex situations and with an interaction of socio-economic, environmental, cultural, and
other factors. In spite of the limitations, this analysis
can be usefully applied to the designing of commercial
belts, urban infrastructure, and urban zones or sprawl,
to determining the future urban growth of a city, and to
evaluating alternative urban planning or design proposals with respect to urban life.
Acknowledgements
This paper is one of the outputs of ongoing PhD
research at the University of Sydney, Australia. The
author is grateful for the greatest support and critique
provided by Dr. Jennifer M. Gamble, honorary associate
at the University of Sydney, Australia.
References
Ahsan, R.M. (1991). Changing pattern of the commercial area of
Dhaka city. S. Ahmed (Ed.), Dhaka past present and future.
Dhaka: The Asiatic Society of Bangladesh, pp. 396- 414.
Bafna, S. (2003). Space syntax: A brief introduction to its logic
and analytical techniques. Environment and Behavior, 35 (1),
17-29.
Baran, P.K., Rodriguez, D.A., & Khattak, A.J. (2008). Space
syntax and walking in a new urbanist and suburban
neighbourhoods. Journal of Urban Design, 13 (1), 5-28.
Refereed Full-Papers
DAD. (2009). City map Dhaka. Retrieved: 25th September,
2010, from: http://www.dhakaamardhaka.com/
english/400YrCelebration/map.php.
Hillier, B. (1996). Space is the machine: A configurational theory
of architecture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Hillier, B., & Hanson, J. (1984). The social logic of space.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hillier, B., Penn, A., Hanson, J., Grajewski, T., & Xu, J. (1993).
Natural movement: Or, configuration and attraction in
urban pedestrian movement. Environment and Planning B:
Planning and Design, 20 (1), 29-66.
Islam, N. (1996). Dhaka from city to megacity. Dhaka: Urban
Studies Programme, Department of Geography, Dhaka
University.
Jiang, B., & Liu, X. (2010). Automatic generation of the axial
lines of urban environments to capture what we perceive.
International Journal of Geographical Information Science,
24 (4), 545-558.
Khan, I. (1982). Alternative approach to the redevelopment of
old dacca. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Katholieke
Universiteit Leuven.
Khan, N. (2008). Study of morphological transformation in
planned residential areas of Dhaka city. Unpublished
MArch, Department of Architecture, Bangladesh University
of Engineering and Technology, Dhaka, Bangladesh.
Kim, Y.O. (1999). Spatial configuration, spatial cognition and
spatial behavior: The role of architectural intelligibility in
shaping spatial experience. Unpublished PhD, University
College London, University of London.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Kim, Y.O., & Penn, A. (2004). Linking the spatial syntax of
cognitive maps to the spatial syntax of the environment.
Environment and Behavior, 36 (4), 483-504.
Mamun, M. (1993). Dhaka smriti bismritir nagari (Dhaka: A city
of memories). Dhaka: Bangla Academy.
Nilufar, F. (1997). The spatial and social structuring of local area
in Dhaka city: A morphological study of the urban grid with
reference to neighbourhood character within naturally grown
areas. Unpublished PhD, Unit for Advanced Architectural
Studies, The Bartlett School of Graduate Studies, University
College London.
Penn, A. (2003). Space syntax and spatial cognition: Or why the
axial line? Environment and Behavior, 35 (1), 30-65.
Rapoport, A. (1977). Human aspects of urban form-towards
a man environment approach to urban form and design.
Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Siddique, K., et.al. (1991). Social formation in Dhaka city-a
study in third world urban society. Dhaka: Dhaka University
Press.
Texas Austin, U. (2004). Southern Asia. Retrieved: 25th
February, 2011, from: http://www.lib.utexas.edu/maps/asia.
html.
Tuncer, E. (2007). Perception and intelligibility in the context
of spatial syntax and spatial cognition: Reading an
unfamiliar place out of cognitive maps. Paper presented
at 6th International Space Syntax Symposium, Istanbul,
Turkey: Faculty of Architecture: İstanbul Teknik
Üniversitesi. from:http://www.spacesyntaxistanbul.itu.edu.tr/
papers%5Cshortpapers%5C127%20-%20Tuncer.pdf.
61
Refereed Full-Papers
Lessons From The Road:
Meaning and Community
Identity Examined Through the
Lens of the Roadside Attraction
By Loraine Fowlow (University of Calgary,
Canada), Christy Hillman-Healey (University
of Calgary, Canada)
Sites for Sore Eyes
The North American roadside attraction is commonly
viewed as an amusing vernacular marker of place, of
notice to the roadway traveler, the passerby, the novelty-seeking tourist, but not of any greater significance
than this. Terminology such as ‘fun’ and ‘wacky’ are
commonly used to describe these structures that take the
form of objects such as the world’s largest duck, ball of
twine, frying pan, peroghy, banana, and so on. Generations of roadside attractions have become common
features of the North American landscape, glorified in
outlets of popular culture as varied as film (such as the
movie “Michael,” 1996), books, and magazines; the
phenomenon has even been used as the name for a film
production company (www.roadsideattractions.com).
The distinction of being ‘the world’s largest’ is a common characteristic for the roadside attraction, naturally
leading to the assumption that these strange structures
were erected solely for the pleasure and entertainment
of the visitor. Often, but not always erected by the
side of a road or highway, the roadside attraction does,
indeed, attract. But is this all there is to their significance? This research is premised on the assumption
that there is more at play with the roadside attraction
than mere entertainment; that at the root of the phenomenon of these oddities lie communities seeking meaningful representation through these apparently simple
structures.
The advancements in automobile travel generated an
opportunity for a new form of engagement with an audience in motion. Physical manifestations of roadside
culture that developed in the 1920s and 1930s included
gas stations, motels, and restaurants, many of which
turned to fantasy as inspiration for their design. Intended as mechanisms for visual marketing to an audience
speeding by in their cars, oddities such as large ducks,
hamburgers and hot dogs, elephants, and milk bottles
62 sprung up across the U.S. roadway system. Andrews
presents this development of fantasy construction as
being uniquely American, given the country’s history
of immigration fueled by dreams of freedom (Andrews,
1984). A national propensity for expressing freedom in
the form of the biggest, best, newest, and fastest found
its way onto the roadsides of America in the form of
‘wacky’ structures now referred to as roadside architecture.
Normatively viewed by the outside world as a visual,
three-dimensional one-liner, the roadside attraction,
upon closer examination, reveals much more than a desire to entertain the passerby. Its very existence speaks
to more than the function of tourist attraction. The
intentions behind roadside attractions are diverse, ranging from the establishment of a regional landmark, the
cultivation of a vernacular identity, commemorating a
historical icon, and other forms of boosterism. The purpose of these roadside oddities spans a variety of intentions, with the most common being a physical representation of a community’s identity. In some instances, the
attraction has taken the form of a resident waterfowl,
wildlife, historical figure, or representational of a local
industry. Fraser Ross of Semaphor Design Company
reflected on the purpose of these roadside attractions
as “historical landmarks in both a literal and figurative
way…[t]hey literally mark a location, but they also
mark a time and place.” (Canada Post, 2010) Roadside
attractions are often referred to as hokey; however the
majority of travelers find them captivating, and as Ross
describes, “[o]n family vacations, we all stop; we stare;
and we rarely leave without a picture.” (Canada Post,
2010) Within the process and result of choosing these
attractions lie potential lessons for urban communities
seeking to manifest local meaning and significance
within the built environment. This research asks why
communities choose to represent themselves through a
roadside attraction, and how the physical manifestation
of their particular roadside attraction identifies them as
distinct from their neighbors.
Architectural scholars have largely ignored the roadside attraction, although roadside architecture and its
cultural import have received some attention (Andrews,
1984; Jakle, 2002; Liebs, 1995; Margolies, 2010; Stager
and Carver, 2006). Scholarly depictions of roadside attractions are scant to non-existent, with publications on
the subject largely in the form of popular literature intended for the lay audience (Butko, 2007; Dregni, 2001,
2006). Descriptive and historical in nature, scholarly
Refereed Full-Papers
accounts of roadside architecture depict the phenomenon from the conceptual framework of the ‘outsider’,
the visitor and tourist, or the historian, almost entirely
neglecting the viewpoints and intentions of the communities that produced the structures. While Andrews
focuses on the programmatic nature of inhabitable roadside attractions (Andrews, 1984), his brief descriptions
of each structure do not delve further than their historical development. Any connection with the community
that spawned the structures is absent throughout the
literature, possibly due to the commercial nature of the
structures under consideration. With most writings on
the subject, the questions of how and why the structures
exist are presumed to be subsumed within the realm
of popular roadside commercial culture. Notably, the
work of the Society for Commercial Archeology and its
membership focuses exclusively on this aspect of the
phenomenon, and is the “oldest national organization
that encourages the study of the unique historic significance of twentieth-century commercial landscape and
culture” (Stager and Carver, xiii). Herein lies a useful
distinction between most structures that fall into the category of roadside architecture, and those referred to as
roadside attractions. Structures erected for commercial
purposes (gas stations, motels, diners, and restaurants)
utilize the unusual and bizarre for marketing purposes,
seeking to gain the attention of the motorist passing by.
Although equally desirous of attention, the communitygenerated roadside attraction couples this with the additional intention of representation of identity.
At this point, a distinction between the Canadian
roadside developments and their U.S. counterparts
requires articulation. While the American development
of roadside architecture and attractions are well documented, analogous documentation of the spread of the
phenomenon to Canada exists primarily in the form of
scattered websites devoted to showcasing roadside attractions for the purposes of tourism (Big Things; Large
Canadian Roadside Attractions), and numerous personal blogs documenting family travels (Alberta’s “Big
Things” Roadside Attractions), with some mention on
Federal and Provincial government websites (Library
and Archives Canada; Travel Alberta). Some provincial governments are providing tourism incentives to
visit the rural communities that have created roadside
attractions, such as Travel Alberta’s Roadside Attraction
Passport program (2010), but there is no scholarly, or
even popular, published accounts of these structures.
Initially documented with delight by aficionados and
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
the curious primarily in the form of photographic and
tourism publications (Margolies, 1993, 1995, 1996,
2010; Patton, 2010; Butko, 2007; Dregni, 2001, 2006),
these structures for many years escaped the attention of
the architectural establishment, which is not surprising,
given the absence of professional architectural input.
Because of the absence of professional design in the
development of roadside structures, one might argue
that roadside architecture and attractions are a form of
vernacular design, and it is in this realm that one finds
some discussion of the subject (Davis, 1997; Fair, 1997;
Liebs, 1995). However, the most noteworthy contribution to the field, which originated directly from the center of the architectural profession, was the seminal 1977
work in of Venturi, Scott Brown, and Izenour, Learning
from Las Vegas, which posited the everyday landscape
and road culture as a new urban form. One year later,
Italian semiotician and novelist Umberto Eco furthered
this shift of interest from the everyday to the spectacular
with his essay Travels in Hyper Reality, documenting
and analyzing wax museums, Ripley’s Believe it or Not,
the Palace of Living Arts and Disneyland as part of his
romp through the wonderland of America. Through his
analysis, as seen through the lens of semiotics, Eco put
forward the built vernacular and spectacular as repositories of significance, and, therefore, as worthy of study
(Eco, 1973).
Since Venturi and Eco, over the course of the last
twenty years or so, scholarly interest in roadside architecture has surfaced, with motor-age landscape scholarship and historic preservation work appearing as a
topic of consideration in academic publications (Vieyra
,1979; Jennings, 1991; Adams and McMurry, 1997).
Similar scholarly interest in roadside attractions has yet
to surface, however.
A recent study of roadside attractions on the Canadian prairies was undertaken by the authors with the
goal of examining the history and meaning of roadside
attractions through visits to communities and interviews
with town residents, municipal officials, and tourism
and economic development authorities. This paper
represents the beginning of the analysis of what is the
first phase of a study that will eventually encompass the
examination of roadside attractions across Canada, and
will culminate in a comparative study of similar attractions in the U.S.
This study encompassed visits to 48 communities
in Alberta, Saskatchewan and Manitoba, across 3,800
miles (6100 km), and included key informant interviews
63
Refereed Full-Papers
with representatives of 22 communities. The database
created for this study includes over 200 communities
across these three provinces with some form of roadside
attraction; these communities will be surveyed through
the form of questionnaires that will cover the same questions utilized in the interviews. The interviews have
been examined for the purpose of this paper with the
goal of lessons learned for urban communities in mind,
the results of which are organized here in the form of
a selection of three community “stories” that have the
most significant and commonly shared lessons.
This is what we learned.
Banana Belt Situated in the southwestern corner of Manitoba, 340
km from Winnipeg, Melita is a community of just over
1,000 that enjoys particularly warm weather for this
part of the country. Known for many years by the TV
weathercasters in nearby Brandon as the “banana belt,”
the residents of Melita have chosen this moniker as representative of their community in the form of a “bright
yellow, Walt Disney-like banana wearing a brown belt
with a buckle that reads “Welcome to the Banana Belt.” 7
As with so many small agriculturally based communities, Melita has experienced a decline in population and
prospects in recent years. Their younger populations
leaving for brighter opportunities in larger centers, these
communities are seeking not just tourists, but visitors and residents. Following the pattern set by other
communities, Melita decided that it needed a roadside
attraction. After years of searching for ways to inject
new life into the community, in 2007 a tourism committee in Melita determined to build an attraction that
would, well, attract. They chose to draw on Melita’s
externally recognized reputation as the banana belt, and
audaciously decided that a giant banana would be their
representation of community identity.
Known as “grasslands bird capital of Manitoba” due
to the abundance of birding opportunities for enthusiasts
who travel from as far away as the U.S., many in the
town felt that a bird would be a more appropriate representation. However, the tourism committee determined
that Melita needed something unique, given the dozens
of bird statues and attractions already in existence across
the Prairies.
The decision to choose a banana drew an immediate and strong response, with residents loudly opposing
the choice of a banana to represent their community. A
vote in the local paper reported opposition of 69 percent,
64 Figure 1: The Banana in Melita, Manitoba
compared with support of 39 percent. A national article
in the National Post headlined the controversy as, “Banana Splitsville,” unable to resist the obvious wordplay.8
Despite the opposition, as well as internal dissention, the tourism committee persevered, and produced
a design that included a blue jay sitting on the arm of
the banana, in deference to those who preferred the bird
capital identifier for the community. Funding for the
attraction was raised through donations and fundraising
efforts (no government funding was used at all), and
the banana was unveiled on August 7th, 2010 during a
weekend event that produced 536 banana splits for attendees and many photo ops with the new celebrity.
Visited two weeks after opening, the banana was
already attracting passersby who couldn’t resist having
their picture taken at the foot of a 30-foot-tall banana.
More visitors have been noticed in town stores by residents, and, according to tourism officials, naysayers are
gradually turning their opinion towards a more positive
view of their banana. The media exposure has been
intense for a small community, which is following the
initial splash of coverage for the unveiling with a new
“Name the Banana” contest that is being covered well
beyond the community. The town has now branded
Refereed Full-Papers
itself as a “town with a-peel.” Time will tell, of course,
if the banana in Melita will fulfill local expectations for
increased visitation and economic benefit, as well as
continue to improve in approval. The controversy and
uniqueness of their attraction has afforded the community with an advantageous opportunity, however, for
building towards fulfillment of their goals.
Lessons Learned I
Proponents of the banana expected a certain degree
of opposition, given their very unusual choice of symbol, but one unexpected outcome for the town was the
bringing together of the community through fundraising efforts, as well as through the debate that produced
broad-based discussion of appropriate representation
for the town. It would appear that everyone had an
opinion of the banana. The debate has been viewed as
positive for the town, “because people are talking about
what to build, not where to go.” 9
Resident participation in meaningful discussions
about community identity was not an expected outcome
of this process, which was essentially closed in nature.
Despite overriding the preferences of the majority of
the population, the end result of the process has the
potential to benefit the community in the short and long
term. The longer-term effects of both process and result
are unknown, but there lies the potential in this experience for the town to continue awareness and decisionmaking regarding both its fate and its physical form. It
will take long-term resident commitment, or, buy-in,
however, to ensure the longevity of the banana as an effective and useful tool for the community through continued inclusion in marketing, branding and events. It
will also take this long-term commitment on the part of
the community to ensure that the banana is maintained
physically, and therefore, maintains its appeal to visitors and outsiders. Although born in an environment of
controversy and dissent, sustainability of the banana as
a working and living town symbol will require broader
support than the relatively small group that brought it
into being.
The strength of will and personality that produced
a giant banana as a symbol for a rural Manitoba town
despite significant opposition is indicative of the results that can be achieved through singular vision and
determination. For good or ill, the result of this process
has initiated a response that can be embraced through
continued effort of promotion and development, or
neglected, and therefore become a missed opportunity.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Of course, even a banana on the prairie may eventually
fail to surprise or interest, but the town of Melita has
the ability at this time to influence its future through a
symbol that was little loved at the outset. Perhaps town
leaders can look to Paris and its once despised, now
famed tower, for inspiration.
What the Heck is a Bunnock?
Situated just off Highway 31, and three miles east of
the provincial border between Alberta and Saskatchewan, the pride of the town of Macklin is in full view and
easy to spot at any speed from the highway. Rising vertically in the air and visually unencumbered by anything
else remotely manmade is a 32-foot, fiberglass bunnock.
The average person will reasonably ask: What is a bunnock? What, indeed.
The word bunnock is Russian for ‘bone’. In this
case, bunnock refers to the anklebone of a horse. Early
settlers in the Macklin area brought with them from
Russia the game of bunnock, apparently developed in
the 1800s by Russian soldiers stationed in Siberia with
nothing to do but attempt playing horseshoes on the
frozen tundra. Failing to penetrate the frozen ground,
and therefore, failing to play the game, the innovative
soldiers utilized something that would stand on the ice
and was in abundance: horse anklebones. And thus,
the game of bunnock was born, and found its way on to
the Canadian prairie in the early 20th century.10
The game remained popular in this region of Saskatchewan, and has spread across the globe to Australia,
Africa, Chile, Mexico, the UK, the U.S. and Japan. As
many as three hundred teams descend each year on the
town of Macklin in August to participate in the World
Bunnock Championship, effectively tripling the size of
the town of 1,400. This singular event provides economic benefit to the area estimated at between $500,000
and $1 million.11
Begun 18 years ago by the Manager of the Macklin
Credit Union and a Credit Union committee, the global
tournament of this highly unusual game has singularly
put the town of Macklin on the global map. Media
coverage has been high and international, as the novelty
of the game has appealed to the curious. Looking for
a means of promoting both the game and the town, the
committee determined that a roadside attraction that
doubled as a tourist booth would be designed and built.
The natural choice, of course, was a bunnock. Vertically
oriented, the shape lent itself well to creating a marker
visible from miles away, as well as providing space for a
65
Refereed Full-Papers
the weekend keep the visitors entertained. The number
of teams allowed to register is limited to 320, presumably to keep the crowds at a reasonable level.
Figure 2: The Bunnock in Macklin, Saskatchewan
tourism booth inside. Constructed of a steel frame and
shaped by chicken wire covered with white fiberglass,
the bunnock is lit from within at night, glowing bright
orange, almost lighthouse-like.
The shape of the bunnock is highly unusual, and
entirely unrecognizable, if not vaguely reminiscent of
… something. Newspaper columnist Dave Barry has
described it this way: “Your family is sure to enjoy
viewing the giant plastic Macklin bone, which looks
vaguely like an enormous naked woman with no arms
or legs or head.”12 That is exactly what it looks like,
which only appears to add to its appeal and mystery.
It is estimated that visitors to the tourist booth
number approximately 2,500 per year, based upon the
numbers who sign the guest book inside, with many if
not all taking their photos with the peculiarly shaped
bunnock. It is unknown, of course, how many stop for
a bunnock photo when the tourist booth is closed, which
includes the winter months. According to Kim Gartner,
Macklin Town Administrator, the bunnock even serves
as background for wedding photos, providing, perhaps,
an interesting contrast to the wedding dress. 13
In addition to the numbers tracked at the tourist
booth are those coming to Macklin for the tournament;
these number 3,500-4,000 each year over the course of
the August long weekend. Most stay overnight in the
town’s campground, and events such as beer gardens,
dances, concerts, and other entertainment throughout
66 Lessons Learned II By all appearances, 18 years after its inception the
tournament and its namesake roadside attraction are
highly successful, as measured by annual visitation to
the town, economic infusion into the community, media
coverage, and international reputation. There appears
to be full community support for hosting the bunnock as
representative of the community’s past cultural heritage, as well as its current international stature as home
to the World Bunnock Championship. Through the
tournament the community’s heritage is celebrated and
renewed annually and into continuing generations; this
is contemporary, living history. Through the roadside
attraction, this identity of heritage is manifest and
broadcast to every vehicle that passes along this major
highway, and well beyond the geographic confines of
the town.
Perhaps herein lies the secret of Macklin’s success:
the authentic coupling of the built attraction with a
living, breathing event that nurtures community spirit,
while simultaneously connecting the town to the larger
world. This community’s relationship with its attraction is not on ‘cruise control;’ it is active and continuous
over a span of almost 20 years. Unlike many towns
visited in this study that are no longer entranced by—or
even interested in—their unusual structure, Macklin
continues to be known as the town with the Bunnock.
As the mayor of the town told the Globe and Mail in
2006, “Bunnock has really put Macklin on the map.
We’re the envy of a lot of small towns. They’re looking
for an event that will bring people in. Well, well, we
have it—we have bunnock.” 14
They also have the three dimensional, 32-foot high
bunnock, a structure that is a real “traffic stopper.” 15
As popular as the game might be, it is the structure that
garners the most media and tourist attention, accessible
as it is to the far larger population who do not even
know what it is. Through continued connection with
the event that brings Macklin to life each year, and the
town’s commitment to maintain the structure physically and symbolically as essential to the community,
there is no reason why the bunnock in Macklin will not
continue to feed local, national, and even international
interest and attention. And in so doing, continue to feed
Macklin as a living, surviving community.
Refereed Full-Papers
Spock Goes Home
Contrary to what popular culture enthusiasts might
think, Vulcan is not actually a planet out there somewhere in the planetsphere; it is, in reality, a small town
in rural southern Alberta. Named in 1915 by the town’s
Canadian Pacific Railway surveyor for the Roman god
of Fire, Vulcan was laid out with streets also named for
other assorted gods and goddesses. A predominantly
agricultural community numbering approximately
2,000 in population, Vulcan’s history has been intricately linked with its agricultural base, and the town was
known regionally for its nine grain elevators, a staple of
the Canadian prairie landscape. Today, only one of the
original nine remains, a silent testament to the changing
face of agriculture over the years. Despite the loss of
the elevators, however, the town maintains strong pride
and attachment to this local history.
With its arrival in the 1960s, and exponential growth
in popularity in the 1970s and beyond, the TV show
Star Trek has organically given Vulcan another identity: home of the intrepid Mr. Spock. What began as
a trickle of visitors looking to have their pictures taken
with town signage mushroomed in the late 1980s as
local business leaders seeking to diversity their failing
economy turned to this ‘gold mine’ in their backyard
for potential benefit.16 Beginning with the formation
in 1989 of the Vulcan Association of Science and Trek
(V.A.S.T.), the town began to engage in promotional
events and activities linking it directly with Star Trek.
In 1991, a radio station in the nearby city of Lethbridge and the Calgary Herald newspaper held a contest, “Voyage to Vulcan,” and in 1993 the first Star Trek
Convention, VulCon 1 (also now known as Spock Days/
Galaxyfest), was held, and included the authors of Star
Trek books as guests. The momentum continued with
this annual event, culminating in the 1995 unveiling
of the Vulcan Starship replica of the famous Star Trek
ship as an entry marker to the town, and the opening
of the newly created Tourism booth at the Trek Station. Seeking to continue building the potential of this
new brand for Vulcan, the town hired Dayna Dickens
from Ontario to serve as Director of Tourism. Vulcan
Tourism quickly developed a new attraction for the Trek
Station, the Vulcan Space Adventure, which garnered
international media attention, as anything Star Trekrelated tends to do. By 2001, the visitor statistics had
been growing steadily, reaching approximately 14,000
per year by the end of 2007.
An attempt in 2007 to hold the premiere of the new
Star Trek movie in Vulcan, despite not having a theatre
in town, ultimately failed, but did succeed in attracting the attention of the man himself: Mr. Spock, also
known as actor Leonard Nimoy. Reaching out to the
town to lend his support for their bid to host the premiere ultimately led to the largest event in Vulcan’s
history: the coming home of Mr. Spock in April 2010.
Held in conjunction with the Calgary Comic Expo, and
attended by the international print and television media,
Figure 3 (left): “Enterprise” float, Welcome Home Spock event, Vulcan, Alberta, April 2010
Figure 4 (right): TrekStation, Welcome Home Spock event, Vulcan, Alberta, April 2010
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
67
Refereed Full-Papers
including the Sci-Fi Channel, the event was covered by
almost 100,000 Internet sites; and drew crowds of an
estimated 6,000 from across the province and attendees
at the Comic Expo. There are over 30 YouTube postings alone for the event. Mr. Nimoy was congenial,
gracious and genuinely interested in the town as he
toured the Trek Station, and later unveiled a statue of
Spock. It was a big day for little Vulcan.
After reaching this branding dream scenario and
pinnacle of international exposure, however, the future
of the relationship between Vulcan and Star Trek lies in
uncertain waters. Within months of the Spock event,
Dickens left her position in Vulcan to take on consulting work, following differences with the Town Council
over strategic planning for Vulcan Tourism. Despite
soaring visitor numbers of over 20,000, and the level of
coverage and exposure, the town is uncertain as to how
to proceed next.17
“It’s time to see what direction Town Council wants
to take the [tourism] department,” says Vulcan Mayor
Tom Grant. “Another question Town Council and
the tourism committee needs to answer is whether the
department should accelerate, break or put on the cruise
control.”18
Lessons Learned III
The reasons for this uncertainty regarding direction
for Vulcan Tourism are unknown, but it is inconceivable why a town that has won the tourism jackpot, so
to speak, and would be the envy of small towns across
the country, would hesitate to continue to capitalize on
this gift of branding and exposure. The very notion of
putting this momentum on ‘cruise control’ would be
unfathomable to anyone in the tourism and economic
development industries.
Perhaps the answer to this oddity may lie in the
source of Vulcan’s fame, which came unbidden and
unexpectedly from the larger world beyond the fields
of southern Alberta. Although initially welcoming the
potential for the economic boon that might follow an
association with Star Trek, the town was not actually
prepared for the level of fame and tourism growth that
resulted, and their very public rebranding as the hometown of Spock. 19 Still attached to their historical association with the nine grain elevators, the local response
to this new identity has been mixed. This is nowhere
more evident than in the cover of the town’s brochure,
which bizarrely overlays an image of the Starship Enterprise over an historic photo of the grain elevators.
68 The town has a difficult choice to make, and it is
doing so without the guidance of experienced tourism or economic development professionals. Whether
Vulcan chooses to capitalize on the opportunities it now
has to continue and grow the new brand, or vacillate
sufficiently until the momentum and interest fade, only
time will tell. If Mr. Spock were asked this question,
however, it is possible that he would choose the logical
path, and advise the town to boldly go where Vulcan
has not gone before.
The dilemma faced by Vulcan’s Town Council is illustrative of the disconnect that can occur when initiatives are externally driven and imposed upon a community and do not authentically arise from within. The
town has been caught off guard by what is, by anyone’s
standards, phenomenal success in achieving a tourism
and development brand that can last far into the future,
and provide the basis for a solid and sustainable economic boon for the community. The fact that the initiative to use the brand to diversify and grow the economy
originated in the town does not negate the additional
fact that the townspeople themselves do not identify
with this brand. This points to the need for a community to achieve buy-in and active participation with
an initiative, particularly when it originates externally.
Without a sense of community ownership, long-term
continuity and sustainability of an initiative will always
be in question.
If You Build It
The examples cited in this paper are but three of
many, and have been chosen for their ability to represent the most significant commonalities found amongst
the larger field of roadside attractions in this study.
Each in its own way manifests noteworthy lessons to be
learned about how these communities function, and, in
turn, offer potential lessons for their urban counterparts
seeking similar goals of meaningful self-identification
through built form.
One of these lessons deals with the need for community participation, buy-in and support in the visualization and design processes. Without the development
of a sense of community ownership in these processes,
a successful outcome may be jeopardized in the longer term, despite the appearance of initial success, as
it is through this sense of ownership that the physical
outcome (whatever it may be) will be maintained and
supported, both physically and economically. The unfortunate scenario of longer term neglect was observed
Refereed Full-Papers
countless times during this study, as numerous roadside
attractions had fallen into disrepair after the initial
vision and energy of a small group of enthusiasts had
decayed over time. Although not a guarantee in and of
itself of longer term sustainability, initial and continuous community support and buy-in, as seen in Macklin,
does contribute positively to the ‘living’ aspect of the
outcome, and therefore, presumably, to its survival.
Another commonality found across most of the attractions studied, but not explicitly articulated, is the
significance of the distinction between the external and
internal point of view of the community in question.
Many communities studied understandably view the
world from the inside out, but do not supplement their
normative viewpoint with that of the outside looking
in. A small but significant example are the communities which do not see the need to couple their roadside
attraction with tourist information, either through joint
physical presence, as in Macklin, or through informative signage. When asked where visitors to the attraction could obtain souvenirs or tourist brochures and
information, the response was often that these could
be found in x store in town, although the visitor had no
means of learning this. The point here is that failure to
understand how it is being viewed by the outside world
or the visitor can likewise lead to failure of intention.
Macklin fully understands this, Melita is on the cusp of
it, but Vulcan verges on missing it altogether. Without
this understanding, communities can fail to connect
the dots—the economic dots, in particular. When the
goal is economic gain, as it is with virtually all of the
attractions studied, this failure can doom the endeavor
entirely, or, at best, put it into ‘cruise control’, resulting
in a failure to benefit from the effort of constructing the
attraction in the first place.
It would appear from these examples, as well as all
of the others in this study, that it can be primarily external validation and attention to the roadside attraction
that is of value to the community, both economic and
otherwise. That much would be obvious, even without a study of any sort. The question that is begged,
though, is: if you build it and they do not come (or like
it), does it matter? The corollary to that question could
be: if you build it and they do come (as in Vulcan),
what then? These questions are faced by communities
large and small who undertake civic projects, whether
they be small roadside attractions or large-scale urban
design. The answer to both questions likely depends
upon where on the spectrum of the commonalities noted
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
above the community in question lies. Although the
rural community’s project may be jeopardized by the
failure to understand the external point of view, the urban community’s work can be just as threatened should
the reverse be true, and the internal point of view be
neglected. Likewise, obtaining community support and
buy-in at the outset of any project can only be beneficial
in the longer term, and assist with short-term success.
These lessons are not new, historically speaking. The
results of this study only point towards, and possibly
confirm, age-old lessons learned but perhaps forgotten
in recent years, as technical considerations and infrastructure development have gained prominence in urban
decision-making. At a time when architects, urban designers and planners are challenged to respond both to
technological as well as civic needs, it is perhaps wise
to recall that most basic of human needs: community.
Endnotes
1 McCulloch, 2010
2 Kirby, 2010
3 Kasikova, 2005
4 Canada Post, 2010
5ibid
6 Wawa News
7 Melita Town website
8 National Post
9ibid
10 Town of Mackin website
11 Interview with Kim Gartner, Macklin Town
Administrator; August 16th, 2010
12Barry
13 Interview with Kim Gartner, Macklin Town
Administrator; August 16th, 2010
14Cook
15 Interview with Kim Gartner, Macklin Town
Administrator; August 16th, 2010
16 Interview with Dayna Dickens, Coordinator
for Vulcan Tourism; July 29, 2010
17 Tipper, July 2010
18 Tipper, August 2010
19 Interview with Dayna Dickens, Coordinator
for Vulcan Tourism; July 29, 2010
69
Refereed Full-Papers
Bibliography
Roadside Attractions: American [examples]
Andrews, J.J. C. 1984. The Well Built Elephant and Other
Roadside Attractions. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Barry, Dave. 2006. “Sites for Sore Eyes,” Jewish World
Review, July 24 <http://www.jewishworldreview.com/dave/
barry072406.php3?printer_friendly> (14 September 2010)
Butko, B. 2007. Roadside Attractions: Cool Cafes, Souvenir
Stands, Route 66 Relics, & Other Road Trip Fun.
Mechanicsburg, PA: Stackpole Books.
Dregni, E. 2001. Minnesota Marvels: Roadside Attractions in
the Land of Lakes. Minneapolis, London: University of
Minnesota Press.
Dregni, E. 2006. Midwest Marvels: Roadside Attractions Across
Iowa, Minnesota, the Dakotas, and Wisconsin. Minneapolis,
London: University of Minnesota Press.
Kirby, Doug. Interview. 2002. “Website of the Week: Roadside
America.” Pop Cult Magazine. <http://www.popcultmag.
com/passingfancies/websiteoftheweek/roadside/roadside1.
html> (14 September 2010)
Lange, Steve. 2004. “Roadside Attractions” American Profile,
August 22 <http://www.americanprofile.com/article/4259.
html> (24 September 2010)
Margolies, J. 2010. Fun Along the Road American Tourist
Attractions. Koln, Germany: Taschen.
Margolies, John. 1981. “The Joys of Roadside America” Historic
Preservation 33(3): 44-47.
McCulloch, Adam. 2010. “America’s Strangest Roadside
Attractions” Travel + Leisure Magazine, August <http://
www.travelandleisure.com/articles/americas-strangestroadside-attractions/1> (14 September 2010)
Sutherland, Cynthia. 1982/1983. “America’s Roadside
Attractions: From Commerce to Camp” The Clarion, Winter:
42-45.
Roadside Attractions: Canada [examples]
(uncredited) 2008. “Banana Splitsville.” National Post, February
18 <http://www.nationalpost.com/life/Great+Canadian+Adv
enture+ways+fill+your+calendar+this+fall/> (10 September
2010)
Alberta’s “Big Things” Roadside Attractions, blog. (August 25,
2010) http://albertaroadsideattractions.blogspot.com/ (5
September 2010)
Big Things: The Monuments of Canada. <http://www.bigthings.
ca/> (14 July 2010)
Cook, Tim. 2006. “Rolling the Bones in Macklin,” Globe
and Mail, July 5 (updated 17 March 2009) <http://www.
theglobeandmail.com/life/article177621.ece> (2 September
2010)
“History of North America.” Library and Archives Canada.
<http://www.collectionscanada.gc.ca/caninfo/ep097.htm>
(25 July 2010)
Karpan, Robin. 2006. Larger than life: Saskatchewan’s big
roadside monuments. Saskatoon: Parkland Publishing.
70 Tipper, Stephen. 2010. “Visitor Numbers to Vulcan
Increases,” Vulcan Advocate, July (Article ID#2677070)
<http://www.vulcanadvocate.com/ArticleDisplay.
aspx?archive=true&e=2677070> (10 September 2010)
Tipper, Stephen. 2010. “Dickens Resigns from Vulcan
Tourism” Vulcan Advocate, August (Article ID#2677103)
<http://www.vulcanadvocate.com/ArticleDisplay.
aspx?archive=true&e=2677103> (10 September 2010)
Town of Macklin website. <http://www.macklin.ca/bunnock.htm>
(10 August 2010)
Town of Melita website. <http://www.melitamb.ca/index.
php?pageid=465)> (14 September 2010)
“Unique Attractions.” Travel Alberta website. <http://www2.
travelalberta.com/en-ab/ThingsToSee/UniqueAttractions/
Pages/default.aspx?Location=Alberta+Central&MetaHashKe
y=E723A4A45ED0754ECA8E4C7BC21FB747&mrkt=Albe
rta> (25 June 2010)
Local/Community [origin | identity]
Amato, J. A. 2002. Rethinking Home: A Case for Writing Local
History. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Benet, Francisco. 1963. Sociology Uncertain: The Ideology of
the Rural-Urban Continuum. Comparative Studies in Society
and History, October 6 (1): 1-23.
Heath, K. 2006/2007. “Assessing Regional Identity Amidst
Change: The Role of Vernacular Studies” Perspectives in
Vernacular Architecture 13 (2): 76-94.
Jacobs, J. 1993. The Death and Life of Great American Cities.
New York: The Modern Library.
Kunstler, J. H. 1996. Home From Nowhere. New York, London:
Touchstone Books, Simon & Schuster.
Kunstler, J. H. 1993. The Geography of Nowhere. New York,
London: Touchstone Books, Simon & Schuster.
Zukin, S. 1995. The Cultures of Cities. Oxford: Blackwell
Publishers.
Roadside Architecture [commercial]
Baeder, J. 1980. Diners. New York: Harry N. Abrams.
Baeder, J. 1982. Gas, Food, and Lodging: A Postcard Odyssey
Through the Great American Roadside. New York: Abbeville
Press.
Davis, T. 1997. “The Miracle Mile Revisited: Recycling,
Renovation, and Simulation Along the Commercial Strip”
Perspectives in Vernacular Architecture (7): 93-114.
Kasikova, Jana. 2005. “2H iconography of the American gas
station.” MArch diss., State University of New York at
Buffalo.
Liebs, Chester, and Marc Trieb. 1980. “The Assessment of
American Roadside Architecture” The Forum: Bulletin of the
Committee on Preservation 2 (1): 1-2.
Liebs, C. H. 1995. Main Street to Miracle Mile: American
Roadside Architecture. Baltimore: John Hopkins University
Press.
Longstreth, Richard W. 2000. The Buildings of Main Street: A
Guide to American Commercial Architecture. Revised ed.
Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira Press.
Refereed Full-Papers
Jakle, J., Sculle, K. 2002. The Gas Station in America.
Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.
Jennings, Jan. (ed). 1990. Roadside America: The Automobile
in Design and Culture. Ames: Iowa State University for the
Society for Commercial Archaeology.
Margolies, J. 2010. The End of the Road: Vanishing Highway
Architecture in America. New York: Penguin Books.
Margolies, J. 1996. Pump and Circumstance: Glory Days of the
Gas Station. New York: Bulfinch Press.
Margolies, J. 1995. Home Away From Home: Motels in America.
New York: Bulfinch Press.
Margolies, J. 1993. Signs of Our Times. New York: Abbeville
Press.
Sculle, Keith A. 1982. “Boosterism and Architecture: The
Origins of Foeller, Schober, and Stephenson’s Gasoline
Station Designs” Pioneer America 14 (1): 1-14.
Venturi, R., Scott Brown. 2004. Architecture as Signs and
Systems: for a Mannerist Time. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
Venturi, R., Scott Brown, D., Izenour, S. 1977. Learning From
Las Vegas. Cambridge and London: The MIT Press.
Vieyra, D.1979. Fill’er UP: An Architectural History of
America’s Gas Stations. New York: Collier Macmillan.
Outcomes [guidebook]
Florida, R. 2008. Who’s Your City? How the Creative Economy is
Making Where to Live the Most Important Decision of Your
Life. Toronto: Random House Canada.
Judd, D., and Fainstein, S., (eds). 1999. The Tourist City. New
Haven and London: Yale University Press.
Kolb, B. 2006. “Tourism and Marketing for Cities and Towns.”
New York, Paris, Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Stager, C. and Carver, M. (eds). 2006. Looking Beyond the
Highway: Dixie Roads and Culture. Knoxville: University
of Tennessee Press.
Ward, S. 1998. Selling Places: the Marketing and Promotion of
Towns and Cities, 1850-2000. New York: Routledge.
Pop Culture [popularity]
Canada Post Corporation. 2010. “Canada Post Launches
Roadside Attractions Collection - Wawa Goose Included.”
Wawa News, July 5. <http://www.wawa-news.com/index.
php?option=com_content&task=view&id=6536&Itemid=11
3> (14 September 2010)
Canada Post Corporation. 2009. “Roadside Attractions”
<http://www.canadapost.ca/cpo/mc/personal/collecting/
stamps/2009/2009_jul_roadside_attraction.jsf.> (14
September 2010)
Kirby, D., Smith, K., Wilkins, M. 1996-2010. RoadsideAmerica.
com. <http://www.roadsideamerica.com/> (14 September
2010).
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Lambert, Steve. 2010. “Travel: Some folks plan vacations
around roadside attractions” TheRecord.com, May 28.
Metroland Media Group Ltd. <http://news.therecord.com/
article/719065> (September 14, 2010)
Patton, P., Peatross, C., Heimann, J. ed., Margolies, J. photog.
2010. John Margolies: Roadside America. Koln, Germany:
Taschen.
Society for Commercial Archeology. <http://www.sca-roadside.
org/> (10 August 2010)
Solonyka, Ed. 1998-2010. Large Canadian Roadside Attractions.
<http://www.roadsideattractions.ca/> (24 September 2010)
History [evolution]
Canadian Oral History Association. <http://www.canoha.ca/
reference> (28 July 2010)
“Cultural Resources of the Recent Past,” CRM Thematic issue 16
(1993).
Buckley, Patricia. 1995. “Jumbo Oranges & Giant Lemons” SCA
Journal, Spring-Summer 13 (2): PAGE NUMBERS.
Francis, Daniel. 2006. A road for Canada: the illustrated story of
the Trans-Canada Highway. North Vancouver, B.C.: Stanton
Atkins + Dosil.
Haney, Gladys J.. 1942. “Paul Bunyan Twenty-Five Years after”
The Journal of American Folklore 55(217): 155-168.
Hollis, Tim. 1999. Dixie before Disney: 100 Years of Roadside
Fun. Jackson, MS: University Press of Mississippi.
Longstreth, Richard. 1991. “The Significance of the Recent past”
APT Bulletin 23(2): 12-24.
Monaghan, David W. 2002. Canada’s “new main street”: the
Trans-Canada Highway as idea and reality, 1912-1956.
Ottawa: Canada Science and Technology Museum.
Patrick, Kevin J. 1995. “The American Roadside Giant: Myth,
Meaning and Cultural Identity” SCA Journal, SpringSummer 13 (2): PAGE NUMBERS.
Sculle, Keith A. 1990. “Oral History: A Key to Writing the
History of American Roadside Architecture” Journal of
American Culture 13(3): 79-88.
Branding | Theme [application]
Eco, U. 1983. “Travels in Hyper Reality,” in Travels in Hyper
Reality. San Diego, New York, London: Harcourt Brace &
Company.
Farrelly, E. 2008. Blubberland: The Dangers of Happiness.
Cambridge and London: The MIT Press.
Gottdiener, M. 1997. The Theming of America: Dreams, Visions,
and Commercial Spaces. Boulder: Westview Press.
Hannigan, J. 1998. Fantasy City. London and New York:
Routledge.
Sorkin, M., (ed). 1994. Variations on a Theme Park. New York:
Hill and Wang.
71
Refereed Full-Papers
Children’s Conception of
Natural Environments and
Associated Play Activities in
Santiago, Chile
Daniela Casanello Frisius (The University of
Melbourne, Australia), Raymond James Green
(The University of Melbourne, Australia)
Abstract
This study investigated preferences of children in
Santiago, Chile, for natural environments and associated natural elements, as well as their desire to use these
environments and elements in their play activities. A
total of 185 children from four schools in middle-lower
income areas of Santiago were surveyed. The children
participating in the study were eight and nine year olds
in the third grade. Two of the four schools are located in
the urban core of Santiago (N=110) and two are located
on the urban fringe of the city (N=75). The later two
groups have a greater possibility of having interaction
with nearby natural settings within the context of their
everyday lives than do the two urban core groups. Each
child was given paper and color pencils and asked to
draw a natural environment in which they would prefer
to play in (drawing 1) and represent themselves playing
in a natural environment (drawing 2). Results indicate
that children from all schools identified similar elements, such as the sun, trees, and lawns. However, there
were some noticeable differences between the urban
core versus the urban fringe groups. Children living
in the urban core drew proportionally more cultural
features, such as houses and streets, within the depicted
scenes than did the urban fringe children.
Introduction
One of the unfortunate consequences of the growth
of cities worldwide has been that less open space is
available for the inclusion of ‘natural environments’
within cities and the benefits such environments can
offer urban dwellers (Kellert, 2005). This is a problem
that particularly affects the lives of inner-city children,
leaving them with fewer opportunities to have contact
with elements of the natural world and limiting their
opportunities to play in ‘natural’ environments.
72 Over the last two decades, urban children in many
cities around the world have been afforded less opportunity to have contact with natural environments within
the urban environment as cities continue to grow (Kellert, 2005), resulting in what Louv (2008) has referred
to as ‘nature deficit disorder’. This has been described
as a lack of contact for children with natural environments, which is a consequence of fragmented urban
open space networks, parental apprehension about
letting their children play outdoors, and the fact that
in many families both parents work and children are
spending more time at school—and when they are out
of school, they tend to spend more of their free time on
the computer or watching television (Louv, 2008).
Yet play within the context of more natural environments has been shown to be beneficial to the physical
and psychological development of children. Numerous
studies have suggested such benefits in terms of the
mental, physical, and emotional development of children through greater stimulation of their imagination,
enhanced creativity, better social skills, self-esteem, and
increased learning (Fjørtoft, 2004; Fjørtoft & Sageie,
2000; 1997). Cohen (1994) suggests that benefits that
children can obtain by playing in natural environments
are derived essentially from the fact that such activity
encourages greater ability for exploration of the environment.
Exposure to natural settings in urban settings, and the
activities that these settings afford, can help to increase
children’s overall awareness of their environment, to
develop a greater ability to think creatively, and to be
more independent. It can also provide them with more
diverse sensory experiences and encourage greater peer
interaction (Valentine, 2004). Having opportunities
to play within the context of more naturalized environments in urban settings can also help to stimulate
children’s curiosity and increase their reaction times
(Kellert, 2005).
The advantage of providing play environments that
incorporate natural environmental elements instead
of the more typical formal built playgrounds, is that
they are not static. Natural environments change over
time, both progressively and cyclically, in the short and
longer term, and thus are inherently more complex than
built play settings. For example, due to changing seasons, changes over the day, and the complexity of natural phenomenon, children are afforded what the Kaplans
(Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989) refer to as ‘soft-fascination’.
Refereed Full-Papers
Natural Environment
The concept of a ‘natural environment’ is somewhat hard to precisely define (Wohlwill, 1983). The
definition and meaning of ‘natural environment’ has
also changed over time (Tuan, 1978). In addition, the
notion of a natural environment will be associated with
different meanings for different persons, depending on
a host of factors, such as their environmental history
and the way they encounter nature in the city (Laurie,
1979). People may also have an inherent, evolutionally
conditioned affinity for natural environments over built
settings, or what has been termed as ‘biophilia’ by Kellert (2005). The concept of ‘nature in the city’ or ‘urban
nature’ is something most often afforded by urban parks
and other types of urban open spaces, such as conserved
land along rivers (Laurie, 1979; Low, Gleeson, Green,
& Radovic, 2005).
Over the last several decades, urban populations have
increasingly sought out such places within cities so they
can have more opportunities to experience natural environments, often simply as a place to seek refuge from
the stresses of urban life (Wohlwill, 1983).
For the purpose of the research presented here,
natural environments are being defined using Kaplan
and Kaplan’s (1989) definition, which they employed in
their book The Experience of Nature: A Psychological
Perspective and Rachel Kaplan’s The Role of Nature
in the Urban Context (Kaplan, 1983). This definition
of natural environments includes green areas that can
be found in cities, such as parks, backyards, riversides,
schoolyards, etc. Such environments have the potential
to be instrumental in urban children’s everyday experiences of nature, which is the focus of the research
presented here.
Nature and the Play Environment
Most outdoor, ‘natural’ environments experienced
today by urban children are found in the form of designed landscapes. The majority of playgrounds today
lack diversity; they are usually composed of manufactured play equipment, some grass and maybe trees, but
the natural elements are often not attractive enough to
engage children mentally and physically (Cosco, 2007).
Yet the complexity that natural environments can offer
children can help promote a greater variety of play
activities than do built, manufactured play equipment.
Play in natural environments also can help to improve
children’s motor skills, including their balance and coordination, as well as providing them with a more men-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
tally and emotionally stimulating environment (Fjørtoft,
2004; Fjørtoft & Sageie, 2000).
Natural environments in the context of urban settings can offer a range of play opportunities, including
opportunities for exploration and adventure, which can
enhance children’s play behavior, creative potential, and
the chances that they will have greater social interaction with other children (Burdette & Whitaker, 2005;
Cosco, 2007; Freeman, 1995; Kirkby, 1989). It has also
been suggested that creative and imaginative play are
promoted by greater amounts of green open spaces that
include vegetation, such as trees, where children are
afforded more open, flexible free play opportunities
(Faber Taylor, Wiley, Kuo, & Sullivan, 1998).
Research Aim
The aim of the research presented here was to better
understand preferences for natural play environments of
urban children living in Santiago, Chile. Differences in
preferences between children in the inner city and children on the urban fringe were explored, as were gender
differences.
Method
Participants
A total of 185 eight and nine year old children (third
grade) participated in the study. They were students
from four schools in three different municipalities
(Puente Alto, Conchalí, and Peñalolén) in Santiago,
Chile. These schools were selected according to their
distance to natural environments and the socio-economic status of their catchment area, with two of the schools
in highly urban areas and two in urban fringe areas.
Household income of their families was middle-low
(average income ranged from US$475 to US$850 per
month). A near equal number of males (51 percent) and
females (49 percent) were included in the sample.
The specific age range of the children was selected
because past research has suggested that values for
nature typically occur between six and twelve years old.
At this age, children seem to start to understand and
get more interested in the idea of nature and the natural
world. Also at this age they start to expand their play
environment, motivated by curiosity and a desire for
more creative play activities (Kellert, 2005; Moore &
Young, 1978).
Procedure
Children from the selected schools were asked to fill
73
Refereed Full-Papers
out a small survey and do two different drawings. The
survey asked them about the total time they spent, and
places where they usually played, outside. They were
also asked their gender, how they get to school (bus,
car, walking, bicycle, other), how long the trip from
their home to the school is, and if they had a plaza or
park near their homes (if they did, they were asked to
write its name).
They were also asked to create two drawings. The
first drawing was to be of a natural environment where
they liked to play or would, if they could, play. In the
second drawing they were asked to draw themselves
playing in a natural environment. The drawing task
took between one and one and half hours to perform.
Each child was given a pack of colored pencils and two
sheets of drawing paper.
Once the drawings were collected from the four
schools, they were content analyzed. This entailed tallying the frequency of elements/activities depicted and
the percentage of area that each element in the drawings
took up on the paper. It was assumed that preferences
for types of natural environments and natural elements,
and the activities the children would like to do in those
environments, would be reflected in the frequency of elements mentioned and the structure (percentage of area
the elements took up) of their drawing. With respect to
this spatial analysis, the size of elements was measured
and these measurements were then translated to relative frequencies (percentages) to reflect how much the
individual elements filled the drawings. This resulted in
the assignment of a percentage of the area of the sheet
for every element, with the whole drawing area being
representative of 100 percent.
For the second drawing, each child was questioned
about the play activity they had drawn. This was done
to clarify their intent with respect to the activity they
were depicting in their drawings, since some of them
were difficult to interpret purely by visual inspection
(e.g., observing nature, playing with a ball). The various activities represented in the drawings were coded
so that each of the codes were mutually exclusive and
comprehensive.
Results
Survey
As mentioned, the children were asked to respond
to the different questions with respect to their outdoor
play activities, where they performed these activities,
and how long they would engage in these activities.
74 The children reported that they played outdoors for an
average of 4.3 days each week and 2.0 hours per day
(see Table 1). The places most frequently drawn as play
areas were parks/plazas and streets, both depicted at a
frequency of 24 percent (see Table 2). However, there
were some differences found between children living
in the inner city and in the urban fringe. Children living
in the urban core drew themselves playing in the streets
more frequently than the children in the urban fringe,
who included parks and plazas most frequently. Table
1 illustrates these differences. It also illustrates the
importance of backyards for the urban fringe children.
In addition, some children in the urban core schools
mentioned the inside of their houses, even when they
were specifically asked about outside play environments. Other children simply wrote playing “outside the
house,” without clarifying if this referred to the street,
yard, or other outdoor setting.
Drawing 1: What elements did children prefer in a
natural environment?
The aim of the first drawing was to find out what
children considered to be natural environments and their
preferences for different places and elements in these
environments (see Table 3). The category ‘countryside’ was the most frequently drawn as a favorite place
across all groups. ‘Countryside’ was considered to be
unstructured environments that could not be defined as
farms or parks. More structured ‘parks’ and ‘plazas’ and
built playgrounds were mentioned next most frequently
Refereed Full-Papers
by all children. However, again there were differences between groups. The urban fringe children drew
playgrounds and ‘forests’ more frequently than did the
urban core children, and the elements included in these
environments covered a higher percentage of the area
of drawings than did other elements. ‘Houses’ identified
as natural environments were also drawn frequently by
inner-city children.
Gender differences are illustrated in Table 3, which
shows that boys included forest and beach scenes more
frequently than did girls, who depicted houses more
frequently.
With respect to the importance of each element in
the drawings, as measured by frequency of depiction
of the different elements in the drawings (see Table 4),
the results indicate that, in general, the sun (90 percent), trees (84 percent), and lawns (74 percent) were
most frequently depicted (see Figure 1 and Figure 2).
‘People’ were the fourth most represented element with
52 percent, followed by the sky (51 percent), clouds (44
percent), and mountains (42 percent).
The percentage of the drawing that each element
filled was analyzed to help shed light on how the children valued each element (see Table 5). The element
with the highest percentage of coverage, as would be
expected, was the sky (31.5 percent), followed by trees
(15.5 percent) and lawns (12.0 percent). Some important differences were detected with respect to the proximity of the schools to natural settings. Children from
schools located in the inner city drew a higher amount
of urban elements than did the children from schools in
the urban fringe.
Figure 1
Drawings illustrate the importance of the sun, grass and trees
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
75
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 4
Frequency of elements included in drawing 1
Table 5
Percentage of area of drawings by depicted element in
drawing 1 by residential status*
Table 6
Frequency of elements in drawings by depicted element in
drawing 1 by residential status*
Schools located in the Urban Core vs. Urban Fringe
The first three elements mentioned by the children in
both the urban core and fringe schools were the same
(see Table 6). However, people were included more
frequently by children living in the urban core, ranked
in fourth place, while children living in the urban fringe
illustrated people as the fifth most frequently depicted
element, just after the sky. Another important difference
was that children living in the urban core included ‘water’, ‘animals’, and ‘houses’ more frequently than did
the urban fringe group (see Figure 2). Children living in
the fringe areas included ‘playgrounds’ in their drawings more frequently (see Figure 3).
The percentage of spatial area that each element took
up in the drawings also exhibited some differences (see
Table 5). Children in the urban fringe drew mountains,
the largest of depicted elements, and they were the third
most frequently depicted elements, just before trees.
However, houses took up more of the drawings made
by children in the inner-city schools.
76 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 2
Drawings of children from urban core schools
Figure 3
Drawings of children from urban fringe schools
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
77
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 7
Frequency of elements depicted in drawing 1
differentiated by gender*
Gender Differences
There were also some differences between females
and males across the entire sample (see Table 7). The
main differences were found to be related to females
illustrating flowers and insects (mostly butterflies) at
a higher rate than males, while males depicted kites
and balls more frequently. More than half the girls (57
percent) depicted flowers as compared to the boys (22
percent).
Drawing 2: Favorite Activities in Natural
Environments
The second drawing was aimed at understanding
the kinds of activities that children liked to perform in
different natural environments. The most frequently
described activities (see Table 8) included playing with
a ball (13 percent), flying a kite (12 percent), jumping
rope, and playing soccer (10 percent), followed by playing on swings (5 percent). Some of the children were
asked to clarify the activities depicted in their drawings,
because it was not always clear to the researcher, for example, when they tried to illustrate ‘observing nature’.
78 Table 8
Activities listed by children in drawing 2*
Children Living in the Urban Core vs. Urban Fringe
The activity with the greatest difference in frequency
of mention between the urban core and urban fringe
groups was playing soccer (see Table 8). Children in the
urban fringe expressed a stronger interest in soccer (15
percent) than did the urban core children. The children
living in the urban core also identified activities related
to urban places, for example jumping rope, bike riding,
eating ice cream, and skating than did the urban fringe
children. On the other hand, children living in the urban
fringe mentioned activities related with natural environments, such as observing nature, jogging, and playing games (hide and seek between trees and touch tag
games).
Refereed Full-Papers
Gender Differences
Girls and boys also exhibited differences in terms of
preferred activities (see Table 8), with girls preferring
to jump rope (20 percent) more often the boys, who
preferred to play soccer more often (19 percent). Flying
a kite was also an activity preferred more by boys.
Discussion
This study attempted to identify preferences of
children for different elements/activities associated
with natural environments, to identify similarities and
differences between children living relatively farther or
closer to natural environments and any possible gender
differences.
Results suggest that the time that the children spend
outside (number of days per week and hours per day)
does not differ much between children living in the
inner city and those in the urban fringe. However, there
were important differences in the types of places where
they liked to play between the inner city and urban
fringe groups. Children living in the urban core mentioned playing in the streets more than did the urban
fringe children, which could be explained by the urban
core children having less access to plazas and parks
and less proximity to nearby natural environments
compared to children in the urban fringe areas. Another
important finding was that the urban fringe children
identified their backyards as an important place to play
more often than did the urban core children, which may
be explained by the fact they are more likely to have
backyards at home. Playing inside their homes was also
depicted by some of the children from the urban core
schools, for example playing with their computers at
home.
Projective drawings have been studied and used as a
research method for a long time, and they have proven
to be a good method of communication to approach
and work with children (Malchiodi, 1998) because they
allow them to share their feelings, ideas, and observations with respect to how they play (Malchiodi, 1998;
Pinciotti & Weinstein, 1988).
In the research presented here, the children’s drawings included a range of different landscapes representative of natural environments. Countryside scenes
were a favorite setting for all children. In addition,
playgrounds and parks were also depicted as natural
environments, which is consistent with our definition
of natural environment (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). The
incorporation of rain forests in the drawings was a
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
surprise, since in Santiago the forest is a sclerophyllous
type (a low, semiarid type of forest). Children may have
seen the rain forest in the south of Chile during vacations or on television. Boys included forests more than
girls; which may be explained by the fact boys might
take more risks in playing and therefore venture into
forested areas more frequently than do the girls.
The natural elements identified most often by all the
children were the sun, trees, lawns, and mountains. This
finding is similar to that found by Loukaitou-Sideris
(2003), where the natural elements that children liked
most in certain public spaces (a school yard, a community center, and park), were grass, trees, and flowers.
The most frequently drawn element drawn by children in our study was the sun. This can be explicated by
the fact that it is sunny in Santiago most of the time. In
Moore’s (1986b) study within a schoolyard in Berkeley,
California, the sun was also one of most important elements frequently drawn by children. In our research, the
sun was most frequently drawn behind the mountains,
another characteristic that may be unique to our sample
of children because mountains were another element
also frequently depicted in the children’s drawings,
which is understandable as the Andes Mountains, which
surround and tower above Santiago, are one of the most
visible aspects of the city’s geography.
This study also provides evidence for the importance of vegetation in the landscape, especially trees.
Schroeder’s (1982) research exploring preferences for
features of urban parks and forests found that vegetation, such as trees and grass, were mentioned most often
by the adults who were the respondents in that study,
followed by water elements, which mirrors the results
of the study that was presented here.
The results of this study also found that children living in the inner city included more urban elements (e.g.,
houses, street elements, and people) in their drawings
of what they considered to be natural environments.
This might be explained by the fact these children often
lack natural environments that afford play opportunities
and have less vegetated open spaces than do the urban
fringe children. Children living in urban fringe areas
have more opportunities to access nearby nature, such
as the mountains and farms, making interaction with
natural environments much easier for them, in addition
to more vegetated plazas and open space areas being
closer to their homes. People were also included with
a relatively high frequency in the drawings, with most
including themselves alone or playing with friends, the
79
Refereed Full-Papers
latter suggesting the importance to them of having their
peers within the play environment.
Differences between the urban core and fringe
groups were also found with respect to water features.
Children in the inner city included water elements at a
higher frequency than did the urban fringe group, which
would be also expected in the suburban schools since
all children are attracted to water features.
It was found that the most favorite activities for both
groups was informally playing ball and flying kites.
Moore (1986a) also found that informal ball playing
was one of the most frequently identified activities in
children’s play. The data was collected in August/September, a time of the year when most children fly kites
in Chile, because it is a popular cultural activity. As a
consequence, this result might be influenced by seasonality.
Activities that the children preferred to do within natural environments also suggested differences between
genders. Girls liked to jump rope, and boys preferred to
play soccer. Soccer is one of the most popular sports in
Chile, especially with males. Loukaitou-Sideris (2003)
contends that the activities that most encourage children
to interact with outdoor settings are related to various
types of sports and to playground activities, which was
also found in this study in that 26 percent of respondents identified soccer, swimming, jogging, and other
sporting activities, with soccer being the most popular.
Playground activities (11 percent) were also referred
to many times, with swings the most well-liked type of
playground equipment. In Moore’s (1986a) research,
swings were also mentioned frequently as a preferred
meeting place within playgrounds. It is important to
emphasize, however, that our results suggest that natural elements, e.g., trees, were found to have a higher
preference than traditional play equipments for the
children.
Overall, these findings help to illustrate children
preferences for different elements/activities associated
with natural environments. Some findings corroborate
previous research, while others provide important additional information about children’s responses to natural
environments, their use as play settings, and differences
between children depending on the proximity to nearby
nature and their gender in Santiago, Chile.
80 Implications and Future Research
The study reported here is the first part of a larger
research project, using a variety of different research
methods, which aims to explore children’s preferences
for natural environments, natural elements, and the
types of activities they like to perform within natural environments. The results of this first part of the research
will be used to inform future research by helping to
identify stimuli elements that can be used in administering other data collection methods aimed at gaining a
much more in-depth understanding of the way children
conceptualize nature, natural environments, and the
types of recreational activities they like to undertake
within, or using, these environments and features in the
context of urban environments.
Most parks in Santiago are equipped with traditional
manufactured play equipment, mowed grass lawns, and
sometimes pruned shrubs. But these environments—
playgrounds, parks, or plazas—often lack elements
of nature, including colors, smells, and other natural
environmental stimuli associated with them.
These findings highlight the importance of creating
more natural open spaces for urban children to play,
incorporating natural elements such as trees, grass,
flowers, water features, and animals that were identified in this study as those features most preferred with
respect to their ideal play environments. The ideal play
environments for these children should be able to support individual activities, such as informal ball playing
and kite flying, as well as various group activities, such
as soccer and other outdoor games (hide and seek and
tag games), in addition to providing places for passive
recreation and simply allowing children to gain connection with nature in the city. Perhaps traditional play
equipment, such as swings, could be included in these
environments, but not to the exclusion of more natural
environmental features.
This research explored children’s conceptions of
nature to inform the design of urban open spaces,
playgrounds, and health care facilities. Understanding
children’s opinions in this way is crucial in helping to be
involved in the design of their own play environment.
The information gathered from the study presented here
can supply important information and assist as in developing guidelines for environmental designers and urban
planners with regard to considering children’s understanding of natural environments and their preferences
for playing within these environments.
Refereed Full-Papers
References
Burdette, H., & Whitaker, R. (2005). Resurrecting Free Play in
Young Children: Looking Beyond Fitness and Fatness to
Attention, Affiliation, and Affect. Archives of Pediatric and
Adolescent Medicine, 159(1), 46-50.
Cohen, S. (1994). Children and the Environment: Aesthetic
Learning. Childhood Education, 70(3), 302-305.
Cosco, N. (2007). Developing Evidence-Based Design:
Environmental Interventions for Healthy Development of
Young Children in the Outdoors. In C. W. Thompson & P.
Travlou (Eds.), Open Space: People Space (pp. 125- 135).
New York: Taylor and Francis.
Faber Taylor, A., Wiley, A., Kuo, F., & Sullivan, W. (1998).
Growing Up in the Inner City: Green Spaces as Places to
Grow. Environment and Behavior, 30(1), 3-27.
Fjørtoft, I. (2004). Landscape as Playscape: The Effects of
Natural Environments on Children’s Play and Motor
Development. Children, Youth and Environments 14(2),
21-44.
Fjørtoft, I., & Sageie, J. (2000). The Natural Environment as
a Playground for Children Landscape Description and
Analyses of a Natural Playscape. Landscape and Urban
Planning 48(1-2), 83-97.
Freeman, C. (1995). Planning and Play: Creating Greener
Environments. Children’s Environments, 12(3), 165- 176.
Kaplan, R. (1983). The Role of Nature in the Urban Context. In
I. Altman & J. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behavior and the Natural
Environment (Vol. 6). New York: Plenum Press.
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature: A
Psychological Perspective. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Kellert, S. (2005). Building for Life: Designing and
Understanding the Human-Nature Connection. Washington:
Island Press.
Kirkby, M. (1989). Nature as Refuge in Children’s
Environments. Children’s Environment Quarterly, 6(1), 7-12.
Laurie, I. (1979). Nature in Cities: The Natural Environment
in the Design and Development of Urban Green Space.
Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Loukaitou-Sideris, A. (2003). Children’s Common Grounds: A
Study of Intergroup Relations Among Children in Public
Setting. Journal of American Planning Association, 69(2),
130-143.
Louv, R. (2008). Last Child in the Woods. Chapel Hill: Algonquin
Books of Chapel Hills.
Low, N., Gleeson, B., Green, R., & Radovic, D. (2005). The
Green City: Sustainable Homes, Sustainable Suburbs.
Sydney: University of New South Wales Press.
Malchiodi, C. (1998). Understanding Children’s Drawings. New
York: The Guildford Press.
Moore, R. (1986a). Childhood’s Domain: Play and Place in
Child Development. London: Croom Helm.
Moore, R. (1986b). The Power of Nature Orientations of
Girls and Boys Toward Biotic and Abiotic Play Settings
on a Reconstructed Schoolyard. Children’s Environments
Quarterly, 3(3), 52- 69.
Moore, R. (1997). The Need of Nature: a Childhood Right.
Social Justice, 24(3), 203-220.
Moore, R., & Young, D. (1978). Childhood Outdoors: Toward a
Social Ecology of the Landscape. In I. Altman & J. Wohwill
(Eds.), Children and the Environment. New York: Plenum
Press.
Pinciotti, P., & Weinstein, C. (1988). Environmental Experiences
and Children’s Drawings. Children’s Environments and
Quarterly 2(3), 21-28.
Schroeder, H. (1982). Preferred Features of Urban Parks and
Forests. Journal of Arboriculture, 8(12), 317-322.
Tuan, Y. (1978). Children and the Natural Environment. In I.
Altman & J. Wohwill (Eds.), Children and the Environment
(Vol. 3). New York: Plenum Press.
Valentine, G. (2004). Public Space and the Culture of Childhood.
Aldershot: Ashgate.
Wohlwill, J. (1983). The Concept of Nature: A Psychologist’s
View. In I. Altman & J. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behavior and the
Natural Environment (Vol. 3). New York: Plenum Press.
81
Refereed Full-Papers
Enhanced Photo-Elicitation
for Accessing Alzheimer’s
Perceptions
Colin Gadoury (Roger Williams University),
Christopher Capozzi (Roger Williams
University), Zachary Taylor (Roger Williams
University)
Abstract:
As partial requirement of an Environment Behavior
class, we attempted to use photo-elicitation interviews
to assess an Alzheimer’s facility from residents and
staffs’ point of view by allowing inhabitants to select
criteria in evaluating places depicted in photographs.
Photo-elicitation proved invaluable with staff and
visitors, but we were unable to establish meaningful
dialogue with Alzheimer’s elders using photographs and
asking simple questions such as “what do you do in this
room?”, “in what way is this a good space for the activities that take place here?”, or “how would you improve
this space?”
Having failed at photo-elicitation with Alzheimer’s
patients, we invented “enhanced photo-elicitation,”
inspired by John Zeisel’s research that describes the
importance of visual cues as environmental prosthesis
to memory. (Zeisel, et al, “Environmental Correlates
to Behavioral Health Outcomes in Alzheimer’s Special
Care Units,” The Gerontologist, October 1, 2003 43:
697-711.) We asked Alzheimer’s elders to match pictures of about 30 everyday objects such as eyeglasses,
light bulbs, briefcases, etc. with photographs of spaces
of the facility. Alzheimer’s patients succeeded in carrying out this matching as a game activity, spontaneously
explaining the reasons for their selections and sometimes responding to requests for explanation. We named
these interviews “Montessori Interviews” because the
facility was using similar Montessori activities as therapeutic activities.
In this paper, we explain the criteria for selecting
photographs and images of objects for these matching
interviews, the process of identifying and recruiting volunteers for interviews, the influence of the space used
for interviews, and methods used to carry interviews,
record, and analyze responses.
An example of findings demonstrating the effective-
82 ness of this method to elicit useful information included
an elder placing a light bulb on a photograph of a corridor while pointing to numerous reflections from metallic surfaces and glass frames of paintings and characterizing reflections as light bulbs. Describing reflections
as primary sources of light brought attention to the high
glare in this space, which according to the literature,
is detrimental and suggested using non-reflective glass
on paintings and applying non-reflective paints over
shiny surfaces. Dr. Zeisel, who is not associated with
this research but owns the facility where we conducted
interviews, proposed renovating half the corridor to test
the hypothesis that the Alzheimer’s elders experience
glare elimination as an improvement.
Introduction: Theory and Research Goals
The research project presented here developed as a
component of a Social Aspects of Architecture class
at Roger Williams University School of Architecture
that explores the relationship between people and the
environment. Students were asked to use ethnographic
fieldwork to access users’ perceptions of buildings by
combining what anthropologists call the etic (outsider)
and emic (insider) points of view. Combining the etic
and emic perspectives is accomplished through photoelicitation, an ethnographic interview technique that
asks inhabitants to use their own criteria in identifying
what features or qualities to discuss in photographs.
For a recent extensive review of photo-elicitation,
refer to Van Aken et al (2010). This method frequently
surprises the interviewers, as users identify features
in photographs that the architecture student who took
the photographs was not aware were present (Pavlides,
1991).
The research taught in this class is what has been
called discovery-based science in recent scientific publications. It deemphasizes seeking cause-effect relationships, because modern theoreticians of science, such as
Singer, Ackoff, and Emery have identified limitations of
the cause-effect paradigm. They proposed, as an alterative, that “the universe is best revealed by viewing it in
terms of producer-product,” which requires a recognition of the importance of a complex environment to provide context for an event to take place. (Lucas, 2000).
Discovery scientific methodology emphasizes initiating
research by data mining large volumes of experimental
data with the goal of finding patterns of correlations,
leading to hypothesis formation (Shneiderman, 2001).
This contrasts with traditional scientific practice, where
Refereed Full-Papers
hypotheses are formed before close examination of
experimental data. A well-published example of such
discovery-based science project in biochemistry is the
genome project (Bower & Bolouri, 2004). In light of
this approach, the goal for this research is not an attempt at finding answers to design issues, but finding
questions that would not have been otherwise asked
and addressed during the design process.
The research project for this course fulfilled the
Roger Williams University’s service learning requirement by making the collected information, reviews of
the architectural and the social science literature of a
building type, and results from the semester-long field
research study available to administrators responsible
for a building and to architects who design these buildings. Students are also given the option to continue
research projects at the same site as in previous years
and expand upon the information and research gathered from preceding semesters. In this particular case,
an Alzheimer’s facility had been studied through the
use of the photographic elicitation method during the
previous two semesters. However, in the earlier studies,
photo-elicitation interviews had been limited to visitors
and staff because Alzheimer’s patients were deemed
unable to participate in this kind of interview. In this
paper, we will discuss methods to access the Alzheimer’s patients’ experience of the building by developing
enhanced photo-elicitation methods, which utilized
visual cues rather than verbal questions to initiate
discussion of spaces depicted in photographs. We will
also report one of the resulting discovery findings that
reflections are experienced as primary sources of light,
the architects’ response on the usefulness of the finding,
and the formulation of a hypothesis resulting from this
finding.
Photo-Elicitation
While photo-elicitation performed in previous
semesters provided valuable insights for how staff and
visitors experience the facility, the methods for conducting interviews utilizing indirect questioning were
unsuccessful in generating responses from Alzheimer’s
residents. However, we were very interested in gathering information from the primary users of the building
and determined to try interviews with Alzheimer’s
elders at the facility.
Initially, we attempted to replicate photo-elicitation
interviews similar to those that had been attempted in
the past. So we set out to design a photo-elicitation
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
instrument. This required site visits to examine the
building in conjunction with reviewing the building
plans to gain an architectural understanding of the
spatial arrangements and relationships within the facility. Through informal observation of activities during
walkthroughs, we determined how to photograph the
facility for the interviews. Photographic documentation
included photographing all of the spaces and aspects
within the spaces. Photographs were taken of the
bedrooms, the dining room, living room, corridor, and
exterior spaces. Reviewing the social science literature
and informal interviews with caregivers also heightened
our awareness about what to photograph that included
attention to details, scrutinizing even the patterns on
the selected couches and walls. The literature research
included references that Alzheimer’s patients often have
trouble distinguishing the reality of objects (such as
flowers) from the patterns on furniture. The research explained that oftentimes, patients with dementia will attempt to pick objects, particularly flowers, from carpets
and furniture. Caregivers within the facility informed
us of such difficulties when we conducted initial visits
to the site. Additionally, the caregivers emphasized the
importance of light and the harmful effects of shadows
within space, especially for people with Alzheimer’s.
Residents with dementia often perceive shadows on
the floor as holes and are cautious when walking by or
through them (Alzheimer’s Association, 2004).
Limitations of Photo-Elicitation
Even with knowledge of the experience from previous semesters, we first attempted photo-elicitation
interviews with Alzheimer’s elders. These attempts at
interviewing residents failed in two important ways.
During pre-testing interviews, it become clear that
the first photographs we attempted to use did not articulate the spaces within the facility with enough clarity
for the residents to recognize the depicted spaces. We
had received guidance from our instructor to attempt to
photograph spaces while attempting to anticipate familiar vantage points for the people we needed to interview. For children or people in wheelchairs, we needed
to photograph at the appropriate heights; in general, it
meant including sufficient context to make the photograph recognizable. Through trial and error, we were
able to take photographs of the facility clearly containing landmarks recognizable by the residents, such as
a television set or a fireplace that made it possible for
them to recognize the spaces from the photographs.
83
Refereed Full-Papers
The second difficulty stemmed from our lack of sufficient understanding of the ability of people with dementia to communicate. Not realizing the reasons why
attempts to ask simple questions in relation to spaces in
the photographs were unsuccessful in previous years,
we attempted to replicate these efforts. During pretesting, several residents quit the interview or became
too preoccupied with other matters, such as asking the
reason for our questioning. Interviews generally were
terminated early at the first sign that residents may become agitated or were inconclusive. We determined that
understanding as much about the Alzheimer’s condition
as possible was the best course in developing a means
to communicate with the residents.
Attempting to develop a non-verbal interview
instrument
As we were becoming more knowledgeable about
how residents interacted within the facility through
observation, by interviewing staff, and by reviewing
the Alzheimer’s literature, we came to the conclusion that we needed to rethink the interview process.
Our thinking of redesigning the interview process was
inspired by Dr. John Zeisel’s research that in a variety
of ways emphasizes the importance of visual cues as
environmental prosthesis to memory. (Moldow, 2006).
The importance of visual information became apparent
in a several ways. Dr. Zeisel’s facilities, including the
one that provided the site for our study, had numerous
photographs hang on the corridor walls, with picture
Figure 1. Corridor condition emphasizing visual
cues.
84 density (Figure 1) resembling that of an art gallery. We
also read about the success that Dr. Zeisel had taking
patients to such art museums as the Museum Of Modern Art in New York City (Kennedy, 2005). We were
also impressed by interviews with Cindy Conant-Arp,
the director of Hope Alzheimer’s Center in Cranston,
who described how Alzheimer’s patients who had been
totally unable to verbally communicate did so through
painting. One of several examples she cited was about
successful visual communication with a very advanced
Alzheimer’s patient who used to be a skilled painter.
In her thoroughly non-verbal condition, it was hard to
know if she had any grasp on reality. When asked to
paint a self-portrait, she painted the back of her head
with stippling suggesting empty space, visually indicating that she was aware her mind was slipping away.
Finally the memory boxes by the patients’ doors are
visual reminders of identity valued by the residents and
their caregivers.
Taking this information together about the importance of visual information, we tried to find ways to
eliminate verbal questioning and attempted to design
a visual “interview” instrument taking into consideration the reality that the residents were having difficulty responding to direct questions in connection
to the photographs. The new method replaces direct
questioning with a collection of distinguishable photographs of typical everyday objects on individual index
cards. We were hoping that these objects would serve
as familiar items to residents, helping to trigger or cue
relationships between spaces and their experiences. The
selection process for the objects on the index cards was
based upon discussions within our group and with the
staff at the facility. We tried to identify objects that may
trigger memories and ultimately help express the way a
space is perceived. There was a significant amount of
conversation with the resident director at Hearthstone,
who made suggestions about the selection of these
objects. We were hoping that patterns would emerge as
Alzheimer’s patients placed the photographs of objects on photographs of spaces so that we could then
decipher what the patterns may mean in terms of the
Alzheimer’s experience.
When we presented the redesigned visual interview
instrument to our instructor, he questioned the viability of being able to meaningfully interpret patterns of
object placement on photographs of the spaces. He
suggested that we should reconsider even trying this.
He was concerned that the cards of objects would be
Refereed Full-Papers
placed on spaces for different reasons, making interpretation impossible. In the end, he acquiesced in the
spirit of letting us find out by ourselves. He was intrigued with how the process of matching photographs
of objects with photographs of spaces resembled the
kind of Montessori activities used for early childhood
education. Furthermore, unbeknown to us at the time
we redesigned the interview instrument, the avant-garde
Hearthstone facility that was the site of our research
had already implemented various “matching games” adopted from Montessori as therapeutic activities for the
Alzheimer’s residents. (DeAngelis, 2009) Our instructor
nicknamed our interview instrument involving matching photographs the “Montessori Interview,” though he
also made it clear he was not expecting results.
Developing Enhanced Photo-Elicitation
There were printed full-color photographs of the
seven primary spaces on 8.5” by 11” sheets of paper,
which were laid out next to one another on the table
in front of the interviewee. The 30 or so “everyday”
objects were printed in color on 3.5” x 5” index cards.
The index cards were presented individually to the
interviewee, who was then asked to place them on the
space they would associate with this object.
We recruited residents for the interviews suggested as
“likely” candidates by the facility sponsor and director
at Hearthstone, Mallory Jenkins, based on their attention span and mood for that particular day. Given the
extensive time Ms. Jenkins and her staff spend with
the residents, they knew who would/may enjoy participation and who may throw tantrums. Each interview
required a time commitment and, most importantly, a
focus commitment of around 20 minutes.
After the pre-selection process, an introduction was
delivered, during which we stated we were architecture
students at Roger Williams University interested to
learn from them about their experiences of the building.
They were asked if they had time and wanted to participate in an activity where they would be asked to match
photographs of objects with photographs of spaces.
There was an intention to avoid using terms such as
research, project, or interview, which have negative
associations. Previous semesters found that omitting
these terms facilitated recruitment for participation.
If potential interviewees agreed to participate,
interviews were conducted in the dining room of
Hearthstone, a centrally located space where it was
easy to conduct the interviews on a table with the many
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 2. Dining room interview location
components of the instrument. The interview location,
like the primary spaces photographed described earlier,
remained the same throughout the time spent at the facility. The location of the interview was very important
because the space chosen would affect the interviews in
a positive or negative way. The dining room (Figure 2)
proved to be an appropriate spot to conduct the research
because of its social environment. The space is a meeting place for visitors as well as meals and activities for
the majority of the day, and it is rare that this space is
ever a place of silence. The dining room was considered a key “activity node” within the spatial environment (Calkins, 1998). This allowed the interviews to
be conducted in an environment that was stimulating,
while at the same time not segregating the individual
from the rest of residents.
That being said, however, there were many activities
and distractions for the residents taking the interview.
One patient was distracted by the sounds of the highway, which was an earshot away from one of the bay
windows, an adverse affect of the facility being located
in easily accessible locations for visitors from the surrounding area.
Another influence on the interviews was the time of
day in which the study was conducted. Some residents
grew impatient as the interview took away from their
activity time. Given Roger Williams’ geographic location away from Hearthstone, the trips taken to conduct
interviews were often made in the early evening. During this time, many of the residents were impatient for
dinner, and this may have also affected the end results.
85
Refereed Full-Papers
We can only speculate about conducting research in the
morning.
The more social residents had a short attention span
and usually sparked conversations with other residents
during the interview. This did not adversely affect the
results, but added time to the total length of the interview. It was often found that these conversations made
the time there very enjoyable because the items would
spark some very interesting dialogues. In fact, substantive comments were contributed by onlookers who
joined in uninvited, as we will examine below.
Evolution of the Montessori Interview
When we entered data in an analysis matrix, it
became clear that every object had been matched by
at least some interviewees with each of the spaces.
This resulted in multiple objects being matched with
each space with no pattern that could provide insights
on the Alzheimer’s experience of the spaces. While
the matched objects failed to provide any information directly, some inhabitants of the facility would
spontaneously respond with a reason for placing or not
placing a card on a specific space (Figure 3). Some of
these explanations revealed extraordinarily strange and
unexpected perceptions. Our instructor had alerted us to
the idea that findings in discovery science are not necessarily the most common responses or the norm, but
rather outlier phenomena such as those detected about
subatomic particles by the HADRON electron accelera-
Figure 3. Early recording form showcasing need to
record comments.
86 tor, which was given as another example of discovery
science. The instructor had labeled such unusual responses as “black swan” findings to make sure students
who tend to discount unusual responses pay attention.
The term “black swan” was used as defined by Taleb
(2007), which was significant unexpected events that
have importance in discovery science because it is not
only about “what most people like” or finding norms.
The term helps emphasize the unusual, which often
leads to finding unmet human needs. One caution that
was given was that “black swan” findings often require
architectural interpretation in order to understand their
design implications.
The importance of the matching activity or objects
and spaces therefore shifted from attempting to interpret
the matching of photographs to recording the explanatory comments. The goal of engaging residents in the
matching activity of the interview was to ultimately
have the residents begin a conversation, which was
heavily driven by their thoughts and beliefs in order
to enable a sustained conversation that may lead to
insights about their experiences with the spaces in the
facility. If this conversation was not initiated spontaneously, as they were choosing an object card to place on
a photograph of a space, then it would be asked why
they placed the particular card where they did.
Analysis Process
The analysis process involved creating matrices for
the collected data. During the brief introduction to the
process, we asked for some general information about
the social characteristics of each person. Such information included age, gender, years at the facility, and
the position within the facility. These gave us a broad
sense of the participant, which we could later use to
compare if these characteristics had any correlation
with the responses. Next, after each card was placed on
a photograph, a team member recorded the data along
with any comments on a data form. From there, we
created a matrix of all responses. We color-coded the
card response to represent the space identified (Figure
4). Each group of like responses was then grouped.
Comments, along with social characteristics, were
then compared between the different interviews. Any
information that proved surprising was then recorded,
described, and even re-evaluated in the context of the
building. In the initial ‘tests’, residents were not encouraged to comment on their placement of the objects.
Their natural reactions and comments were recorded
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 4. Analysis matrix recording the index card
placement on one of the seven photographs of primary
space within the environment.
with any other particular gesture or reaction.
Originally, we thought that there may be useful
information by what most people chose. Soon it became clear that there was no real pattern, as different
individuals had selected all objects for all spaces. After
analyzing the data, it was realized that many of the
objects were placed in rooms that were not expected
and the current condition of the instrument showed no
reasoning for such an interesting placement. In subsequent interviews, asking residents to comment on their
decision became an integral part of the interview. It
is much easier to retrieve information from residents
with dementia when they are commenting on a recent
decision rather than answering a direct question, which
ultimately would lead to confusion and frustration. This
process enabled a dialogue that made it possible for the
residents to reveal their experiences as they provided
their reasoning behind their decisions.
In the end, some surprising and unintended actions
included handing back cards that did not belong or
setting aside cards for a space that was not represented
by any of these images. The results were recorded on a
simple chart and notes were taken as to why they made
their selections.
Findings
In this section, we elected to present a key finding as
an example of the how the explanation of placing a card
on a space can lead to insights about the perceptions of
an Alzheimer’s elder. This finding was supported with
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
additional field research findings from both a Montessori photo-elicitation with a woman with Alzheimer’s as
well as photo-elicitation with an elder man at a senior
center, as well as additional references from the social
science literature. When architects with experience
of designing buildings for elders and for Alzheimer’s
patients were presented this information, they characterized it as useful and suggested ways to address the
issue during design. Finally, a social scientist with deep
knowledge and experience of how architecture affects
Alzheimer’s symptoms quickly came up with a research
protocol to test the hypothesis suggested by this discovery finding.
In particular, one critical finding discovered through
this process emerged from an Alzheimer’s resident’s
response when placing the image of a light bulb on
the image of the corridor (Fig. 5). When asked why,
he pointed to the light fixtures, but surprisingly he
also pointed to the numerous reflections on the picture
frames created by the metallic and glossy surfaces of
the railing, which he perceived as primary sources of
light. He began to count not only the actual primary
light sources but also the reflections, believing the
reflections were indeed light sources (Figure 6). The
information was an astonishing discovery to us, bringing attention to an Alzheimer’s elder’s experience of
high glare within the built environment. Other residents
similarly placed the light bulb card on the image due to
the high reflectivity of the space.
Additional Alzheimer’s patients’ responses of reflec-
87
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 5. Re-creation of the index card (with light
bulb image) placed atop the 8.5” x 11” photograph of
the corridor space.
Figure 6. Highlighting “reflections” pointed out by
resident in interview within which the “light bulb card”
was placed on the image.
tions causing confusion included comments from two
female patients who joined in uninvited during a photoelicitation interview and made comments about the
actual space where the research was taking place. While
pointing to the reflections on the floor, one said that “it
is not always safe in there,” while the other added that
“someone needs to clean up the water,” confusing the
light reflecting off the floor for water. In the same space,
another woman during a different interview looked at
the photograph of the reflection on a TV screen and
referred to it as a mirror. Finally, during a photo-elicitation interview at a newly constructed senior center,
several elders, when asked if there was anything in the
room that needed fixing, complained about the reflections on the floor shown in a photograph of a room with
west-facing windows (Figure 7). The photograph was
taken late in the day when the low angle of the sun was
shimmering on the shiny finish of the hardwood floor.
These findings are backed by published social science research on elder populations. Corridor glare has
been recognized as often becoming a problem with regards to housing environments for elders. Any facility
can be hazardous with uncontrolled light and reflective
surfaces (Hiatt, 1987).
As part of developing this research, we sought architects with experience in designing Alzheimer’s facilities. These findings were reported to several architects
experienced with designing for both Alzheimer’s and
non-Alzheimer’s elders. They were interested and appreciative about the information we presented. While
in general experienced architects knew that glare needs
to be avoided in buildings for older populations, some
of the architects to whom we presented this information
took special note of the reflections off the floor—an
idea they had not considered as an issue prior to our
presentation. They asked their staff to join our presentation and discussed the relative advantages of a variety
of ways of addressing floor reflection, from non-shiny
finishes to special blinds, and took notes on the presentation.
Finally, we reported to the owner of the facility the
finding about reflections being perceived as primary
source of light by Alzheimer’s patients, suggesting
that glare maybe a problem in that space. Dr. Zeisel,
the owner of the facility and a nationally recognized
leader in Alzheimer’s research, immediately proposed a
follow-up experiment that could test the suggested hypothesis that resulted from the discovery science investigation that we carried out at his facility. He proposed
that if glass was replaced on the pictures with a nonreflective glass and the metallic surfaces were painted
with a non-reflective paint along half of the facility’s
lengthy corridor, we may be able to evaluate the impact
on behavior. In this way, he formulated a hypothesis
based on our discovery research.
88 Refereed Full-Papers
Reflections
In the beginning of the project, we were confronted
with the issue of understanding and investigating the
reality of architecture through examining the user’s
experience. Environment behavior research for Alzheimer’s patients is extremely well developed, and the
sophisticated residential facility where we were allowed
to conduct research was also a site for federally funded
research. The hope was that this work would be useful
beyond the confines of the classroom, but our realistic
expectations were that this would remain a student
exercise conducted for our benefit. It was therefore beyond our expectations that the process we developed for
interviewing as well as outcomes were of interest to the
research community. It was especially gratifying that
Figure 7. Photographs emphasizing reflections on
floor in a room with west-facing windows.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
it was also deemed as useful to architects experienced
with the particular building type. In our social science
readings, we found numerous references that architects
are not “tuned in” or even interested to the needs of the
buildings’ inhabitants. However, through discussions
with professional architects, we discovered an extraordinary repository of detailed insights, information, and
interest about the user’s experience.
As a student exercise, we learned that for inhabitants in such a sensitive condition, minor distractions or
imperfections in the architecture could cause serious
disruption in the lives of the residents. Beyond the immediate lessons about the specific users, as architecture
students, this process heightened our awareness of how
every design move affects lives for better or worst, and
this will be the lasting lesson.
Acknowledgements
We would like to give special thanks to Lefteris Pavlides, Ph.D., AIA professor in Roger Williams University’s School of Architecture, for his guidance and help.
Much of the work presented here was inspired through
social science research techniques explored and examined within Dr. Pavlides’ class and text.
We would also like to thank our sponsor, Mallory
Jenkins of Hearthstone Alzheimer’s Care in Brockton,
Mass., and to Dr. John Zeisel for allowing research to
be conducted within the care facility.
Additionally, much of this work would not be possible without the support of the faculty of Roger Williams
University, particularly Dean Stephen White, who has
demonstrated great interest and support in the knowledge being compiled and shared through this research.
Our presentations at EDRA 41 in Washington, D.C.,
and at EDRA 42 in Chicago, Ill., would not have been
possible otherwise..
We also thank Professor Robert Bechtel of the
University of Arizona, who attended our preliminary
presentation of this research at EDRA 41 and encouraged us to publish it.
Special thanks to John Robinson, RA, and Marc A.
Maxwell, AIA, two architects with significant experience designing buildings for Alzheimer’s elders who
took the time to review the results of our research and
in the process vastly expanded our understanding of the
users’ needs based on their vast experience and command of the literature.
89
Refereed Full-Papers
Bibliography
Bower, James M, and Bolouri, Hamid. (2004). Computational
modeling of genetic and biochemical networks. Cambridge:
The MIT Press.
Calkins, M. (1998). Design for dementia. Washington, DC:
National Health.
Collier, J. Jr. (1957). Photography in Anthropology: A Report on
Two Experiments. American Anthropologist, New Series, 59
(5), 843–859.
DeAngelis, Tori. (2009). Removing fear, promoting joy. This
psychologist is applying Montessori and other methods
to help people with Alzheimer’s disease lead richer lives,
Vol 40, No. 11 Print version: page 44 <http://www.apa.org/
monitor/2009/12/alzheimers.aspx_>.
Hiatt, Lorraine G. (1987). Designing for the vision and hearing
impairments of the elderly. In V. Regnier and J. Pynoos
(Ed.), Housing the aged: design directives and policy
considerations (pp. 343). New York: Addison Wesley
Longman.
Kennedy, Randy. (2005). The Pablo Picasso Alzheimer’s
Therapy. New York: New York Times.
90 Lucas, Michael. (2000). Understanding Business Environments.
New York: Routledge.
Moldow, Leslie G. (2006). AIA News from Design for Aging,
Workshop: Neuroscience of facilities for the aging and
people with Alzheimer’s.
Pavlides, E. (1991). The Missing Dimension in Environmental
Research. Environmental Design Research Association
Conference Proceedings 22, Mexico.
Shneiderman, Ben. (2001). Inventing Discovery Tools:
Combining Information Visualization with Data Mining.
Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Volume 2225/2001, 58
Taleb, N. N. (2007). Black Swan and Domain of Statistics. The
American Statistician, August 2007, Vol. 61, No. 3
Van Aken, P., Frisvoll, S., Stewart, S. (2010). Visualizing
Community: Using Participant-driven Photo-elicitation for
Research and Application. Local Environment, 15: 4, 373 –
388.
Ziesel, J., Silverstein, N., Hyde, J., Levkoff, S., Powell Lawton,
M., Holmes, W. (2003). Environmental correlates to
behavioral health outcomes in Alzheimer’s special care units.
The Gerontologist, Vol 43, No. 5, 697-711.
Refereed Full-Papers
The Perception of Place in
Planning for Urban Change:
A Small World Paradigm
Mary Ganis (The University of Queensland,
Australia), John Minnery (The University of
Queensland, Australia), Derlie Mateo-Babiano
(The University of Queensland, Australia)
Abstract
Urban change is often difficult and disappointing
because people’s expectations sometimes fail to be realised. This research seeks to understand what people’s
expectations are for good urban places in a changing
urban context and why their expectations are sometimes not satisfied. The research examines the process
of perception and the content and structure of people’s
expectations. The stakeholders’ expectations are framed
within the influence of their knowledge structure and
individual agency. Concept maps are derived from
a semantic differential task and analysed using nonmetric multidimensional scaling (MDS). The results
show that each group’s conceptual structure of an urban
place differs; however, an aggregate concept map illustrates a compelling cluster of urban design variables
that describes urban density and movement pathways
categorised here as the dynamic of Cluster and Connectivity. This dynamic resounds with ‘small world’
network theory, proposed as an inherent in the process
of perception and its conceptual structure of place. The
implication is that a small world network metaphor may
offer a paradigm for planning urban change using principles that resonate with people’s perception of place.
Introduction
Urban change is sometimes fraught with difficulties. Some of these difficulties occur because people’s
expectations are not satisfied. We need to understand
people’s expectations for urban places from their point
of view. We need to elicit an underlying conceptual
structure of their perception of places in an urban
change context if we aim to satisfy people’s expectations for good urban places. To this end, this paper
discusses the process of perception as self-organising
small world networks and aspects of stakeholder
knowledge structures and individual agency that influ-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
ences their expectations. Finally, an empirical study
elicits stakeholder concept maps that plot the network of
urban design qualities of each stakeholder group, as well
as what all participants expect from a good urban place.
It is argued that small world network theory offers a
metaphor with which to structure a planning paradigm
that facilitates adaptable, resilient, and coherent places
within stakeholders’ expectations of good urban places.
Perception of Place
Planning for urban change that aims to satisfy stakeholders’ expectations requires an understanding of the
conceptual structure of their expectations. We need to
build a planning process that acknowledges these expectations in an urban change context. By understanding
this, we may better be able to plan urban change in ways
that allow stakeholders to adapt. It is proposed here,
that an Adaptive Perceptual Cycle describes how people
perceive their world and adapt to change (Neisser, 1976;
Portugali, 1996; Bak, 1997; Haken and Portugali, 2003).
The Adaptive Perceptual Cycle (Figure 1) illustrates
how our perception self-organises as we explore the
world. We experience a place guided by existing expectations (or schemata). As we explore the world, information is absorbed from the context (physical, social,
cultural, etc.) that tests a schema. Contextual information may either confirm our schema or conflict with it.
If our schema is no longer a “good fit” with the context,
a crisis of relevance occurs and a choice must be made
to either adapt or not. This is the point of criticality
(Portugali, 1996) whereby the schema either resists
Figure 1. Adaptive Perceptual Cycle (based on
Neisser, 1976; Portugali, 1996; Bak, 1997; and,
Haken and Portugali, 2003).
91
Refereed Full-Papers
change (systemic rigidity) or adjusts and adapts to the
new context (systemic shift). But how does this change
imperative occur? Resistance to change persists until
the stability of a whole system is threatened—that is to
say, small local instabilities engulf a whole system and
threaten global instability (criticality), creating a change
imperative (self organisation).
Synergetics (from the Greek word meaning “working
together”) contributes to the idea of self- organisation.
Consider the complex network of factors that make up
an urban context (these might be political, economic,
social, etc.). These factors present order parameters
(such as regulations, policies, traditions, etc.) which
structure personal and social knowledge. Order parameters compete to capture control of a knowledge
structure. As Haken (1996) puts it, the order parameters
that ‘capture’ and ‘enslave’ their competitors win.
A more benign explanation of synergetics is through
an analogy. Imagine being a member of an orchestra.
In the orchestra there are the strings, percussion, horns,
woodwinds, etc. But this orchestra has no conductor and no sheet music. So, for the orchestra to make
music, it will have to improvise. The members of this
orchestra are professional musicians and therefore
understand how to make music using order parameters
such as harmony, rhythm, and so on. As each member
begins to play, they listen carefully to their neighbour
and try to recognise what order parameter they are
using. Each member and section of the orchestra can
either compete by playing loudly or insisting on their
individual rhythms creating—as you can imagine—a
discordant noise, or they can listen and find a way of
working together (a synergetic relationship). Our musicians adjust to each other by acknowledging shared
musical ideas so that each part of the orchestra adapts
and enables the emergence of music.
This musical analogy attempts to demonstrate how
self-organisation may emerge by a process of working together in a way that is mutually acceptable. In an
urban context, synergetics suggests that urban change
is open to many powerful order parameters, such as the
competing interests of developers, politicians, economists, environmentalists, design professionals, community members, and so on. The role of each of these
stakeholder groups is to present their agenda during the
sometimes difficult negotiations in a planning process.
And the aim of an urban planning negotiation is to
‘capture’ and ‘enslave’ the concept of what makes a
good place.
92 In an urban context, change is influenced by urban
order parameters derived from culture to politics or
pressures from population to climate change. Synergetics implies that competing urban order parameters destabilise existing urban paradigms until a critical point
is reached that impels a shift to a new urban paradigm.
This research seeks stakeholder groups’ network of order parameters or urban design variables that constitutes
their perception of a good urban place.
Network Structure
A network of urban design order parameters represents stakeholders’ conceptual structures of a place. It
is proposed that this is a dynamic concept that shifts to
adjust and adapt to our experiences in the real world.
Our schema of a place represents a network of urban
parameters that is tested in the real world—testing our
expectations. But what is the underlying network of urban parameters that structure stakeholders’ expectations
for urban places?
First, a network is a relationship of vertices (nodes or
points) and edges (links or connectors). Traditionally,
network topology identifies the stability of a regular
network and the instability of a random network. Within
the last decade, network topology has introduced a middle way between regular and random networks termed
‘small world’ networks (Figure 2), derived from the
popular sociological notion known as ‘six degrees of
separation’ (Milgram, 1967; Watts and Strogatz, 1998).
These ‘degrees’ are the number of ‘short-cuts’ across
social groups that expedite our social affiliations—
purportedly an average of six affiliates between any two
people on the planet. The relevance of small world networks to this discussion is that these networks represent
a dynamic network of nodes and links that self organise
and adapt in a way that is characteristically ordered as
well as random—their order lends stability and coherence, and their randomness affords rapid adaptation.
Now, consider the construct of a schema. Envisage
the schema as a mind map that is sketched to organise
our thoughts; this mind map may look like a web of
ideas with lines drawn to connect and cluster ‘good-fit’
ideas and to disconnect ‘bad-fit’ ideas. We may circle
clusters of good-fit ideas and draw short-cuts across the
page of our mind map to connect with other clusters of
good-fit ideas.
If we pursue the metaphor of network structure,
schemata are a network of ideas that connect with equal
relevance and weight, somewhat like a regular grid
Refereed Full-Papers
pattern. This means that no particular idea ‘enslaves’
the others, but all ideas are in equilibrium or stasis.
The schema is balanced at a point just before criticality
cascades. If no order parameter captures and enslaves
the schema, there is no change imperative. The schema
is systemically rigid. The advantage of such a network
is that it is coherent. The disadvantage is that it is slow
to adapt to change.
Conversely, a random network of ideas is constantly
enslaving one another. An order parameter may emerge
momentarily, only to be supplanted by another in the
next moment. In this case, we would be in a constant
state of instability and crisis with randomly emerging
change imperatives from one moment to the next. The
advantage of this random network is that it is quick
to adapt to change, but the disadvantage is that it is
incoherent.
Although both of these scenarios offer a distressing
mental picture, there are advantages to both network
types: a regular network of ideas offers coherence and
stability, and a random network of ideas offers flexibility and rapidity. A small world network combines
the coherence and stability of a regular network and the
flexibility and creativity of a random one, enabling it to
adapt quickly to change within a stable structure.
The structure of a small world network is a dynamic
one. Local connections form dense local clusters. To
expedite efficient connectivity, short-cuts connect local clusters, creating a global link. The stability of the
local cluster of ideas is enriched and diversified by the
global connection to another local cluster of ideas. If
we apply this small world network metaphor to the
Adaptive Perceptual Cycle, this represents how schemata are enriched and changed by new ideas. Just like
the network of our mind map in which we might draw
a line connecting a cluster of ideas to another cluster
of ideas, a new schema emerges that gives us a new
perspective until the real world experience no longer fits
that schema and we begin again with the battle of order
parameters.
Real world experiences that test schemata may come
from many sources. In this research, the change imperative is limited to the urban context and participants
limited to particular groups of stakeholders who were
asked to respond from the point of view of their professional or community role. It may be that the role of the
stakeholders frames their perception of place—after all,
whose place is it?
Knowledge Structure and Individual Agency
Structure and agency are aspects of a fertile sociological debate that has spawned many subtle theories
(Hays, 1994) and many applications from the economic
to the cultural posited by social theorists from Marx’s
historical materialism to Durkheim’s functionalism to
Bourdieu‘s notion of ‘habitus’—the human capacity for
structured improvisation (2011). This discussion will
limit the use of the terms structure and agency to the
notion of a ‘knowledge structure’—or the schema that
directs our interpretation of the world and ‘individual
agency’, or free will. But first, a brief explanation of a
relationship between knowledge structure and individual agency as used in this discussion may illuminate the
role of our stakeholders.
Our knowledge structure develops through our experiences of traditions, education, regulations and those
constructs and expectations to which we generally adhere so that we lead a relatively ordered life. Individual
agency is our power or free will that allows us to realize
our expectations or the output of our knowledge structure so that we may lead a relatively interesting life. For the purposes of this discussion, envisage individual
agency as a continuum of power between determinism
and free will and knowledge structure as a continuum
Figure 2. Network topology (Watts and Strogatz, 1998; Watts, 2004)
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
93
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 3. Knowledge Structure/Individual Agency
Grid
of knowledge between regulation and creativity. At one
extreme, our knowledge structure may be authoritarian;
rigid only affording repetitive actions and ideas. At the
other extreme, our knowledge structure may be laissez
faire so that boundless, free will holds sway. In each
case, our level of individual agency enables or disables
our capacity to implement our knowledge. A knowledge structure that neither enables a level of free will
nor one that is boundless does not offer stability and
adaptation, order and free will. That is to say, we function well neither as automata nor as eccentrics.
Idealistically, the power of individual agency is fuelled by experience and knowledge, but it is more likely
that one’s power and freedom varies depending on the
context: for example, our authority in the home may be
very different than in the workplace. This means that
the role that we play in a particular context calibrates a
level of individual agency that enables our knowledge
structure to flourish.
Consider this calibration as an orthogonal relationship between the factors of knowledge structure and
individual agency (Figure 3). If knowledge structure is
built through education and experience and individual
agency represents our power to apply that knowledge
in the real world, it may be that our reclusive philosopher, despite being the repository of an ordered knowledge structure, has very little agency in the real world.
Similarly, our village idiot may have very little agency
in the real world, but also lacks an ordered knowledge
structure.
Conversely, our philosopher king has not only the
94 benefit of an ordered knowledge structure but also free
agency which may or may not (depending on the appropriateness of the knowledge structure to the context)
benefit the real world. Our idiot king has the worrisome
calibration of total free agency but very little ordered
knowledge structure, the results of which may be at best
a creative autocracy and at worst a tyrannical debacle.
Ideally, one would prefer to be located at least in the
middle of this orthogonal grid (or in the right half of
it), in which our knowledge structure has some stability
and our free will has some agency—in other words, the
stability of order and the randomness of creativity.
The calibration of this orthogonal relationship is
likely to shift according to the stage upon which we
strut. Consider a professional as someone who has
a relatively ordered knowledge structure based on
education and regulation and whose agency is legally
accountable. As such, their knowledge structure and
agency circumscribes their choices. If this is so, then
for the sake of professional safety or comfort, their
choices may become conservative and repetitive. And
yet, professional responsibility requires their knowledge
structure to adapt to a real world context. This implies
that changes in a real world context demand a recalibration of their knowledge structure and power of agency.
If a design professional sees that an irregular adaptation of their traditional knowledge structure is a good
idea (for example, a mid-city community farm), they
may defer their power of agency to that irregular urban
change. Alternatively, if a design professional considers
an adaptation of their knowledge structure to be a bad
idea (for example, a Nazi death camp), they may rigidly
retain their traditional knowledge structure that rebuffs
this erratic urban change. In this way, a professional’s
power of agency facilitates the level of adaptation.
Whether design professional or lay, people perceive
places as a structure of their experience and expectations: schemata, which provide a stable knowledge
structure, direct how we interpret the world and our
actions in it. However, if one’s knowledge structure and
individual agency is a co-dependent relationship, as this
discussion suggests, this means that the structure of the
schema that guides the perception of place is subject to
our level of agency that enables our free will or limits it,
depending on the role that one inhabits.
Concept Mapping Task
It is proposed here that the perception of place is
likely to depend on the role and conceptual structure of
Refereed Full-Papers
the stakeholder. The intent of the concept mapping task
(Trochim, 2010; 2008) is to reveal the differences and
similarities between stakeholder groups in the urban
change context of South East Queensland, Australia.
Non-parametric Multidimensional scaling (MDS)
supported by Cluster Analyses is used to plot the
concept maps to reveal the clusters and connectors of
the network of urban design variables. This network
of urban design variables represents the stakeholders’ conceptual structure of good urban places. It is
expected that some stakeholder groups may have very
similar conceptual structures of what makes a good
urban place because of their design, development or
community experience and urban design practice in
South East Queensland. A Kendal Tau correlation elicits
those groups whose conceptual structures are similar
and those that differ.
It is proposed that the interpretation of the plots
may benefit from recent insights into network theory.
Currently, small world networks have been described
(mathematically) for neural networks, electricity
networks, social networks and many other case studies
(Watts and Strogatz, 1998; Barabasi and Albert, 1999),
but as yet research into dynamic psychological networks is limited (Schnettler, 2009). MDS plots may be
a small step towards illustrating schemata as metaphorical small world networks. What MDS analysis does
is run several iterations of correlations until a ‘bestfit’ emerges and plots the variables using Euclidean
distance. The MDS plots are intended to illustrate the
content and the network structure of the stakeholders’
schemata as their expectation for good urban places.
Non-parametric techniques are used because some
groups consist of <10 participants: Architects (10);
councillors (i.e., local government elected representatives) (8); developers (7); landscape architects (7);
planners (24); transport engineers/planners (11); and
others (9)—in total, 76 participants. The participant
groups are a stratified random selection of design,
development, community professionals and a lay group,
selected because they represent the key negotiators in
masterplanning processes in South East Queensland
from the initial concept through to the statutory adoption and the development negotiation stages. The
participants are requested to retain their design, development or community role for all of their responses so
that they are framed within their particular knowledge
structure and individual agency. The urban context is
described so that their responses are circumscribed by a
real world context with which they are familiar—South
East Queensland. “The predicted growth in South East
Queensland over the next 20 – 25 years will trigger
changes for existing urban centres. These changes will
have an impact upon the urban design qualities of urban
centres.
Please mark with an X, the ranking number that best
fits your idea of good urban design qualities for South
East Queensland.” The bipolar categories of urban
qualities that the participants rank (from 1-7) are:
Method
A semantic differential task presents bipolar categories of urban design qualities that are ranked by
the participants. These categories are derived from a
content analysis of the urban design literature (Gosling
and Maitland, 1976; Rowe and Koetter, 1978; Lynch,
1984; Trancik,1986; Attoe and Logan, 1989; Broadbent,
1990; Nasar, 1990; Calthorpe, 1993; Bentley, 1999;
Calthorpe and Fulton, 2001; Layard, Davoudi and
Batty, 2001; Alexander, 2002; Cowan, 2002; Duany
and Plater-Zyberk 2003; Carmona et al, 2003; Lang,
2005; CABE, 2008; PPS, 2008). Words and phrases are
categorised into major topics; minor topics and residual
topics in accordance with their conceptual similarity
(Tesche, 1991). Final descriptors of categories for the
semantic differential task are elicited from the written
verbatim data.
Var. 3 Clear Way-finding - Mysterious Exploration
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Var. 1 Compact, Distinct Centres - Dispersed
Sub-centres
Var. 2 Local Character Building Style - Unusual,
Landmark Architecture
Var. 4 Technical Efficiency - Sensory Experience
Var. 5 Small-scale Local Infrastructure Large-scale Regional Infrastructure
Var. 6 Adventurous Places - Safe Places
Var. 7 Urban-Rural Fusion - Ecological
Conservation
Var. 8 Evolving Places - Completed Places
Var. 9 Movement Network Connectivity Movement Network Separation
Table 1. Coding and Bipolar Categories for Semantic
Differential Task
95
Refereed Full-Papers
Results
The results of the MDS analyses configure plots
consisting of clusters of variables (shown as groups of
dots) and dimensions (shown as a line or curve of dots).
The results are two-dimensional for clear interpretation
(Figure 4).
The stress value of an MDS Analysis is an evaluation of the results’ goodness-of-fit: a stress value of
<0.15 is a ‘good fit’, and one that is >0.25 is a ‘bad
fit’. Lower stress values indicate greater reliability and
higher stress values indicate that the results may be too
complex to interpret. In this analysis, the stress values
for each group ranges between >0.01 and <0.16 indicating a “good fit” not only because the MDS plots are
two-dimensional (a better fit is attained with more di-
mensions), but also because the variables are relatively
complex concepts open to broad interpretation.
The results show that each group’s plot is different
(Figure 4). However, there is one compelling relationship of variables confirmed in the All Groups plot
(76 participants): Variable 1 (compact, distinct urban
centres); Variable 3 (clear way finding); and Variable 9
(movement network connectivity).
A Kendal Tau correlation analysis (Table 2) reveals
the influence of the participants’ power of agency and
knowledge structure in a planning process. The correlation coefficient indicates those groups that have a similar conceptual structure and those that do not. Briefly,
a co-efficient that is nearest 1 indicates the groups that
are most closely related (a perfect correlation being 1)
Figure 4. Multidimensional Scaling Plots
96 Refereed Full-Papers
Table 2. Kendal Tau Correlations pairwise deleted
in their conceptual structure of good urban places, and
those furthest are less related.
The results show that the only group that has some
correlation with the councillors (C) is the group of
landscape architects (LA), with a coefficient of 0.5.
Although this correlation may be somewhat borderline,
it represents a counter-intuitive result; it was expected
that the landscape architects would have a stronger
correlation with the other design professional groups.
The reasoning for this unexpected correlation is merely
conjecture, nevertheless it may be that councillors (C)
and landscape architects (LA) focus on the restorative
effects of landscaping and open space in an urban environment, whereas the other design professionals focus
on the built form, or socio-cultural-economic aspects,
or place identity and so on.
The groups with a coefficient indicating a similar
conceptual structure are the architects (A) and the planners (P), with a coefficient of 0.94. The groups that are
least correlated with the Councillors (C) are architects
(A) with a coefficient of 0.25; developers (D) with a
coefficient of 0.29; and planners (P) with a coefficient
of 0.31. This significant difference indicates a possible
source of conceptual misalignment between the important professional stakeholder groups of architects,
planners and developers and the community stakeholder
group of elected representatives, the councillors.
The almost perfect correlation between the architects
(A) and the planners (P) indicates that those groups—
possibly through their education and experience—have
developed a similar knowledge structure of their perception of good urban design qualities, which appears
to concur with neo-traditional notions of New Urbanism. The councillors (C) appear to be less exposed
to these ideas and tend to retain vestigial modernist
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
concepts popular in a previous era, such as “movement
network separation” and dispersed sub-centres’.
The councillors’ (C) knowledge structure tends to be
at the polar opposite of most other design and development professionals and the lay group of others (O). Yet
the councillors (C) are the group with the greatest power of agency in their role as adopters and implementers
of urban planning changes. It may be that in time, the
councillors’ (C) schema may join the other professional
affiliates, but as the Adaptive Perceptual Cycle suggests,
the other stakeholders may have moved on.
Although the design and other professionals may
have relatively less power of agency in a planning context, their knowledge structure of urban design quality
is presumably based on coherent and ordered principles.
However, these results do not reveal if the principles of
their knowledge structure are retained in a negotiation
context, which is a test of their individual agency. They
may express a particular point of view in the context of
this anonymous research, but in a real world planning
scenario, which is often politically charged, they may
(or may not) adapt their principles.
Discussion
This research was triggered by the pressures of urban
growth and change in South East Queensland, Australia, a region dominated by the City of Brisbane and the
developing areas of the popular tourist areas of the Sunshine Coast to the north, the Gold Coast to the south,
and new communities to the west of the city.
South East Queensland is one of the fastest growing
regions in Australia and has had a consistent migration
of people from other Australian states over many years.
Currently, the accommodation of this population drives
a long-term urban development and planning process
97
Refereed Full-Papers
that considers statutory masterplans as the appropriate vehicle to direct future urban development. Planning processes typically consist of several community
consultation meetings, a statutory process of review,
public notification and amendment of plans, and finally,
adoption by the local government authority. But community satisfaction after this long process is sometimes
disappointing. This begs the question: why does dissatisfaction occur? Why is there an apparent misalignment
between stakeholders’ perception of good urban places
even after rigorous consultation?
This research sought an inherently dynamic paradigm for urban planning in a dynamic context as found
in South East Queensland. First, we need to understand
the perception of place which may offer guidance in
a context of urban change, and second, we need to
understand the differences and similarities between the
stakeholder groups.
In this research, the ‘content’ of a good urban place
consisted of the urban design qualities that have been
popularly proposed in the urban design literature over
the past few decades, mainly emanating from the neotraditional notions of North American New Urbanism
and the well established British planning and urban
design traditions, as well as the remains of International
Modernism. All these notions have been adopted by
Australian design and planning affiliates. This research
does not promote one urban design school of thought
over another. Indeed, the intent of the empirical study
is to be neutral on what qualities constitute good urban
places. Rather, this research seeks those urban design
principles that have been absorbed by stakeholder
groups and that they perceive make good quality urban
places. The ‘structure’ of that content is the interrelated
network of urban design qualities and represents the
stakeholder groups’ schemata of good urban places.
The stakeholder groups in this discussion are typically the most important actors in urban change processes
in South East Queensland. Additional professionals
such as hydrologists, ecologists, economists, sociologists, among others, often contribute their specialist
input; however, this research focused on the typical
stakeholder groups who participate in urban planning
in this study region. This discussion also tapped into
groups’ capacity to influence, circumscribed by their
knowledge structure and individual agency. To glimpse
into this aspect, the stakeholders were requested to
respond from the point of view of their professional
role so that their perception of a good urban place may
98 be framed by their knowledge structure and individual
agency.
The Adaptive Perceptual Cycle was proposed to
explain how our perception of place adapts to a changed
context (Neisser, 1976; Portugali, 1996; Bak, 1997;
Haken and Portugali, 2003). Briefly, we explore the
world guided by schemata—this is how we make sense
of the real world. Our schema is tested in real life, and
if things do not make sense we either adapt our schema
or not—this critical point represents a change imperative which impels a systemic shift of our schema or,
if resisted, systemic rigidity may lead to failure of the
schema in a real context.
The aim of the concept mapping task was to reveal
the differences and similarities between stakeholders’
schemata of good urban places. The results appeared
to elicit a small world dynamic in which local clusters
of good-fit variables link globally and distance from
bad-fit variables. This interpretation is possible because
Euclidean distance is the premise for the concept map
plots as well as for small world networks—good-fit
variables cluster and bad-fit variables distance.
The results of the concept mapping task illustrated
that the schema of a good urban place of each stakeholder group was different (Figure 4). The difference
between the groups was confirmed by the Kendall Tau
correlation that showed the councillors’ conceptual
structure to be generally, the polar opposite of most
other groups except for the landscape architects. Nevertheless, the compelling result of the All Groups concept
map offered an insight into a schema that resounds with
Figure 5. All Groups cluster analysis; MDS plot; and,
small world diagram
Refereed Full-Papers
all the participants and so may be useful as identifying
their shared expectations for good urban centres.
The content of the most compelling cluster of the
All Groups’ plot is (Figure 5): Var.1 (compact, distinct
centres); Var. 3 (clear way-finding); and Var. 9 (movement network connectivity, here categorised as Cluster
and Connectivity). As an urban design concept, this relationship of variables describes a good urban place as
one that has density and connectivity. The urban design
variable of “compact, distinct centres” promotes urban
density and strong local connections not only of the
built form, infrastructure and transportation network,
but also of community and professional affiliations
through the ‘distinctiveness’ of socio-cultural identity
and the nuances of place identity.
In this study, the category “compact, distinct centres” is opposed by “dispersed sub-centres” so that the
participants have a measure of comparison for their response, Translated into the imagery of a real place, one
represents a traditional construct of urban life in which
there is a defined centre of town that is accessed, recognised, shared and used by a whole community. This
may be at varying scales from a local neighbourhood to
a village, town, city or distinctive parts of a megalopolis. Conversely, “dispersed sub-centres” represented
sub-urban places that are typically less dense and more
remote, with smaller activity centres that are accessible
to only a limited community. These places tend to have
a greater focus on open space, seclusion, and exclusion
rather than urban density, population and congregation.
The urban qualities of “clear way-finding” and
“movement network connectivity” were interrelated, as
they both connote a transportation system that is easy
and efficient rather than convoluted and obscure. “Clear
way-finding” was opposed by “mysterious exploration,” the former implying a street network that is easy
to remember and relatively predictable, whereas the
latter suggesting a street network that is unpredictable
and relatively adventurous or challenging. In this study,
the participants preferred an exploratory experience
that was efficient and easy rather than convoluted and
obscure.
The exploratory experience of “clear way-finding”
was associated with “movement network connectivity,”
which implies a network type that expedites the connections between places near and far. “Movement network
connectivity” was opposed by “movement network
separation.” The former implies a transportation system
that is integrated from pedestrian, bicycle, private
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
vehicles and mass transport within a street network that
is connected throughout. Alternatively, network separation implies a transportation system that isolates these
varying pathways so that they do not interconnect but
are exclusive movement systems—pedestrians only,
bicycles only, trucks only, and so on—with safety and
possibly order as the main driver.
The cluster of variables defined as locale included:
Var. 5 (small-scale local infrastructure), Var. 8 (evolving
places). Var. 2 (local character building style) and Var.
4 (sensory experience) was locally connected with each
other, but the distance between this cluster and the core
cluster—Cluster and Connectivity—indicated a global
connection between them. This cluster of variables was
defined as locale because these qualities describe the
urban setting or background not only in concrete terms
such as infrastructure and building style, but also in the
abstract terms of urban evolution and sensation. The
interrelationship of these urban design variables appeared to confirm the conceptual structure—Cluster and
Connectivity—represented by a traditional urban centre.
This cluster favoured smaller scale local infrastructure
and a distinctive architectural style, a construct typically
associated with traditional urban places. An important aspect of the locale cluster was that of “evolving
places”—this indicated the participants’ recognition
of the dynamic process of urban change over that of
the static product focus of “completed places.” In the
context of this discussion, time and change are inherent
in small world networks and may indicate a fundamental alignment between small world networks and the
participants’ schemata of good urban places.
The final cluster of urban qualities is defined as stewardship, and it is the one with the longest global connection with the core cluster of Cluster and Connectivity.
It consisted of Var. 6 (safe places) and Var. 7 (ecological conservation). It was defined here as stewardship
because these urban design variables are associated with
a sense of responsibility for the safety of people and the
environment. If this is so, it implies that the stewardship
cluster represented the participants’ role as part of their
schema and indicated the influence of their knowledge
structure and agency in this particular context. The relatively closer global connection with locale implied that
within the participants’ schemata of the notion of locale
was associated a sense of responsibility for that locale.
In sum, this research sought an inherently dynamic
framework for planning urban change. A small world
network paradigm was proposed because it is a dy-
99
Refereed Full-Papers
namic model that accommodates time and change.
Small world characteristics were evident in the All
Groups’ concept map which illustrated the hub Cluster
and Connectivity—a small world dynamic—and an
overall network structure of dense local clusters and
global connections. This implies that the stakeholders’ schemata of good urban places align with a small
world network paradigm and may offer an insight into a
schema that resounds with all the participants as a guide
for planning in an urban change context.
Conclusion
The expectations of different stakeholder groups may
significantly influence the satisfaction with the outcome
of a planning process. Acknowledging each stakeholder
group’s perception of good urban places offers guidance in a context of urban change. The research sought
to understand people’s perception of good urban places
through the content and structure of their schemata
The Adaptive Perceptual Cycle proposed that
people’s expectations are subject to a dynamic process
of testing new ideas and crises of relevance that impel
adaptation. People’s knowledge structure and power of
agency facilitates the adaptation of these expectations
or schemata. Stakeholder groups’ expectations for good
urban places were investigated in a concept mapping
task and analysed using MDS analysis and cluster
analysis. The results of the MDS analyses revealed that
the conceptual structure of good urban places for each
stakeholder group differed. However, the All Groups’
plot revealed a compelling conceptual structure of three
clusters defined as cluster and connectivity, locale, and
stewardship. The All Groups’ concept map appeared
to align with a small world network dynamic—cluster
and connectivity. It was proposed that a small world
network paradigm offers a metaphor for planning urban
change that resonates inherently with the stakeholders’
schemata of urban places and represents their expectations in planning urban centres.
Acknowledgements
Dr. Phil Smith and Dr. Mike Gillen, University of
Queensland.
100 References
Alexander, C. 2002. The nature of order: an essay on the art
of building and the nature of the universe. Berkeley, Calif.:
Center for Environmental Structure
Attoe, W. and Logan, D. 1989. American urban architecture:
catalysts in the design of cities. Berkeley: University of
California Press
Bak, P. 1997. How nature works: the science of self-organized
criticality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Barabási, A.-L. and Albert, R. 1999. Emergence of Scaling in
Random Networks. Science, 286, pp. 509-512.
Bentley, I. 1999. Urban transformations: power, people and
urban design. London: Routledge
Broadbent, G. 1990. Emerging concepts in urban space design.
London ; New York : Van Nostrand Reinhold.
CABE 2008. By Design: urban design in the planning system
towards better practice. http://www.cabe.org.uk/publications/
by-design
Calthorpe, P and Fulton, W. 2001. The regional city: planning for
the end of sprawl. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Calthorpe, P. 1993. The next American metropolis: ecology,
community, and the American dream. New York: Princeton
Architectural Press.
Carmona M. et al. 2003. Public places-urban spaces: the
dimensions of urban design. Oxford: Architectural Press.
Cowan, R. 2002. Urban design guidance: urban design
frameworks, development briefs and master plans. London:
Thomas Telford.
Duany, A and Plater-Zyberk, E. 2003. Smart Code. http://www.
smartcodecentral.org/
Geography and Geology Encyclopaedia. 2011. http://www.geodz.
com/eng/d/habitus/habitus.htm
Gosling, D. and Maitland, B. 1976. Design and planning of
retail systems. Architectural Press: London. Haken, H. and
Portugali, J. 2003. “The face of the city is its information”.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23: 385-408. Elsevier
Ltd.
Haken, H. 1996. In Portugali, J. (eds). 1996. The Construction of
Cognitive Maps. London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Hays, S. 1994. “Structure and Agency and the Sticky Problem of
Culture”. Sociological Theory, 12:1. American Sociological
Association: Washington.
Lang, J. 2005. Urban design: a typology of procedures and
products. Oxford; Burlington, MA: Elsevier/Architectural
Press
Layard, A., Davoudi, S. and Batty, S. (eds.). 2001. Planning for a
sustainable future. New York: Spon Press.
Lynch, K. 1984. Good city form. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press
Milgram, S. 1967. “The Small World Problem”. Psychology
Today. 1:1 pp. 60 – 67
Nasar, J. 1990. Evaluative image of the city. Chicago: American
Planning Association
Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and Reality: principles and
implications of cognitive psychology. New York: W. H.
Freeman and Company.
PPS 2008. How to turn a place around. Project Public Spaces.
http://www.pps.org/
Refereed Full-Papers
Rowe, C. and Koetter, F. 1978. Collage city. Cambridge, Mass.:
MIT Press
Schnettler, S., 2009. “A structured overview of 50 years of
small-world research”. Social Networks, 31: 165-178. www.
elsevier.com/locate/socnet.
Trancik, R. 1986. Finding lost space: theories of urban design.
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Trochim, W. 2010. . http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/
Trochim, W. 2008. Introduction to concept mapping for planning
and evaluation. http://www.conceptsystems.com.com/papers/
intro_article.php
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Watts, D. J. , 2002., A simple Model of Global Cascades on
Random Networks,. In M. Newman, A-L Barabasi and D.
J. Watts, 2006. The Structure and Dynamics of Networks.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Watts, D. 2004. “The ‘New’ Science of Networks”. Annual
Review Sociology 340:243-270.
Watts, D. and Strogatz, S. 1998. “Collective dynamics of ‘small
world’ networks”. Nature. 393:4, 440-442.
101
Refereed Full-Papers
Effects Of Garden Design
On Quality Of Life
Seiko Goto (Rutgers University)
ABSTRACT
Clare Cooper Marcus discussed the therapeutic
effects of gardens in various settings including nursing home gardens and Alzheimer’s treatment gardens
(Marcus, 1999). While a particular garden may answer
the needs of one group, she speculated that gardens
can be as varied as the patient groups they are meant
to serve. She urged the investigation of this idea and
asked the question, “Do variations in garden characteristics produce variation in health outcomes?” (Ibid,
p.587) To take a small step towards answering this
question, I have conducted three small pilot studies
to evaluate preferences for viewing different gardens
and their effects on users between 2007-2009. This is a
summary report of these three studies. In the first study,
I used questionnaires to evaluate the garden types in
terms of their likeability and participants’ interest in
returning. In the second study, I surveyed visual garden
design preference among different age groups using
questionnaires, and in the third study, I assessed mood
state using POMS/Profile of Mood States and cardiac
output. Cardiac output was assessed using a portable
electrocardiograph monitor before and during exposure
to the green spaces. Results indicated that the Japanese
garden was rated as the most preferred style. Heart rate
in the Japanese garden was significantly lower than in
the other environments, and sympathetic function was
significantly lower as well. I conclude that exposure
to green spaces can affect both the mood and cardiac
physiology of elderly individuals, depending on their
special arrangement.
EFFECTS OF GARDEN DESIGN ON QUALITY
OF LIFE
Many studies have demonstrated that involvement
with nature has a positive effect on health and on what
is now commonly referred to quality of life (QOL).
Horticulture therapy was developed in the rehabilitative
care of hospitalized war veterans because the therapeutic benefits of gardening have been valued in the U.S.
The main purpose of such a program is to encourage patients to get involved in hands-on gardening activities.
However, the benefits of gardens come not only from
102 gardening but also from viewing gardens and walking
thorough them. Viewing is the simplest and easiest but
the most traditional way to experience a garden.
For example, in a widely cited study, Ulrich, Simons,
Losito, et al. (1991) demonstrated through a review of
medical charts that patients in a hospital with a window
view of nature recover sooner, take less pain medication, and have a more positive outlook on their health.
In contrast, patients without a window view had less
favorable outcomes in these domains. Given the apparent importance of the view of nature, the number
of facilities for the elderly, such as adult day centers,
retirement communities, and long-term care facilities,
that have gardens for the purpose of improving their
residents’ health and QOL is increasing (Thomas, 1994;
Therapeutic Landscapes Resource Center, 2008).
In order to evaluate the suitability of garden designs,
I conducted three pilot studies:
1. Design Analysis of Community Gardensi
2. Survey of Garden Design Preference Among
Different Age Groupsii
3. Assessing Stress Level & Aesthetic Preference for
Different Garden Designsiii
The goal of these pilot studies was to evaluate preferences for viewing different gardens, and their effects.
Following are the summary of the three pilot studies.
Study 1: Design Analysis of Community Gardens,
Medford Leas Gardens
The first pilot study was designed to evaluate aesthetic preference for gardens in the Courtyard Apartments of Medford Leas during the summer and winter
of 2007.iv The Courtyard Apartments have 33 small
courtyard gardens designed by professional design
firms/organizations. (Figure 1) Each courtyard garden is
exactly same dimension and shape and is surrounded by
5 to 6 single or double occupancy apartments. Research
participants (N = 62) in the summer and (N=30) in the
winter were drawn from the population of persons living in the Courtyard Apartments, who were all highly
functional in terms of basic and instrumental activities
of daily living. Most of the residents were from New
Jersey. At the time when study data were collected, the
Courtyard Apartments had 150 residents. The mean age
of residents was 84; nearly 100 percent were Caucasian,
65 percent were women, 52 percent were single, and 48
percent were married.
Refereed Full-Papers
Procedures
I first explained the objectives and methods of the
project at a housing committee meeting. With approval
from the committee, a poster inviting participation in
the study was hung on a bulletin board in the entrance
hall of the community building. Invitations to complete
the questionnaire were also put in individual residents’
mailboxes. The poster remained in the entrance hall for
two weeks until the due date for returning the questionnaire to the central office. Because using all 32 gardens
for the survey would have been confusing and strenuous
for the residents, the number of gardens was reduced
to seven by grouping the 32 gardens into seven types.
Descriptions of the seven types of gardens follow.
1. American(court 28). This garden is mainly composed of birch trees and a lawn. Because of
its “open” concept, it reflects an American garden
style: a garden with a spacious lawn and shade trees.
(Figure 2)
2. Resting (court 27). This is the only garden that
contains furniture. A wooden bench and table are
provided in the center facing a wooden screen.
(Figure 3)
3. Symmetric (court 24). The most geometric and
symmetric garden. The geometry of the garden is
emphasized by the pruned hedge. (Figure 4)
4. Japanese (court 7). The Japanese-style garden has
a stone lantern, a pond and plants, including those
of a Japanese variety. The layout of the garden is
asymmetrical. (Figure 5)
5. Jungle (court 20). This garden is full of overgrown hydrangeas and trees. There is no pathway into
the garden, and views are blocked by the dense
vegetation. (Figure 6)
6. Herb (court 15). The herb garden was designed
by a specialist in healing gardens for the elderly.
Each plant was carefully selected from herb groups
commonly used in America, and was chosen based
on the premise that people feel comfortable when
they see familiar plants. The herbs are arranged
according to a geometric plan. Stepping stones are
arranged so that visitors can cross the garden as they
touch, smell, and pick the herbs. (Figure 7)
7. Perspective (court 26). Rows of symmetrically
arranged trees lead the eye into the garden and
beyond, all the way to an entrance leading to a
parking lot. This garden has the strongest emphasis
on directing the perspective view toward the outside.
(Figure 8)
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
A written explanation of the study’s procedures was
provided to each resident. Study participants were asked
to choose the one garden type they liked most and the
one they liked least. They were encouraged to visit each
of the seven gardens before making their decision. The
participants were also encouraged to comment on the
gardens that they chose. Since the appearance of the
vegetation changes in winter, two surveys with the same
questionnaire were conducted, one in September and
another in January.
Results
• Summer. There were 62 participants, 60 of whom
were Caucasian and one who was Asian; 50 were
female. Table 1 is a summary of the number of
participants who chose a given garden as most
favored or least favored. In the summer survey, the
Japanese garden (court 7) was the most favored
garden and no one voted for it as the least favored
garden. The herb garden (court 15) was the least
favored.
• Winter. There were 30 participants for the winter
research, 29 of whom were Caucasian, and one
who was Asian; 26 participants were female,
and 21 participants visited the gardens to make
their decisions. For both the winter and summer
surveys, the most favored garden was the Japanese
garden (court 7), the second most favored was the
symmetric garden, and the least favored was the herb
garden. However, the herb garden was chosen even
more prominently as the least favored garden in the
winter study.
The survey shows that the level of preference for a
garden varied depending upon its design.
The Japanese garden (court 7) was perceived as preferable to the herb garden (court 15). Positive comments
about the Japanese garden are edited for brevity and
summarized below:
• “Stones are excellent.”
• “Pine tree is wonderful.”
• “Very nice varied trees.”
• “Nice bright shrubs.”
• “Great variety of plantings, a pond, rocks, some
trees, and provided a pleasing, restful appearance.”
• “Nice open view across.”
• “Overall feeling is beautiful for contemplation.”
103
Refereed Full-Papers
• “Overall design is good.”
• “Oriental motif is very nice.”
Participants favored the Japanese garden (court 7) because of its view(s) created by plants of different height
and texture, as well as other garden elements such as
stones and the pond. Critical comments about the Japanese garden (court 7) mainly focused on its maintenance
condition:
• “Holly is too tall.”
• “Needs a little filling in at one end.”
• “Slightly overgrown. Would do well with pruning
and eliminating one overgrown holly.”
• “Please remove the shrub. It is overgrown and not
consistent with the character of the garden which is
otherwise excellent.”
• “Pond needs maintenance.”
By contrast, there were no positive comments on the
herb garden (court 15). Reasons for dislike of the herb
garden included the following:
• “No trees or shrubs to block the light from open
door.”
• “Needs more height variation.”
• “Needs more variety of texture and direction for the
eye.”
• “Too many grasses, not a pleasing sight.”
• “Too busy. No overall design.”
• “Too heavy at one end and thin at the other.”
• “I dislike clutter.”
• “Needs trees—maybe a bench—older people need
places to sit and rest.”
• “Far too weedy, has an overgrown look.”
• “Fill bare spaces.”
• “Generally messy—not neat.”
• “Would be more appealing if it had some trees.”
• “More height; put in more mature trees. It is open,
flat and too bright.”
Although the designer of the herb garden (court 15)
used various herbs, study participants commented that
the garden needs more variety. Since the garden gives
an open prairie feeling, it does not provide any one
viewpoint. Although the garden was in full bloom in
104 the summer survey, nobody appreciated its fragrance or
tactile quality. Instead, the textures and subtle colors of
the herbs caused participants to see it as “messy” and
full of “weedy grass.”
The most interesting discovery of this study is that
the Japanese-style garden was the most favored garden
even though its style and plantings are not very familiar
to most Americans. By contrast, the herb garden (court
15) was ranked as the least favored garden although the
plants it contains are quite familiar to Americans.
Study 2: Survey of Garden Design Preference
Among Different Age Groups
The second pilot study was designed to find out
whether the Japanese garden (court 7) is preferred visually or experientially, and whether it is preferred just by
an older group or by younger groups as well. Since the
responders of the first study were residents of the Courtyard Apartments, their decision might have also been
influenced by familiarity with and spatial experience
of the gardens. Using the same images of the seven
gardens used for the questionnaire analysis in Medford
Leas, preference for garden types was surveyed among
different age groups in this study (Figure 9). The
subjects of this survey were from three different age
groups: Residents of Parker Home (60+), Rutgers students (18-30) and Highland Park Public School children
(5-12).
Procedures
• Parker Home: Parker Home is a nursing home
located in Piscataway, New Jersey, that includes 60
apartments with approximately 60 residents. For
this study, PI first shared the objective and method
of the research with the residents, in collaboration
with Parker Home staff. Then PI posted posters
with photographs of the seven gardens in the
entrance hall. Simultaneously, questionnaires with
consent forms were put in the individual residents’
mailboxes, which requested that residents fill out
and return them to the administration office within
two weeks. The poster remained in the entrance hall
for two weeks until the due date stated on the form.
• Rutgers Students: Experiments were advertised
on designated department recruitment boards
requesting voluntary participation. The consent
form and questionnaire were distributed by
designated professors of each department and
returned to them.
Refereed Full-Papers
•
Highland Park Public Schools: Bartle Elementary
School in Highland Park, New Jersey, is a school for
children (aged 7-12) known for its cultural diversity.
Bartle School has a courtyard surrounded by
classrooms and the cafeteria. Because the courtyard
was underused, the school was considering a new
design as a part of its art program. With the consent
of the school board, PI sent a letter explaining the
project along with a consent form to all parents.
Then PI collaborated with art teachers who showed
a poster only to children whose parents did not
object to their participation in the project. Children
were asked to pick one image they wanted for their
courtyard .
Results
Elementary school children from 2nd grade to 5th
grade (N=377), university students from the Department of Landscape Architecture (N=23), Department
of Art History (N=29), Department of Neuroscience
(N=16), and retirement home residents (N=24) participated in the survey. Although the PI added a picture of
a flower garden, predicting that elementary school children might prefer a garden with bright colors, results
showed that the Japanese garden (court 7) was voted
the most favored garden for the Bartle School courtyard
space. The art teacher who conducted the survey reported that the school children knew that their courtyard
should be a quiet space, and it should be viewed from
the surrounding classrooms and the cafeteria in all seasons, except when they have summer vacation.
While the elementary school children chose a Japanese garden (court 7) for “a quiet place,” the university
students’ groups and the residents of Parker Home
chose a garden without any specific objective in mind.
Although the sample size is too small to arrive at any
conclusion, there was a trend of female and Art History students preferring the flower garden. Furthermore,
students in Landscape Architecture preferred formal
gardens (flower garden, geometric garden, and perspective garden) rather than the Japanese garden (court 7).
The Japanese garden was ranked second as the most
favored garden. However, the herb garden (court 15)
was still ranked as the least favored garden, and nobody
chose the Japanese garden (court 7) as the least favored garden. By contrast, the Japanese garden (court
7) was ranked first as the most favored garden in the
Parker Home, where there was one vote for the Japanese garden as least favored garden among 469 votes.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
It is worth noting that the responder commented on the
questionnaire that “I don’t like Jap,” which suggests that
her preference was based on cultural bias.
The results showed that the Japanese garden (court 7)
is preferred not only when experienced spatially but also
when viewed in a photograph. Furthermore, the Japanese garden (court 7) was preferred not only by an older
group or by elementary school children.
Study 3: Assessing Stress Level and Aesthetic
Preference for Different Garden Designs
To clarify the results of the first survey at Medford
Leas, the PI conducted a study to determine if there
were any statistically significant differences between
the responses to gardens that participants liked most
and least. This study measured mood and heart rate
while participants observed gardens from outside and
used a mood state inventory (POMS: Profile of Mood
States Brief Form, McNair & Lorr, 1964; DiLorenzo et
al., 1999) to track behavioral responses and heart rate
monitors to measure physiological responses of the
same individuals to the three landscaped spaces: the
Japanese garden (court 7), which they liked the most;
the herb garden (court 15), which they liked the least;
and an open space with a tree as a control space. (Figure
10) The control space was a landscaped space but not a
garden space.
Procedures
22 subjects were recruited from Medford Leas residents for this study. Demographic information, including age, sex, ethnic background, presence or absence of
pacemaker, and use of heart medication was obtained by
interviewing subjects at the site on their first day of the
experiment before viewing the three landscaped spaces.
While all were in reasonably good health, one was on
medication, two had pacemakers and were on medication, and one had a pacemaker with no medication.
Since three of the 22 participants could not visit all three
spaces, their results were discarded and the data from
the remaining 19 participants summarized as the basis
of this report.
To measure mood changes during the experiments,
the participants were administered the POMS before
and after observation of the three spaces. The POMS is
a adjective rating scale composed of 30 items with a 0-4
score. The 30 items are broken down into 6 mood states:
Tension, Depression, Anger, Vigor, Fatigue, and Confusion. In addition to the POMS Brief Form, participants’
105
Refereed Full-Papers
heart rates were recorded to assess variability before
and during the observation using a portable electrocardiograph monitor (Activtracer AC-301A, GMS, Japan).
Participants were asked to observe each of the three
landscaped spaces on three different days at about the
same time of day, not less than two hours after a meal in
order to avoid having influence of the meal on cardiac
data. The order of the observation of the three spaces
was randomized. A station was set up for completing
the questionnaire and attaching the monitor. The process
of the experimental design was as follows:
1) The participant arrives at the designated green space
at the designated time;
2) The participant is asked to sit facing a wooden wall
with their back to the garden or control space;
3) if it is the first day of the experiment, the participant
is asked questions that supply the demographic data;
4) The research assistant connects the participant to the
portable electrocardiograph monitor and turns on the
monitor;
5) The participant completes the pre POMS;
6) The participant looks at the wooden wall for 3
minutes;
7) The participant turns the swivel chair around and
observes the green space for 15 minutes;
8) The participant turns the swivel chair to face the
wooden wall again and the participant completes the
post POMS;
9) The research assistant removes the monitor;
10) The monitor is turned off.
Results
• POMS. Since 30 items of POMS with score of 0-4
are further grouped into the six domains, the range
of each domain is 0-24 (lower values represent
better mood). A value of “Before-After” greater than
zero means the score increased (mood improved)
after viewing the garden, and a negative value of
“Before-After” means that the score decreased. The
mean scores on POMS, both before and after the
viewing experience, are summarized in Table 8.
Table 9 is the change (Before- After) of the value
and the result of standard AOVA performed using
individual changes. Analysis of these data by
ANOVA reveals a significant difference in the score
for “Depression” (p = 0.01) and a strong, but not
significant, trend for “Anger” (p=0.06). The moods
of “Tension,” “Vigor,” “Fatigue,” and “Confusion”
were improved after viewing at all three landscaped
106 spaces but no significant differences were found.
Note, however, that subjects viewing the green
space with a single tree had the smallest decrease
in scores in these categories. We also note that the
scores for each site appear to differ in the variability
of the responses from subject to subject. In this
regard, subjects observing the single tree showed
the largest variability and while those observing the
Japanese garden (court 7) showed the smallest.
• Heart Rate. Average heart rates of subjects over
successive one-minute periods were gathered for
approximately 13 minutes. For each subject, an
initial three-minute period of gazing at a blank
screen was followed by a 10-minute period
during which the subject was shown one of the
landscape spaces. Heart rate data for each minute
of observation were averaged. Table 10 shows the
results of all subjects averaged at each time point.
The average heart rate of participants when viewing
the Japanese garden (court 7) is significantly lower
than the average heart rate when viewing the
control. Moreover, a significant difference was
obtained at 2 minutes, 3 minutes and 10 minutes
after the starting point of viewing.
Several points emerge from this these data. The
first is that differences in heart rates can be detected in
participants when they view different environments .
The average bpm (all subjects/all time points) while
viewing the Japanese garden space (72.3 ± .52 bpm)
was substantially lower than while viewing the herb
garden space (75.6 ± .54 bpm) or simple green space
(75.4 ± .82 bpm), which produced similar effects.
Second, a comparison of the initial three minutes of the
various conditions suggests that environmental factors
beyond the garden viewing itself differ among the three
landscape spaces. These differences were not statistically significant, but it bears noting that on average
subjects waiting to view the herb garden (court 15) had
a higher heartbeat rate than did those waiting to view
the Japanese garden (court 7). Our study was short-term
in nature; each green space exposure was 10 minutes or
less in duration. Despite this, clear responses could be
found in both types of outcome measures.
Summary
These studies are preliminary and have limitations.
Because most of the responders at Medford Leas were
female, one could argue that our results are only representative in terms of gender. However, since the gender
Refereed Full-Papers
distribution in retirement homes in this country generally favors women by a margin of three-to-one (American
Association of Homes for the Aging, 1991), our gender
distribution may in fact be representative. Having a
woman’s perspective in this area is therefore germane.
The study was also limited by our focus on one set of
gardens only; thus results are based on only one of each
garden type and may have been influenced by particular features not evident in the description of the type
of garden. The outcomes should not be generalized to
Japanese/herb gardens. Furthermore, the sample size
was relatively small, and the distributions were skewed.
Despite these limitations, the results showed sufficiently
strong trends to allow us to assert the following: 1) the
Japanese garden (court 7) is preferred among multiple
different garden types, not only by the elderly but also
by younger people; 2) the herb garden (court 15) is not
favored by the elderly or by younger people; 3) viewing
a Japanese garden (court 7) can reduce stress.
The results of these three small studies suggest that
the Japanese garden (court 7), not the herb garden
(court 15), is one of the most favorable garden types,
and it can improve the emotions and cognition of viewers. This was an unexpected finding, since the herb
garden was specifically designed by an expert to be aesthetically pleasing to residents at this facility. I note that
only one of the subjects in the first and third studies was
of Asian ethnic origin, making it likely that our findings are more universal in their impact and not heavily
dependent on prior exposure to, or learned preference
for, this particular style. Indeed, it was interesting to
note that the Japanese garden (court 7) was the most
favored garden even though its style and plantings are
not very familiar to most Americans. By contrast, the
herb garden (court 15) was ranked as the least favored
garden, in spite of the fact that the plants it contains
are quite familiar to Americans. The herb garden (court
15) was well planned to engage all five senses, but this
dynamic feature did not entice research participants to
rate it as highly pleasing.
Although the Japanese garden may be a foreign
style for most Americans, it can be ideal for certain
kinds of spaces. Some Japanese gardens have been
designed to occupy a small outside space just for viewing. To make such a small space look bigger, designers
of the Japanese garden developed techniques to create
multiple viewing points. Such a garden might be able
to draw observers’ attention longer from one viewing
point than other style gardens.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Ulrich, Simons, Losito, et al. (1991) showed that
having a view of nature helps hospitalized patients to
recover sooner. While this is an interesting finding, I
can conclude that the level of effect of a view of nature
differs based on the level of enjoyment of a garden by
its observers depends upon its overall design. Indeed,
more research must be conducted on elders’ aesthetic
preferences for garden designs. Such studies could give
us clues as to the mechanisms by which gardens have
their often-claimed healing properties.
The continuing demonstration that gardens can have
a desirable effect on the mood of an elderly individual
suggests that gardens may have a valuable place in any
number of institutional settings. While pharmacological approaches to behaviour management are of proven
efficacy, they carry safety risks that include side effects
such interaction with other needed medications. Gardens and other natural settings offer a non-pharmacological strategy that is safe and increasingly proving to
be effective. However, what aspect of a garden makes
its space therapeutic? Is it layout, plant selection, stone
placement, or some combination of features? Much
further investigation is needed to answer this question.
i
#E08-026
09-185Mc
iii
08-545M
iv
Medford Leas is a continuing care retirement community managed by a non-profit Quaker-related corporation. The Courtyard Apartments have 33 small courtyard gardens designed by professional design firms/
organizations.
ii
107
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 1: Medford Leas 7 selected garden locations
Figure 2: court 28 American Garden
Figure 3: court 27 Resting Garden
Figure 4: court 24 Symmetric Garden
Figure 5: court 7 Japanese Garden
Figure 6: court 20 Jungle Garden
108 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 7: court 15 Herb Garden
Table 1: Summer Most Favored and Least Favored
Gardens
Figure 8: court 26 Perspective Garden
Table 2: Winter. Most Favored and Least Favored
Gardens)
Table 3. Demographic data
Table 4. Highland Park Public
School Most and Least Favored
Garden
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
109
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 9: Questionnaire sheet
110 Refereed Full-Papers
Table 5. Rutgers University Most and Least Favored Garden
Table 7. Demographic data
Table 6. Parker Home Most and Least
Favored Garden
Table 8. Changes in POMS score: mean ± standard deviation
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
111
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 11. Three selected locations
Table 9. Before-After Summary p<0.05
Figure12. Control Space
Table 10. Heart Rate Summary result N=19
112 Refereed Full-Papers
REFERENCES
American Association of Homes for the Aging (1991).
Continuous Care Retirement Communities, An Industry in
Action Vol. II. Washington DC: Ernst and Young.
Connoly, T., Arkes, K. R., & Hammon, K. R., eds. (2000).
Judgment and decision making: An
interdisciplinary reader. Second edition. Cambridge
University Press: Cambridge, England.
Delorme, E. P. (1996). Garden Pavilions and the 18th Century
Garden Court. Woodbridge, Suffolk: Antique Collectors
Club Ltd.
Earl, J. Ed. (2000). Infinite Spaces: The Art and Wisdom of the
Japanese Garden. Boston, MA: Tuttle Publishing.
Heath, Y. (2004). Evaluating the effect of therapeutic gardens.
American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other
Dementias 19: 239.
Heerwagen, J. H., & Orians, G. H. (1993). Humans, habitats,
and aesthetics. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (eds.), pp.
138-172. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island
Press.
Lundin, R. W. (1985). Theories and Systems of Psychology. III
Edition. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath and Company.
Marcus, C. C. (1999). Healing Gardens: Therapeutic Benefits
and Design Recommendations. New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Marcus, C. C. (2002). Great greenery. Some basic advice on
creating a healing garden. Health Facil Manage, 15, 20-23.
Michaels, C., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct Perception.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Nassauer, J. I. (1995a). Messy ecosystems, orderly frames.
Landscape Journal, 14:161-169.
Nassauer, J. I. (1995b). Culture and changing landscape structure.
Landscape Ecology 10: 229-237.
Orians, G. (2001). An evolutionary perspective on aesthetics.
Bulletin of Psychology and the
Arts 2: 25-29.
Perneger, T. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments.
British Medical Journal 316:
1236-1238.
Thomas, W. (1991). The Eden Alternative: Nature, Hope,
and Nursing Homes. Sherburne, NY: Eden Alternative
Foundation.
Therapeutic Landscapes Resource Center (2008). Therapeutic
Landscapes Database. Accessed February 24, 2008 at: http://
www.healinglandscapes.org/index.html.
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., et al.
(1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban
environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology 11:
201-230.
113
Refereed Full-Papers
Integration of Ecology,
Sociology and Aesthetics in
Urban Design: Fantasy or
Reality? A Study through
the Lens of Landscape
Architecture
Amir Hossein Hajrasouliha (University of
Michigan)
Introduction:
The contemporary profession of urban design dates
all the way back to the mid-20th century. Since then,
this young discipline has tried to define its position
among other disciplines such as architecture, urban
planning and landscape architecture. The practical
indistinctness of urban design’s borders has led other
practitioners and professions to be active in this realm,
which in truth facilitates the constant reform and expansion of the theory of urban design. One discipline
that has greatly impacted the field of urban design is
landscape architecture. Landscape architecture is a
multi-disciplinary field that incorporates the urban
context and ecological aspects into the design process.
Landscape architecture has greatly influenced urban
design principles and helped to transform it into a
broader ideology that combines both social and ecological aims. This paper explores these contributions
in the course of contemporary landscape practices and
writings, particularly through the evaluation of two active landscape architects and theorists, Randolph Hester
and James Corner. Despite being completely different
in their theory and practice, both have tried to integrate
social and ecological aspects of the urban context into
the artistic logic of design with dissimilar shares of
idealism and pragmatism. After a brief introduction of
both landscape architects and their theories, the valuable lessons for urban design from the practice of these
landscape architects in the framework of ecology, sociology, and aesthetics will be reviewed. James Corner
and Randolph Hester both demonstrate how treating
urban design as a multidisciplinary field with a focus on
1
social and ecological goals will drastically improve the
profession in an innovative way.
Randolph Hester
Randolph Hester was a professor of landscape architecture at the University of California, Berkeley from
1980 until his retirement in June 2010. His research
mainly focused on the role of citizens in community
design and ecological planning. Within his work and
writings, Hester has tried to bridge the gap between different disciplines. He suggests that urban design should
consider social, ecological, and aesthetical issues simultaneously in order to achieve a livable environment.
” For the last fifty years … poor city design divides
us from others in our communities, undermines our
sense of community and place, destroys natural habitats that once gave us immeasurable joy … and fails to
inspire our spirits.”1
In his book Design for Ecological Democracy, Hester
proposed 15 design principles that have emerged from
sociology and ecological contexts. He promotes the
idea that “we need to reform our cities so that we act as
communities” by using five principles that he termed
Figure 1: Sacredness is the initiating force of city
form that supports ecological democracy, Hester,
Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006: 420
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 1.
114 Refereed Full-Papers
“Enabling Form: We got to know our neighbors.” He
creates the notion that “we need to reform our cities to
be ecologically resilient” in five principles that he called
“Resilient Form: life, liberty, and the pursuit of sustainable happiness.” Hester also promotes the concept that
“we need to reform our cities to impel us by joy” in five
principles titled “Impelling Form: “Make a city to touch
the people’s hearts”.2
The vague interaction of these principles and the
uncertainty in implementing this methodology is what
makes these concepts hard to grasp. Hester portrays his
theory as “messy—more like a mass of mating salamanders than a regression analysis.”3 But his main contribution is the source of these principles, which is from
the inner circles composing sociology, ecology, and art.
Each principle is a discrete amphibian subject, “but the
value of this theory lies in the entirety of intertwined,
squirming mass of fifteen principles.”4 Figure 2 shows
the intertwined character of these principles based on
the context of their emergence.
Among these 15 principles, ‘sacredness’ has the most
prominent position. Hester defines the sacred landscape
Figure 2: 15 principles in 3 contexts
as “places that are consecrated by highly revered convictions, values, and virtues.”5
In Hester’s ideology, “sacredness serves the practical purposes of preservation and creates gestalts that
express the soul of a place and inspire the details of
city form.”6 In this and other principles, a combination
of imagination and rationality conquers the traditional
values embedded within urban design.
During Hester’s retirement ceremony, his son described his father by saying: “He taught me to interact
with the miracle and majesty and power of the natural
world. He taught me about outrageous creative hope.
Outrageous in taking risks and creative in proposing
solutions…he’s been brave enough to hope for a better world.”7 Randy Hester is a visionary, a poet and a
social activist in the drab of a designer. Dreamers like
Hester not only help to expand the boundaries of the
discipline, but to also inspire others for more meaningful acts, in both their personal and professional lives.
James Corner
James Corner is a prominent landscape architect
who is currently the Principal of Field Operations and
the Department Chair of Landscape Architecture at the
University of Pennsylvania. Throughout his career as a
professor and professional landscape architect, Corner
has tried to redefine modern urbanism through the lens
of landscape architecture. He is one of the key figures in
the discourse of landscape urbanism. He is famous for
working with graphic artists, photographers, and other
similar artists from various fields. Corner has also collaborated with some of the world’s leading architects,
planners, engineers, ecologists, and artists from various
multidisciplinary fields.8 Some of his projects are based
on a multi-disciplinary process that integrates and regenerates urban design principles. He describes his own
design process as “working synoptic maps, alongside
the intimate recordings of local circumstance, comparing cinematic and choreographic techniques to spatial
notion, entering the algebraic, digital space of the
computer while messing around with paint, clay, and
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006).
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006, 419.
4
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 419.
5
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006),117.
6
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 135.
7
“In an article on Randy Hester’s retirement tribute (The Daily Berkeley Planet, June, 2010), his colleagues and friends honored him
for a life time teaching”
8
Fehrenbacher, Jill. “Architect James Corner On NYC’s High Line Park”, (Inhabitat, 2010), http://inhabitat.com, (accessed August 2010)
2
3
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
115
Refereed Full-Papers
ink, and engaging real estate developers and engineers
alongside the highly specialized imaginers and poets of
contemporary culture.”9
Corner’s attitude showcases a fundamental aspect of
landscape urbanism in which process, both in analysis
and design, are favorable to a basic static formal process.
Evolution in landscape urbanism is preferred to mere
creation:
“Landscape urbanism’s methodology is multidisciplinary by definition. Expanding from the legacy of landscape design to consider the complexity of contemporary
urban dynamics, it integrates knowledge and techniques
from such disciplines as environmental engineering,
urban strategy, landscape ecology, the development
industry and architecture.”10
Corner defined four themes of landscape urbanism as:
(1)Process over time: “The promise of Landscape
urbanism is the development of a space-time ecology
that treats all forces and agents working in the urban
field and considers them as a continuous network of
inter-relationships,” Corner asserts.11 The process of
urbanization is characterized more by contemporary
techniques that represent the fluidity, spontaneous
feedback, and non-linearity of urbanization than a
fixed, rigid, and definite process.
(2)Staging of surfaces: Landscape urbanism accepts
and embraces the current situation of horizontally
sprawling American suburbs as the new urban reality
in an attempt to understand the reasons behind this
development. Landscape urbanism replaces the
existing ‘program’ of a site with a site’s ‘potential’
in order to define a territory use. “The thrust of
this work is less toward formal resolution and
more toward public process of design and future
appropriation,” Corner writes. “Concerned with a
working surface over time, this is a kind of urbanism
that anticipates change, open-endedness, and
negotiation.”12
9
Figure 3: 4 themes of landscape urbanism in 3
contexts of sociology, ecology and aesthetics
(3)Working method: Landscape urbanism suggests
a reconsideration of traditional, conceptual,
representational and operative techniques that
function across a range of scales and implicate a
host of players.
(4)The imaginary: “The collective imagination,
informed and stimulated by the experiences of the
material world, must continue to be the primary
motivation of any creative endeavor,” Corner
writes.13
The Sociological Context of Urban Design
The formal establishment of the urban design program at Harvard University was conceived a year before the appearance of Jane Jacobs’s controversial book,
The Death and Life of Great American Cities.14 Since
then, the up-and-coming urban design field has been
the target of numerous complaints from different social
movements. At the time, urban design projects as physi-
Corner, James. “Terra Fluxus” in The Landscape Urbanism Reader, edited by Charles Waldheim, 21-33.( New York: Princeton
Architectural Press, 2006), 31.
10
Waldheim, Charles. The Landscape Urbanism Reader. (New York: Princeton Architectural Press, 2006), 13.
11
Corner, James. “Terra Fluxus” in The Landscape Urbanism Reader, edited by Charles Waldheim, 21-33.( New York: Princeton
Architectural Press, 2006), 30.
12
Corner, James. “Terra Fluxus” in The Landscape Urbanism Reader, edited by Charles Waldheim, 21-33.( New York: Princeton
Architectural Press, 2006), 31.
13
Corner, James. “Terra Fluxus” in The Landscape Urbanism Reader, edited by Charles Waldheim, 21-33.( New York: Princeton
Architectural Press, 2006), 32.
14
Sorkin, Michael, The End(s) of Urban Design, in Urban Design, ed. Alex Krieger and William S. Saunders, (Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota Press, 2009).
116 Refereed Full-Papers
cal entities were considered to be for the most part antisocial. In response to the complexity of social issues
prevalent in urban developments, urban design began to
focus more on social goals that were in accordance to
its principles. “Community-Based Design,” “Everyday
Urbanism,” and “New Urbanism” are the products of
this sudden shift. But still the constant clashing between
urban development and social goals are continuing in
contemporary urbanism to this day.
Like urban designers, landscape architects are facing
the same challenges in their urban scale projects. Hester
and Corner are both effective figures that throughout
their practice and ideology have elevated the expectations of urban design projects to broadly address social
concerns.
While Hester has been one of the leading community
design experts and an advocate of the citizen’s role in
urban design, Corner has been active in a completely
different way. Although Corner believes in the social
consultant and the bottom-up design process, his projects are mainly large-scale projects with a limited possibility of sufficient public participation in the design
process. But his projects have not ignored social considerations. His approach to society as a complex and unpredictable system makes his projects both flexible and
feasible. The contrast between Hester and Corner could
be better followed in their respective works.
One project that illustrates this distinction is the
revitalization of the central waterfront of Manteo, North
Carolina. Hester referred to this project in the chapter on sacredness of his book, Design for Ecological
Democracy. Manteo faced a 22 percent unemployment
rate, with few prospects for improvement. The designers tried to uncover the important aspects of the communities’ life. Hester calls this process “Uncovering
Sacredness,” which came about in a series of interviews
and behavior mapping. The result was a map that
disclosed places that were important to the social fabric
of Manteo. Identification of these places makes it hard
for developers to change them in their proposals. The
residents wanted these places protected because they
have a “higher value than dollars.”15 Those places are
so essential to the lives of residents that the community
collectively identifies with the places.16 Preserving
the “sacred structure” of Manteo cost the town half a
Figure 4: Sacred structure of Manteo, Hester,
Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006:121
million dollars each year in the 1980s, but this sacrifice
eventually inspired a significant economic recovery that
made Manteo a model for other cities.17
Based on Hester’s basic ideology, urban design in the
context of sociology should act as a revealer, preserver
and promoter of social values. Design in this realm
is not passive; instead there exists a high obsession
about the social impact of the physical configuration of
spaces. Corner also believes that with the failure of the
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006).
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 120.
17
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 123.
15
16
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
117
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 5: a “Water City” for 1.5 million people, http://www.archdaily.
com/66650/james-corner-field-operations-to-design-qianhai/
planning and design approach that ignored local characteristics and values, the landscape is now seen as the
means to resist the homogenization of the environment
while also heightening local attributes and a collective
sense of place.18 Hester’s projects predominantly occur
at the neighborhood scale. Corner, on the other hand,
has tried his ideology in mega-cities like New York,
Toronto, and Shenzhen. A large criticism of Hester’s approach is that it might not work in mega-scale developments like those in China. The Chinese don’t have time
to “uncover the sacred structure” of their cities. They
desire and need tall and dense buildings, and they need
them fast.
In a recent prestigious Chinese urban design competition to design a new major city called Qianhai near
Shenzhen, James Corner Field Operations was fortunate
enough to win the competition. Managing communities
and neighborhoods for a city with 1.5 million residences
that has to be built in a very short period is a tough challenge. Corner’s project is a top-down approach because
of the scale and character of this project made it to some
extent impossible to count on the citizen’s role in the
design process (one of the main proponents of Hester’s
design process). Field Operations’ innovative proposal
used landscape features as the main source to shape the
city form. “Five ‘Water Fingers’ divided the massive
18
site into manageable sub-districts. The fingers extended
from the existing rivers and channels and function
as both innovative hydrological infrastructures and
new public parkland. Within each of the sub-districts,
the fabric is designed in the conventional scale of a
typical Shenzhen block and inter-connected with the
urban neighborhoods.”19 In this case, incorporation of
landscape architecture, urban design, and social/cultural
concerns promotes a city with social and ecological
sensitivity.
The Ecological Context of Urban Design
Urban design largely deals with the quality of the
built environment that is vital for preserving the natural
environment. In some cases, the natural environment
is like a natural park, waterfront, or mountain skirt,
where the built environment becomes intertwined with
the cities. Mainly, landscape architects are in charge of
development in these fields. Their ecological insight has
brought verdancy to urban spaces and the subsequent
design principles as well. Landscape architects such as
Randy Hester and James Corner are prominent figures
in this discipline.
Both address urban and natural contexts simultaneously in their projects, but in different scales and situations. As mentioned before, Hester’s projects are usually
Corner, James. Recovering landscape: essays in contemporary landscape architecture. (New York: Princeton Architectural
Press, 1999), 13.
19
Cilento, Karen. “ James Corner Field Operations to design Qianhai”, (arch daily, 2010), http://www.archdaily.com/, (accessed
August 2010)
118 Refereed Full-Papers
approached from a community scale and Corner’s projects are mainly done from the city scale. They also have
their own specialties. Hester integrates natural amenities
with the neighborhood context, while Corner transforms
abandoned industrial spaces to green, post-industrial
urban spaces. However, both brought a new vision to
urban design, specifically where the urban fabric meets
the natural environment. One of Hester’s projects lies
in Casper, California. After a real estate crisis struck the
town, citizens banded together in an attempt to respond.
Their first action was to purchase ecologically fragile
lands to save their natural environment. They then
identified a natural structure for Casper based on the
landscape features. Basically the design framework was
whittled down to three conceivable actions: “(1) create a
backbone along the creek, (2) make the center the heart
of the community, and (3) save views by clustering new
villages like old ones.”20 Creating this simple framework gave developers enough flexibility to make the
projects feasible. The interaction between community
and developers based on the consensus on the framework was constructive and gentle. Finally, the developers and the community achieved their main objectives
while preserving the ecological context of the region
without any financial sacrifice.
This approach was derived by tracing a natural
structure on the ecological context of the built environment. “Natural process creates the overall structure that
accommodates change while maintaining fundamental
city form, and the built landscape and architecture provide malleable detailed settings.”21
The key principle in this approach is ‘adaptability’,
which means the capacity of an ecosystem to adapt in
changing conditions at a minimum cost.22 This principle is actually one of the common threads between
Hester and Corner’s ideology. Both propose the flexibility of design for the sake of adaptability. Flexibility
and adaptability of space is not just for the ecological
benefits, but they also help to define the program of
space in the process of adaptation. In that sense, Hester
suggests that “the most sensible solution for urban open
space is to create less defined and more flexible settings
that are suitable for any recreational or social purpose
that comes along.”23
Figure 6: Sacred Structre, flexibility about land use
decisions, Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological
Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006),
267.
Figure 7: Fresh Kills Lifescape, Corner, James.
“Lifescape - Fresh Kills parkland [Staten Island, New
York].” Topos: the international review of landscape
architecture and urban design, no. 51 (2005).
Large city landscapes are usually more flexible and
adaptable than small open spaces. In that sense, Corner
has acted as a spatial genius in turning large abandoned
urban spaces into resilient, usable public spaces. These
20
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 266.
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 256.
22
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 255.
23
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006), 259.
21
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
119
Refereed Full-Papers
large areas are usually industrial sites that have become
lost spaces in cities. Deteriorated train tracks, waste disposal land, and abandoned waterfronts are some of the
preferable locations for Corner’s work. They are large
enough to be flexible and small enough to be designed.
One of his famous projects in this case is Fresh Kills
Park on Staten Island in New York. Formerly the largest
landfill in New York City, the site is almost three times
larger than Central Park and would eventually become
a touristic and cultural destination for the city. Corner’s
strategy for the development of this huge park resembles Hester’s design for the small town. He does not
define the whole program of the park. A natural structure has been decided and the rest should be shaped in
the long term process. Minimum intervention in the
land form, maximum flexibility in the program, and the
creation of open vistas has increased the adaptability of
the land. It has been expected that after the evolution of
the land into a grand park, it would soon become ecologically rich and diverse. In this project, the inflexible
rigidness of some traditional urban design principles
has no place in the process.
The Aesthetic Context of Urban Design
Artistic principles are the main core of the urban design discipline. From the first writings of Camillo Sitte
and the American City Beautiful Movement, which
were the first initiators of this discipline, aesthetic
quality of city’s public spaces has been the focal topic
in urban design. But since the 1960s, one significant
criticism of urban design is that higher priority is given
to aesthetics over ethics and other social and ecological
issues. For example, the design of public spaces in a
neighborhood can be more important than the gentrification impact within that area, or the design of a beautiful suburb can be more significant than the ecological
shocks in the region. Design over social and ecological
management is one of the conflict points between the
aesthetic context and the sociological and ecological
context of urban design.
Through their own practices, Hester and Corner have
helped to ease these conflicts, although they have different approaches toward the aesthetic context of their
work. For Hester, art is not just a fine physical form but
the “sacredness” of that form or object which integrates
24
25
the art with sociology, psychology, and culture. On
the other hand, Corner has utilized the contemporary
technology and engineering of landscape architecture as
a catalyst to conciliate between art and ecology. What
Hester and Corner have done is spread the concept of
art into the broader territories of sociology and ecology.
In their work, aesthetics of space comes from the social
values, the natural landscape or the high tech artificial
landscape, which is in harmony with the ecology of the
place.
In Hester’s view, the appreciation of physical form
is mainly dependant on the social acceptance of that
form. In that sense, social engagement in the design
process takes on the character of aesthetics as well as
esthetically. When people become the sole creators of
a place, that place will have a totally different meaning
for them. Who will appreciate the artifact more than the
artist? In other words, what Hester has suggested is that
rather than a solo act of an artist to create a masterpiece,
turn the people into artists. Artists have enough knowledge from their place and are active to change it and
enjoy it. In that sense, Hester, in the last chapter of his
book, designs for ecological democracy, proposing five
design principles that are mainly social and ecological
facts, but will eventually “touch the people’s heart”:
•
Design for what people do all day
•
Design rooted in naturalness
•
Design based on local wisdom
•
Design with reciprocal stewardship
•
Design embedded with the rhythms of life24
On the other hand, Corner creates artistic spaces not
based on public participation but with the involvement
of many sophisticated artists, imaginers, engineers, and
technicians. In his well-known project, the High Line in
New York City, Corner’s firm collaborated with more
than ten corporations to design a park on an abandoned
elevated train track. The result is truly a work of art, an
industrial transportation facility reclaimed into a postindustrial instrument of recreation, leisure and greenness.25
The High Line is a distinct example of a natural
and artificial mixture. The combination of organic and
building materials needs an engineer’s expertise as
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy. (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006).
American Society of Landscape Architects. “2010 ASLA Professional Awards”, (ASLA,2010), http://www.asla.org/2010 awards,
(accessed August 2010).
120 Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 8: High Line, an ambitious urban reclamation project,
American Society of Landscape Architects. “2010 ASLA
Professional Awards”, http://www.asla.org/2010 awards/173.html.
Figure 9: the invention of a new paving and planting system,
American Society of Landscape Architects. “2010 ASLA
Professional Awards”, http://www.asla.org/2010 awards/173.html.
much as an artist’s imagination. One peculiarity of the
High Line is that its uniqueness is a main attraction to
the park, and engineering technology plays a huge role
in this sense of uniqueness.
Accordingly, the main distinction between Hester
and Corner is in their perception of what is aesthetically
beautiful. They both define art based on their ideology and apply it to the urban environments in different
ways. One is more socially oriented and the other is
more technically oriented. However, their main input to
the urban design discipline was the notion of utilizing
a multiplicity of tools and theories in a collaborative
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
approach through the lens of art and imagination. Based
on their practice and writings, urban design literature as
a whole has been expanded, and urban design principles
have been revitalized.
Conclusion
Is it possible to find physical solutions for the social
and ecological problems of cities? The first step to
answer this broad question is to define the nature of
“problems.” Scholars take different approaches. While
some put more emphasis on the current urban challenges, others try to establish a vision for urban life. A
121
Refereed Full-Papers
general distinction between these two trends is somehow similar to the old conflict between idealism and
pragmatism. This paper briefly explored the theory and
principles of two landscape architects, Randolph Hester
and James Corner, both of whom have tried to approach
the same goal with different proportions of idealism and
pragmatism.
Obviously these two men cannot represent the whole
contemporary urban design practice. But they are different enough to show the importance of scale in the
urban design methodology, while at the same time, they
are similar enough to prove the possibility of integration between ecology, sociology, and aesthetics in urban
design.
While James Corner put the “working method” in
the center of his theoretical framework, Randy Hester
emphasizes the “sacredness” of the sites. The difference
is the same as that between a planner and a poet; vision
vs. dream; meaning vs. senses. A vibrant discipline
needs both aspects. These two approaches demonstrate
that urban design is not a discrete discipline. It is essentially a multidisciplinary practice that defines itself
with the collaboration of theorists and technocrats. It
needs them to inclusively define current problems and
to find practical solutions. Therefore, it is impossible to
define a series of rigid principles applicable to different
locations and situations. In that sense, exploring other
disciplines, their practices and their theories, will help
to accomplish the flexibility and broadness that urban
design needs. Landscape architects Randy Hester and
James Corner have different approaches toward urban
design, but each of them has brought something new
that extends the borders of the field.
122 References
Architectural Association (Great Britain). Landscape Urbanism:
a manual for the machinic landscape. London: Architectural
Association, 2003.
Corner, James. Recovering landscape: essays in contemporary
landscape architecture. New York: Princeton Architectural
Press, 1999.
Corner, James. “Lifescape - Fresh Kills parkland [Staten Island,
New York].” Topos: the international review of landscape
architecture and urban design, no. 51 (2005).
Corner, James. “Shelby Farms Park: one park, one million
trees, twelve landscapes [Memphis, Tennessee].” Topos: the
international review of landscape architecture and urban
design, no. 66 (2009).
Corner, James, and Alex S. MacLean. Taking measures across
the American landscape. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1996.
Farr, Douglas. Sustainable urbanism: urban design with nature.
A Wiley book on sustainable design. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley,
2008.
Hester, Randolph T. Design for Ecological Democracy.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 2006.
Hester, Randolph T. Neighborhood Space. Stroudsburg, Pa.:
Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross , 1975.
Hester, Randolph T. Planning Neighborhood Space With People.
2nd ed. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co., 1984.
Kolb, Jaffer. “The High Line parkland project paves the way for a
new kind of urbanism in the U.S..” Architectural review 226,
no. 1351 (September 2009).
Krieger, Alex, and William S. Saunders. Urban Design.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 2009.
Mostavi, Mohsen., Gareth. Doherty, and Harvard University.
Ecological urbanism. Baden, Switzerland: Lars Muller, 2010.
Waldheim, Charles. The Landscape Urbanism Reader. New York:
Princeton Architectural Press, 2006.
Refereed Full-Papers
Positive Environments:
Emotional Relief Through
Evidence-Based Design of a
Neonatal Intensive Care Unit
Elizabeth Heroux (Savannah College of Art
and Design)
Abstract
The purpose of this paper is to aid in the design of
a neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) that addresses
the physical, psychological, and emotional needs of
parents, while implementing patient-focused care and
promoting staff efficiency. The thesis will provide a
prototypical design, accompanied by guidelines, which
together will serve as tools to streamline the design
process of a specific healthcare environment. Through
the compare and contrast of existing facilities, including a site visit to one local NICU, the thesis argues that
although numerous NICU designs claim to address the
needs of parents, none have successfully addressed
all needs of the parents. This argument will also be
addressed by survey findings gathered from parents
who have experienced a NICU in conjunction with an
analysis of literature relating to parents within a NICU
and environment psychology. Specific programmatic
requirements for parents, infant-patients, and staff will
also be addressed in the thesis. The survey findings,
interviews, literature review, precedent studies, and
site visit will serve as research-based evidence for the
prototypical design and guideline content.
Introduction
Thesis Statement
To demonstrate how research-based design evidence
can generate a psychologically supportive environment
by promoting emotional relief for a parent in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU).
In the past, designers have approached the NICU
environment with the infant-patients’ needs, as well as
staff needs, as the priority. This project will approach
the third user group within the NICU environment by
addressing the needs, emotional as well as physical,
of the parent and/or visitor. Since the visitor within a
NICU is most commonly a parent or direct relative to
the infant-patient, there are many psychological, physi-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
ological, and emotional triggers within the environment,
such as sudden equipment alarms and flashing lights,
which need to be addressed due to their current negative
association to the infants’ condition.
Through the combination of visceral, behavioral, and
reflective design, the proposed NICU will eliminate
the negative associations of the physical environment.
Because of stress reduction, the negative associations
within the space will then be replaced with positive associations. Maintaining comfortable levels of parents’
emotions by addressing the fundamental psychological
processes (arousal, overload, affect, adaptation, and
personal control) through design will allow the negative
associations to be replaced with positive associations
throughout the environment, promoting emotional relief
and a reduction in stress.
History - Neonatology & NICUs
Despite its development in the late nineteenth
century, neonatology gained the most significance in
the healthcare world during the late 1950s and 1960s
(Hellmann, 2006). Sparked by the invention of the
incubator in 1880, neonatal health care has consistently
improved, resulting in a decrease of prenatal mortality
rates (PMR).
Another major contributor to the decrease in prenatal mortality rates was the development of the first
Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) in the United
States in 1965 at the Yale-New Haven Hospital in New
Haven, Connecticut (Neonatology on the Web, 2009).
NICUs are specialized units, typically within a hospital,
“designed and equipped to support premature babies”
(Hellmann, 2006), as well as full-term newborns with
other conditions. NICUs are classified by Level (I, II,
or III). Level I NICUs are equipped to treat less serious
cases, while Levels II and III are designed to support
the more serious cases. Since most hospitals are only
equipped with a Level I NICU, transport is provided
either for the infant directly or for the pregnant mother
prior to birth (typically in known high-risk cases) to a
hospital with a Level II or III.
In many cases, preterm birth is an unplanned event,
which, depending on the condition of the newborn, can
be classified as an emergency. Due to the unwelcome
surprise-factor emotionally associated with a newborn
being admitted to a NICU, “life in the NICU is often
one of intense and conflicting emotions” (Hellmann,
2006). Often thought of as an emotional rollercoaster,
life in the NICU for a parent is filled with stress, fear of
123
Refereed Full-Papers
the unknown, worry for the life of the baby, and even
guilt. Sometimes parents are even reluctant to bond
with their newborn in the NICU with fear of the bond
breaking through tragedy (Hellmann, 2006).
While admitted to a NICU, the infant is constantly
being monitored and cared for by the hospital staff and
doctors. Treatment of a patient in a NICU involves
various forms of technology and machines, depending
on the condition of the infant.
The proposed research-based design project of a
Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) will address the
psychological, physiological, and environmental needs
of the parent and/or visitor. NICUs have historically
been designed around family-centered care, patientfocused design, and to increase staff efficiency. The
proposed design will combine the previously listed
considerations while focusing on the needs of the parent
and/or visitor. Hence, instead of founding the design
solution upon a patient-focused perspective, the final
design solution will be intended to address and fully
meet the psychological, physiological, and emotional
experience within the space for the parent(s).
By gaining an understanding of the psychological
and medical considerations pertaining to a NICU, the
research-based design may be able to provide the parent focus population with the environmental support
required to address their psychological and physiological needs.
Prototypical & Guidelines
Taking the place of the client in the thesis, a prototypical design, along with an accompanying set of written guidelines, will provide solutions for the goals and
objectives for the thesis project. Spatial solutions will
be provided for the three separate levels of NICUs (levels I, II, and III) limiting restrictions for future clients
(organizations, etc.) who would implement the prototypical. These spatial variations will allow any level of
NICU to be designed and configured according to the
prototypical guidelines to not only achieve the overall
goals, but also to allow applicants to apply the designs
within an existing space (with regards to restrictions
and constraints provided by the existing space).
The prototypical guidelines will also provide a set
of materiality guidelines to aid designers in material
selection for specification in accordance with national,
federal, state, and (for the purpose of the thesis) Atlanta-specific guidelines. The guidelines will also provide
instruction on the locations of spaces, which spaces
are required or applicable to each level (I, II, or III)
124 of NICU, recommended location of amenities, recommended space planning for each space, recommended
locations of windows, lighting requirements and restrictions, and material requirements and restrictions.
Goals and Objectives
The thesis will have four major design goals that will
support the goals in regards to the user (parent) and the
overall prototypical and guideline goals. The first design goal will be to provide an environment that focuses
on patient-focused care, while supporting the physical
and psychological needs of the parents, and encouraging family-centered care, which will maximize parental
participation in the care of the infant-patient. This will
be achieved by including, and designing, spaces within
the NICU, or in close proximity, to provide support for
the needs of the parent (eating, sleeping, etc.). One example of a space type which will provide for the parental need of privacy with their infant is the use of single
occupancy rooms for infant care levels requiring more
than a short-term (one week or less) stay in the NICU.
The second design goal will be to provide space planning that enables maximum staff efficiency and supports
future advancements in technology and accessibility.
By providing prototypical designs and guidelines for a
range of sizes for each space, (example: minimal space
requirements, median, and maximum recommended
space size), the potential future client would be able to
implement the appropriate size space to allow for the
necessary space adjacencies. Additionally, by providing
criteria and guidelines for design concept application
and the specification of materials to be applied within
the space, the future client will be able to successfully
customize the layout, design, and feel of their space.
This level of flexibility will allow any organization or
corporation to adopt the prototypical and guidelines if
their mission, goals, and objectives accept the overall
goal and purpose of the thesis.
The material specification guidelines and criteria
will include specific requirements per space, including:
acoustical requirements, material compositions, finishes,
clean-ability codes, indoor-air quality considerations,
and sustainability considerations and requirements.
Program
Interior Spaces
Each interior space in the prototypical NICU will be
designed with regard to column spacing requirements
and regulations from the Facility Guidelines Institute
(FGI), National Fire and Protection Agency regulations
Refereed Full-Papers
(NFPA), International Building Codes (IBC), and the
Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA). Spaces not
listed below, but included in the prototypical design,
will utilize program prototypical square footage.
Single-Family Patient Rooms
Since it is the most important room within the NICU,
the single family patient room serves as the hub of all
activity. The patient room is designed for maximum
parental privacy in a level III NICU due to the elevated
level of critical care for the infant-patients. Each patient
room will be designed to accommodate a single patient
or multiples and their parents, and will be planned according to three user-zones to allow for the needs of
each group (infant-patient, parents, and staff) to be met.
By creating a hierarchal adjacency with the infant as the
majority and an even proximity between the staff and
parents, the collaborative staff-parent communication
level will be met. Parental psychological needs will be
met by providing a private and safe area within the patient room for parents to have privacy with their infant,
allowing for a more natural bonding of parent and child
while in the NICU. Additionally, providing seating and
access to daylight within the space provides emotional
relaxation opportunities. Allowing the parents to have
a designated area within the room keeps parents out
of the way of staff duties while observing and receiving constant updates of their baby’s condition. Also, a
sofa with built-in storage provides a sleeping space for
parents in the same room as their infant, allowing for
parents to remain in close proximity to their baby at all
times.
Dual-Family and Multiple-Family Patient Rooms
Following suit of the single-family patient room, the
dual and multiple family patient rooms provide all of
the same privacy considerations for parents and their
infant. However, due to the shared nature of the rooms,
the privacy is on varying levels. This change in privacy
through shared space is due to the different severity levels of patient conditions justified by the level of NICU.
In contrast to the single-family patient room, sleeping
spaces will be provided in rooms within close proximity
to the patient rooms, allowing parents to get rest within
the NICU while not invading the privacy of other families in the shared patient rooms.
Isolation Patient Rooms
In addition to the standard patient rooms, each level
of the NICU will have numerous isolation patient
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
rooms to provide for the care of special patient conditions. Each isolation room is capable of dropping from
normal to zero pressure in order to contain health conditions within the space. The isolation patient rooms
are planned to be identical to the single-family patient
rooms, but their location within the NICU is adjacent
to the support stations to allow for optimum observation by staff. A sofa will be provided within the rooms
for parents; however, sleeping within the room will be
dependent on staff authorization due to the condition of
the patient.
Transient Care Rooms
Transient Care rooms will be provided in both the
level I and II NICUs due to the dual and multiple family
patient rooms. The transient care rooms will be utilized
by parents and their infant during the last several nights
(dependent on hospital policy) their infant is admitted
to the NICU. Parents will provide all care for the infant
within this space with staff supervision. Each transient
care room will provide a space for the infant bed, life
support connections, sleeping space for two parents,
seating for two parents, a full bathroom, television,
electrical outlets, workspace, and lockable storage.
Parent Sleep Rooms
Each level of the NICU will provide a minimum of
two parent sleep rooms. Similar to a staff on-call room,
these rooms will be solely utilized for personal sleep
spaces while within the NICU. Each sleep room will
provide a twin size bed, small table, and single chair.
Parent sleep room locations will be dependent on NICU
layout, yet proximity to patient rooms and/or bathrooms
is preferred. Parent sleep rooms will not utilize windows.
Staff Charting Areas
In order to provide a space for staff to work and chart
while observing the patients, staff charting areas will
be located on the exterior of every two patient rooms.
Locating the charting areas outside of patient rooms
also allows for parent privacy within the rooms. Each
charting area will be equipped with computer equipment for patient charting and vital monitoring, as well
as storage, task seating, and angled windows viewing
into the patient rooms for complete observation. Each
charting station will be paired with the charting area
directly across the corridor to allow one staff member to
fully observe up to 4 rooms individually.
125
Refereed Full-Papers
Support Stations
Numerous support stations will be located within
each level of the NICU. Each support station will be
centrally located within the corridors and will have
multiple points of entry/exit for ease of circulation for
staff. The stations will provide work space for a minimum of four staff members each, hold administrative
and nursing equipment, and provide “perch” points for
doctors and specialists. Each station will be easily approachable for parents and provide a clean line of sight
across the corridor spaces. In addition, central monitoring systems will be in place for secondary observation
of patient vitals.
NICU Reception and Waiting Area
Each level of the NICU will have its own reception
and waiting area separate from the main facility. The
reception and waiting area will be located off of the
main lobby of the building level (proposed as level
four) behind the NICU main entry door, which will be
kept in the closed position, allowing for division and
privacy. Located directly across from the main entrance
of each NICU will be the restricted entry to the actual
NICU. Located in direct line of sight upon entering the
NICU main entry will be the reception window, which
serves as not only visitor check-in but also security for
the unit entrance. After seeing the receptionist, each
visitor will be seated in the NICU waiting area that will
be located to the side of the reception and main entry
area, allowing for privacy of visitors and sound control.
The waiting area will contain a variety of soft seating
in conversation and community layouts, along with
various forms of entertainment including television and
reading components.
Lounge for Parents
In addition to parent spaces within patient rooms
and the NICU waiting area, each level of the NICU
will provide a parent (and/or family) lounge. Each
lounge will vary in size and scale of amenities, with
the minimum requirements including: a kitchen space
with microwave, sink, and mini refrigerator, soft seating, dining area with chairs, and a television. The level
III NICU will include a full kitchen, eating area, soft
seating area with television, reading area, workstation,
laundry room, and full bathroom. The lounge provides
a space for parents to relax, escape, or socialize outside
of the patient rooms.
126 Full Bathrooms
Full bathrooms equipped with shower stalls will be
provided for parents in every level of the NICU. These
bathrooms will be located off of the main corridor
and adjacent to patient rooms. Each bathroom will be
unisex, and provide a toilet, shower stall with privacy
curtain or glass enclosure, vanity with counter space,
and robe hooks. Additional bathroom and/or shower
spaces will also be located through the parent lounge,
parent gowning area, and NICU waiting area (where
applicable).
Resource Center
A resource center will be available for parents and
families to use while within the NICU. In a level I
NICU, the resource center will be located within the
parent lounge, while in the level II and III NICUs, it
will be adjacent and separate from the lounge. Each resource center will provide specific library resources for
parents in various formats, including: books, journals,
magazines, periodicals, and digital formats. A combination of soft seating and workspaces will be provided
both with and without privacy barriers. Windows are
preferred in the space where applicable.
Sibling Play Area
Each NICU will provide a sibling play area for
existing children of parents (and/or families) of infantpatients admitted to the NICU. The play area will be
separate from the existing facility and will only be available for use by NICU families. Due to the strict visitation restrictions regarding children under the age of
twelve in a NICU, the sibling play area will be accessed
through the NICU waiting area, preventing children
from entering the NICU main corridor without a staff
escort. Restrooms will also be adjacent to the play area
and will be accessed through the waiting area biding
staff remittance. The play area will provide various levels of entertainment and resources for various child age
groups, including: television, computer (with restricted
Internet access), soft toys, seating areas, reading, and
limited art activities. A combination of soft seating and
workstations will be provided at various scales.
Meditation Room
A minimum of one meditation space will be located
in each level of the NICU. In a level I NICU, the meditation space will be accessed through the parent lounge,
and for the level II and III NICUs, meditation spaces
Refereed Full-Papers
will be located separate and adjacent to the parent
lounge. Each meditation space will consist of minimal
furnishings and will provide a view to outside (where
possible). Maximum acoustical considerations will be
applied to assure maximum privacy.
Lactation Room
Each level of the NICU will provide a minimum of
two lactation support rooms for parental use. The lactation room will provide privacy for parents when breastfeeding and/or pumping breast milk for their infant(s).
The space will be large enough to accommodate an
incubator, a minimum of one parent (the mother), and
one staff member. The room components will include:
a hands-free hand washing station, storage, a minimum
of one comfortable chair for the mother, a secondary
seat, a staff stool, and privacy curtain. A window will
be provided with a view into the corridor to allow the
space to be multifunctional.
Privacy and Security Requirements
The delicate nature sometimes associated with childbirth and pregnancies requires all NICUs and maternity
units to be located above ground level within a facility.
The level of high security at entry points provides both
space planning and design restrictions, while promoting complete physical and psychological security for all
users with in the space. Because of these heightened
restrictions, the NICU is typically off limits to all who
are not directly related to the specialized unit, including
hospital staff and maintenance (see Figure 5-1). Parents
of infant-patients are provided special identification
numbers corresponding to their infant within the NICU,
which must be used even when simply calling for status
updates on their infant’s condition. Additionally, many
hospitals admit “special” civil case infants who have
been exposed to custody disputes. These special cases
of added threat require many hospitals, like Wellstar
Cobb in Austell, Georgia, to attach special security sensors or disks to the infant by way of their identification
band, which would lock down the NICU in the chance
the baby were to be removed from the unit.
Therefore, the NICU within the confines of this
thesis will be located on the fourth (4th) floor of the
prototypical hospital facility. Within the unit, all
patient rooms will be located a specific distance from
all stairwells and indirect entrances and/or exits for the
protection of the infant-patients and parents. Video surveillance will be installed and utilized at all entry and
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 5-1: Privacy & Security Diagram. Prototypical
NICU.
exit points of the NICU (including emergency exits) and
will be monitored within the unit. Additionally, all staff
members and maintenance personnel with authorization to enter the NICU will be granted access cards for
remittance into the unit. Additional security regulations
and precautions may be added, or existing regulations
removed, at the discretion of the institution utilizing the
information provided in this thesis.
NICU Location
The placement of the NICU within a new or existing
facility will be dependent on various considerations.
Due to the privacy and security regulations previously
stated, the NICU will be located a minimum of two
levels above street level or the main entrance level of
the facility. Additionally, the NICU will never have
a direct entrance and/or exit from the outside into the
unit. Once located on a building level, the NICU will
be a closed unit (as previously stated) and will not serve
as through access to other units on any level. Adja-
127
Refereed Full-Papers
cent units sharing a building level with the NICU will
include maternity, labor/delivery, post-partum, and
recovery. Requested, but not required adjacencies for
parental benefit include: chapel, cafeteria, fitness center,
resource center, and green spaces (see Figure 5-2).
Design Development
Parental Experience
A parent experience originates from a few different pathways. If a parent is admitted to the hospital
as a patient themselves, they would access the NICU
from the unit, or department, where they are a patient,
such as maternity or post-partum. However, if the
parent(s) are not currently patients within the hospital,
their journey begins when they leave their home to
visit the NICU. Once on the facility site, the parents
will interact with other visitors, patients, and staff from
various departments separate from the NICU. Depending on the level of NICU where their infant is admitted,
the parent(s) will spend various amounts of time in the
main facility utilizing amenities including the cafeteria, chapel, and fitness center (where applicable). The
parents then travel in the elevator to the floor where the
NICU is located, which for the purpose of this thesis is
the fourth floor. The fourth floor is still considered the
main facility, as it is open to and shared by other patients, visitors, and staff accessing the maternity, postpartum, delivery/labor, and the nursery. However, once
Figure 5-2: NICU Location. Prototypical NICU.
128 the parents enter the NICU, they are in a restricted area
which is not accessible from any other department. The
restricted access regulations of the NICU go beyond security and privacy to form a community within the unit.
After checking in at the reception desk, the parent(s)
will wait in the waiting area until they are granted access to the unit. Before the receptionist buzzes them
in, the parents must utilize the scrub and gowning area,
where they will verify they are gowned and scrubbed
clean to limit the contaminants they bring into the
NICU. The parents will be required to scrub up again
every time they wish to enter the NICU, despite the
amount of time spent outside of the unit. If the parents
bring children with them on their visit, they will be
required to check the child into the sibling play area,
where the child must remain while their parents are
within the restricted area. A child above the age of
twelve (may vary according to facility policies) will
only be permitted to enter the NICU with staff and parent supervision. While in the sibling play area, a child
may access the restrooms located in the waiting area
with permission and supervision from staff. Additionally, the age restrictions on children left in the play area
without parental supervision will be determined by the
facility.
Once they are beyond the restricted access door, the
parents will typically go straight to their infant’s room
and remain there for the majority of their visit. During
staff shift changes (determined by the facility), parents
will be required to vacate the patient room for a specific amount of time, during which they may stay in the
parent spaces, waiting area, or leave the NICU entirely.
While within the NICU, the parents have access to all
parent spaces, including the lounge, kitchen, eating areas, restrooms, resource center (where applicable), and
sleeping spaces. Parents must scrub up upon leaving
the parent spaces before re-entering their infant’s room.
Additionally, resources available to parents while in the
NICU vary depending on the level of NICU they are in,
and may include lactation support, consultation rooms,
and a psychologist. Some of these support spaces may
be utilized by both the parents and staff, through interaction and individually.
When the parent(s) leave the space, despite the reason or circumstances, they will proceed back through
the restricted access door, visit the gowning area and
locker room if needed, then return to the level four main
elevator lobby, then proceed through the building and
leave the site.
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 6: Parental Experience. Prototypical NICU.
Concept
Despite the end deliverable of this thesis being a
prototypical, a concept needs to be applied in order
to maximize the justification of the design solutions.
Since the purpose of this thesis is to stress the importance of designing a NICU to adhere to parental needs,
both as individual entities as well as in relation to the
benefits provided to the infant-patient(s), it is imperative that the concept stress the same points. Due to
the fact that when a baby is admitted to the NICU the
natural bonding of mother and child is interrupted, the
author looked to nature in search of any resemblance
to a NICU. A prime example of a NICU (in theory)
found in nature is the bird’s nest. Prior to the breeding
season, birds gather materials such as twigs, grass, hair,
and string, which are then used to build nests for future
young. Unlike humans, birds are born from eggs which
are laid rather than live birth. Once laid, the mother
and father birds alternate sitting on the eggs to incubate
them or keep them warm. Upon hatching, the baby
birds (plural due to the typical number of eggs laid at a
time) have yet to acquire their feathers, open their eyes,
or be able to fly. While the birds complete their development in the nest, the mother bird protects the nest as
well as hunts for and feeds the babies. Once they have
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 7: Baby Birds in Nest. Getty Images.
finished developing and are able to fly, the babies will
leave the nest and venture off into the world.
While the care for the young is provided by the parent birds, the nest serves as the physical protector of
the babies, acting as the barrier from the young and the
world. The nest is therefore symbolically translated
into the NICU. Taking the concept of the nest a step
further, the author examines the caregiver. While the
care in the nest is provided by the parents, in the NICU
the majority of care is provided by staff with parental
graces (or inclusions) in the menial tasks. This causes
129
Refereed Full-Papers
an interruption in the natural bonding of mother and
child (and/or father and child) which may lead to communication difficulties in the child’s developmental
future. The concept supports the increased role of parents in the care for the infant-patients by allowing the
parents to act as caregivers and lapse the interruption in
natural bonding of mother and child.
First, one must look at what conditions are currently
preventing parents from remaining in the NICU with
their infant(s). In some cases, directly after the birth of
an infant, a mother must remain in the maternity ward
(which in many hospitals is located separate from the
NICU) and therefore cannot physically visit the NICU.
Many NICUs have limited visitation hours for parents
and visitors, which once the mother is no longer admitted to the hospital herself, can cause conflicts when
travelling to and from the hospital at specific times. In
addition to the travel conflict facing parents, there are
very few NICUs that allow parents to stay overnight.
According to Abraham Maslow (1943), each person
has a set of physiological needs which must be met in a
specific order for a person to remain within an environment for an extended period of time. Many NICUs do
not provide basic amenities to parents; for example, the
NICU at Wellstar Cobb Hospital in Georgia does not
even provide a restroom for parents and/or visitors to
use while in the NICU. According to Wellstar Cobb
neonatal nurse manager Tiffany Perry, parents must exit
the NICU and utilize the public facilities located in the
building core, then scrub up again before re-entering
the NICU. In addition to failing to provide basic resources for parents and visitors, these restrictions also
open the NICU to heightened contamination levels as a
result of the number of times parents have to leave the
NICU and return.
Conclusion
By allowing parents to remain in the “nest” of the
NICU and care for their babies (even if not physically), the psychological and emotional stresses will be
reduced simply by granting parents the ability to remain
with their infant(s). Providing parent spaces in each
patient room allows parents to have a visual protection
over their infant(s), as well as provide physical and
emotional support, which contributes to the healthy
development of the infant. Additionally, this will allow
mothers to provide nutrients through breastfeeding
when the infant’s condition permits, eliminating staff
formula feeding due to the mother’s proximity to the
infant.
130 Emotional solutions will be provided through proximity and function, while being supported by materiality and light. The thesis acts as a preliminary tool for
designers to apply within an existing facility plan, test
fitting the proposed research-based design guidelines.
Further development of the materiality guidelines,
proposed fixtures, furniture and equipment (FF&E), and
lighting recommendations are proposed by the author
before a test fit could be granted possible success.
Furthermore, by utilizing design as a tool to provide
psychological, physiological, and emotional relief to
a user group within a space, specific programmatic
requirements for parents, infant-patients, and staff may
be brought together to create a more positive experience
which would otherwise be just the opposite.
Bibliography
An Introduction to Evidence-Based Design: Exploring Healthcare
and Design. (2008). . The Center For Health Design.
Averill, J. R. (1973). Personal control over aversive stimuli and
its relationship to stress. Psychological Bulletin, 80(4), 286303. doi: 10.1037/h0034845.
Blackburn, S. (1998). Environmental impact of the NICU on
developmental outcomes. Journal of Pediatric Nursing,
13(5), 279-289.
Brown, P., & Taquino, L. T. (2001). Designing and delivering
neonatal care in single rooms. The Journal of Perinatal &
Neonatal Nursing, 15(1), 68-83.
Byers, J. F. (2003). Components of developmental care and the
evidence for their use in the NICU. MCN. The American
Journal of Maternal Child Nursing, 28(3), 174-180; quiz
181-182.
Carpman, J. R., & Grant, M. A. (1993). Design That Cares:
Planning Health Facilities for Patients and Visitors (Second
Edition.). San Francisco, California: American Hospital
Publishing, Inc.
Carter, B. S., Carter, A., & Bennett, S. (2008). Families’ views
upon experiencing change in the neonatal intensive care
unit environment: from the ‘baby barn’ to the private room.
Journal of Perinatology: Official Journal of the California
Perinatal Association, 28(12), 827-829. doi: 10.1038/
jp.2008.102.
Cleveland, L. M. (2008). Parenting in the neonatal intensive
care unit. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal
Nursing: JOGNN / NAACOG, 37(6), 666-691. doi: 10.1111/
j.1552-6909.2008.00288.x.
DesigningNNU_May2004b.pdf. (n.d.). . Retrieved December
14, 2009, from http://www.bapm.org/media/documents/
publications/DesigningNNU_May2004b.pdf.
Fegran, L., & Helseth, S. (2009). The parent–nurse relationship
in the neonatal intensive care unit context – closeness
and emotional involvement. Scandinavian Journal of
Caring Sciences, 23(4), 667-673. doi: 10.1111/j.14716712.2008.00659.x.
Floyd, A. M. D. (2005). Challenging designs of neonatal
intensive care units. Critical Care Nurse, 25(5), 59-66.
Refereed Full-Papers
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A.,
& Gruen, R. J. (1986). Dynamics of a Stressful Encounter:
Cognitive Appraisal, Coping, and Encounter Outcomes.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 9921003. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992.
Foucault, M. (1973). The Birth of the Clinic. Tavisstock
Publications Limited.
Hughes, M., McCollum, J. A., Sheftel, D., & Sanchez, G.
(1994). How Parents Cope With the Experience of Neonatal
Intensive Care. Children’s Health Care, 23(1), 1-15.
Johnson, B. H., Abraham, M. R., & Parrish, R. N. (2004).
Designing the neonatal intensive care unit for optimal family
involvement. Clinics in Perinatology, 31(2), 353-382, ix. doi:
10.1016/j.clp.2004.04.008.
Kilment, S. A. (Ed.). (2008). Building Type Basics for Healthcare
Facilities (Second Edition.). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley &
Sons, Inc.
Kopec, D. (2006). Environmental Psychology for Design. China:
Fairchild Publications Inc.
Lawson, K., Daum, C., & Turkewitz, G. (1977). Environmental
Characteristics of a Neonatal Intensive-Care Unit. Child
Development, 48(4), 1633-1639. doi: I: 10.1111/1467-8624.
ep10400208.
Leibrock, C. A. (2000). Design Details for Health: Making the
Most of Interior Design’s Healing Potentional. Canada: John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Lewis, M., Bendersky, M., Koons, A., Hegyi, T., Hiatt, I. M.,
Ostfeld, B., et al. (1991). Visitation to a Neonatal Intensive
Care Unit. Pediatrics, 88(4), 795-801.
Malkin, J. (2002). Medical and Dental Space Planning: A
Comprehensive Guide to Design, Equipment and Clinical
Procedures (Third Edition.). New York: John Wiley & Sons,
Inc.
Marberry, S. O. (Ed.). (1995). Innovations in Healthcare Design.
Canada: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Miller, R. L., & Swensson, E. S. (2002). Hospital and Healthcare
Facility Design (Second Edition.). Singapore: Richard L.
Miller and Earl S. Swensson.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Neonatology on the Web: Timeline of Neonatology. (n.d.). .
Retrieved January 15, 2010, from http://www.neonatology.
org/tour/timeline.html.
NICU Design Standards. (n.d.). . Retrieved December 14, 2009,
from http://www.nd.edu/~nicudes/.
Shepley, M. M. (2004). Evidence-based design for infants
and staff in the neonatal intensive care unit. Clinics
in Perinatology, 31(2), 299-311, vii. doi: 10.1016/j.
clp.2004.04.005.
Sosnowitz, B. G. (1984). Managing Parents in Neonatal Intensive
Care Units. Social Problems, 31(4), 390-402.
The Center for Health Design. (2009). Evidence Based Building
Design for Healthcare. Retrieved July 5, 2009, from http://
www.healthdesign.org/.
Thurman, S. K., & Korteland, C. (1989). The Behavior of
Mothers and Fathers toward Their Infants during Neonatal
Intensive Care Visits. Children’s Health Care, 18(4), 247252.
Ulrich, R. S. (1991). Effects of Interior Design on Wellness:
Theory and Recent Scientific Research. Journal of
Healthcare Design, 3, 97-109.
White, R. D. (2004b). Mothers’ arms--the past and future locus
of neonatal care? Clinics in Perinatology, 31(2), 383-387, ix.
doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2004.04.009.
Wigert, H., Johansson, R., Berg, M., & Hellstrom, A. L. (2006).
Mothers’ experiences of having their newborn child in
a neonatal intensive care unit. Scandinavian Journal
of Caring Sciences, 20(1), 35-41. doi: 10.1111/j.14716712.2006.00377.x.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1970). The emerging discipline of environmental
psychology. American Psychologist, 25(4), 303-312. doi:
10.1037/h0029448.
Zeskind, P. S., & Iacino, R. (1984). Effects of Maternal Visitation
to Preterm Infants in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. Child
Development, 55(5), 1887-1894. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.
ep7304603.
131
Refereed Full-Papers
A Survey on the Social Activity
of the Demented Elderly by
RFID in the Special Care Unit
Yao-Rong Hwang (National Yunlin University
of Science & Technology, Taiwan), Tan-Kui Hsu
(Chungyu Institute of Technology, Taiwan)
Abstract
The demand for social activities for Alzheimer’s
disease patients has been considered much more
important than that for the frail elderly, because social
activities are able to facilitate the cognitive function of
demented elders. The Assessment of Quality of Life in
Alzheimer’s disease evaluates a patient’s interpersonal
relationships and ability to participate in meaningful
activities as major items affecting their quality of life.
In the past, the relationships between activity and place
were processed by the researcher’s observations or by
interviews with users. A technology that can replace the
proxy method and directly receive information from a
patient is desirable. The purpose of this research is to
apply RFID in tracing the travel routes of demented
elderly individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, in order to
understand their social activities in a special care unit.
A special care unit accommodating 64 demented residents was represented as a typical local care unit offering caregiving for the demented elderly and providing
multiple spaces for occupational therapy and activities.
It had four floors, with the day room on the second
floor. Seven subjects out of 17 demented elders who
were recognized as having daily social activity after a
one-month pilot study were selected to participate in
this survey with their families’ consent. The content of
the survey included any social activity happening in the
day room.
The results showed that the subjects had verbal social
activities, including saying hello, chatting, talking,
saying goodbye, saying thanks, and singing. Most of
the subjects were capable of saying hello, chatting, and
talking. Although the content of the conversations was
short, the subjects could express a variety of language.
The subjects also had non-verbal social activities,
including smiling, embracing, or touching the shoulders
of others, listening to others’ conversations, feeding
other residents, and holding other residents’ hands.
However, most of the subjects were only capable of
132 smiling. Subjects engaged in verbal social activities in
the day room, the dining area, leisure area, public area
(in front of the elevator and in the corridor) and playing
area. When the subjects engaged in non-verbal social
activities in the day room, they did so in any place in
the room. Dining and leisure areas played the major
role for the social activities of the demented elderly.
The differences of social activity between the residents
were caused by individual characteristics without the
influence of variables such as gender, age, education,
and residence length, according to the results of this
survey. Many demented individuals with severe behavioral problems need to record their behaviors by tracing
the travel route of a whole day in order to analyze the
occurrences of behaviors. RFID has the potential to
assist special care units in such areas as exit control and
behavior management.
Introduction
For a resident, admission to a nursing home involves adapting to other people and other activities, creating new social relationships, and finding resources for
support while being disabled by various impairments.
Being successful in social engagement can be regarded
as a critical component of the quality of life for nursing
home residents (Mor, Branco, Fleishman, Hawes, Phillips et al., 1995). Social functioning focuses on engagement and aims to measure the residents’ sense of initiative and involvement. It allows every resident to have
a high sense of initiative and involvement and respond
adequately to social stimuli in social environments
through participating in social activities and interacting
with other residents and staff. There is evidence that
engagement in meaningful social activities is related to
the quality of life for individuals residing in long-term
care facilities (Gonzalez-Salvador, Lysetkos, Baker,
Hovanec, Roques et al., 2000). For example, participation in activities such as music, exercise, or cooking
is associated with less depression and better cognition
and mobility (Kiely, Simon, Jones, & Morris, 2000).
Previous research has associated low social engagement
with increased mortality and cognitive decline (Dobbs,
Munn, Zimmerman, Boustani, Williams, Sloane et al.,
2005). Depressed residents are likely to have more difficulty in engaging themselves in new environments, as
residents who are not socially engaged are more likely
to be depressed (Achterberg, Pot, Kerkstra, Muller,
& Ribbe, 2003). Thus, the positive aspects of social
functioning were included in the Resident Assessment
Refereed Full-Papers
Instrument that was mandated by the U.S. Congress.
The Assessment of Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s
disease evaluates a patient’s physical condition, mood,
interpersonal relationships, ability to participate in
meaningful activities, and financial situation as major
items affecting quality of life (Logsdon, Gibbons, McCurry, & Teri, 2000). Agitation, aggression, depression,
and social withdrawal have been chosen as critical
variables in many studies because they occurred frequently in Alzheimer’s special care units. Cognitive and
ADL impairments have been found to be related to low
social engagement, as have sensory and communication losses (Frijters, Achterberg, Hirdes, Fries, Morris,
& Steel, 2001). The demand for social activities for
Alzheimer’s disease patients has been considered much
more important than that for the frail elderly because
social activities are able to facilitate the cognitive function of demented elders. Traditionally, a special care
unit differs from nursing homes in that they promote
a more social model of care (e.g., resident autonomy
and choice in a home-like environment) (Cohen & Day,
1991). However, their families may expect them to
have more activity involvement than residents in nursing homes. Thus, it is useful to understand residents’
involvement in activities.
Common spaces with a unique non-institutional
character are associated with reduced social withdrawal.
In the United States, some states have required multiple
social activity spaces to be provided for the demented
elderly to have leisure, dining, group games, and
entertainment within the special care unit (Herdman,
& Behney, 1992). Attributes of both the physical and
the social environment are included under the title of
“environmental proximal factors.” (Zeisel, Silverstein,
Hyde, Levkoff, Lawton, & Holmes, 2003) It is timely
to carefully consider how the living environment can
more humanely and cost-effectively contribute to managing and reducing these symptoms (Day, Carreon, &
Stump, 2000). In past years, the relationships between
activity and place were processed by the researcher’s
observations or by interviews with users. Studies on the
activities of the elderly with Alzheimer’s disease have
been performed either using the care provider’s vision,
which is completed by a proxy and rates both activities
and their effects, or using the resident’s vision, which
is completed by the person with dementia and which
rates only activities (Lawton, 1995). Nevertheless, there
are difficulties involved in questioning demented elders
owing to their cognitive and verbal limitations. Thus,
a technology that can replace the proxy method and
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
directly receive information from a patient is desirable.
In recent years, GPS (Global Positioning System)
technology has been applied in the tracing of travel
routes during surveys of elders’ activities. However,
it has been demonstrated that GPS technology is only
effective for the recording of outdoor open spaces.
Radio Frequency Identification (RFID) is considered
as a useful technique for recording interior objects at a
near distance (Hightower, & Borriello, 2001, Mauve,
Widmer, & Hrtenstein, 2001). The purpose of this
research is to apply RFID in tracing the travel routes of
demented elderly individuals with Alzheimer’s disease,
in order to understand their social activities in the day
room of a special care unit. It is necessary to provide
more meaningful activities and more available spaces
to benefit demented individuals who reside in a special
care unit. It is also expected to look for new technologies to overcome the methodological difficulties in the
study of the relationships between the environment and
demented individuals.
Method
The Application of Radio Frequency Identification
(RFID)
RFID (Radio Frequency Identification) is an automatic identification technology, which is composed of
a tag, readers, and a terminal computer. The tag is able
to sense the radio frequency from the reader at a near
distance, and the transceiver of the reader will send the
position message of the tag to the terminal computer for
recording. For this study, RFID readers were installed
on the ceiling of the day room and inspected to find the
effective scope of the radio frequency. The space of
the day room was divided into ten zones, with each dot
representing the position of one reader. Engineers tested
the effective scope of each reader and revised its zoning
and position (see Figure 1). The terminal computer was
located at the nursing station, which was on the same
floor as the day room, to receive the message transmissions from the readers. The database of the travel routes
included the time, position, and floor area, with x and y
represented as a horizontal and a vertical sign to locate
the position of movement (Hightower et al., 2001;
see Figure 2) and connect each position as a sequence
of movement (Mauve et al., 2001; see Figure 3). The
researchers then arranged the database to show the continuous movement of the travel route. During the RFID
survey process, two researchers who had the pre-training of survey and record followed seven subjects in the
day room to record the occurrences of social behavior,
133
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 1: RFID reader distribution in the day room on the second floor
Figure 2: Tracing of travel route in terms of floor area,
position and time of occurrence
134 Figure 3: Tracing of travel route as a sequence
movement
Refereed Full-Papers
identifying with whom the subjects socialized, as well
as the contents of that socialization.
Participants
A special care unit accommodating 64 demented
residents was represented as a typical local care unit
offering caregiving for the demented elderly and providing multiple spaces for occupational therapy and
activities. It had four floors, with the day room on the
second floor. Seven subjects out of 17 demented elders
who were recognized as having daily social activity after a one-month pilot study, were selected to participate
in this survey with their families’ consent. The content
of the survey included any social activity happening in
the day room, such as interactions with other residents,
caregivers, volunteers, and other residents’ families
in the leisure area, dining area, and playing area, as
well as other public areas such as the corridor, elevator
waiting area, and entrance area of the toilet. During the
RFID survey process, the subjects wore a tag card from
morning (8 a.m.) to night (8 p.m.), and the caregivers
checked them regularly during morning, noon, afternoon, and night to ensure each tag card was kept with
the subject. The RFID survey was operated four times
weekly for three months.
Record and Analysis of RFID information
The recorded RFID information included characteristics of the subject, such as gender, age, education, residence length, marriage, ADL, symptoms and
severity of dementia, as well as time of activity, type
of socialization, people interacted with, the location of
interaction, and a photo of the activity. The contents of
activities observed and recorded by researchers were
compared with the travel route, position, and time of
occurrence recorded by RFID to ensure the occurrence
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
of social activity. The most frequent social activity was
selected for analysis during morning, noon, afternoon,
and night. During the three-month survey, the analysis
of social activity focused on verbal social activity and
non-verbal social activity to understand how the subject
had interactions with others, including when, where,
with whom, and what kind of activity. Furthermore, a
comparison of these variables was processed to find the
factors that might have an influence on social activity.
Results
The RFID (Radio Frequency Identification) survey
on the social activities of demented elders included the
characteristics of the subjects, the timing and movement
of their travel routes, and photos of their social activities. Although it was not permitted to take photos of the
subjects, researchers overcame this limitation by taking
photos of social activities at a distance and screening
each subject’s face, thus preserving their privacy. A
tracing of each subject’s travel route was performed
four times between morning and night. Characteristics
of the subject, such as gender, age, education, residence
length, and marriage, were supplied by the special care
unit. Nursing staffs assessed the ADL of demented
individuals by the Barthel index, and the symptoms and
severity of dementia were diagnosed by neuropsychiatric doctors according to the MMSE (Mini Mental Status
Exam) and CDR (Clinical Dementia Rating). In general,
the activity of socialization includes verbal social activity and non-verbal social activity (Chiou, 2006). People
engage in language communication during verbal social
activity and may have non-verbal social activities, such
as body contact, smiling to each other, and listening to
others’ talking (Han, 2008). With reference to the results
of the survey, the record of the seven subjects were
described in the following sections.
135
Refereed Full-Papers
Subject A sat at the left area of day room; she said hello to the front resident and chatted with the next
resident when she watched TV during 08:14-10:23 in the morning. Subject A said hello to the caregiver
and volunteer when she went to the dining table. She said hello to the other residents before sitting down
and told other residents that her nephew would come to bring her outside during lunch time from 11:3012:30.
Subject A embraced the caregiver and smiled to the other residents as they waited for the elevator
to go to the ground floor for occupational therapy at 15:00. After dinner, Subject A shook hands with
volunteers who accompanied residents before they left. Subject A went to the right area of the day room
during the activity of group games at 18:30. She smiled to the residents who were playing the game,
touched the shoulder of the next resident, and spoke to the resident’s family: “Wonderful! Very good!”
136 Refereed Full-Papers
Subject B chatted with the next resident when she watched TV at the left area of the day room. She
smiled to the residents who walked past her and to the residents who were talking with the caregiver while
she waited for the elevator in the morning. Subject B always said hello to people no matter if they were
residents or caregivers, including at the entrance of the toilet, before dining or in the corridor. She said,
“You are welcome” to the resident who happily accepted the chopsticks she had passed, and she chatted
with the next resident during lunch, saying things such as, “You ate faster than me!”
Before dinner, Subject B went to the right area of the day room to get chopsticks and bowls for dinner and smiled to the residents and caregiver who stood in front of the elevator. After dinner, Subject B
watched TV at the left area of the day room and said hello to the caregivers, volunteers, and residents,
and at times she chatted with the next resident, saying things such as, “You made a beautiful hat!” During
the group games held at the right area of the day room, she smiled to the residents who were playing the
game.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
137
Refereed Full-Papers
Subject C said hello to the residents who walked toward her when she sat at the left area of the day
room in the morning, and she fed the resident next to her during lunch. She smiled to the caregiver and the
volunteer when they assisted her in walking.
Subject C always said hello to residents and caregivers as she moved. She smiled to the caregiver and
the volunteer when they waited for the elevator to go to the ground floor for occupational therapy at 15:00.
After dinner, Subject C happily held other residents’ hands to exercise under the instruction of the caregiver. Subject C went to the right area of the day room during the group games at 18:30, and she smiled to
the residents who were playing the game.
138 Refereed Full-Papers
Subject D sat at the left area of the day room. He said hello to the next resident when he watched TV
during 08:00-9:41 in the morning. Before lunch, Subject D had active social activity with the other residents. He walked to the dining table to listen to residents’ conversations and walked to the nursing station
to get chopsticks for himself and other residents. When he returned to the sofa area, he said hello to the
caregiver in the corridor.
After lunch, Subject D sang songs and smiled to the other residents as they watched TV at the right area
of the day room. At 15:00, Subject D said goodbye to the volunteer who was going to leave, as he sat on
the sofa in the left area. Subject D went to the right area of the day room and participated in group games
at 18:30. He said hello to the caregiver and smiled to the other residents during the game.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
139
Refereed Full-Papers
Subject E sat at the right area of the day room. He read the newspaper and talked about the articles with
nearby residents and caregivers in the dining area at 10:30. After lunch, Subject E stayed in the dining area
continuously and chatted with the caregiver, saying,“Your stature looks like a man!”
Before Subject E sat at his chair for dinner, he said hello to the residents who were in the same dining
group. After dinner, he walked to the nursing station and said hello to the caregivers, then returned to the
dining area to read the newspaper, where he said hello to the next resudent. Subject E liked to read the
newspaper, even though most of the residents were participating in group games. Nevertheless, Subject E
watched the process and smiled to the caregivers.
140 Refereed Full-Papers
Subject F smiled to residents as they waited for the elevator to go up to the fourth floor and participate
in group activities at 10:00. She said hello to the next resident and chatted with the caregiver before lunch,
and she fed the next resident during lunch.
During her movement through the corridor, Subject F smiled and said hello to the caregivers and residents. Subject F smiled to the residents before dinner, as well as to the caregiver during dinner. Subject F
did not participate in the group games, even though most of the residents were playing. She sat in the dining area and happily watched the residents and caregivers as they played group games.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
141
Refereed Full-Papers
Subject G talked with the volunteers—“Who are you? This is my place! Are you painting? Where are
your children?”—when she wandered around sofa area in the morning. Before lunch, Subject G did a lot of
walking; she said hello to residents and residents’ families and talked with them: “Where are my children?
I am going with you to look for my children”.
Subject G said hello to the caregiver who was waiting for the elevator, as well as to residents when she
went to the dining area in the afternoon. She also happily played games and held the hand of next resident
as well as said hello to the caregiver during the activity of group games.
142 Refereed Full-Papers
Discussion
The subjects had verbal social activities, including
saying hello, chatting, talking, saying goodbye, saying
thanks, and singing. Most of the subjects were capable
of saying hello, chatting, and talking, but Subject C
only said hello. Nevertheless, Subject G was talkative.
Although the content of the conversations was short,
the subjects could express a variety of language, such
as “wonderul! very good!”, “You ate faster than me!”,
“You made a beautiful hat!”,“Your stature looked like a
man! ”, “Who are you?”, “This is my place.”, “Are you
painting?”, “Where are your children?”, etc. In addition, some subjects could articulate a complete sentence
when they talked with residents or caregivers, saying things such as, “Nephew would come to bring me
outside.” and “Where are my children? I am going with
you to look for my children.” Subjects could have verbal interactions with residents, caregivers, the residents’
families, and volunteers.
The subjects had non-verbal social activities, including smiling, embracing or touching the shoulder of
others, listening to others’ conversations, feeding other
residents, and holding other residents’ hands. However,
most of the subjects were only capable of smiling. Subjects might have non-verbal interactions with residents,
caregivers, the residents’ families, and volunteers;
however, most of the subjects only had interactions with
residents and caregivers.
Most of the subjects had verbal interactions in
the leisure area of the day room. They said hello and
chatted with each other while watching TV or sitting
on the sofa. Basiclly, the subjects chatted with nearby
residents, and the furniture arrangement (such as sofas)
assisted in talking with each other. In the dining area
of the day room, most of the subjects had verbal and
non-verbal interactions. They said hello to the caregiver and volunteer before and after dining when the
caregiver and volunteer assisted in the arrangement of
dining. Some subjects talked with the other residents
and listened to residents’ conversations during dining,
as each dining table had six residents sitting face to
face. Furniture arrangement benefitted the interactions
of residents not only by allowing them to chat with
nearby residents on the sofa, but also to talk with other
residents while dining.
Most of the subjects had non-verbal interactions in
the playing area of the day room. They smiled to residents and held the shoulder or hands of residents while
playing group games. Obviously, group games involved
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
body contact, which increased the interactions of residents. However, the subjects didn’t have active verbal
interactions when they concentrated on playing. As for
the nursing station area, most of the subjects had no
social activity there. Even if the subjects went to nursing station to take chopsticks and bowls for dining, they
didn’t interact with the caregivers at the nursing station.
In the public area of the day room, most of the subjects
had active social activities in front of the elevator,
including verbal and non-verbal activities. They said
hello and smiled to residents, caregivers, and volunteers
while waiting for the elevator. Improving the elevator
waiting area to make it become a better potential space
for social interaction would be a very meaningful issue.
In summary, subjects engaged in verbal social activities in the day room, the dining area, leisure area, public
areas (in front of the elevator and in the corridor), and
the playing area. Subjects engaged in non-verbal social
activities in any place in the day room. Dining and
leisure areas played the major roles in the social activity
of the demented elderly.
With reference to the characteristics of subjects affecting social activity, some subjects were talkative and
moved frequently at any time, which showed they had
active verbal and non-verbal social activities. The subjects who did not participate in group games or spent
a lot of time reading newspapers would have a lower
frequency of social activities and be limited to interactions with residents and caregivers. The differences of
social activity between the residents were caused by the
characteristics of the residents without the influence of
variables such as gender, age, education, and residence
length, according to the results of this survey. However,
the analysis was based on the activities of only seven
subjects, so further research will be necessary to discuss
the influence of these variables. Some subjects had
less verbal social activity (only saying hello), but had
a variety of non-verbal social activities. However, the
severity of the dementia affected the social activity of
the subject. The results showed that severely demented
subjects were incapable of a variety of non-verbal social activities except for holding others’ hands, but were
capable of having active verbal social activities, such as
being talkative. Given the limitation of a single subject,
more severely demented individuals participating in
surveys of social activities will be needed to confirm
the above findings.
Many demented individuals with severe behavioral
problems need to record their behaviors by tracing the
143
Refereed Full-Papers
travel route of a whole day in order to analyze the occurrences of behaviors in terms of the time, position,
and floor area. Especially with reference to demented
individuals who tend to wander or suffer from disorientation, it is important to prevent them from getting lost,
falling, or becoming harmed. In fact, RFID will have
the potential to assist special care units in such areas as
exit control and behavior management.
References
1. Achterberg, W., Pot, A. M., Kerkstra, A., Muller, M., and
Ribbe, M., 2003, “The Effect of Depression on Social
Engagement in Newly Admitted Dutch Nursing Home
Residents”, The Gerontologist, pp.213 -218.
2. Chiou, J. P. (2006). The development of contemporary
socialization for the elderly, Journal of the American
Geriatrics Society, 72, 15-25.
3. Cohen, U., & Weisman, G.D., 1991, “Holding On to Home-Designing Environments for People with Dementia”, The
Johns Hopkins University Press.
4. Day, K., Carreon, D., & Stump, C., 2000, “The Therapeutic
Design of Environments for People with Dementia: A
Review of the Empirical Research”, The Gerontologist, 40,
pp.397-416.
5. Dobbs, D., Munn, J., Zimmerman, S., Boustani, M., Williams,
C.S., Sloane, P. D., and Reed, P. S., 2005, “Characteristics
Associated With Lower Activity Involvement in Long-Term
Care Residents with Dementia”, The Gerontologist, pp.81-86.
6. Frijters, D., Achterberg, W., Hirdes, J. P., Fries, B. E., Morris,
J. N. & Steel, K., 2001, “Integrated Health Information
System Based on Resident Assessment Instruments [in
Dutch] ”, Tijdschrift voor Gerontologie en Geriatrie, 32,
pp.8-16.
144 7. Gonzalez-Salvador, T., Lysetkos, C. G., Baker, A., Hovanec,
L., Roques, C., Brandt, J., & Steele, C., 2000, “Quality of
Life in Dementia Patients in Long Term Care”, International
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, pp.181-189.
8. Han, F. R. (2008). The technique of socialization for people
with mental retardation, Journal of Special Education, 20,
291-313.
9. Herdman, R. C., & Behney, C. J., 1992, “Special Care Units
for People with Alzheimer’s and Other Dementias: Consumer
Education, Research, Regulatory, and Reimbursement
Issues”, University of Massachusetts at Boston Press.
10. Hightower, J., Borriello, G., 2001, “Location systems for
ubiquitous computing”, IEEE Computer, 32(8), pp. 57-66.
11. Kiely, D. K., Simon, M. A., Jones, R. N. & Morris, J.N.,
2000, “The Protective Effect of Social Engagement on
Mortality in Long-Term Care”, Journal of the American
Geriatrics Society, 48, pp.1367-1372.
12. Lawton, M. P. (1995). Environmental approaches to research
and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. USA: National
Institute of Mental Health, Department of Health and Human
Services.
13. Logsdon, R. G., Gibbons, L. E., McCurry, S. M. & Teri,
L., 2000, “Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease: Patient
and caregiver reports.” In Albert, S. M. & Logsdon, R. G.,
Assessing quality of life in Alzheimer’s disease (pp. 17-30).
New York: Springer Publishing Company.
14. Mauve, M., Widmer, A., and Hartenstein, H., 2001,
“A survey on position-based routing in mobile ad hoc
networks”, IEEE Network, 15(6), pp. 30-39.
15. Mor, V., Branco, K., Fleishman, J., Hawes, C., Phillips,
C., Morris, J. & Fries, B., 1995, “The Structure of Social
Engagement among Nursing Home Residents”, Journal of
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, pp.1-8.
16. Zeisel, J., Silverstein, N.M., Hyde, J., Levkoff, S., Lawton,
M.P., and Holmes, W., 2003, “Environmental Correlates to
Behavioral Health Outcomes in Alzheimers Special Care
Units”, The Gerontologist, pp.697-711.
Refereed Full-Papers
Emerging Learning Commons
in Japanese Academic
Libraries: Behavioral Aspects
of Actual Usage and
Communication Patterns
Fahed A. Khasawneh (Mie University, Japan),
Yoriko Shibayama (Mie University, Japan),
Akikazu Kato (Mie University, Japan), Shiho
Mori (Mie University, Japan), Gen Taniguchi
(Nagoya University, Japan)
ABSTRACT
The learning commons in the central library of Nagoya University is a new innovation. It was introduced
as a response to recent information technology developments, emerging learning pedagogy, and the diversification of a new generation of users. This study considers
these circumstances, with focus on understanding the
actual usage and communication patterns of the spaces
in the learning commons. Behavioral mapping was
the primary data collection method, using a specially
developed legend and an observation sheet to gather
information regarding the space usage in terms of route
choice and movement spines, activities of users, tools
used, as well as seat choice modes of users and their
communication patterns, with emphasis on understanding learners’ collaborations. The results showed that
the group study area and writing support area had more
collaboration between users who were engaged in
learning activities. The users showed multitasking in
their activity profiles, even single users in the individual
study area simultaneously used PCs, books, and papers.
Leaving a table for short breaks was a common practice. Collaboration between users was influenced by
many of the space design aspects. For example, table
layouts that provided appropriate levels of proximity, continuous eye lines, and dynamic configurations
were successful. A learning commons should provide
a relaxed environment that combines openness, flexibility, and IT resources with learning initiatives. The
users should feel that they are welcomed to study, use
PCs, read and/or even hang out. Such freedom leads to
feelings of comfort and promote innovation.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Introduction
Learning commons are being introduced steadily
into many Japanese academic libraries. This emerging trend is gaining popularity among the diversity of
campus users and is being promoted by facility managers and campus planners as the long awaited innovation
to revive the campus library, inspire the community of
learners, and lead the way for further changes in the
design of learning space elsewhere in campus. Beagle
(2008) defines the learning commons as a new model
of service delivery in academic libraries that builds on
the traditional library service models and integrates
information technology with learning initiatives in cooperation with other campus units to create a one-stopshopping concept. Bailey (2005) promotes the learning
commons as an effective model of integrated library
services. Catering for high-level research needs and
knowledge creation is the driving force to create such
innovative facilities.
Lippincott (2010) points out that the learning commons seem to be popular among the new generation of
users, generally referred to as the Net Generation; such
facilities cater to the new users’ learning preferences:
to work in groups, use technology extensively, and tendency to mix academic and social lives. Understanding
the users’ needs helps to create better learning environments that engage students and lead to innovation and
creativity. However, studies on such facilities in Japan
are still limited. The aspects of actual space use are emphasized in this study. Tools used in environmental behavior studies were used to grasp a better understanding
of the learning commons, focusing on the user activity
patterns and the prominent features of the new facilities.
Such an understanding would provide feedback on the
users’ needs, the effects of table configuration and space
planning decisions on users’ learning behaviors, and
successful elements to create an effective learning commons in campus libraries. Brown (2005) stresses that a
learning commons is the product of integrating learning
theory principles, information technology innovation,
and users’ needs to create a flexible conversational
space. Such a process should guarantee that such a
facility would correspond to the needs, preferences, and
aspirations of the Net Generation, which requires special skills to be successful in their practical lives. The
Association of College and Research Libraries (ACRL)
(2000), within its information literacy standards, emphasizes that information literacy requires an individual
to have special abilities to recognize when information
145
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 1. Facts About Nagoya University Central
Library
is required, and be proficient in locating, evaluating,
and manipulating the data effectively to solve problems
and create knowledge. Stuart (2008) urges designers of
learning facilities, and particularly those concerned with
learning commons development, to collect information
on the actual needs of students and other facility users
and not to depend solely on needs perceived by the
librarians.
Research Objectives and Methodology
The main objectives of this study are two-fold. First,
it tries to shed light on the application of the learning
commons concept in the context of Japanese academic
libraries, where the prominent physical features are
highlighted with reference to the service philosophy
adopted and major characteristics of space. The second
goal is to tackle the actual patterns of learning commons
use with particular focus on users learning behaviors,
space occupation, and interactions. As a methodology,
behavioral mapping was used to collect data.
Nagoya University Learning Commons Case Study
About The Commons:
The central library learning commons in Nagoya
University Higashiyama Campus was selected as a case
study, because it was the first learning commons to be
introduced in a national university in Japan (Table 1).
The main floor was renovated into a learning commons during a two-year plan that focused on creating
two main areas: a group study area and a learning area.
The group study area, which is located mainly in the
southern part of library, was installed in 2008, while the
learning area and related support services were opened
on December 31, 2009 (Figures 1&2).
146 Figure 1. Main Areas in the Learning Commons
Philosophy and Background:
The philosophy behind the development of the commons was that the university wanted to implement an
integrated environment that merges human resources,
information technology, and a variety of printed and
digital resources. Such a vision was adopted in 2008
with an aim to meet the diverse student learning objectives and learning styles. The focus was to stimulate
the creation of qualified students that are IT proficient,
talented, and innovative.
The learning commons would provide the following
support services through qualified staff and in cooperation with other departments on campus:
1. IT support: troubleshooting problems in relation to
PC usage, networking, and printing.
2. Learning support: research tips, clues, and help for
students and faculty support services.
3. Writing support: technical tips on academic writing
skills.
4. Peer (tutor) support: covering a wide range of
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 2. The Central Library Learning Commons, Nagoya University Higashiyama Campus
topics, including student academic life or selected
university courses.
Survey Procedures:
A survey was conducted over two consecutive days
in January 2009, covering an interval of seven hours.
Data analysis focused on the intervals between 15:0018:00, which witnessed the densest use of the learning
commons. Behavioral mapping was used in 15-minute
sessions with five-minute breaks to collect data regarding where (location of activity), who (male–female),
and what (activity and tools used). Each table area was
later given a code to ease analysis of data. Four mapping techniques were used to collect data covering the
following aspects:
1. Activity type: location, type of activity, and tools
used.
2. Duration of action: time spent on each task and seat
occupation rate.
3. Movement spines: user route selection and
frequency.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
4. Communication: parties involved, frequency, and
prominent locations of interaction points.
Learning Commons Patterns Of Use:
Movement Spines and Seat Occupation
Having two zones separated by a service core influenced the choice of routes; the multipurpose learning
area and the newspaper corner had more dense movement as more visitors went toward the northern part of
the learning commons. Two major longitudinal movement spines were observed; the most congested was in
the learning area, and another vertical spine led from
the main entrance to the northern part of the learning
commons (Figure 3). The users tended to use spaces
closer to these spines if available. The line of sight is
obscured by the service core, which leads to visual
separation between the two main parts of the learning
commons, yet within each area different components
of space are in view, which easily provides chances for
spontaneous meetings of users.
The overall seat occupancy ratio within time was 66
147
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 3. Cumulative Users Routes Choice and Movement Spines
percent, which indicates consistent use of the learning
commons facilities. Many tables in the writing support
area, such as Wa, were continuously occupied by users
to yield an occupation ratio close to 100 percent; this
may be attributed to the proximity of this table to the
movement spine and support counter besides the nature
of tasks performed here. While other tables, especially
in the newspaper corner, such as Nc, Nh, and Ni had a
seat occupancy ratio of less than 50 percent; this corner
had many visitors that mostly stayed for short periods.
User Activities
A wide range of activities were noted (Figure 4).
The group study areas had a high percentage of communication and learning activities, especially table Gd,
which had around 50 percent communication compared
to other activities. Table Ph, which provides a seminarlike configuration, and tables Pc and Pd, which provide
whiteboards, witnessed presentation rehearsals. Tables
found in the multipurpose learning area, such as Mc
and Mh, and tables including Wa and Wc in the writ-
148 ing support area were used in collaborative computer
work. Two users were frequently noticed sharing the
same section of the table, using the PC and debating in
a productive manner. It is noteworthy that tables supplied with desktop PCs, such as Pa, Wa, Wb, Mc, Me,
and Mh, witnessed excessive computer-assisted work,
although many users also used books and laptop PCs.
The writing support area tables had generally more PC
work activities with lower rates of desk work, while the
multipurpose learning area witnessed a combination of
computer work, reading, and desk work more frequently; here computer work was more prevalent on tables
provided with desktop PCs.
Communication and talking were more frequent in
the group study areas, due to the table layouts, proximity of users, and continuous uninterrupted eye lines.
Studying is a series of activities and breaks, where a
few moments are taken to refresh; therefore, provision
of snacks and drinks in a café with nearby lounges are
recommended to create a more relaxed atmosphere.
The use of laptop PCs was highest in individual
Refereed Full-Papers
study areas, where users would work for long hours on
activities including the use of books, papers, and laptop
PCs in a concentrated manner to produce reports or
similar tasks. The multipurpose learning area (Me-Mj)
had less conversation and a higher percentage of learning activities assisted by reading, computer work, and
desk work. Users made use of the available work space
by spreading their belongings and books all over the
desks (Figure 5).
In the newspaper corner (Na-Nj), the dominant activity was reading and desk work, with the latter slightly
prevalent. Talking was noticed, although it was a less
dominant activity compared with the group study area,
which witnessed lengthy conversations more frequently.
Book usage was noted in all areas with similar percentage, except for the tutor-dependent writing support
area, which stresses the success of the facility to create
a balance of PC and printed material use. Many visi-
Figure 4. Percentage of Activity by Table in the Learning Commons
Figure 5. Percentage of Tool Usage According to Area
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
149
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 2. Average Communication and Learning Activity Time by Zone
tors to the north area went to the newspaper corner to
read magazines and newspapers for a while, after which
some would leave to go to other learning commons
sections, while many would leave the library, which
suggests the need for a refresh space.
Communication Patterns
The library was known to be a quiet place, yet as the
learning commons emerged, such spaces became lively
places customized for socializing and interaction to
facilitate learning. This facility attempted to provide a
balance between quiet study areas and other spaces with
accepted levels of noise.
The highest rate of communication between users
was generally observed in the group study area (aisle)
including tables Ga, Gb, Gc, Gd, Ge, Gf and Gg, followed by the group study area (window) including
tables Pc, Pd, and Ph, the writing support area, including table Wa, and the newspaper corner including tables
Nb, Nd, Ne, Nf, Nh, and Nj. This shows that the group
study area was successful in attracting groups of users who made use of this environment that facilitates
collaboration by providing appropriate table sizes and
configurations with continuous lines of sight. In particular, tables Gd and Gf provided a 1:1 ratio of work to
communication, which indicates the constructive nature
of communication that provides for collaboration.
Conversely, the multipurpose learning area, including
tables Ma, Mb, Mc, Md, Mf, Mh, Mi, and Mj, was less
successful in terms of facilitating collaboration with
reasonable intervals of communication, which indicates
that this configuration hinders collaboration and attracts
mostly users that complete their tasks individually.
Relating Learning Activities and Communication
Durations
Less time was spent talking compared to being engaged in other activities (Table 2). For the group study
area (aisle) including tables Gel-Gh, Pa, and Pb, the us-
150 ers of this area had the highest average of communication (17.5 min) and the lowest average learning activity
time. Users tend to work and have longer conversations
frequently as required during a collaboration process
that requires participation of all parties, which results
in shorter continuous learning activity times and more
time dedicated to communication to ensure smooth
performance. This may also be attributed to the nature
of the table layouts in this zone, which is sociopetal
to enable more eye contact to enhance and encourage
communication.
The group study area (window), including tables
Pc-Ph, had an average communication time of 12.7
min and an average learning activity time of 61.2 min.
Compared to the group study area (aisle), this area
had less communication and longer learning activity
times, which may be attributed to the staging of several
presentation rehearsals where users would listen to a
presentation and then provide comments, an activity
that was considered a learning activity. Therefore, many
users had long working times and less communication
times, while others had long communication times and
less learning activity time (Figure 6).
The writing support area including tables (Wa-Wc)
had moderate communication times of 11.9 min and
the longest average learning activity time, which corresponds to the nature of the area task; users would
obtain brief advice and spend more time applying the
advice and preparing papers or presentations. This area
was preferred by those who wanted to use a PC to finish their concentrated work, which lead to lower seat
turnover.
The multipurpose learning area, including tables
Ma-Mj, had the shortest average communication time
of 3.5 min, although the average learning activity time
was relatively long at 96.1 min. It seems that individual
learning activities contributed to these results; users
conducted concentrated individual work that made use
of available PCs. Some tables with PCs witnessed high
Refereed Full-Papers
table turnover, which indicates that such PCs were used
for short-term functions such as checking e-mail, rather
than being used for group collaborations supported by
PCs.
The newspaper corner, including tables Na-Nj, had
an average communication time of 12.9 min and an
average 61.7 min of learning activity time. It seems
to resemble the group study area (window), although
this area was more static in terms of layout and configuration, and attracted more single users that arrived
particularly to read newspapers, providing for somewhat long stay time and few communication incidents.
However, some users arrived at this area in groups and
were engaged in interactions more frequently.
The individual study area, including tables Ia and
Ib, had a low average communication time of 4.1 min
and a relatively moderate average learning activity time
Figure 6. Relationship Between Learning Activities
and Communication Duration
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
of 59.6 min, which corresponds to the nature of the
activity. Many single users stayed for a long time, while
others occupied seats for a few minutes, although the
seat turnover was generally low.
Work Flow Profile Of Selected Users
Three examples are discussed with respect to understanding and interpreting the work flow profile of
selected users (Figure 7). The first case is for a user in
the group study area (window) table (Pe); this table was
occupied by two users who sat beside each other. Desk
work was predominant with fruitful communication,
where collaboration was observed in the form of short
intervals of communication followed by desk work
periods. Such a configuration is ideal for collaboration
by groups of two users; sitting beside each other and in
close proximity enables viewing of each other’s work,
Figure 7. Activity Profiles of Selected Users
151
Refereed Full-Papers
sharing materials and facilitates communication. The
user leaves the table once, performs other activities
briefly, then engages in concentrated individual desk
work again, taking short breaks every once in a while,
which confirms the need for a refresh space in the commons.
The second case is for a user in the group study
area (window) table (Pd), where the user made use
of the available whiteboard. The work flow profile is
separated into two parts; the first includes preparing
slides using books and conducting desk work interrupted by communication, and the second begins with
a presentation rehearsal followed by more communication and desk work. Communication was important in
this profile, because the users placed themselves closer
to the whiteboard selecting to face each other to ease
conversation and use the whiteboard. This demonstrates
the importance of table layout to provide continuous
eye contact and a clear view of the whiteboard to afford
for collaboration.
The third case is for a user in the writing support
area table (Wa), where two students used the table in
close proximity. Such close proximity would indicate
a tendency for more frequent communication and collaboration. The work flow profile for one of the users
can be divided into two parts; the first part was mostly
individual work that included longer periods of computer work, desk work, and audiovisual usage while
preparing a presentation or a report. The second part
was mostly composed of long periods of communication in a repetitive manner with desk work and much
computer work, which represents a collaborative type
of work related to tutoring and accepting advice to
acquire better skills. The layout dedicated for tutoring
in the writing support area requires considerably more
privacy compared to areas with other functions; this
was provided for here by using table partitions that
provided each section with more privacy and screened
unwanted noise or even eye contact between users who
shared the same table.
Conclusion
The patterns of communication and activities for
users varied significantly according to the tasks performed, tools used, and configuration of space layout.
The group study area had more communication incidents, and users here showed more freedom in actions,
including talking, using phones and hanging out. These
findings demonstrate that the new type of library users
152 multitask and have more diversified needs. Areas that
were more refreshing and flexible encouraged collaboration, and with the exception of layouts that provide
proximity, a reasonable degree of privacy and sociopetal
organization.
Collaboration consisted mostly of desk and computer
work interrupted by fruitful interaction among group
members. There is a need for more soft furniture such
as lounges and refresh spaces where users may have
short breaks between their learning activity sessions;
many users continuously left their tables or posed to
chat with others. The learning service hours should be
extended to cope with the high seat occupancy rate,
which is expected to increase in the future.
Overcoming the visual separation between the two
main zones of the learning commons needs to be addressed; wide screen panels could be fixed near the
main entrance of space to present different views of
available space, especially from the northern part, and
PCs available for use within the facility. Providing a
combination of table layouts is necessary to provide for
the potential users work flow profiles; the group study
area provides a mixture of table layouts that attracts
users who seek collaboration. Such users included
those who used the space briefly or those who stayed
for many hours; the space planners should cater for
the needs of both users in a balanced way. Finally, the
learning commons has definitely changed the image of
the library at Nagoya University from a quiet space into
a lively learning space where students feel welcomed,
although further development is required to emphasize
the informal nature of the space.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Bailey, D. (2005). Information Commons Services for learners
and researchers: Evolution in Patron Needs, Digital
Resources and scholarly Publishing, presented in INFORUM
2005: 11th Conference on Professional Information
Resources, Prague, 24th May 2005, http://www.inforum.cz/
pdf/2005/Bailey_Russell.pdf (August 7, 2010)
Beagle, D. (2006). The Information Commons Handbook. New
York: Neal-Schuman Publishers Inc.
Beagle, D. (2008). The Learning commons in Historical Context.
Annals of Nagoya University Library Studies (7), 25-34,
2008, http://libst.nul.nagoya-u.ac.jp/pdf/annals_07_03.pdf
(August 7, 2010)
Bennett, S. (2003). Libraries Designed for Learning.
Washington, D.C: Council on Library and Information
Resources, http://www.clir.org/pubs/reports/pub122/
pub122web.pdf/ (August 7, 2010)
Refereed Full-Papers
Brown, M. (2005). Learning Spaces. In Oblinger D. & Oblinger
J. (Eds.). Educating the Net Generation, Boulder, CO:
EDUCAUSE, http://net.educause.edu/ir/library/pdf/pub7101.
pdf (August 7, 2010)
Lippincott, J. (2010). Information Commons: Meeting
Millennials’ Needs. Journal of Library Administration,
50(1),27-37, January 2010. http://www.cni.org/staff/
joanpubs/IC.jlibadmin.lippincott.preprint.pdf (August 7,
2010)
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Somerville, M. & Harlan, S. (2008). From Information Commons
to Learning Commons and Learning Spaces: An Evolution
Context, in Schader B. (Ed.), Learning Commons Evolution
and Collaborative Essentials, Oxford, U.K: Chandos
Publishing Ltd.
Stuart, C. (2008). ARL Learning Space Pre-Planning Tool Kit.
Washington, D.C.: Association of Research Libraries, http://
www.arl.org/bm~doc/planning-a-learning-space-tool-kit.pdf,
(August 7, 2010)
The Association of College and Research Libraries (ACRL)
(2000). Information Literacy Competency Standards for
Higher Education, Chicago, http://www.ala.org/ala/mgrps/
divs/acrl/standards/standards.pdf (August 7, 2010)
153
Refereed Full-Papers
The Role of Built Heritage on
Aesthetic Evaluation of Urban
Landscape
Maria Cristina Dias Lay (Federal University of
Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil),
Mirian Rodrigues (Federal University of Rio
Grande do Sul, Brazil)
Abstract
The article addresses the process of visual perception of the built environment, as related to cultural built
heritage and its contribution to the visual quality of
the urban landscape, in order to ascertain the level of
importance attributed to heritage buildings and identify
the physical characteristics of existing buildings on
sites of heritage value that are more and less attractive
to users, the indication of architectural, historical, and
affective values, among others, which possibly influenced the perception of its users with respect to environmental and aesthetic quality. It follows the assumption that the aesthetic quality of the urban landscape
is related to physical attributes and associations of
morphological and typological elements that make up
the urban landscape. It is considered that the evaluative
response is related to the physical-spatial environment
and previous experience of the observer, their views,
expectations, and cultural experiences involving the
processes of perception and cognition. Aspects related
to the important role urban legislation plays when
directed to preservation of urban areas and historic centers were also considered. Historic areas of two cities
were selected as case studies. Piratini represents cities
with preserved historic centers and pioneering urban
legislation, and São José do Norte represents cities
where cultural heritage was adulterated due to a lack of
legislation that guarantees the preservation of built heritages. The research was implemented through the use
of qualitative and quantitative methods in two stages of
investigation. The first aimed at gathering elements to
define the study area by applying the technique of mental maps and interviews to users of historic areas, which
allowed the identification of the strongest positive and
negative images of public buildings and urban spaces.
In the second stage, questionnaires were administered
in order to evaluate images of urban scenes with dif-
154 ferent levels of homogeneity, chosen based on criteria
established to investigate the role built cultural heritage
has on the visual quality of urban landscapes, measure
the damage to the aesthetics of the city caused by the
lack or non-inclusion of issues relating to preservation
of cultural heritage in the process of urban planning,
and subsidize the elaboration of public policies on the
preservation and planning issues. The role of the built
heritage on the visual quality of the urban landscape
was verified by evaluating the visual appearance of
the buildings according to their formal and symbolic
attributes. The relevance of bringing together experts in
the field of preservation of cultural heritage and users
of historical areas emphasized the importance of user
engagement with public policy issues of heritage preservation, urban aesthetics, and urban planning, which
allow the appropriation of cultural heritage by local
communities. Results indicate the relevance of studies
focused on the visual quality of the urban landscape as
a need to promote actions for qualification of public
spaces, and in this context include the recognition that
the facades of buildings that make up the urban landscape are collective assets that should be considered for
the establishment of guidelines for new interventions
and projects. On one hand, results confirm the positive contribution of buildings which constitute the built
cultural heritage in the visual quality of the urban landscape; on the other, the adulteration of cultural heritage
generates negative evaluations. Accordingly, the need
to curb the actions of distortion, mutilation, and even
demolition of buildings located in the ancient period of
historic centers is evident.
Introduction
Visual quality and cultural built heritage
In the process of planning and ordering of the
dynamics of urban growth, the cultural built heritage,
considered in its full scope and complexity, begins to
impose itself as a major component to be considered
in the process of evolution and transformation of cities. Despite the statement of its importance, cultural
built heritage in most Brazilian cities is absent from
public policies and land management. Regardless of
national heritage, at the regional or local level, the
Brazilian municipality has the constitutional authority
and obligation to protect it. However, in most cities,
cultural heritage issues were not accepted, understood,
or prioritized. The lack of control and concern about the
aesthetic quality and the consequences brought to the
Refereed Full-Papers
visual appearance of cities characterized the problem in
this research. Generally, the regulatory instruments of
projects are more directed to define the constructive potential than the aesthetic quality of new buildings, their
insertion into the landscape, and compatibility with preexisting buildings. Consequently, the absence of urban
legislation for preservation of cultural built heritage, as
well as the absence of regulatory mechanisms and control of urban aesthetics, leads to the destruction of local
cultural heritage and the growing disqualification of the
landscape and visual appearance of historic cities.
This study investigates the level of importance attributed to the built cultural heritage by the population
and the meanings it conveys, identifies environmental
aspects and qualities that are more and less attractive
to users when evaluating a set of buildings of a certain
area (site value sheet), which establishes the values
(architectural, historical, emotional, etc.) present in that
area and that may influence the perception of its users
with respect to urban aesthetics. Moreover, considering
that the environmental image affects the attitudes of
individuals in relation to urban space, awareness of the
visual appearance can result as an important component to be considered in the search for improvements
in the quality of landscape aesthetics. The literature
suggests that built heritage is an essential element in
the rescue of pleasant things and transmitters of a sense
of well-being, these being gradually lost in the process
of building modern cities, as well as the relationship
between man and environment. In the environmental
assessment process, the historical buildings tend to be
perceived positively and aesthetic values associated
with the formal and symbolic (Lang, 1987). Usually
located in the central areas of cities where changes
occur more quickly and frequently, the permanence of
historic buildings is considered essential to maintaining
the sense of continuity of places, while the destruction
of heritage buildings and landscape change can affect the perception of individuals. According to Lynch
(1997), rapid changes in the urban environment added
to technical and functional changes can be emotionally
upsetting for people and disrupt their perceptual image.
One of the objectives of the study of aesthetics is seeking to identify and understand factors that contribute to
the perception of an object or a process as beautiful, or
how it can provide a pleasant experience (Lang, 1987).
Stamps (1989) explains the significance of studies on
the visual quality of the perceived environment based
on the fact that the aesthetics of the urban landscape is
related to the human need to have pleasant sensations.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Thus, one can infer that pleasant surroundings would be
potential generators of pleasant sensations and discovering how to preserve or create these environments
perceived positively by the local population should be a
constant objective of the urban planning process. Still,
studies with an emphasis on cultural heritage buildings
(e.g., Azevedo Lemos, Lay, Reis, 1999) indicate that the
predominance of historic buildings as a reference in the
mental process of structuring an urban area are related
to certain attributes such as the outside appearance of
the buildings, their historical importance, and use.
Evaluation of visual quality of urban landscape
The visual quality contributes to the appearance of
cities and affects the well being of individuals, whose
senses are stimulated through continuity, variety, and
existing formal standards, as well as through images
compiled from the cognitive process of the individual
(Reis, 2002). The evaluative response is directly related
to the physical-spatial environment and previous experience of the observer, their views and expectations, and
cultural experiences involving the processes of perception and cognition. In the evaluative response, perception and cognition have probabilistic relationships
with one another and with the physical characteristics
of the built environment, resulting from the interaction between individuals and the environment. This
model suggests two broad components of the evaluative
response—perception and cognition—and two types of
environmental variables—formal and symbolic (Lang,
1987). That is, while the formal attributes consist of
physical elements of buildings that comprise the architectural form used to describe it objectively, buildings
and urban space have symbolic attributes, the result of
the experiences and values acquired in the interaction
between the individual and the urban landscape. There
are some visual qualities of landscape attributes that
transform them into objects of attention inevitably, despite the ability of selective vision. Compatibility refers
to the formal relationship of a building and the other
buildings in context, for example, in terms of volume,
coverage, front, empty and full relationship, types and
shapes of openings, color and texture of materials, and
the ratio of building with natural elements (Reis, 2002).
This is associated with visual coherence, characterized by the repetition of certain architectural elements,
attributes, or formal similarities that give identity and
strengthen the character of urban areas.
The compatibility of formal and contextual new
155
Refereed Full-Papers
insertions is also mentioned as an important element in
evaluating the urban landscape, since the composition
of the buildings suggests an idea of aesthetic order in
visual perception. On the other hand, an urban setting
where there was concern about the pre-existing buildings, with great contrast and variety of heights and high
volumes and diverse visual environment, can generate
a confusing, chaotic landscape, where individuals may
feel disoriented (Lozano, 1988). According to Nasar
(1998), cities can increase their positive image evaluation, enhancing the visual coherence or order through
a variety of features that can aid in the perception of
order, such as readability, repetition, replication features
of facades, uniform texture, little contrast between elements or between buildings and their natural context,
and identifiability—characteristic features and focal
points. Lang (1988) argues that some architectural
variables carry symbolic meanings, considering their
relationship with the dimension of affective experience, such as composition (architectural style), spatial
configuration (volume ratio), materials, lighting, and
the nature of pigmentation (color). Therefore, numbers
of buildings or buildings of a particular style show
cognitive relations associated with them as symbols of
an idea or historical time, acquiring values that affect
the aesthetic evaluation, such as historical significance,
age, urban references, and positive associations with a
period in history. Still, Coeterier (1996) highlights the
importance of historic buildings as an existential value
for people on three levels: the place identity, personal
identity, and group identity, and argues that the historical buildings amplify the sense of community and
collective identity. According to Lynch (1975), people
usually respond favorably to historic sites for a variety
of reasons, and argues that “many historic and symbolic
places convey a sense of security and continuity,” adding that “the character of the personal image of time is
crucial for individual welfare, as well as it to achieve
success in time to coordinate the environmental transformation and for the outer physical surroundings play
a role in building and maintaining this image of time”
(Lynch, 1975). This article deals with aesthetic issues
in the process of visual perception of the built environment related to the built cultural heritage and its contribution to the visual quality of the urban landscape, with
the aim of emphasizing the damage to the aesthetics of
the city by the lack or non-inclusion of preservation issues of heritage buildings in the process of urban planning, as well as gathering input for the public policies
on the preservation and planning.
156 Methodology
In this study, the aesthetic response was measured
based on the different levels of satisfaction expressed
by individuals regarding the formal and symbolic attributes of buildings. The ratings herein are based on the
premise that there is an interplay of influences between
individuals and visual aspects that make up the urban
landscape. The role of cultural heritage buildings in
the urban setting was investigated in two cities initially
settled in the eighteenth century with different degrees
of preservation: With its pioneering urban legislation,
Piratini represents cities with preserved historic centers,
and São José do Norte represents cities where cultural
heritage was adulterated due to a lack of legislation that
guarantees the preservation of built heritages.
The research was implemented through the use of
qualitative and quantitative methods in two stages of
investigation. The first aimed at gathering elements to
define the study area by applying the technique of mental maps and interviews to users of historic areas, which
allowed the identification of the strongest positive and
negative images of public buildings and urban spaces
(Figure 1). In the second stage, 113 questionnaires
were administered in order to evaluate images of urban
scenes with different levels of homogeneity, chosen
based on criteria established to meet the objectives of
the investigation. Data obtained through questionnaires
were analyzed quantitatively by means of frequencies
and nonparametric tests. Three scenes from each city
were selected in order to accommodate the necessary
prerequisites to meet the following criteria: a) being located within areas of study defined in the first stage, b)
having different levels of homogeneity considering ex-
Figure 1- Definition of study areas. a) Piratini;
b) São José do Norte
Refereed Full-Papers
ternal formal features, heights, and construction times,
resulting in a more homogeneous scene, mixed (more or
less homogeneous) scene, and an heterogeneous scene,
c) presenting in its composition representative buildings
of cultural heritage (buildings of the ancient period),
and d) presenting in its composition buildings representative of the modern period, buildings of the contemporary period, and/or those that have been adulterated.
For the purposes of research, the different styles and/or
mixtures of styles were classified according to the following periods: a) the early period (until 1930), including the buildings in this period with language influenced
by Luso-Brazilian colonial style, the eclectic buildings that anticipated modernism, called by Nauomova
(2009) as pre-modernist, basically corresponding to the
art nouveau and protomodernist; b) the modern period
(1930 to 1980), influenced by various architectural
currents responsible for the consolidation of the Modernist movement such as Art Deco, the Chicago School,
European rationalism, expressionism, and neo-classical
revival (Kiefer and Luz, 2000), c) the contemporary period (after 1980), marked by the revision of the modern
movement, and d) buildings from any period disfigured
by the loss of their original typological characteristics
due to profound changes and/or replacement of items
and construction materials. Regardless of typological
classification, this research was focused on identifying
the building in periods of time in order to verify the role
that cultural heritage buildings—represented by buildings of the ancient period—carry on the visual quality
of the urban landscape.
Results
Relationship between cultural built heritage and visual
quality of the urban landscape
In order to investigate the role that built cultural heritage has in an urban setting, especially if it contributes
positively to the visual quality of the urban landscape,
three selected scenes with different degrees of homogeneity were assessed by respondents in each city.
Assessment of urban scenes - Piratini
Scene 1
Scene 1 is the most intact in terms of preservation of
cultural heritage buildings and also the most ordered
(Figure 2). The buildings that make up the scene are
mostly from the ancient period.
Buildings 1 and 3 are the best-preserved cultural heritage buildings, protected by preservation law. Building
7 maintains most of the external formal characteristics. Building 4 has the characteristic structure of the
buildings of Luso-Brazilian architecture and introduces
certain eclectic decorative elements. Buildings 5 and 6
had their roofs and frames changed. Building 2 is the
only one from the contemporary period that incorporates old elements like arches and French-style frames.
The first scene was viewed favorably by 70 percent of
the sample of respondents. Their main reasons justifying positively and negatively the visual appearance of
the scene are shown in Table 1.
The main reason for positive evaluation of the appearance of the scene is “preservation of old buildings,”
emphasizing the importance that respondents from Pi-
Figure 2 – Scene 1 – Piratini.
Table 1 – Justifications related to visual appearance of scene 1 - Piratini
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
157
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 3 – Scene 2 – Piratini.
Table 2 - Justifications related to visual appearance of Scene 2 - Piratini
ratini give to cultural heritage buildings. A second positive justification is “historical meaning/symbolic value
of historic buildings,” indicating meanings and values
attributed to built cultural heritage. With the same
percentage is the perceived pleasant visual appearance, reflecting the pleasantness and beauty associated
directly with the homogeneity of the scene. The main
negative reason cited is “lack of harmony in colors,”
suggesting how this attribute is valued by piratinenses
(as residents of Piratini are called). The issue of visual
pollution caused by lack of regulation in the use of
advertising media on the perimeter of the historic center
is also perceived by users. The conservation status was
the third most frequently cited negative reasons and
cannot be ignored, as the state of conservation seems to
be relevant in aesthetic judgments (Nasar, 1998; Reis
and Lay, 2006).
Scene 2
The second scene is mixed (consisting of old and
new buildings) that represent different styles, blending
styles and periods of architecture as the Luso-Brazilian,
eclectic, modern, and contemporary (Figure 3).
The five buildings of the early period (1, 2, 6, 7,
and 10) are protected by municipal law. Building 1 has
constructive characteristics of the Luso-Brazilian architecture. Building 2 has the same formal characteristics
of traditional building, but its frames were replaced by
the French-style windows. Building 3 is a contemporary
building, with a retreat of gardens and side setbacks and
vegetation that differs from the others. Building 4, from
the modern period, has elements straight and trimmed.
Building 5 suffered several alterations. Buildings 6 and
158 7 form a single volume and have the structure of the
Luso-Brazilian architecture and decorative details of
standard neoclassical. Building 8 is from the contemporary period and is the tallest, with a balcony running
the front facade that spreads along the promenade. With
two floors, Building 9 belongs to the contemporary period, and building 10 has a different typology, with the
structure of the Luso-Brazilian architecture as roof tiles,
but with arched and French-style openings.
This scene was viewed favorably by more than 50
percent of respondents. The main positive and negative
reasons given by respondents to evaluate the appearance of the scene are shown in Table 2.
“Beautiful appearance” is the main positive justification, followed by “the presence of modern buildings and old,” which suggests integration between the
buildings of different periods. In other words, there was
compatibility between the new formal inserts and preexisting buildings. The integration of buildings from
different periods can be considered a major factor in the
aesthetic evaluation of the scene, which, although less
homogeneous than the first, was considered positive for
more than 50 percent of respondents and with a beautiful appearance. The “outstanding preservation of old
buildings” is the third positive justification used, which
shows the duality of views on evaluative responses to
the appearance of the scene and about what and how,
whether positively or negatively, the buildings that
compose the scene contributed. “Presence of modern
buildings and old” was considered positive by some
and negative for many others, as the main negative
justification, which is further reinforced by the second
most significant response that consider “new and ugly”
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 4 – Scene 3 Piratini.
Table 3 - Major reasons related to the visual appearance of Scene 3 - Piratini
modern buildings. The visual pollution was negatively
perceived by users and exemplifies the intensity with
which it can affect the visual quality of the urban scene.
Scene 3
Scene 3 is heterogeneous, as amended by recent
constructions and alterations, and it was considered one
of the “ugly sites” in the mental maps because it has
three contemporary insertions that altered the structure
of this ancient quarter, both with respect to external and
formal characteristics and with respect to number of
floors (Figure 4).
Of the five buildings from the ancient period in the
scene, four are protected by municipal law (1, 2, 9,
and 10). Buildings 1 and 2 have the structure of the
Luso-Brazilian architecture, but standard neoclassical
elements were added. Buildings 9 and 10 have typical
characteristics of the Luso-Brazilian architecture. Building 7 is from the ancient period. Buildings 3, 5, and 8
belong to the contemporary period. Building 4 is from
the modern period and despite having been included in
the Inventory of Property, it was uncharacteristic. The
same happened with building 6, which had the spans
and frames changed/replaced.
This scene was viewed favorably by only 23 percent
of respondents. It is the less orderly scene and the only
scene among the three where “ugly” is indicated as an
evaluative response. Justifications focused on the negative (Table 3). The “presence of modern and ancient
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
buildings” was the negative justification with the highest frequency, suggesting that in this scene there was
no integration between the buildings of the early period
(pre-existing) and new inserts. The diversity of styles,
different forms of buildings, and modern buildings/new
profile contributed to the chaotic scene. When evaluating the contribution of each building to the visual
quality of the scene, similar situations occurred where a
preference for ancient buildings is favored and buildings of the modern and contemporary periods were
negatively evaluated.
Analysis of the aesthetic preference of scenes in Piratini
The order of preference of scenes 1, 2, and 3 was
confirmed by 66.7 percent of respondents, while 8.3
percent preferred the order 1, 3, 2. The more homogeneous scene (Scene 1) was evaluated positively by
approximately 70 percent of respondents. The second
scene was rated positively by over 50 percent of the
sample. The more heterogeneous scene (Scene 3) was
evaluated positively by only 23 percent of respondents.
Analyzing the results on the visual appearance of the
scenes, it can be inferred that the greater the degree of
homogeneity, the more visual quality the scene has, and
vice versa. The comparison between the frequencies obtained on the aesthetic assessment of each scene shows
the trend of positive assessments on Scenes 1 and 2
and the most negative evaluations in the third scene
(Figure 5).
159
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 5 – Comparison of evaluative responses about visual appearance– Piratini.
Figure 6 – Scene 1 – São José do Norte.
Table 4 - Justifications related to the visual appearance of scene 1- São José do Norte
Scene 1
Despite its peculiar appearance, Scene 1 in São José
do Norte is the one presenting the original structures to
the greatest extent (Figure 6).
With the exception of building 11, from the contemporary period, the other buildings were all listed by
the Institute of Historical and Artistic Patrimony of the
State (IPHAE). Building 2 has recently been recycled.
Buildings 1, 3, and 8 are old, but were adulterated in
greater or lesser degrees. Buildings 5, 6, 7, and 9 had
facades upgraded into the “deco” style. Building 10 is
one of the few terrace houses from the Luso-Brazilian
style that have been preserved, both externally and
internally. Building 12 is a corner house of the modern
period. Scene 1 was evaluated positively by 70 percent
160 of respondents. The main positive and negative reasons
given by respondents to evaluate the appearance of the
scene are in Table 4.
The main positive justification highlights the preservation of old buildings and the second deals with
their symbolic value and historical significance. The
third reason concerns the positive contrast perceived
by the presence of ancient and modern buildings. Since
this scene was rated negatively by only 5.6 percent of
respondents, it received little justification. The main
negative justification was the “conservation status,”
mentioned by 20 percent of respondents, followed by
lack of attractiveness of the scene and the changes performed on the facades.
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 7 – Scene 2 São José do Norte.
Table 5 – Justifications related to visual appearance of scene 2 – São José do Norte.
Scene 2
Scene 2 maintains the land structure from the colonial
period, with a few buildings remaining from the original
built heritage, currently adulterated or in poor states of
conservation, with modern and contemporary insertions.
This scene was positively evaluated by 55 percent of
respondents, while 17.5 percent considered it ugly. The
main reasons justifying the positive and negative assessment of the scene are shown in Table 5.
It appears that the main positive justifications on
the assessment of the scene is the “conservation status” and the “colors of the facades,” which highlights
how maintenance of buildings is an attribute valued by
nortenses (as residents of São José do Norte are called).
The existence of modern and new buildings was the
third positive justification, as respondents believed that
the buildings contribute to the renewal and upgrading
of the urban scene. However, the mix of modern and
old buildings was considered one of the main negative
justifications, along with conservation. The demolitions
and changes in the facades are the third justification
presented. The scene presents such a situation due to
the partial demolition of a two-story colonial terrace
that dominates the center of the scene (building 7) and
adulteration of facades in varying degrees.
Scene 3
Scene 3 (heterogeneous) of São José do Norte
collects representative examples of Luso-Brazilian
architecture from the colonial period (Figure 8). The
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
single-story row houses were adulterated. The colonial
mansion located in the center of the scene (Building 6),
which dominates by its volume, height, and other formal
and symbolic attributes, is in disrepair.
Both Building 7 (modern period) and Building 8 were
identified through mental maps as “ugly” places. This
scene was positively evaluated by 15 percent of respondents. Compared with the heterogeneous scenes of the
other city, it achieved the highest negative rating (37.5
percent). The main reasons justifying the perceived
visual appearance of the scene are shown in Table 6).
Although most of the justifications are negative with
respect to the visual appearance of the scene, “Sobrado
dos Imperadores,” or Emperors terrace house (Building 6), despite its poor condition, was highlighted with
the following statements: “could be restored and would
be beautiful; is poorly preserved; conservation is not
good but it gives life to the scene; the unique beautiful
building in the scene is not well maintained.” The state
of preservation was the main negative justification (42.5
percent), and in this context, mischaracterization such as
alteration in the facades, was more accurately perceived
than in the previous scene, where five adulterated buildings were considered positive.
Analysis of aesthetic preference of the scenes in São
José do Norte
The order of the scenes 1, 2, 3 was preferred by 42.5
percent of respondents, followed by the order of scenes
2, 1, 3 (32.5 percent). Scene 1 received 47.5 percent
161
Refereed Full-Papers
of residents’ preferences. Despite respondents’ preference for Scene 1, the rating was slightly higher than the
second scene, which was considered mixed. Comparing
the two scenes, the second presents a greater number of
adulterations, six in all, five of which were positively
assessed. It transpires in the questionnaire responses
that, due to the loss of much of the original structure
of the city and the state of ruination of the remaining
historic buildings, preservation seems to be the key
variable that affects preference. As opposed to Piratini,
where there is a rigorous and critical look from the
residents regarding the inclusion of new buildings and
adulterations, in São José do Norte evaluation is not
dependent on whether the building is ancient, modern,
contemporary, restored to the criteria of good technique,
or uncharacterized . For example, two respondents older
than 60 expressed a preference for new and modern
buildings, rather than the historic ones. Another aspect
that respondents make clear is a preference for buildings with more than one floor and row houses, which
even when adulterated are considered positive, suggesting some preference for this type of architecture. Findings about the aesthetic assessment of each scene shows
the trend of positive assessments on the scene 1 and 2
and negative evaluations concentrated on the scene 3
(Figure 9).
In the third scene, although chaotic, adulterations
were perceived as negative. Analyzing the responses as
“adulteration of terrace house; modified façade; other
buildings have been altered; because it is very uncharacteristic, nothing is as it would be” allows to infer that
the domain of the Emperors terrace house as its entire
Figure 8 – Scene 3 – São José do Norte.
Table 6 – Justifications related to visual appearance of scene 3 – São José do Norte
Figure 9 – Comparison of evaluative responses about visual appearance – São José do Norte.
162 Refereed Full-Papers
formal and symbolic weight, positively influenced the
aesthetic response. Some respondents commented that
the buildings should be restored to their original characteristics, and almost all respondents pointed the Emperors terrace house as a priority for restoration.
Relationship between evaluation of the visual
appearance of the scenes and formal attributes
The relationship between the assessment of the visual
appearance of the scenes and the composition of the
buildings was obtained by evaluating the formal attributes “volume,” “roofs,” and “facades” in each scene.
In homogeneous scenes (Scene 1), the correlation
between the “assessment of the visual appearance of
the scene” and “perception of compatibility of facades”
(Spearman coef .= 0.244, sig. = 0.00) was confirmed,
suggesting that recognition of the presence of order and
typological patterns of the facades that constitute Scene
1 play an important role in the positive evaluation of
homogeneous scenes (69 percent). In the mixed scenes
(Scene 2), statistical support was found for asserting
that the “assessment of the visual appearance of the
scene” is directly linked to the “perception of compatibility of facades” (Spearman coef .= 0.283, sig. = 0.00),
that is, perception of formal compatibility between the
facades was a relevant attribute for positive evaluation (51.3 percent) of mixed scenes. The research also
identifies a correlation between the “assessment of the
visual appearance of the scene” and the “perceptions of
compatibility of roofs” (Spearman coef .= 0.235, sig.
= 0.01). This relationship suggests that formal compatibility of roofs contributed to the positive assessment of
the scene.
In heterogeneous scenes (Scene 3) where negative
evaluation was higher (32.8 percent negative and 22.2
percent positive), a correlation was found between “assessment of the visual appearance of the scenes” and
“perception of compatibility of volume” (Spearman
coef. = 0.222, sig. = 0.00), which suggests that the lack
of formal compatibility between the volumes of the
buildings that compose the scene contributes to negative evaluation of the heterogeneous scenes—the lack
of adequate volume reduces the level of satisfaction
with the visual appearance. Correlation was also identified between the “assessment of visual appearance”
and “perception of compatibility in terms of facades”
(Spearman coef. = 0.194, sig. = 0.03), revealing that the
evaluation of appearance of the scene is directly linked
to compatibility between the facades of buildings. In the
case of heterogeneous scenes, the trend of appearance
evaluation was negative; that is, lack of formal compatibility decreased the level of satisfaction. In assessing
the visual appearance, only the formal attribute “facades” presented statistic significance in the homogeneous, mixed, and heterogeneous scenes, indicating the
importance of this attribute in the urban setting. This result allows us to infer that the greater the compatibility
between the facades, the higher the level of satisfaction
with the visual appearance of the urban landscape.
Relationship between evaluation of the visual
appearance of the scenes and formal compatibility with
pre-existing buildings
Analyses were conducted to verify how formal characteristics of pre-existing buildings (formal compatibility) were perceived in relation to new buildings inserted
in the pre-existing scenario (Table 7).
In all the scenes (homogeneous, mixed, and heterogeneous) in the two cities, respondents did not perceive
the existence of formal compatibility with new insertions in the urban setting. In the homogeneous scene in
Piratini, where only one building of the contemporary
period was inserted (with two floors and height similar
to the neighboring house), there is the lowest percent-
Table 7 – Perception of formal compatibility
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
163
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 8 – Relationship between visual appearance and formal compatibility of new insertions
age of formal incompatibility (44.4 percent), but this
shows the accuracy as the new inserts were valued by
the respondents, especially in a best-preserved ancient
structure. Relationships between evaluation of visual
appearance, perceived formal, volume, roofs, and façades were further explored (Table 8).
Correlations were significant between “assessment of
visual appearance” and “perception of formal compatibility” with pre-existing buildings only in Scene
2 (Spearman coef. = 0.271, sig. = 0.00). This trend
makes sense because due to its characteristics—not as
homogeneous as the first, and not as heterogeneous as
the third—Scene 2 received the most intense negative
assessments about the insertions of new buildings by
piratinenses (75 percent), which confirms perceived
incompatibility of the new insertions with respect to
pre-existing buildings. The perceived lack of compatibility of volume was detected in six scenes studied.
In Scene 1, statistical support was found for asserting
that the new insertions are not compatible with the
volume of pre-existing buildings, where the lack of
compatibility of volume between the buildings indicates
the importance of adequate volume in the aesthetic
response to visual appearance of urban scenes, especially when the number of buildings tends to be more
homogeneous. In Scene 2, correlation is also identified
between “formal compatibility of the new insertions”
and “compatibility of volume” (Spearman coef. =
0.342, sig. = 0.00). Scene 3 repeats the correlation between “formal compatibility of the new insertions” and
“compatibility of volume” (Spearman coef. = 0.407,
sig. = 0.00), indicating that the perceived lack of formal
compatibility between pre-existing buildings and new
insertions was influenced by the lack of compatibility of
volume. When correlated with the presence of “compatibility of roofs,” the influence of lack of compatibility
of roofs in the perception of formal compatibility of the
scenes was also verified. Note that the negative ratings
will increase inversely with the degree of preservation
of the scenes, so Scene 3 (heterogeneous) was the most
164 negatively evaluated regarding the compatibility of
roofs. When evaluated separately, in Scene 1 correlation
was identified between “formal compatibility between
pre-existing buildings and new insertions” and “compatibility of roof” (Spearman coef. = 0.354, sig. = 0.00),
indicating once again that the lack of compatibility
of roofs negatively affects perception of formal compatibility of the scenes. In Scene 2, there is the same
correlation (Spearman coef. = 0.486, sig. = 0.00), and
in Scene 3, this correlation is even stronger (Spearman
coef. = 0.496, sig. = 0 , 2000). Besides confirming that
there was no concern of integrating roofs of the new
insertions in relation to pre-existing buildings, it can be
seen that the more heterogeneous is the scene, the lower
the perceived compatibility in terms of roof.
The compatibility of facades plays a key role in the
aesthetic preference of the scenes and on the perception
of formal compatibility between the pre-existing buildings and new inserts. The perceived lack of compatibility in the three scenes indicates that most respondents
considered that there was no such concern. The homogeneous scenes show correlation between “perception
of compatibility of the new insertions” and “perception
of compatibility of facades” in the scene (Spearman
coef. = 0.331, sig. = 0.00), indicating, according to
the frequencies obtained (Table 8) that compatibility
was negatively perceived, and that the facades of new
buildings that were inserted in the urban landscape did
not consider the characteristic features of pre-existing
facades, affecting the perception of formal compatibility of the scene. The mixed and heterogeneous scenes
also show a significant correlation between “perception of formal compatibility of the new insertions” and
“perceived compatibility of facades” (Spearman coef.
= 0.540, sig. = 0.00), demonstrating the importance of
reconciling the facades of the old and new buildings.
In this respect, the results confirm results obtained by
Groat (1988) on the suggestion to “incorporate some
degree of replication (repetition of certain elements,
but with current design) in the design of facades, in
Refereed Full-Papers
addition to replication of the spatial pattern (contextual
appropriateness) and mass (volume).”
Conclusion
Results indicate the role of built heritage in the aesthetic evaluation of the urban landscape and emphasize
the relevance of studies focused on urban aesthetics
as a need to promote actions to qualification of public
spaces. The importance of a particular order, established
by formal consistency, is confirmed. For example, when
still present in the urban scene in the form of sets, the
old buildings tend to fit into a recognizable pattern,
suggesting an idea of order, which justifies the preference of the more homogeneous scenes over the others.
On the other hand, the perceived chaotic profile of
the heterogeneous scenes highlights the lack of order,
justifying the arguments of authors such as Lozano
(1988), Weber (1995), Nasar (1998), and Reis (2002),
who consider order as a human need and recognize it
as an important component that affects evaluation of
visual appearance of the environment. The valuation of
the buildings of the early period is confirmed by both
their particular formal and symbolic attributes when
related to the urban context, and especially for their
contribution in qualifying visual aesthetics of the urban
landscape. Also confirmed is that the symbolic attribute
“historical value” can positively affect aesthetic preference, corroborating studies by Coeterier (1996). Results
indicate that both the preservation of heritage buildings
and the aesthetic quality of new buildings can not be
conceived without considering the set of pre-existing
buildings. Even belonging to different periods and different styles, the buildings form relationships with each
other and can compose harmonic sets and an organic
environment, with pleasant visual appearance and positive evaluations. They can also establish ruptures as a
mixture of missing pieces, leading to chaotic appearance and negative evaluations. It was possible to identify relevant aspects in relation to matters of cultural
built heritage and the importance of including issues
of urban aesthetics in the process of city planning. It
also underscores the importance of bringing the users
of the historic core concerned with public policy issues relating to preservation of cultural heritage, urban
aesthetic, and urban planning. On one hand, research
results confirm the positive contribution of the buildings
of the ancient period to the visual quality of the urban
landscape; on the other, the need to curb the actions of
distortion, mutilation, and even demolition of buildings
of ancient period located in historic cores is evident.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
References
Azevedo L. N.; Lemos, J. C.; Reis, A. T.; Lay, M. C. (1999).
Morfologia, uso e referenciais urbano no centro de Porto
Alegre – ênfase a prédios históricos. In: Encontro Nacional
da Associação Nacional de Pós-graduação e Pesquisa em
Planejamento Urbano e Regional. Porto Alegre: ANPUR,
1999. 1 CD-ROM.
Coeterier, J. (1996). Permanent Values in a Changing World.
The Case of Historic Building. In: Proceedings of the 14
th Conference of the International Association for PeopleEnvironment Studies. Stockholm: IAPS, 120-128.
Groat, l. N.(1988). Contextual compatibility in architecture: an
issue of personal taste? In: NASAR, J. L. (ed.), Environmental
Aesthetics, Theory, Research & Applications. Cambridge:
University Press, 228-153.
Kiefer, F.; Luz, M. (2000). “A Arquitetura de Porto Alegre”.
ELARQ, vol. 10, n° 33. Montevidéu: Ed. Dos Puntos SRL,
fev. 38-49.
Lang, J. (1987). Creating architecture theory: The role of the
behavioral sciences in environmental design. New York: Van
Nostrand Reinhold.
Lang, J. (1988). Symbolic aesthetics in architecture: toward
a research agenda. In: NASAR J. L. (ed.), Environmental
Aesthetics, Theory, Research & Applications. Cambridge:
University Press 11-26.
Lozano, E. (1988). Visual needs in urban environments and
physical planning. In: NASAR J. L. (ed.), Environmental
Aesthetics, Theory, Research & Applications. Cambridge:
University Press, 395-421.
Lynch, K..(1975). ?De qué tiempo es este lugar? Barcelona:
Editora Gustavo Gili.
Nasar, J. L. (1998). The evaluative image of the city. California:
Thousand Oaks.
Reis, A. (2000). Repertório, análise e síntese: uma introdução ao
projeto arquitetônico. Porto Alegre: Editora da UFRGS.
Reis. A.; Lay M. C. (2006). Avaliação da qualidade de projetos:
uma abordagem perceptiva e cognitiva. Ambiente Construído,
Porto Alegre, vol. 6, n.3, 21-34.
Stamps, A. E. III (1989). Are environmental aesthetics worth
studying?. Journal of Architectural and Planning Research, ,
6:4.
Weber, R. (1995). On the aesthetics of architecture: a
psychological approach to the structure and the order of
perceived architectural space. Aldershot, England: Avebury.
165
Refereed Full-Papers
Evaluating the Impact of Urban
Form on Older Adults’ Social
Activity and Public Health
Mehran Madani (Washington State
University), Christine Oakley (Washington
State University)
Introduction
Due to the rapid growth of an aging society, it is
necessary to understand the social capability and level
of socio-behavioral immersion of older adults in public
places correlated with physical environments, psychological ambiance, and the quality of their public health.
The current lack of literature on older adults’ public
health related to the social activity and urban form
suggests that more research is needed to understand the
complex set of factors that impact how different aging
population groups react to various urban forms, social
activity interventions, and physical activity patterns in
regard to their public health. Accordingly, the specific
aims of this study are first, to investigate the impact of
physical geography on the public health of older adults
(by objectives such as 1) assessing the impact of components of the urban form and the built environment
that contribute to the social activities of older adults in
selected suburban and urban neighborhoods/communities, 2) assessing the impact of components of the social
geography that contribute to the public health of older
adults in selected suburban and urban neighborhoods/
communities); and second, to evaluate the shift in the
current demographic trend of older adults residing in
care facilities located in suburban areas (by objectives
such as 1) collecting data from individual older adults
relevant to their social, physical, and neighborhood context, and 2) reviewing the current literature through an
interdisciplinary study including architecture, landscape
urbanism, urban planning, sociology, gerontology, and
public health).
Background and Significance of Study
Importance of the Problem
1) Fast-growing populations of older adults as
a significant population cluster in contemporary
society. Since the transition from an industrial society
to today’s information age, there has been significant
worldwide progress in enhancing both personal and
166 public health, resulting in increased longevity and a
greater life expectancy (Hodge, 2008). Demographers
predict that by the mid twenty-first century, people 65
and over will compose about 15 percent of the world’s
population, twice what it is currently (Abbott, 2009).
This aging cohort has a faster growth in developed
countries such as Japan, Italy, Sweden, Canada, and the
United States. “The United States aging population,
age 65 and over, will be around 72 million in 2030,
twice the number of 2000” (Abbott, 2009, p.23). While
the aging population will face physical and mental challenges that may restrict activities by limiting mobility,
agility, hearing, seeing, and speaking (Birren, 1991),
Weiss and Bass (2002) believe that aging in society is
a dynamic and adaptive phenomenon best understood
as an outcome of ongoing interactions and transactions
among a complex set of interdependent public health
factors that include physical environments, sociobehavioral characteristics, and emotional well-being.
This study offers an opportunity to assess the reciprocal
relationships among these factors on independent and
semi-independent older adults.
2) Change in the household organization of
American families. Cantor (1991) argues that the
structure of the American family has become more
“vertical” as family size decreases, adult children leave
the home, and grandparents become more independent
from younger family members (as cited in Knox, 1995).
As horizontal relationships that form strong interpersonal ties are weakened, older adults rely more on
extra-familial social and health services, thus forming
relationships with caregivers rather than depending on
their family members. Knox (1995) asserts that these
changes in family structure increase the socio-spatial
differentiation between older and younger members of
the family. Arber and Evandrou (1993) assert that older
adults will therefore miss the mutually interdependent
family relationships which are important components
of the social support network from which older adults
derive meaning in their lives. Changes in family structure, therefore, create a seemingly paradoxical situation
for independent elders in which independence is an
indicator of both well-being and social isolation. Empirical data are needed to unravel this paradox so that
reasonable solutions to maximize adult public health
can be identified.
3) Social and physical dynamics of urban form.
Urban form is a general term that refers to the amalgamation of towns and cities. Urban form is defined
Refereed Full-Papers
by such terms characteristics as density, concentration,
levels of connectivity, centrality, diversity, mixed uses,
and proximity (Frank and Engelke, 2009). Thus different manifestations of urban form will vary in the degree
to which they reflect these characteristics. Suburbanization as an aspect of urban form describes the growth of
areas on the fringes of major cities. Currently more than
half of the U.S. population lives in “the suburbs” (Palen
2008). A consequence of continued suburbanization,
however, is “urban sprawl” (Frumkin, Frank, and Jackson, 2004). “Sprawl” refers to automobile-dependent
communities on the outer reaches of metropolitan areas
characterized by homogeneous land uses, less physical activity, the absence of walkable environments, and
the risk of pedestrian fatalities (Frumkin, 2004). While
much sprawl is driven by development that favors the
exchange value of land over its use value (Logan and
Molotch 1987), the impact of sprawl reaches beyond
the physical environment. According to Knox (1995),
the creation and reshaping of the physical fabric of the
urban form (morphogenesis) leads to a change in the
context of urban social geography. Some scholars have
suggested that suburbanization produces new environments, new types of people, and new ways of life;
however, there is little research assessing how these
new environments will affect the social geography of
older adults.
Knox (1995) claims that the urban form can turn
people outward, providing them with experiences of
otherness. The power of the city, for example, to reorient people in this way lies in its diversity, even if it is
just for a short while. As older adults are exposed to
otherness, their capacity to transact with their environment increases, enabling them to increase their participation and contribution to social and civic life. Yet
the relationship between spatial aspects of urban form
and the social well-being of older adults has not been
adequately explored.
4) Examining spatial impacts of urban form and
the built environment on the capacity of older adults
to create social capital. An integral element in all urban forms is the built environment. In this project, we
propose to study the built environment as a medium of
communication for older adults. According to Schneider
(2010), physical geography of neighborhoods impacts
the people who live there and fundamentally affects
their perceptions of the built environment. The built environment is an expression of not only human attempts
to fulfill personal and societal needs and wants, but also
personal and collective values and aspirations. While
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
human values may be more abstract than basic needs,
they can be evidenced, for example, through participation in social networks and in levels of civic engagement. Schneider’s (2010) work identifies the social
dynamics of a neighborhood’s capacity to create social
capital. Developing social capital involves a process
of constructing trusting relationships and establishing
social networks across neighborhoods in order to experience a sense of community (Schneider, 2010). She
stresses, however, that social networks only represent
social capital if they help to access needed resources.
It is important, therefore, to examine the relationship
between built environment affordances* that reflect urban form and their capacity to enable the development
of social networks that enhance access to resources that
increase the well-being of older adults.
5) Change in the physical geography of older
adults. According to Hodge (2008), Americans 65
and older living in urban areas were evenly distributed
between the city core and suburban areas as recently
as 1977. Current data, however, reveal that the number
of older adults living in the suburbs of metropolitan
areas now exceeds the number living within the city
(Census, 2000), a shift consistent with the geographical redistribution of the general population. Combined
with changes in the family structure, one may assume
that independent and semi-independent older adults are
more likely to be concentrated in low-density residential neighborhoods, which characterize the suburbs and
where services no longer provided in a family context
are difficult to access by walking (Patterson and Chapman, 2002).
6) Influence of land use policies on the geographic
location of older adults. Also contributing to the shift
in the physical geography of older adults are contemporary urban design and urban planning policies (Hodge,
2008). The geographic locations of supportive facilities
in which older adults live are strictly controlled by public policy, zoning regulations, land development, and
infrastructure investment, for example. Such policies
and regulations have the capacity to determine older
adults’ physical environment, thereby influencing their
social geography, social networks, attitudes, and behaviors (Knox, 1995). Since the characteristic features
of urban sprawl discourage physical activity, they have
the potential to contribute to not only a downturn in the
physical health of the elderly, but also their access to
social environments in which social capital is created.
Thus the surrounding environment plays a major part
in shaping patterns of independence and dependence
167
Refereed Full-Papers
among older adults (Clarke & Nieuwenhuijsen, 2009).
Socially cohesive neighborhoods may facilitate outdoor activities and may be more attuned to the needs
of potentially vulnerable residents (Frumkin, Frank,
and Jackson, 2004). However, the degree to which a
neighborhood’s social geography affects the walking
behavior of older adults remains unclear. A growing
body of research demonstrates that urban settings, community design, and in general, our built environment
affordances—or “what objects or things offer people to
do with them…creat[ing] potential activities for users”
(Jordan et al., 1998, p. 4)—have enormous potential for
addressing people’s behavior and many of our public
health concerns. But there is little empirical study on
the impact of the built environment on older adults’
socio-behavior and their public health.
Improvement in Scientific Knowledge
1) Assessing public health of older adults from
a socio-spatial perspective. Dummer (2008) argues
that the location and nature of environmental features,
lifestyle, functional interaction with the social, built,
and natural environments, and the arrangement of
health services are crucial in assessing the interrelations
inherent in many health-related risk exposures. He also
emphasizes that the growth of cities causes significant
changes in the built environment and continues to have
a profound effect on the health and well-being of its
citizens. There is evidence that policy decisions and
spatial factors influence the structure of built environments and reinforce the shift to suburban areas which
leads to socio-cultural shifts that affect physical, social,
and health geographies (Dummer, 2008; Frumkin,
Frank, and Jackson, 2004). Dummer (2008) asserts
public health and health policy is best informed by understanding the linkages among place, well-being, and
disease. In a similar definition, Clarke & Nieuwenhuijsen (2009) believe public health is shaped by complex
interactions between individuals and the environments
in which they live, work, and play. Clarke & Nieuwenhuijsen (2009) also emphasize that environments
embrace urban physical settings (physical environment), attitudes and relationships with others (social
environment), and political systems and policies. They
point out that the impact of environments (physical
and social) on the physical health, mental health, and
social well-being of individuals has emerged as a growing body of research in population health and health
disparities. Yet the majority of studies in this area do
not focus on older adults, even though older adults are
168 particularly susceptible to the characteristics of their
local environments.
2) Advantages to interdisciplinary approaches.
Our case study approach to assessing the environmental
and social contexts of supportive facilities in which independent and semi- independent older adults reside enables a more holistic approach to the assessment of the
relationships among space, place, and the public health
of older adults. It has the potential to contribute to multiple literatures that share similar goals to increase the
functional capacity of older adults, but have approached
the problem from a more discipline-specific perspective. Informed by studies from urban planning, urban
design, public health, sociology, and gerontology, our
proposed research suggests interdisciplinary approaches
to understanding complex social phenomena provide
a scholarly synergy not possible in more disciplinespecific studies. Past research has shown that there is a
relationship among the health and well-being of older
adults and their physical and social geographies. What
our study offers is an assessment of the interactional
and transactional processes that structure built environment affordances that acknowledge the role older adults
play in maintaining social, physical and public health.
Potential For Change in the Field of Urban Planning, Community Development, and Conceptualization
of Older Adults
1) Policy implications. Because urban planning
is driven by public policy, land use and zoning regulations, and development interests, the results of our
study have the potential to impact decisions about the
location of future supportive facilities for independent
and semi-independent older adults. If policies enable
such facilities to be located in areas that positively influence the creation and maintenance of social networks
that provide access to needed resources, the need to rely
on more expensive skilled nursing facilities for longterm care may be reduced. If our study provides evidence that older adults’ public health is higher in more
urbanized environments and lower in suburban settings,
as we hypothesize, then an examination of policies and
regulations favoring suburban locations of supportive
facilities will be needed.
2) Research approaches. Case studies, such as
ours, call for additional research to implement practical
solutions to complex social problems. The results of our
examination of older adults’ public health in both social
and physical geographic contexts encourage the use of
two research strategies with applied foci. The first is to
Refereed Full-Papers
utilize methodologies associated with the field of health
geography. According to Dummer (2008), “advances in
multi-level modeling, geographic information systems,
and spatial analysis further supports research investigating the relative influence of individual and community
level health risks within a unified framework” (p. 1179).
The second is utilization of Health Impact Assessments,
which provide data on the health impacts of proposed
policies or programs. Linking these techniques to
research studies such as ours helps bridge the gap between theory and practice.
Innovation
Paradigm Shifts
1) Re-conceptualizing the importance of place for
older adults. Some scholars have initiated the notion of a paradigm shift in the contemporary theory of
urbanization which encourages people to migrate back
to the inner cities instead of fragmenting the population in outer urban areas. Frank and Engelke (2009)
argue that modern exclusionary zoning regulations,
requiring greater travel distances between where people
live, work, and play, may counter the original health
intent imbedded within the Zoning Enabling Act, which
granted cities the power to regulate the use, type, placement, and density of structures within a given jurisdiction. Density and land use together determine the
geographic proximity between activities, whereas street
connectivity affects the ease of access between activities (Frumkin, Frank, and Jackson, 2004). These authors
suggest that these kinds of settings produce undersocialized individuals with declined social well-being
and civic-mindedness. Moudon et al. (1997) found that
neighborhoods with greater connectivity and better
facilities generated higher pedestrian traffic volumes
than those with poorer levels of connectivity and poorer
facilities. Furthermore, they also found out that, on
average, urban sites have about three times the pedestrian volume of suburban sites (Moudon, et al., 1997).
However, it remains uncertain what geographic scale,
density, and type of land use should be measured, and
what an “ideal” combination of uses would be maintained within differing urban forms (Frank & Engelke,
2009). This study is aligned with this shift and attempts
to provide further support for the city as a more appropriate environment for the health of older adults.
Innovation and Improvements on the Application of
Theoretical Concepts
1) Convergent insights of design, public health,
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
and sociology. The interdisciplinary approach to
examining the relationship between urban form and
social well-being of older adults enables the emergence
of convergent insights from three theoretical perspectives. For example, public health theorizes the relationship between the design of the built environment and
its positive or negative impact on physical activity and
social integration of older adults in terms of populationbased outcomes—healthy communities, health promotion, disease prevention, cost containment. Urban design scholars contribute to the conceptualization of this
relationship by contributing insights such as the role
of affordances in enabling positive health outcomes of
design decisions. Sociologists add their theoretical assumptions about the self as a social construct in relation
to the individual and her/his social environment. By
integrating the convergent insights of these approaches,
our study suggests a more comprehensive understanding of the interactional dynamics between older adults,
urban form, and physical/social/health geographies.
2) Potential for expanded theoretical assumptions about urban design, older adults, and public
health. It has been well documented that social contacts
and self-expressions in the urban context are enhanced
by the presence of three variables in communities: the
opportunity for passive social contact, proximity to others, and appropriate space to interact (Festinger et al.,
1950). The social geography of the urban form is itself
likely to generate or reinforce differences in values from
one neighborhood to another. Therefore, older adults
living in different urban forms conceive different social
activities, social identity, and social contribution. These
social attributes are influenced by interactional spaces
in different urban forms. In this study, we distinguish
two kinds of social interactions in older adults’ public
life. One is the type of passive interaction in which
meanings are not exchanged and over which older
adults have little control. The second is a more active or
expressive interaction or “social transaction” in which
social meanings define the activities and therefore serve
as stimuli for older adults’ contribution to the social
environment.
3) Innovative application of community design
theory. The analysis of built environment affordances
utilizes two main features of urban design: “demand
quality & invitation quality” (Gehl, 1987). Demand
quality of the built environment addresses the functional
purpose of affordances and self-motivated actions,
which requires outdoor physical settings and facilities
for inhabitants’ ease of use. Invitation quality meets the
169
Refereed Full-Papers
optional and social activities of the dwellers, in which
affordances enable public activities and programs to
enhance the quality of social transactions (Gehl, 1987;
Giarrusso, 2001). By utilizing James Gibson’s (1986)
definition of affordances* as all “action possibilities” latent in the environment, they are objectively
measurable—and although often independent of the
individual’s ability to recognize them, affordances are
always assessed in relation to the social actor. The built
environment affordances indicate not only the ability,
but also the capability of physical and social actions in
a community (as cited in Knox, 1995).
Approach
Research Design
1) Embedded case study. Yin (1994) argues that
the case study allows an investigation to retain the holistic and meaningful characteristics of real-life events
such as individual life cycles, organizational and managerial processes, neighborhood change, and national
and international relations. As an empirical inquiry,
case studies enable the investigation of contemporary
phenomena within real-life context, especially when
the boundaries between phenomena and context are not
clearly evident (Yin, 1994). An embedded case study
design was selected for use in this project. Embedded
case studies enable researchers to capture the complexity of social interrelationships by allowing for multiple
units of analysis (Yin, 1994; Caronna et al., 1997).
Caronna et. al (1997) state that embedded designs also
employ at least two levels of analysis as independent
sources of data, “such as individuals, organizations, and
environments” (p. 4), which are categorized as “cases”
and “contexts.” In the proposed research, older adults
living in selected supportive facilities are identified as
“cases” embedded in socio-spatial contexts. In addition, supportive facilities will be considered as cases
embedded in geo-political, regulatory environments that
determine their location, function, and structure. Caronna et al. (1997) allow for the combination of these two
case/context models in a multi-level design.
2) Case selection. Urban form: One of the goals
of the study is to understand how the existing spatial
distribution of the aging population impacts older
adults’ public health. This study will select facilities
and individual cases from two distinct urban forms—
inside the growth development boundary and suburban
areas—from two major metropolitan areas in two West
Coast states, Portland (Oregon) and Seattle (Washington). Facilities: Eight facilities will be selected, four
170 per metropolitan area, with two located in the inner
city and two in the suburban area. Facilities will be
selected based upon residents’ level of independency—
functionally independent (use of outdoors without any
assistance), or semi-dependent (use of outdoors with or
without supervision and with some device like a cane,
wheelchair, or walker). Facilities such as “retirement
homes” and “assisted living centers” will provide minimum support to accomplish activities of daily living.
Although matched cases are not possible, facilities will
be similar enough to collect appropriate comparative
data. Individuals: Thirty individuals in each of the settings (total 240) will be selected to complete questionnaires using purposive and maximum variation sampling to address our research power analysis (Carbtree
& Miller, 1999). An additional 40 individuals (five per
setting) will be recruited to volunteer for participation
in photovoice* focus groups.
3) Data collection. Washington State University
Institutional Review Board approval will be obtained
prior to the beginning of this research. Data will be
collected from each of the cases and contexts identified
above. Two general sets of data will be collected from
each context: those measuring physical geography and
those measuring social geography. Data measuring
the physical geography of both urban form and their
embedded facilities will be collected using GIS urban
measures such as residential density, land use, street
connectivity, proximity to physical activity resources,
and public transit access (Papas et al., 2007). Zoning
and regulatory data will be collected using standard record and document retrieval techniques. Data on regulatory and licensing requirements for supportive facilities
will also be collected.
Data on social geography will be collected through
the survey questionnaires (open and close ended questions), participant observation, and photovoice technique to verify the quality of spatial parameters, urban
features, quality of the built environment affordances,
the number of hours that residents spend outdoors, the
types of outdoor activities, and the types and quality
of older adults’ social transactions. Demographic data
will be collected from both the participant and facility
* Photovoice is a participatory action research tactic by which
people create and discuss photographs as a means of catalyzing
personal and community change (Wang, 1998). It is important
to note that photovoice techniques produce qualitative data that
rely on participants’ ratings of the built environment qualities to
validate built environment measurements, which may not correspond to the visual preferences of professional observers.
Refereed Full-Papers
records. Self-reported health status questions are included in the questionnaire. All participants will receive
informed consent documentation for their information
and participation approval.
4) Analysis. All quantitative data will be analyzed
using GIS. All GIS measures will be constructed using
ArcGIS 9.2 and ArcView 3.3. to provide diagrams of
spatial features and to measure block face characteristics. Also, GIS provides visual definitions of the most
commonly used geometries, such as streets and sidewalk lines, blocks, tax lots, block face lines, building
footprints, and building frontage lines (Purciel et al.,
2009). Use of GIS enables comparison of the associations between factors of physical geography, such as
built environment affordances and socio-behavioral
activities of study participants. In the absence of GIS
equivalents for some physical settings, more contextdependent spatial data may be included and some
organizational features which are not well measured
will be omitted (Holahan & Sorenson, 1985; Lynch,
1960). Text-based qualitative data will be managed
with NVivo8 software to organize and analyze complex
non-numerical information. Initial observational will be
open coded to enable identification of common themes
(Lincoln & Guba, 1985).
The association between each of the spatial geography variables and social and physical activity patterns will be tested after adjusting for individual-level
correlates of older adults’ self-reported health status
indicators. Multilevel regression models will be used
to test the association of the urban form’s level of
social cohesion with physical activity applicable to
the older adults’ health quality. While this may be an
optimal analytic strategy, its power may be limited by
the small number of participant cases. The individual
level of data such as age, sex, education, income,
marital status, years of residence in the neighborhood,
and self-reported health status are considered nested
within social interactions and transactions as defined
by census block groups and models. In the process of
data analysis, three-way interactions among the variables will be tested. The first is the correlation between
physical geography and social activities; the second will
measure the relationship between social activity and
physical activity; and the third will analyze the association of social activity and public health (a composite
variable of measures of social interaction, health status,
and network ties). These correlations will be tested to
retain the theoretical model for possible amendments to
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
future urban planning and design guidelines according
to health considerations. The credibility and validity of
the data will be established by peer debriefing, which
presents analyses and conceptual abstractions of the
data to other experts to explore the investigators’ biases
and to clarify meanings and interpretations. Member
checks, which present the analysis of data to selected
participants for their confirmation or revision, will be
performed (Lincoln & Guba, 1985).
5) Design limitations. Because these data are
cross-sectional, they cannot directly capture the timespace continuum inherent in the process of ongoing
social transactions for which a longitudinal design is
better suited. Giddens (1984) suggests, however, that
although social research must be “sensitive to the timespace constitution of social life,” attending to “the contextual features of locales through which actors move
in their daily paths and the regionalization of locales
stretching away across time-space” (p. 286) enables the
analysis of social activities to occur. The embedded
case study design that enables collection and analysis of
data on multiple units of analysis from both cases and
contexts is intended to provide the “sensitivity” Giddens
(1984) finds necessary in conducting research on social
life.
Outcomes and Impacts
Outcomes:
• Finding the pros and cons of different urban forms that older adults residing in supportive facilities
confront.
• Increasing opportunities for built environment
affordances to promote expressive social interaction
(social transaction) that maximizes mobility, social
capital, access to health and human services, and
finally, public health.
Data collected from individual older adults relevant
to their social, physical, and neighborhood contexts will
be used to construct models for practical application in
urban planning and facility design. Models will also
serve as the basis for continued applied research such as
Health Impact Assessments, and for continued scholarly
contribution to the study of in health geography.
Impacts:
• Increased public health of older adults has the
potential to decrease cost of long-term care, increase
capacity for contributions by older adults to create
171
Refereed Full-Papers
social capital, and enhance social and physical
environments within the urban core.
• Possible shifts in planning and design trends from
continued suburban development of supportive
facilities for independent and semi-independent
older adults to those in the urban core.
• Increases in social contribution and self-efficacy for
older adults despite living in supportive facilities.
Bibliography
Abbott, P., & Carman, N., & Carman, J., & Scarfo, B., (2009).
Re-Creating Neighborhoods for Successful Aging, Health
Profession Press, Inc.
Arber, S., & Evandrou, M., (1993). Aging, Independence and
Life Course, British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data.
Augustin, T., GlassA. T., James, D. B., & Schwartz, S.
B., (2008). Neighborhood Psychosocial Hazards and
Cardiovascular Disease: The Baltimore Memory Study.
American Journal of Health, 98, 912.
Bartuska, T., (2007). The Built Environment: Definition
and Scope. In McClure, W., & Bartuska, T., The Built
Environment: a Collaborative Inquiry into Design and
Planning (pp. 3-15). John Wiley & Sons.
Birren, J., (1991). Quality of Life in the Frail Elderly. Academic
Press, INC.
Burgess E. J., (1924). The Growth of The City: an introduction to
a research project. Publications of the American Sociological
Society, 18, 85-97.
Caronna, C., & Pollack, S., & Scott, W. R. (1997). Cases and
Contexts: Investigating Micro-Macro Linkages Through
an Embedded Case Study Design. Presented at the annual
meeting of the American Sociological Association, August
11, Toronto, CA.
Clarke, P., & Nieuwenhuijsen, E., (2009). Environment for
Healthy Ageing: A Critical Review. Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
Cervero, R., & Roger, G., (1995). Commuting in transit versus
automobile neighborhoods. Journal of the American Planning
Association, 61, 210-25.
Crabtree, B. F., & Miller, W. L., (1999). Doing Qualitative
Research (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA. Sage.
Crane, R., (1999). The impacts of urban form on travel: A critical
review. Lincoln Institute of Land Policy, Cambridge, MA.
Cummins S, & Curtis S, Diez-Roux AV, et al., (2007),
Understanding and representing ‘place’ in health research: a
relational approach. Soc Sci Med, 65, 1825-38.
Dear, M., Gaber, S., Takahashi, L. & Wilton, R., (1997) Seeing
People Differently: The Social and Spatial Construction of
Disability. Princeton University Press.
Dummer, T., (2008), Health Geography: Supporting Public
Health Policy and Planning. Canadian Medical Association.
CMAJ, 1177-1180.
Evans, G. (2003). The Built Environment and Mental Health.
Journal of Urban Health, 80, 536–555.
172 Ewing, R., (1997). Transportation and land use innovations:
When you can’t pave your way out of congestion. Chicago:
American Planning Association.
Ewing, R., Clemente, O., Handy, S. L., Brownson, R. C., &
Winston, E. (2005). Identifying and Measuring Urban
Design Qualities Related to Walkability - Final Report.
Princeton, NJ: Robert Wood Johnson Foundation.
Ewing, R., & Handy, S. L. (2009). Measuring the
Unmeasurable: Urban Design Qualities Related to
Walkability. Journal of Urban Design, 14, 65–84.
Festinger, L., & Schachter, S., & Back, K., (1950). The Spatial
Ecology of Group Formation. University of Minnesota Press.
Frank, D. L., & Engelke, O. P., (2001). The Built Environment
and Human Activity Patterns: Exploring the Impacts
of Urban Form on Public Health. Journal of Planning
Literature @ Sage Publications, 16, 202-218
Frumkin, H., & Frank, L., & Jackson, R., (2004). Urban Sprawl
and Public Health. Island Press (Washington, Covelo,
London).
Gatrell, A. C., Bailey, T. C., Diggle, P. J., & Rowlingson, B. S.
(1996). Spatial Point Pattern Analysis and its Application in
Geographical Epidemiology. Transactions of the Institute of
British Geographers, 21, 256–274.
Gehl, J., (1987). Life Between Buildings. Van Nostrand
Reinhold., I.C.L.
Gibson, J. J., (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual
Perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Publishers.
Giarrusso, R., & J. Beth Mabry, J., & Bengtson, V., (2001),
Handbook of aging and social sciences, The aging self in
social contexts, fifth edition, Academic Press, (P.295-312).
Giddens, A., (1984). The Constitution of Society. Cambridge,
UK: Polity Press.
Gordon, P., & Richardson, H., (1989). Gasoline consumption and
cities: Areply. Journal of the American Planning Association,
55, 342-46.
Guite, H. F., Clark, C., & Ackrill, G. (2006). The Impact of the
Physical and Urban Environment on Mental Well-being.
Public Health, 120, 1117–1126.
Hess, P., (1997). Measures of connectivity. Places. A Forum of
Environmental Design, 11, 58-65.
Hodge, G., (2008). The Geography of Aging. Mc Gill-Queen’s
University Press.
Hoehner, C. M., Ramirez, L. K. B., Elliott, M. B., Handy, S.
L., & Brownson, R. C. (2005). Perceived and Objective
Environmental Measures and Physical Activity Among
Urban Adults. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28,
105–116.
Holahan, C. J., & Sorenson, P. F. (1985). The role of figural
organization in city imageability: an information processing
analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 279–286
Johnston, R. J., (2000). The Dictionary of Human Geography.
Wiley-Blackwell.
Jordan, T., & Raubal, M., & Gartell, H. (1998) An affordancebased model of place in GIS. Proceedings of the 8th
International Symposium on Spatial Data Handling, SDH’98
98–109.
Knox, P., (1995). Urban Social Geography. Longman Group
Limited.
Refereed Full-Papers
Lawton, PG., (1989). The a Multidimensional View of Quality
of Life in Frail Elders. In the Concept and Measurement of
Quality of Life in the Frail Elderly. Academic Press, INC.
Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic Inquiry. Beverly
Hills, Sages.
Logan, J., Molotch, H. (1987). Urban fortunes: the political
economy of place. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Lynch, K., (1960). The Image of the City. The M.I.T. Press.
McPhillips, JB,. Pellettera, KM,. Barrett-Connor, E,. Wingard,
DL,. & Criqui, MH., (1989). Exercise patterns in a
population of older adults. American Journal Preventive
Medicine, 5, 65–72.
Mendes, F. C., Cagney, A. K., Bienias, L. J., Barnes, L. L.,
Skarupski, A. K., Scherr, A. P., & Evans, A. D., (2009).
Neighborhood Social Cohesion and Disorder in Relation to
Walking in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Journal of
Aging and Health @ Sage Publications, 21, 155-171.
Moudon, A., Hess, P., Snyder, M., & Stanilov, K., (1997). Effects
of site design on pedestrian travel in mixed use, mediumdensity environments. Transportation Research Record, 1578,
48-55.
Moudon, A. V., & Hubner, M. (2000). Monitoring Land Supply
With Geographic Information Systems: Theory, Practice, and
Parcel-based Approaches. New York: John Wiley & Sons,
Inc.
Newman, P., & Kenworthy, J., (1989). Gasoline consumption
and cities: A comparison of U.S. cities with a global survey.
Journal of the American Planning Association, 55, 24-37.
Palen, J. (2008). The Urban World. Boulder, CO: Paradigm
Publishers.
Papas, M. A., Alberg, A. J., Ewing, R., Helzlsouer, K. J., Gary,
T. L., & Klassen, A. C.(2007). The built environment and
obesity. Epidemiologic Reviews, 29, 129–143.
Patterson, P., & Chapman, N., (2004). Urban Form and Older
Residents’ Service Use, Walking, Driving, Quality of Life,
and Neighborhood Satisfaction. American Journal of Health
Promotion, Inc., 19, 45-52.
Pearce J., (2007), Incorporating geographies of health into
public policy debates: The Geo- Health Laboratory. N Z Geo,
73, 149-53.
Purciel, M., & Neckerman, K., & Lovasi, G., Quinn, J., &Weiss,
C., & Bader, M., & Ewing, R., & Rundle, A., ( 2009),
Creating and validating GIS measures of urban design for
health research. Elsevier Ltd.
Rapoport, A., (1977). Human Aspects of Urban Form. Pergamon
Press (Oxford, New York, Toronto, Sydney, Paris, Frankfurt).
Relph, E., (1976). Place and Placelessness. Pion Limited,
London.
Rowles, G., & Ravdal, H., (2002). Aging in Place, and Meaning
in The Face of Changing Circumstances (pp. 81-115). In
Weiss, S.R., & Bass, A.S. Challenges of the Third Age.
Oxford University Press.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Scarfo, B., (2009). Collabration as the Key to the Successful
Future of Aging (pp. 225-252). In Abbott, P., & Carman,
N., & Carman, J., & Scarfo, B., (2009). Re-Creating
Neighborhoods For Successful Aging, Health Profession
Press, Inc.
Scheider, J. A., (2010). Social Capital and Social Geography.
The Annie E. Casey Foundation.
Settersten, R., (2002). Social Sources of Meaning in Later Life
(pp. 55-81). In Weiss, R. S., & Bass, S. A., (2002). Challenge
of the Third Age. Oxford University Press.
Song, Y., & Knaap, G., (2004). Measuring Urban Form. Journal
of the American Planning Association, 70, 210-224.
Southworth, M., (1997). Walkable suburbs? An evaluation of
neotraditional communities at the urban edge. Journal of the
American Planning Association, 63, 28-44.
Stafford, P., (2009). Elderburbia: Aging With a Sense of Place in
America. ABC-CLIO, LLC.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J., (1990). Basics of Qualitative Research:
Grounded Theory Procedure and Techniques, Sage, Newbury
Park, London.
Simonsick, E. M., Guralnik, J. M., Volpato, S., Balfour, J., &
Fried, L. P., (2005). Just get out the door! Importance of
walking outside the home for maintaining mobility: findings
from the women’s health and aging study. Journal of the
American Geriatrics Society, 53, 198.
Skjaeveland, O., & Garling, T., (1997). Effects of International
Space on Neighbouring. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 17, 181-198.
Tilt, J. H., Unfried, T. M., & Roca, B. (2007). Using Objective
and Subjective Measures of Neighborhood Greenness and
Accessible Destinations for Understanding Walking Trips
and BMI in Seattle, Washington. American Journal of Health
Promotion, 21, 371–379.
Tunstall, H., & Shaw M., Dorling D., (2004). Places and health.
Journal of Epidemiol Comm Health. 58, 6-21.
Untermann, R., (1987). Changing design standards for streets
and roads. In Public streets for public use, Anne Moudon, ed.
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
U.S. Department of Human Services., (1996). Physical Activity
and Health. A Report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta,
Ga: U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic
Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.
Wang, C., &Redwood, Y., (2001), Photovoice Ethics:
Perspectives from Flint Photovoice. Sage Publications.
Weiss, R. S., & Bass, S. A., (2002). Challenges of the Third Age.
Oxford University Press.
Weuve J, Kang, J. H., Manson, J. E., Breteler, M. M., Ware,
J. H., & Grodstein, F., (2004). Physical activity, including
walking, and cognitive function in older women. Jama, 292,
1454–1461.
Yin, R., (1994), Case Study Research. Sage Publications. 292,
1454–1461.
173
Refereed Full-Papers
Threshold In School – As
Event and Place for the Autism
Spectrum Disorder Pupil
Keith McAllister (Queen’s University Belfast,
Ireland), Barry Maguire (Barry Maguire
Chartered Architect)
Abstract
As architects and designers, we have a responsibility to provide an inclusive built environment. For the
Autistic Spectrum Disorder (ASD) sufferer, however,
the built environment can be a frightening and confusing place, difficult to negotiate and tolerate. The challenge of integrating more fully into society is denied
by an alienating built environment. This barrier can be
magnified for ASD pupils in a poorly designed school,
where their environment can further distance them from
learning. Instead, if more at ease in their surroundings,
in an ASD-friendly environment, the ASD pupil stands
a greater chance of doing better.
Whilst researchers have looked at the classroom
environment, the transition of classroom to corridor and
beyond has so far been largely ignored. However, the
need for a well-considered threshold between class and
corridor needs to be considered. In this regard, threshold is much more than a doorway, but instead an event
that demands a carefully considered place. The following paper firstly outlines why threshold as place and
event for the ASD pupil should be given consideration.
It then goes onto highlight, through case studies in an
Irish context, the opportunities for aiding the ASD pupil
integrating in a mainstream school environment through
sensitive use of threshold. Finally it highlights in
conclusion, some of the benefits for an enriched school
environment for all pupils, if considering threshold as
design generator.
The objective is straightforward. By increasing
awareness of the relationship between the ASD child
and the built environment it will hopefully facilitate
greater inclusion of the ASD pupil into mainstream
education and society at large.
Introduction
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a term that
covers the many subgroups within the spectrum of
autism. Autism can be termed as a lifelong complex
174 developmental disorder. It is characterised by a triad of
qualitative impairments in social communication, social
interaction, and social imagination. (Wing & Gould,
1979) The range of the spectrum is such that while some
sufferers may be able to live relatively independently,
others will require lifelong continuous support.
In addition to these problems, sufferers often struggle
with sensory sensitivity to visual, auditory, tactile,
proprioceptive, gustatory, and olfactory stimuli (Hinder
2004). Hence one of the very many difficulties for the
ASD sufferer can be to simply feel at ease in their own
environment. For such people, the built environment
can become difficult, confusing, and even threatening
(Grandin, 1995; Harker & King, 2002; Williams, 1996).
Adding to this concern is the fact that recent statistics
suggest the incidence of ASD is on the increase and
even growing at alarming levels. The UK National
Autistic Society has put the current incidence of ASD
at around 1 percent of the population (National Autistic
Society, n.d.)(Baird et al, 2006).
For architects and designers, this is indeed a stark reality. The architectural profession has long been entrusted with the duty, responsibility, and privilege to provide
a built environment that will promote well-being, be
inclusive, and enrich life. By contrast, the disorientation
and fear experienced by many ASD sufferers is very far
removed from this ideal and greatly distances them from
the possibility of ever feeling the “pleasure and protection when the body discovers its resonance in space”
(Pallasmaa, 1996, p.67).
Being sensory sensitive, and unable to fully integrate and communicate with others means that the ASD
sufferer can find the world a disorientating and even
frightening place. For the school child, this is especially
damaging. Any unwanted distraction can impact badly
upon that child’s ability to learn. The background and
surrounding environment that most of us are able to
ignore or cope with will actually act as a barrier between child and teacher, further hampering the child’s
development.
Discussion
For children, school has a special and important place
in their formative years. To a child, a school is many
things; not just a place of learning but also a place of
new experiences, a test-bed to develop social skills, and
a supportive environment in which to develop and find
themselves. In all aspects, it should be an environment
in which the child feels comfortable. Accordingly, it is
encouraging that a number of researchers have recently
Refereed Full-Papers
looked specifically at the design criteria of ASDfriendly classrooms (Humpreys, 2005; Vogel, 2008;
Scott, 2009; McAllister, 2010). Measures employed in
maximising the optimum learning environment in the
ASD-friendly classroom include reduction in detail,
flexibility, zoning, and lighting strategies.
However, the classroom is only one component
of the school environment. A school for children is
in many ways a world within a world, a ‘micro-city’
within the city (Hertzberger, 2008, p112). There, assembly hall, playground, music room, principal’s office,
and classroom represent for the child, agora, piazza,
concert hall, town hall, and street, respectively. In this
analogy, the classroom becomes the pupil’s home—a
place of peace, structure, and calm. It is the place of
safety, tranquillity, and respite in the city that is school.
This is especially true for the ASD pupil, where visual
timetables, structured zoning, and teaching strategies
help make them feel ‘more at home’.
The corridor or street, on the other hand, is a place
of constant flux, movement, noise, and change. It is
the place of chance encounter, the impromptu and
unplanned. Like the street in the city, it is a constantly
changing environment, and one where surprise and
delight for some is not the case for others. For the ASD
pupil, the corridor can be a place of anxiety where
they cannot feel comfortable. For the vulnerable, it is
also where discipline problems frequently arise, when
“students change classes and travel from one part of the
school to the other” (Curtis, 2003, p12). The desired opportunity for excitement and exploration where a child
can encounter occasional mystery and drama (Dudek,
2005) can for the ASD pupil, because of their disorder, become a Lewis Carroll Alice in Wonderland-like
environment of confusion and fear. This fear and worry
of street is poignantly illustrated by the ASD artist
Elaine, with her picture entitled A Safe Place 2 (Myscape 2010). Elaine shows herself sitting in the safety
of her bedroom viewing the street through a screen of
vegetation while the front door of the house is clearly
barricaded (Figure 1).
Therefore, the transition to and from both classroom
and corridor to other school venues needs to be carefully considered. If the link from corridor to room is
abrupt, it can unsettle and undermine the effectiveness
of the classroom intervention. Hence threshold becomes
a vital consideration. Moreover, if considered carefully,
the threshold can help limit the difficulties inherent with
transition for the ASD pupil.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
FIGURE 1 - A Safe Place 2, by Elaine
Of course, transition is an integral part of many
schools. This can take many forms. It may be the
age-dependant progression from junior to intermediate and then to senior classrooms. This is the case in
the seminal Hans Scharoun Darmstadt school design of
1951, where children, as they get older, move along the
school, which itself also acts as a transition between the
busy public square at the entrance and parkland setting beyond (Blundell Jones, 1995). Similarly, so too in
the Bolles-Wilson designed kindergarten for Frankfurt
Griesheim in 1991, the form of the school is one that
from the entrance increases in both area and volume—
representing, in built form, the ‘building growing’
alongside the progression and transition of the pupils
during their time at the school (Dudek, 2000, p77).
However, transition for pupils between different
school areas, and specifically between class and corridor, is different. For the ASD pupil, the transition is
not so much from corridor to classroom but from setting
to setting. This is magnified by the settings having such
different characters.
175
Refereed Full-Papers
In the classroom, to help clarify pupil activities, layouts are best designed and ordered into clearly defined
zones. One activity will take place in one zone. The
identification of one activity to one area is an important
one for the ASD sufferer (Mostafa, 2010). Therefore,
the actual threshold or place of transition is for the ASD
pupil an event that needs a specifically considered and
designed place. ASD sufferers themselves have written
about the difficulties encountered when trying to cope
with the sensory overload from the physical environment (Grandin, 1995). In doing so, they relate the coping mechanism of single-thinking on one issue at a time
to help themselves deal with the challenges of sensory
overload. Having a clearly defined threshold dedicated
to transition can help with this strategy.
Yet all too often as architects and designers, we
pay tacit attention to threshold. It can simply be a few
lines drawn on a plan that can represent a door swing,
a change of floor finish, a change of level, pattern, or
spatial zone. The threshold is not genuinely considered as a “place in its own right” (Hertzberger, 2005,
p32). More so, often in school design, the main design
generator can be employing the corridor as street or
central spine, off which the rest of the accommodation
is spatially arranged. Character, quality, and place are
subservient to the order and organisation that the corridor brings to the school design.
In a school setting, most pupils will be able to move
directly along the streetscape of corridor from starting
point to destination without interference. Distraction
may come in temptation to detour, to visit, or to see. Not
so for the ASD sufferer. The distractions along the corridor can bombard the senses. Disoriented and unsettled,
the pupil then enters the classroom. The safety, quiet,
and security felt in the classroom are compromised. In
effect, the corridor and disruption enter the classroom.
Instead, a buffer is required; a threshold where corridor
can be left behind before entering the classroom.
As an architectural device, this is not new. There are
many examples of change between public and private
realms via a semi-private area. The hallway of many
domestic houses works in this way. It is a filter between
the outside world and the private home domain. The
use of church steps or gathering space outside a church
entrance is a similarly a threshold—between the sacred
and profane. At the Siren-designed chapel at Otaniemi
(1957), the gathering space and threshold outside the
entrance is a place in itself, designed to increase the
quality of the experience of those visiting the church
(Architectural Record, 1958).
176 This is a far cry from the reality in many schools,
where a row of coat pegs alongside a door in a corridor
is all that signifies the transitional zone to classroom. It
becomes a place of transition only in that it is the place
where coats, shoes, and schoolbags may be deposited.
This is not only potentially unsettling to ASD pupils,
but also an opportunity lost. The threshold could be so
much more: a place of respite and collection, a place to
reinforce learning, a filter or starting point for those venturing into the bustle of the wider school environment,
and not just a buffer between corridor and classroom.
Case Studies
Many lessons can be learned when evaluating the
effectiveness of intervention and design in existing
schools. At St John’s Primary School in Newry, a former
educational diagnostic base was refurbished as an ASDfriendly classroom four years ago. The diagnostic base
was a collection of rooms where young children would
be educationally assessed. It therefore did not conform
to the rigorous classroom space allocation schedules
prevalent today in school design. Instead, it was a collection of rooms that collectively had a floor area of 92
square meters. With the additional floor area compared
to a typical 60-square-meter classroom, the staff were
able to implement a zoning strategy best suited to their
pupils. Specifically, this included a coat and shoes area
within the classroom—this is carefully considered with
identifiable storage for each pupil. Situated as you come
into the classroom, it is defined by wall and storage
units making it an identifiable place where pupils can
prepare, rest, and ready themselves for the change in
environment and setting when both leaving and entering
the classroom. From there, pupils can either prepare for
leaving the class or alternatively orientate themselves to
their classroom timetable.
Of course, not all classrooms can accommodate an
area within its floor area for the threshold transitional
cloakroom area. If this is the case, then attention needs
to shift to the corridor. In this regard, a lesson can be
learned when looking at the example of Armagh High
School for Boys. It too had an ASD-friendly classroom
implemented four years ago, this time converted from a
general use classroom for boys with ASD between the
ages of twelve and sixteen. The school follows a common typology of post-war schools by having a central
corridor with a series of classrooms accessed directly
off both sides of the corridor.
By chance, the classroom chosen for conversion to
an ASD-friendly classroom was adjacent to two stores
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 2 – Forward Steps Diagnostic Centre, Belfast, Todd Architects (2004)
that were set back from the corridor, thereby forming
a 14-square-meter recess outside the classroom doorway. This fortuitously positioned recess has proved to
be a useful and welcome place for the boys waiting to
enter the classroom. It affords the opportunity to gather
and wait without being in the corridor itself. While
still noisy, there is at least respite from the jostling and
physicality of the main circulation area. This is extremely important for ASD sufferers, many of whom
require greater personal space around themselves in
which to feel comfortable. Here, the recess provides
this and gives an important place of transition between
classroom and corridor. Once again, threshold becomes
an identifiable place. It is an intermediary threshold
to and from the classroom, and one in which the ASD
pupils can start to become part of the corridor culture of
the school.
The concept of making more of the use of threshold
and actually employing it as a design generator is one
that can also be employed to good effect. This has been
the case at the Forward Steps Diagnostic Centre in Belfast (2004) designed by Todd Architects, where the idea
of threshold as place informs the design of the groundfloor layout of the building. The Centre is used for early
intervention in ASD children between the ages of three
and five and also in educating their parents with regard
to the disorder.
Although small in scale, the promenade from exterior
to classroom is one of a series of identifiable places.
This is reinforced by including a series of display opportunities at those places along the circulation route
where children are encouraged to stop and look. Here,
children can orientate themselves and prepare for the
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
forthcoming change in environment. Specifically, views
out are avoided to limit opportunities for visual distraction. Instead, views are curtailed by solidity (Figure 2).
Special consideration has been given to the entrance.
The roof overhang comes down over the entrance, making a covered porch. The slope of the roof also makes
the threshold one of changing scale and a shield against
sunshine. To heighten the feeling of place in which to
gather oneself in, the windows overlooking the approach are purposely downplayed. They are articulated
as a series of vertical slots in the vertical timber cladding. This helps lessen the likelihood of visual distraction for the ASD child. Similarly, the staff office also
does not overlook the entrance. The porch therefore
becomes a self contained element and a genuine place of
transition from exterior and interior. It is a threshold and
buffer to the change between the two.
Then when travelling to the classroom via the reception area, views are ended by communicative visual
pictures that further prepare the child for entry into the
classroom. Ending the transition, each classroom has a
zoned cloakroom area within it. In effect, the promenade
becomes a series of events on a journey—importantly,
a series of events that prepare the pupil for the changes
ahead.
While the three examples highlighted thus far deal
specifically with environments designed solely for
pupils with ASD, the Celbridge North Kildare Educate
Together Primary School (NKET PS) is unusual and
worthy of closer study because it was designed for both
ASD and non-ASD pupils (Betsky et al, 2004) (Architecture Ireland, 2004). Recognizing the need for, but
also the opportunities provided by genuinely engag-
177
Refereed Full-Papers
FIG. 3 – North Kildare Educate Together Primary School, Celbridge, Grafton Architects (2005)
178 Refereed Full-Papers
ing with threshold as place can result in a very fertile
school environment. In this regard, NKET PS, designed
by Grafton Architects, is a triumph that illustrates a
range of rich spatial sequences that can result when including appropriate use of threshold as one of a variety
of strategies in the school design (Figure 3).
Designed as an eight-classroom primary school for
children between the ages of five and eleven, the school
also incorporates an additional two-classroom ASD unit
as an integral part of the school. Designed by architects
who had never designed a special needs school environment, the design team first visited and liaised with
special needs schools and teaching staff. This led to the
realisation that there is a very real need for both quiet
spaces to withdraw into and social spaces in which to
learn how to interact with their peers. The idea of including places for both withdrawal and interaction then
led the architects to look at monasteries as a precedent
typology that might help inform their design work. In
a monastery, the environment is one of solitude in the
case of the monk’s cell, interaction in the case of the
chapel or refectory, and the cloister as intermediary
between both. Whilst the monastery is arguably still a
micro-city in the city, it is one that is better aligned to
the internal environment that suits the characteristics
of the ASD pupil where the cloister takes the place of
street.
The cell, or classroom, was carefully considered and
designed to suit both ASD and non-ASD pupils. One
form is used throughout the school to encourage integration of ASD pupils into the mainstream classroom
when appropriate. All classrooms have a number of
shared components and features, further strengthening
this factor. All have direct access to a garden area, often
used as an external classroom. All classrooms have a
‘den’, a place that can be used by pupils to retire into
if needed or alternatively that can be used as a stage, a
reading room, or for other uses. Situated on the garden
side of the classroom, the dens punctuate the garden
elevations and help articulate the important transition
between classroom and garden.
All classrooms also have toilets within the classroom, and storage and sink areas on the wall facing the
garden. They also all have ‘sun boxes’ in their sloped
ceiling. These help bring reflected sunlight into the
classroom and bring warmth to the quality of light in
the interiors. There are, of course, some slight differences between the ASD and mainstream classrooms—
the ASD classrooms are the only rooms to have smooth
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
plaster walls and underfloor heating to help reduce
the visual distraction that wall-mounted radiators and
exposed blockwork might provide. The entrances to
both types of classroom are also handled differently, but
both carefully consider the importance of threshold in
doing so.
The ASD classrooms are accessed via a room off
the corridor that acts as a cloakroom from which the
children can pass into the classrooms. The cloakroom
therefore acts as threshold and buffer, a place in which
to orientate and prepare for change. It also means that
the cloakroom helps act as a visual and acoustic barrier
between classroom and corridor, lessening the possibility of sensory distraction from the corridor. The classroom therefore can become a “nest” or place of safety
and tranquillity in which the teaching environment is
enhanced.
The mainstream classes are treated differently. All
have a coat and shoe storage area next to the door inside
the classroom. However, this is part of a service zone
consisting of cloakroom area, sink, storage, and toilets
which the pupils pass through to get to the teaching
spaces beyond. This is the threshold and intermediary
zone between classroom and corridor. Even with the
inclusion of a display window beside the door of each
mainstream classroom, there is an identifiable threshold
between corridor and teaching area for the pupils. The
pupils know where teaching takes place and respect this
division.
This sensitivity towards the use of threshold is also
used elsewhere in the school. Moving the cloakrooms
into the classrooms allows the corridors to be treated
more like the cloisters of monasteries. This is especially
the case where corridors pass by the exterior and main
sensory garden at the heart of the school. The corridors
themselves become a quiet intermediary zone between
interior and garden. Here, pupils can sit on the windowsills and talk, see artwork on the wall, and access the
garden. The corridors are given different qualities and
characteristics; some are open to the exterior, others
are quieter and more still with changes in fenestration
and materiality. Communal spaces overlap and interlink
with one another; for instance between the ASD pupils’
play area and the sensory garden. Importantly, this
gives choice for the ASD pupil and the chance to decide
in which area they feel more comfortable. They are, in
effect, opportunities to collect oneself and then choose.
179
Refereed Full-Papers
Conclusion
By recognising that ASD sufferers often feel more
comfortable in a place they associate with one activity,
it is of benefit to offer them a place in which to decide
and prepare for change in environments throughout the
school. This can help in promoting integration between
ASD and non-ASD pupils. As ASD pupils commonly
suffer with difficulties in social interaction, increased
exposure to mainstream school life can be of great benefit. Where this occurs, the school becomes a genuine
teaching and informative teaching place for many of the
skills for later life.
This is not to preclude the increasing use of corridor
outside the classroom as an extension of the classroom.
This is increasingly common, with corridors used as
makeshift teaching areas or shared resource areas in
which independent learning or small-group teaching out
of the classroom can take place. But if this is the case,
the threshold between classroom and corridor once
again needs to be given due consideration. Treating
the threshold as a specific place in which an important
event occurs is the key and one that can and arguably
should be used throughout the school environment.
Considering the threshold as a place for the event of
transition is not limiting, as it opens up the possibility
of choice and opportunity to both ASD and non-ASD
pupils. This can be the buffer between different activities, such as classroom and corridor, or a place in which
to decide where pupils want to go in the school. In
both cases, the well-considered and designed threshold provides the opportunity for schoolchildren to feel
comfortable—surely no bad thing.
180 References
Architecture Ireland (2004) NKET School, Celbridge,
Co.Kildare, 201, Oct 2004, 23-26.
Architectural Record (1958), December 1958, 142-147
Baird et al. (2006). Lancet. July 15; 366 (9531), 179-181.
Betsky et al. (2004).New Irish Architecture 19. AAI Awards
2004. Gandon Editions: Dublin
Blundell Jones, P. (1995) Hand Scharoun The Alternative
Tradition Ten Projects. Sheffield: A3 Times.
Curtis, E. (2003) School Builders. London: John Wiley & Sons.
Dudek, M. (2000) Architecture of Schools. Architectural Press,
Oxford.
Dudek, M. (2005) Children’s Spaces. Architectural Press, Oxford.
Harker, M. & King, N. (2002). Designing for Special Needs.
London: RIBA Press.
Hinder, S. (2004) keynote address to Good Autism Practice
Conference. Oxford 19-04-2004.
Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in Pictures. New York: Vintage.
Hertzberger, H. (2005). Lessons for Students in Architecture. (5th
ed rev.) Rotterdam: 010 Publishers.
Hertzberger, H. (2008). Space and Learning. Rotterdam: 010
Publishers.
Humpreys, S. (2005, Feb/March). Autism & Architecture. Autism
London Bulletin. 7-8.
Myscape (2010) A Journey Into The Architectural Landscape,
Exhibition Catalogue. 26.1-20.2.2010, Glasgow: Project
Ability
McAllister, K. (2010) The ASD Friendly Classroom – Design
Complexity, Challenge and Characteristics. DRS2010
Conference, Montreal, Canada 07-09 July 2010.
Mostafa, M. (2010) Housing Adaption For Adults with Autistic
Spectrum Disorder. Open House International Vol 35, No.1,
March 2010, 37-48.
National Autistic Society (n.d.) Retrieved on 21-09-10 from
http://www.autism.org.uk/about-autism/some-facts-andstatistics/statistics-how-many-people-have-spectrumdisorders.aspx
Pallasmaa, J. (1996). The Eyes of The Skin: Architecture and the
Senses. (2nd ed). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Scott, I. (2009) ‘Designing learning spaces for children on the
autistic spectrum.’ Good Autism Practice. 10(1), 36-59
Vogel, C. (2008) Classroom Design for Living & Learning with
Autism. Autism Aspergers Digest, May-June. Retrieved
01-07-09 from http://www.designshare.com/index.php/
archives/901
Williams, D. (1996). Autism – An Insideout Approach. London:
Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Wing, L. & Gould,J. (1979). Severe impairments of social
interaction and associated abnormalities in children:
epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 9, 11-19.
Refereed Full-Papers
A Systemic Theory for Historic
Preservation: Land Use
Management Strategies as
Preservation Policy
Galen Newman (Clemson University)
Abstract:
Historic preservation efforts need to move beyond
saving single objects of historical or aesthetic significance to the broader context of rural and urban planning. Rapid development within civic hinterlands
throughout America has helped to facilitate the relocation of both populations and land uses, leaving rural and
urban core areas replete with functionless, unused, and
unmaintained heritage structures. This rapidly expanding, decentralized growth is spreading a homogenous
form across the landscape and destroying multiple
layers of cultural history in its wake. Many once-vital
structures have been removed, while others have been
abandoned and left to decay—a process known as demolition by neglect. While historic preservation efforts
have enacted policies to salvage these structures, these
efforts have initially focused on the preservation of
each building individually, based on its historical value
and architectural merit, not taking into account its role
in a constantly changing contextual landscape. Rather
than treating the sources of this neglect, the current
movement in historic preservation seeks to preserve
entire municipalities, in effect freezing them in time.
Attempts to counteract this process and the negative
effects of fringe developments through land preservation have gained momentum since the 1970s. However,
contemporary policies enacted by preservationists
only micromanage this epidemic internally through the
utility of floating zones such as historic districts. New
methods need to be explored to avoid the rigidity of applying locality-sized zones in an effort to retain historic
structures. In response to growing concerns about the
climbing rate of neglected historic structures, utilizing
a historical-interpretive method, this research traces the
epistemological evolution of cultural landscape theory
employed by both historic preservationists and cultural
geographers (two disciplines which stemmed from identical origins), critically explores the fallacies involved
with preservation theory and preservation policy, and
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
suggests a new paradigm, a systems approach, which
will expand the scale in which historic preservation
policy should be applied. Because a primary driving
force of demolition by neglect lies in the swiftly developing outskirts of our cities, this research suggests that,
through new systemic management of historic structures, external land use management strategies could
be employed as part of historic preservation policy
schemas. This new approach would then shift the scope
of preservation theory and move the discipline beyond
traditional preservation ordinances, transforming the
theoretical underpinnings from positing landscape as a
product to viewing it as a process.
Introduction
The predominant tenet for the management of cultural resources in the United States involves the process
of documenting and either continuation or reuse of
existing structures and sites, known broadly as historic
preservation. The enactment of the National Historic
Preservation Act in 1966 emphasized, primarily, the
cultural and aesthetic values of these structures. That
rationale carries an inherent weakness: the resulting
plans are based on a philosophy that values architectural merit and historic integrity over utility and viability.
The current range of policies to help manage historic
buildings or historic districts still only attempts to deal
with the problem within these small-scale contexts—
i.e., by saving each individual building one at a time.
Rather than examining external (i.e., related to broader
regional development planning) approaches to help
manage this dilemma, the current movement in historic
preservation simply shifts preservation efforts to a
larger scale through the preservation of entire municipalities. Those who were once satisfied with individual buildings and districts now speak of preserving
entire landscapes (Francavigilia, 2000). Large-scale,
community-wide preservation has been highly criticized and does not address the cultural landscape as an
entity situated within a constantly changing, interrelated
system of causal mechanisms. The vitality and viability of these structures is equally important to allow
them to remain functional in an active and engaging
environment. Rather than expand the scale of historic
preservation to include entire localities rather than a
collection of historic districts which has been highly
criticized, it could be possible to incorporate different
land use management strategies as a part of an overall
preservation policy scheme.
181
Refereed Full-Papers
The Costs of Sprawl
The historic resources of our American cities are
disappearing at an accelerating rate. Our landscape
may be the richest historical record we possess, but
many of the remaining fragments of our past ways of
life are threatened by removal due to decay and neglect.
As vitality (people) and viability (function) flee to the
peripheries of our towns, the result is a propensity to
remove heritage structures that have deteriorated due
to a lack of utility. A five-year study conducted by the
Transportation Research Board and the National Research Council on the impact of sprawl (Transportation
Research Board, 2002) identifies urban decline and land
conversion as two of the primary, interrelated consequences of the sprawling American landscape. Because
major concentrations of historic structures are located
in urbanized centers, these two outcomes must be addressed simultaneously in order to reverse the process.
Historic preservation efforts have attempted to reverse
the effects of decentralization and counter-urbanization
by enacting various local preservation ordinances.
These include financial incentives, interim zoning controls, design review strategies, transfers of development
rights, incentive zoning, and the application of floating zones such as historic districts (Collins, Waters, &
Dotson, 1991). Unfortunately, because these strategies
are internally oriented and do not address contextual
change, they seem to be only delaying the inevitable
demise of historic structures (Jigyasau, 2003).
Parallel to the aforementioned dilemma in the city
core, attempts to preserve peripheral areas, thereby
slowing decentralization and sprawl and helping to
sustain inner cities and towns, have become increasingly popular. However, there is currently little understanding of the effect of these land use management
strategies on cultural landscapes and, in particular,
whether these efforts have helped to slow the decay of
existing historic structures to the point where demolition is cheaper than rehabilitation, a process referred to
as “demolition by neglect.”
Demolition by neglect is defined as the destruction
of a heritage landscape or area through abandonment or
lack of maintenance (Goldwyn, 1995). This epidemic is
a recognized challenge both globally and nationally, as
the number of demolition applications being submitted
continues to rise (Wallace & Franchetti, 2007). Goldwyn (1995) noted its severity 15 years ago:
“The State Preservation League of New York held
a conference on DBN in 1993, the National Trust
182 for Historic Preservation included this as a topic
for a panel discussion and presentation at the 1994
national convention in Boston, MA, and the United
States Preservation Commission Identification
Project report, released in 1994, listed it as the most
difficult situation for local commissions to solve,
with only 25% of respondents reporting that they
have the authority to protect designated structures
from demolition by neglect.”
Decay of a structure is inevitable; it is the expression of its duration through time. This decay can
actually add character to structures and can even be a
creative intent of a designer. Demolition by neglect, on
the other hand, occurs when an owner lets a building
deteriorate until it becomes a structural hazard and then
turns around and asserts the building’s advanced state of
deterioration is the primary reason to remove it. Causes
include deferred maintenance, developmental pursuits,
absentee ownership, circumstantial outcome, and function relocation (Wallace & Franchetti, 2007).
The process of demolition by neglect directly contradicts the traditional philosophy of historic preservation
in America (Goldwyn, 1995). For example, preservation policy places a value on factors long considered
intangible, such as architectural merit or societal
importance. Preservation philosophy demands that
property owners recognize and accept this value. Value,
however, is contingent upon interpretation and the value
that preservationists place on a structure is not always
the same as the value that the owner may have for it.
Despite substantial restrictions on the demolition of
historic buildings imposed by local historic preservation
ordinances, many historic properties are destroyed each
year as a result of conscious efforts by their owners
to avoid the application of these restrictions (Pollard,
1989).
The Theoretical Underpinnings of Cultural
Landscape Studies
There are two different approaches to protecting the
cultural landscape. Although both cultural geography
and historic preservation stemmed from Carl Sauer’s
coining of the term “cultural landscape,” the theoretical
evolution of each discipline is severely dichotomized
with one field interpreting landscape as a system (cultural geography) and the other interpreting landscape
as a singular entity (historic preservation). The field of
cultural geography interprets the cultural landscape as
a dynamic system, constantly in flux and continually
Refereed Full-Papers
altering. Preservationists, however, view the cultural
landscape as purely material and hold steadfast to its
earliest definition put forth by cultural geographer
Carl Sauer. Whereas cultural geographers utilize more
recent productions of geographic thought, these discussions have not penetrated the realm of historic preservationists.
In a 1925 essay entitled “The Morphology of Landscape,” Sauer set forth his definition. “The cultural
landscape is fashioned from the natural landscape by a
cultural group. Culture is the agent; the natural area is
the medium, the cultural landscape the result.” (Wilson
& Groth, 2003) Sauer believed that interaction between
nature and culture created materialized pockets that
were more culturally influenced than naturally based.
Dissatisfied with the efforts toward environmental
determinism and the view that the physical environment rather than social conditions determined cultural
outcomes, Sauer believed that the human impact on
landscape played a larger role in cultural outcomes than
did the physical environment. This impact, according to Sauer, then revealed itself within the image of
landscape through what is known as material culture, a
stable form based on cultural interaction which could
be observed and interpreted. The term material culture
was coined to label those parts of the physical environment which had been transformed from the natural
state by human action for human purposes (Kyvig and
Myron 2000).
While Sauer’s approach dominated cultural geography through the first half of the twentieth century,
other scholars began to alter and expand his theoretical
positions. Fred Kniffen, a student of Sauer’s, believed
that there were similarities in this material culture and
these similarities were geographically clustered. He
called these collections “cultural strains” (Riesenweber, 2008). While Sauer sought to define individual
natural and cultural forms, Kniffen concentrated purely
on cultural relationships and sought to delimit cultural
regions through revealing their relationships with one
another. Although the goal of these cultural strains
was to identify specific boundaries for in which similar
material culture was present, Kniffen never achieved his
goal due to immense overlaps in cultural characteristics
which were difficult to decipher.
Geographers such as D.W. Meinig and Allen Noble
utilized Kniffen’s theory in attempts to generate what
they called “cultural hearths.” These hearths were
another effort to delimit the boundaries of different collections of material culture. Similar to cultural strains
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
but different in that they spatially located specific
similar cultural forms into geographical boundaries
on a regional scale rather than just a municipal scale,
utilizing building material and typology as dominant
characteristics. Cultural hearths illustrated original
source areas with distinctive settlement forms, as well
as other cultural attributes, from which certain clearly
identifiable elements were carried to other parts of the
continent (Noble, 1984).
One of Kniffen’s disciples, Henry Glassie, took this
concept even further. Whereas Kniffen and Sauer were
primarily concerned with the more the permanent fixtures upon the landscape and enduring material culture,
Glassie introduced the concept of material folk culture.
He mapped municipalities based on the distribution of
these more localized cultural forms, which split material
culture into typologies. Glassie also believed that there
was a discrepancy in cultural landscape studies and that
they were too reliant upon historical change rather than
spatial variation. He stated that “material folk culture
exhibited major variation over space and minor variation over time” and that “students of folk culture should
listen more closely to the cultural geographer than the
historian to establish cultural regions (Riesenweber,
2008).” Hence, from Sauer to Kniffen, the boundary of
the cultural landscape was growing larger and larger
(from specific material culture to regional sized pockets
of material patterns) as well as encompassing more and
more components (enduring landmarks and material
folk culture).
A synthesis of all of these approaches would inevitably alter the field of cultural landscape studies. John
Brinkerhoff Jackson, a landscape architect, would
legitimize the field more so than any other practitioner
by creating a framework which combined the existing
theories, interpreting landscape as a complete multiscaled entity with multiple individual components. It
was Jackson, also, that made the concept of cultural
landscapes a familiar one with the design professions
at the University of Berkeley in 1957 (Riesenweber,
2008). He had an interdisciplinary approach to landscape which created an alliance between the design
fields and historic preservation. This merging of disciplines would eventually lead to the consideration of historic districts, entire communities, and rural landscapes
as cultural landscapes that merited preservation due to
their distinguishing characteristics.
Jackson’s theoretical framework for the cultural landscape was based on two dimensions: the establishment
and the vernacular. The establishment would serve
183
Refereed Full-Papers
as the stage in which vernacular stage props would
constantly be removed and recreated as scenes changed.
The establishment, according to Jackson, was made up
of the relatively permanent fixtures upon the landscape
and the consistent fabric which was well thought out in
regards to the future and promoted order, architecture,
law, and property. Establishmentarian elements of the
landscape described rigid spaces within the landscape
which create stability, uniformity, and sequence among
the other chaotic actions which constantly occur upon
it. Political boundaries, national, state, and local laws,
large scale architectural structures, and primary arterial roadways are good examples of what he labeled as
components of the establishment.
The vernacular, on the other hand, consisted of the
more dynamic and ephemeral elements of landscape,
including but not limited to the material folk culture
investigated by Kniffen. Vernacular spaces described
those spaces which constantly alter as cultural preferences change. They are the ordinary, common, typical,
and everyday elements of landscape. These elements
of landscape are temporary elements which are built by
necessity and dependent upon the materials with which
they are constructed. According to Jackson (1984),
“The beauty that we see in the vernacular landscape is
the image or our common humanity: hard work, stubborn hope, and mutual forbearance striving to be love.”
In Jackson’s viewpoint, the human impact on landscape
had become so widespread that delimiting boundaries
based on cultural characteristics had become nearly
impossible. What makes this framework important is
not only that it combined each of the prior theories, but
is also initiated the idea that there were portions of the
landscape that were unstable and constantly in flux.
Although Jackson was a landscape architect, the
Berkeley School of Cultural Geography was still
continuing the expansion of the theoretical scope of
cultural landscape studies. Pierce Lewis (1979), a
cultural geographer, took Jackson’s premise of the
cultural landscape as a two-tiered network of materialized culture unable to be categorized by distinguishable
boundaries, and promoted the idea that all landscapes
had cultural meaning. This theory signified the idea
that all landscapes were in fact cultural at least to some
degree and put forth the notion that the human impact
on the natural landscape was omnipresent. However,
the cultural imprint on the natural landscape, according to Lewis, was also a direct reflection of human
desires. He believed that the human interaction with
184 nature left an indelible mark of the environment and
these imprints were inscribed within the landscape and
could be read like a text through deep interpretation.
“If we want to understand ourselves, we would do well
to take a searching look at landscapes. The human
landscape is an appropriate source of self knowledge
because it is our unwitting autobiography, reflecting our
tastes, our values, our aspirations, and even our fears.”
(Lewis, 1979 p.12) Other, more current, theorists such
as architectural historian Spiro Kostoff and Anne Spirn
have also accepted this position. While Spirn’s The
Language of Landscape (2000) presents a theoretical
methodology in which to interpret this text, Kostoff still
concentrates on the historical aspects of the cultural
landscape as a means for contemporary interpretation.
His statement, “We are, indeed, what we have built,”
embodies both Lewis’ and Sauer’s theoretical positions (Kostoff, 1991). Whereas the cultural landscape
was originally viewed through a historicist lens, Lewis’
more post-structurally oriented view assumed that the
image of the landscape was both a direct reflection of
and a collective depository of our cultural wants and
needs.
The problem involved with every landscape now
being viewed as cultural was that if all landscapes were
cultural landscapes, then there could be no such thing as
either a cultural or natural landscape; they must then be
interactive and one in the same. Culture, like nature, is
a reactive and dynamic phenomenon. James and Nancy
Duncan, in 1980, posited that culture was “superorganic” and even questioned its manifestation as a materialized entity (Riesenweber, 2008). This theory interpreted landscape as a superorganism comprised of multiple
interactive organisms. These organisms unremittingly
interacted with one another and altered the visible image of the landscape. Cultural forms were superior to
individual interactions, and were dependent upon entire
societal values. This marked the point where the (cultural) landscape began to become viewed as a system,
presupposing that existing “cultural manifestations”
were so rapidly changing and unstable that they were
actually unattainable and therefore uncategorizable.
Other scholars such as Denis Cosgrove supported
the Duncans’ criticisms but proposed that within these
societal systems, there must also be a physical system
which it interacts with, and that due to the superorganic nature, individual mental process also had a role
in the generation of cultural form. Cosgrove posited
that perhaps landscape was as much an idea as it was
a tract of land. In other words, the superorganic social
Refereed Full-Papers
aspects of landscapes which composed the material
realm were also reliant upon the imaginative processes
emergent from interaction within a specific space at a
specific time. This concept put forth the notion that we
shape the landscape and it, in turn, shapes us. Cosgrove
believed that there was a simultaneous shaping of both
space and inhabitants. Landscape, as idea, meant that
landscape was constructive, not reproductive.
There then had to be a symbolic undertone to the
built environment, which also played a role in this
constant making and remaking of landscapes. These
inquiries led to questioning about the entire theoretical framework for cultural landscape studies. Because
multiple interactions now composed a landscape which
with both tangible and intangible dimensions, observational methods alone proved to be insufficient means of
landscape interpretation. Instead, there was a push to
surface the symbolic dimensions of cultural forms and
the meaning invested in these forms by those who have
produced and sustained them (Riesenweber, 2008). The
unquantifiable, systematic (cultural) landscape now had
both a material and an immaterial character.
Successive theorists began to consider landscape
meaning to be fluid and shaped by the circumstances
and social positions of its occupants and presumed
that simultaneous understandings of landscapes must,
therefore, also exist. Landscape presents itself as stable
but is constantly in flux; it is both manipulated and
manipulable. The (cultural) landscape was now viewed
as a concretized version of commonly held interpretations and transformations of the changing ideologies
involved within shared realities. Stephen Daniels calls
this transformative concretization the “duplicity of landscape” (Daniels, 1989). In Daniel’s view, landscape is
discourse, a framework in which all interactions and
practices are negotiated. This discourse is then constantly be re-materialized and must be unraveled like a
tapestry.
Inversely, historic preservationists still utilize the
original concept of the cultural landscape that originated in the 1920s from Carl Sauer. Accepting the
National Park Service’s formal definition of the cultural landscape as “a geographic area associated with
a historic event, activity, or person or exhibiting other
cultural or aesthetic values” (Alanen & Melnick, 2000),
preservationists still seek to retain material remnants of
the past and concentrate on the appearance of material
culture. This premise still views the cultural landscape
as a stable product. By concentrating on historical significance and integrity as cornerstones for designation,
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
preservationists can sometimes ignore the larger context
which this material culture resides within and more
importantly, the impact of changes within this context.
Edge cities, strip malls, and suburban development
pull the population and building functions away from
historic buildings, generally resulting in demolition of
these structures either by removal, neglect, or a combination of both. As long as the fringe areas continue to
welcome corporate office parks, along with the residential and commercial development they generate, our
(historic) inner cities will continue to rot at their cores
(Daniels, 1999).
The Fallacies Involved with Contemporary
Preservation Philosophy
Despite the attempts at conserving historic structures through policy implementation, the philosophical
foundations of only promoting aesthetics and historical
relevance can sometimes hinder preservationist goals.
Historic preservation of aged structures prioritizes form
to the extent that it can sometimes overlook the need for
a structure to remain functional in its context. Goldwyn
(1995) calls this epidemic a “loophole” in preservation
tactics. This loophole in the preservation system creates a situation where historic structures are preserved,
but not utilized, thus resulting in their ultimate demolition due to neglect. Our tendency to destroy old forms
justifies the preservationist movement to an extent in
the sense that these forms are unique and cannot be
recreated. However, since landscape is a continuum,
our towns must also be flexible enough to absorb some
modernization to cater to the inherent populace and
functional economy.
Structural preservation on a local scale can sometimes sever the civic chord of progression, resulting in
museumized relics and artifacts within the landscape.
New construction within historic areas has received
much debate. However, historic context is not limited
to only neighboring sites. Development patterns within
the peripheries of our cities and towns can also have a
huge impact on their inner cores. The ability of preservation standards to support both viability of sites and
historic character increasingly depends on effective processes for examining changes within the larger town or
urban context (Alderson, 2006). But, because context
is constantly in flux, form and function rarely coincide
for very long in any environment (Jackson, 1997); our
contemporary tendency is to give priority to form. The
flaw in this premise is that when building function is
dissolved, too often building form, itself, is simply
185
Refereed Full-Papers
removed. The primary concern for promoting the management of historic core areas has been, in particular, to
develop strategies to restore the original rationality for
their existence. The viability of these historic structures
must also be incorporated into preservation philosophy,
because the lack of the ability to attract investment is
what is causing their removal, not their lack of integrity.
As a result of these conditions, there are four fallacies involved in the theoretical foundations of historic
preservation. First, there is an incompatibility between
preserved landscapes and landscapes allowed to continue the process of change. The rift created between
a significant decrease in the transformative process of
preserved landscapes and those allowed to progress rapidly creates a lopsided appreciation of landscape, which
devalues those portions of landscape which are not in
preservation programs while emphasizing other human
artifacts on display for enjoyment. This incompatibility
places simultaneously places a higher value on those
structures enrolled within preservation programs while
devaluing other portions of the landscape. The various fortunes each historic structure came to know and
the altercations which took place upon the landscape
created the very characteristics which call for is preservation. Severing this chord of progression not only
discontinues the very process which created its distinctiveness, but the overuse of this technique will generate
the same outcome.
Second, historic preservation, in its purest form, is
virtually impossible, and the current effort to preserve
entire localities only amplifies this impossibility. It is a
physical impossibility to completely stop the transformation of a landscape. Human interaction with heritage
sites unavoidably alters both their nature and context
(Lowenthal, 1985). Since the passing of the National
Historic Preservation Act of 1966, the pace of historic
preservation activities has vastly increased (Longstreith, 2008). But because historic preservation has
been accused of “freezing landscapes in time” (Cook,
1996), it is not always the appropriate tool to achieve
such a large goal, especially on such a dramatic scale.
The preservation of entire cities makes them no longer
relevant to their occupying culture, but simply historic
remnants of a population whose time has passed. In
this sense, these cultural landscapes will be no longer
cultural, only culturally historic. The restrictive laws
and guidelines imposed by historic preservation regulations can place too much strain on large-scale landscapes, which need some modern amenities to serve
society’s current needs.
186 Third, the over-use of preservation, if used on a
municipal scale, of historic landscapes will devalue
the very objects which are attempted to be appreciated.
A large scale approach to historic preservation will
eventually result in huge pockets of historic buildings
everywhere, the very pattern that preservationists are
fighting against. An overabundance of preserved landscapes will create a situation where those landscapes
which are not preserved will become unique. Landscapes are different places based in part on their ability
to change (Melnick, 2000). The attempt at eliminating
this change will eventually lead to stale, ubiquitous
landscapes. The irony in this statement is that historic
sites are preserved, mostly, due to their character and
ability to stand out from the repetitious stamped-out
developments which currently characterize the American landscape.
Finally, the pluralism of landscape history is impossible to untangle. Attempting to disentangle the
multiple histories which exist on each site inevitably
leads to the appreciation of some aspects of cultural
change and the depreciation of others (Clay, 1976).
This has created an aperture in the acknowledgement of
the differing ethic histories in a multicultural civilization. Many Native American histories are only cloaked
in the guise of place naming while other histories
such as African American and Hispanic histories are
in many cases, pushed to the background and forgotten in remembrance of a history which relates better to
population majorities. Also, communication about the
past is inextricably intertwined with communication
about particular places. This communication is thus
dependent upon the values and historical associations
we place upon the landscape. These values can taint
the historical references we sometimes refer to in an
effort to decipher the past. Lowenthal (1998) states that
“history is told by the victors,” so the history we choose
to preserve may not be entirely accurate. In this sense,
attempts at disentangling these histories may be in vain
because they are not fully precise. Treasuring heritage
as authentic history can blind us to our own legacy’s biased limits. Moreover, these histories are not limited to
specific parcels of land. The built environment does not
exist in a vacuum, it is surrounded by a complex system
of elements which support it and form its setting, and
the setting which encompasses our historic structures
not only tells a portion of their story, but also forms the
context which dictates their function.
Refereed Full-Papers
A New Paradigm: The Cultural Landscape as a
System
The field of cultural geography has changed the
meaning of landscape from a noun to a verb (Riesenweber, 2008). The epistemological evolution of cultural
landscape studies changed its interpretation from a
being viewed as an artifact to a being seen as a progression. The problem lies not within specific buildings and
infrastructure, where historic preservationists attempt
to address the quandary, but in the swiftly developing
outskirts of our cities to which the structures are inescapably connected. This rapidly expanding, decentralized growth is spreading a homogenous form across
the landscape and destroying multiple layers of cultural
history in its wake. As long as the fringe areas continue
to welcome corporate office parks, along with the residential and commercial development they generate, our
inner cities will continue to rot at their cores (Daniels,
1999). The protection of the cultural landscape is therefore not only a product, but a process. Preservationists
must then begin to value both viability and aesthetics.
“Failing to plan for and manage growth, or
leaving the fate of cities to the random collision of
economic forces, is likely to result in the destruction
of our historic places and in diminished cities…..We
must look beyond traditional preservation ordinances and landmark commissions to address those
planning forces that have the most influence over
their city’s future development.” (Collins, Waters, &
Dotson, 1991 p.8).
Stated in the original guidelines of the National
Trust’s Critical Issues Fund (CIF) in 1981 is for historic
preservation to “play a responsible role in the processes
that will decide the future for historic properties” (Collins, Waters, & Dotson, 1991). The CIF has actually
begun to encourage efforts to weave preservation values
in land use management and zoning policy in order to
limit demolition of these structures. Hence, the current
efforts involved in the historic preservation of cultural
landscapes call for a new paradigm which will expand
the magnitude of the population which it attempts to
serve. Preservation efforts need to move beyond saving
single objects of historical or aesthetic significance to
the broader context of rural and urban planning (Cook,
1996). A systems approach to landscape preservation
will require not only the recognition that the cultural
landscape is only one part of a living environment, but
also assessment of the political, economic, and natural
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
process that shape it. We must then view the entire
landscape as a collection of links within an entire dynamic network of interacting processes.
Attempts to counteract the process and the negative
effects of fringe developments through land preservation have gained momentum since the 1970s. The
techniques of land use management strategies as a
means to limit the effects of suburbanization has also
broadened since this time through the implementation
of agricultural preserves, wildlife preserves, conservation easements, and recreational networks carving out
non-developable spaces in civic hinterlands. Utilizing
these techniques as a portion of and in combination
with existing preservation policies then treats the cause
of demolition by neglect at its source, rather than attempting to deal with circumstances as they inevitably
arise. However, this systems approach will require
national, state, and local input.
This expansion then begins to simultaneously address
many of the aforementioned fallacies involved with
current preservation philosophy. First, it will help to
limit the effect of urbanization on historic structures
by allowing for more external measures and peripheral
land use management techniques to be included as portions of local and regional preservation schemes. As
Jigyasau (2005) has explained, historic sites have two
fundamental dimensions: the first deals with aspects of
integrity, and the second deals with their relationship to
the living environment in which they exist. Most risk
factors threatening living heritage sites are progressive,
making these historic treasures increasingly vulnerable to the current trend of growth in the United States.
These risks extend into all portions of the landscape,
but they are especially present in areas where development pressures and urbanization have occurred.
These historic areas have not received the attention
and support they deserve to maintain their viability,
protect their structural integrity and heritage value, and
stimulate their local economic base as their populations
undergo various incremental processes of transformation (Jigyasau, 2005).
The use of growth limiting strategies as preservation
policy also addresses the pluralism of historic narratives
embedded within preserved structures by allowing for
the context to also be preserved. The built environment does not exist in a vacuum; it is surrounded by a
complex system of elements that support it and form
its setting. Because the setting that encompasses our
historic structures not only tells a portion of their story,
187
Refereed Full-Papers
but also forms the context that dictates their function,
land preservation as a growth management strategy
can actually add to the historic character of localities and historic sites through telling portions of their
historic narrative. Also, utilizing land use management
strategies as preservation policy helps to sidestep the
widespread criticism that historic preservation freezes
landscapes in time and provides a structure that allows
for more flexible landscapes which can cater to more
modern technological alterations. New disciples of
cultural landscape preservation, by virtue of this dual
nature, must then also mediate two forces: the accommodation of the values of a larger society and the ability
to retain historic structures despite these accommodations. These systems will then continually acquire new
significance which can inform the present (Cook, 1996).
This will create a heritage with which we continually
interact. Better management of this entire system,
along with the current small-scale policies, is the only
way to prevent the removal of historic landscapes while
still allowing for growth within a living environment.
Literature Cited
Alanan, A. & R. Melnick (2000). Preserving cultural landscapes
in America. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Alderson, C. R. (2006). Responding to context: Changing
perspectives on appropriate change in historic settings. APT
Bulletin, 37(4): 22-33.
Collins, R. C., Waters, E. B., & Dotson, A. B. (1991). America’s
downtowns: Growth, politics, and preservation. Washington,
DC: The Preservation Press.
Cook, R. (1996). Is landscape preservation an oxymoron? The
George Wright Forum,
13(1): 42-53.
Clay, G. (1976). Whose time is this place? The emerging science
of garden restoration. Landscape Architecture Quarterly,
66(3): 217-218.
Daniels, S. (1989). Marxism, culture, and the duplicity of
landscape,” New Models in Geography, ed. Richard P. and
Nigel T. Winchester, Mass.: Unwin-Hyman, p.196-220.
Daniels, T. (1999). When city and country collide: Managing
growth in the metropolitan fringe. Washington, DC: Island
Press.
Francaviglia, R. (2000). Selling Heritage Landscapes. In Alanen,
A. & Melnick, R. (Eds.), Preserving cultural landscapes in
America (pp. 44-69). Baltimore: John Hopkins University
Press.
Goldwyn, A. (1995). Demolition by neglect: A loophole
in preservation policy. Philadelphia, PA: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
188 Jackson, J.B. (1984). Landscape in Sight: Looking at America.
Helen Lefkowitz Horowitz (Ed). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Jackson, J. B. (1997). The future of the vernacular. In Groth,
P. (Ed.), Understanding ordinary landscapes (pp. 145-156).
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Jigyasau, R. (2002). Monuments and sites in their setting:
Conserving cultural heritage in changing townscapes and
landscapes. India: ICCROM.
Kostoff, S. (1991). The city shaped. New York: Bullfinch Press.
Kyvig, D. E., & Myron, A. M. (2000). Nearby history: Exploring
the past around you. New York: Altamira Press.
Lewis, P. (1979). Axioms for reading the landscape: Some
guides to the American scene. The Interpretation of Ordinary
Landscapes. Oxford University Press, p.11-32.
Longstreith, R. (2008). Cultural landscapes: Balancing nature
and heritage in preservation practice. Minneapolis, MN:
University of Minnesota Press
Lowenthal, D. (1985). Age and artifact: Dilemmas of
appreciation. In Meinig, D. W. (Ed.), The interpretation
of ordinary landscapes (pp. 103-128). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Melnick, R. (2000). Considering nature and culture in historic
landscape preservation. In Alanan, A., & Melnick, R. (Eds.),
Preserving cultural landscapes in America (pp. 22-43).
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Noble, A. (1984). Wood, brick, and stone: The North American
settlement landscape. Amherst, MA: University of
Massachusetts Press.
Pollard, D. (1989). Empirical processes. Statist, Sci.(4), 341-346
Riesenweber, J. (2008). Landscape preservation and cultural
geography. In Longstreith, R. (Ed.), Cultural landscapes:
Balancing nature and heritage in preservation practice.
Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press, 23-35.
Spirn, A. (1998). The Language of Landscape. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Transportation Research Board, National Research Council.
(2002). The costs of sprawl. Transit Cooperative Research
Program at Rutgers University. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
Wallace, P. & Franchetti, A. (2007). Heritage at risk: Addressing
the issue of the demolition by neglect of historic heritage in
New Zealand. (NZHPT) New Zealand Historic Places Trust,
Wellington: Sustainable management of Historic Heritage,
Discussion paper No. 6, August.
Wilson, C, & Groth, P. (2003). Everyday America: Cultural
landscape studies after J.B. Jackson. Berkley, CA:
University of California Press.
Refereed Full-Papers
Valuation of Cultural Heritage:
Toward a Conceptual Model
and Potential Evaluation
Strategies
Zena O’Connor (The University of Sydney,
Australia)
Abstract
Places of cultural significance bring a range of benefits for the wider community, as well as shared responsibilities and opportunities for public participation. As
such, places of cultural significance represent a community’s cultural capital. However, can cultural capital
be measured or quantified? The benefits of applying
a valuation process to cultural heritage is an important
step beyond identifying places of cultural significance,
as it provides a basis for decision-making in regard to
short and long term conservation and maintenance strategies. To arrive at a point where valuation may proceed
requires identifying relevant value factors followed by
a determination of the means by which to assess and
measure these value factors in order to achieve valuation and comparative examination. However, the valuation of cultural heritage may also be influenced by the
impact of time as well as contextual and sustainability
issues; and the process of valuation may also be complicated by a diversity of culturally based values and
competing intentions. Given the complexity of these
issues, it is important that whatever valuation process is
devised, it must be fair, balanced, and transparent. This
paper proposes a conceptual model for valuation of cultural heritage as a starting point for further discussion.
In addition, a range of potential evaluation strategies
including assessment and measurement methods are
proposed, along with recommended group participants
and likely outcomes.
Identifying Items of Cultural Significance
Prior to assessing the valuation of cultural heritage,
it is important to initially discuss how items of cultural
significance may be identified and the link with cultural
heritage. Therefore, an ‘item’ of cultural significance
is herein defined as a resource that has the capacity to
attract existing or potential cultural heritage value. This
term is used as an extension of the term ‘place’, defined
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
under Australia’s Burra Charter as a “site, area, land,
landscape, building or other work, group of buildings
or other works, and may include components, contents,
spaces and views“ (AICOMOS, 1999, p. 2). In addition,
the term ‘item’ also encompasses the following:
- Monuments: architectural works, works of
monumental sculpture and painting, elements or
structures of an archaeological nature, inscriptions,
cave dwellings, and combinations of features, which
are of outstanding universal value from the point of
view of history, art, or science;
- Groups of buildings: groups of separate or
connected buildings which, because of their
architecture, homogeneity or their place in the
landscape, are of outstanding universal value from
the point of view of history, art, or science;
- Sites: works of man; combined works of nature and
of man, and areas including archaeological sites
which are of outstanding universal value from the
historical, aesthetic, ethnological, or anthropological
points of view (UNESCO, 2008a).
The Link Between Cultural Heritage and Items of
Cultural Significance
Putting aside Lord Charteris’ claim that cultural
heritage means “anything you want,” cultural heritage
arises from the interaction between people and items
of cultural significance (Charteris cited in Hewison.
1987, p. 32; Mitchell, Rossler, & Tricaud, 2009). That
is, it is the “set of values that the current generation
place on artefacts and identities associated with the
past” (Gilmour, 2007, p. 4). Australia’s Burra Charter
suggests that these values include “aesthetic, historic,
scientific, or social” values relative to “past, present, or
future generations” (AICOMOS, 1999, p. 12). Similarly, the Council of Europe’s Faro Convention suggests
that cultural heritage represents:
a group of resources inherited from the past which
people regard, irrespective of who owns them, as
a reflection and expression of their own constantly
evolving values, beliefs, knowledge, and traditions. It
includes all aspects of the environment resulting from
the interaction between people and places through
time (COE 2005 ¶1).
Identifying Value Factors of Cultural Heritage
Cultural heritage is clearly a relative rather than absolute concept, and the interface between people and items
189
Refereed Full-Papers
of cultural significance appears to be multi-dimensional
and open to influence from a range of different, interrelated factors which may be objective or subjective.
Throsby identified five broad sets of values: aesthetic
value, spiritual value, social value, historical value, and
symbolic value (Throsby cited in Avrami & Mason,
2000, p. 26). While under Australia’s Burra Charter,
four main values are identified as follows (AICOMOS,
1999):
- Aesthetic value, including aspects of sensory
perception;
- Historic value, which encompasses historical
figures, events, and activities within the history of
aesthetics, science, and society;
- Scientific value links with research value and these
depend on the rarity, quality, and representativeness
of the place under consideration;
- Social value, the spiritual, political, national, or
cultural sentiment relative to a majority or minority
group.
More specifically, UNESCO uses the following
(abridged) criteria to assess sites for the World Heritage
Listing:
i. Represent a masterpiece...;
ii. Exhibit an important interchange of human values,
over a span of time or within a cultural area of the
world...;
iii.Bear a unique or at least exceptional testimony to a
cultural tradition...;
iv.Be an outstanding example of a type of building,
architectural, or technological ensemble or
landscape...;
v. Be an outstanding example of a traditional
human settlement, land-use, or sea-use which is
representative of a culture...;
vi.Be directly or tangibly associated with events or
living traditions, with ideas, or with beliefs, with
artistic and literary works of outstanding universal
significance...;
vii.Contain superlative natural phenomena or areas of
exceptional natural beauty and aesthetic importance
(UNESCO, 2008b).
Similarly, Carter and Bramley (2002) offer the following value indicators:
- Cultural traditions: History of association;
190 - Appeal: Outstanding natural beauty or artistic
qualities;
- Aesthetic qualities: Grandeur, contrast, colour,
pattern, texture, and ‘viewability’;
- Scientific interest: A history of base-line research;
- Existing level of protection;
- Threats: Level of impact and risk of the impact
occurring;
- Sustainability: The ability of the resource to be selfsustaining or the level of management input needed
to protect values;
- Location: The resource’s ability to provide other
community benefits (such as income generation);
- Fragility: The level of resistance and resilience
of the resource to likely perturbations (Carter &
Bramley, 2002).
It is important to note that cultural traditions tend to
depend on the perspectives and frames of reference of
different individuals and groups (Gilmour, 2007; Lynch,
1960; Rapoport, 2005). While cultural significance may
link with historical elements or events, interpretations
of cultural traditions and past events tend to reflect the
perspectives and outlooks of the present, as well as
different intentions or frames of reference. Therefore,
individual and cultural frames of reference in conjunction with changes over time impact on cultural heritage
value.
Evaluation of Cultural Heritage: Towards a
Conceptual Model
As a starting point for further discussion, a model
representing valuation of cultural heritage is proposed
that is derived from Lewin, who considered behaviour
as a function arising from the interactions between
people and their environment (Lewin, 1967):
B = f (P, E)
In regard to cultural heritage, ‘behavior’ implies a
broader range of interactions occurring within a transactional framework that is complex and influenced by
a range of external, environmental factors and internal
cognitive and affective factors at different scales (as per
Moore, 1987). Using an amended version of Nasar’s
probabilistic model of aesthetic response to building
attributes, this framework may be expressed as follows
(Nasar, 1994).
Refereed Full-Papers
Within the context of this framework, the following
conceptual model of the valuation of cultural heritage is
proposed. This model includes value factors identified
as relevant and can be modified to include additional
value factors:
VCHx = f (SG • HT • RO • AE • CX • S)
Wherein the valuation of cultural heritage n respect
to Item x is a function of:
• SG refers to an assessment of the significance of
Item x in respect to a cultural group, sub-cultural
group, ‘expert’ group or stakeholder group (i.e.,
interested parties such as government agencies,
NGOs, funding bodies, developers, etc). This value
factor encompasses the qualities by which the item
has accrued or is likely to accrue spiritual, political,
national, or social sentiment or symbolism relative
to a particular group;
• HT refers to an assessment of the historical
interpretation of historical figures, events and
activities associated with Item x, as well as the
likely changes in historical interpretation that may
occur over time;
• RO refers to an assessment of the relative rarity,
quality, and representativeness of Item x and
whether it exists as a unique or outstanding
example;
• AE refers to an evaluation of the overall aesthetic
qualities of Item x as determined from different
perspectives: individual/public, cultural /subcultural group, ‘expert’ group, stakeholder group;
• CX refers to an assessment of the relevance of Item
x within a broader physical context, landscape or
setting;
• S refers to as assessment of issues relating to
sustainability (both internal and external) in respect
to Item x.
Figure 1. Probabilistic framework for cultural heritage evaluation
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
191
Refereed Full-Papers
Potential Evaluation Strategies, Methods and
Outcomes
The determination of valuation of cultural heritage
may rely on an amalgam of different assessments relative to each value factor. Therefore, a range of potential
assessment methods are proposed as follows.1
1.1Identification of items of cultural significance
Identification of the actual/potential spiritual,
political, national, and/or social significance of
items of cultural significance may be achieved using
nominal group consensus via survey or panel group
interviews.
Nominal group consensus
This technique is one of a number of techniques
used to gain consensus in respect to research aims.
Unlike the Delphi technique, which only uses a
panel of experts, the nominal group consensus
technique comprises a broader group of people
considered to have relevant knowledge or
experience specific to the aims of a research study
(Campbell & Cantrill, 2001; Keeney, Hasson, &
McKenna, 2001).
It is suggested that a non-probabilistic purposive
sampling approach is applied and stratified cluster
sampling is recommended. Stratifying allows
for gender balance and a range of different age
groups; while clustering allows for a sample that is
representative of particular groups: cultural, subcultural as well as ‘expert’, etc (Coolican, 2004;
Groat & Wang, 2002; Hinton, 2004).
The key outcome would be nominal group
consensus in respect to the identification of items
considered to be culturally significant according to a
range of different groups: cultural, sub-cultural, and
‘expert’.
1.2 Assessment of historical interpretation
Key points of historical interpretation and meaning
need to be clearly articulated for each item of
cultural significance and a reiterative process
is recommended. Firstly, an ‘expert’ group of
historians conducts an assessment, and this is
followed by a review conducted by a broader range
of participants (sourced from relevant cultural, subcultural, ‘expert’, and stakeholder groups).
The key outcome would include clearly articulated
statements of significance, historical interpretation,
and meaning in respect to items of cultural
significance achieved via broad consensus.
1.3 Measurement of significance and historical
meaning
It is suggested that significance and historical
interpretation and meaning are assigned values
to enable individual and comparative rating, and
semantic differential rating scales may provide a
means of clustering and rating items.
Semantic differential rating scales
These scales are used to elicit cognitive judgements
and affective appraisals (dependent variable/s) about
independent variable/s (usually represented by
visual stimuli). Responses from semantic differential
rating scales can be analysed using factor analysis
and analysis of variance.
There is no standard set of semantic differential
rating scale items; instead it is usual to use
rating scale items that are considered relevant or
meaningful to the construct or stimulus about which
judgements are to be made (Heise, 1970; Ward &
Russell, 1981). Therefore, rating scale items may
include those relevant to judgements of significance,
historical value and preference: Significantinsignificant; Meaningful-inconsequential;
Important-not important; Historically valuablehistorically insignificant; Priceless-worthless;
Familiar-unfamiliar.
It has been suggested that more than one rating scale
item per construct or dimension is necessary and
four rating scale items per dimension is desirable
(Heise, 1970). Heise (1970) also suggests that the
maximum number of evaluations should not exceed
fifty, as any more than this may lead to participant
fatigue and affect the reliability of results.
Seven anchor points are considered to provide
“good reliability values and correlates well with
other attitude scales thus producing high concurrent
1
Parametric statistical analysis that relies on standard normal distribution may be inappropriate when valuing cultural heritage given that
outlier scores arising from such analysis may in fact represent key cultural or sub-cultural groups for whom an item of cultural significance
holds particular value. To address this issue, stratified cluster sampling is recommended and findings shouldn’t be generalise findings to a
broader population.
192 Refereed Full-Papers
validity” (Coolican, 2004, p. 176). In addition,
numeric values as labels coupled with subjective
rating scale adjectives have been found to influence
the way participants complete a semantic differential
rating scale (Schwarz, Knauper, Hipler, NoelleNeumann, & Clark, 1991). However, labelling of
anchor points may not lead to a scale that contains
equally balanced positions between each point; and
the distance between ‘very’ and ‘fairly’ may be a
perceptually larger distance between the next anchor
points of ‘fairly’ and ‘slightly’ (Friedman & Amoo,
1999).
It is recommended that visual stimuli are used in
conjunction with a rating scale and the following
stimulus sampling process is recommended to
ensure that visual stimuli are representative,
visually consistent and free from visual distractions
(Schroeder, 1988; Wohlwill, 1977). This process
involves collecting a large range of images and
developing selection criteria that includes:
- Representative exemplar image/s of item x;
- High photographic quality;
- Nil visually distracting elements (i.e., people,
cars, etc);
- Consistency of ambient lighting, background
details, etc.
It is suggested that a non-probabilistic purposive
sampling approach is again applied as per the
discussion of stratified cluster sampling above.
The key outcome would be articulated ratings in
respect to significance and historical interpretation
of items of cultural significance achieved via
nominal group consensus.
1.4 Assessment of rarity, quality and
representativeness
It is recommended that the Q-sort technique is used
in conjunction with visual stimuli to achieve levels
of nominal group consensus in respect to decisions
about the rarity, quality and representativeness of
items of cultural significance.
The Q-sort technique, developed by Stephenson in
the 1930s, elicits perceptions and judgments of a
subjective nature by directing participants to sort
visual stimuli (representing independent variable/s)
using categories defined by the researcher.
Participants may be directed to find patterns of
similarity or dissimilarity using categories devised
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
by the researcher or to rank visual stimuli using
a system devised by the researcher (Amin, 2000;
Groat & Wang, 2002; Stephenson, 1953).
It is proposed that a nominal group (comprising
members of particular cultural/sub-cultural and
‘expert’ groups) are asked to sort images of items
of cultural significance into categories such as:
“Rare example of...”; “Outstanding example of...”;
“Unique example of...”; and “Representative
example of...”
Once items have been categorised as above, a
second Q-sort can be conducted to further categorise
and rate items of cultural significance as follows:
“Extremely rare example of...”
“High quality example of...”
“Rare example of...”
“Good quality example of...”
It is suggested that a non-probabilistic purposive
sampling approach is again applied as per the
discussion of stratified cluster sampling above.
The key outcomes of this process would be clearly
articulated ratings in respect to rarity, quality and
representativeness of items of cultural significance
achieved via consensus among nominated group(s).
1.5 Aesthetic evaluation
Aesthetic response is known to vary due to
individual and cultural differences and is considered
to involve a complex interaction of affective and
cognitive responses, many of which may be of
a non-linguistic nature and therefore difficult to
quantify (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1975;
Ulrich, 1983; Zajonc, 1980; Zajonc & Marcus,
1982).
Despite the complexity of aesthetic response,
semantic differential rating scales are often used as
a measurement device in conjunction with visual
stimuli (Janssens, 2001; Osgood et al., 1975; Ou,
Luo, Woodcock, & Wright, 2004; Russell, 1988;
Russell, Ward, & Pratt, 1981; Taft & Sivik, 1997;
Tannenbaum & Osgood, 1952; Urland, 1997;
Wohlwill & Harris, 1980). Rating scale items
may include: Like-dislike; Pleasant-unpleasant;
Beautiful-ugly; Harmonious-disharmonious;
Exciting-gloomy; and Stimulating-dull.
Both factor analysis and analysis of variance could
be applied to the resulting data. Factor analysis is
193
Refereed Full-Papers
an exploratory data reduction technique previously
applied to semantic differential rating scale data
which allows for the identification of key factors via
eigen values and factor loadings which provide a
statistical basis for linking variables to constructs. In
addition, analysis of variance provides an indication
of variance in terms of aesthetic response ratings.
However, outlier scores would need to be carefully
considered and included in the resulting data
analysis (Coolican, 2004; Hinton, 2004; Tabachnick
& Fidell, 1996).
It is suggested that a non-probabilistic purposive
sampling approach is again applied as per the
discussion of stratified cluster sampling above.
The key outcomes would include an indication
of relevant factors (rating scale items) linked to
positive aesthetic response in respect to individual
items of cultural significance. Analysis of variance
would indicate which items of cultural significance
are rated more highly in terms of aesthetic response.
1.6 Contextual assessment
An item of cultural significance generally exists
within a broader context, landscape or setting and
may be contingent on, or impacted by, contextual
factors. A contextual assessment may provide an
indication of the environmental factors relevant to
an individual item of cultural significance such as
the nature of the contextual setting; the existing
level/s of environmental protection; and likely
threats in terms of types of impact and risk of
occurrence.
Beyond identifying and articulating relevant
contextual factors, a rating scale based on levels
defined by nominal group consensus could be
applied that indicates the relative weighting of key
contextual factors in respect to a particular item of
cultural significance.
Given the mostly objective nature of contextual
assessment, this task could be performed by an
‘expert’ group comprised of people with expert
knowledge in terms of environmental assessment
and economics.
Key outcomes would include contextual assessment statements that identify key contextual factors; plus
a range of scores (from high to low) of items of
cultural significance in respect to defined levels of
contextual evaluation.
194 1.7 Sustainability evaluation
Cultural heritage links with sustainability from two
angles. Firstly, cultural heritage must exist within
a system that supports the objectives of sustainable
economic development. Secondly, cultural heritage
can contribute to sustainable economic development
when the economic potential of cultural heritage is
utilised in economic policy without compromising
its inherent values (COE, 2005). UNESCO suggests
that the key decisions regarding the sustainability
of heritage items includes defining which elements
of an Item are (i) to be conserved at all costs, (ii)
subject to limited change provided that the overall
character and significance is maintained, and (iii)
suitable for exchange in return for other benefits
(Mitchell et al., 2009).
In light of the above, a sustainability assessment
would need to address the following:
- The item’s ability to be self-sustaining;
- The inherent ability of the item to provide
community benefits (such as income
generation);
- The level of management needed to conserve,
protect, and maintain the item
- The ability of the community to maintain levels
of protection, maintenance, etc.
In addition, each of the above factors could be
evaluated in terms of a rating scale which provides
different scores in respect to levels defined by
nominal group consensus.
Given the objective nature of a sustainability
evaluation, this task could be performed by an
‘expert’ group comprised of people with expert
knowledge of sustainability issues, tourism issues,
economics, etc.
The key outcomes would be sustainability
evaluation statements that clearly articulate key
aspects of sustainability in respect to particular
items of cultural significance; plus a range of scores
(from high to low) of items of cultural significance
in respect to defined levels of sustainability.
Discussion
The valuation of cultural heritage is a multi-dimensional process and the conceptual model of cultural
heritage and the range of evaluation strategies detailed
above are offered as a starting point for further discussion. These strategies, which follow a cumulative
Refereed Full-Papers
approach, may assist with quantifying overall value in
respect to items of cultural significance. However, the
process of acknowledging history within the broader
environment needs to be about more than just nostalgia.
Blakely suggests that “heritage buildings are no heritage
when they interfere with the future of a community...
if everything is preserved, then nothing has value” and
that it is the act of selective preservation that generates new value (Blakely cited in Gilmour, 2007, p. v).
Therefore, the process of assessing and quantifying
value needs to be methodical and systematic as well as
fair, balanced, and transparent.
References
AICOMOS. (1999). The Burra Charter: The Australia ICOMOS
Charter for Places of Cultural Significance. Retrieved 8 April
2010. from http://australia.icomos.org/images/pdf/BURRA_
CHARTER.pdf.
Amin, Z. (2000). Q methodology - A journey into the subjectivity
of the human mind. Singapore Medical Journal, 41, 410-414.
Avrami, E., & Mason, R. (2000). Values and Heritage
Conservation (Research Report). Los Angeles: The Getty
Conservation Institute.
Campbell, S. M., & Cantrill, J. A. (2001). Consensus methods in
prescribing research. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology and
Therapeutics, 26(5-14).
Carter, R. W., & Bramley, R. (2002). Defining heritage values
and significance for improved resource management: An
application to Australian tourism. International Journal of
Heritage Studies, 8(3), 175-199.
COE. (2005). Presentation of the framework convention on
the value of cultural heritage for society (Faro, 2005).
Retrieved 18 August 2010, from http://www.coe.int/t/dg4/
cultureheritage/heritage/identities/faro_pres_en.asp
Coolican, H. (2004). Research methods and statistics in
psychology (4th ed.). London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Friedman, H. H., & Amoo, T. (1999). Rating the rating scales.
Journal of Marketing Management, 9(3), 114-123.
Gilmour, T. (2007). Sustaining heritage: Giving the past a future.
Sydney: Sydney University Press.
Groat, L., & Wang, D. (2002). Architectural research methods.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Heise, D. R. (1970). The semantic differential and attitude
research. In G. Summers (Ed.), Attitude measurement.
Chicago: Rand McNally.
Hewison, R. (1987). The heritage industry: Britain in a climate
of decline. London: Methuen.
Hinton, P. R. (2004). Statistics explained (2nd Ed.). New York,
NY: Routledge.
Janssens, J. (2001). Facade colours: Not just a matter of taste.
Nordic Journal of Architectural Research, 14, 11-16.
Keeney, S., Hasson, F., & McKenna, H. P. (2001). A critical
review of the Delphi technique as a research methodology.
International Journal of Nursing Studies, 38, 195-200.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Lewin, K. (1967). Field theory in social science: Selected
theoretical papers. London: Social Science Press.
Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Mitchell, N., Rossler, M., & Tricaud, P. M. (2009). World
Heritage Cultural Landscapes: A handbook for conservation
and management (World Heritage Paper 26). Paris:
UNESCO.
Moore, G. T. (1987). Environment and behaviour research in
North America: History, developments, and unresolved
issues. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of
environmental psychology
(Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.
Nasar, J. L. (1994). Urban design aesthetics: The evaluative
qualities of building exteriors. Environment and Behaviour,
26(3), 377-401.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1975). The
measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois.
Ou, L. C., Luo, M. R., Woodcock, A., & Wright, A. (2004).
A study of colour emotion and colour preference. Part
III: Colour preference modelling. Color Research and
Application, 29(5), 381-389.
Rapoport, A. (2005). Culture, architecture and design. Chicago,
IL: Locke Science Publishing.
Russell, J. A. (1988). Affective appraisals of environments. In J.
L. Nasar (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research
and applications. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Russell, J. A., Ward, L. M., & Pratt, G. (1981). Affective
quality attributed to environments: A factor analytic study.
Environment and Behaviour, 13(3), 259-288.
Schroeder, H. W. (1988). Visual impact of hillside development:
Comparison of measurements derived from aerial and
ground-level photographs. Landscape and Urban Planning,
15, 119-126.
Schwarz, N., Knauper, B., Hipler, H. J., Noelle-Neumann, E., &
Clark, L. (1991). Numeric values may change the meaning of
scale labels. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(4), 570-582.
Stephenson, W. (1953). The study of behavior: Q-technique and
its methodology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate
statistics (4th ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
Taft, C., & Sivik, L. (1997). Color meaning and context. Paper
presented at the Colour Report: Colour and Psychology F50,
Gothenburg.
Tannenbaum, P. H., & Osgood, C. E. (1952). Effect of color on
the meanings of advertised products. In C. E. Osgood, G.
J. Suci & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), The measurement of
meaning. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural
environments. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human
Behaviour and Environment (Vol. 6). New York: Plenum
Press.
UNESCO. (2008a). Operational guidelines for the
implementation of the World Heritage Convention. Paris:
UNESCO World Heritage Centre.
UNESCO. (2008b). World Heritage Sites: The criteria for
selection. Retrieved 10 August 2010, from http://whc.
unesco.org/en/criteria/
195
Refereed Full-Papers
Urland, A. (1997). Attempt at finding the objective factors
underlying subjective aesthetical evaluations of exterior
architectural colour schemes - an experimental research
study. Paper presented at the Colour Report: Colour and
Psychology F50. AIC Interim Meeting, Gothenburg.
Ward, L. M., & Russell, J. A. (1981). The psychological
representation of molar physical environments. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 110(2), 121-151.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1977). Visual assessment of urban riverfront.
Unpublished manuscript.
196 Wohlwill, J. F., & Harris, G. (1980). Response to congruity or
contrast for man-made features in natural-recreation settings.
Leisure Science, 3(4), 349-365.
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151-175.
Zajonc, R. B., & Marcus, H. (1982). Affective and cognitive
factors in preferences. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2),
123-131.
Refereed Full-Papers
Conceptual Foundations for
Modeling the Sociospatial
Structures of Activity in
Facilities Programming
Lubomir Popov (Bowling Green State
University)
Introduction
Human activity constitutes a major theme in the
social sciences. It is such a fundamental category that
no matter how much it is explored, some areas are still
need additional development. While there is a tremendous concentration of resources and publications in a
number of social science domains, in others, there are
almost no explorations of human activity. Many areas
do not attract enough scholarship efforts because they
are not typical of social science endeavors, they do
not threaten the social structure and order, or they are
not supported by government or foundation funding.
Such areas also require an interdisciplinary engagement and the bringing together of intellectual resources
from quite diverse fields. This project is dedicated to
one such domain: the social aspects of the built environment, including applied research such as facilities
programming. The current paper presents a study of
human activity for the purpose of creating a conceptual
foundation for construing the structures of activity that
encompass both social and spatial elements. Such activity structures will form the core of facilities programming research designs.
Facilities programming provides project-specific
information for design decision-making. The facilities
program describes building users and their activities,
norms, needs, and preferences. During the programming phase, user behavior patterns are studied, priorities about allocation of resources are made, and user
requirements are formulated. Facilities programming is
a complex process that involves information gathering
and analyzing, decision-making, and spatial reasoning (Harrigan, 1987; Preiser, 1993; Sanoff, 1992). The
core of programming is the study of activities and user
operations that take place in the built environment. An
analysis of the literature on programming shows that
activity analysis constitutes the foundation for developing user requirements (Harrigan, 1987; Bechtel,
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
1977; Moleski, 1974; Sanoff, 1992). The activity theme
exists, directly or indirectly, in almost all publications
on programming. It is presented in different disciplinary
languages, ranging from the terminology of the social
sciences, to the lingo of operations managers, to the
plain language in leisure and recreation planning. There
are several reasons for this attention to human activity.
In brief, the concept of activity should have a privileged
status within facilities programming research because
it is activity that forms the nexus of all relationships; it
constitutes the medium of interaction, and it is the realm
wherein other sociospatial categories acquire meaning
and importance.
The numerous models of human activity developed
in the social sciences—within a multitude of different
domains—are well known. Most of these activity models have been created for purely academic pursuits or
for the particularized needs of management, marketing,
social work, education, human-computer interaction,
and other fields (Bedny & Karwowski, 2007; Berglind,
1988; Good & Brophy, 1995; Kaptelinin & Nardi,
2006; Kotler, 2003; Lamport, Coffey, & Hersch, 2001;
Robins, 2003; Staubmann, 2006; Stewart, 1998; Valach,
Young, & Lyman, 2002). These conceptualizations
of activity predominantly envisage the sociocultural
aspects of human action, and they usually focus on the
relationships between its social elements and attributes.
Such models are not conceived for the study of the
built environment and the sociospatial interactions, and
in many aspects they are not relevant to and efficient
for such applications. Besides, the variety of activity
models is often a source of information overload and
confusion, impeding the search for conceptualizations
that are relevant to sociospatial research.
Sociospatial studies, and programming research in
particular, have their own specific requirements regarding activity models. These requirements result from
the nature and objectives of programming research, the
nature of sociospatial phenomena, and the information
needs of the design process. Such circumstances influence all of the discourse on activity and its spatial dimensions, and they also constitute the subject matter for
activity studies in programming. Correspondingly, they
will generate the basic criteria for assessing activity
models in terms of their appropriateness and efficiency
regarding programming research.
The goal of this paper is to present conceptual foundations for modeling human activity for the purposes
of facilities programming research. The objective is to
197
Refereed Full-Papers
suggest directions for analyzing activity systems and
for building activity models with respect to necessary
conditions. These directions will also facilitate the
search for specific aspects and structures of activity that
relate social and spatial elements. Additionally, they
will help to reveal the structures of activity that allow
for the identification of the necessary conditions and
undesirable influences (impediments, restrictions, and
limitations) that impact the facilitating and sustaining of
activity processes. The development of specific activity
models remains outside the scope of this paper.
A Short Review of Selected Activity Models
There are myriad perspectives on and models of
human activity in dozens of disciplines, areas, and
domains. This makes the task of finding the appropriate point of view for analyzing human activity both
challenging and time-consuming. This section discusses
only selected positions, thus restricting the scope of
conceptualization to a manageable size. The emphasis
is on four major ways of analyzing human activity in
terms of (a) its constituent individual processes; (b) the
scale of the segmentation of activity; (c) the content of
activity; and (d) pragmatic models from practice fields.
Activity Models Based Upon Constituent Individual
Processes
These models usually consider physiological
and psychological processes. Many authors (Brannick, Levine, & Morgeson, 2007; Lamport, Coffey,
& Hersch, 2001) add the motor processes as well as
selected social aspects. The physiological processes are
relatively uniform for most user groups, with the exception of groups formed on the basis of specific health
status, ability, or age. At the psychological level, the
main processes that constitute activities are cognitive,
emotive (affective), and conative. Within this perspective, there is a tendency to analyze and optimize processes one by one. The models are very productive at
the micro scale, in human factors and ergonomics, but
they are not created for analyzing the holistic relationships between activities at the building scale.
In accordance with many authors, real activities will
be viewed as entities on a large scale—that is, molar
phenomena (Kuutti, 1999). Correspondingly, the identification of the necessary conditions (needs) in facilities
programming should be determined not in relation to
isolated, segmented, and fragmented elements (such as
physiological, psycho-physiological, and psychologi-
198 cal processes) but in the context and on the basis of the
whole stream of human behavior.
Activity Models Based Upon Scale of Segmentation of
Activity
Activity can be modeled in terms of actions, operations, and acts (Brannick, Levine, & Morgeson, 2007;
Davidov, 1990; Engestrom, 1999). Such a perspective is used in the psychology of work, human factors
engineering, and job design (Aamodt, 2006; Brannick,
Levine, & Morgeson, 2007; Smither, 1998). The value
of this approach might be obvious to professionals engaged in productivity enhancement, personnel training,
and job design. These models are usually developed for
the study of operations and operators’ reactions, as well
as separate individual processes. Such models tend to
focus too much on human performance in small-scale
situations. However, despite a number of positive contributions, these models fall short of describing holistic
large-scale activities at building scale.
Activity Models Based Upon Content of Activity
In contrast, there are alternative models that tend to
embrace larger units of study that are different from the
sum total of all constituent processes. These models
also represent the units of study in a larger context (Hewitt, 2003; Strauss, 1993). The content-based models of
activity include information about the type of operations
performed, the interconnections between operations and
processes, the algorithms of organizing these operations and processes, the agents (actors, participants),
goals and motivation specificity, functions and outcomes, meaning and importance, volume of transactions
and flows, and similar considerations. In the content
model perspective, the basic assumption is that it is not
the separate individual processes and qualities of the
participants that shape the environmental interactions,
but rather the syncretic result of their synchronized and
interrelated flow (Davidov, 1999).
One example of the content-based approach to human activity is the concept stream of activity (Hewitt,
2003), which is similar to the concepts stream of behavior (Barker, 1968) and activity chain (Perin, 1970).
This concept is operationalized by breaking down the
activity processes into more simple units on the basis of
their content. The activity structures are construed by
describing the various “things” a person does (Hewitt,
2003). These descriptions inevitably include or refer to
the environment where the individual acts are embed-
Refereed Full-Papers
ded. Such an ideographic method naturally bridges the
abstractness of social science conceptualizations and
the experiential approach with which space planners
and designers think about human activities.
Pragmatic Activity Models From Practice Fields
In environment and behavior research, and particularly within the field of space syntax, the predominant
research problems are related to the spatial structures
of activity and are expressed in terms of movement,
physical communication, and proximity (Hillier, 1996;
Peponis & Wineman, 2002). These problems are
interconnected, because face-to-face communication
requires the arrangement of all participants together,
closer in space. That proximity is, in turn, provided by
movement. However, no matter how important and heuristic these problems are, they strongly overshadow all
other considerations and bias the general picture.
Another tradition, one that is both holistic and experiential, has grown in the field of leisure and recreation.
These models display descriptive stance, content orientation, and everyday rationality. Kraus (1997) implicitly
uses a framework that consists of participants, type of
activity, activity events (contests, shows, and so forth),
social significance of activity, value for participants, developmental functions of activity, personality functions
of activity, skills and abilities needed for participation,
administration and leadership, and community development. Rossman and Schlatter (2008) work within
the same tradition, although they make a somewhat
different categorization: activity (content), patterns of
organization, and format. The format is described in
terms of instructors, personnel, officers; registration
and participant fees; class size, meetings, and units; and
location, facility, and publicity. The level of specificity
is guided by the requirements of particular professional
routines and responsibilities.
An important perspective can be gleaned from the
fields of organizational studies and organizational
design (Cummings & Worley, 2001). The conceptualization and the parlance are different from the traditions
in the major social sciences. From this vantage point,
it is more likely to construe basic activity models that
can be used as foundations for developing theories of
organization and guidelines for management consulting.
The major interests focus on organizational structure,
processes, and outputs. These areas are interpreted in
terms of communication and flows, applied to several
different categories: resources, participants/actors, and
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
information. The emphasis is on exchange and interaction, both among social agents and among activities.
Conceptual Directions: Activity Dimensions
Regarding the Organization of Space
These directions for activity modeling are developed
by interpreting the major dimensions and relationships
among activities, as well as among activities and their
settings. The relationships among activities situated
within built environment evolve along several lines:
functions; operations, flows, and communication; space
and time; and conditions and resources. Each one of
these lines is elaborated below in order to provide conceptual grounds and guidance for designing situationspecific activity models that can be used in facilities
programming projects. The actual model-building will
take place in a later project, and the results will be presented in a subsequent paper.
Functions
Function can be construed as a relation of contributing within and among activities. In this study, function
refers to the contributions made by one activity to the
“existence” or “flow” of another activity, as well as to
each activity’s maintenance and support of the activity
system. The functional structure of activities emerges
on the grounds of “demand-supply” or “input-output”
relationships. Functional analysis describes the interdependence between activities, and explicates the social
meaning of these relationships. It is the stepping stone
for the conceptual re-envisioning of the activity systems
that are to be serviced by the building. When a proactive approach is adopted, advocating redesign of activity systems and the social organization as the grounds
for functional programming, function is determined
first; only then are the operations necessary for its performance designed.
From a functional perspective, activities can be
categorized according to the effects they supply, their
performance, and their role within the broader context.
The list of functions is virtually endless, and its content
depends on the specificity of the situation. Some functional classifications can be designed on the grounds of
importance, for example, basic and secondary activities.
In addition, it is productive to conceptualize a layer of
“servicing” activities. They are defined on the grounds
of the support they provide to basic and secondary
activities.
199
Refereed Full-Papers
Operations, Flows, and Communication
The flows of resources, participants/actors, and information within an organization or a setting constitute
a system of exchanges among activities and generate a
cluster of structures that set the corresponding relationships among activity units. For example, the exchange
of resources encompasses raw materials, products, and
byproducts. The exchange can be conceptualized in
terms of inputs and outputs. The products or results of
one activity might become “inputs” for another activity.
The inputs are raw materials or prefabricated components that may inform another activity. The concept of
“output” is broad enough to encompass not only desired
outcomes but also all types of emissions, including side
effects and counterproductive influences.
In complex organizations, the flows acquire special
importance. The specific way the flows are interrelated
exerts a profound effect on the overall organizational
design. When the participants are viewed as bodies
(anthropometric aspect), their movements constitute one
of the flows (Hillier, 1996; Peponis & Wineman, 2002).
However, since actors are more than inanimate objects,
this flow is strongly affected by a drive to minimize
loss of energy, time, and level of comfort. These factors affect activity and its operational patterns through
influencing the personal dispositions of the social agents
toward one or another course of action. At the level
of information flows (which go along several distinct
lines), close attention should be paid to face-to-face
communication and other modes of information-transfer
that require particular spatial conditions.
Space and Time
Activities take place in time, and they have to be
organized along this dimension as well (Gren, 2001;
Whipp, 2002). Temporal synchronization is even more
important than spatial organization, because the flow
of time is unidirectional, and disrupted connections
cannot be substituted or changed, as is possible in
space. The view of activity as a stream or chain stresses
its transience and underlines its temporal aspect. The
temporal aspect is of primary importance in organizing
cooperative action, because cooperation presupposes
synchronization of flows of people, materials, energy,
and information. In this respect, the synchronization of
start, end, and duration is of greatest importance. Duration is another parameter that is often influenced by the
actual possibility of the social system to carry out all its
activities within the limits of natural cycles.
200 Space and time are interrelated (Crang, 2001, Gren,
2001, Stein, 2001). For example, moving in space takes
time, and using one and the same room/place for different activities requires a time schedule. Temporal separation among activities can be substituted for spatial separation. Different and often incompatible activities can be
carried out in the same room—if they are separated in
time and are conducted at different times according to a
schedule. Temporal organization is the simplest means
of solving such problems. From a certain point of view,
sequential use of spaces for different activities is also a
way of saving resources, because with close attention to
temporal considerations, the use of the premises may be
maximized and the need for new rooms may subside.
Conditions and Resources
Following these conceptualizations, activity can be
viewed as a medium of interaction and interrelation
between social agents and spatial configurations. Spatial
configurations provide the “stage” and supply conditions necessary for the agents to function and sustain
social relations. These conditions have an impact on the
trajectory of activity and the psychological states of the
agents. Sometimes the environment is supportive of the
premeditated action plans; in other situations, the action
plans have to be modified in order to prevent collisions
with the “hard” environment. From such a perspective,
agent-environment interactions are analyzed as necessary conditions and resources.
The basic principles of people-environment interactions and the goal structure of programming research
imply a differentiation of activity systems according to
the conditions they need. The interrelation of activities
and spatial arrangements brings about the problem of
necessary environmental conditions. Conditions constitute the contact points in sociospatial interactions.
The artificial physical environment is built with the
purpose of producing (supplying) necessary conditions
for activities and processes. From this point of view,
facilities programming research aims to identify needs
and need-related phenomena with the ultimate goal of
translating them into design requirements. This aspect
of activity constitutes the basis of activity studies in
facilities programming and generates the basic criteria
for assessing activity models regarding their productivity in programming.
The process of environmental interaction is dynamic
and variable. The psychological and social states of the
social agent change as a result of the impact of the en-
Refereed Full-Papers
vironmental conditions. At the same time, in each new
state, the agent requires new conditions. In other words,
the agent enters into a new series of relationships with
the environment. The same interactional dynamic takes
place when the social agent moves from one type of
behavior setting to another. He or she enters into a different situation characterized by a different set of conditions. The new conditions presuppose that the agent
will enter into different relations with environment.
Every specific situation generates new need-states—and
thus new requirements and necessary conditions.
Synthesis: A View of Activity From a Sociospatial
Perspective
The notions presented above constitute the grounds
for construing the sociospatial structures of activity and
for designing new activity systems. The real sociospatial effects emerge when activities are organized within
physical reality. When constructing activity patterns,
both the spatial and the temporal influences must be
considered. From this point of view, sociospatial structures are comprised of the relationships among activities and their necessary conditions and, respectively,
undesirable influences, and other impeding and blocking factors that act to limit or restrict the activities. The
sociospatial structures can also be identified in relation
to those dimensions of activity that are influenced by
the size and the configuration of space, adjacencies,
and temporal coordination, to name several of the most
basic characteristics.
The study of activity for the purposes of facilities
programming presupposes an emphasis on the relationships among activities that emerge with respect to the
environmental conditions necessary for sustaining and
reproducing human action. Also of vital interest are
the relationships among social agents that emerge with
regard to these conditions. The sociospatial analysis
focuses on the structures of activity that emerge in the
process of social interaction with and in the physical
environment. These sociospatial structures represent the
existence of social phenomena in space, as well as the
social functioning of built artifacts. In a metatheoretical plan, the notion of sociospatial structures of activity
shapes the research goals and the criteria for information collection in facilities programming.
These ideas serve as a platform for generating a new
conceptualization of the social realm, a notion focused
on the environmental aspects of activities. Following
this line of thinking, activities interact and enter into
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
relationships because of their goals and the products,
effects, and emissions they produce, and also because
these elements constitute or contribute to the environment of other activities in the system. When activity
interactions are managed by spatial or temporal means,
new, derived relations emerge among activity units
in respect to the spatial environment—relations like
compatibility or incompatibility, cooperation or competition, complementarity or conflict, mutual support
or impediment, succession or parallelism, autonomy or
dependency, and so forth. These dimensions of activity interactions and interrelations influence the spatial
organization of activities in real situations/settings. In
its own turn, the organization of activities in space often
brings about changes in their operational structures and
specificity. The newly modified activity configurations
interact again with one another and with the ambient environment. As a result, change takes place within these
configurations, and a change in the resulting relations
occurs as well. In reality, this is a cyclic, iterative process. The impact of space and the corresponding sociospatial effects and reconfigurations are natural outcomes
of the reciprocal person-environment interactions.
Concluding Remarks
The propositions mentioned above constitute a way
of thinking that is intended to serve as conceptual foundations of activity studies in facilities programming.
In a metaphoric sense, activity is a medium that binds
together participants, organization of participation, necessary conditions, and spatial structures. This vantage
point establishes the grounds for a specific conditions
and resources perspective on activity and also for a new
kind of activity models for sociospatial research. The
conceptualization of activity as a medium of interaction
provides the basis for a new approach to analyzing the
patterns of building use and the sociospatial structures
of social reality. The purpose of the built environment
is to supply necessary conditions for the functioning of
human individuals, and this purpose presupposes the
information needs of designers and the goal structure of
facilities programming research.
The results of the present study can be utilized in a
number of ways. Other scholars can critically analyze
the ideas presented here and offer new conceptualizations of activity regarding programming research. Furthermore, the paper can initiate a discussion and foster
the development of alternative approaches, guidelines,
and models. The author intends to use the conceptual
201
Refereed Full-Papers
directions in a second project that will develop more
concrete activity models, the purposes of which will be
to guide the research design process in programming
projects, to delineate the type of information to be collected, and to facilitate the interpretation of the data.
The programmers will have the liberty and flexibility to
adapt the models to their projects.
References
Aamodt, M. G. (2006). Applied industrial/organizational
psychology. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.
Barker, R. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Bechtel, R. (1977). Enclosing behavior. Stroudsburg, PA:
Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross.
Bedny, G. Z., & Karwowski, W. (2007). A systemic-structural
theory of activity: Applications to human performance and
work design. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis.
Berglind, H. (1988). Towards an action theory for social work:
Five essays. Stockholm, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell
International.
Brannick, M. T., Levine, E. L., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Job
and work analysis: Methods, research, and applications for
human resource management (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA:
SAGE Publications.
Crang, M. (2001). Rhythms of the city: Temporalised space
and motion. In J. May & N. Thrift (Eds.), TimeSpace:
Geographies of temporality, (pp. 187-207). London, UK;
New York, NY: Routledge.
Cummings, T., & Worley, C. (2001). Organizational development
and change (7th ed.). Cincinnati, OH: South-Western
College Pub.
Davidov, V. V. (1990). Problems of activity as mode of human
existence and the principle of monism. In V.A. Lektorsky
(Ed.), Activity: Theories, methodology & problems (pp. 127132). Orlando, FL: Paul Deutsch Press, Inc.
Davidov, V. V. (1999). The content and unsolved problems
of activity theory. In Y. Engestrom, R. Miettinen, & R.
Punamaki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 127132). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Engestrom, Y. (1999). Activity theory and individual and
social transformation. In Y. Engestrom, R. Miettinen, & R.
Punamaki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 19-38).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. (1995). Contemporary educational
psychology (5th ed.). White Plains, NY: Longman.
Gren, M. (2001). Time-geography matters. In J. May & N. Thrift
(Eds.), TimeSpace: Geographies of temporality, (pp. 208225). London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.
Harrigan, J. (1987). Human factors research: Methods
and applications for architects and interior designers.
Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Hewitt, J. P. (2003). Self and society: A symbolic interactionist
social psychology (9th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
202 Hillier, B. (1996). Space is the machine: A configurational theory
of architecture. Cambridge, MA; New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Kaptelinin, V., & Nardi, B. (2006) Acting with technology:
Activity theory and interaction design. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Kotler, P. (2003). A framework for marketing management. (2d
ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kraus, R. G. (1997). Recreation Programming: A benefits-driven
approach. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Kuutti, K. (1999). Activity theory, transformation of work, and
information systems design. In Y. Engestrom, R. Miettinen,
& R. Punamaki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp.
360-376). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lamport, N. K., Coffey, M. S., & Hersch, G.I. (2001). Activity
analysis and application (4th ed.). Thorofare, NJ: Slack Inc.
Moleski, W. (1974). Behavioral analysis in environmental
programming for offices. In J. Lang, C. Burnette, W.
Moleski, & S. Vachon (Eds.), Designing for human behavior:
Architecture and the behavioral sciences, (pp. 302-315).
Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchenson & Ross.
Peponis, J., & Wineman, J. (2002). Spatial structure of
environment and behavior. In R. Bechtel & A. Churchman
(Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology (pp. 271291). New York, NY: John Wiley.
Perin, C. (1970). With man in mind: An interdisciplinary
prospectus for environmental design. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Preiser, W. (Ed.). (1993) Professional practice in facility
programming. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Robins, S. P. (2003). Essentials of organizational behavior. (7th
ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rossman, J. R., & Schlatter, B. E. (2008). Recreation
programming: Designing leisure experiences (5th ed.).
Champaign, IL: Sagamore Publishing, 2008.
Sanoff, H. (1992). Integrating programming, evaluation and
participation in design: A theory Z approach. Aldershot,
England; Brookfield, VT: Avebury.
Staubmann, H. (Ed.). (2006). Action theory: Methodological
studies. Wien, Austria: Lit Verlag.
Stein, J. (2001). Reflections on time, time-space compression and
technology in the nineteenth century. In J. May & N. Thrift
(Eds.), TimeSpace: Geographies of temporality, (pp. 106119). London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.
Stewart, R. (Ed.). (1998). Managerial work. Brookfield, VT:
Ashgate.
Strauss, A. L. (1993). Continual permutations of action. New
York, NY: Aldine De Guyter.
Smither, R. D. (1998). The psychology of work and human
performance (3d ed.). New York, NY: Longman.
Valach, L., Richard Y., & Lyman, M. (Eds.) 2002. Action theory:
A primer for applied research in the social sciences. Wesport,
CT: Praeger.
Whipp, R., Adam, B., & Sabelis, I. (Eds.). (2002). Making time:
Time and management in modern organizations. Oxford, UK;
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Refereed Full-Papers
The Architecture and Urban
Design Project Viewed as a
Catalyst of Identity:
Analysis of Recent
Contributions to ‘Montrealness’
Alena Prochazka (University of Montreal,
Canada)
Abstract
How and toward what evolves the “Montrealness”
of the built environment of Montreal? How does the
built landscape’s personality evolve as fact (in terms of
physical landscape) and as idea (in terms of ideational
landscape), as seen in recent projects? Between the typomorphological urbanism, the new modernism and the
intent to adapt new technologies to the enduring nature
and spirit of a place, architects and designers are creating a renewed Montrealness. The idea of the specific
character (in terms of materials, construction systems,
forms, styles, scale, density, networks) that is to be imposed on Montreal’s built environment to maintain and
support its unique identity is created by its interpreters:
architects and designers (through their projects) and observers (through the critical reception of the projects).
The idea emerges from recycling, from one project to
another, those characteristics that typify it. Despite and
beyond a certain codified tradition of Montrealness, we
have recently witnessed the emergence of projects that
seem to break with the established imagery of identity.
Is this a paradigm shift or a slow effacement of a Montreal personality recorded in the earlier strata that makes
up the cityscape? By identifying the emerging trends
among architects and designers who care about the continuity of Montreal’s imagery of identity—which gives
physical reality to the personality of Montreal through
the built environment— but who also demonstrate the
innovation and creativity characteristic of the neomodernists, our research has revealed the emergence of
a new hybrid paradigm, “critical contextuality.”
Introduction
My doctoral research was involved with the idea
of “The Montrealness of Montreal;” more precisely,
we examined the process by which recent urban and
architectural projects in Montreal contribute to create
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
its urban identity, i.e., the specific perceived personality
of a built landscape. Montrealness has been a focus of
interest since architect and urban artist Melvin Charney
coined the expression in the late 1970s. Our position is
that Montrealness is a result of representations and discourses which are attributed to architectural and urban
features, and thus would originate during the genesis of
projects, i.e., in the design choices made with respect to
figures and strategies. Hence, our methodology is based
on an analysis of projects’ “genetic” documents such as
drawings, models, and other material used during the
creative process, which communicate architectural concepts and intentions while confronting their Montrealness with the critical discourse surrounding the projects.
This approach, while contributing to investigate the
role of representations in the production of space, is primarily concerned with the continuity of urban form—
in space as well as in time—within the perspective of
change. In other words, we are looking into the process
of updating the idea of Montrealness.
The renewal of urban identity is particularly crucial
in the context of globalization, which creates tensions
between the local and the global (Castells, 1997),
namely in the form of economic and cultural rivalry
between cities. Indeed, research shows (Dear, 2001) that
a strong self-image may be a factor in positioning cities
in the global context. From this standpoint, how—by
what mechanisms and tools during the design process—
may urban projects contribute to asserting, intensifying,
or updating such identity?
Learning from Montreal, a Model or a Personality?
There is an ambiguity indeed in the “learning from”
question, particularly when discussing the built form of
one city, in this case Montreal, with actors concerned
with cities elsewhere. By “learning from,” do we mean
studying a model to serve as an example for planning
elsewhere, or are we interested in “learning from” as
a design method for planning and building the urban
landscape of that particular city? Trevor Boddy has
made this crucial distinction, even though his focus was
not on design methods but on discourse (writings) about
Vancouver.
Speaking of Vancouverism (Boddy, 2004), he rightly
distinguishes between Vancouverism as a kind of “New
Urbanism” prototype for planning new high-density
residential neighbourhoods (in this case tall, skinny
high-rise towers set on townhouse podiums) as opposed
to Vancouverism meant as an intimate Vancouver sense
of place. In other words, he contrasts the Vancouver
203
Refereed Full-Papers
model with what one might call the Vancouver style (let
us rather use the word personality).
In Trevor Boddy’s (2003) words:
“Vancouverism is evolving a second and more interesting sense, that of the latent [urban] character. [...]
Call it the theory, or the legacy, or the idea of Vancouver, but increasingly our writers are producing books
that capture this precious moment of self-knowledge,
as this good-looking adolescent of a city enters a more
complicated young adulthood.”
However, Canadian cities, young and old, are the
subject of this process of self-knowledge not only for
the sake of historical insight. Our focus is that the very
idea or image of the personality of a place which is
being asserted by such investigations or discourse is a
potent ingredient in the design of the urban landscape
when it is taken into account in design methods. As a
result, a unique urban identity is conferred upon that
landscape. Certainly, this identity is continuously subject to change. We propose to show that such change,
such an updating process, is brought forth through a
few seminal projects combined with the recognition—
or critical reception—that surrounds them. Urban
identity is thus the result of a negotiation between the
material landscape and the ideational landscape of its
representations and the discourses created around it.
In our doctoral research, we were looking particularly into the urban and architectural design methodologies that recycle this precious stuff of which the identity
of a place is made. In their projects, how do urban designers and architects promote and/or update the special
quality attributed to a particular city?
To tackle this subject, two questions are raised with
respect to the case of Montreal. How did the particular
personality of Montreal’s built landscape which we call
Montrealness become codified and established as an
ideational landscape? What is the mechanism that enables some projects to contribute to the evolution of the
“Montrealness of Montreal,” and in what way does this
occur? Two concepts will be explored in order to get to
the heart of the matter:
1- The process of codification of the idea or image
of Montreal’s built form—this involves observers
(critics, theorists, and artists who consecrate images
of a city in their productions and writings);
2- The concept of recycling and/or updating this idea
or image in projects—this involves urban designers
and architects.
204 The Montrealness of Montreal
With respect to the codification of the special quality
attributed to a city, Montreal has an interesting and rich
history which we will not go into in detail; however, let
us briefly mention its milestones.
The first coherent manifesto that involved Montreal’s
specificity was published in 1971 by artist and architect Melvin Charney, then a professor at the School
of Architecture of the University of Montreal. This
neo-rationalist manifesto explores a populist approach
to urban form: a sort of argument for “Learning from
Montreal,” published a year before Robert Venturi
and Denise Scott Brown’s landmark Learning from
Las Vegas (published in 1972). Charney’s approach is
resolutely urban vernacular (according to him, authentic
modern architecture typical of Québec is to be found in
the streets), and populist (this architecture has evolved,
as he says, independently from official and academic
architecture). He maintains that beyond its monuments,
the architecture that is characteristic of a city is revealed
in the street and in the buildings that constitute it: the
street provides the built landscape with its overall cohesiveness. Charney uses this fertile ground as the basis
for asserting the necessity, or as he puts it, the quoteunquote “project,” of affirming the Québécois identity
and renewing it through the original works of architects.
New buildings are to be linked to the urban fabric and
are to respect the autonomy, so to speak, of a character
specific to Montreal. In 1980, Charney coined the expression “the Montrealness of Montreal” to express this
specific character of the urban landscape.
In 1978, Charney founded the Urban Architecture
Unit at the University of Montreal’s School of Architecture. The Unit would give rise to a project approach
which became known as the School of Montreal (Martin, 1995:10).
Of course, this posture, a sort of “urbanism of the
street,” was at that time, in the 1970s, in line with postmodern ideologies devoted to reintroducing the semantic value of the built environment into design methods.
In Quebec, project methods were marked by a typomorphological contextualism that took into account the urban dimension of the architectural intervention. Themes
such as “continuity,” “insertion” and “integration” were
typical of the era. For proponents of this approach, urban identity was a matter of reprise (a French word for
mending and recycling) or emulation of the characteristics of earlier eras or of the “existing” built form. Urban
continuity consisted in “fitting in” and in preserving the
traces of existing structures, whether manifest or hidden
Refereed Full-Papers
(evolving around the idea of a palimpsest). From the
1980s onward, Montreal’s projects were praised and
rewarded precisely for their integration into the existing context. Vaguely based on Charney’s paradigm and
that of supporters of historical conservation, critics
used Montrealness as a generic attribute without further
exploring its figures. Meanwhile, a rather restricted idea
of Montrealness became codified and is now pervasive
in Montreal’s municipal bylaws, perhaps owing to
former Urban Architecture Unit graduates hired by the
city throughout the years. The intention to recycle the
existing characteristics captured in charts and laid down
in regulations is being strongly implemented.
Today, in spite of the regulatory framework, is this
codification being transgressed in some recent projects
that are successfully updating the very concept of Montreal’s ideational landscape?
Our studies have revealed that in recycling such a
codified identity, architects and urban designers have
stood up to the contemporary challenge and have come
up with a new blend of neo-modern and post-modern
attitudes. Before we point out some brief examples of
this in Montreal, let us discuss a couple of precedents
from elsewhere.
An inspirational example is Christian de Portzamparc’s redevelopment of the Parisian Left Bank neighbourhood around the new Bibliothèque Nationale de
France. In the late 1990s, he devised an interesting
urban regulation tool he calls l’ilot ouvert (the open
urban block). Based on his hybrid urban model, suitable
for what he calls the Third Age of Cities, he makes
reference to the 20th century heritage that includes both
progressist and cultural approaches to city form.
He rejects both the classical concept of contiguous
buildings that shape the streetscape and the total independence of buildings from the street. His model for
recycling the idea of the typical Parisian urban fabric
is in fact a hybrid that lies somewhere between, on one
hand, Le Corbusier’s ideal of fresh air and natural light
for most individual dwellings on one hand, and on the
other hand, the 19th century’s idealized integrity of
the street as a living urban civic space. In other words,
he refers to the two paradigms that either coexist or
oppose one other everywhere today: the “culturalist”
paradigm in which the city is understood as an urban
fabric—referring to the classical culture of the “active
void” between buildings—and the modern “progressist”
paradigm in which the city can be considered the sum
of buildings as autonomous isolated objects.
We can also consider another “learning from” mani-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
festo that might be useful to revisit today. Rem Koolhaas’ Delirious New York1—a sort of “Learning from
New York” published in 1978, seven years after Charney’s “Learning” and six years after Venturi and ScottBrown’s—is concerned with the volumetric space of the
urban streetscape combined with modernist free-standing high-rise buildings. In the image “The City of the
Captive Globe”, Koolhaas and Zoe Zenghelis imagine
the possibility of creating a continuous space of streets
shaped by an alignment of urban blocs punctuated by a
collection of “metropolitan” buildings’ free-reference
shapes. In contrast to what Trevor Boddy describes as
Vancouverism as a model, Koolhaas’s study is not an
example of how to build elsewhere. Instead, it inspires
as a method of investigation through tools proper to architects and urban designers: the project as both instrumental know-how and as knowledge or, in other words,
as both task-oriented thinking and critical thinking.
Throughout our research, we have been fascinated
by examples of projects everywhere that recycle the
existing characteristics of urban landscape (an attitude based on typomorphology) while hybridizing
with a neo-modernist freedom of invention within the
contemporary paradigm. These projects may subsequently be recognized by critics and observers of urban
architecture for their contribution to a city’s specific
character, thus changing the expected image codified in
the ideational landscape while embracing the ideal of
urban continuity in time and in space. In doing so, these
projects do in fact succeed in reinventing and updating
the urban identity of a particular city. Their impact on
this shifting identity is achieved through a serial effect,
as other designers and architects become aware of the
revised ideational characteristics and use them. In this
manner, a process that can thus be called “learning from
Montreal” takes place.
Meanwhile, rather than merely identifying these
seminal projects, our research was concerned with the
mechanisms intrinsic to the design methods used and
the transformation of the ideational characteristics (in
our case, features of Montrealness) that speak of a
specific urban identity of built form (the Montrealness
of Montreal).
“Learning From” as a Design Method
We have observed that this hybrid attitude—which
appears to be developing concurrently with other attitudes in architecture, some of which are much less concerned with physical context or local identity—seems
to have appeared simultaneously in many places (Paris,
205
Refereed Full-Papers
Singapore, Vancouver, Montreal).
I am tempted to call this observed trend a “critical
contextualism” which is specifically concerned with
the urban landscape, in contrast to Kenneth Frampton’s
well-known critical regionalism, which seems to rely
rather on examples from regional architecture or isolated urban buildings.
In Montreal, I have documented examples of such
hybrid updating of Montrealness. They show how the
already codified Montrealness is recycled in new and
unexpected ways. This would be the second concept
mentioned earlier: the updating process as we were
able to observe it in the “genetic” material (drawings,
models, notes) of “delinquent” projects: delinquent visà-vis the expected, the codified image of the city’s built
form at a particular time. In this case, “learning from”
becomes truly a design method, an emerging paradigm
among design postures that refer to the physical context
of projects to be built.
Let us briefly examine a few examples of the features
of Montrealness which were produced to implement
this new paradigm. One such feature would be a type of
urban contingency new to Montreal. In some seminal
projects that have recently shaped the new face of the
Quartier international de Montréal—namely Daoust
Lestage’s much praised Centre CDP Capital2, set in the
newly revitalized urban project Le Quartier international de Montréal, also designed by the team, and the
new addition to the Palais des congrès de Montréal,
designed by a consortium lead by Saia Barbarese Topuzanov3—we can observe the emergence of a particular
blending of the modernist model of layered segregation
which gave birth to Montreal’s underground city as it
developed from the Place Ville-Marie project in the
1960s (Figure 1) with the traditional model of horizontal organic contingency of public and semi-public
pedestrian space which is characteristic of the historical urban fabric typical of Montreal up to the mid-20th
century. In line with the modernist ideal (Figure 2), as
Vanlaethem puts it (Vanlaethem, 2004), of verticality
(the tower, as a built object, surrounded by open space)
and three-dimensional urban space (a base of infrastructures performing at the level of the urban block and the
region, including its depressed and elevated highways
and the underground networks of its indoor city) which
prevailed during Montreal’s Urban Renewal episode,
the neighbourhood of the future Quartier international
was thoroughly devastated, mainly by the construction
of the Ville-Marie Expressway (Figure 3). As Daoust
Lestage set out to mend and reconstruct the public
206 realm in this very central part of the city, they acknowledged both these contradictory traditions in their hybrid
proposal, which combines the modernist ideal of multilayered verticality with the typomorphological ideal of
spatial continuity of the public realm indoors and in, under and above ground level, in order to provide the city
with alternative pedestrian itineraries that are intertwined
with the fabric of streets and sidewalks linked to the
underground city (Figure 4). A similar interest in urban
connectivity seems to be occurring in some recent projects elsewhere, such as in the Singapore “landscrapers.”
Thus, the indoor city, a distinctive feature of Montreal,
is complemented by underground passages or alternative
paths that pass through buildings, sometimes at groundfloor level (Palais des congrès), sometimes across
buildings suspended, as veritable bridges, above former
streets that have been reconfigured (a linear hall, the
“Parquet”, runs through the Centre CDP Capital from
east to west, forming a sort of hyphen connecting two
urban squares, Figure 5).
This variety of pedestrian itineraries creates the
sort of relationship between outdoor public space and
indoor public (or semi-public) space so essential to the
typomorphological approach from which the paradigm
inspired by Melvin Charney has emerged. One of the
founding images of this approach, Gianbattista Nolli’s
1748 plan of Rome, an analytic horizontal view of the
solids and voids of the city, which was rediscovered and
adopted by the neo-rationalist proponents of typomorphology4, exemplifies the typomorphological ideal of
organic horizontal contingency. Initiated in this project,
the typomorphological ideal was successfully blended
with the modernist model of layered segregation. As
this idea is being used in other projects, a new feature of
Montrealness, genetically linked to Montreal’s imagery
of identity, is being borne.
Another feature of Montrealness is the reference to
the emblematic Montreal greystone in the use of contemporary materials and technologies in curtain walls.
In contrast to the glass façades of the 1960s, which were
widely criticized for their lack of integration into the
existing traditional urban landscape, some designers
and architects are reinventing contemporary tectonics to
provide continuity with Montreal’s typical stone façades
(Figure 6) in terms of a greystone-like stereotomic quality of shadows, textures and thickness. These recent projects demonstrate a rich plasticity of surface, a tectonic
quality that distinguishes them from the first generation
of glass curtain walls. An inspirational example is again
Daoust Lestage’s Centre CDP Capital. According to
Refereed Full-Papers
the designers, and as is apparent in the design drawings, the concern with using an innovative construction system—a double glass wall that was developed
specifically for this project and provides exceptional
thermal performance—and adapting it to the idea of
the Montrealness of stone walls can be considered an
example of an investment in identity through invention.
Montreal greystone, which is both a material and an
imagery of identity, can be considered “translated” into
a contemporary expression.
Indeed, according to Éric Gauthier, one of the
architects of the project, the intention was to use this
articulated curtain wall to make reference to the details
of a stone wall by creating a kind of effect of thickness (Figure 7). The changing lines of shadows reveal
projections and hollows, the play of thicknesses within
the double glass wall, of transparency and of relief.
An effect of grain, of texture and grey colour is thus
achieved, enhanced by the use of strips of greystone
cladding. All of these tectonic impressions refer back to
the craft of the stonecutter.
So after its frequent use in the 1960s and 1970s,
the all-glass curtain wall is back to Montreal. Will a
second generation be more concerned with preserving
the memory of a built environment consisting predominantly of masonry? And will this concern be expressed
as a contemporary translation of a generic Montreal
characteristic rather than as an imitation, as is the case
with artificial stone clad façades? A careful look at the
contemporary architectural production in Montreal does
point to a sort of a series effect of this genetic feature of
Montrealness.
Our research has yielded these and other examples
of such contemporary transfer of ideational characteristics that are a translation rather than an imitation of
Montreal’s existing cityscape. Here, the mechanism
involves an abstracted idea of Montrealness rather than
any specific architectural vocabulary of forms and ways
to build. A common aspect of this emerging paradigm
is the hybridization or cross-fertilization between the
modern and classical paradigms. While keeping with
the modern heritage that shaped Montreal’s identity
as a modern metropolis decades ago, architects and
designers are drawing on references within the evolving universal culture and simultaneously making a clear
reference to the city’s classical or traditional heritage.
A fascinating example of this hybridization is found
in another aspect of Daoust Gauthier’s Centre CDP
Capital project. We refer to the evolution of the crowning of a building. Since the completion of this project,
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
many recent buildings mark a return to the classical idea
of tripartite composition: base, body, crowning (Figure
8). While this feature is part of the recent universal
architectural culture, it resonates with the continuity of
Montreal’s character: most historical buildings surrounding the new projects in the Quartier international
de Montréal do in fact follow this principle (Figure
9). However, a closer look reveals that the roof-level
strategies of these new projects have more in common
with the idea of the fifth façade, or “living on the roof”:
one of the most striking images of the new and the
modern which was so important to the Modern Movement from the outset5. In contrast, consider an example
of a recycling of architectural features which lacks the
updating that would ideally take place: a project from
the 1990s which merely imitates Victorian roof shapes
to top a contemporary high-rise hotel on the edge of Old
Montreal (Figure 10).
This feature of modernity has slowly made its way in
Montreal’s urban landscape since the 1970s, when the
initial attempts to domesticate rooftops appeared in the
form of ad hoc sundecks and eventually more formal
rooftop additions and penthouses, which later developed
into genuine crowning elements that involved the entire
volumetric composition of the building. The whole
concept of the fifth façade points to other future developments such as the increased popularity of the idea of
“green roofs” and the “Urban Agriculture” movements.
We hope to have shown how our research reveals
design methods that recycle and/or update the idea of
Montrealness. In this sense, “learning from Montreal” is
part of a design process that draws upon the talent and
imagination of architects and designers who virtually
create the sense of place in their projects by contributing to the identity of the place called Montreal. Heirs of
post-World War II project methodologies that recycle
the semantic value of the built environment in new
buildings and urban improvement schemes and take into
account the context in which they will be set, these projects contribute to the urban identity of the built environment through the recognition of the imagery of identity
which they convey. While identity does rely on a good
measure of continuity in time and in space, nevertheless
these architects do not adhere to merely retrospective
attitudes. In the approach that underlies their projects,
will there be a meeting of the minds to overcome the
apparently irreconcilable opposition between supporters of the recent enthusiasm for the neo-modern option,
indifferent as it often is to context, and supporters of the
realism of practices that look to the conservationist op-
207
Refereed Full-Papers
tion to provide a sense of perennity and urban continuity?
Thierry Paquot (2005) —a French scholar at the
Institut d’urbanisme de Paris, a prolific scientific writer
and the editor of the magazine Urbanisme— reminds us
that in this age of globalization of urbanism, it is more
than ever important to investigate the links between
professional practice (architecture, urban design, urban
planning) and the culture, identity, and history of a
place and its inhabitants. In an era of reconfiguration of
identities, it seems to us that in updating codified identities the contemporary challenge is to produce a new
amalgam of the modern and the post-modern heritage.
Some illustrations (many more will be added to the
presentation on screen)
Figure 1 The modernist model of layered
segregation which gave birth to Montreal’s
underground city as it developed from the Place
Ville-Marie project (architects I.M. Pei and Ass.,
with Affleck, Desbarats, Dimakopoulos, Lebensold,
arch., 1958–65). Place Ville Marie, was developed
as an entire city block by Canadian National Railway
Company Real Estate. It confirmed the importance
of Montréal’s below-ground pedestrian walkway
system, which had its origins in CN’s Central Station.
(Source: Pei Cobb Freed & Partners Architects
website [http://www.pcf-p.com/a/p/5503/s.html]
accessed on March 4, 2005)
208 Figure 2 The modernist ideal of verticality (the
tower, as a built object, surrounded by open space)
and three-dimensional urban space (a base of
infrastructures performing at the level of the urban
block and the region, including its depressed and
elevated highways and the underground networks)
as illustrated in Ludwig Hilberseimer’s 1924
watercolour Cité verticale: vue en perspective, rue
nord-sud from the collection of The Art Institute of
Chicago, a gift of George E. Danforth (Source: Ruth
Eaton, 2001©:177)
Figure 3 During Montreal’s Urban Renewal episode,
the neighbourhood of the future Quartier international
de Montréal was thoroughly devastated, mainly
by the construction of the Ville-Marie Expressway
from 1966 on. The Ville-Marie Expressway was
part of the site for the Centre CDP Capital project.
Daoust Lestage’s building would eventually span the
depressed expressway. (Source: Daoust Lestage,
architecture et design urbain archives)
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 5 A linear hall, the “Parquet,” runs through the
Centre CDP Capital from east to west, forming a sort
of hyphen connecting two urban squares. Drawing by
N.O.M.A.D.E. (Source: Daoust Lestage, architecture
et design urbain archives)
Figure 4 Inspired by the idea of the palimpsest,
Daoust Lestage’s urban improvement project for the
Quartier international de Montréal set out to mend
and reconstruct the public realm in this very central
part of the city by combining the modernist ideal of
multilayered verticality with the typomorphological
ideal of spatial continuity of the public realm
indoors and in, under and above ground level, in
order to provide the city with alternative pedestrian
itineraries that are intertwined with the fabric of
streets and sidewalks linked to the underground city.
Illustrated here in a drawing titled Les éléments de
la composition urbaine: interventions souterraines
(Source: Le projet du quartier international
de Montréal, Volet aménagement, Esquisse
préliminaire, June 1999, Gauthier Daoust Lestage
Inc. – Provencher Roy & Ass. Architecture et design
urbain. Illustrtation by N.O.M.A.D.E.)
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Figure 6 Montreal’s typical greystone façades feature
a stereotomic quality of shadows, textures and
thickness. (Source: photo Alena Prochazka)
209
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 7 Daoust Lestage’s Centre CDP Capital
features an articulated curtain wall that makes
reference to the details of stone walls by creating a
kind of effect of thickness. (Source: Daoust Lestage,
architecture et design urbain archives)
Figure 8 Daoust Gauthier’s Centre CDP Capital
project is an example of the evolution of the crowning
of Montreal’s most recent buildings. Since the
completion of this project, most landmark buildings
have followed this trend. While this feature is part of
the recent universal architectural culture, it resonates
with the continuity of Montreal’s character.
210 Figure 9 The 1912 Unity Building designed by David
Jerome Spence for the Unity Building Company is
neighbouring Doaust Lestage’s project in the Quartier
international de Montréal. (Source : Ville de Montréal
Heritage Website [http://patrimoine.ville.montreal.
qc.ca/prix/speciaux03.htm#wawanessa] accessed on
March 15 2006)
Figure 10 Centre de commerce mondial de Montréal,
designed in 1991 by Provencher Roy architectes
(Source: Daoust Lestage, architecture et design
urbain archives)
Refereed Full-Papers
NOTES
1
This work (Koolhaas, 1990) presents Koolhaas’s theory that
“Manhattan has generated its own metropolitan Urbanism–a
Culture of Congestion…” and suggests the hypothesis that “the
metropolis needs/deserves its own specialized architecture, one
that can vindicate the original promise of the metropolitan condition and develop the fresh traditions of the Culture of Congestion
further…,” p. 242.
2
Designed by the Consortium Gauthier, Daoust Lestage inc. /
Faucher Aubertin Brodeur Gauthier / Lemay et associés.
3
Designed by the Consortium TDS inc.: Les architectes Tétreault
Parent Languedoc et assoc., Saia Barbarese Topouzanov Architectes, Les architectes Dupuis, Dubuc et associés (Ædifica) and
Hal Ingberg Architect as independent consultant.
4
Inspired by the work of the Italian neo-rationalists and taken
up again by Castex and Panerai (the French School) as well as
by the School of Montreal, urban typomorphology as a design
approach has remained active under the auspices of the group
around the magazine Urban Morphology, along with other European partners, Michael Conzen and Anne Vernez-Moudon.
5
The idea of the fifth façade or “living on the roof” was suggested
to le Corbusier by the FIAT racetrack in Torino as he published it,
speaking of his fascination with industrial architecture, in his Vers
une architecture (Paris: Arthaud , 1977). In fact, Le Corbusier’s
1930 terrace apartment on the roof of an apartment building
in Paris, commissioned by a wealthy client named Charles de
Beistegui is one of the very first Modern Movement projects featuring living on the roof. In this, Le Corbusier was took also inspiration from Mediterranean vernacular architecture.
References
Andrieux, Jean-Yves « Préface », dans Capucine Lemaître et
Benjamin Sabatier (dir.) (2008), Patrimoines : fabrique,
usages et réemplois, Québec, Éditions MultiMondes,
Collection Cahiers de l’Institut du patrimoine de l’UQAM,
no 6, p. iix-x.
Andrieux, Jean-Yves et Fabienne Chevallier (dir.) (2005), La
réception de l’architecture du mouvement moderne : Image,
usage, héritage, Saint-Étienne, Publications de l’Université
de Saint-Étienne.
Balschaw, Maria et Liam Kennedy (dir.) (2000), Urban Space
and Representation, Londres, Pluto Press.
Bilodeau, Denis (1997), Precedents and Design Thinking in an
Age of Relativisation. The Transformations of the Normative
Discourse on the Orders of Architecture in France between
1650 and 1793, Delft, Delft Baukunde Publicatie.
Boddy, Trevor (2004), “New Urbanism: ‘The Vancouver
Model’”, Places, Form and Territory, 16.2, Berkeley.
Boddy, Trevor (2003), ‘Vancouverism and its Discontents:
Feature Review of Dream City by Lance Berelowitz and
Vancouver Walking by Meredith Quartermain’, Vancouver
Review Magazine, Vancouver, BC, June.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Boyer, Marie-Christine (2001 [1994]), The City of Collective
Memory: Its Historical Imagery and Architectural
Entertainments, Cambridge (Mass.), MIT Press.
Castells, Manuel (1997), The Power of Identity, The Information
Age: Economy, Society and Culture, Vol. II. Cambridge, MA;
Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Chassay, Jean-François et Bertrand Gervais (dir.) (2002), Les
lieux de l’imaginaire, Montréal, Liber.
Charney, Melvin (1971), “Pour une définition de l’architecture
au Québec”, a presentation at J.A. de Sève Architecture et
urbanisme au Québec conference, later published by Les
Presses de l’Université de Montréal, 1971, 11-42. A shorter
version also published as “Towards a Definition of Quebec
Architecture”, Progressive Architecture, vol. 53, no 9 (Sept.
1972), 104-107.
Charney, M. (1980), “The Montrealness of Montreal. Formations
and Formalities in Urban Architecture”, The Architectural
Review, n° 999, May, 299-302.
Corboz, André (2001), Le Territoire comme palimpseste et autres
essais, préface de Sébastien Marot, Besançon, Éditions de
l’Imprimeur.
de Biasi, Pierre-Marc. « Pour une approche génétique en
architecture », dans Pierre-Marc de Biasi et Réjean Legault
(dir.) (2000), Genesis. Manuscrits. Recherche. Invention :
revue internationale de critique génétique, no 14, Paris, Jean
Michel Place et Montréal, Centre Canadien d’Architecture,
p. 13-66.
Dear, Michael J. (ed.) (2001), From Chicago to L.A. : Making
Sence of Urban Theory, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Eaton, Ruth (2001), Cité idéales, L’utopisme et l’environnement
(non) bâti, Bibliothèque des Amis du Fonds Mercator,
Anvers, Belgium: Fonds Mercator and Ruth Eaton.
Koolhaas, Rem (1978), Delirious New York: a retroactive
manifesto for Manhattan, New York: Oxford University
Press.
Martin, Louis (1995), « De l’école à la ville : la naissance d’une
École de Montréal », Architecture-Québec ARQ, no 83,
Febuary, Montreal, 10-11.
Paquot, Thierry (2005), Lettre de recommendation, an
unpublished manuscript.
Paquot Thierry and Patrick Baudry (eds) (2003), L’urbain et ses
imaginaires, Pessac, France : MSH.
Pinson, Daniel (dir.) (2008), Métropoles au Canada et en
France : dynamiques, politiques et cultures, Rennes, Presses
universitaires de Rennes.
Vanlaethem, France (2004), “Le centre-ville moderne de
Montréal,” talk given at the ACFAS meeting in Montréal,
May, unpublished.
211
Refereed Full-Papers
Environmental Values and
Their Relationship to Ecological
Services
Herbert W. Schroeder (USDA Forest Service,
Northern Research Station)
Abstract
Natural environments give rise to value in diverse
ways for different people and groups. In this paper, I
present a conceptual scheme for how different kinds
of value may arise from a natural environment. I use
this scheme to illustrate the different ways in which
people value natural environments and to clarify the
relationship between environmental values and ecological services. The concept of ecological services calls
attention to beneficial ecological functions of natural
environments that have historically been neglected
because they are not priced or traded in markets. When
first introduced, the idea of ecological services focused on certain biological and physical services that
are important for human well-being, but that often go
unrecognized in environmental design and management. As the concept has gained in popularity, however,
there has been a tendency to broaden its definition, so
that the definition of ecological services now commonly
includes all forms of environmental values, from tangible products to cultural, aesthetic, and even spiritual
values. Although some of the ways in which value
arises from natural environments involve services that
nature provides for people, people also may value an
environment (or some part of it) for its own sake, even
if it performs no services for them. Thus, it is inappropriate to equate environmental values with ecological
services. The concept of ecological services contributes
to environmental design and management by giving a
more complete account of how nature benefits human
well-being, including ways that have heretofore been
overlooked in society’s decisions about natural environments. Attempting to understand all environmental
values in terms of ecological services, however, may
lead us to misunderstand or leave out of our thinking
other important ways in which people value natural
environments.
212 Introduction
Natural environments give rise to value in diverse
ways for different people and groups. Various systems
and models have been proposed for identifying, cataloging, and considering these values in environmental decisions. Recently, increasing attention has been focused
on the idea of ecological services (also called ecosystem
services or environmental services; see for example
Daily, 1997; Farber, Costanza, & Wilson, 2002; National Research Council, 2004). There does not appear to
be consensus on precisely what this term means, but the
general intent seems to be to provide a fuller accounting
of the contributions that natural systems make to human
well-being, in a form compatible with the economic
criteria that dominate public and private decision-making in the modern world. By doing this, it is hoped that
environmental design and management decisions will
give appropriate consideration to essential ecological
functions that have historically been neglected because
they are not priced or traded in markets.
Because so much of decision-making about environments and land use in modern society is driven
or constrained by economic considerations, framing
environmental values in economic terms seems like a
logical approach to getting them included in society’s
decision-making process. Indeed, advocates for protecting natural environments and incorporating natural
features into the design of built settings have been eager
to embrace research showing that natural features and
systems provide tangible services that can be measured
and assigned an economic value. Despite the current enthusiasm for ecological services, however, I wish to argue for taking a broader view of environmental values,
even when doing so does not lend itself to an economic
assessment of benefits. Value is a complex concept
that relates to many different aspects of human experience and behavior. It may be difficult or impossible to
encompass all the relevant senses of the word “value”
into a single analytical or conceptual framework. Taking
a broader view of values can help us to keep in mind
aspects of the human-nature relationship that might be
overlooked in an analysis of ecological services, and to
grapple with the complexities and ambiguities that often
arise in discussions of environmental values.
In this paper, I describe a conceptual scheme for
thinking about how different kinds of value may arise
from a natural environment. I use this scheme to discuss some of the different ways in which people value
natural environments and to examine the relationship
between environmental values and ecological services.
Refereed Full-Papers
Pathways to environmental value
The word “value” is defined in different ways in
different disciplines (Brown, 1984). Readers wishing
an introduction to the literature on values in natural
resource management are referred to review articles
by Dietz, Fitzgerald, and Shwom (2005), Farber et
al. (2002), and Satterfield and Kalof (2005). For the
purposes of this essay, I approach the concept of value
in terms of human experience. That is, I understand
“value” to mean the feeling or sense of importance,
worth, or significance that something has for someone.
Figure 1 illustrates several different ways in which
an experience of value may arise from a natural environment. The large rectangle on the left side of the
figure represents an environment, with the perimeter of
the rectangle corresponding to the environment’s spatial
boundaries. The smaller boxes located inside the large
rectangle stand for things or processes that are part
of or that take place within the environment, and the
boxes located outside the rectangle stand for processes
or things that can take place outside the boundaries of
the environment (although they might occur within it as
well). Value is experienced at the endpoints of different pathways that begin within the environment. When
a symbol indicating value lies inside the rectangle,
it means that a person must be physically within the
Figure 1. Pathways to ecosystem value.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
environment to experience that value. When the value
symbol lies outside the rectangle, it means that a person
may experience that value when they are outside the environment’s boundaries as well as when they are within
it. The boxes for “design” and “management” at the top
of the figure reflect how the values associated with an
environment are influenced by human interventions and
modifications.
Natural environments are a source of physical materials, which can be transported out of the environment
and used in various ways. These materials may be of
value when directly consumed, or they may be used to
manufacture products that people value. In the diagram,
this is represented by the pathway leading to value-1.
In terms of the user’s experience, value-1 is directly
associated with the materials or products at their final
point of use, not with the environment from which they
came. A person does not have to be in the environment
to experience this value, and may not even know where
the materials to make the valued product came from.
For example, a person can experience the value of owning and living in a house without stopping to think that
some of the materials to build the house and its furnishings came from trees and other plants harvested from a
natural environment.
A second pathway begins with biological and physical processes taking place within the boundaries of
the environment. These may lead to outcomes (that is,
changes in the state of the world) that people value or
that positively affect their quality of life. Some of these
outcomes may extend beyond the boundaries of the
environment, for example improved air and water quality in the surrounding region, buffering of storm surges,
and mitigation of global warming (Daily, 1997). This is
represented by value-2 in the diagram. A person’s experience of this kind of value is directly associated with
these final outcomes, rather than with the environments
in which the outcomes originated. As with value-1, a
person may experience this kind of value without actually being in the environment and may not be aware
of where or how the valued outcome arose from the
environment. For example, a resident of a flood-prone
region may enjoy the benefit of a dry basement without
being aware that flood water retention in the watershed
upstream from their home helped to create that outcome.
A person’s experience of both value-1 and value-2
occurs one or more steps removed from the environment in which the pathway leading to that value
213
Refereed Full-Papers
originates. A person may experience these two kinds
of value without being involved in any direct contact
with the environment itself. Value-3, by contrast, occurs only when a person is within the environment and
is engaged in some form of direct interaction with it.
Value-3 represents the enjoyment or appreciation that
occurs during the person’s immediate experience of that
interaction. The interaction may involve physical activity and movement, such as kayaking on a river or riding
a bicycle on a forest trail, or it may be less physically
active and more focused on the perceptual experience
of the environment, for example the aesthetic appreciation of a scenic landscape. In either case, the value
arises in the person’s direct experience of their interaction with the environment, which occurs while they are
physically within it.
Value-4 also has its origin in a person’s direct interaction with the environment. In this case, however, the
value derives from subsequent psychological, social, or
physiological outcomes of this interaction, which might
not occur until after the person has left the environment.
For example, as a consequence of walking on a trail in
a park, a person may experience improved cardiovascular health, which improves their quality of life beyond
just those occasions when they are recreating in the
park. Time spent in natural environments may help a
person to recover from mental fatigue (Kaplan, 1995)
and may reduce symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity syndrome (Kuo & Taylor, 2004), which may
bring subsequent improvements in their performance at
work or at school. Other people might also benefit from
outcomes of a person’s contact with a natural environment. For example, an increase in job performance
may mean higher productivity and more profits for the
person’s employer. A reduction in mental fatigue may
make a person less irritable and more enjoyable for
their family and friends to be with. As with value-1 and
value-2, a person’s experience of value-4 is directly
associated with these subsequent outcomes and not
with the environment itself. A person might experience
this value without being aware that it is a result of their
(or someone else’s) earlier interaction with a natural
environment.
Value-5 stems from the many ways in which the
experience of being in a natural environment or a place
can evoke meanings that are important for a person.
Examples of such meanings might include sense of
place (Williams & Stewart, 1998), community identity,
and knowledge of the natural, historical, or cultural
214 aspects of a place. Value-5 also includes meanings that
a place or environment has for a person when they are
not currently in it, such as memories from visiting a
place in the past, family connections to a particular area,
or symbolic meanings associated with a particular kind
of environment or land use, such as wilderness (Cole,
2005). Meanings are readily communicated from one
person to another and can be formed based on indirectly
obtained knowledge about an environment, for example
by reading about it, hearing friends tell about it, or seeing it in movies or on TV. Thus, to experience value-5,
a person does not necessarily need to have ever been in
the environment.
Relationship between environmental values and
ecological services
The diversity and complexity of ways in which
value can arise from a natural environment leads to the
question of how the different value pathways in Figure
1 relate to the notion of ecological services. When the
idea of ecological services first began to appear, the
discussion focused mainly on biological and physical
functions of natural environments that are important for
human well-being, but which often go unrecognized.
In a National Research Council (1999) report about
the values of natural systems, for example, ecosystem
services were included as one heading under the general
category of biological values. Ecosystem services in
that report included things like water purification, erosion control, pollination, and protection of coastal zones
from storm damage. Ecosystem services were seen as a
special subset of biological values, while social and cultural values were treated as a separate class of values.
But as the concept of ecological services has gained
in popularity, there has been a tendency to broaden its
definition, so that now many people define ecological services as an all-inclusive category that covers
all forms of environmental values, from production of
tangible products to social, cultural, aesthetic, and even
spiritual values. For example, the Millennium Ecosystem Assessment (2003), following the lead of De Groot,
Wilson, and Boumans (2002), includes four broad
categories of ecosystem services, ranging from products
such as food, fresh water, and fuel to “cultural services”
such as spiritual and religious benefits, aesthetics, inspiration, sense of place, and cultural heritage. Thus, the
tendency now is to subsume all forms of environmental
value under the rubric of ecological services.
A service by definition is an activity performed by
Refereed Full-Papers
someone or something for the benefit of somebody
else. Thus, when we speak of “ecological services,” we
are viewing the environment in terms of how it serves
our desires and needs. Our focus of concern—what we
value—is not the environment itself, but the things that
it does for us. This way of looking at environmental
values seems appropriate when we consider value-1,
value-2, and value-4. For those kinds of value, it is the
products and outcomes derived from the environment
that have value, and as noted above, a person might
experience the value of these services without even
realizing that a natural environment was involved in
creating them. An ecological services approach makes
an important contribution by explicitly identifying
these kinds of values and their linkages to the natural
systems from which they originate.
Some instances of value-3 might also be appropriately conceptualized as ecological services. For
example, for a person engaging in a recreation activity,
the environment may have value because it provides
the necessary conditions and features that allow the activity to be enjoyed. What the person actually values is
the recreation activity, and the environment performs a
service by enabling the activity to occur. In other cases,
however, a person’s immediate experience may involve
a sense of appreciation for the environment (or some
part of it) in and of itself. In this case, the experience of
value is not so much concerned with what the environment can do for the person as with the environment as
an entity that is valued for its own sake. For at least
some people, experiencing aesthetic appreciation for an
environment involves valuing the environment for the
beautiful place that it is rather than for what it does for
the person. To speak of this kind of value in terms of
ecological services may not be an accurate representation of the way in which such people value places and
things that they love.
Similar considerations apply to value-5. In some instances, an environment or place may play an enabling
role in evoking or representing a meaning that is central
to a person’s or group’s sense of identity. For example,
a natural feature such as a tree might take on symbolic
meaning because of a key historical event that occurred
there. If the feature is not valued for its own sake but
functions as a carrier of a valued symbolic meaning,
then it might be appropriate to consider this to be an
ecological service. On the other hand, the meanings
that people find in natural environments and places
sometimes do focus on the environment or place per se,
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
as an entity worthy of admiration for what it is in itself.
A person’s attachment to a natural environment may
involve a sense of respect and obligation that motivates
the person to care for and protect a place for its own
sake. If the term “ecological service” is to be applied to
this situation, then it must be understood more broadly,
to include not only how environments serve the needs
of people, but also how people feel called on to serve
the needs of environments, places, and ecosystems.
The distinction I am making here—between valuing a natural environment for the services it provides
to humans versus valuing it (or some part of it) for its
own sake—is similar but not identical to the distinction
between instrumental and intrinsic value, which is a focal issue in discussions and debates about environmental values and ethics (Light & Rolston, 2003). I am not,
however, arguing that natural environments and systems
have objective value or moral standing independently of
the human beings who value them. I am simply pointing out that people are capable of valuing and caring
about natural environments, places, and things for what
they are in themselves, rather than only valuing them
instrumentally as a source of goods and services (see
Sagoff, 2009).
The fact that people can and do value nature for its
own sake is reflected in several areas of environmental
research. Economists have created the concept of existence or nonuse value to account for the fact that people
may value a natural place or thing even if they never
make any actual use of it (Freeman, 2003; Krutilla,
1967). Research on existence value suggests that it
may be based in part on a moral and ethical concern
toward nature that cannot be captured in economic cost/
benefit analysis (Brookshire, Eubanks, & Sorg, 1986).
In a study of environmental attitudes and beliefs, Stern
and Dietz (1994) drew a distinction between egoistic,
altruistic, and biospherical value orientations, with the
latter defined as a concern for the wellbeing of non-human species or the whole biosphere. Biospheric concern
may be related to a personal sense of empathy with and
inclusion in nature (Schultz, 2000; Schultz, Shriver,
Tabanico, & Khazian, 2004). Studies of ecosystem
restoration volunteers found that they may be motivated
by a strong desire to defend and help threatened natural
organisms and systems that they perceive as struggling
for survival (Grese, Kaplan, Ryan, & Buxton, 2000;
Schroeder, 2000). In the place attachment literature,
Brooks, Wallace, and Williams (2006) suggest that
recreationists develop committed relationships with
215
Refereed Full-Papers
valued places in much the same way as they do with
other people. In all these cases, it appears that natural
environments and organisms are valued and appreciated
for their own sake and not just as a source of services.
Attempting to cast all environmental values as
ecological services could lead us to draw some inappropriate conclusions concerning human interactions with
natural places. For one thing, if what we value are the
services provided by the environment and not the environment itself, then we must consider whether there are
other ways to provide these same services. Perhaps we
can substitute a service provided in one environment
for an equivalent service provided in another environment some distance away—or perhaps we could even
derive the same service from a non-natural source. This
assumption of substitutability is the basis for establishing markets for trading ecological services, for example
carbon credits (Hoover, Birdsey, Heath, & Stout, 2000).
This is a reasonable assumption for carbon sequestration and for many other instances on the value-2 pathway, but it may not be so reasonable for some instances
of value-3 and value-5.
If, for example, we think of sense of place as an ecological service, then we might conclude that a unit of
sense of place in one location can substitute for a unit
of sense of place somewhere else. If a management action diminishes sense of place in one location, then we
might think we could compensate by creating an equal
or greater amount of sense of place at a different location. We might even set up a market for trading sense
of place, analogous to what has been done for carbon
credits. But this runs counter to the very definition of
sense of place, which is place-specific and non-substitutable (Mitchell, Force, Carroll, & McLaughlin, 1993;
Williams, Patterson, Roggenbuck, & Watson, 1992). It
may be inappropriate to think of sense of place as an
ecological service, because it is not the sense of a place
that is valued, but the unique place itself.
The distinction between valuing something for what
it is versus valuing it for what it does for us is not
always clear-cut, however. An awareness of the many
ways in which an environment enhances a person’s life
may, over time, lead the person to have a sense of appreciation for the environment itself, that goes beyond
a simple valuing of the services provided. At the same
time, when a person cares about an environment or a
place for its own sake and not simply out of concern for
their own self-interest, this may create a deeper sense
of meaning and purpose in that person’s experience
216 of their own life. This enhancement of a person’s life
experience arguably could be considered an ecological service. Thus, valuing natural environments for the
services they provide is not mutually exclusive with
valuing them aesthetically, emotionally, or spiritually
for what they are in and of themselves. In a person’s
experience of an environment, these two different ways
of valuing may exist side-by-side, mutually reinforcing
each other. Thinking about all environmental values
only in terms of services, however, could limit the ways
in which we can understand and experience environmental values and the human-environment relationships
on which they are based.
Conclusions
The concept of ecological services makes an important contribution to environmental design and management by increasing awareness of certain ways in which
nature contributes to human well-being—ways that
have often been overlooked in society’s decisions about
natural environments. It is not appropriate, however,
to subsume all the ways in which people value environments under the umbrella of ecological services.
Services represent one way in which value can arise in
the relationship between a person and a natural environment, but it is not the only way. In a person’s relationship with the natural world, as much as in their relationship with another person, the experience of value can
arise out of a sense of gratitude, compassion, justice,
obligation, attachment, identity, respect, or beauty, even
when no service is being performed.
A key question in this regard is whether it is the
environment per se that is valued, versus some product
or outcome provided by the environment. If the latter,
then it seemingly would make no difference whether the
service is provided by the environment or in some other
way. Attempting to encompass all environmental values
in an ecological services framework seems to imply
that natural environments in and of themselves have no
value for people, that their degradation or destruction
need not concern us as long as we have alternative ways
of obtaining the same services at a reasonable cost. This
does not accurately describe the full range of ways in
which people experience and care about nature.
The concept of ecological services seems most useful and relevant for the pathways to value-1, value-2
and value-4 in Figure 1. Much of the current interest
in ecological services does in fact focus on value-2
and value-4, which have hitherto not been adequately
Refereed Full-Papers
recognized in environmental design and management. Research on ecological services is providing the
conceptual and analytical tools we need for identifying
and taking into account these important services. But if
we come to rely on a single concept such as ecological
services to understand all forms of value that arise in
the relationship between humans and nature, we may
misunderstand or leave out of our thinking other important aspects of how people value environments and
places, particularly for value-3 and value-5. By keeping our thinking open-ended and drawing on a broad
range of concepts for environmental values we may be
better able to understand and represent these values in
environmental design and management.
References
Brooks, J. J., Wallace, G. N., & Williams, D. R. (2006).
Place as relationship partner: An alternative metaphor
for understanding the quality of visitor experience in a
backcountry setting. Leisure Sciences 28, 331-349.
Brookshire, D. S., Eubanks, L. S., & Sorg, C. F. (1986).
Existence values and normative economics: Implications
for valuing water resources. Water Resources Research 22,
1509-1518.
Brown, T. (1984). The concept of value in resource allocation.
Land Economics 60, 231-246.
Cole, D. N. (2005). Symbolic values: The overlooked values that
make wilderness unique. International Journal of Wilderness
11(2), 23-27, 10.
Daily, G. (ed.) (1997). Nature’s Services: Societal Dependence
on Natural Ecosystems. Washington, DC: Island Press.
De Groot, R. S., Wilson, M. A., & Boumans, R. M. J. (2002).
A typology for the classification, description and valuation
of ecosystem functions, goods and services. Ecological
Economics 41, 393-408.
Dietz, T., Fitzgerald, A., & Shwom, R. (2005). Environmental
values. Annual Review of Environmental Resources 30, 335372.
Farber, S. C., Costanza, R., & Wilson, M. A. (2002). Economic
and ecological concepts for valuing ecosystem services.
Ecological Economics 41, 375-392.
Freeman, A. M. III. (2003). The measurement of environmental
and resource values: Theory and methods (2nd Ed.).
Washington, DC: Resources for the Future.
Grese, R .E., Kaplan, R., Ryan, R. L., & Buxton, J. (2000).
Psychological benefits of volunteering in stewardship
programs. In P.H. Gobster & R.B. Hull (eds.), Restoring
nature: Perspectives from the social sciences and humanities,
265-280. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Hoover, C. M., Birdsey, R. A., Heath, L. S., & Stout, S. L.
(2000). How to estimate carbon sequestration on small forest
tracts. Journal of Forestry 98(9), 13-19.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an
integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology
15, 169-182.
Krutilla, J. (1967). Conservation reconsidered. American
Economic Review 57, 777-786.
Kuo, F. E. & Taylor, A. F. (2004). A potential natural treatment
for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a
national study. American Journal of Public Health 94, 15801586.
Light, A. & Rolston, H. (eds.). (2003). Environmental Ethics: An
Anthology. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Millennium Ecosystem Assessment. (2003). Ecosystems and
Human Well-Being. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Mitchell, M. Y., Force, J. E., Carroll, M. S., & McLaughlin, W.
J. (1993). Forest Places of the Heart: Incorporating special
spaces into public management. Journal of Forestry 91(4),
32-37.
National Research Council. (1999). Perspectives on Biodiversity:
Valuing Its Role in an Everchanging World. Washington, DC:
The National Academy Press.
National Research Council. (2004). Valuing Ecosystem Services:
Toward Better Environmental Decision-Making. Washington,
DC: The National Academies Press.
Sagoff, M. (2009). Intrinsic value: a reply to Justus et al. Trends
in Ecology and Evolution. 24:643.
Satterfield, T. & Kalof, L. (2005). Environmental values: An
introduction -- relativistic and axiomatic traditions in the
study of environmental values. In L. Kalof & T. Satterfield
(eds.), The Earthscan Reader in Environmental Values, xxixxxiii. London: Earthscan.
Schroeder, H. W. (2000). The restoration experience: Volunteers’
motives, values, and concepts of nature. In P.H. Gobster &
R.B. Hull (eds.), Restoring nature: Perspectives from the
social sciences and humanities, 247-264. Washington, DC:
Island Press.
Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: The effects of
perspective taking on concern for environmental issues.
Journal of Social Issues 56, 391–406.
Schultz, P. W., Shriver, C., Tabanico, J. J., & Khazian, A.
M. (2004). Implicit connections with nature. Journal of
Environmental Psychology 24, 31–42.
Stern, P. C. & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental
concern. Journal of Social Issues 50, 65-84.
Williams, D. R. & Stewart, S. I. (1998). Sense of place:
An elusive concept that is finding a home in ecosystem
management. Journal of Forestry 96(5), 18-23.
Williams, D. R., Patterson, M. E., Roggenbuck, J. W., & Watson,
A. E. (1992). Beyond the commodity metaphor: Examining
emotional and symbolic attachment to place. Leisure
Sciences 14, 29-46.
217
Refereed Full-Papers
Rethinking Architectural
Research
Benyamin Schwarz (University of Missouri)
Abstract
While architectural research is a unique mode of inquiry that needs to inform and guide architectural practice, it should not emulate the natural sciences. Because
scientists and architects work within irreconcilable
paradigms of inquiry, efforts to reconcile them or search
for commonalities may not serve either profession. Attempting to offer substantiated knowledge to architects,
whose occupation requires devising a course of action
under specific conditions where such knowledge does
not and cannot exist, seems to be a futile conduct. The
aim of this paper is to call attention to the central problem of architectural research, namely that research in
this field has to lead to sensible and useful knowledge.
It simply cannot follow in the footsteps of the natural
sciences. Instead, architectural research must pursue its
own paradigm of inquiry and ways to generate practical
knowledge.
Introduction
Current architectural research includes several
spheres: Environmental Research investigates the
physical context of architecture; Cultural Research
studies place-making and norms of the inhabitants of
natural and built places; Social Research examines
the people who inhabit and use the spaces of architecture; Technological Research investigates the physical
materials, methods, element, systems, and science of
architecture and the design and construction processes;
Design Research considers the processes of shaping and
making places; Organizational Research examines the
way in which individuals and teams collaborate in the
practice of architecture and in the client organizations;
and Educational Research examines the pedagogies of
architecture and related fields.
With the exclusion of architectural theory and criticism, which has emerged as an autonomous branch of
the humanities, this comprehensive list is a testament
to the breadth of architectural inquiry on the one hand,
and a demonstration of the confusing assortment of the
types of research in architecture on the other. Located in
several adjacent and sometimes overlapping domains,
architectural research has taken many forms. However,
218 it is noticeable that except for Technological Research,
all the listed branches of research are nestled in the
social sciences. Much of the research conducted in the
field, however, imitates methods and processes that
characterize natural science. Regretfully, a great deal of
this so-called “body of knowledge” finds itself on dusty
shelves in a depository of an academic library instead of
the hands of professional practitioners.
In their attempt to classify and organize the field,
Joroff and Morse (1984) proposed a hierarchical framework for architectural research by placing different
modes of inquiry along a continuum based upon how
systematic they are. Observations of one’s environment—“a random stumbling process, never organized
or recorded in any formal way” (Wampler, 1979)—
lie at one end of their continuum as the most loosely
formed architectural inquiry, and the laboratory model
of research lies on the other end. In between these two
extremes, the authors place the act of design, review of
precedents of current knowledge, manifesto, normative
theory, development, scholarship, and social science
type research. This classification is somewhat vague,
but the authors claim that the lack of a comprehensive
framework that encompasses and integrates various
research activities hinders the field of architectural
research from “systematic acquisition of ‘knowledge’
that may effectively inform practice” (p.16). Joroff
and Morse (1984) struggle with the notion that many
of the categories on their continuum are ambiguous
and confusing. For example, they acknowledge that
using design as a research tool may be self-defeating
because the artifacts of architectural design are specific
solutions, applicable in particular context at a specific
time, whereas “the artifacts of science are generalizations, theories, or laws of nature from which empirically
testable propositions can be deduced” (Krippendorff,
2006, p. 29). Despite the inherent differences between
scientific research and architectural inquiry, there have
been numerous attempts to frame architectural research
in the context of natural sciences. These attempts have
failed. Architecture still has no commonly shared body
of knowledge, and the lack of consensus about what architectural research is continues to plague the field. We
can see the problem—architectural research has not had
the kind of success in developing a comprehensive body
of theoretical and methodological knowledge that can
inform practice, like other practice-oriented professions
have had. The solution, however, seems less clear.
Since the establishment of research-based universities
Refereed Full-Papers
in the mid-nineteenth century, architects have looked
up to academia for readily available knowledge that
can be easily applied in their practice. But universities
have found it difficult to balance the training of young
practitioners through increasing demands with the expectations of academic research. In addition, the more
closely architectural researchers try to align themselves
with the traditions of academic research in humanities,
natural, and social sciences, the more they distance
themselves from the real concerns and the agenda of
active professional practitioners (Allen, 2008).
It is the primary thesis of this paper that while scientific research is a unique mode of inquiry that needs
to inform and guide architectural practice, it should not
emulate the natural sciences. Because scientists and architects work within irreconcilable paradigms of inquiry, efforts to reconcile them or search for commonalities
might not serve either profession. Scientific research,
with the theories it produces, “is rooted in past observations, and the predictions it suggests assume that
the patterns found in the past will persist in the future.
This kind of knowledge, celebrated since the Renaissance, is of little interest to designers who are intent to
change the world in ways not predictable from natural
laws” (Krippendorff, 2006, p. 29). Attempting to offer
substantiated knowledge to architects seems futile; their
occupation requires action under far less certain conditions. Consequently, the commonly understood notion
of “architectural research” is an oxymoron. The aim of
this paper is to call attention to the central problem of
research for architecture, namely that research in this
field has to lead to sensible and useful knowledge, but it
simply cannot follow in the footsteps of the natural sciences. Instead, research for architecture must pursue its
own paradigm of inquiry and ways to generate practical
knowledge.
A Short History of Scientific Research and
Architecture
By definition, research is an exploration of uncharted
territories. It involves conscious attempts to make sense
of discoveries in order to place them in the changing
map of knowledge through the development of theory.
The nature of research is based on the identification
of a problem that needs to be investigated while being
associated with answerable questions. Conducted from
an awareness of previous, related inquiry, research is
an inquisitive process that is planned and carried out in
a methodical and rigorous manner. The outcomes are
characterized by testable results, which are accessible to
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
others (Cross, 2006). In a narrow sense, the product of
research is knowledge that describes an existing reality
or truth. This knowledge may or may not be applied in
practice. When the primary purpose of a study is to acquire knowledge for the sake of knowing more, it is often termed basic research. When a research is centered
on practical usefulness, such as making better buildings,
or improving the efficiency of building process, it is
often termed applied research.
Bruce Archer proposed that “research is a systematic inquiry, the goal of which is knowledge” (Archer,
1981). This minimalist definition was proposed at
a time when architectural inquiry was searching for
generalizable methods whose success recommended
wider use, systematic treatment like in other professional inquiries, and teaching methodology. The study
of methodology entered the design discourse in the
1950s. It was originally fueled by the impressive rigor
of natural science. It has been generally recognized that
through the use of science, as opposed to other ways of
inquiry, we may more likely obtain correct answers to
questions and better solutions to specific problems. As
Ackoff (1962) noted:
This is to assert not that better results are always
obtained by science, but that such results are
more likely to be obtained by its use. This follows from the superiority of the scientific process
of inquiry. This superiority of scientific inquiry
derives from the fact that it is controlled. A process is controlled to the extent that it is efficiently
directed toward the attainment of desired objectives (p.3).
In other words, science aims to tell us, and often
succeeds in telling us, what the world is like. And according to some views, a field is scientific when it uses
rigorous methods and delivers results we should trust.
Natural sciences do not ask the same questions as other
branches of knowledge-seeking disciplines. Science
mainly seeks to uncover natural laws, causal and explanatory relations between events and variables while
maintaining an interest in answering a special class of
questions. “The same holds for simplicity and explanatory power. Science is particularly interested in finding
answers to the questions, ‘What theory explains suchand such a set of phenomena?’ ‘What is the simplest
theory that would explain X, Y, and Z?’ and so forth”
(Goldman, 1999, p. 246). Obviously, when questions of
this sort are posed, scientific practices have a significant
219
Refereed Full-Papers
advantage in answering them over any set of non-scientific practices available to human beings.
The success of science has been undeniable. Its
products—technological spinoffs, such as antibiotics, computers, and airplanes—dominate our lives. In
Western society, we assign an astounding credibility
to any claim that is successfully represented as being
scientific. We are so impressed with science that we
tend to ignore the fact that sometimes it can impoverish
our lives or even take them away. Be that as it may, the
major aim of science is to provide us with empirically
adequate theories (van Fraassen, 1980).
It was Thomas Kuhn (1962) who introduced the
philosophy of science to the concept of paradigm. By
his account, a mature science always has a paradigm.
In the narrow sense, a “paradigm” is an “exemplar,”
an influential presentation of scientific theory. In the
Postscript that Kuhn added to a later edition of his
book, he disclosed that in much of his original text
“the term ‘paradigm’ is used in two different senses.
On the one hand, it stands for the entire constellation of beliefs, values, techniques and so on shared by
the members of a given community. On the other, it
denotes one sort of element in that constellation, the
concrete puzzle-solution which, employed as models
or examples, can replace explicit rules as a basis for the
solution of the remaining puzzles of normal science”
(Kuhn, 1970, p. 175). Kuhn used the phrase “normal
science” for scientific work that is well organized and
occurs within the framework provided by a paradigm.
The most significant thing that happens when the work
in an area constitutes normal science is that various
puzzles are solved in a manner of the exemplary work
and scientists agree on how to approach problems, as
well as how to assess possible solutions. However, after
some time, a number of unresolved puzzles occur. As
anomalies multiply, science in that area enters a period
of crisis that is resolved when a new paradigm replaces
the old one (Godfrey-Smith, 2003).
The search for the relationships between architecture
and science has been part of the architectural discourse
for several centuries. Architecture and science were
linked at the very inception of Western tradition. In
the writings of Plato and Socrates, architecture had
the same status as science and was positioned above
practice. Scientia was contemplated as the proportional
order that architecture embodied, not only within a
building ”but as a human situation, in the space-time
experience” (Perez-Gomez, 1999). In the fourteenth
220 century, the art of architecture meant the skill of building, and the science of architecture, techne, meant the
theoretical or scientific root of the skill of building.
What was understood at that time was that the architect
needed both skills: “art,” the skill of practical knowledge, and “science,” or the skill that requires theoretical
knowledge (Ackerman, 1949). The necessity for both
kinds of knowledge in architecture was held up earlier
by Vitruvius who noted in The Education of the Architect:
The architect’s professional knowledge is enriched by contributions from many disciplines
and different fields of knowledge, and all the
works produced by these other arts are subjects
to the architect’s scrutiny. This expertise derives
from practice and theory. Practice consists of the
ceaseless and repeated use of a skill by which any
work to be produced is completed by working
manually with the appropriate materials according
to a predetermined design. Theory, in contrast, is
the ability to elucidate and explain works created
by such manual dexterity in terms of their technical accomplishment and their proportions (25
B.C./2009 p. 4).
However, this state of affairs started to change during
the seventeenth century, when Perrault asserted that
theory was no longer absolute truth, and its purpose was
simply to be as easily “applicable” to guide architectural practice. From the seventeenth century onwards,
an increasing separation was launched between art and
science. The disconnection between the two disjointed
practice from theory, empiricism from reasoning, and
the subjective from the objective—which completed the
schism between the creative arts and the scientific fields
of nature (McCleary, 1984).
Efforts to rationalize the practice of architecture
according to scientific principles became widespread
during the twentieth century (Frampton, 1999). The
concerns about the relationships between science and
design emerged in the 1920s with a search for scientific
products at the inception of the Modern Movement.
Perhaps the most famous manifesto was reflected in the
words of Le Corbusier, who wrote in 1929 about the
house as an objectively designed “machine for living.”
The aspirations of many designers of that generation
were to produce objects of art and design founded on
objectivity and rationality, just like in science. The quest
for scientific design process took another form during
Refereed Full-Papers
the 1960s with the establishment of the design methods movement (Cross, 2006). The movement intended
to improve the quality of design by responding to the
rapidly changing nature of design tasks at that time.
The rapid growth of scientific foundations in disciplines
such as engineering, material science, building science, and behavioral science set this idea in motion and
convinced intellectuals that modern architectural design
had become too complex for intuitive methods. What
resulted was an interest in the type of non-intuitive
methods that stem from scientific knowledge. But the
design methods movement was unsuccessful because
it failed to address what designers actually do. Ironically, some of the pioneers of the movement rejected
the underlying values of the design methodology they
helped to formulate and adopted new, more explicitly
intuitive design approaches. Christopher Alexander,
J. Christopher Jones, and C. Thomas Mitchell—three
of the most prominent pioneers of the design methods
movement—criticized the movement they helped fashion during the 1970s. Despite their earlier interests in
design methodologies, all three have shifted their focus
in favor of more applicable approaches to design over
what Jones called, in 1977, the “machine language, the
behaviorism, the continual attempt to fix the whole of
life into a logical framework.” In an interview with C.
Thomas Mitchell in 1980, Jones said:
We sought to be open minded to make design
processes that would be more sensitive to life than
were the professional practices of the time. But
the result was rigidity: a fixing of aims and methods to produce designs that everyone now feels
to be insensitive to human needs. Another result
was that design methods became more theoretical,
and many of those drawn to the subject turned it
into the academic study of methods (methodology) instead of trying to design things better. The
language used to describe designing, and describe
the aims and purposes of things designed, became
more and more abstract. The words lost touch
with how it feels to be a designer and how it feels
to inhabit system being designed (Mitchell, 1992,
p. xi).
Natural Science and Architectural Inquiry:
The Odd Couple
Whether they follow empiricist, structuralist, or
instrumentalist research models, scientists look for
knowledge and methods that transcend the idiosyn-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
crasies of observations that are made in specific points
in place and time. Generalization and development of
explanatory and predictive theory are the hallmarks of
scientific research. In fact, unique events that cannot
be generalized are of little interest to natural scientists
that would rather believe they do not exist. Predictions
about future events in science always presume that the
theorized properties are stable and unchanged within
theoretical limits. Moreover, scientists are required to
be value-free and detached from the data they intend
to analyze “to assure that scientific findings, theories,
and laws are about an observer-uncontaminated nature”
(Krippendorff, 2008, p. 28).
Natural science requires compliance with at least
three criteria of scientific practice: inter-subjectivity,
reliability, and verifiability in an empirical sense.
Inter-subjectivity, the quality of scientific result, allows
the resulting information to be interpreted by different
people in the same way. Scientific reliability means that
repetition of measurements under unchanged conditions
should render identical results. This needs to be true in
circumstances in which the same investigator judges
a particular situation more than once, and each time
comes to the same conclusion, which is called internal
reliability. In addition it needs to be true in external
reliability, where different investigators judge the same
situation and conclude with the same results. The third
criterion of verifiability means that in order to qualify as
a scientific study, the structure of the study, the data collection, the data analysis, and their interpretation must
be transparent and open for debate and criticism. This
allows other researchers the opportunity to repeat the
study in different times and different places and challenge the results.
Only a fraction of architectural research can meet
these criteria of scientific investigation. Yet scholars in
this field maintain that scientific research can make the
design process and physical settings it produces more
efficient. Many scholars in a variety of branches of
architectural inquiry have repeated these assertions over
the past fifty years. Domains such as the design methods movement, computer-aided design, system building, environment-behavior studies, or most recently
evidence-based design have promised, at different times
and different places, to help architects become more
efficient and cause their buildings to become more effective through the use of scientific procedures.
The field of Environment and Behavior Studies
(EBS) is a case in point. Since its inception in the late
221
Refereed Full-Papers
1960s, those who follow EBS philosophy have believed
that architectural design should be based on an understanding of the intricacies of human and environment
relations, and that design should be shaped by researchbased knowledge. Inherent in this approach to architectural design has been the assumption that studying the
behavior of people in relation to the environment can
elicit knowledge, which can be generalized and applied
in various design contexts. EBS came into existence in
response to various social and political developments of
the late 1960s. A small number of social and behavioral
scientists, along with a few architects, committed to the
need for more socially responsible approaches to planning and design of the environment and attempted to
articulate a new model or paradigm for the design process. Both the need for a theoretical understanding of
the relationship between people and their surroundings
and an immediate, pragmatic concern over mismatches
between people, institutions, communities, and other
designed environments provided impetus for this field.
The motivating force was the premise that knowledge
of the fundamental principles of human behavior helps
designers clarify an understanding of the relationship
between environment and behavior. This in turn, helps
architects consider how the designed environment
can afford people of different backgrounds distinctive experiences and diverse activity patterns. Lang
(1987) claimed that this knowledge enables designers to
understand what they can predict with confidence and
when they are really “going out on a limb.” The practice model promoted by EBS involves two fundamental
steps: gathering research-based social knowledge and
then using the resulting information in a programming
stage that integrates this social knowledge in the design
process. The presumption of this model has been that
if followed, this process would result in buildings that
would better fit the needs of their future occupiers and
users. The last step in EBS practice, a post-occupancy
evaluation, completes the circle by providing the source
for new knowledge to inform the design process for the
next round of projects.
Two kinds of criticism of EBS surfaced over time.
One was aimed at the environmental psychologists in
the domain who have sometimes treated the built environment simply as a relatively incidental stimulus array
rather than as a meaningful environment. The other
criticism targeted the reliance on behavior rather than
cognition as an exclusive outcome of human interaction
with the physical environment. This approach stems
222 from the long-lasting approach of the natural sciences
to inquire into nature as if it existed independently of
its investigators. This approach conflicts with the notion
that architects cannot thrive in operationally constructed
reality because architectural design is concerned with
real people’s interaction with their environment, and
relies on people’s ability to understand their settings and
create their worlds; have insights, intelligence and feelings; and communicate freely with each other as well as
researchers, observers, and critics. The natural science
approach has reduced human beings to billiard balls
moving aimlessly, a version of humans that is a far cry
from the character of people that should be developed
for architectural inquiry.
Studies in environment and behavior continue, as
in other spheres of architectural inquiry. Yet despite
a wealth of confirmed research findings on building
use and human behavior, perception, and emotions in
various environments, the mainstream of architectural
practice and education seems almost unaffected. The
“gap” is still there.
Social Science and Architectural Inquiry
In his book, Making Social Science Matter: Why
social inquiry fails and how it can succeed again, Bent
Flyvbjerg (2001) reviews the history and rationale of
social science and shows “why social science never
has been, and probably never will be, able to develop
the type of explanatory and predictive theory that is the
ideal and hallmark of natural science” (p. 4). He begins
by introducing the Aristotelian concept of phronesis,
which is translated as prudence or practical wisdom.
To quote Aristotle, phronesis is a “true state, reasoned,
and capable action with regard to things that are good
or bad for man” (Aristotle, 1976. Quoted in Flyvbjerg, 2001, p.2). This concept surpasses the analytical,
scientific knowledge (episteme) as well as the technical
knowledge of the know-how (techne) and has to do with
judgments and decisions made by an expert social and
political actor. The introduction of phronesis into the
discussion of what social science is and can be helps
Flyvbjerg describe the predicament of the field when it
attempts to emulate natural science in order to produce
explanatory and predictive (epistemic) theory. Flyvbjerg
argues that;
The two types of sciences have their respective
strengths and weaknesses along fundamentally
different dimensions, a point which Aristotle
Refereed Full-Papers
demonstrated but which has since been forgotten.
At present, social science is locked in a fight it
cannot hope to win, because it has accepted terms
that are self-defeating… in their role as phronesis,
the social sciences are strongest where the natural
sciences are weakest: just as the social sciences
have not contributed much to explanatory and
predictive theory, neither have the natural sciences
contributed to the reflexive analysis and discussion
of values and interests, which is the prerequisite
for an enlightened political, economic, and cultural
development in any society, and which is the core
of phronesis (2001, p. 3).
Flyvbjerg claims that context and judgment are
central to the understanding of human action, however
theory of context and judgment are impossible. Flyvbjerg further maintains that by emulating the natural
sciences, social sciences set for themselves an impossible task in the study of human affairs, especially
in the efforts to develop a theory, which typically is
perceived as the peak of scientific endeavor. Therefore,
he argues, mainstream social science and methodology
are in need of reorientation. Unlike the natural sciences, social sciences do not evolve through scientific
revolutions. Rather, social sciences go through periods
where various constellations of power and waves of
intellectual fashions dominate the field (Dreyfus, 1982).
When change occurs, it may look like a paradigm
shift, though it is actually a process in which researchers within a given area abandon a “dying” wave for a
new one without the accumulation of knowledge as
in Kuhn’s model of normal science. Style changes,
rather than paradigm shifts, characterize social science.
Instead of a case of evolution, social science changes
through fashion substitution.
Most of architectural research goes through similar
changing fashions. The lack of a cumulative body of
knowledge, the missing guiding paradigm or central
theory, and the ineffective attempts of emulating natural
sciences, reflects the similarities with social science. This resemblance is apparent when we compare
architectural inquiry to the natural science conduct as
described by Kuhn (1970). Architectural research takes
place in an unstructured system of discourse, which is
rarely shared by the participants in this environment.
Unlike natural science, the knowledge in architectural
research does not accumulate like building blocks in
a structure we all understand and does not lead to a
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
“body of knowledge.” At best, it leads to a “body of
discourse” (Foucault, 1973).
The recent trends in professional practice, such as
sustainability, new materials, urbanism, and digital
technologies, are changing the architectural agenda.
These new domains promise to become venues for
collaborative research among academicians and practitioners. However, significant research domains of
the recent past, such as Environment and Behavior or
Environmental Gerontology, decline just like fading
fashions.
Conclusions and Implications: Rethinking
Architectural Research
Because architectural research to a large extent
is located in the domain of social science, we need
to make it phronetic research; one that is pragmatic,
context-dependent, and oriented toward action. As
such, architectural research should focus on values and
praxis. It should take its point of departure from the
value-rational questions: “Where are we going? Is it
desirable? What should be done?” (Flyvbjerg, 2001, p.
130). Therefore, to empower architectural inquiry, we
need to do three things. First, we should stop attempting to emulate natural science. Producing cumulative
knowledge and predictive theory does not work in
architectural research. The purpose of architectural
inquiry is to carry out analyses and interpretations in
order to contribute to both professional practice and
society. It may take many forms. Sometimes it may
focus on clarifying. Sometimes it may take a form of
action or intervention, and sometimes it may center on
generating new insights and perspectives. Architectural
research needs to “serve as eyes and ears in our ongoing efforts at understanding the present and deliberating
the future” (Flyvbjerg, 2001, p. 166). Second, we must
address architectural and environmental problems that
matter to the local, national, and global communities
in which we live. Finally, we must effectively communicate our findings to practitioners of architecture and,
through them, to society at large. Results of architectural research have to be approachable to architects and
other designers and have to be communicated in helpful
means. Architectural research needs to be practiced
with an emphasis on the practical before the epistemic
knowledge. Our goal should be to contribute to society’s capacity for forethought and action. We are not
there yet, but we should make every effort to be there.
223
Refereed Full-Papers
References
Ackerman, J. S. (1949). ‘Art since Scientia Nihil Est’: Gothic
theory of architecture at the cathedral of Milan. The Art
Bulletin 31-32: 84-111.
Ackoff, R. L. (1962). Scientific method. New York: Wiley.
Allen, S. (2008). Collaborative, practice-based research. In
N.B. Solomon, (Ed.), Architecture: Celebrating the past,
designing the future. Washington, DC: The American
Institute of Architects.
Archea, J. (1987). Puzzle-making: What architects do when no
one is looking. In Y. E. Kalay (Ed.) Principles of computeraided design: Computability of design. New York: John
Wiley & Sons.
Archer, L. B. (1981). A view of the nature of design research.
In R. Jacques and J. Powel (Eds.) Design: Science: Method.
Guildford, UK: Westbury House.
Aristotle. (1976). The Nicomachean Ethics. Translated by J.A. K.
Thomson. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Cross, N. (2006). Designerly ways of knowing. London, Springer.
Dreyfus, H. (1982). “Why studies of human capacities
modeled on ideal natural science can never achieve their
goal.” Revised edition of a paper presented at the Boston
Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science, October 1982.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2001). Making social science matter: Why social
inquiry fails and how it can succeed again. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Foucault, M. (1973). The order of things: an archeology of the
human sciences. New York: Vintage.
Frampton, K. (1999). The Mutual Limits of Architecture
and Science. In P. Galison and E. Thompson (Eds.) The
Architecture of Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
224 Godfrey-Smith, P. (2003). Theory and reality: An introduction
to the philosophy of science. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
Goldman, A. I. (1999). Knowledge in a social world. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Jones, J. C. (1977). How my thoughts about design methods have
changed during the years. Design Methods and Theories, Vol.
11, No. 1, pp. 50-62.
Joroff, M. L. and Morse, S. J. (1984). A proposed framework for
the emerging field of architectural research. In J. C. Snyder
(ed.) Architectural Research. New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold.
Krippendorff, K. (2006). The semantic turn: A new foundation
for design. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis.
Kuhn, T. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Second
edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (First
published in 1962).
Lang, J. (1987). Creating architectural theory. New York: Van
Nostrand Reinholds.
McCleary, P. (1984). History of technology. In J. C. Snyder (ed.)
Architectural Research. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Mitchell, C. T. (1992). Prefaces to the second edition, 1992. In
J. C. Jones Design Methods (2nd Edition). New York: Van
Nostrand Reinholds.
Perez-Gomez, A. (1999). Architecture as Science: Analogy or
Disjunction? In P. Galison and E. Thompson (Eds.) The
Architecture of Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
van Fraassen, B. (1980). The scientific image. Oxford: Clarendon
Press.
Vitruvius, M. P. 25 B.C. (2009). On architecture. Translated by
R. Schofield. New York: Penguin Books.
Wampler, J. (1979). Watching, Journal of Architectural
Education 32:20 (May 1979).
Refereed Full-Papers
Therapeutically Enhanced
School Design for Students
with Autism Spectrum
Disorders (ASD): A
Comparative Study of the
United States and the United
Kingdom
Ghasson Shabha (Birmingham City University,
UK), Kristi Gaines (Texas Tech University)
Abstract
This paper compares and contrasts the findings of
the empirical studies conducted in both the United
Kingdom and the United States to assess the impact of
sensory environmental stimuli on students’ behaviour in
school buildings.
Analysis of teaching layouts and the sequence of
activities in selected school buildings were conducted.
Opinions of focus groups including teachers and carers
working with students with Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD) were initially explored to assess the extent
of the sensory problems and to highlight any design
limitations and constraints. A questionnaire was formulated based on the feedback gleaned from the focus
groups and subsequently administered to target groups.
Relevant perceptual information about behavioural reactions to varying sensory stimuli was compiled. Visual
information of selected schools, including photos and
drawings of ASD workstations, were gathered, annotated, and analysed for further examination. Several
visual triggers were identified, including bright colours
and light, pattern, and glare. Sound triggers. including
sudden and impact sound, high and low pitch sound,
and background noise levels, were also identified.
There are a few variations between the two studies
regarding particular sensory triggers and their importance, which can be attributed to differences in school
design, layout, internal finishing, and workstation
configuration. Other factors, including the size of the
study sample, location, and climatic differences, are
also implicated. The study provides further understanding of the key factors contributing to the quality of
the teaching environments in school buildings in both
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
the UK and the U.S. This might assist in developing
alternative school design guidelines based on a more
user-centred approach; therefore, creating a responsive
teaching environment that is more humanely attuned to
the needs of affected groups.
Background
Autism is a developmental disorder of neurological
origin that occurs in a large variety of forms, ranging
from mild to severe. It is currently defined by behavioural criteria, which include impairments in social
interaction, impairments in verbal and nonverbal communication, and restricted interests and activities. Other
impairments including lack of facial recognition and
poor social skills have also been identified together with
limited and repetitive gross motor and cognitive skills
(Eldelson, 1995; Jordan & Powell, 1995). Individuals
and adults with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) are
among the most vulnerable groups in our society. This
is due to the overwhelming effects of the sensory built
environment within which they live and the difficulties
of modulating and processing sensory information in
their own surroundings. The latter have been identified
as the main culprit underlying stereotypical and selfstimulatory and ritualistic behaviour exhibited by individuals, which can have profound effects on their learning (Jordan & Powell, 1995; Eldelson, 1995; Williams,
1996; Waltz, 1996; Hatch-Rasmussen, 1997). Individuals can be described as hyposensitive or hypersensitive
to environmental stimuli; they engage in this behaviour
either to block out or activate sensory stimuli.
Grandin (1995) argued that treatment and awareness of sensory problems are often overlooked in many
educational programmes, claiming that stereotypical
and ritualistic behaviours are the result of fear and harsh
sound stimuli that cause pain and confusion. Eldelson
(1995) attributed this behaviour to the difficulties in alternating and shifting attention to varying sound stimuli
in their immediate and wider surroundings.
Visual perception problems are specifically associated with ASD and have more profound and far-reaching
effects; many individuals see the world in a maladaptive
and malfunctioned manner that makes them reactive to
surface glare and shine as much as to daylight contrast,
which significantly affects their space perception and
impedes navigation in the wider built environment.
This is contrary to the claim that natural light improves
academic performance (Lyons, 2003; Fielding, 2002),
relaxes pupils, permits better concentration, and reduces
hyperactivity in typically developing children (Dunn,
225
Refereed Full-Papers
et al., 1993; Benya et al., 2002). However, many ASD
individuals have difficulty processing bright light. According to Dr. Megson (2001), this can be attributed to
involuntary dilation of the pupils involved in regulating
both natural and artificial light. Secondary impairment
emanating from above can be discerned by i) lack of
focus and ii) reduction in visual acuity problems, both
of which militate against learning and are compounded
by light contrast. These visual perception problems are
particularly relevant as many individuals with ASD are
visual learners. Most rely on visual memory in retrieving information, necessitating visual cuing and “serial
mapping,” both of which become increasingly instrumental to initiate communication and facilitate learning
(Grandin, 1994, 1995; William, 2006).
Distraction due to motion stimuli, which is essentially processed through the visual cortex of the brain,
has also been observed in an ASD subgroup to increase
hyperactivity and motor agitation. Many individuals
with ASD cannot process incoming motion stimuli and
moving objects, such as a train approaching a platform
and a moving car (Green, 2001; Gepner, et al., 1995;
Gepner & Mestre, 2002 a, b), which pinpoints impaired
motion perception in autism.
Both hypersensitivity and hyposensitivity to sound
stimuli have been recognised; which can be depicted
as opposite extremes of the same continuum (HatchRasmussen, 1997; Chang, 1998). This is attributed
to wide range of factors including, inter alia, sound
abnormal activities, sound sensitivity, and difficulties in modulating auditory input. There is mounting
evidence that many individuals with ASD have difficulties in controlling the volume of sound or filtering
low-frequency background noises such as “white” and
“pink” noises and whooshing and humming noises,
leading to symptoms similar to tinnitus (Hazell, 2000).
Adverse reactions to higher-frequency shrills and spinning noises from extractors, vacuum cleaners, and cold
water pipes pressure surge were also observed. This
can be compounded by other sensory inputs, primarily tactile and olfactory, leading to sensory overload
(Lowton, 1997; Shires, 1998; Chang, 1999). According
to Porges, et al., (1996) a lack of tension in the tympanic membrane and middle ear muscles could lead to
i) an inability to filter out lower frequency noises which
interfere with speech comprehension and ii) inability to
cushion higher intensity and impact sounds generated in
the immediate surroundings.
Porges (1996) argued that the perception of sound is
226 not equal at all frequencies. Sounds at low frequencies
seem as if they are softer than they really are. In contrast, we are relatively accurate in estimating the acoustic energy of human voice. The Fletcher-Munson equal
loudness contours (Fletcher & Munson, 1933) illustrated how human perception attenuated the “loudness”
of low frequency sounds. As measurement technologies
improved, researchers refined the perceived loudness
contours and sound meters were modified to include a
scale known as dBA, which adjusted for the perceived
differences in loudness as a function of frequency (i.e.,
the acoustic energy of lower frequencies had to be
greatly increased to be perceived at the equivalent loudness of a higher frequencies). This contrasts to sound
pressure level, which describes the physical energy of
the signal and does not apply any perceptually based
weighting to the frequencies that constitute the acoustic stimulation. The perceptual process of hearing low
frequency sounds as softer parallels the anti-masking
functions of the middle ear muscles (attenuating the
sounds at low frequencies). This is an area which merits
further examination.
Porges (2006) hypothesised that many individuals with ASD exhibit “predator vigilance.” This is a
phenomena where they in are in constant sound stress
due to heightened alertness to sound stimuli in their immediate vicinity. Training software utilizing modulated
human voices has been developed by Porges (2006) as
an active intervention approach to rectify the problem.
Other sensory and auditory integration interventions
have been developed over the last two decades to ameliorate the impact of these problems. These proved to
be beneficial, although their long-term efficacy remains
controversial. There is growing recognition that extent
of such sensory distortion might be detrimental to learning, leading to poor attention, poor concentration and
lack of communication and social interaction.
The Theoretical Framework of the Study
The literature on the sensory environment in school
buildings shows that there are multiplicities of interrelated and somehow conflicting spatial, visual, and
acoustic stimuli which have to be considered (Bright,
et al., 1997, 1999; Cook, et al., 1997, Cook, 1998,
Wright, et al., 1999, Edwards, 2002; Shabha, 2006;
Gaines, 2008). There seems to be a lack of understanding about what constitutes a therapeutic and sensory
teaching environment. Most research on school designs
are descriptive and qualitative (Powell, 1999; Allen,
Refereed Full-Papers
Figure 1: Common layout for a U.S. classroom with
recessed fluorescent downlights and low levels of
natural light.
2000). The focus has been mainly on assessing the
impact of a single environmental factor or parameter on
a restricted aspect of the learning process, in particular
teaching settings such as pupil tasks, attitudes, visibility, and reading performance (Wilkins & Carven, 1987;
Wilkins & Chronicle, 1991; Wilkins, 1995). Overall
effects of the sensory environment on users’ psychological and behavioural responses have largely been
overlooked or ignored.
For instance, there are some clues about the impact
of chronic noise exposure on stress levels and cognitive performance of pupils in classrooms (Haines &
Stansfeld, 2001; Evans & Maxwell, 1997), the effect of
fluorescent lighting on headaches and migraine due to
invisible flickering (Yunfan, 1983; Wilkins & Craven,
1987), and visual stress (Wilkins, 1995). The effect of
light on body metabolism and hormone levels has also
been demonstrated, along with its impact on mood ,
arousal, and aggression. Symptoms of stereotypy and
hyperactivity have been demonstrated in windowless
and brightly colored school buildings, providing an
insight about the phenomenon of the “biorhythm” of the
body (Karfikova, 1994).
Quantitative studies are mostly prescriptive in their
scope. The overall effects of the sensory environment
on users’ behaviour have largely been overlooked or
ignored. For instance, there are some clues about the
impact of noise levels on pupils’ anxiety and stress
levels in ordinary classrooms and the effect of fluorescent lighting on headaches and migraine due to
invisible flickering (Yunfan, 1983; Wilkins & Craven,
1987; Wilkins, 1995, 1996). Echoing and background
noise level were observed to affect sound comprehension and verbal performance of pupils in classrooms
(Elmallawany, 1985; Lercher, et al., 2003). Symptoms
of stereotypy, agitation, and hyperactivity have been
widely observed in windowless school buildings and
those with bright and patterned walls (Zental, 1986; Cohen, 1990; Sheri, 2003; Wohlfarth & Sam, 1989; Kuller
& Lindsten, 1992). Similarly, indoor surface shine and
glare due to natural light reflection and refraction were
equally observed as triggers for undesirable behaviour both indoor and outdoor (Observation by staff at
Sunfield, Clents UK; Williams, 2006). Bright daylight
and sharp contrast between shade and light has been
consistently noted to exasperate visual perception, as
observed by the authors of this study in both localities
in their random research visits to schools in the West
Midlands, UK, and Texas. Colour and tonal contrast
of main surfaces (e.g., floors, walls, and ceilings) and
secondary surfaces (e.g., furniture and equipment) can
act as visual triggers. It was claimed that colour effects
are perceived visually but are also absorbed by the skin
(Torice & Logrippo, 1989; Wohlfarth & Sam, 1982).
Figure 2: Shine and refraction on walls and ceilings- UK
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
227
Refereed Full-Papers
Table 1: The main visual and sound triggers.
These effects include changes in mood and attention,
blood pressure, and brain development (Engelbrecht,
2003). Red, for instance, increases blood pressure and
heightens the sense of smell, while blue decreases pulse
rate and lowers body temperature. Lighting also affects
colour perception (Morton, 1998). It remains to be
seen what influence colour and tonal contrast have on
students with ASD.
Clearly, very limited research has been conducted
to assess the overall impact of sensory environment on
students with ASD. Apart from anecdotal evidence and
personal accounts from teachers and carers in the field,
the secondary data is largely fragmented and inconclusive. A more holistic approach is, therefore, needed to
address such complexity of environmental stimuli since
its impact on impaired individuals seems to be interrelated. A matrix of key visual and sound triggers have
been refined and identified, as shown in Table 1. The
authors believe that sensory stimuli are interwoven in
the way they affect behaviour: one particular stimulus
might have a cascading effect and act as a catalyst for
further reactions, leading to worsening behaviour. A
global hypothesis was formulated that the interrelated
effect of sensory environmental stimuli significantly
influences the daily learning performance of individuals
with ASD and adversely affect their behaviour. An attempt will be made to identify the most influential sensory triggers and to enable their impacts on individual
behaviour to be fully assessed and understood.
Methodology
Setting
In both studies, a multi-faceted approach was adopted in compiling factual and perceptual information
about teachers’ and carers’ observations of students’
228 response to varying sensory stimuli. Based on focus
groups, opinions of teachers and carers were initially
explored to assess the extent of sensory problems in
the existing teaching settings in the UK and U.S. and to
highlight any design limitations and constraints. Focus
groups are particularly useful tools to be considered
given the geographical spread of the schools and the
number of schools selected for the empirical study in
both localities. This method is essentially based on systematic analysis process. According to Krueger (1994),
this method yields two dimensions in the way data are
gathered and handled, and by incorporating specific
processes both of which dictate sequential process.
A pilot questionnaire was distributed to selected
teachers and carers to allow further refinement and
embellishment of both the form and the content of the
questionnaire. This also provides snapshot feedback
about the appropriateness of technical phraseologies
and terminologies, as well as clarity of the questions
against any likely misinterpretation by respondents. It
also serves to identify further key issues and problems
based on the respondents’ observations of their own settings and to pinpoint to any potential design problems
associated with existing classrooms. Feedback from the
pilot study was used to formulate the final questionnaire
in the UK study, while initial responses from the focus
group were used to develop the final questionnaire in
the U.S. study.
Given the geographical spread of schools in Texas,
a Web-administered questionnaire survey using SurveyMonkey was considered to be more appropriate.
The common emphasis in both studies, however, was
almost entirely the same: collecting a diverse range of
data in order to enable research objectives to be better
addressed.
Refereed Full-Papers
Measures
This study argues that a survey questionnaire targeting teachers and carers in selected schools is the most
effective tool to gauge and assess students’ reaction to
the sensory environment. It is entirely based on their
verbal responses and observations of individual behaviour in different teaching settings. This provides a
quantifiable description of attitudes, opinions, or trends
of the study through a sample (Creswell, 2003; Babbie, 2009). This study argues that students with ASD
are conscious of their immediate surroundings and are
interactive and responsive to their own settings in ways
which cannot be understood by studying artificial simulation (McNeil, 1992). However, teachers’ and carers’
verbal responses to the ways individuals react to the
sensory environment is difficult to measure objectively.
The authors believe that if consistent patterns emerge
through the responses of the questionnaire, then it can
be argued with higher probability that a causal effect
exists.
Procedures
The questionnaire focused on gathering primary data
about the schools, teaching settings, and behavioural
reactions. Both open-ended and closed-ended questions
were used in both studies. Questions were formulated
based on the feedback, fine-tuned by omitting technical jargons and needless words, and refined further to
ensure clarity, coherence, and understandability prior
to being administered. While SurveyMonkey was used
to administer the questionnaire in the U.S. study, the
UK questionnaire was administered face-to-face. The
responses and identity of the respondents remained
confidential.
Secondary data were also collected using the researcher’s notes, videotape, and memory. The data
were then reviewed for dominant themes that emerged
throughout the discussion. These themes were selected
according to verbal and nonverbal responses to questions, the number of participants mentioning an issue,
and agreement or disagreement on an issue.
Target Groups
Special education educators in Texas public schools
were the participants for Phase 2 of the U.S. study.
Most special education teachers work with children
with mild to moderate disabilities, although some work
with severe cases of autism or mental retardation. Since
schools are becoming more inclusive, special education
teachers’ roles include designing and adapting curricu-
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
lum and teaching techniques to meet varying individuals’ needs. They are also involved in their students’
behavioural and social development. At the time the
questionnaire was distributed, there were an estimated
32,546 staff involved. A total of 608 teachers participated in the Texas study, based on random sampling,
which was reduced further to 546 to ensure reliability
and representativeness of the sample following initial
analysis of responses. The U.S. study sample represents
1.8 percent of the target sample in Texas area. In the
U.K. study, 96 out of 588 teachers and carers participated in the West Midlands, where there are much fewer
schools.
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics were used to analyze the gathered data, including percentages and frequency distributions, in order to provide a matrix of most correlated
factors and triggers affecting behaviour. The period of
analysis was adequate to allow a greater understanding
of the underlying issues and the significance of sensory
triggers on students’ behaviour.
Findings
The purpose of this study was to assess the realtionship between visual and acoustic/auditory learning
environments and the behaviour of students with ASD.
Both studies show that visual and auditory stimuli have
adverse effect on the behaviour of students with ASD.
The ability to focus attention is necessary for learning and managing behaviour. Distractions within the
sensory environment should be minimized considerably
to enable students to manage their learning environment
more effectively. The focus of this study is becoming
more critical due to current drive toward inclusion in
the both the U.S. and UK. Although the results from
both studies primarily support the association between
sensory stimuli and the behaviour of students with ASD,
the results differ in the sensory parameters and triggers
that were perceived to be most problematic.
Visual Triggers
In the UK study, it was found that the source of light
(96 percent responding affirmatively), luminance (87
percent), and intensity of light (82 percent) were the
primary visual triggers with the greatest effects on students. In contrast, respondents in the U.S. study identified the source of light (44 percent), intensity of light
(38.9 percent), and luminance (19.7 percent) as major
visual triggers. The participants of the UK study consid-
229
Refereed Full-Papers
ered visual changes and distraction—the most frequently selected choice of the U.S. study, at 67 percent—as
much less problematic (11 percent).
The discrepancies in the order of importance of
visual triggers might be attributed to differences in
daylight factors, which are affected by classroom height
as much as window areas and heights in both localities. This is given the climatic variations; the climate
is mostly overcast in the West Midlands and brighter
in Texas. Greater reliance on artificial lighting and internal luminance in schools in the West Midlands most
likely contributed to such a significantly heightened
visual reaction.
The UK study did not include “undefined space” and
“clutter” as choices for the question concerning visual
triggers. The inclusion of these sensory triggers came
about due to the discussion within the focus group.
The choices were second and third most often selected
by the participants in the U.S. Other choices for the
questionnaire that were not present on the UK survey
include highly decorated classrooms, which 41 percent
of teachers identified as a main visual trigger, compared
to windows at 19.2 percent ,items hanging from lighting (18 percent), and linear furniture arrangement (2.6
percent).
Conversely, “stripes on radiators, grills, gratings, and
light diffusers” were considered significant by only 3.1
percent of the participants in the U.S. study, compared
to 36 percent in the UK study. This could be attributed
to the extent to which these fittings have been used in
classrooms in the UK as compared to Texas.
Sound and Auditory Triggers
The most common sound trigger in the UK study
was “unduly harsh and lengthy echoes caused by large
uncurtained window areas and hard floors and ceilings
in larger areas,” as indicated by 77 percent of the respondents, as compared to 20 percent in the U.S. study.
“Noise from heating and air conditioning systems” was
selected as an auditory trigger by 15.7 percent of teachers in the U.S. study, compared to 42 percent of participants in the UK study. This may possibly be attributed
to effective noise-control measures and better specifications in Texas schools compared to relatively outdated
service systems in West Midlands schools. The third
most frequent response in the UK study was “low pitch
sounds from main road traffic” at 37 percent, compared
to only 12.2 percent in the US study. This can be attributed primarily to proximity to main roads and the type
and quality of external glazing. However, “background
230 noise from adjacent rooms and corridors” was identified by 38.6 percent of respondents in the U.S. study,
compared to 15 percent in the UK. Such variations are
indicative of the type and specifications of materials for
internal partitions, where a lightweight timber construction is commonly used in U.S. schools, compared to
heavy concrete blocks as the main construction material
in the UK.
Conclusions
The disparity in findings between the UK and the
U.S. can be attributed to several factors. The design of
the learning environments may differ between Texas
and West Midlands. There are some differences in
the classroom area and size. The overall size of public schools in Texas, as compared with special needs
schools in the West Midlands, could be a factor. In
addition, climate and the amount of sunlight could
account for the variation in the choices of lighting
between the two studies. The internal design layouts of
the classrooms vary. Areas and size of the classrooms
also differ. The findings of the two studies largely
support Grandin’s (1995) observation and her claim
that students with ASD experience heightened sensitivity to light and other visual stimulation. This partially
supports the commonly held claim that students with
ASD display strengths in visual learning (Mesibov et
al., 2004). Based on the results of this study and others,
the learning environment should be designed to reduce
the impact of moving visual stimuli and distractions.
The study highlighted spatial organization, lighting, and
colour as three of the factors that should be addressed in
achieving this purpose.
Learning Space Organisation (U.S. study only)
The importance of visual and physical organization
cannot be overemphasized when designing learning
environments for students with ASD. Based on the
results from the focus and survey groups, a cluttered
and a highly decorated classroom may lead to undesirable behavior. Items hanging from overhead lighting
were cited by approximately 18 percent of the teachers
as having a negative influence on behaviour. The focus
group participants reported that no negative influence
occurred as long as the items had meaning within the
context of the classroom. For example, signs over centers, or decorations involved in the theme of the classroom were determined to enhance rather than to distract
attention in the learning environment. According to the
survey group, space should be well defined because
Refereed Full-Papers
undefined space is one of the major factors contributing to undesirable behaviour. Different areas of the
classroom should provide for independent work, group
work, and leisure (Mesibov, et al., 2004). The focus
group reported that an open concept space is difficult
for students with ASD, as they will desire to roam or
run around the area. The TEACCH approach (Mesibov,
et al., 2004) and claims by Stokes (2003) emphasize
the need for organization of the physical environment.
These assertions are backed up by the current study,
since undefined space was the second most common
visual trigger selected by the participants.
Lighting
Natural light improves academic performance
(Lyons, 2003; Fielding, 2002), relaxes students, per-
Table 3: Comparison of the findings of UK and U.S. regarding visual triggers
Table 4: Comparison of the findings of the UK and the U.S. regarding acoustic/auditory triggers.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
231
Refereed Full-Papers
mits better concentration, and reduces hyperactivity in
children (Dunn, et al., 1993; Benya et al., 2002). Torrice & Lagrippo (1989) observed full-spectrum lighting
increased productivity, improved mental attitudes and
alertness, and had therapeutic effects. Both studies reported that intensity of light, source of light, luminance,
and presence of windows were all found to trigger
sensory hypersensitivity and stereotypic behavior. In
this comparative study, intensity of light was found to
have the greatest effect of the three. Megson’s (1995)
claims regarding involuntary dilation of the pupil are
instrumental in regulating a higher intensity and bright
light. While this is beyond the scope of this study, it is
significant to identify the need for cross-disciplinary
research to investigate the interrelation between the
trios of physio-cognitive-sensory environment. However, unmasked windows were also seen as a source of
distraction for students with ASD.
Colour and Pattern
Colour and pattern were found to serve as visual
triggers for students with ASD, but to a lesser extent
than factors contributing to space organization and
lighting. Certain colours and/or colour contrast were
each selected by almost 15 percent of the teachers as
having a negative effect on behaviour for students with
ASD. Patterned fabrics were selected by 12.4 percent
of the teachers, while patterned wallpaper was reported
as having less impact on behaviour. These findings may
be a result of the nature of school design in the United
States instead of the true influence these sensory parameters have on students with ASD. For example, the
participants of the focus group stated that they believe
they had little control over colours in the classroom.
Many classrooms are painted neutral colours. In addition, patterned wallpaper may be used less frequently in
school interiors than other finishes. These factors were
not a focus of the study; however, they may have an
effect on the results of the empirical research.
Colour and tonal contrast of main surfaces (floors,
walls, ceilings, and doors) and secondary surfaces
(furniture) were selected by less than 7 percent of the
survey participants as a main visual trigger. This may
be due in part to the lack of contrast within the space
(Texas Centre for Educational Research, 2006). Colour
effects are perceived visually but are also absorbed
by the skin (Torice & Logrippo, 1989; Wohlfarth &
Sam, 1982). These effects include changes in mood
and attention, blood pressure, and brain development
(Engelbrecht, 2003). For example, red increases blood
232 pressure and heightens the sense of smell. Blue slows
the pulse rate and lowers body temperature. Lighting
also effects colour perception (Morton, 1998), but this
is beyond the scope of this study.
Noise
Hard surfaces should be avoided as much as possible
to help control noise. Noise from flickering fluorescent
lights was found to be a problem for 17.7 percent of
respondents in this study. Noise levels of up to 50 db
(A) have been recorded from fluorescent lighting (Manlove et al., 2001). Only 9.7 percent selected plumbing
noise as a main acoustic/auditory trigger. However, the
open-ended responses from the survey group revealed
that some students with ASD display sensory processing problems due to toilets flushing. Even modest noise
levels (typical of North American neighbourhoods)
can affect the development of the cognitive systems of
children (Lercher et. al., 2003).
Conclusions
This research aims to assess the relationship among a
few sensory environmental parameters and individuals’
behaviour in school buildings both in the UK and U.S.
The findings clearly demonstrate that sensory stimuli
have an adverse effect on individual behaviour in
varying teaching settings including classrooms, gymnasiums, and main halls. This supports previous claims
(Jordan & Powell, 1995; Eldelson, 1995; Williams,
1996; Waltz, 1996; Hatch-Rasmussen, 1997).
The authors believe that students with ASD learn
and interpret the world around them through their
senses. Unfortunately, when their sensory receptors are
bombarded by information simultaneously, this leads
to processing difficulty and sensory jumbling. In turn,
heightened sensory perception and stereotypic behaviour interferes with attention and affects their learning.
The findings strongly support the claims that sensory
stimuli are a complex and multi-faceted issue which
might be affected by the spatial characteristics, design
layout, and finishing of classrooms.
Stereotypical behaviour is particularly aggravated by
the source of light, intensity, luminance, and contrast,
supporting previous claims by Yunfan (1993), Zental
(1986), Cohen (1990), Sheri (2003), Wohlfarth & Sam
(1989), Kuller & Lindsten (1992), and Megson (2001).
Sound echoing and reverberation in larger spaces have
similar influence supporting Grandin’s (1996) claim.
This is determined by the volume of space as much as
the internal finishing, such as having large uncurtained
Refereed Full-Papers
window areas and hard floors. These findings add credence to previous claims by Elmallawany (1985) and
Lercher, et al. (2003).
Low frequency sound from traffic noise, extractor fans, and PC fans have been shown to influence
behaviour significantly. This supports previous claims
by Haines & Stansfield (2001), Evans & Maxwell
(1997), and Porges (1996, 2006). These might interfere
with sound intelligibility, leading to fragmentation and
loss of meaning of the perceived sound both in terms of
content and context.
Outcomes, Practical Benefits, and Implications
This study represents a leap forward by providing a
comprehensive measuring framework by which some
sensory environmental attributes can be assessed objectively and their impact on individual behaviour can
be thoroughly assessed. The findings have pinpointed
the key factors that need to be considered to ameliorate
the adverse impact on the teaching environment. Many
benefits could accrue to sensory impaired students who
are the ultimate beneficiaries of this study. Other intended beneficiaries are policy makers, teachers, carers,
psychologists, design and construction firms, school
managers, architects, facilities managers, building
materials manufacturers, and researchers in the field as
follows:
1. Providing a measuring framework by which some
sensory attributes can be assessed objectively and
their impacts on children’s comfort and well-being
can be thoroughly examined. Other likely risks of
harm can be minimised or mitigated.
2. Providing further insight and in-depth understanding
about what constitutes a therapeutically enhanced
learning environment for sensory-impaired
individuals. Also, identifying the key influential
environmental factors contributing to quality school
buildings while enhancing the level of prediction
in dealing with similar problems in the wider built
environment. The findings might have a wide range
of implications to sport, leisure, commercial, retail,
and other communal facilities, along with promoting
inclusive approaches which facilitate the use of
buildings, equipment, and services by those with
sensory impairments.
3. Maximising the level of control on the design and
construction of school buildings by further reducing
the undesirable consequences of the aforementioned
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
sensory problems towards improving the operational
efficiency of school buildings. Ultimately, to
promote inclusive approaches which facilitate use
of buildings, equipment, and services by those with
sensory impairment. Clearly, educational policies in
both countries advocate integrating sensory impaired
individuals into mainstream schools. This might
have wider implications in terms of ameliorating
the impact of these problems when considering
renovating and adapting existing schools. Based on
the findings, there is a need to develop new design
guidelines and design tools and to amend the current
guidelines in both countries (DEFS, 1997, a, b), as
well as to inform architects and interior designers
involved in renovating existing buildings or
designing new school buildings.
Areas for Future Research
In the light of the findings, there is a need to develop
a cross-disciplinary approach to the impact of the environment on individual behaviour. The findings need to
be interpreted further by using behaviour mapping and
participant observations, which will be the focus of the
next research stage.
Another area for further research might include new
intelligent materials to mitigate the changeable flow and
fluctuating impacts of acoustical and visual stimuli. Further considerations will be given to explore and develop
new performance specifications for adaptive materials.
This might enable manufacturers of building materials
to optimise their method of production and specifications to effectively meet the needs of individuals with
sensory/perceptual impairment.
There is also a need to examine new adaptive and
sensory substitution technologies (SST) to mitigate the
undesirable impacts of the sensory environment and to
enhance sensory processing in the wider environment.
This, in turn, might assist rehabilitation of sensory impaired individuals (SII) to perform to their full potential
in the wider built environment. Adaptable and versatile
technologies need to be fine-tuned to meet different
individuals’ needs.
References
Allen, I. (2000). Harmeny School: AJ Building Study. The
Architects Journal, 27 July.
Babbie, E. (2009). The Practice of Social Research. USA.
Wadsworth Thomson Learning, Inc.
Benya, J.R. (2001). Lighting for Schools. Retrieved October 10,
2003 from http://www.edfacilties.org.htm.
233
Refereed Full-Papers
BRE (1993). Effects of Environmental Noise on People at Home.
BRE Information Paper 22/93.
Bright, K., Cook, G., Harris, J. (1997). Colour, Contrast
& Perception: Design Guidance for Internal Built
Environments. Brooker Publications.UK.
Bright, K, Cook, G. (1999). Project Rainbow: A Research project
to Provide Colour Contrast Design Guidance for Internal
Built Environments. Winner of the Chartered Institute of
Building’s Research Paper Competition 1997, Presented in a
CIB occasional paper number 57.
Brockett, S. (1999). Earobics Auditory Development and
Phonics Programme CD-ROM. Innovative Development for
Educational Achievements, Inc. North Haven, CT, USA.
Chang, K. (1998). The Science of Sniff and Smell. ABC News,
19 March, www.abcnews.com/sections/science/DailyNews/
smell0318.html.
Cook, G., Hill, S., Slater, A. (1997). Lighting for Visually
Impaired People in a Non-uniformly Lit Office Environment.
Proceedings of Lux Europa 8th European Lighting
Conference, Amsterdam, Netherlands, pp. 503-510.
Cook, F. (1998). Lighting Quality Aspects: Essential
Requirements for Visually Impaired People. Proceedings
of the First CIE Symposium on Lighting Quality, Ottawa,
Canada, 9-10 May 1998, pp 48-59.
Cohen, S., Trostle, S.L. (1990). Young Individual Preferences for
School-Related Physical-Environment Setting Characteristic.
Environment and Behaviour, Vol. 22, No 6, pp 753-766.
Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research Design. USA. Sage
Publications.
DfES (1997a). Meeting Special Educational Needs. Oct 1997,
Green Paper, Stationary Office, London.
DfES (1997b). Access for Disabled People to School Buildings.
Management and Design Guide, Building Bulletin 91,
Stationary Office, London.
Dunn, R. & Dunn, K. (1993). Teaching secondary students
through their individual learning styles: Practical approaches
for grades 7-12. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Elmallawany, A. (1985). Schools layouts and noise nuisance.
Building Research & Practice, Jan/Feb, 21-24.
Engelbrecht, K. (2003). The Impact of Color on Learning.
Perkins & Will, Chicago.
Gepner, B. (1995). Postural Effects of Motion Vision in
Young Autistic Children. Cognitive Neuroscience and
Neuropsychology, 6(8), 1211-1214.
Grandin, T. (1994). The Learning Style of People with Autism:
An Autobiography.
Quill, K. (ed.). Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies
to Enhance Communication and Socialization. Delmar
Publisher, pp.33-51. Delmar Publisher, Boluder, Colorado.
Hazell, J. (2000). Hypersensitivity of hearing: Hyperacusis,
Phonophobia and recruitment. Allergy-induced-Autism
(AiA) Newsletter Volume 3, 8-11, London.
Fielding, R. (2006). Lighting the Learning Environment.
Retrieved June 12, 2006, from http://www.designshare.com/
Research/Lighting.htm.
234 Gaines, K. (2008). Brain compatible Learning Environments
for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Dissertation.
etd-10092008-142401, available online, http://etd.lib.ttu.edu/
theses/available/etd-10092008-142401.
Gepner, B (1995). Postural Effects of Motion Vision in
Young Autistic Individual. Cognitive Neuroscience and
Neuropsychology Journal, Vol. 6, No. 8, pp1211-1214.
Gepner, B. & Mestre, D. (2002a). Rapid Visual-motion
Integration Deficit in Autism. Trends Cognitive Science, 6,
455.
Gepner, B. & Mestre, D.R. (2002b). Brief Report: Postural
Reactivity to Fast Visual Motion Differentiates Autistic
From Children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism
Development Disorder. 32, 231–238.
Grandin, T. (1994). The Learning Style of People with
Autism: An Autobiography” as cited in K. Quill (ed.).
Teaching Individual with Autism: Strategies to Enhance
Communication and Socialisation” Chapter 2, Delmar
Publisher, pp. 33-51.
Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in Pictures and Other Reports from
my Life with Autism. New York: Doubleday.
Grandin, T. (1996). Sensory Sensitivity in www.autism.org/temple
Hazell, J. (2000). Hypersensitivity of Hearing: Hyperacusis,
Phonophobia and Recruitment. AIA Newsletter, Autumn
2002.
Hatch-Rasmussen, C. (1997). Sensory Integration. http://www.
Autism.org/si.htm
Kuller, R. & Lindsten, C. (1992). Health and Behavior of
Children in Classrooms with and Without Windows. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, Vol. 12, pp. 305-317.
Krueger, R.A. (1994). Focus Groups: A Practical Guide
for Applied Research. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage
Publications, Inc.
Lemonick, M. (1995). Glimpses of the Mind: What is
Consciousness, Memory, Emotion? Science Unravels the
Best-Kept Secrets of the Human Brain. TIME Magazine, July
17, Vol. 146, No. 3.
Lercher, P., Evans, G.W., Meis, M. (2003). Ambient Noise
and Cognitive Processes among Primary School Children.
Environment and Behaviour, 35, 725-735.
Lyons, J.B. (2002). The Learning Environment: Do School
Facilities Really Affect Child’s Education. Retrieved
September 12, 2003, from www.asbj.com/lbd2002/inprint/
environment.html.
Manlove, E.E., Frank, T. & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2001). Why
Should We Care about Noise in Classrooms and Child Care
Settings? Child &Youth Care Forum, 30(1) 55-64.
Mesibove, G., Shea, V., & Schopler, I. (2004). The TEACCH
Approach to Autistic Spectrum Disorders. New York:
Springer Science and Business Media, LLC.
Megson, M., (2001). Visual Perceptual Problems in Autism.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Xs7EqjafvnY&feature=re
lated retrieved 13 July 2010.
Morton, J. (1998). Color Voodoo for the Office. www.colorcom.
com.
Refereed Full-Papers
Panerai S., Ferrante L.& Zingale M.(2002). Benefits of the
Treatment and Education of Autistic and Communication
Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as compared
with a non-specific approach. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research. 46(4), pp. 318-327.
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Greenspan, S. I.
(1996). Infant Regulation of the Vagal “Brake” Predicts
Child Behavior Problems: A Psychobiological Model of
social Behavior. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(8), 697712.
Porges, S., (2006). “Polyvagal Theory” in www.
naturalworldhealing.com/porges_information.htm, Retrieved
12 May 2008.
Stokes, S., (2003). Structured Teaching: Strategies for
Supporting Students with Autism? Retrieved 25 June, 2003
from www.cesa7.k12.wi.us/sped/autism/structured/str10.htm.
Shabha, G. (2006). An Assessment of the Impact of the Sensory
Environment on Individuals’ Behaviour in Special Needs
Schools. Facilities, Vol. 24, issue 1/2, pp
Texas Center for Educational Research (2006). Texas Study
of Personnel Needs in Special Education”, Executive
Summary. Retrieved 11 April, 2008. Available www.tcer.org/
research/specialed/documents/sped_execsum_2006.pdf
Torrice, A F., Lagrippo, R. (1989). In my Room: Designing for
and with Children. New York: Ballantine Books.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Waltz, M. (1996). Pervasive Development Disorders: Finding,
Diagnosis and Getting Help. Chapter 6, O’Reilly &
Associates, USA.
Williams, D. (1996). Autism- An Inside Out Approach. Jessica
Kingsley Publisher, London.
Williams, D. (2006). Jumbled Jigsaw: An Insider’s Approach
to the treatment of Autistic Spectrum. Jessica Kingsley
Publishers, London and Philadelphia.
Wilkins, A. (1995). Visual Stress. Oxford University Press.
Wilkins A. & Chronicle E (1991). Colour and Visual Discomfort
in Migraineurs. Lancet, 338, 890. London.
Wilkins, A. & Craven, B. (1987). Visual Display Units and
Fluorescent Lighting Enlarge Movements of the Eyes Across
Text. as cited in J. Crony-Dillon, E.S. Rosen & J. Marshall
(Eds.), “Hazards of Light (Eye and Skin): Myths and
Realities”, pp. 229-234. Pergamon Press.
Wohlfarth, H. & Sam, C. (1982). The Effects of Color
Psychodynamic Environment Modification upon Psychophysiological and Behavioral Reactions of Several
Handicapped Children. International Journal of Biosocial
Research., 3(1): 30-38.
Yunfan, P. (1983). Evaluating Discomfort from Lighting Glare.
Building Research and Practice. Sept/Oct., pp 317-321.
Zentall, S.S. & Dwyer, A.M. (1989). Color Effects on the
Impulsivity and Activity of Hyperactive Childrren. Journal
of School Psychology, 27 (2), 165-17 http://encarta.msn.com/
encyclopedia761565821/Sense_Organs.html.
235
Refereed Full-Papers
Residential Experience of Rural
Immigrants: Understanding
Migrant Enclaves in Urban
China
Yushu Zhu (University of Illinois, UrbanaChampaign)
Abstract
Migrant enclaves encroaching on rural settlements
have dramatically changed the social, spatial, and economic landscapes of the Chinese city and have aroused
heated debate over housing policies. Drawing upon 43
semi-structured interviews about residential experiences of rural immigrants in three urban villages and
one shantytown in three cities, this research provides
new perspectives towards of the formation of such
social space in urban China through the lens of spatial
and social exclusion. It also adds to the debate of social
exclusion and housing in western societies by providing
empirical evidence within the context of a developing
country. It is argued that rural immigrants are spatially
trapped in such residential space, which is a spatial
manifestation of multidimensional social exclusions imposed on them. Yet on the other hand, migrant enclaves
play a significant role in promoting social inclusion of
rural immigrants in the long run by providing cheap accommodation and employment opportunities in the city.
With the vacuum of provision of public housing for
rural immigrants, elimination of these migrant enclaves
can intensify social exclusion if no consideration is
given to rural immigrants.
Introduction
Housing stratification in urban China is manifested
in the prevalence of migrant enclaves, including “urban
villages” (chengzhongcun) and shantytowns (penghuqu). The two types of migrant enclaves are distinct
from each other in terms of formation history, yet they
are both mostly dilapidated and sub-standard residential neighborhoods and accommodate the bulk of rural
migrant households in urban areas. Migrant enclaves
are regarded as “new urban space” formed (Ma and Xiang 1998) by rural immigrants as a result of exclusion
from various social resources and civil rights due to the
rural-urban dichotomy of social system (Fan, 2002; F.
L. Wu, 2004). This new social space is made possible
by the market reforms implemented since the 1980s.
On the one hand, since the housing reform in late
1990s, extensive urban expansion and redevelopment
have encroached on the rural settlements, dramatically
changing the social, spatial, and economic landscapes of
the Chinese cities (Ma and Xiang, 1998). However, due
to the duel land use systems—state-owned in urban area
versus collectively owned in rural areas, the municipal
governments tend to leave the residential land in rural
villages intact to avoid costly relocation and compensation. On the other hand, labor reform has freed rural
labor surplus to urban areas and given rise to growing demand of cheap housing in the cities, which left
indigenous villagers room for rent seeking. As a result,
indigenous villagers built informal buildings upon their
land and let them to rural immigrants. Many villagers
were able to move out of the village to better commodity housing, leaving rural immigrants the dominant
residents in these villages. The proliferation of such
migrant enclaves has been witnessed over the last two
decades. In 2000, the 277 urban villages in Guangzhou
had 1 million inhabitants (Zhang, Zhao et al., 2003);
similarly, 241 urban villages in Shenzhen accommodated approximately 2.15 million inhabitants (Song, Zenou
et al., 2008); in Xi’an, there are 187 such villages, 55 of
which are located in the downtown area (Wang, 2004).
While urban villages are most pronounced in the Pearl
River Delta and Beijing, the shantytown (penghuqu) is
another representative type of migrant enclaves which
feature the migrant settlements in Shanghai1.
These migrant enclaves, which accommodate a substantial number of rural immigrants, have been given
a negative image as the pathology of the city, which
give rise to crime, disorder, and many other problems
(Ma and Xiang 1998; Song, et al. 2008). Consequently,
urban village renewal projects are implemented by
urban governments, with an aim to turn the “pathological” space into profitable projects without taking into
account of the interests of migrant dwellers. It is not
until recently that migrant enclaves were recognized as
1
No systemic statistics of penghuqu are found in Shanghai. This statement is confirmed by Professor Lixun Li, an urban village expert in
School of Geography and Planning of Zhongshan University, China, and by Professor Yinfang Chen, the director of Sociology Department
specializing Shanghai penghuqu.
236 Refereed Full-Papers
the primary housing resource for rural immigrants by
Chinese governments, which came to understand that
rural migrants could have positive impacts on urban
economics through their contribution of labor in fields
shunned by permanent residents of the cities. In 2007,
the Chinese central governments issued Guidelines on
Improving Living Conditions of Rural Workers (Ministry of Construction, Development and Reform Commission (abbr.) et al., 2007), announcing the responsibility of work units to improve housing conditions for
rural workers and that the housing provision for rural
workers should be included in the planning of housing
development for each city government. Yet a wide gulf
still exists between recognition of the positive benefits
of migrant enclaves and actual active support among
municipal authorities.
Extensive research has been done on the formation
of the migrant enclaves, most of which is on urban villages. Most scholars attribute the emergence of urban
villages to institutional factors related to the rural-urban
dual social system (Tian, 2008); others, on the contrary,
argued that aside from the structural mechanism, urban
villages result from rational choices made by indigenous villagers, such as rank-seeking activities during
the rapid urbanization (Zhu, 2004; Lan and Zhang,
2005). However, missing in the literature are mechanisms for the formation of migrant enclaves from the
perspectives of rural immigrants who are the de facto
users of such space. It has been pointed out in growing literature that urban villages, as a form of migrant
enclaves, play a critical role in housing rural immigrants (Wu, 2002; Liu and Wu, 2006; Song, Zenou et
al., 2008). This paper therefore aims to understand the
space of migrant enclaves, drawing upon their residential experiences in the city.
Space and social relations are mutually integrated.
Social relations are constituted through space and constrained and mediated by it (Wolch and Dear, 1989; Ma
and Xiang, 1998). Therefore, this paper employs the
concept of “social exclusion,” which has been widely
applied in housing policy studies in European countries
to understand the inequality in access to urban space
(Marsh and Mullins 1998; Somerville 1998). “Exclusion through housing” has been observed in public
housing products in these western countries (Ratcliffe,
1998; Hills, 2001). Nonetheless, the empirical research
of China, where a distinct housing system is in effect,
could provide new evidence to this debate.
In a nutshell, this study intends to provide a new perspective of understanding the space of migrant enclaves
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
through the lens of social exclusion. Specifically, this
study adopts case studies with in-depth interviews to
investigate two main questions: What is the relationship of the residential experience of rural immigrants,
including housing condition, housing choices, and
residential mobility, to migrant enclaves? What is the
relationship of social exclusion to housing: is housing
causation of social exclusion or representation of social
exclusion?
Housing Accessibility Of Rural Immigrants And
Migrant Enclaves In Urban China
The open-up policy of Chinese government in market
systems, including the labor market in the 1980s, freed
numerous rural labors from agricultural areas to seek
new opportunities in higher-wage coastal cities with
high growth of economic development (Liu and Liang,
1997). Two categories are defined for migration—
permanent migration with changes of household
registration (hukou) and temporary movement without
changes of hukou, or origin. The latter group is officially referred as “floating population” or “temporary
migrants.” According to the 2005 census, the floating
population, who had left their household registration
place for at least half a year, accounts for 24 percent of
the city population. As for the floating population, about
35 percent are less-educated rural migrant workers, or
“nongmingong” who hold an agricultural hukou. (State
Council and State Statistical Bureau, 2007).
The influx of the large stream of rural-urban migration gives rise to challenging issues concerning housing accessibility of rural immigrants for Chinese city
governments. Rural immigrants are largely excluded
from the mainstream housing distribution system (Wang
and Murie, 2000; Wu, 2006). Market-rate commodity
housing units are out of their reach, either because they
cannot afford them or a bank mortgage is not available for rural immigrants. Meanwhile, participation in
the secondary housing market usually requires a local
hukou (Wu, 2004). In addition, subsidized public housing, including the Economic and Comfortable Housing (jingji shiyong fang) and affordable rental units,
are exclusively offered to local urban residents. The
constraints rural migrants face lead to limit choices of
housing products for them.
One spatial response to shortage of housing resources
for rural immigrants is the wide-spread migrant enclaves in urban areas (Liu and Liang, 1997), which are
regarded as informal settlements in which housing units
are built on rural land but traded in urban markets. Ur-
237
Refereed Full-Papers
ban villages in China are literally rural communities in
urban built-up areas. They were originally rural settlements where indigenous villagers lived on agricultural
activities organized by collective units such as village
committees, and they hold rural household registration.
As urban expansion reached the urban fringe, the Chinese government converted agricultural land to urban
use while leaving the residential land intact to avoid
costly relocation and compensation. As a result, the
planning, land use, organizational structure, and people
of these villages are excluded from the urban system.
Urban village, village-in-the-city, and chengzhongcun are interchangeable synonyms referring to this
unique migrant enclave in China. However, unlike urban villages in the contexts of western societies, which
respond to a planning ideology to pursue a village-style
living as a way of escaping contemporary alienating
cities, Chinese urban villages are not developed from
any planning schemes, rather, they are believed to result
from the structural deficiency of the country’s planning
and social system inherited from the pre-reform period.
These two neighborhood forms, therefore, fundamentally differ from each other in origins, physical structures,
and social compositions. A comparison between these
two contextualized notions has been systematically
reviewed by Chung (2010).
The spread of urban villages in China has provoked
extensive academic debates. Chinese language literature
has provided detailed documentation on the physical
settings, land use, and living conditions of urban villages (Zhou and Gao, 2001; Chung, 2010). Zhejiang
village in Beijing is the first documented and one of the
most notable urban villages for case studies in China
(Wang, 1995; Ma and Xiang, 1998). It was estimated
that in the mid-1990s, about 70 percent of the 50,000
Zhejiang migrants in Beijing lived in Zhejiang village
(Wang, 1995). Lan (2004) and Wang’s (2004) detailed
investigations in Guangzhou and Shenzhen respectively
have suggested that as much as 90 percent of the land
available in these settlements has been built on.
From a macro perspective, the deficiency in planning
systems and social structures are attributed as causes of
urban villages. Urban villages are believed to be shaped
because of dualism land-use patterns, governance inefficiency, and housing marketization (Li, 2001). From
a micro perspective, the rank-seeking activities of
aboriginal villagers is believed to be the driving forces
behind the formation of such residential space (Zhu,
2004; Lan and Zhang, 2005). It is not until recently that
the importance of urban villages to urban development
238 and to rural immigrants has been widely acknowledged
(Chan, Yao et al., 2003; Song, Zenou et al., 2008;
Chung, 2010). However, the role of rural immigrants
who are the de facto users of the space has been largely
overlooked in shaping the urban village. Although
literature has documented the social life, such as social
network and self identity, of rural immigrants (Ma and
Xiang, 1998), little has been found on the relationship
between their residential experiences and the formation
of space per se.
Shantytowns, also known as penghuqu, are mostly
found in Shanghai and Jiangsu. Unlike cities in the
Pearl River Delta in southern China, there are few urban
villages in urban areas of Shanghai. Instead, the role
of urban village as accommodating low-income immigrants in the inner city has been taken mostly by the
shantytowns. The history of penghuqu dates back to the
Opium War and continues through the civil war in the
1940s. Many local refugees or those from surrounding
provinces, such as Jiangsu, Zhejiang, Anhui, and Shandong provinces, built their own shelters in Shanghai to
escape from wars or natural disasters and settled down
there. After decades of urban development, these selfbuilt shelters gradually deteriorated and the areas where
these settlements were concentrated became shantytowns (interview with a Chinese researcher studying
shantytowns in 2010). In the 1990s, as migration from
rural to urban areas grew, most of the first settlers and
their families in penghuqu moved to better housing and
more and more rural immigrants resided in penghuqu.
Despite the distinct institutional driving forces that
shape urban villages and shantytowns, the two types of
migrant enclaves share much in common with regard to
their scattered locations in cities, sub-standard housing
conditions, and low socio-economic composition. Yet,
while urban villages are the cause of heated debates,
little attention has been paid to the shantytowns. Chen
(2006) adopted ethnography to investigate the life
history of native villagers, but immigrants have been
missed in this book.
Space And Social Relations: Social Exclusion
Through Housing?
Space is a production of social practices (Lefebvre,
1991). The perspective on socially integrated space is
essential to understand urban space. Madanipour (1998)
adopted the concept of “social exclusion” to interpret
urban space in European counties. He articulated the
relations between social exclusion and the production
of urban space by arguing that multi-dimensional social
Refereed Full-Papers
exclusion—economic, political, and cultural exclusions,
which constrain social choices of marginalized groups,
would give rise to constraints in spatial choices and
consequently finds its spatial manifestation in urban
areas.
Social exclusion is a relatively new concept, which
dates back to the late 1990s and finds its antecedents
in earlier debates in France, Britain, and the U.S. on
poverty and the “underclass.” There are diverse interpretations of this concept with regard to different
emphasis. Room (1995) argued that the notion focuses
primarily on inadequate social participation, lack of social integration, and lack of power, emphasizing social
participation and citizenship. Other scholars (Walker,
1997) regard the concept as caused by social processes
and the multi-dimensional nature of disadvantage, in
contrast with the static nature of poverty.
Housing is an appropriate choice for illustrating the
applicability of such a theory to a specific set of social
relations in that housing is both an essential element
of the domestic labor process and an important product of capitalist labor processes (Somerville, 1998).
Empirically, starting in the late 1990s (Lee, Murie et al.,
1995), the concept of “social exclusion” has been applied in analysis of public housing in mostly European
countries. The main area of such debates lies in the
processes of “exclusion through housing” in two ways:
residence in particular localities may reinforce difficulties in securing access to employment opportunities and
citizenship benefits; and housing processes which deny
certain groups control over their lives and reduce access
to wide citizenship rights (Marsh and Mullins, 1998).
Some also argue that housing is not the causation of
distinctive bases of social exclusion, but rather an expression of exclusionary effects (Somerville 1998).
To what extent can the concept of social exclusion
through housing (Smith, 1989; Massey and Denton,
1993) be applied to understanding migrant enclaves in
the contexts of Chinese cities? Some China scholars
argued that migrant enclaves have a signification role
in the urbanization process in China by promoting
considerable housing for rural immigrants. It is hence
tempting that instead of causation of social exclusion,
migrant enclaves can have positive effects with regard
to social exclusion in Chinese contexts.
Method and Sample
The data for this research was collected with the aid
of semi-structured, in-depth qualitative interviews. The
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
fieldwork was conducted between May and July 2010.
Four villages, one urban village in Guangzhou, two in
Shenzhen, and one shantytown (penghuqu) in Shanghai
were selected as study cases. Oral consent was obtained
before interviews. The interviews lasted 20 to 40 minutes.
The interviews were audio recorded and later transcribed verbatim in order to analyze the information.
The questions for this interview were open-ended and
were designed to explore residential experience of rural
immigrants. Specifically, we investigated the housing
conditions of rural immigrants living in these neighborhoods, their residential history, housing-related decision-making process, and residential mobility. It aims
to understand how rural migrants adapt their housing
conditions and housing choices in response to institutional barriers.
Interviews are tailored to rural immigrants, yet some
urban immigrants are also included so that comparisons
can be made between rural and urban immigrants to
discern the effect of hukou. Interviewees were recruited
by random indoor visits. I visited each village at late
morning and late afternoon to avoid meal time or bed
time. Two steps were taken before an interviewee was
recruited: first, asking for permission to interview; and
second, asking for background information, such as
length of stay in the city, to decide whether they fall in
the category of our target interviewees. Age is one important indicator to decide whether one is a household
decision maker. Teenagers are usually with their parents
or brothers/sisters, therefore, they are excluded from the
samples.
In sum, 43 interviews were conducted, among which
14 were conducted in Guangzhou, 17 in Shenzhen and
12 in Shanghai (Table 1). Three group interviews were
conducted (No. 0104, 0112, and 0201). As a result, the
total number of participants is 47, 11 of whom hold
urban hukou. Their ages range from the early 20s to late
50s.
Selected cases
Guangzhou, Shenzhen, and Shanghai are three major
destination cities for rural immigration. In 2005, nearly
half of the de facto population in Shenzhen were rural
workers; and for Guangzhou and Shanghai, the percentages were 18 percent and 15 percent respectively,
double the national average of 8 percent (State Council
and State Statistical Bureau, 2007).
239
Refereed Full-Papers
Shipai village in Guangzhou
Shipai village is the first urban village in Guangzhou.
Due to its proximity to two local prestigious universities and the prosperous economic development of its
neighbors, this urban village accommodates a large
population of immigrants. It is estimated that Shipai
village hosts about 60,000 rural migrants, which also
makes it the biggest urban village in Guangzhou.
Dachong village and Baishizhou village in Shenzhen
Dachong and Baishizhou were selected as two cases
in Shenzhen because, on the one hand, most residents
have moved out of Dachong village due to the ongoing
redevelopment of this village; on the other hand, many
of those who moved out of Dachong went to Baishizhou village due to their proximity. Therefore, the two
cases are conducive to understanding the dynamic
process of residential mobility of rural immigrants by
comparing the two villages.
Dachong and Baishizhou are two big urban villages
located within Shenzhen’s special economic zone.
Dachong village is adjacent to the Shenzhen high-tech
industrial park. Before redevelopment, this village used
to accommodate over 60,000 residents and more than
1,400 buildings. Baishizhou is one of the biggest urban
villages in Shenzhen. Located to the southwest of Dachong village, this village is one of the alternatives for
residents who have to move out of Dachong village.
Caojiacun village in Shanghai
Caojiacun village is one of remaining shantytowns
in Shanghai. It is located to the west of Shanghai’s inner city. According to local villagers, to date, there are
around 3,000 households living here and the population
exceeds 20,000. Most residents are immigrant renters.
Understanding Migrant Enclaves Through The Lens
of Rural Immigrants
Substandard living conditions
There are four types of residential patterns for immigrants in the three migrant enclaves. Most respondents
(30 out of 47) rent a residence for one single household;
some (nine respondents) live with other households to
split the rent. Those who run a business in the village
tend to rent a place for both residence and business,
which are usually located on the first floor (four respondents). Their apartments not only serve as a place to
live, but also as a storage room or a “factory” for their
business. Finally, there are immigrants (four respon-
240 dents) who live with colleagues in a dormitory provided
by their employers.
The fieldwork observation is consistent with previous
findings that the overall neighborhood environment in
migrant enclaves is substandard: overly narrow streets
and pathways, cracked road surfaces and “hand-shaking” buildings. The building heights in the three urban
villages mostly range from four to seven stories, which
are much higher than that in the shantytown (typically
three stories). Due to the high density and height of the
buildings, most housing properties in the urban villages are hardly ever exposed to sunlight. Darkness and
dampness are common issues for most housing units in
urban villages. While Caojiacun has fewer of these issues; due to old age, the structural condition of its buildings is much worse than the other three neighborhoods.
Exterior walls of many buildings have started to peel
off. Most housing units in urban villages have individual
bathrooms and kitchens, although these two are usually
combined in one single room. The majority of housing
units in Caojiacun do not have bathrooms or kitchens.
Instead, residents have to use public toilets in the village
and public shower rooms outside of the village.
Overcrowding is a severe issue for rural immigrants
in all four villages. Statistics shows that the average living space per capita of Guangzhou city in 2008 reaches
20.3 square meters (Guangzhou Economic and Social
Development Statistics Bulletin, 2009). However, the
majority of rural immigrants in this study live in a space
smaller than half of that. Living space per capita for
most households ranges from 3 square meters to 10
square meters. Only six housing units visited exceed 10
square meters per capita, and four of them are occupied
by households with urban hukou.
For most rural immigrants, their room is no more
than a place to sleep and rest. Many of them live in a
studio, where there is one living room/bedroom and one
bathroom/kitchen. A bed, a table or desk, a cabinet, and
a TV are the only furniture for many households. Home
appliances such as a refrigerator and washer are mostly
not available, because there is not enough room and
many of them cannot afford these appliances or utility
costs.
In addition to crowding, rural immigrants are more
likely to live in the worst locations in the village, such
as end of an alley, or a less-preferred floor in a building, usually the first floor and the garret at the top floor.
Rooms at these locations are mostly dark, damp, and
with poor ventilation systems. The garrets are places
Refereed Full-Papers
where thefts usually take place. Among respondents in
these villages, those who live in these locations are all
rural immigrants. Overall, although the living conditions of rural immigrants could meet their basic needs, crowding, lack of
housing facilities and poor neighborhood environments
make migrant enclaves far from quality residential
space, such as the expensive commodity housing estates
for the urban nouveau riches.
Residential Mobility: Spatially Trapped Tenants
Consistent with previous studies (Wu, 2006), rural
immigrants are found to have high rates of residential mobility between different locations in the city or
between properties in one neighborhood. Most rural immigrants interviewed live in one housing unit for three
months to three years; very few of them would stay in
one place for longer than that.
It has been argued that groups with higher incomes
and better education tend to make an active use of
space whereas lower income groups tend to be trapped
by it (Pahl, 1965). This is true for rural immigrants in
migrant enclaves. Despite high rates of residential mobility, the majority of rural immigrants only move from
one migrant enclave to another, or between migrant
enclaves and work sheds. There is little mobility out of
such a residential space.
Only a few interviewees ever lived in other forms of
neighborhoods, such as commodity housing estates or
danwei compounds. In cases where they had, interviewees had social networks or had found enough acquaintances to split the rent so that they could afford to live
in these places; once the network in the new neighborhood breaks down or their roommates were gone, they
move back to migrant enclaves. A rural immigrant
(Case 0411) in Shanghai used to live in two commodity
housing units with her colleagues for a couple of years.
For the first one, a two-bedroom unit, the rent of 1,000
CNY was split between six people. For the other unit,
the rent of 1,200 CNY was split between four people.
She had to move back to the shantytown after her colleagues left because she could not find colleagues to
share the rent with her.
Among the four major reasons found associated
with interviewees’ moving decisions, improving living
conditions or neighborhood environments was the least
mentioned. Given other conditions, no moving decisions will be made unless the housing unit is so poor
that safety or their properties are threatened, and this
usually results in intra-neighborhood mobility. A male
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
in Shipai village moved to the unit he lives now because
the prior unit was flooded in a big rain.
Moved because of theft, Shipai village, Guangzhou
Case 0112, a couple, around 40 years old. Rural immigrants, both work in a hotel near the village. They
live in a sandwich room between two floors of about
8 square meters. They came to Guangzhou when
they were in their 20s and lived in current housing
unit for more than three years.
“We always looked for places to live within this
village. We moved four to five times. In the beginning, we just wanted to find a cheap room only for
sleep after work. Gradually we found it was important that the room stayed dry even in rainy days,
because clothes never got dry in a damp room… We
used to live in a garrote on the top floor because it
was cheap and our salary was just about 600 CNY
(about 90 U.S. dollars). At that time, the landlord
didn’t take any measures against theft or other safety
issues so the thief got a chance to come to our apartment from the garrote of a neighboring building and
stole our gas tank while we were sleeping. We were
afraid of living there since then.”
Changes in household structure, such as getting
married, having a child, or the arrival or departure of a
household member, usually lead to mobility from one
housing unit to another in the same neighborhood. They
tend to move to a bigger unit with a larger household
size and a smaller unit if the household size decreases.
For instance, a female in Baishi village moved from a
studio she shared with her husband to a much smaller
room after her husband left for another city.
Relocation to another neighborhood takes place when
one changes a job or when migrant workers are forced
to move against their will. In most cases, the respondents would live near their working locations to save
commuting time and costs. In some circumstances, rural
immigrants do not have choices but to move, either to
another unit or another neighborhood. These reasons
include an increase in rent which makes the current
housing unit no longer affordable to them; a housing
unit that needs renovation and the landlord forces them
to leave; or that the village they currently live in is to be
rebuilt.
Regardless of the different reasons for their residential mobility decisions, migrant enclaves seem to be
the only resort for most rural immigrants. They tend to
be trapped in the lower end of housing stock in a city,
241
Refereed Full-Papers
particularly migrant enclaves, because they are unable to achieve transfers off the state or have no social
networks outside of these neighborhoods. On the contrary, although most urban immigrants live in migrant
enclaves, they have more choices of housing units than
rural immigrants and are able to make active decisions
to stay in such a space. For instance, one female in
Shipai village who owned a property in Guangzhou city
claimed that she and her husband would live in the village because they wanted to save money for investment
in their real estate business.
Constraints of Housing Choices: Social Exclusions
Instead of “exclusion through housing,” the proponents of which argue that residence in particular
localities may reinforce difficulties in securing access
to employment opportunities and citizenship benefits, it
is found that the spatial entrapment of rural immigrants
results from multiple constraints of the housing choices
they face, which indicates multidimensional exclusionary effects arising from institutional structures, labor
process, and deprivation of welfare. Rural immigrants
tend to make passive housing decisions. On the one
hand, they complain about the poor living conditions
and desire better housing units; but at the same time,
they expressed helplessness with the current situation
due to limited choices and have to adapt to it.
This woman has to move to another place, and she
was looking for an affordable to live yet a better
one so that her son and his girlfriend could live with
them, “… We can’t afford a house with a rent of
higher than 1,500 CNY… but most housing units with
kitchens and bathrooms in this area are above that
price. We would like to find a better place, but it’s too
expensive; yet at the same time, neither do we want
to wrong our son by living in a worse apartment…
“—Case 0403, Caojiacun, rural immigrant
The first and foremost constraint facing rural immigrants in housing sector is low income and inaccessibility to desirable housing units. The commodity housing
units are mostly out of their reach. Most respondents
only have a budget of 150 to 250 CNY (about 20 to 35
US dollars) per person for rent, which is about 10 to 20
percent of their monthly household income, not including other housing expenses. This proportion could add
up to an estimate of over 90 percent or more on an
apartment in commodity housing estates in the inner
city.
242 “The upper limit of rent for us is 1,000 yuan (for
three family members). … We all come here to make
money, which is not easy. Like me, I’m doing a cleaning job and make only about 1,000 yuan every month.
My husband earns about 3,000 to 4,000 yuan each
month. We need to pay the rent and the utilities, that
leaves us barely any money. So we can’t afford to live
in a better house… ” — Case 0303, Baishizhou village, rural immigrant.
In addition to high rent, insecure job and employment
status is another hurdle for rural immigrants to overcome. Even if some of them may consider sharing rent
with others in a better neighborhood, it is not always
their decision to make because they are not welcomed
in these neighborhoods, as the home owners there
would not trust their financial ability to live there unless
they have social networks.
Lack of social networks outside migrant enclaves is
therefore another factor that confines workers in these
enclaves. Scholars have pointed out the importance of
kinship and friendship ties acting as social institutions
in housing decisions (Banerjee, 1983; Conway, 1985).
And it is shown that rural immigrants tend to rely on
strong ties, which are relatively socially homogenous,
rather than weak ties. Most respondents found the
current village to live through references from their
family members, relatives, or other fellow-townsmen
(laoxiang). Some are recommended by friends. Only a
few found a place to live by themselves or through an
estate agent. To a number of immigrants, reference of
an acquaintance is essential to find a place to live when
they first come to the city (case 0110). Those who have
settled down in the city tend to find a place on their own
through posted rent advertisements in the neighborhoods or an estate agent.
“I have fellow townsmen there (so I came here). Nonlocal people wouldn’t come here unless they have
fellow townsmen who are familiar with it, otherwise
how could you know (it’s good to come here)…You
don’t know here well, where could you live?” – Case
0110, Shipai village, rural immigrant
Kinship and friendship ties are also important for
housing decisions involving moving into a more expensive commodity housing estate or when workers are
in need of help finding a place to live. They would not
move into a commodity housing unit unless they could
Refereed Full-Papers
find their relatives or colleagues to move with them. In
cases when they could not find a place to live, staying with fellow townsmen is one way for them to get
through a transitional time.
Rural immigrant couples or households with children
face more constraints than individuals without a family
in the city. They could not live in dorms provided by
their employers if they want to stay with their family;
neither would they consider living with other unacquainted households in order to maintain privacy and
convenience. For case 0202 in Dachong village, who
was forced to move out of the village due to the renovation project, it was more difficult for him to find a
suitable place to live because of his child:
“We are now looking around this area… If we can’t
find any place to live, I don’t know (what to do). We
don’t have any choices. If it were not for the child,
we could live anywhere… (Would you consider living with other households?) We have a kid, sot it’s
not convenient to live with others. And it’s not an
economic way either. Now it may cost as much as
2,000 yuan to 3,000 yuan to rent an apartment with
two bedrooms and one living room. Housing prices
are too high. ” – Case 0202, Dachong village, rural
immigrant
As a result, rural immigrants have to minimize their
requirements for housing so that they can at least afford a place to stay in the city. Rural immigrants would
rather sacrifice comfort of living conditions for marginal economic benefits in the city because they cannot
“spend every penny they earn on housing” (excerpt
from an interview). However, their urban counterparts
have more choices. One urban immigrant in Baishzhou
village indicated that he could move to a better housing
unit if he wanted, yet he thought it was not necessary.
Role of Migrant Enclaves: Exclusion or Inclusion
Through Housing?
What do migrant enclaves mean to rural immigrants?
To understand the role of migrant enclaves in rural
immigrants’ residential experience, interviewees were
asked whether the renovation of such neighborhoods
could affect their life in the city and what they would do
if such villages are reformed.
Different attitudes were found among interviewees in
Shipai village of Guanzhou, Baishizhou village of Shenzhen, and the Caojiacun of Shanghai. Most of them
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
hold positive attitudes. They anticipated no tremendous
affects that the redevelopment projects would bring to
their life. Some of them have dorms provided by their
employers as “backup” accommodation, which to some
extent relieves their concerns, while others believed
that there would always be a way out, such as moving
to suburban area or sharing an apartment with others.
However, some concerns were still expressed about the
difficulty of finding an affordable place to live after the
renewal of migrant enclaves. Some said they would
have to go home if they could not find one.
Despite the indifference with the future of the village
they currently live in, migrant enclaves are still regarded as the primary, even the only choice for rural immigrants in Shipai, Baishizhou, and Caojiacun villages
should the current village be redeveloped. Most of
them expected to move to other villages on the fringe
of the city. Other neighborhoods, including commodity
housing estates which were regarded as unaffordable,
were not an option for most of them.
“I don’t wish to tear down this village. … Where are
we supposed to move then? For one thing, you have
to rent a place. As for those housing units in commodity housing estates, those are definitely out of our
reach. … Those units are all apartment complexes
which are always expensive. There are no studios for
us. Why tear it down? ... We would have to go back
home if it really happens…. We would only consider
villages like this. What else could you consider? You
can only find places worse than this, not better.“ — Case 0306, Baishizhou village, rural immigrant
While respondents in Shipai, Baishizhou, and
Caojiacun villages responded to a hypothetical scenario, Dachong village in Shenzhen, which is now
undergoing a redevelopment project, provides a real
picture of how rural immigrants respond to such a rapid
change. Contrasting positive attitudes in the other three
villages, much depression and passive responses were
observed in this case. The renewal of this village has
resulted in difficulty for rural immigrants in finding a
suitable place to stay. A large number of immigrants
who moved out of the village to the surrounding neighborhoods are believed to have given rise to increasing demand and rising rents in housing units in those
villages. One respondent in Baishizhou village told us
that the rent in this village has doubled since before
the renovation of Dachong village. Consequently, most
243
Refereed Full-Papers
interviewees had to move away, often as far as beyond
the Special Economic Zone of Shenzhen. Many of
them are even forced to go home because of high living
expenses in the city.
Households who have school-age children and those
who owned a small business in the village are facing
more constraints due to the renewal project, such as
Case 0202, who is considering sending his child, currently at primary school, back to his hometown because
he could not afford a place to keep his business near the
school district. Another male had to end his business
because his established networks in the area will be
ended after the demolition of the village.
While public housing in European countries has been
criticized for intensifying social exclusion due to their
fixity in poor locations, proximity to job opportunities
is another major reason for rural immigrants to move
into migrant enclaves. As a matter of fact, in the long
run, migrant enclaves could serve as a bridge for rural
immigrants to achieve social and residential mobility. In
their home cities, few rural immigrants have obtained
homeownership, even though the housing prices are
much lower than in the coastal cities. In this regard,
in the Chinese contexts, where deficiency in housing
provision for rural immigrants is widely felt, migrant
enclaves play a significant role as a transitional space
for them to realize upward social mobility by providing
considerable affordable accommodation and job opportunities in the city.
Conclusions and Discussion
Drawing upon 43 semi-structured interviews about
the residential experiences of rural immigrants in four
migrant enclaves of three cities, this research provides
new perspectives towards of the formation of such
social spaces in urban China through the lens of spatial
and social exclusion. It also adds to the debate of social
exclusion and housing in western societies by providing
empirical evidence within the context of a developing
country.
It is found that rural immigrants live in sub-standard
housing units with regard to living space, air conditioning, and housing facilities. Despite the high residential
mobility observed during their stay in the city, rural
immigrants are spatially trapped in the residential space
of lower-end housing units because most of them only
move between different migrant enclaves or those
housing units with equivalent, if not worse, living
conditions, such as dorms or work sheds. Such spatial
entrapment is believed to represent multidimensional
244 social exclusions imposed on rural immigrants, which
have incurred multiple constraints of housing choices,
such as low income, insecure employment status, and
lack of social networks. As a result, rural immigrants
tend to make passive housing decisions. They comprise
comfort of living space to achieve marginal economic
benefits in the long run, have low standards of housing preferences, and have to adapt to the environment
despite their dissatisfaction with it.
Due to affordability and proximity to job locations,
migrant enclaves like urban villages and shantytowns
hence serve as a prime, if not the only, residential
space for rural immigrants. Despite many respondents’
indifference, the undergoing reform of such residential
neighborhoods can force most rural immigrants to move
farther to the fringes of the city or to more expensive
commodity housing units, sharing with other households. The case of Dachong village, a reforming village,
reveals a picture where a good number of rural immigrants are placed into a desperate situation as a result of
redevelopment. Despite their poor living environments,
migrant enclaves play a significant role in promoting
social mobility of rural immigrants in the long run by
providing cheap accommodation and employment opportunities in the city, where they have the chance to
achieve a decent life. In this regard, social inclusion is
somewhat promoted through migrant enclaves.
Therefore, it is critical for policymakers to realize the
significant values of migrant enclaves to society. Within
the vacuum of provision of public housing for rural
immigrants, renovation of these migrant enclaves can
intensify social exclusion if no consideration is given
to rural immigrants. New initiatives, such as improving
unsatisfactory living conditions rather than eliminating
the entire space, should therefore be considered for the
redevelopment of these villages.
References:
Banerjee, B. (1983). “Social networks in the migration process:
Empirical evidence on chain migration in India.” Journal of
Developing Areas 17(2): 185-196.
Chan, R. C. K., Y. M. Yao, et al. (2003). “Self-help housing
strategy for temporary population in Guangzhou, China.”
Habitat International 27: 19-35.
Chen, Y. (2006). Penghuqu: Jiyi zhong de shenghuo shi (Shanty
town: Life history in the memory). Shanghai, China, Shanghai
Guji Press.
Chung, H. (2010). “Building an image of Villages-in-theCity: A Clarification of China’s Distinct Urban Spaces.”
International Journal of Urban and Regional Research
34(2): 421-437.
Refereed Full-Papers
Conway, D. (1985). “Changing perspectives on squatter
settlements, intraurban mobility, and constraints on housing
choice of the Third World urban poor.” Urban Geography
6(2): 170-192.
Hills, J. (2001). “Inclusion or exclusion? The role of housing
subsidies and benefits.” Urban Studies 38(11): 1887-1902.
Lan, Y. (2004). Dushi lide cunzhuang (A village within the city).
Beijing, Sanlun Shudian.
Lan, Y. and R. Zhang (2005). “An approach to causes of
emergence of villages in cities - a case study on Shipai
village in Guangzhou city.” Chinese Rural Economy(11):
68-74.
Lee, P., A. Murie, et al. (1995). The Price of Social Exclusion. N.
F. o. H. Associations). London.
Lefebvre, H. (1991). The production of space, Wiley-Blackwell.
Li, L. (2001). Guangzhou chengzhongcun xingchen he gaizao
jizhi yanjiu (A study on the formation and reform mechanism
of villages-in-the-city in Guangzhou). School of Geography
and Planning. Guangzhou, Zhongshan University. Doctoral.
Liu, X. L. and W. Liang (1997). “Zhejiangcun: Social and spatial
implications of informal urbanization on the periphery of
Beijing.” Cities 14(2): 95-108.
Liu, Y. and F. Wu (2006). “Urban poverty neighborhoods:
typology and spatial concentration under China’s market
transition, a case study of Nanjing.” Geoforum 37: 610-626.
Ma, L. J. C. and B. Xiang (1998). “Native place, migration
and the emergence of peasant enclaves in Beijing.” China
Quarterly(155): 546-581.
MADANIPOUR, A. (1998). Social exclusion and space. Social
Exclusion in European Cities: Processes, Experiences and
Responses. G. C. a. J. A. A. MADANIPOUR. London,
Jessica Kingsley: 75–94.
Marsh, A. and D. Mullins (1998). “The social exclusion
perspective and housing studies: Origins, applications and
limitations.” Housing Studies 13(6): 749-759.
Massey, D. S. and N. A. Denton (1993). American Apartheid:
Segregation and the Making of the Under Class. Cambridge,
MA, Harvard University Press.
Ministry of Construction, Development and Reform Commission
(abbr.), et al. (2007). Guanyu gaishan nongmingong juzhu
tiaojian de zhidao yijian (Guidelines on Improving Living
Condition of Rural Workers)
Pahl, R. (1965). Urbs in rure. Geographical paper 2, London:
London School of Economics.
Ratcliffe, P. (1998). “’Race’, housing and social exclusion.”
Housing Studies 13(6): 807-818.
Room, G. (1995). “Poverty in Europe: Competing paradigms of
analysis.” Policy and Politics 23(2): 103-113.
Smith, S. J. (1989). The Politics of ‘Race’ and Residence:
Citizenship, Segregation and White Supremacy in Britain.
Cambridge, Polity Press.
Solinger, D. J. (1993). “China’s transients and the state: a form of
civil society?” Politics and Society Vol. 21(No. 1): pp. 91.
May 2011 – Make No Little Plans
Somerville, P. (1998). “Explanations of social exclusion: Where
does housing fit in?” Housing Studies 13(6): 761-780.
Song, Y., Y. Zenou, et al. (2008). “Let’s not throw the baby out
with the bath water: The role of urban villages in housing
rural migrants in China.” Urban Studies 45(2): 313-330.
State Council and State Statistical Bureau (2007). 2005 Nian
Quanguo 1% Renkou Diaocha Ziliao (Tabulation on 2005
1% National Population Sample Survey). Beijing, China
Statistics Press.
Tian, L. (2008). “The Chengzhongcun land market in
China:Boon or bane? — A perspective on property rights.”
International Journal of Urban and Regional Research
32(2): 282–304.
Walker, A. (1997). Introduction: the strategy of inequality.
Britain Divided. The Growth of Social Exclusion in the 1980s
and 1990s. A. Walker and C. Walker. London, CPAG.
Wang, C. (1995). Shehui liudong he shehui chonggou —
Jingcheng Zhejiangcun yanjiu (Social mobility and social
restructuring: a study of Zhejiangcun in Beijing). Zhejiang,
China, Zhejiang Renmin Chubanshe(Zhejiang People’s
Press).
Wang, F. and X. J. Zuo (1999). “Inside China’s cities:
Institutional barriers and opportunities for urban migrants.”
American Economic Review 89(2): 276-280.
Wang, F. L. (2004). “Reformed migration control and new
targeted people: China’s Hukou system in the 2000s.” China
Quarterly(177): 115-132.
Wang, Y. (2004). Urban poverty, housing and social change in
China. London and New York, Routledge.
Wang, Y. P. (2000). “Housing reform and its impacts on the urban
poor in China.” Housing Studies 15(6): 845-864.
Wang, Y. P. and A. Murie (2000). “Social and spatial implications
of housing reform in China.” International Journal of Urban
and Regional Research 24(2): 397-417.
Wolch, J. and M. Dear (1989). How territory shapes social life.
The power of geography: How territory shapes social life. J.
Wolch and M. Dear. Boston, Unwin Hyman: 9.
Wu, W. (2004). “Sources of migrant housing disadvantage in
urban China.” Environment and Planning A 36: 1285-1304.
Wu, W. P. (2002). “Migrant housing in urban China - Choices
and constraints.” Urban Affairs Review 38(1): 90-119.
Wu, W. P. (2006). “Migrant intra-urban residential mobility in
urban China.” Housing Studies 21(5): 745-765.
Zhang, L., S. X. B. Zhao, et al. (2003). “Selfhelp in housing
and Chengzhongcun in China’s urbanization.” International
Journal of Urban and Regional Research 27: 912-937.
Zhou, D. and C. Gao (2001). “Chengxiang jiehebu shequ de
yanjiu (Studies on urban-rural integration zones).” Shehuixue
Yanjiu (Studies on Sociology) 4: 99-108.
Zhu, J. M. (2004). “Local developmental state and order in
China’s urban development during transition.” International
Journal of Urban and Regional Research 28(2): 424.
245
Refereed Full-Papers
246