Today`s children – Tomorrow`s parents no 37-38
Transcription
Today`s children – Tomorrow`s parents no 37-38
CONTENTS EDITORIAL Barbara ONGARI........................................................................................................................2 THE RESILIENCE OF ADOPTED CHILDREN IN ROMANIA Ana MUNTEAN, Roxana UNGUREANU....................................................................................8 DISORGANIZATION AND RESILIENCE: INTERVENTION WITH CHILDREN IN THERAPEUTIC INSTITUTION Estelle GRAVRAND...................................................................................................................15 EARLY DEPRIVATION AND BEHAVIOURAL ADAPTATION IN A SAMPLE OF ITALIAN ADOPTED ADOLESCENTS Paola MOLINA, Marta CASONATO, Barbara ONGARI, Alessandro DECARLI.....................24 ASSESSING ATTACHMENT IN FOSTER PARENTS AND ADOPTIVE PARENTS TO BE Karin LUNDÉN.........................................................................................................................33 ATTACHMENT, MOURNING AND RESILIENCE IN KAFALA AND ADOPTION Badra MOUTASSEM-MIMOUNI..............................................................................................39 UP FRONT AND PERSONAL: ADOPTIVE PARENTS’ PERCEPTIONS OF THEIR EASTERN EUROPEAN CHILDREN’S ADOPTION OUTCOMES IN CONTEXT Josephine A. RUGGIERO..........................................................................................................49 PILOT STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSIVE TEACHING ON YOUNG ADOPTED CHILDREN AND THEIR PARENTS: A COMPARISON OF TWO LEVELS OF TREATMENT INTENSITY Gerald MAHONEY, Sunghee NAM, Frida PERALES...............................................................67 RESILIENT LIVES AND AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL SUGGESTIONS ITALIEN NATIONAL TRAINING PROCESS IN THE FIELD OF INTERCOUNTRY ADOPTION Giorgio MACARIO....................................................................................................................85 Instructions for authors ............................................................................................................93 Advertising ................................................................................................................................96 1 EDITORIAL: ADOPTION, ATTACHMENT AND RESILIENCE Barbara ONGARI1 This monographic number of the TCTP Journal is in honor of the chairperson of its scientific committee, Blaise Pierrrehumbert, developmental psychologist, director of the Research Unit of the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at the Lausanne University (SUPEA). He is one of the most important Authors who strongly contributed to the development of the attachment theory, in particularly to the realization of reliable measure tools for attachment for middle childhood and adulthood. His research topics are attachment, emotional regulation, stress responses, neuro-endocrinian reactivity, traumatism, and prematurity. As leader of several studies granted by the Swiss National Science Foundation, Pierrehumbert and colleagues showed that the premature birth (threat for the child’s survival, child exposed to invasive care procedures) could represent a traumatic experience for the parents and could affect later child outcomes. Also the relationships between attachment, as an expression of emotional regulation, and psychopathology in children have been explored. With other grants, Pierrehumbert and colleagues explored specific pathologies such as hyperactivity in children and adolescents. His studies on adults who experienced a trauma in childhood or adolescence (i.e. sexual abuse) showed the prevalence of disorganized attachment representations in these individuals, which can interfere with the endocrine responses to mildly stressful circumstances, causing a disconnection between the acute subjective perception of stress and natural defensive body reactions (suppression of HPAA response). One of his most significant scientific initiative, during last years, has been the creation of “The Attachment and Adoption Research Network “ (AARN), an unique cross-cultural group of scientific researchers in psychology, coming from many different parts of the world on the basis of a common interest in understanding the development of attachment bonds in children deprived of their blood family. In particular, they shared a common research core project proposed by Blaise Pierrrehumbert, in order to analyze the attachment bonds towards both 1 Professor, PhD, University of Trento, Italy E-mail: Barbara.Ongari@unitn.it 2 parental figures and with friends during adolescence, using the same battery of narrative tools, as well as other behavioral measures. Each national study realized within the AARN network aimed at explore the nature of the attachment relationships between the internationally adopted children and their adoptive families, as well with peers and other people of the community, in an ecological perspective. Moreover, each national team did integrate this common plan with other research topics, using specific methods. The AARN network met in correspondence of some international congress: Barcelona 2009, Leiden 2010, Oslo 2011, Paris 2012, Lausanne 2013 and Timisoara 2014. It appears that these meetings have been held on an annual base and their main characteristic has been not only the opportunity for exchanging and discussing the scientific findings: a network of colleagues and friends has resulted. Some of the articles proposed in this number of TCTP (Ana Muntean and Roxana Ungureanu, Estelle Gravrand, Karin Lunden, Paola Molina, Marta Casonato, Barbara Ongari and Alessandro Decarli, Badra Moutassem-Mimouni) are the product of the work of some AARN fellows. The issues focus on adoption in the perspective of the attachment theory as well on the possible connections between successful adoption’s outcomes and future resilience, starting from the evidence of the ill-quality of the previous relational experience of the adopted children. In this sense, the multiple facets of the concept of resilience itself are addressed. Each contribution proposed here offers interesting considerations coming from the research as well as from the clinical, training and caring practice, highlighting some specific aspects of the complexity which characterizes the nature of the interactions inside the adoptive families. Adoption could be actually considered in the Western Countries an important form of “social parenthood”, so that many studies already contributed to investigate the nature of the relational bonds among adopters and adoptees. The negative role played on the adoptees’ psycho-social development by some crucial factors as their age at adoption, the nature of the previous relationships inside the blood families, the multiple placements, the length of institutionalization, the quality of the functioning of the hosting institutions has been empirically demonstrated. The negative early relational experiences increase the risk for short-term and long-term developmental dysfunctional behaviors (Castle, Groothues, Beckett, Colvert, Hawkins, Kreppner, Kumsta, Schlotz, Sonuga-Barke, Stevens, Rutter, 2009). The possibility of experimenting high quality care giving environments inside the adoptive family has been indicated as a main turning point in the children’s development, because this could empower their socio-affective and emotional skills, their emotion regulation and their behavioral competence, opening better opportunities for their future. The task for the adoptive parents is a complex one, because they not only must cope with the children’s current difficulties and needs, planning sensitive and helpful strategies that could allow them to feel loved and valued within the new family, but also of healing their previous experience of loss and trauma, in a way that has been defined as “therapeutic” (Schofield, Beek, 3 2006). This could explain the greater amount of requests for psycho-social and clinical support claimed by the adoptive parents and the fact that adopted children are over-represented in the mental health services (Juffer, Van IJzendoorn, 2005). Anyway, this search for help and this openness for receiving advice and support should be considered in itself a protective factor. The consciousness that parenting these children requires a great amount of flexibility, mind-mindedness and deep reflective abilities could help the adopters in re-adjusting their personal lives as well as the whole family functioning. This implies also a deep and difficult inner work for processing the pain related to the limit that stops the couple’s expectation of having a biological generation: so that they must leave the desire of having their own child and work hardly for developing the motivation of caring a child who has had other blood parents. The protective role of adoption could be implemented by the quality of the psycho-social interventions aimed at preparing and supporting the adoptive families. A large body of research based on attachment theory demonstrated that a warm, empathetic, coherent and reflective care giving allow the adoptees to progressively heal their inner negative representations and their expectations of themselves and of the others, constructing more regulated and adjusted behaviors. So that the most important factor in order to build a safe relationship between adopters and adoptees might be the secure adopters’ state of mind toward the relationships, which in turn influences the quality of their daily care and of their discipline strategies. The effects of early deprivation appear of paramount evidence during the adoptees’ adolescence. Adopted teenagers are therefore often considered as an “at risk population”. Some studies indicated that among the various factors that could affect these outcomes an important role is played by the fact that, while entering this developmental period, the adoptees begin to question more intensively their identity, as their awareness of the cultural and ethnic differences with their parents increase (Bimmel, Juffer, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2003; Van den Dries, Juffer, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2009). In effect, to date very few studies investigated the cross-cultural impact on adopted children played by the different child-rearing practices. The article of Ana Muntean and Roxana Ungureanu focuses the connection between adoption and resilience in a sample of Romanian adopted teenagers, hypothesizing that resilience should be considered a positive outcome of adoption. The fact of having developed a secure attachment to the new parental figures is supposed as acting as a dynamic process which can foster resilience. This could be inferred by the result that, despite their aversive previous relational experience of abandonment, the narratives of these children evidenced their ability of constructing positive relationships inside and outside the family, their sense of personal worthiness, a well developed self-confidence and sense of humor and their positive expectations toward their future lives. 4 Estelle Gravrand presents the first results of some case-analysis which are part of a longitudinal in progress study concerning the effects of the therapeutic care offered to deprived children by a family type facility, inspired to an artistic approach which is supposed as mediator for building resilience. The assessment of attachment made for three severely deprived boys at their first entrance inside the care center evidenced some important elements of attachment disorganization and behavioral disturbances, which could easily considered as consequences of the deep emotional trauma and neglect they have experimented during their first years of life. A second evaluation of attachment, two and half years after, provided also ethological observations of their verbal and non-verbal behaviors and other behavioral measures. Obviously each individual profiles of these three boys is unique and differs from the others for what concerns the subjective modalities with which everyone is processing his own history of life and is trying to construct more adequate relational strategies. It’s not at all surprising that the healing and the recovery psychological dynamics could take a long time, so these three teenagers appear still working in this direction. Nevertheless the results demonstrate that they have achieved a better capacity of mentalizing and offer-organizing their relational strategies in a more resilient way. The two research units in charge of the core project in Italy aimed at analyzing the attachment representations of internationally adopted adolescents (respectively Paola Molina with Marta Casonato at the University of Turin and Barbara Ongari with Alessandro Decarli at the University of Trento) focus on the quality of the behavioral adjustment of sample of 28 participants adopted before their sixth year of age. The role of some risk factors that characterized their pre-adoptive experience, collected from their parents as well from the participants themselves, un-expectedly resulted not associated to these adolescents’ present social adaptation. Only the high rate of their attachment disturbances at the placement inside the adoptive family, indicated by the parents while fulfilling a standardized questionnaire (a retrospective form of the Disturbances of Attachment Interview, DAI), seem correlated with their high levels of internalizing modalities for expressing emotions. The correlation among the early deprivation they have experienced and their actual adjustment has been investigated, considering the possible moderating role played by the positive attachment representation of their adoptive parents. In her contribution Karin Lunden discusses an issue of crucial relevance related to the professional practice of approval for becoming foster and adoptive parents: the necessity to include in the psycho-social protocols a precise evaluation of the attachment security and sensitivity of the parents to be, considered as the most important predictor for a successful match with deprived children and for their future safe development and resilience. She presents some preliminary results of a qualitative research aimed at investigating the attachment patterns of foster parents using a standardized and reliable tool designed to analyze the quality of relationships to partner and to close others as well as the overall attachment style, the Attachment Style Interview (ASI). Emerged the evidence that only half of the future foster parents show secure, or even mild insecure, attachment patterns. This result indicates the need that the authorized agen- 5 cies and the professionals re-think the protocols of the assessment procedures for approval, as it confirms the indications coming from a body of studies which clearly demonstrated that foster and adopted children are at risk for developmental difficulties for what concerns their socioemotional and their cognitive skills. The main opportunity for them of healing and recovering, developing healthy, depends on the possibility of having a secure, sensitive and emotionally available inter-personal environment. In her study Badra Moutassem-Mimouni proposes some in–depth psychological considerations concerning the relational dynamics, typical of the context of the Algerian culture, involved in the search of origins acted by adolescents who have been adopted. The feelings and the different modalities of reactionacted by the adopters, when they must cope with the discovery of the family origins made by their adopted child, are analyzed. Discussing some clinical cases, Moutassem-Mimouni demonstrates that the mourning process is a two-ways one, as it concerns the parents as well as the adoptees. The importance of offering a psychological support, based on the assessment of the attachment patterns respectively of the parents and of their children, in order to facilitate the inner work of processing the mourning is stressed, as well as the necessity of implementing more precise and in-depth pre-adoption protocols, that could allow the future adopters to develop a clearer awareness of their own psychological needs and traumas. The point of view of some American adoptive parents and their perceptions of the outcomes of their adopted children coming from Eastern Europe have been deeply explored by Josephine Ruggiero in an innovative qualitative investigation conducted with 46 adopters, with the aim of analyzing the parental satisfaction from their personal and up front perspective. The core issues of the interviews were the strategies they used to deal with the challenges encountered within the long-term post-adoption period, as well as their pre-adoptive expectations toward adoption, the perceived role of the agencies that mediated the adoption process and the level of pre-adoption preparation they got. This study appears especially insightful concerning the cases where parents indicated the outcomes of their adoptees as problematic and challenging all the family lives. In the same time they propose interesting considerations on the factors that could provide successful outcomes when adopting children coming from Eastern Europe. The issue of the effectiveness of interventions designed for supporting the parents of young adopted children is questioned in the contribution of Gerald Mahoney. As maternal responsiveness has already been demonstrated by many studies as the main factor that affects the children’s socio-emotional well functioning, this pilot study assesses the feasibility in the adoption field of a parent-mediated developmental intervention called Responsive Teaching (RT) with children less than 6 years. RT is a treatment designed for encouraging parents to increase their responsive interactions and for ameliorating the adoptees’ socio-emotional well-being. It has been demonstrated as a successful strategy with non–adoptive populations of children affected by some developmental delays or disabilities, like Down syndrome or autism. Even if the results presented here are only promising and do not completely support the efficacy of RT, nevertheless the evidence of a small decrease in parenting stress as well as of an improvement in 6 mothers’ responsiveness and in children’s social functioning encourages the viability of RT as a community based intervention for providing support to pre-school adoptees and their families. Finally, Giorgio Macario discusses the positive role for the adoptees of the autobiographic approach, along all the life cycle, as a mean for constructing resilient identities. In this sense the fact of being adopted could be seen, according to Cyrulnik (2009), as an opportunity for developing resilience, on the basis of the new promising meeting with a new caring interpersonal context. The autobiographic method, in that emphasizes the value of the narrative writing, is here proposed as a main road for exploring some of the multi-disciplinary and multi-factorial components of the concept of resilience itself. To conclude, the collection of articles presented here proposes some empirical evidence as well as some clinical and training considerations, where adoption is analyzed in the light of attachment theory and as possibly connected with resilience. During the last recent years the most part of these issues did represent a central scientific concern of Blaise Pierrehumbert and his team and a shared field for collaboration and reflection with a number of colleagues all over the world. The researchers in these domains could find interesting ideas and impulses for their future investigation. The students who are approaching psychology, social work or other topics connected with social sciences can enlarge their knowledge of adoption in its complexity. We hope also that the professionals who work in the domain of adoption would be inspired for developing more and more accurate practices. References Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2003). Problem behavior of internationally adopted adolescents: A review and meta-analysis. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 11, 64-77. Cyrulnik, B. (2009). Resilience: How your inner strength can set you free from the past, Penguin Books, London, England. Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A meta-analytic approach. JAMA, Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2501-2515. Castle, J., Groothues, C., Beckett, C., Colvert, E., Hawkins, A., Kreppner, J., Kumsta, R., Schlotz, W., Sonuga-Barke, E., Stevens, S., Rutter, M. (2009). Parents’ evaluation of adoption success: A follow-up study of intercountry and domestic adoptions, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, Vol 79(4), 522-531. Schofield, G., Beek, M. (2006). Attachment Handbook for Foster Care and Adoption, BAAF, London, England. Van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2009). Fostering Security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 410-421. 7 THE RESILIENCE OF ADOPTED CHILDREN IN ROMANIA Ana MUNTEAN1 Roxana UNGUREANU2 Abstract In his famous ERA (English Romanian Adoptions) study Michael Rutter and his team found a percent of about 25% of children adopted from Romania, from very bad conditions in institutions, as being resilient. Following this observations the concept of resilience was extended to include the genetic heritage as well as the cultural framework. Our study is based on the evaluation of 56 adoptees, aged 11-16 years old, within Romanian adoptive families. The complex evaluation of adopted children and adoptive families was done within a national research project on domestic adoption, FISAN (Factors which supports the success of adoption), funded by the Minister of Education in Romania, during 2008-2011. The data used here are collected based on Friends and Family Interview (FFI), a semi-structured interview aimed to identify the quality of child’s attachment. The resilience is considerate within our study as being proved by the secure attachment of the adopted child toward his/her adoptive parent or another significant person. The complex procedure of evaluation as well as the qualitative work on the data will highlight the important emotional and social factors which support the resilience of the adopted children. Keywords: resilience, adoption, adolescents, identity Professor, PhD, Social Work Department, West University of Timisoara, Romania, E-mail: ana.muntean@e-uvt.ro 2 Researcher, Research Centre for Child Parent Interaction (CICOP), West University of Timisoara, Romania 1 8 Introduction Within human development, the resilience is a fascinating topic. When adverse existential conditions expose at risk the development of the person, the resiliency is pushing a new bounce back and even is bringing new quality in development. The concept was developed in relation with the ‘invulnerable’ and ‘invincible’ children thriving despite the scarce conditions for their life. In a classic longitudinal study (Werner&Smith, 1982) 505 individuals were followed, from their birth, until their 40s, in the Island of Kauai, Hawaii. Despite the poverty and associated adversities faced during their life, a third of them developed in a healthy, resilient way. (Luthar&all. 2000) talks about having personal capacities which can contribute to resilient outcomes following stressful conditions as well as about resilience which is a ‘dynamic’ process in place only in relation to adverse conditions. Based on the large existing literature, the authors mentioned “two critical conditions: (1) exposure to significant threat or severe adversity; and (2) the achievement of positive adaptation despite major assaults on the developmental process.” (Luthar &all, 2000). Due to this definition to talk about the resilience of adopted children is just normal. The adoption of an abandoned child brings to the child traumatized by abandonment as well as by possible events before and following the abandonment a new chance for re-bounding. Adoption as a new chance is tightly connected with the resilience process of the adopted child. During the last years more and more professionals and researchers talked about interventions aimed to stimulate the resilience (Ionescu, 2011). Even there are not yet very many voices to claim the resilience of the child as the goal of the adoption, the resilience of the child should be a pervasive objective of the child protection system in any country. Michael Rutter (2010) and his team highlighted the resilience of Romanian children adopted in UK. His study is probably one of the most known in the literature focused on child’s adoption. Following the Romanian children adopted from terrible traumatizing environment in institutions in Romania, 25% of those children, placed in adoptive families in UK, managed very well and found their way for a healthy development. They were resilient despite all the traumatic past conditions in their life. The theoretic framework of our research Our study is based on the evaluation of 56 adoptees within Romanian adoptive families. The evaluation of adopted children and adoptive families was done within a national research project on domestic adoption, FISAN (Factors which supports the success of adoption), funded by the Minister of Education in Romania, between 2008-2011. We intend to explore here the resilience of 12 Romanian adopted children, aged 11-16. Despite the traumatizing early life these children could develop a secure attachment toward their adoptive parents. The secure attachment is the guarantee for mental health and for pervasive healthy development (Schore, 2001, a). We do not identify the secure attachment with the resilience but we consider the secure attachment as being the sign of the child’s re- 9 silience as well as assurance for further life’s adversities (Fonagy, 1999). More than that, taking in account the cascade theory (Masten, Cicchetti, 2010) we consider the secure attachment as a barrier of the aversive events cascade in the life of these children who started their life unfortunately with an early traumatic event: the abandonment. Procedure Out of the 56 adoptees assessed, 12 adopted teens were assessed and identified as having clearly secure attachment. The evaluation was done with Friends and Family Interview/ FFI (Steele, & Steele, 2009). FFI is a semi structured interview assessing attachment representations in late childhood and adolescence. “The FFI holds significant research and clinical value in its unique approach to eliciting and systematically rating autobiographical narratives from an age group that has been notoriously difficult to assess from an attachment perspective.” (Kriss &all. 2012). A cross-country comparison on the invariance of FFI, focusing on the coherence in attachment narratives confirmed the validity of the coherence assessment with no difference between Romania and Belgium (Stievenart &all, 2012). In our qualitative investigation we will try to identify within the narratives of the 12 adopted teens the aspects which reveal their resilience. Following the analysis done by (Earvolino-Ramirez, 2007) on the concept of resilience we will pick up for our investigations the common protective factors which she selected based on the work of (Anthony, 1974), (Bernard, 1991), (Garmezy, 1991), (Masten, 1994), (Rutter, 1993), and (Werner&Smith, 1982). 10 These are: (1) Positive relationship, (2) sense of personal worthiness, (3) believes in her or his self efficacy, (4) sense of humor and (5) high expectations. In order to find the items above within the attachment narratives of the 12 adopted adolescents we focused on the following questions in FFI: 1. Who are the persons you are close to? 2. Tell me how you are? 3. What do you like about yourself? 4. How are you at school? Did you have recent examinations? 5. What are your three best desires for your future? Beside these items we take in considerations some common aspects within the narratives of these securely attached adopted children. We are working on our data using the qualitative narrative analysis method (Esin, 2011) focused on the narratives of the respondents and based on the 5 relevant items mentioned above. Results We analyze here the narrative of the 12 adoptees based on the 5 criteria mentioned above as being the signs of the resilience. 1. In respect to ‘positive relationship’ all these children in the sample here are securely attached. This means that they have at least one exceptional positive and healthy relationship with him or her attachment figure. The secure attachment is proving a positive internal working model (Bowlby, 1973) which in turn works as a pattern for the relationship with significant others in the child’s life. We further investigated their positive relationship through the question: Who are the persons you are close to? The 12 adolescents mentioned first the mother but half of them mentioned also the father and the friends. Grandparents and siblings are also mentioned (3 times, grandparents; 2 times, sibling). 2.The “sense of personal worthiness” was mostly investigated through the question: Tell me how you are? or What do you like about you ? The respondents mentioned very different aspects considered as personal worthiness: the courage, the talent to learn new languages, the pleasure to learn mathematics, the interest for learning, the way of thinking and even “I like the way I look”. There are two answers more elaborated: a. I like my way of thinking which is a bit different comparing with my classmates… I am not interested on the things which are common interest among peers… b. My qualities are connected. Being creative I need social relations. I cannot be creative without others. For instance I like to write and I write about people. What I like the best on me is my altruism. 3. All 12 adolescents “believe in her or his self efficacy”. The questions used to explore the self efficacy were: How are you at school? Did you have recent examinations? or even Tell me about you? All the respondents mentioned the good results in the school, insisting on different disciplines according with their interest. Two answers are relevant in this respect: a. Excepting the 5th form I was always the best in my class…yes, we had some examinations and I had good results and some of my classmates were jealous on me…but I ignore them and doesn’t matter what they say I follow my dreams.. and do what I want to do… b. I can be shay sometimes…But I like to make acquaintance with new friends and I like to fight. If I have some problems or something I cannot do it I will work on that till I become good… 4.The sense of humor is quite common within the sample of adolescents here. As an answer to the question: Tell me how you are…, out of 12 respondents, 8 are explicitly mentioning:”I like to laugh…” Mostly this assertion is followed by the mentions of different significant persons within child’s social environment: mom, the father or friends. 5.The high expectations item is explored through the question: What are yours three best desires for your future? The answers have a large variety but all of them have the power of a drive which can orientate and lead the future development of the respondents. The 12 adolescents mentioned within their narratives the wish to continue attending the school despite the fact that to this precise question only 8 respondents were explicitly mentioning the school. The dreams 11 of the children in our sample are always positives even they are more or less realistic. We give here two answers: one where the answer is formulated at general level. a. To be always beloved, to have what I need...I do not talk here about money…never to feel alone. The other one is more realistic in its expectations: 1. To study and to become doctor. To have my job and to manage by myself and not to overload my mom…to have a dog. Discussions Being a qualitative research the size of our sample is very convenient. The 5 items found globally within the literature on resilience gave us a simple tool to work on the data. This is a superficial and easy way to demonstrate an idea which is common: the secure attached children are resilient. But the question raised through our analysis is connected with the stability of attachment quality and the dynamic of resilience. According with our results we can expect that these adolescents will show always as being resilient persons; they proved once their resilience when being abandoned and placed in institutions after words, before being adopted, and being successful within their new family in setting-up a secure attachment. The literature on resilience stresses the variety of manifestations and the dynamic of resilience (Luthar &all. 2000). The person can be resilient in one situation but the same person will behave differently exposed to a new stressful situation; on the other hand, being resilient in one domain does not mean an overall resilience in 12 any other existential field. This common idea within the literature of resiliency is somehow in opposition to the theory of developmental cascades: “…effectiveness in one domain of competence in one period of life becomes the scaffold on which later competence in newly emerging domains develop: in other words, competence begets competence.” (Masten, Cicchetti, 2010). This theory is supported by the characteristic of stability of attachment. We consider the respondents who are securely attached as being resilient. This is hazardous as the resilience can be proved only within the analyses of the past and stressful events. Taking in account the cascade theory and the secure attachment they already proved, it would be better to say just that they are better equipped for behaving resiliently when confronting life’s adversities. Conclusions Within the little sample here of securely attached children we found all the most common items mentioned as being the protective factors in case of resilience. This conclusion stress one of the aspects always mentioned within the literature on resilience: the importance to benefit of the support of a trustful significant person. From the attachment perspective, having secure attachment means having attachment figure available and ready to protect, to calm down the anxieties and to support. An adolescent in our sample, stated the relationship between secure attachment and resilience in the best way. She answered the question regarding the temporary separation from her parents by saying: “when I was separated it was like walking on a stream and I can fall down any time…but when they are there I know there is a support under me and I cannot fall down.” Especially due to their age, when the developmental task is to build-up the self-identity, the dreams on the future are like a powerful drives for these adolescents. On other aspect which is not investigated within our study, through the instrument which we used, but just came up during the narratives of the children is the way in which they are equipped to ask for support when they need. All of them mentioned resource persons in such situations. Mostly of them mentioned mom but also friends or other relatives. The healing process post trauma and the secure attachment include the capacity to ask for support and not to deal alone with adversities and not feeling abandoned by others. The theories of attachment stress the capacity of secureautonomous adults to ask for support when they face difficulties. The narratives of adolescents here prove again their resilience and secure attachment. Examining the data we notice another particular aspect: all these securely attached and resilient children have some talents and practice arts or sports. We consider that this way to express them self is for highest importance for the resilience of the adolescents. Arts and sports bring rules and limits which contribute to self-development and self-education. As we found in other researches in which our team was involved, the individual capacity to push him or her to overpass the comfortable limits in doing some tasks is a common item for the resilience. It is like resilience is asking for self-organization and capacity to rise and keep personal standards. The last aspect which we found and we have to mention here is the importance of pets in the life of these securely attached and resilient children found in our research. The particular aspects mentioned above which can be involved in the process of building up the secure attachment as well as the resilience needs and deserve further explorations. References Anthony, E.J. (1974). Introduction: The syndrome of the psychologically vulnerable child. In: E.J. Anthony,C. Koupernik, (eds). The child in his family: Children at Psychiatric Risk. Vol. 3. Wiley; New York, pp. 3–10. Bernard, B. (1991). Fostering Resiliency in Kids: Protective Factors in the Family, School, and Community. Portland, Ore.: Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol.2. Separation: Anxiety and anger, London: Hogarth Press. Earvolino-Ramirez, M.(2007), Resilience: A concept Analysis, Nursing Forum, vol.42 (2):73-82 Esin, C. (2011). Narrative Analysis Approaches. In N. Frost, Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology, Combining core Approaches. McGrawHill, Open University Press, pp. 92-119. Fonagy, P. (1999), Transgenerational Consistencies of Attachment: A new Theory. Paper to the Developmental and Psychoanalytic Discussion Group; available online at DSPP. 13 Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children’s adaptation to negative life events and stressed environments. Pediatrics. 20:459–466. the era study, inferences, and research implications. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 75: 212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2010.00557.x. Ionescu, S. (coord.) (2011). Traité de résilience assisté, Ed. PUF/Quadrige, Paris. Rutter, M. (1993). Resilience: Some conceptual considerations. Journal of Adolescent Health. Vol. 14, pp. 626–631. Kriss, A., Steele, H., Steele, M. (2012). Measuring Attachment &Reflective Functioning in Early Adolescence: An Introduction to the Friends and Family Interview. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, vol15, no.2, pp.87-95. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., Becker, B. (2000). The Construct of resilience: A Critical Evaluation and Guidlines for Future Work, Child Development, 71 (3):543-562. Steele, H. & Steele, M. (2009), Friends and Family Interview, Center for Attachment Research, New School for Social Research. Masten, A.S. (1994). Resilience in individual development: Successful adaptation despite risk and adversity. In:M.C. Wang, E.W. Gordon, (eds). Educational resilience in inner-city America: Challenges and prospects. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 3–25. Stievenart, M., Casonato, M., Muntean, A., Van de Schoot, R. (2012). The Friends and Family Interview: Measurement invarince across Belgium and Romania, European Journal of Developmental Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2012.689822. Masten, A., Cicchetti, D. (2010). Editorial: Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22: 491-495. Werner, E., Smith, R. (1982).Vulnerable but invincible: A study of resilient children. McGraw-Hill; New York. Rutter, M. and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2010). X. Conclusions: overview of findings from 14 Schore, A.N. (2001, a). Clinical implications of a psychoneurobiological model of projective identification. In S. Alhanati (Ed.), Primitive mental states, Vol. lll: Pre- and peri-natal influences on personality development. New York: Karnac. DISORGANIZATION AND RESILIENCE: INTERVENTION WITH CHILDREN IN THERAPEUTIC INSTITUTION Estelle GRAVRAND1 Abstract: This presentation concerns family deprived children due to proven carelessness and ill-treatment. Their serious behaviour disorder making their placement in a standard institution ineffective, the social services and judges direct those children towards the PREMA Association whose global care is built on a family type facility with the means of artistic expression as a resilience vector with the progressive return to school learning. Realized within the AARN network, the hereby related study has for aim to develop an assessment grid of the children’s progress during their care. The tools are the following: Attachment story completion task (ASCT), Friends and Family interview (FFI), Child Attachment Interview (CAI), Disturbance of attachment interview (DAI retrospective), and the longitudinal and unconditional ethological observation of verbal and non-verbal behavior. The aim is to assess the 10 children from the association; this communication concerns for now a sib ship of three children 8 and 9 years old (twins). If the ASCT and the DAI show important elements of disorganization and reactive disorders concerning attachment at the beginning of their care, two and a half years later the FFI/CAI joins the ethological observation of verbal and nonverbal behavior to point out the fact that in spite of the remaining emotional deprivation, the children are able to revisit their story and it seems as if they had begun a resilience process. Keywords: family; deprived children; behavioral disorder; institutions; intervention by artistic expression; child’s resiliency; school. 1 Association PREMA, Faculty of Toulon Sud, France. 15 Introduction: It is within the draft of a memoir on ethology of which the axis is the clinical and family systems attachment in the University of Toulon Sud (France) directed by Professor Boris Cyrulnik and Professor Michel Delage and the encounter with Professor Pierrehumbert that this presentation takes place. To our knowledge there are few communications concerning reactive disorders symptom intensity of children having developed an insecure type of selective attachment. Thanks to the DAI we are going to investigate the presence of a number of symptoms by the means of « stories to be completed « , explore their attachment representation , then by using the FFI/CAI with the CAME coding, the organization or disorganization of their thought processes. The verbal and nonverbal ethological observation evaluation scale will complete those results. We start from a basic premise: Judges, Social Services in charge of the children in cure at PREMA and the institution pros admit the fact that the children are in a better condition. The main idea of this work is to try to make the subjective assessments « objective less » by the means of the attachment specialist’s proper evaluation tools and through a longitudinal ethological observation method. Hereby, we present the ongoing work; the gathering and the interpretation of the data as well as the hypothesis we develop will evolve as the months go by. If those children make progress, re socialize, tame their suffering and redevelop after their trauma, yet is it within the scope of a resilience process? 16 Among the three evaluated brothers are they equal in their ability to bounce back and « heal/give sense « to their wounds? What is the impact of their initial attachment tie on their development? Is there a possibility of a positive transfer of the attachment tie towards other care-givers? Are the children able to rebuild those ties? A child that hasn’t developed a selective attachment in his early childhood, will he be able, later, to embed organized and secure working internal models? Méthodology Tools: • ASCT: Stories to be completed with the children in order to measure their attachment representation developed by Bretherton, Ridgeway and Cassidy in1990. • DAI (Disturbance of Attachment Interview) in order to measure in a retrospective way the changes of the child’s attachment disturbance. The items of this partly structured interview investigate the presence of attachment trouble signs (reactive attachment disorder) developed by the AAARN network. This transmission is retrospective, the indications for rating are excerpted from: Smyke AT. Dimitrescu A. Zeanah CH. Attachment disturbances in young children. I: The continuum of caretaking casualty. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 41(8):972-82, 2002 Aug. • FFI (Friends and Family Interview) and CAI (Child Attachment Interview) that measure organization and disorganization of thought, FFI developed by Howard and Nancy Steele, New-York (2003) and CAI by Target, Fonagy and Shmueli-Goetz, (2003). Code: CAME (Maps, Attachment, Mentalization, child) developed by Ayala Borghini and al. (On-going). • Ethological evaluation scale (ad hoc) of verbal’s and non-verbal’s child behaviors by Estelle Gravrand (2014). II.1. Protocol: It is by the means of a weekly 45 minute filmed interview for each child that the ethological observation methods have been applied over a period of some 18 months. The results of the verbal and non-verbal behavior scale are related to the following video dates: John:Keith: Bob: A: 03/05/12 A: 30/04/12 A: 30/04/12 B: 03/10/12 B: 03/09/12 B: 24/09/12 C: 08/10/12 C: 26/09/12 C: 14/11/12 D: 19/11/12 D: 31/10/12 D: 30/01/13 E: 03/12/12 E: 05/12/12 E: 07/11/13 F: 11/02/13 F: 11/02/13 It is also during these interviews that the different tests FFI/CAI and ASCT have been practiced until this date. The DAI has been submitted to the educators already present in the facility since the arrival of the children. There are three video clips showing the evolution of the three children ( Bob and John - 9 year old twins , and Keith – 8 years old), whom we’re speaking of today , and the ethological observation has been based on the first 20 minutes of the selected interviews ( January 2014 evaluation. John – 9 years old: John had a score of 8/10 for inhibited subtype trouble, two years later his score was 3/10. The disinhibited subtype troubles moved from 4/8 to 3/8 and a few hyper-dependant selective attachment sign troubles have been observed and diminished from 5/8 to 2/8. (Fig1) some six interviews over a period of around two years ) . III. Results: III.1: DAI: When arriving in the institution (august 2011) the children had big reactive attachment troubles as shown in the drafts below. Those signs have for a major part diminished: 17 Bob - 9 years old: Bob showed attachment reactiv troubles of inhibited subtype evolving from 10/10 to 2/10 after 24 months cure. Troubles of deinhibited subtype stayed level: 4/8. However signs indicating a few marks of hyper dependant attachment raised by one point: from 2/8 to 3/8. (Fig 2) III.2. Ethological observation scale of verbal and non-verbal behavior: These are nominal scales designed for this purpose. Rating: Even if the questions are different, we use the same QCM type (5 questions) to measure the verbal and non-verbal behavior. The scores are noted on 10. The nearer the score to zero, the more the child will have synchronized and adapted behavior. The videos are shown in a chronological order. /8to 3/8. (Fig 2) • Verbal behavior results: The graph has been calculated after the mean value of the three children’s scores. Keith - 8 years old: The results of Keith show that he had attachment reactional troubles of the inhibited subtype of 8/10 when arriving in the institution and that 24 months later they are 3/10. He also showed troubles of de-inhibite subtype when arriving of 6/8, and they are now 2/8. We have noticed an important presence of hyper-dependant attachment signs (7/8 when arriving, they dismissed to 3/8. (Fig 3) John 9 years old: In the A video the words are not coherent with the expressed emotions. As from the B video words and emotions are misaligned but coherent, then from the C video they are matching. At first the language is maladjusted, blurred with a lot of invented words, then it will become clear, fluent and concise. The use of verb, subject, and complement will happen from the E video and John is going to build elaborated sentences with a careful vocabulary. 18 • Nonverbal behavior results: All along this year the child will mix in his speech real and imaginary facts but he is conscious of this agreement he creates with reality. The neutral tone and the unassured voice at the beginning of the records will settle and equalize in the course of the interviews. Bob 9 years old: As from the A video there is no coherence between his emotions and the words used. He is, given his age, in a low mean value concerning verbal expression until the C video, then he evolves positively. Bob has got a stutter and has great speaking difficulties that improve as from the C video. Today, his speaking is fluent, the flow easy, his voice calm. The child’s speech is adapted to reality, events and elements pertaining to his history are named and sense given. Keith 8 years old: At the beginning words and emotions aren’t in ad equation, in the C video they are misaligned but coherent, then in ad equation from the D video. The language at first is cut up, hesitating but still coherent. In the C video it is unadapt then from the D video it becomes clear, fluent, and concise. From the B video the child uses subject, verb, and complement. Altogether his speech is adapted to reality and from the C video, events and elements pertaining to his history are named and given sense. At the beginning of the recordings the prosody and vocality of words lack in harmony: discordant voice, use of an unfitting tone. From the D video the voice is calm and the tone fits. Bob: Regularly decreasing curve. John: Curve connected to emotions and feelings, still decreasing. Keith: Curve connected to emotions and feelings Mouvements du corps 600 500 400 300 200 100 0 AB CDE F Bob Keith John Bob: Curve slightly decreasing, the child remains restless but with less anxiety. John: Irregular curve, the child is always moving but without being aggressive. One notices that the more his body moves the less his gesture is self-centered. Keith: Clearly decreasing curve. Signes faciaux d’inquiétude 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Bob Keith John AB CDEF Bob: Decreasing curve. John: Fairly linear decreasing curve. Very expressive child. 19 Keith: Decreasing curve. John 9 years old: Q scores en format T Fréquence regard à l’interlocuteur 300 100,00 90,00 80,00 70,00 60,00 50,00 40,00 30,00 20,00 10,00 0,00 250 200 150 100 50 0 A B Bob C Keith D John E F 1 2 3 4 1234 The stories to be completed say for John that Bob: Very evasive eye contact, the slightly rising curve is a sign of progress. He makes contact through the eyes, at first evasive, then direct. John: Seeking the eye of his interviewer as a reassuring sign, first checking whether what So he is avoiding insecure with a high disorganized propensity. The scales show a high reaction to separa- he says is received without judgment; then tion, an adapted collaboration, and a low level John builds confidence and converses without being obliged to stay watchful. of appropriate affect expression. Keith: Very disorganized, the child’s moods condition his state. Mainly, he is communicating when he looks at his interviewer and more introverted when he casts his eyes away. The frequency of eye contact with the interviewer may have different functions, such as reassurance or control, thus we do not expect that those curves would be decreasing as for the other index. (Confirmed by the graphs) III.3 ASCT: CCH code: These coding methods give four scores based on attachment representation, built on a Q-sort method: Secure, desactivation-avoidance, hyper activation-preoccupation, disorganization, and 7 scales: Collaboration, parents’ support representation, positive narrative, appropriate affects expression, separation reaction, symbolic distance, poor narrative ability. 20 he is disorganized: 50.45 points, hyper-activated: 38.95 points, deactivated: 56.99 points and secure: 43.91 points. Bob 9 years old: Q scores en format T 100,00 90,00 80,00 70,00 60,00 50,00 40,00 30,00 20,00 10,00 0,00 1 2 3 4 1234 The stories to be completed say he is disorganized at 67, 80 points, hyper-activated at 59, 01, deactivated at 63, 83, and secure at 34, 97. (It is noticeable that he is John’s twin brother and that their scores are quite different). Bob is disorganized insecure with a high desactivation level (avoiding). The scales show a high reaction to separation, a correct narrative skill, a very low symbolic distance and a very low appropriate affects expression. Keith 8 years old: talization ». There are 8 scales : Coherence , Self-reflective , Access to souvenirs , Mentalization , Q scores en format T 100,00 90,00 80,00 70,00 60,00 50,00 40,00 30,00 20,00 10,00 0,00 Anger/worry , traumatism , Need of comfort , Life struggle . 1 2 After two and a half years in Prema: 4 John: 3 12 34 The stories to be completed show he is disorganized: 75.17 points, hyper-activated: 64.38 points, deactivated: 67.23 points and secure: 29.13 points. Keith has a low score of attachment secu- Raw Q score of the subject: secure 51.98, desactivated 41.87, hyper activated 69.19, disorganized 55.84, Mentallization 58.86. John has a high hyperactivation score for his attachment process, and a good Mentallization score. rity, a very high score of disorganization. His scores for deactivation and hyper-activation are also substantial. The scales show a low level of appropriate affects expression, a correct level of collaboration, and a high reaction to separation. III.4: FFI/CAI, code CAME: We have submitted the FFI/CAI test to the elder children – 9 and a half years old – and used the CAME code (charts, attachment, Mentalization, children) built by Ayala Borghini, in charge of the SUPEA from Lausanne and Professor Blaise Pierrehumbert. Fully detailed in the score analysis, it allows us to refine and enlarge our fields of reflexion. Keith however was overwhelmed by his The scales show a high level of access to souvenirs – 65.84 – and of his ability for Mentallization – 61.12 - , the worries/anger score is high – 60.46 - , the trauma endures under the surface -51.23- and is confirmed through the life troubles score – 56.95 - , coherence is good – 49.05 – and the need for comfort lower – 47.08 – the self-reflective score is – 60.15 - . Bob: emotions, questions on his family and his story brought back his suffering. So we put an end to the protocol with him. The CAME Code system allows to measure quantatively the four dimensions of attachment: «Security, Desactivation, Hyperactivation, Disorganization « and the dimension of «Men- Raw Q score of the subject: secure – 56.16 21 - , deactivated – 44.90 - , hyper activated – 49.55 - , disorganized – 47.77 - , Mentallization – 56.78. Bob has a high attachment security score, rather hyper activated, and a good Mentallization score. The scales show a high coherence – 68.31 - , a high level of access to souvenirs – 64.93 - , a fair ability for Mentallization – 62.68 , a high score for anxiety/anger – 61.61 - , traumatism – 57.38 - and life troubles – 55.97 – over the average , need for comfort – 52.01 and self-reflective – 50.90 . IV. Conclusion: The first results confirm the hypothesis upon which the children develop positively. • Among the three children studied, are the three equal in their ability to bounce back and « heal/give sense to » their wounds? • What is the impact of their initial attachment on their development? It is interesting to notice the attachment representation differences between the twins John and Bob, yet they have never been separated. However one has noticed while working with the family that John had not been supported by his parents unlike Bob « close » to his father and mother, while Keith was over-supported by his mother and very much impacted by his father’s violence. The mother has being accused of incest by the three brothers. But only Keith, separated from the twins, and alone with her most of the time had undergone recurring incest (according to his statements). The attachment reactive problems being high both on inhibited, de-inhibited and 22 hyper-dependant levels, one may wonder whether he may have suffered from hyperdependant attachment that could evolve into a disorganized insecure attachment during his cure. Still too sensitive and disorganized, even if Keith progresses, we didn’t wish to evaluate him further with the use of the FFI and CAI quiz. Sometimes he is able to elaborate and mentalize his emotions but it occurs through brief glimpses. John , being “put aside” from the family , could have “escaped” and developed an elusive insecure attachment allowing him to have more fitting behavior, but still marked by physical escape pulsions (runaway) or psychological pulsions (proven suicidal tendencies at his arrival). Compared to his ASCT attachment representation, the CAME shows that he hyper-activates his attachment process (+ 30 points), he is more secure (+ 8 points), and paradoxically more disorganized (+ 5 points) which is normal considering that during a reorganization, a disorganization may precede new organization. • Are the children able to reorganize these ties? John is able after two and half years of cure to acknowledge his anger , his trauma due to his story and he shows the will to understand and give sense to his past in order to advance : « to limit my suffering , I tell myself that each day of my life is a new adventure « . Bob made a lot of progress in two and half years, from denying his story he switched to recognizing it and is beginning to accept it. It can be seen especially through his verbal behavior and when comparing the ASCT and CAME results we notice that his scores have diminished for Disorganization (- 20 points), Hyperactivation (- 10 points), Deactivation (20 points), and have risen for Securization (+ 11 points). • A child who has not developed a selective attachment in his early childhood will he be able to build internal models, efficient, organized and secure? While coding another child from the Association victim of hospitalism, we noticed that they had the same ASCT scores, which didn’t fit with the reality of their day to day behavior. It could put in perspective the hypothesis according to which they would not have developed a selective attachment but have made a copy/paste of the representations that they found in the institution in which they developed. It brings us to the next question: is it possible that a child without a selective attachment embeds internal models, efficient, organized and secure, thanks to mimetism and could be permeated over a period of time by the environment in which they live? Here we do not talk of “as-if” but of a sound evolution. The CAME scores tends to confirm this hypothesis. When looking at these results and observations, these children seem to be able to mentalize and organize their emotions on different levels. Their age and traumatism taken into account, this progress is encouraging for the continuation of their life story. References: Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship. An attachment story completion task for 3-year-olds. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Miljkovitch, R., Pierrehumbert, B., Bretherton, I., & Halfon, O. (2004). Associations between parental and child attachment representations. Attachment & Human Development, 6, 305-325. Smyke, AT., Dumitrescu, A., Zeanah, CH. (2002). Attachment disturbances in young children. I: The continuum of caretaking casualty. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 41(8), 97282. Steele, H., & Steele, M., &Kriss, A. (2009). Center for Attachment research, New School for Social Research. Steele, H., & Steele, N., New York 1990. CAI: Child Attachment Interview. Target, Fonagy and Shmueli-Goetz (2003) CAME: Carts Attachment Mentalization Enfant, Borghini et al. (On going.) 23 EARLY DEPRIVATION AND BEHAVIOURAL ADAPTATION IN A SAMPLE OF ITALIAN ADOPTED ADOLESCENTS Paola MOLINA1 Marta CASONATO1 Barbara ONGARI2 Alessandro DECARLI2 Abstract Introduction: Adoption is one of the major protective factors for the adjustment of children who experienced abandonment and institutionalization (Zeanah et al., 2009). Studying the effects of early deprivation in adolescence is particularly useful, because adolescence is a critical period of development and because this period allows to consider the effect of an enduring positive experience in the adoptive family. Moreover, parents’ secure attachment representations can help to repair early negative experiences, particularly considering their reflective function. In this paper we will analyse the effect of adoption in relation to early deprivation in a sample of 27 Italian adolescents, adopted before the age of 6 and observed in adolescence (1116 years). These results are part of an international research project (Pierrehumbert, 2009), aiming at collecting information on internationally adopted children. Method: Early deprivation was rated from biographical informations provided by parents University of Torino (ITALY) University of Trento (ITALY) E-mail: paola.molina@unito.it, marta.casonato@unito.it, barbara.ongari@soc.unitn.it, aledecarli@hotmail.com 1 2 24 (age at adoption, changes in pre-adoptive care, period in institution, etc.) and from a retrospective form of the Disturbance of Attachment Interview (DAI)(Smyke. A. & Zeanah, 1999). Adolescents’ adaptation was rated by CBCL/YSR(Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001), filled by both parents and by adolescents. Measures of different aspect of parental caregiving representations, were derived from the Parent Development Interview (PDI)(Aber, Slade, Berger, Bresgi, & Kaplan, 1985), a semi-structured interview, which explores the parental representations of the ongoing relationship with their children, addressed to adoptive mothers and fathers separately. Each interview has been video-recorded, transcribed, and rated on a 4 point scale. Through correlation, we analyzed the relationship between early deprivation and adaptation in adolescence, considering the effect of parental attachment measures. We hypothesized that risk factors in pre-adoptive experience negatively correlate with adolescents’ adaptation rated by CBCL/YSR, and that parental positive caregiving representations, particularly parental competence, could moderate this relationship. Results and conclusions: Contrarily to our preliminary hypotheses, the level of pre-adoption risk was not associated with the behavioral adjustment in adolescence. This could be due to the protective effect of having lived for a number of years in a supporting adoptive family. With respect to the caregiving characteristics, our results are apparently less encouraging: maternal competence does not influences adolescents’ adjustment, and only the relationships between actual difficult experiences are evident. Further research could help to disentangle the influence of different variables affecting adopted adolescents’ behavior adaptation. Keywords: adoption; adolescence; behavioural problems; Parent Development Interview; Disturbances of Attachment Interview; CBCL; YSR 1 Introduction Adoption is one of the major protective factors for the recovery and adjustment outcomes of children who previously experienced abandonment and institutionalization (Zeanah, 2009). Age at adoption and the experience of institutionalization have been largely considered as risk factors for the adoptees’ later adjustment (Gunnar, &all. 2007; Judge, 2003; Verhulst, &all. 1990). Since the interest towards adoptive sample comes from the experience of attachment disruptions lived by these children, it is also important to test whether multiple changes in caregiving could affect the adolescents’ adjustment (Erich, &all., 2009). David Brodzinsky, one of the most important American experts in the psychology of adoption, considers family relationships as the most important background factors in the adopted child’s adjustment (Schechter, & Brodzinsky, 1990). Through continuous reiteration over time, the offering of a new and positive relational model can play a significant role on the change of the child’s attachment representations (Schofield & Beek, 2006). It is within the context of sensitive, thoughtful and reflective relationships with their caregivers that adopted children learn to 25 feel safe, to explore, to make sense, and to grow (Howe, 2006). Studying the effects of early deprivation in adolescence is particularly useful, because adolescence is a critical period of development and because this period allows considering the effect of an enduring positive experience inside the adoptive family. Indeed, adolescence represents a critical period for adopted children. In addition to the typical issues faced by each individual in this period of life, the variety of physical and cognitive changes of adolescence implies, among adoptees, the development of concerns about identity issues such as who they are, where they come from and what they will become (Bimmel & all., 2003). Literature has also shown that adoptees, in the urgency of developing a stable identity, tend to face the typical adolescence issues before their non-adoptive peers (Juffer, & van Ijzendoorn, 2005). Thus, among adoptees the possible difficulties linked to this period of life may occur even before entering the teen-ages. In this perspective, our research will analyse the effect of adoption in relation to early deprivation in a sample of 27 Italian adolescents (11-16 years), adopted before the age of 6. These results are part of an international research project (Pierrehumbert, 2009), collecting information on internationally adopted children. We hypothesized that risk factors in preadoptive experience negatively correlate, and that time spent in the adoptive family positive correlate, with CBCL/YSR adolescents’ adaptation. Moreover, we explored the relationship between parental characteristics and adolescents’ adaptation. 26 2 Method 2.1 Procedure The sample recruitment was carried out through social services and agencies for international adoption. Eligibility criteria were to be 10 to 16 years old at assessment and to have been adopted internationally (between birth and 9 years of age). Informed consents were given by both parents and each adolescent before collecting the data. 2.2 Sample Participants include a total of 27 adolescents (51.9% boys) and their adoptive parents. The adolescents’ average age at assessment was 13 years old (SD=2 years). They had been internationally adopted from various geographical areas (52% from Asia, 22% from South America, 19% from Eastern Europe and 7% from Africa). For most of the parents, the choice of adoption was due to infertility (82%). At placement, adolescents were 13 years old (SD=2 years). With respect to siblings, 14.8% are only children, whereas the remaining adolescents have one or more siblings (biological siblings, biological children of the adoptive parents -18.5%-, and adopted as well) (see Table 1). Table 1 Sample (N=27) M Girls Age Age at adoption (months) Adopted for infertility Siblings Years spent within the adoptive family 2.3 Measures Early deprivation was rated through biographical informations provided by parents (age at adoption, changes in pre-adoptive care, institutionalization, etc.) and through a retrospective form of the Disturbance of Attachment Interview (DAI) (Smyke & Zeanah, 1999). Adolescents’ adaptation was rated by both parents and their adolescents through CBCL/YSR (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The scores for each parent attitude were derived from the Parent Development Interview (PDI) (Aber, & all., 1985), a semistructured interview exploring the parental DS Range 13 44 2 31 10-16 1-111 10 3 3-16 % 48% 81,5% 85,2% representations of the ongoing relationship with their children, addressed to adoptive mothers and fathers separately. Each interview has been video-recorded, transcribed, and rated on a 4 point scale. On the basis of PDI, 5 scales have been created in order to synthetize the numerous informations present in the interviews. To this purpose, items were chosen on a theoretical basis as well as looking at their inner correlations. Thus, their internal consistency was controlled through Cronbach’s alphas (>.70, see Table 2). Table 2. PDI Scales Scale composition 6 items (Coherence; Richness of perceptions; Reflective functioning; Parental competence; Parental capacity Level of child focus; Attachment awareness and promotion) Positive parental experience 2 items (Joy/pleasure; Warmth) 3 item (Anger degree; Disappointment/despair; Negative parental experience Hostility) Child’s positive description 2 items (Child happiness; Child affectionate) 2 items (Child aggression/anger; Child Child’s negative description controlling/manipulating) 2.4 Data Analyses First, descriptive analysis of risk factors and different informant adolescents’ adaptation were performed. Using correlation analysis the relationships between early deprivation and adaptation in adolescence N 13 Cronbach’s alpha .922 .867 .883 .735 .760 were examined, considering the effects of the length of the adoptive experience, of the parental experience and of their perceives competence. Due to the ordinal level of some variables, in order to compare dichotomous 27 risk groups, we used non-parametric exact tests (Mann-Whitney Exact Test, Montecarlo Method). 3 Results 3.1 Descriptive analyses 3.1.1 Pre-adoption risk factors Age at placement is very heterogeneous in our sample (M=4 years old; SD=3). 85% of the adoptees had experienced institutionalization, at least for few months. It is hard to control for the number of changes in caregiving, but we can state that they range between one and three. With respect to the level of attachment disturbance during the first year of placement, 21 families answered the retrospective form of DAI (10% of respondents were fathers, 90% were mothers). At placement, the average score of attachment disturbances were 7.4 (SD=3.2) whereas one year later the same scores decreases to 2.3 (SD=2.3) (see Table 3). Table 3. Pre-adoptive experience M DS Range N % Pre-adoptive care - Biological family 25,9% -Foster-care 22,2% -Institution 85,2% -Placements 1-3 DAI inhibited disturbs: - At placement - After 1 years 2.4 .6 2.0 1.0 0-8 0-3 DAI disinhibited disturbs: - At placement - After 1 years 2.4 .7 2.0 1.2 0-7 0-4 DAI selective attachment: - At placement - After 1 years .3 .1 .7 .5 0-2 0-2 3.1.2. Behavioural adjustment Data for adolescents’ adjustment, rated by self-report (YSR) and parent-report (CBCL, filled by mothers) are reported in Table 4, both for the total sample and for boys and girls separately. Internalizing scores are significantly higher for girls, as underlined 28 also on normative samples. On average, behavioral problems in our sample are similar to those observed in a cross-country comparison on adopted adolescents (Roskam, I., & Al., in preparation). Table 4. Behavioural problems scores: self- and parental-reported Our sample YSR (N = 25) M SD Problem scale Max score Internalizing 62 Roskam et al., in preparation CBCL (N = 24) M SD YSR (N = 309) M SD CBCL (N = 309) M SD 13,88 10,94 11,38 8,45 Boys 7,77* 6,21 10.18 8.07 10.44 10.32 9.69 9.60 Girls 20,50* 11,27 12.38 0,96 11.78 9.30 8.54 8.24 Externalizing 64 11,48 9,43 7,50 8,08 Boys 12,08 11,98 8,73 10,84 13.46 8.86 12.21 11.31 Girls 10,83 12.56 8.94 8.76 8.68 Total 210 6,07 6,43 4,98 45,52 24,56 30,92 22,58 Boys 37,15 24,16 31,64 24,83 Girls 54,58 22,52 30,31 21,88 (*) Mann-Whitney Exact Test, Monte Carlo Method, p < .005 (Two tails) 3.1.3 Parental representations In Table 5 the average scores for the five major scales derived from mothers’ interviews are reported. Table 5. Mothers’ PDI principal scores (N = 27) Range Mean SD Parental capacity 6-24 16.26 4.39 Positive parental experience 2-8 5.74 2.30 Negative parental experience 3-12 5.33 1.98 Child’s positive description 2-8 5.19 1.73 Child’s negative description 2-8 3.81 1.52 Parental capacity highlights aspects linked to parental attachment, investment in the parenthood, coping strategies and sensitivity towards the child. Parental experience and the description of the child, both divided into positive and negative, respectively refers to the perception of the pleasure linked to the experience of being a parent, and to whether the parent describes the adoptee in a positive or negative way. 3.2 Risk factors and adjustment during adolescence In order to highlight the effect of possible risk factors, correlations between variables concerning pre-adoptive experience and behavioral problems have done. Contrarily to our hypotheses, no significant results emerged (p> .05). Since possible differences could be found within adoptees who have experienced a higher amount of pre-adoptive distress, risk 29 variables (institution, number of changes, age and level of attachment disturbances at placement) were recoded into dichotomous variables, considering their distribution within the sample (half subjects in the higher range and the other half in the lower range). By comparing them through the MannWhitney Exact test, a unique significant result emerged: adoptees who differed with respect to the DAI scores (high versus low presence of disturbances at placement), showed significantly different internalizing problems (see Table 6). Adoptees who scored higher on attachment disturbances, during adolescence showed a higher level of internalizing problems, even when controlling for gender. Table 6. Maternal-reported behavioral problems with respect to high/low DAI scores DAI at adoption Internalizing * Externalizing Total N. M (DS) M (DS) M (DS) Lower score 9 6,67 (5.39) 5,44 (4,82) 20,11 (17,21) Higher scores 10 14,60 (8,67) 10,20 (11.35) 39,60 (25,90) Total 19 10,84 (8,19) 11,35 (8,94) 30,37 (23,81) (*) Mann-Whitney Exact Test, Monte Carlo Method, p < .05 (Two tails) On the contrary, having lived in an institution, having experienced multiple changes in caregiving, and having been adopted later does not influence, in our sample, the rate of behavioral problems in adolescence. 3.3 Parental competence and experience Concerning the relationships between parental competence and experience, and adolescents’ adjustment (see Table 7), we found a systematic correlation among the externalizing problems (both self- and maternal-reported), the parents’ negative perception of child, and their negative experience as parents. Table 7. Parental caregiving representation and adolescents’ adjustment YSR (N=25) Mother PDI Scales Intern. Extern. -,14 -,19 Positive parental experience ,01 Negative parental experience Intern. Extern. -,25 -.11 -.07 -.11 -,36 -,26 - .22 - .42 * - .35 -,17 ,50* ,24 . 02 . 23 . 19 Child’s positive description -,20 -,25 -,34 - .10 - .31 - .19 Child’s negative description -,30 ,56** ,19 -.07 .47* .23 Parental capacity 30 Mother’s CBCL (N=24) Total Total Contrarily to our hypothesis, higher scores on parental capacity do not correspond to lower behavioral problems among adoptees. Parental experience is negatively associated with externalizing problems, and positively associated with child’s negative description, meaning that adoptees who show higher externalizing problems have mothers who describe their parenthood experience as more negative and their child as more difficult and aggressive. 4 Discussion and conclusions Our results are preliminary and descriptive, as our sample size is limited. Nevertheless, we can highlight the absence of relationships between early risk factors and adjustment in adolescence. Indeed, contrarily to our preliminary hypotheses, the level of pre-adoption risk was not associated with the behavioral adjustment in adolescence in our sample. Only the high rate of attachment disturbances at placements seems to be correlated with a higher level of internalizing problems during adolescence. Since just a few studies analyzed the effect of pre-adoptive risks among adolescents, this unexpected result could be linked to the reparatory value of having lived several years (on average 10) within the adoptive family. The daily and continuous experience in a good familiar environment could limit or even help canceling the effects of the negative experiences, underlined by the adoption literature (Schofield & Beek, 2006). Nevertheless, this effect could be enhanced by a selection bias: although we cannot control for the number of request sent by adoption agencies and services, we know that only few of contacted families accepted to take part in our study, and these families could be the better adjusted. Concerning the caregiving characteristics, our results are less encouraging, as it seems that parental competence does not influence the adolescents’ adjustment. In our sample only the correlations among actual the experiences are evident. Deeper analyses are need in order to better differentiate the maternal and paternal role, the individual profiles, and the possible relations among different risk and protective factors. For instance, analyzing in a longitudinal perspective each individual path will allow a deeper understanding of the time spent in the adoptive family. References Aber, J., Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., & Kaplan, M. (1985). The Parent Development Interview. Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont , Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van, Ij. M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2003). Problem behavior of internationally adopted adolescents: a review and meta-analysis. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 11(2), 64–77. Erich, S., Kanenberg, H., Case, K., Allen, T., & Bogdanos, T. (2009). An empirical analysis of factors affecting adolescent attachment in adoptive families with 31 homosexual and straight parents. Children and Youth Services Review, 31(3), 398–404. externalizing behavior in adolescent-parent pairs Does being adopted make a difference? Gunnar, M., van Dulmen, M., & the International Adotion Project Team. (2007). Behavior problems in postinstitutionalized internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 19(1), 129–148. Schechter, M. D., & Brodzinsky, D. M. (1990). The psychology of adoption. New York: Oxford University Press, 167 – 186 . New York: Oxford University Press. Howe, D. (2006). Introduction. In G. Schofield & M. Beek (Eds.), Attachment Handbook for Foster Care and Adoption. London: BAAF. Judge, S. (2003). Developmental recovery and deficit in children adopted from Eastern European orphanages. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 34(1), 49–62. Juffer, F., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: a metaanalysis. JAMA, 293(20), 2501–2515. Pierrehumbert, B. Attachment & Adoption Research Network (2009). Retrieved from http://aarnetwork.wordpress.com/ . Roskam, I., & Al., E. (in preparation). Cross-informant ratings of internalizing and 32 Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2006). Attachment Handbook for Foster Care and Adoption. London: BAAF. Smyke. A., & Zeanah, C. (1999). Disturbances of Attachment Interview. Section of Child andAdolescent Psychiatry Tulane Univeristy School of Medicine. Verhulst, F. C., Althaus, M., & Versluis-den Bieman, H. J. (1990). Problem behavior in international adoptees: I. An epidemiological study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(1), 94– 103. Zeanah, C., Egger, H., Smyke, A., Nelson, C., Fox, N., & Marshall, P. (2009). Institutional Rearing and Psychiatric Disorders in Romanian Preschool Children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(7), 777–785. ASSESSING ATTACHMENT IN FOSTER PARENTS AND ADOPTIVE PARENTS TO BE Karin LUNDÉN1 Abstract Secure attachment is considered a very important factor in resilience. Literature has shown that both foster parent´s and adoptive parent´s own attachment significantly affects the development of a more secure attachment in children placed in their care. To measure attachment will therefore be essential in professional´s assessments. In Sweden there is limited knowledge of attachment in both foster parents and adoptive parents. A descriptive study was therefore conducted in order to investigate attachment in both groups of parents. To measure attachment the Attachment Style Interview (Bifulco, 2002) was used. The group under study consisted of 50 foster parents under assessment for approval. Following questions were investigated: how many of the foster parents had a secure attachment; how many foster parents where children already were placed had a secure attachment; how many foster parents with secure attachment lived together with a more insecure partner. Tentative results showed that just over half of the foster parents had a secure attachment. Almost half of them had an insecure attachment. Just over half of the foster parents had same level of attachment as their partners. Almost half of foster parents with secure attachment lived together with a more insecure partner. As we can see conclusively there are a substantial amount of foster parents with insecure attachment. Professionals have investigated all of them for approval before they were selected as foster parents. As secure attachment in foster parents have been proven to be such an important factor for the development of their placed children the tentative results highlight the need for the possibility to measure attachment as part of the assessment process. Keywords: assessment, attachment, foster parents, adoptive parents to be PhD. , Dept. of Social work, University of Gothenburg, SWEDEN. E-mail: Karin.Lunden@socwork.gu.se, karinlunden@comhem.se 1 33 Introduction Children develop in relation to their caregiving environment. For most children their environment consists of their biological parents but for some children it consists of foster parents and for again some it is adoptive parents. Today there is considerable knowledge of the importance of parent’s ability to take part in their children’s development. There are some issues that parents have to perform good enough in order to facilitate the development of their children. Issues that have to do with protection and comfort but also contribute to children’s cognitive development. Today we know the correlation between brain development in small children and parental sensitivity and how well parents perform their tasks. There are some parental issues that are found to be more important than others. Parents have to be sensitive enough, they have to be emotional available enough and they have to have a good enough reflective function i.e. ability to mentalize. They also have capacity to provide a secure base. Attachment and care giving The development of children is a complicated process, which is affected by the interaction between inborn or acquired biological conditions and different factors in the environment (Sameroff, Fiese, 2000). Attachment between children and parents play an important role in children’s on going development. Especially through it’s effect on the development of the brain. Neurological studies have given us evidence of how mental processes are formed and the role small children’s experiences play hereby (Siegel, 2001). Children 34 who are adopted of placed in foster homes may have experienced trauma and/or maltreatment early in life. There is, however, a possibility to heal and to continue development in a more favourable direction. To make sure children do develop well in Sweden like in many other countries, society has a special responsibility to ensure children a development as optimal as possible. This is especially relevant for adoptive children and children placed in foster care. Earlier Swedish research has shown, however, that a considerable amount of pre school children are at risk for maltreatment. Despite a mandatory reporting obligation few of them were reported to Child Protection Services and CPS assessed even fewer. The possibility for the children and their families to get professional help thereby diminished significantly (Lundén, 2011). Several Swedish cohort studies have shown that both adopted children and foster children were at risk for different kinds of psychological and psychiatric difficulties later on in life (Lindbland & all., 2003; van IJzendoorn, & all., 2005). Attachment and non-biological parent’s care giving There is a well-documented connection between both biological and non-biological parent’s caregiving capacity and type of attachment in children. Studies on adopted children, for example, have shown that many children, who have been in orphanage before adoption, had developed a disorganized attachment, which is known to be connected with several kinds of psychological and psychiatric difficulties later in life (Smyke &all., 2010; Rutter &all., 2004). Studies conducted by the English and Romanian Adoptee Study Team (ERA), have illuminated the risk for children concerning attachment. They found a correlation between the length of children’s stay at orphanage and severe attachment disturbances. They also found these disturbances declined in many children when they settled down in their adoptive family. Kanuik, Steele and Hodges (2004) have compared children adopted at the age of 4 years to 8 years with experiences of child maltreatment with children adopted before the age of 1 year. The children were followed up at 2 years of age and results showed that they were doing very well in general. Especially those children who were placed with securely attached mothers. Parental sensitivity has shown to be important in other studies as well. Children and their adoptive mothers were followed until the children were 7 years of age (Stams &all., 2002). Result showed that adoptive mother’s sensitivity together with the attachment in the mother-child relation predicted developmental difficulties. The more secure attachment and sensitivity in child-mother relation the better social and cognitive development in the child. Emotional availability can thereby be considered an important factor for care giving abilities in parents. Similar findings have been found concerning children placed in foster care (Dozier &all., 2001). The concordance between foster mother’s attachment state of mind and the attachment quality of their foster placed babies were examined. The babies had been placed into the care of their foster mothers between birth and 20 months of age. Attachment quality was assessed when the babies were between one and two years of age. The correlation between foster mother’s attachment state of mind and babies’ attachment qualities were similar to the level seen among biologically intact dyads. It turned out that age of placement was not related to attachment quality. It seems there is a capacity for small children after disruption to organize their behaviour around a new caregiver. The results highlight the existence of a transmission of attachment between generations that is not genetic. For obvious reasons the attachment process in adoptive – and foster placed children are a bit different from children in general. Many adoptive or foster children have experienced difficult life circumstances earlier in life such as for instance different forms of child maltreatment. Many of them have developed a disorganized attachment and/or several kinds of psychological difficulties. Hopefully enough studies also shown that children can heal (Lundén, 2010). Experiences from studies in both adoption and foster care highlight the need for adopted and foster placed children to develop an attachment as secure as possible. Research shows a correlation between a more secure attachment development in these children and the quality of attachment in their adoptive- or foster parents. In Sweden all adoptive parents and foster parents to be are extensively assessed in many areas but not attachment. Aim and research questions There are some earlier Scandinavian studies, where different aspects in adoption and 35 foster care have been investigated. So far no Swedish study has been conducted, where attachment in adoptive- or foster parents to be have been investigated. In order to learn more about first foster parent’s attachment a descriptive study was conducted where attachment was assessed using Attachment Style Interview (Bifulco &all., 2008). Research questions were • How many foster parents were securely attached • How many foster parents where foster children already were placed, were securely attached • How many foster parents lived together with someone more insecurely attached. Methodology Participants and procedure The present group under study consisted of 50 foster parents involved in an assessment process for approval. All of them were married or cohabitants. Just around half of them were between 40 and 49 years old. The remaining participants were mostly older than 49 years of age. Few of them were younger than 40 years of age. Their attachment style was assessed in connection with their wish to be approved as foster parents in a particular community. Measurement To assess attachment Attachment Style Interview (Bifulco &all., 2008) was used. The Attachment Style Interview is a standardised, semi structured, investigator-based interview designed to investigate quality of close rela- 36 tionships to partner and to very close others, ability to make and maintain relationships, the degree of secure/insecure attachment and the overall attachment style. Tentative results The present foster parent group of 50 participants is part of a larger group consisting of additional 50 adoptive parents to be and 50 parents in child protection cases. In comparison adoptive parents to be had a more secure attachment style than did foster parents to be. On the other hand foster parents to be were more securely attached than were parents in child protection cases. Tentative results showed that even if participants were assessed for approval to be foster parents three fifth of them already had children placed within their family. As can be seen in table 1 just over half of the foster parents to be showed a secure or mildly insecure attachment style while almost half of them showed a highly insecurely attachment style. A similar amount of foster parents, who already had placements, were assessed to have secure/mildly insecure attachment as those with a highly insecure attachment style. All foster parents to be lived together with a partner. Just over half of them had the same degree of attachment style as their partners. Almost as many of them lived together with someone with a highly insecure attachment style. Table 1. Amount of foster parents to be and degree of secure attachment style Foster parents Secure/mildly insecure Highly insecure No - foster children 8(4%) 12(6%) 20(40%) Yes - foster children 19 (63%) 21(70%) 30(60%) 27 (54%) 23 (46%) 50 In average foster parents to be already had 3.6 children placed in their families. As many as nineteen of them had more than five children all together including biological children, adoptive children, foster children etc. Conclusions Earlier Swedish cohort studies have shown that both adoptive children and foster placed children are at risk for developmental difficulties later in life. There are reasons to believe that attachment is one contributing factor. Both adoptive children and foster children most often have experiences of trauma and/or maltreatment earlier in life, which are considerable threats towards a healthy development. Several studies have shown the impact of both adoptive- and foster parent’s attachment on children’s ability to solve developmental issues and for the healing process (Smyke &all., 2010; Rutter &all., 2004; Dozier &all., 2001). Preliminary results showed that over half of the foster parents to be had a secure/mildly insecure attachment style assessed by the ASI. Almost as many foster parents, however, had a highly insecure attachment style. Data revealed that a considerable amount of the foster parents to be already had foster children placed within their families. A large amount of these foster parents had a highly insecure attachment style. Probably all of them have been assessed for approval by authorities using current assessment tools. Even if just preliminary the results from this study stresses the need to assess attachment in both adoptive parents and foster parents to be using a reliable measurement. Until now there have not been common to measure attachment in assessments of any kind. Today, however, we have access to a standardized research based attachment interview. There is now a Swedish version of Attachment Style Interview - ASI. More and more social workers are trained in the use of the ASI. The result is that the Swedish ASI is used in some communities to assess attachment style as part in assessments for approval for adoptive and for foster parents to be. References Bifulco, A. (2002). Attachment style measurement - a clinical and epidemiological perspective. Attachment & Human Development, 4(2), pp. 180-188. ISSN (print) 1461-6734. Bifulco, A., Jacobs, C., Bunn, A., Thomas, D. & Irving, K. (2008). The Attachment Style Interview (ASI). A support-based adult assessment tool for adoption and fostering practice. Adoption & Fostering. Vol. 22, no 3, 33-45. 37 Dozier, M., Stovall, C., Albus, K., E. & Bates, B. (2001). Attachment for Infants in Foster Care: the Role of the Care giver State of Mind. Child Development. Vol. 72, no 5, 1467-1477. Sameroff, A. J. & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Models of Development and Developmental Risk. In C. H. Zeanah Jr. (Ed.) Handbook of Infant Mental Health. 2nd edition. NY: The Guilford Press. Kanuik, J., Steele, M. & Hodges, J. (2004). Report on a longitudinal research project exploring the development of attachments between older, hard-to-place children and their adopters over the first two years of placement. Adoption 6 Fostering. Vol. 28, no 2. 61-67. Siegel, J. D. (2001). Towards un Interpersonal Neurobiology of the Developing Mind: Attachment Relationships, “Mindsight” and Neural Integration. Journal of Infant Mental Health. Vol. 22(1-2), 67 – 94. Lindbland, F., Hjern, A. & Vinnerljung, Smyke, A. T., Zeanah, Ch., H., Fox, N. A., Nelson, Ch. A. & Guthrie, D. (2010). Place- B. (2003). Intercountry Adopted Children as Young, Adults – A Swedish Cohort Study. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. Vol. 73, 190-202. ment in Foster Care Enhances Quality of Attachment among Young Institutionalized Children. Child Development, January/February. Vol. 81, no 1, 212-223. Lundén, K. (2010) Att identifiera omsorgssvikt hos förskolebarn. Vad kan vi lära av forskningen. [To identify children at risk for maltreatment. What can we learn from research] Allmänna Barnhuset. Stams, G-J., J., M., Juffer, F. & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2002). Maternal Sensitivity, Infant Attachment, and Temperament in Early Childhood Predict Adjustment in Middle Childhood: The Case of Adopted Children and their Biologically Unrelated Parents. Developmental Psychology. Vol. 38, no 5, 806821. Lundén, K. (2011). To identify pre-schoolers at risk for maltreatment. “Todays children are tomorrows’ parents. Vol. 30-31, 30-41. Rutter, M., O’Connor, T. and the English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team (2004). Are There Biological Programming Effects for Psychological Development? Findigs From a Study of Romanian Adoptees. Developmental Psychology. Vol. 48, no 1, 81-94. 38 van IJzendoorn, M., Juffer, F. & Poelhuis, C., W. (2005). Adoption and Cognitive Development: A Meta-Analytic Comparison of Adopted and Nonadopted Children’s IQ and School Performance. Psychological Bulletin. Vol. 131, 301-316. ATTACHMENT, MOURNING AND RESILIENCE IN KAFALA AND ADOPTION Badra Moutassem-MIMOUNI1 Abstract The search for the origins, the need to know of the adopted / kafil child: is it a need to leave the foster home, or is it a need to strengthen its links with her? For the adopting, try to know his origins means to lose their foster child really or symbolically. Our work of several decades with deserted children at birth, with adopted children or in kafala or within the framework of the circulation of the children, of the donation of child or the taking in of a child without family in Algeria, brought me to qualify my understanding of the desire to know his(her) family of origin. The reasons are complex and require a deeper analysis in order to encircle better the deep mechanisms of this need to know: What are the effects of the discovery of the secret on the adopting and the adopted or mekfoul? What role plays the type of attachment in the resolution of the mourning of the adopted child and in his resilience? This text treats these questions through studies of cases met in my practice and my researches over more than thirty years. Keywords: adoption, kafala, desire to know, secret of the origins, the attachment, the mourning, Algeria. Teatcher in Department of psychology, University of Oran Es-Senia Algeria ; Research Director, Center of research of Anthropology (CRASC) Oran. Algeria ; Coordinator graduate school in anthropology ; Member of the Editorial Board of the journal Insaniyat (CRASC) since 2005; Email : HYPERLINK «mailto:mimbadri2013@gmail.com» mimbadri2013@gmail.com 1 39 1.Introduction Algeria, country of the shores of the Mediterranean, is a part of North Africa which is in the crossroads of diverse influences. Arab-berber and muslim country, african country, bordering country of Europe, Algeria presents a culture of a big diversity. As in all the cultures, the child occupies a special place and gives sense to the couple. The prohibition to procreate out of legal wedlock makes the child precious, strongly invested and sought. For more than thirty years I accompanied the evolution of the kafala in Algeria. If the adopted undergo pressures and live sufferings, the adopting is not exempted from it. If the kafala allowed helping thousands of rejected children born out of wedlock by offering them a substitution family, it is not made without difficulties. According to their conception of the kafala, adopting and adopted are confronted with more or less painful situations and have bereavements to be made. 2. Of the donation in the kafala A few decades ago, in case of infertility or of definitive celibacy, we adopted a child of the family so that everything stays in the family (inheritance, not to introduce a foreigner, not to take risk that this child introduces the discord into the family, etc.). So, as in all Africa (Lallemand, 1996), the donation of child always existed in Algeria, but since a few decades it is less and less practiced. Three factors worked against this mode of collection: - The first factor is connected to the social changes: the reduction in the fertility, the distension of the family links by the dispersal of the members of the family and the weakening 40 of the social links, made that the children are more and more adopted in the services of the State (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001) within the framework of the kafala (which is a legal and simple adoption without filiations and without inheritance) or taken in the greatest secrecy to be declared legitimate (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2012). - The second factor is of legal order: indeed, Algeria forbade the complete adoption from its independence en 1962. The kafala was promulgated by the code of the family with 1984 and according to the sharia the child does not take the filiations of the kafil (adopting), and will not inherit except on provision of the will of the kafil which can bequeath him(her) the fifth of its properties and more if the heirs agree. Under the pressure of associations and researchers, decree that concern change of name in 1992 (N 92-24 of January 13th, 1992) authorize the kafil to give his name, but not his filiations and can put him on the family book to protect the child and reassure kafils. This last measure was disputed and questioned and brings adopting to by-pass the law by dealing directly with the unmarried mother and to declare the child as biological child (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001). - The third factor is of psychological order: the adopting prefer more and more to take child of unknown origin (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2012) hoping, by there, not to have to face the risks of conflicts with the biological family. For others, they hope to be the unique referent “he will have nobody but me “what fills their narcissism and compensates their infertility. The adopting sometimes goes as far as feigning a pregnancy in a way that very few people know that she is going to adopt. Since the seventies, the births out of wedlock increased a lot (Freud, 2001) these children are often abandoned in the birth and taken in the institutions of the State (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001). The couples suffering from infertility are the main applicants of kafala, but more and more single women who did not get married resort to it (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2012) this practice extends to the three countries of the Maghreb (Algeria, Tunisia and Morocco) (Boucebci, 1982). The born children out of wedlock present two characteristics which are going to complicate the situation and to strengthen the determination of the secret of the origins: be illegitimate child (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2012) child of the sin thus of the haram (illicit, forbidden from the religious point of view) what is going to urge families to keep silent about the origins of the child either by giving him (her) illegally their name and filiation, while keeping silent fiercely about the adoption in spite of the obvious proofs that the child can discover, or to lie by inventing him(her) a filiation as for example « it is the son of my cousin from whom the husband died and left her without resources «. The fact that the child has no known family is thus going to increase the desire and the temptation to keep the secret. 3. The research for the origins The research is a powerful desire which is felt as a vital need. An enormous energy is invested in this quest of the origins. This mobilized energy or rather immobilized prevents from investing in other spaces of action and knowledge. Often the adopted which antici- pated prematurely the secret have school and relational difficulties, are often unstable, etc. … At the same time this energy carries them and establishes a constant pressure which supports them and exhausts them at the same time. When the adopting refutes this need, it can constitute a destructive brake. 3.1.The discovery of the secret The discovery of the secret by the child is made often late either in the preadolescence or in the adolescence. Whether it is for the child or for the adopting, it is an event which is completely going to upset relationships between both and to cause sometimes very important relational and psychological disorders at the child and the parents. 3.1.1 Effect of the discovery of the secret on the child The adopted is going to pass by several states: i) the refusal, ‘ I did not believe my ears, I said to myself they are jealous because my parents love me “; ii) the doubt “ I looked for details which would support this thesis “, this fifteen-year-old girl tells the psychologist how she wakes up at night, while her foster mother sleeps to look for indications “ everywhere, including in the refrigerator, the cooker, the toilet! “. The mother denies all her strengths the adoption and shows proofs: photos of sbou ‘ it is the seventh day when the child is named ‘ tesmiya ‘ the party organized this day can be grand (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2012); iii) the confirmation « one day my mother told me ‘ I did not carry (wear) you in my stomach ‘ and this day the world seemed strange. I went out and I walked for hours. I did not know what to do nor to whom to speak «. Disorientation, confusion, is often 41 evoked by the adopted during the late discovery of the adoption. After this confirmation the reactions are much contrasted: - The secret is going to be perceived as treason and there, the reaction can be very violent. It is the case of Nadia that is going to learn by her cousin that she was adopted. She is brutally going to change, she runs away of the house, and rejects quite altogether (we shall see this case more in detail). - Very early Lamia anticipated that there was a secret, but said nothing. Her reaction is totally contrary to the previous one. I met Lamia in the direction of the social action (DAS). It was an eighteen-year-old girl. Her mother takes me in private conversation and tells me “I adopted her, but you should not tell her «, the girl takes me in private conversation and tells me « I am adopted, but you should not tell to my mother that I know «. It was very moving this secret which guaranteed their mutual attachment (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001). - Other cases are going to adapt to this situation sometimes while keeping a rancor and a hostility more or less marked to their adopting, without damaging too much the relation : Karim 16 years will say I have no other mother than her, ‘ the other one ‘ threw me and I do not want to return to the institution «. 3.1.2. The adopting and their suffering in front of the discovery of the secret by the child Few studies dealt with the adopting and their sufferings to lose really or symbolically the child whom they so much invested, so liked and for whom they built projects in 42 the shorter or longer term. The adopting are never completely reassured, it is about a restless parenthood which brings them a lot of enjoyment but this one is often corrupted by the doubts which they can have and the fears of losing this child. For those who informed the child, they are always afraid of seeing the one of the biological parents trying to get back this child, fruit of their efforts and their education … As for those who bet everything on the absolute secrecy, we have two categories: for the first one, their terror is that the child learns that it is not theirs, that the secret is aired “ I would not bear it, it would kill me, it is unthinkable “ the discovery of the secret makes them lose their fantasized child, even if he tries to reassure them that he does not try to leave, that he wants just to know. A day a mom who hung on desperately to the secret tells me “if he learns, I shall not bear it, I cannot keep him”, another one answers the psychologist” when he will know I shall certainly have died “(she is sixty years old and he is twelve years old)! Another mom come “ return her daughter “ in the DAS, while we tried to convince her (by explaining her that it is just a crisis, that it is the adolescence, and that it is going to pass) to give up the project to undo the kafala, answers “ I cannot bear more that she knows that I am not her mother, I could not continue to make just like that “. For these cases, something is definitively broken. It’s as if their attachment to the child depended on this secret and could not survive it. This child was only a fantasy and to lift the veil on the adoption show that the real child was only a representation. As long as he did not know he could make illu- sion and play without him/her knowing the extra. The adopting make their mourning by rejecting definitively any link, any attachment. It’s as if the child had never existed. For the second category, it is the fear of being denied, rejected, of returning to the state of ‘infertility’ in its sense of ‘ space, absence, and drought ‘. Thirdly, even when their child does not want to leave them (and often he cannot, because when he manages to find his mother, this one is far from wishing for this meeting), the adopting feel relegated, they lose their place as parent “ the first one and the last one “ as they say when they want to take child without family. They have the feeling to be relegated, of becoming a second-class parent, “it is not anymore the same thing, I feel as swindled. It is me who made everything and now I am only the other one, ‘the adopting’ “. On the other hand, after all these years of secrets how to explain to the neighbors, to the friends, to the knowledge, that everything was ‘lie’. It is particularly testing for the single women who did not get married and who hid their celibacy by inventing a marriage, a divorce or the death of the husband “what am I going to tell to people?” A mom wonders with despair. This single woman who adopted a newborn girl, who was sick, that she looked and raised in the greatest secrecy. When the girl was 5 years old, she even moved towards a district where she was not known and told that her husband died in an accident. When her daughter discovered the secret at the age of fourteen, her big concern was “what am I going to tell to people?” “What are going to think the neighbors?” Her daughter asked her to explain her, to tell her story. “Am I going to tell her that she is an illegitimate child thrown by her mother the day of her birth, that I am a spinster?” This situation put the girl in all her states; she didn’t understand why her mother hangs on to her lies. She is fast going to discover that the mother was afraid of what will be said about her, so she starts to threaten her to tell everything to the neighbors, to make a scandal… An arm-wrestling started between both. Mediation allowed bringing a reassurance to the mother and to the child. On the psychological plan, during these crises, we attend a real narcissistic bleeding. The narcissism of the adopting which was enhanced by the adoption is badly shaken. The self-respect is devalued and the frustrations re-appear, the feeling of injustice “ why me? “, resurgences of the sufferings bound to the infertility or to the absence of marriage: my stomach betrayed me said the sterile; my Saad (fate, luck, part of the life…) betrayed me, if I had the opportunity to marry someone I would have my own children and I would not be in this humiliating and testing situation. It is the adopted child who makes the adopting parent and the risks of losing this child return them to the square one. 3.2.Contrasted resolutions of mourning: case studies We see by those examples that the mourning has to be made by both sides. Whether it is for the adopting or for the adopted, the work of mourning must be accompanied by the nursing (Schofield, Beek, 2006). As soon as symptoms appear, it is necessary to act quickly to prevent the situation from becoming inflamed between adopting and adopted. Still the adopting have to speak about it, ask for help. Of course the ideal is to make the accom- 43 paniment from the beginning of the adoption. The role of the adopting is undeniable in the resolution of the mourning. The type of attachment must be considered as much in the child’s as in the parents. Often we are in front of pre teenagers or of adolescent or young overexcited adults, hurt by the silence of the adopting and their lack of confidence. The quoted cases higher also show that the adopting are in insecurity, they are afraid of losing their child really or symbolically, of losing their status, their place within their social and family environment. The following three cases show how the type of attachment and the reactions of the adopting to the discovery of the secret can help or aggravate the situation: 1st case: Farida was adopted in the birth after the death of her father, her mother Fatna having six children was pregnant of the last one (Farida in this particular case) and was completely deprived. She meets a woman Fouzia in the bath, the latter had miscarriages and both was too discouraged for opposite reasons: the one had an additional child while she had difficulty in making live those whom she already has, the other one because she did not manage to have viable children. They develop a plan: when Fatna is going to feel the first contractions she has to go to Fouzia to give birth. Farida is going to be registered on the family book of Fouzia and her husband. The birth will be celebrated with all the rites relative to this event through the tesmiya. Farida will be cherished and raised in the secret. She is going to learn indirectly that she was adopted but she built an identity enough solid which allowed her to appreciate the benefactions of her adoption, she made as if 44 she knew nothing. While was thirty-year-old, the biological family makes surface. Having aged, seized with remorse or with greed, the mother informs her children that their sister had not died in the birth as she had told them but that she was very alive and lived in such place. They are going to come to ask to see their sister at first to know her, and then gradually they want to incite her to leave her foster home and make her glitter that she is going to be able to travel, go in France to her sister who was settled there, etc.... The temptation is big and in spite of its ambivalence Farida wants to know ‘ this family ‘. Her foster father, who loved her profoundly and who had understood the dilemma in which his child struggled, recommends her not to leave her work and suggests her leaving at first and then ‘ you will see ‘ he tells her. She leaves for France, at the beginning everything was magnificent, she was treated in distinguished guest, then gradually, the life takes its course, each resumes his role and she finds herself in an ordinary family with her stinginess, her conflicts, her obvious or latent hatreds. She caused jealousies … Of the blow she is going to compare both families and realizes that her real family it is the one who adopted her, with the other family she shared nothing ; neither values, nor habits, nor attachment which would have been able to make the faults bearable. From the fifteenth day she is going to begin calling her foster parents, crying and asking to return home. When she is going to return she will categorically refuse to see again her biological family and is definitively going to close the subject, when she remembers herself the event she smiles there “I was crazy and stupid, it is incompa- rable. We imagine things but finally it is only of the imagination, I buried them in my head! «. The mourning is made: “nobody will love me as much as dad «. These words echo the conclusion. These words echo the conclusion of SOULE (Verdier, Soule, et coll. 1986) “the confrontation with the idealized but often dirty parents, without qualities, or simply real and ordinary, is most of the time unbearable and harmful «. For Farida, and for a lot of case whom we met, this episode is far from being harmful, quite the opposite it was beneficial, builder and even helped in the resolution of the mourning. For her foster parents, this episode ached, but they were rather confident in their daughter and in their mutual attachment and they were finally released from their fears, at the same time it strengthened their narcissism, they had to say themselves « finally we are better parents than the ‘truths’ «... For more worried, ambivalent adopting or refusing to make the mourning of their infertility, to see their child discovering this adoption can destroy definitively the link based on a conditional attachment, based on the possessive appropriation of the child. 2nd case the impossible mourning: when sixteen-year-old Nadia is going to learn by a cousin that she was adopted, she was very far from suspecting her situation. The worst it is because the adoptive mom did not tolerate that her daughter discovers the secret, of the blow she put her at a distance and rejected her. The father on the other hand made every effort to keep her. But not tolerating to be not only the ‘illegitimate child’, but also the adopted and the rejected by her 2nd mom, she is going to begin at first to make all which is forbidden in the ambient culture: go out with the boys, smoke, be on drugs, to wear indecent clothes and to oppose everything. What has even more revolted the mother who saw her fears strengthened on the fact that “nothing good can come from such a child «, this bad object in charge of all the defects. Nadia eventually leaves the family place of residence and roam in the street with young people in escheat, be on drugs, to fight … Her father made every effort to bring her out from there but did not succeed. He supported her as he was able financially. It has been more than twenty years since she roams. In this case Nadia was not able to say goodbye and the only person (the foster mother) who would have been able to help her did not assist her; the wound was not able to heal to allow her to take back her life. 3rd case: a restoration. Faiza was placed in kafala in France there while she was already seven-year-old. Faiza raised by the sisters spoke French and thus had no particular difficulties to become integrated into the school. The question of the origins did not arise because she knew that she was adopted. But in the adolescence, Faiza begins to oppose, to smoke, to go out with the boys and gets pregnant. Kafils, not having been able to bear the shame in front of this behavior, is going to return Faiza in Algeria on the pretext of holidays, to deposit her in the center and to undo the kafala. Her reaction was extremely violent, she is bewildered, she does not understand that we can deposit her as a cumbersome parcel. She is going to live this 2nd abandonment as treason and is going there to blame her adopting but also herself. She is going to show for years of the anxiety, 45 disorders of sleep and of behavior (violence, aggressiveness, strong consumption of cigarettes). She had even hallucinations without loss of contact with the reality. Accompanied by a psychologist, she managed to stabilize her behavior, but more than thirty years after this second abandonment, she remains marked and continues to speak about it with a lot of ambivalence. S. Freud clearly defined the mourning in its text “Mourning and melancholy” (Freud, (2001) as a painful reaction to a loss. The feelings of revolt, sadness and even despair gradually have to give way to the reassurance to allow the saddened to take back his life by investing new external objects. It was necessary to release a part of energy of the traumatic event to be able to prelaunch the temporal process allowing to register new memories, new projects. Whether it is for the foster parents or for the adopted child there are bereavements there to be made. If Nadia had so much suffering and destroyed her life it is because she could not bear this second abandonment by her foster mother who could not accept that her daughter knows that she is only the adopting. Maybe that Nadia unconsciously perceived this refusal for a long time. Such a denial could be the base of more or less unconscious conducts which corrupt the relation to the child and weaken his attachment. So when he discovers his origin, the shady feelings that he perceived at his adopting created a crack, a wound which is going to open during the discovery and to prevent the necessary healing and the mourning in pursuit of his life and of his link with his foster home? 46 While numerous cases, having made research and which managed to find the mother, become aware that the fantasized mother is far from the real mother and whom she is finally only an ordinary, sometimes pathetic woman, lasts, rejecting; after a desperate collision, they finish by « yabardou « (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001) literally « they cool «, what expresses a relaxation of the tension which supported them during all these years of hope, and they eventually make generally the mourning of this mother so dreamed. Most that I met during these last thirty years, on second thought told me “sometimes I regret having looked for her, but in fact I do not regret because that allowed me to know. Now I am going to take care of my life «For the parents this incapacity and this phobia to tell his origins to the child finds and reveals in our sense the incapacity to make their mourning of the «biological» child, make the mourning also of their infertility or their forced celibacy. Families about which the child knew without their knowledge that he was adopted, brutally cut investment in the child, because he knows, he does not belong any more to «them». Of object of all the care, the child becomes a bad object which is necessary to expel, to reject, and to return in the DAS. What increases the pain of the child which does not include, which is going to relive the second abandonment? Often they express their fear: “if she sends me back, where I am going to go? I am afraid that she abandons me as the other one «. The analysis of the cases of failure of kafala questions in first position the secret on the origins. But behind this refusal we detect the insufficiency of the preparation of the adopting for their role. As any birth requires a gestation allowing the parents to get ready for this radical change in their life, the adopting need to make the mourning of the biological child to let enough energy invest on this substitution child who risks to remain it and to be only “the replacement «. As soon as the real child shows him, the adopted one is rejected and denigrated. That’s why the accompaniment of the adopting is essential in the maturation of the project of adoption, then in a good care of the child and finally to help to solve as one goes along the conflicts, the misunderstandings, the difficulties inherent to any parentalisation. In conclusion, the adoption as the kafala raises problems in all the countries. The foster parents just like their children have difficulty and are confronted with major existential questions. Some as the others are sometimes in sufferings and require an accompaniment and psychosocial care to help them to set their lives in order and to reduce the returns of kafala, extremely expensive on the emotional plan for the adopting and the adopted. There is enough services of help and of support to the adopting and to the adopted. It is essential to convince the foster homes that to know his origin is beneficial for their child but also for them. In a work (Moutassem-Mimouni, 2001) we so ended « The secret on the origins caused many tragedies, and however unpleasant it might seem to S.FREUD, the tragedy of Œdipe is, before being the tragedy of the incest and the loving desire for the parents, that of the abandonment and the secret on the origins «. References Baraud, E. (2008). Genre en Méditerranée. Les femmes face aux transformations socioéconomiques. Conflits, négociations et émergence de nouveaux rapports sociaux. Ramsès/ 21-24 avril 2008, Rabat, Maroc Berger, M. (1997). L’enfant et les souffrances de la séparation. Dunod, Paris. Boucebci, M. (1982). Psychiatrie, société et développement. Alger : Editions ENAL. Freud, S. (2001). Totem et Tabou, Editions, Petite Bibliothèque Payot, Paris. Freud, S. (2001). Métapsychologie. Editions, Petite Bibliothèque Payot, Paris. Lallemand, S. (1996). La circulation des enfants en société traditionnelle. Prêt, don échange. L’Homme, Tome 36 N°140, pp. 119-122. Moutassem-Mimouni, B. (2001). Naissance et abandon en Algérie, Karthala, Paris, Réédité par Ibn Khaldoun, Oran, 2003. Moutassem-Mimouni, B. (2012). La société algérienne face aux enfants privés de famille de l’indépendance à nos jours. Workshop de réflexion « Algérie : Penser le changement Quels apports des Sciences humaines et sociales ? les 04 et 05 janvier 2012, CRASC, Oran. 47 Moutassem-Mimouni, B. (2012). « Les célibataires-mères au secours des mèrescélibataires ». In Y/ Kniebieler & Francesca (eds) La maternité à l’épreuve du genre : métamorphoses et permanences de la maternité dans l’aire méditerranéenne. Presses de l’EHESP. Moutassem-Mimouni, B. (2012). Les enfants nés hors mariage en Algérie. Evolution des Représentations et de la prise en charge (Chap. 12), in « Modèles d’enfances. Successions, transformations, 48 croisements. Ouvrage collectif sous la direction de D. Bonnet & C. ROLLET & Ch. E. de SUREMAIN. . Paris : Edition des archives contemporaines. Schofield, G., Beek, M. (2006). Guide de l’attachement en familles d’accueil et adoptives. La théorie en pratique. Traduction française. 2011, Elsevier Masson. Verdier, P., Soule, M. et coll. (1986). Le secret sur les origines, ESF, Paris. UP FRONT AND PERSONAL: ADOPTIVE PARENTS’ PERCEPTIONS OF THEIR EASTERN EUROPEAN CHILDREN’S ADOPTION OUTCOMES IN CONTEXT Josephine A. RUGGIERO1 Abstract U. S. parents of 70 Eastern European children participated in telephone interviews with the author in which they shared their perceptions of their children’s adoption outcomes along with information about a variety of demographic and contextual variables. The study objective was to improve understanding of the variables connected with parents’ perceptions by conducting an in-depth investigation of adoption outcomes. The author spoke personally with 46 parents of adoptees who had typically had lived in their families for at least two years at the time of the interview. Respondents rated their adoption outcomes into one of four categories: very successful, pretty successful, somewhat problematic, or very problematic. Although these respondents rated the outcomes of three out of four children a very or pretty successful, one in four of the adoption outcomes were described as somewhat or very problematic. The author provides case examples to illustrate each adoption outcome. This study 1) challenges highly optimistic reports of questionnaires which claimed parental satisfaction of U. S. adopters of Eastern European children to be in the high 90% range, 2) recognizes and supports other research indicating that a sizeable minority of Eastern European adoptees exhibit behavioral and other serious problems which may require long-term post-adoption services, and 3) addresses an important gap in the scholarly literature on adoption outcome by grounding parents’ ratings in important demographic variables including the child’s sex and age at adoption, sibling adoptions, and the number of children adopted into a single family at the time of the interview and contextual variables 1 Professor Emerita of Sociology at Providence College. E-mail: jruggier@providence.edu This research was supported in part by a grant from the Providence College Committee to Aid Faculty Research. The author wishes to acknowledge Shannon Hoey for her assistance with literature searches and Sabrina Raulerson for her assistance with data preparation and tables. 49 including respondents’ perceptions of each child’s compatibility with them, their pre-adoption expectations of each child’s adoption outcome, perceived accuracy and completeness of the information they got about each child from the agency they used, pre-adoption preparation to adopt a child from a high-risk institutional setting, access to post-adoption services, support from family and friends and perceptions of the child’s relationship with them and other family members. The author discusses the implications of this research for adoptive family functioning and for the adoptees in regard to navigating the world of intimate relationships and future parenthood. Keywords: adoption outcome, Eastern Europe adoptees, family functioning, qualitative research Introduction Adoptive Parents’ Perceptions of Their Eastern European Children’s Adoption Outcomes in Context. U. S. citizens have a long history of adopting orphans born in other countries. This history dates back at least to the post-World War II period. Between 1990 and 2013, a new wave of immigrant orphans entered American families. Some of these new arrivals began to come from Eastern European countries. To date, Eastern European adoptees to U. S. families number more than 93 thousand children. Sixty-four percent of them have come from Russia. Adoptions of Russian-born orphans by U. S. citizens began as a trickle in 1991 and 1992, peaked in 2004 and declined between 2005 and 20131. Recently, Russian authorities halted adoptions of Russian orphans by U. S. citizens. Between 1993 and 2013, nearly 11 percent of the Eastern European orphans who joined American families during this time frame came from Ukraine. Between 1990 and 2004 inclusive, 8.9% of the Eastern European immigrant orphans adopted by U. S. citizens came from Romania. Adoptions of Romanian orphans by U. S. citizens stopped in 2005. The remaining 16% of the adoptees came from other Eastern European countries.2 50 Since 2005, international adoptions to the U. S. have declined overall. Literature Review A growing cross-disciplinary literature on Eastern European adoptees to the U. S. has emerged from the work of scholars and practitioners (Federici, 1998; Gindis, 1998; Goldberg, 1997, 2001; Groze, 1996; Groze & Ileana, 1996; Hawk & McCall, 2011; Johnson, 1997; Kreider & Cohen, 2009; McGuinness & Pallansch, 2007; Merz & McCall, 2011; Miller, L., Chan, W., Tirella, L. & Perrin, E., 2009; Miller & Adamec, 2004; Miller, Kiernan, Mathers, & Klein‑Gitelman,1995; Ruggiero, 2007, 2009). These researchers and practitioners have investigated Eastern European adoptees’ pre-adoption issues and postadoption needs including those experienced by their adoptive parents. For example, Miller et al. (2009) reported behavioral problems as a common trait in Eastern European adoptees and explored the risk factors involved. These authors evaluated cognitive and behavioral outcomes, as well as parenting stress, in relation to factors such as arrival age, growth, and prenatal alcohol exposure. They concluded that, although IQ and achievement scores were generally average or higher, behavior and school problems were common and correlated inversely with IQ. Thus, parents whose child had such problems were likely to experience increased stress. Through a longitudinal study conducted at two points in time, McGuinness & Pallansch (2007) analyzed problem behavior in children adopted from the former Soviet Union. Their study focused on the impact of risk factors and protective factors on the behavior of a group of 105 adoptees. These authors concluded that the protective factors of adoptive family environment have apparently increased in importance relative to pre-adoptive factors (except for birth weight), showing that families can play a significant role in the behavior of these children. In an thorough review of the literature on post-adoption interventions for at-risk internationally adopted children and their families, Welsh, Viana, Petrill & Mathias (2007) reported that “very little systematic information exists regarding the effectiveness of interventions designed to prevent and remediate these difficulties in IA children” (p. 285). They pointed out the challenges to parents and practitioners trying to untangle pre-natal and genetic influences from post-natal environmental variables like pre-adoption neglect, abuse, and institutional deprivations. In addition, Welsh et al. (2007) identified an important limitation in the literature they reviewed: the general focus on infants and toddlers rather than on older adoptees. An important implication of the Welsh et al. (2007) research is that savvy parents as well as practitioners, including health care professionals and educators, may not be able to identify accurately their children’s specific conditions. When children’s diagnoses are inaccurate, obtaining essential and effective services may be delayed or never be obtained. In response to media reports claiming that parents of Eastern European adoptees faced widespread post-adoption challenges, Cradle of Hope Executive Director, Linda Perilstein, commissioned a questionnaire survey of adoptive parents of Eastern European children who had worked with 18 adoption agencies as well as with Cradle of Hope, the study sponsor. More than 1200 parents completed the survey, 57% of the targeted parents. Essley and Perilstein (1998) reported that most of the children were doing well in regard to health, attachment, adjustment, and development. The Cradle of Hope Adoption Center survey reported a parental satisfaction rate of 97.5% in response to the statement: We are/I am pleased that this child has become a member of the family. Other questionnaire surveys have reported positive findings (Claus & Baxter, 1997; Price, 2000). Unfortunately, some reports of adoption outcomes of Eastern European children have lacked systematic investigation into the variables which provide a context for adoptive parents’ views about their children’s adoption outcomes. The study reported in this article includes demographic and other relevant social variables such as parents’ perceptions of their children’s connection to them and to other family members. Sample and Method Forty-six U. S.-citizen adoptive parents of 70 Eastern European children comprised the snowball sample of participants who agreed to speak in depth about their adopted children 51 and their adoption experiences through a telephone interview with the author. The author put out a call for participation that aimed to attract parents of Eastern European adoptees who had lived in their adoptive families for at least two years, were typically older than infancy when they were adopted, and/or were part of a biological sibling group adopted into the same family. The author tried to obtain as diverse a sample as possible.3 The large majority of interviews were completed by the primary caretaker-- the adoptive mother. Two couples requested to complete the interview together. The data reported here are based on 44 interviews with 46 adoptive parents. The interview was designed by the author and consisted of both open- and close-ended questions. Interviews averaged 90 minutes in length and covered areas in addition to the principal variable investigated here: parents’ perception of adoption outcome. Respondents also answered questions about their adoption experience(s), how each adoptee was doing at the time of the interview, the adoption agency (or facilitator) the respondents used, a typical weekend day for each adoptee, how the adoptee related to the respondent and to other members of the adoptive family, adjectives that came to mind to describe each child, their hopes and expectations for their child’s future and background questions about themselves, the adoptive family and the adoptees. Because of space limitations, the focus here is on parents’ perceptions of adoption outcome(s) and the important variables that help explain the reasons behind their perceptions. Demographic information about the adoptees is displayed in Table 1. Table 1 presents six single-variable distributions showing 52 percentages and numbers for each category. These variables include the children’s birth country, number of children adopted into each family, the child’s age at adoption, biological siblings adopted at the same time, length of time adoptee lived in their family, and sex of each child. As Table 1 shows, the majority of adoptees were born in Russia. Romanian adoptees comprised the next largest grouping. Children adopted from other Eastern European countries comprised the remainder. The number of children adopted into a single family ranged from one to five. Although having one adoptee in the home was typical of the majority of parents, four in 10 respondents had adopted two children. Two respondents had adopted three children and one had adopted two sibling groups at different points in time. At the time of their adoption, six in 10 of the adoptees were between 18 and 59 months old. Almost one in five was between 60 and 119 months old; and four percent were 120 months (10 years) old or older. Only a small minority of the children was between seven and 17 months old. Unique to this research is that siblings adopted at the same time comprised about 45% of the adoptees. Female adoptees outnumbered male adoptees. Because time spent in the adoptive family is an important variable related to the validity of parents’ perceptions of adoption outcome, the author tried to ensure that the children in this study lived in their adoptive homes long enough for their parents to become familiar with any major medical, behavioral, or emotional issues and needs they had. More than half of adoptees had lived with their adoptive families for between 24- 59 months. Because nearly 9 in 10 of the adoptees had lived with their adoptive parent(s) for at least two years, the author is confident that participants had sufficient experience with their children to accurately place them in one of the four adoption outcome categories. Table 1. Demographic Information about Adoptees at Time of Interview Variable Percentage Total Percentage Number Birth Country of Adoptees Russia 59 (41) Romania 23(16) Other Eastern Europe 18 (13) 100% (70) Number of Children Adopted into Respondent’s Family One52 Two41 Three or more 7100%(70) Age of Child at Adoption 7-17 month s17(12) 18-59 months60(42) 60-119 months19 (13) 120 months or older 4(3) 100% (70) Biological Siblings Adopted at Same Time Yes45 No55100% Length of Time Adoptee Lived in Adoptive Home Under 24 months 13 (9) 24- 59 months54(39) 60 months or longer 33(22) 100%(70) Sex of Adoptee Female58.5(41) Male41.5(29) 100% (70) At the time they were interviewed, 95% of the respondents lived in the United States. Those living in the U. S. were spread out geographically across five regions of the country. Nearly half lived in either the southwest or northeast. Seventy five percent of them were married. The large majority of the remaining 25% of participants identified themselves as single/never married. Only two respondents were separated or divorced. Regarding work status, families comprised of couples were slightly more likely to have one spouse working full time than both working full time. In fewer than 10% of these families, neither parent worked full-time at the time of the interview. In contrast, all single respondents re- 53 ported working full time. The large majority of respondents who worked full-time were typically employed in fields that required wither a four-year college degree or a postcollege professional degree. Examples of job titles included an attorney, a college professor, a college administrator, a CEO of a company, a nurse, and a nutritionist. Working spouses of married respondents were employed in a wider diversity of fields, both professional and other. Their professional fields included an engineer, a business manager, an educator, a salesperson, and a communications specialist. Other fields included employment as a carpenter, a contractor, a retail clerk, and a member of the armed forces. By extrapolation from information on jobs held, most of these adoptive families would be considered as either middle or upper-middle class at the time they respondents were interviewed. Four out of five respondents had no biological children living with them then. Of the respondents who did have biological children in the home, two-thirds said they had a single biological child. In response to the question: Overall, how successful would you say your adoption is? respondents chose from four options: very successful, pretty successful, somewhat problematic (good and bad days), and very problematic (more bad than good days). The author asked participants to provide an adoption outcome for each internationally-adopted child. After respondents answered this question, the author then read them the following: I am going to read a list of possible reasons which explain successful or problematic adoptions, especially of older children. Please choose the answer which best fits your situation. The 54 author then read each interviewee seven statements. These statements asked about participants’ perception of the match/compatibility of their adopted child with them; pre-adoption expectations regarding their child’s adoption outcome; amount of pre-adoption preparation they received regarding the effects of institutional living on a child’s development and behavior; amount of the pre-adoption preparation they received to parent a child with the issues or needs their child has; accuracy and completeness of the information their agency/ facilitator gave them about their child prior to adopting her/him; perception of the availability of various types of post-adoption services; and post-adoption support from family and/or friends. Respondents then ranked each statement from most important to least important in explaining their perception of their child’s adoption outcome. Results and Analysis4 Parents categorized three out of four of the adoptees as having a positive outcome-either very successful or pretty successful. They categorized one in four of their children’s adoption outcomes as either somewhat problematic or very problematic. A hierarchy of positive reasons emerged for respondents’ perceptions of the adoption outcomes as either very or pretty successful. Perception of the adopted child as a good match for them ranked as the most important (85%). The top reasons respondents gave for choosing this answer were that the child was similar to them in likes and dislikes; they could meet the child’s needs; and they believed the adoption was meant to be. The next reason parents tended to give was having access to appropriate or very good post-adoption services (63%). About one-third of respondents said that they found the services their child/children needed themselves. For school-aged adoptees, getting necessary school services was identified as important. Parents’ responses to access to school services and the appropriateness of the services they obtained for their child/children were not always positive. State of residence and school district as well as whether the school was public versus private school made a notable difference. Respondents’ getting a lot of post-adoption support from family and/ or friends was next in importance. Almost six in 10 (59%) respondents reported a high level of post-adoption support from either (or both) family and friends. Fourth, a majority of these parent(s) said that they had realistic expectations (57%) for how the adoption would turn out. Participants who fell into this category said that they knew of possible problems and/or had prepared themselves by doing their own research whereas parents who described themselves as having unrealistic expectations said they were either unaware of possible problems with the child/children or thought that the length of the child’s adjustment period in the family would be shorter. The health of a child is a very important factor in parents’ perceptions of adoption outcome. Behavioral and emotional issues are much more challenging for parents to manage than are physical health issues which can be corrected through surgery or managed with medication. Therefore, questions about each child’s health were included in this survey. Analysis of these data shows that almost two thirds of the children whose adoption outcomes were described as very successful had no diagnosed emotional, psychological, or behavioral problems. Close to one in four of the adoptees had some problems which respondents perceived as short term or manageable with medication. The remaining minority was reported as having some serious problems. Of the six children whose parents perceived their adoption outcomes as pretty successful, half had no diagnosed problems and half reportedly had serious problems. Two of the children in latter were adopted by the same respondent. The large majority of children who fell into the “somewhat problematic” adoption category had serious diagnosed conditions, according to their adoptive parents. Listed in alphabetical order—not in order of importance to respondents, these conditions included, Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD), Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), ADHD with Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD), Bi-Polar Disorder (BPD), Central Auditory Processing Disorder (CAPD), Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD), Sensory Integration Disorder (SID), Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD), communication processing problems, minor skin problems, and vision problems. Only two (17%) of the children in the “somewhat problematic” adoption category had no diagnosed problems upon their adoptions. In contrast, all six the other children in the very problematic adoption outcome were reported to have serious, longterm problems including bi-polar disorder, Asperger’s syndrome, RAD, Fetal Alcohol Effects (FAE), institutional autism, PTSD, ADD, ODD, moderate to severe hearing impairment, and language processing problems. 55 Case examples of respondents who described their adoption outcome as “somewhat” or “very problematic” help to illustrate what these families experienced after the child or children were in their new families. When these respondents adopted, adoption agencies varied considerably in the amount of pre-adoption preparation they provided to prospective adopters. About six in 10 respondents reported that the agency/agencies they used required no pre-adoption preparation of them. It is not surprising, therefore, that they were more likely to answer the two agencyrelated questions negatively than positively. A majority of respondents said that the agency/ facilitator they used did a poor job (62%) of preparing them on the effects of institutional living on a child’s development and behavior or did not prepare them at all (12%). The responses regarding preparing them for their child’s specific issues were similar: 64% said they were poorly prepared and seven percent said they were not prepared at all. A number of parents said that they did their own research and taught themselves because the agency they used was inexperienced in international adoptions and/or gave them no guidance. Responses regarding the accuracy and completeness of the information their agency/facilitator provided about their children prior to the adoption were mixed. Respondents were allowed to choose more than one applicable response to this question. Only about one in three (29%) of these parents thought they had received all the information available from the agency/ facilitator about their child/ren. Twenty–four percent said the information they received was accurate but fewer than 10% thought that it was complete. The principal reason respondents gave for the answers of incomplete and 56 inaccurate was that they believed the agency withheld information from them. The case scenarios provided below illustrate what parents shared about successful and problematic adoption outcomes and the reasons behind their categorization of the adoption outcome. The scenarios offer readers a personal glimpse into these parents’ perspectives on what they knew and what they expected in relation to each adoption outcome and on the child-family dynamic. Case Scenario #1: The Ideal Type of a “Very Successful” Adoption Outcome A married couple adopted two biological sibling girls from Russia when the girls were 11 months and two years, eight months old. The parents perceive their daughters’ adoption outcomes to have worked out perfectly. At the time of the interview, the children had lived with their adoptive parents for two years. The primary caretaker (adoptive mother) rated the adoption outcome for both girls as very successful. The most important reason she gave for this outcome was they the couple worked with an excellent, well-connected adoption agency which made the adoption process fast (seven months from start to finish). She also stated that they were well prepared by their home study agency. The home study agency they used required them to attend seminars, listen to guest speakers from a variety of fields and read a large manual. Their social worker, who had seven internationally adopted children herself, was very knowledgeable about the effects of institutional living on children. They got accurate and complete information about their daughters, had realistic expectations about how the adoption would turn out, obtained appropriate post-adoption services for the girls’ hearing and language needs and received a lot of post-adoption support-- especially from friends. This respondent described her children as “healthy and normal, a good match for them.” Case Scenario #2: A “Pretty Successful” Adoption Outcome of Romanian-Born Daughter A married couple adopted a girl from Romania when the child was almost two years old. The girl had lived with her adoptive family, including a stepbrother, for about four years. The respondent, a college professor, ranked the poor preparation by their adoption agency as the most important factor explaining some of the challenges they faced with their daughter. She mentioned not being told anything about sensory problems, attachment issues, ADHD, how to discipline the child without spanking her, how to handle language acquisition, or how to build a bond with the child. This respondent described the couple’s pre-adoption expectations as unrealistic in that they thought love and good food would be all that their daughter needed to thrive. Having a lot of support from the respondent’s father and siblings and thinking that the child was a good match for them-- because of her intelligence, good nature, and sunny disposition, made a big difference both in dealing with her issues and in the respondent’s perception of the adoption as pretty successful. Accessing appropriate pre-school services for her daughter from people knowledgeable about institutionalized children and as well as recommendations of specialists and free counseling also helped. Case Scenario #3: A “Somewhat Problematic” Adoption Outcome of Hungarian-Born Son A married couple who had adopted two unrelated children, a boy and a girl, from Hungary at the same time completed the interview together. These respondents used the rating of somewhat problematic for their son, the older of the two children. They rated their daughter’s adoption as very successful. Their son was five years old when they adopted him. He had lived with his U. S. family for two years when the interview took place. According to his parents, their son had a number of extremely serious emotional, psychological, and behavioral disorders, including RAD, PTSD, ADHD with ODD, SID, and CAPD. He also had some physical problems. These parents described their son as unprepared to live in a family and identified the lack of information about relevant post-adoption services as the most important reason for considering this child’s adoption outcome as somewhat problematic. They also said the school services they obtained for him were “piecemeal” and “inadequate.” They found many of the post-adoption services their son needed themselves. Regarding post-adoption support, this couple stated that it “dwindled” over time. Although their extended family was supportive, they did not live close enough to provide significant help. They also stated that their contemporaries had much older kids and that their son’s behavior alienated them from making friends with other families who had children the same age as him. About the adoption agency they used, this couple said that nobody mentioned any of their son’s issues before his adoption. They described the information the agency provided about their son as inaccurate and incomplete, saying that the agency had a lot of information which 57 they could have translated into English and given to the couple but did not. After the adoption, this couple was left to figure out what was going on with their son and to find resources on their own. They said their pre-adoption expectations for how things would turn out with their son’s adoption were unrealistic, stating that “they thought they could/would be loving parents and easily so.” These respondents described their son as a good match for them in terms of his love of learning but a poor match because of his many problems which affected his ability to live in a family. Case Scenario #4: A “Very Problematic” Adoption Outcome of Russian-Born Son This respondent was separated from her husband at the time she participated in the interview. They had adopted their son from Russia at seven years old. The boy lived with his mother and saw his father on weekends. He was their only child and was then 14 years old. She described her son as having many “severe” conditions including BPD, RAD, Asperger’s Syndrome, OCD, ODD, ADHD and a mild case of Tourette’s Syndrome. She stated that the medications on which he was placed at the time helped him only “a little.” After he entered puberty, this boy’s behavior became more extreme. He became more destructive (e.g., trashing toys, banging walls), began cussing, and started shoving and slapping his mother and children he encountered. His mother said he badgered her a lot to get what he wanted but was less aggressive with his father. The respondent stated that tensions surrounding their son’s behavior were instrumental in causing the couple’s separation. The top ranked variable she chose to 58 help explain her rating of this child’s adoption outcome was poor preparation by the adoption agency regarding the effects of institutional living on a child’s development and behavior. She said that the agency did not seem to understand these issues even though the social worker they used had herself adopted two children with whom she was having problems at the time. Second, the respondent thought the information the agency gave them about their son, although probably complete, was inaccurate. Third, although the couple had two agencies in their adoption of this child, neither agency prepared them to deal with the seriousness of his issues. She stated that their expectations were unrealistic because they didn’t know about the kinds of serious problems their son might have. She felt that this child was a poor match for them because “they are not (trained as) therapeutic parents or psychiatrists.” She also said they got no post-placement services from the medical community because health care providers had no experience with older post-institutionalized children, especially with children who has emotional problems. The lack of support from family and friends made it even more difficult to deal with their son’s many serious needs. Respondents’ perceptions of how well their adopted child fit into their family was an important contextual variable which was very likely associated with their choice of the child’s adoption outcome as successful or problematic. Respondents described 90% of the children as having a positive relationship with them. For three quarters of the adoptees, parents described this relationship as very good to excellent. For an additional 14%, respondents described the child’s relationship as good. Of those parents who had adopted two or more children, often biological siblings, they perceived their relationship with their second (or younger) child as more positive than their relationship with their first child. Parents who rated their child’s relationship with them in positive terms described the child as being “close or very affectionate.” These descriptors included enjoying each other’s company and spending one-on-one time together. For the remaining one in 10 of the children-those categorized as having a “so so” or “not very good” relationship, these parents made comments like: “We are just caretakers.” “I am resented as an authority figure.” “There are underlying tensions.” “The child is very difficult, jealous of spouse, or still has some institutional effects.” Married respondents perceived their child/ children as generally having equally good relationships with their spouse. They described this relationship as very good to excellent for about six in 10 of the children, and as good for about one in 10. Parents with spouses either present in the home or who maintained contact with the child, gave the same explanations that they mentioned for themselves, some adding that their spouse was more lenient or played more often with the child. When relationships were problematic, respondents typically described their spouse as less tolerant or understanding of the child’s issues than the respondent was. Respondents stated that the children’s relationships with their adoptive grandparents varied depending on the grandparents’ health, proximity (geographical location) of the grandparents’ residence to the child’s home, grandparents’ age and activity level, his/her interest in interacting with the adopted grandchild, and his/her ability to handle a child who is challenging. Nine out of 10 of the children who had interaction with their grandparent(s) related very well or well with them. When grandparents lived close, the children tended to see them regularly and to develop a loving relationship with them. For adoptees with one or more other children in the home, either biological or nonbiological siblings, three out of four of the respondents reported that the adoptee had either a very good or a good relationship with her/ his sibling(s). Although a number of parents referred to the existence of “typical sibling rivalry” or of behaviors of the child or sibling that were barriers to positive interaction, some also talked about their children spending lots of time together and the younger child idolizing his/her older sibling(s). In sum, the most successful adoption outcomes reflect a connection between the child/ ren and their adoptive family. In contrast, the problematic adoption outcomes reflect either ambivalent or no connection between the child/children and their adoptive family. Other Variables Examined in Relation to Perceived Adoption Outcome Tables 2-4 present cross-tabulated data. Table 2 shows perceived adoption outcome and the child’s age at adoption. Table 3 shows perceived adoption outcome and the child’s sex. Table 4 presents the distribution of adoptees by sex and age. The data in Table 2 suggest an association between a child’s age at adoption and parents’ perceptions of the adoption outcome. 59 As Table 2 shows, respondents described all the children whom they adopted at 17 months old or younger as having either a “very successful” or a “pretty successful” adoption outcome. Parents also reported a “very successful” adoption outcome for all three of the children who were adopted at ages 10 years (120 months) or older. The two age categories in the middle showed the most variation in parents’ perception of adoption outcome. All the children with “somewhat problematic” and “very problematic” adoption outcomes fell in the two middle age-range categories with children adopted at ages 18-59 months most likely to be identified by their parents as having problematic adoption outcomes. Still, the adoption outcomes of the majority of children even in these two middle-age range categories were categorized by their parents as positive. Table 2. Perceived Adoption Outcome by Child’s Age at Adoption Adoption Outcome Very Successful Pretty Successful Somewhat Problematic Very Problematic TOTAL 7-17 months n 11 1 0 0 12 % 92 8 0 0 100 18 -59 months 60- 119 months 120 months and older n 25 4 10 3 42 n 7 1 2 3 13 n 3 0 0 0 3 The data in Table 3 suggest an association between sex of the adoptee and parent’s perception of adoption outcome. Specifically, More than eight in 10 of the female adoptees were described by their parent(s) as having either very or pretty successful adoption out- % 59.5 9.5 24 7 100 % 54 8 15 23 100 % 100 0 0 0 100 TOTAL n 46 6 12 6 70 % 66 8.5 17 8.5 100 comes. In contrast, two-thirds of the male adoptees were described as having either a very or pretty successful adoption outcome. Thus, male adoptees were 21% more likely than female adoptees to have parent-perceived problematic adoption outcomes. Table 3. Perceived Adoption Outcome by Child’s Sex Adoption Outcome Male n % n % Very Successful 30 73 16 55 Pretty Successful 4 10 2 7 Somewhat Problematic 6 15 6 21 Very Problematic 1 2 5 17 TOTAL 41 100 29 100 The data in Table 4 suggest an association between the child’s sex and his/her age at adoption. Females in this study tended to 60 Female be adopted at younger ages than males. Specifically, one in four of the female adoptees as compared to seven percent of the males were adopted between seven and 17 months old. Close to two thirds of the females as compared to about six in 10 of the males were adopted at ages which varied between 18 Table 4. Child’s Sex and Age at Adoption Age at Adoption 7-17 months 18-59 months 60-119 months 120 months and older TOTAL When viewed together, the data in Tables 2-4 appear to support a joint interaction among the variables of age, sex, and adoption outcome for the children in this study. Discussion and Future Study By the end of fiscal year 2013, more than 93,000 Eastern European children were part of U. S. families. That number of adoptees from Eastern European sending countries to a single receiving country, the United States, is huge. It is of paramount importance that well-conducted research on these adoptees and their families study them at different phases of the children’s lives through adulthood. Participants in this study stated that virtually all of the adoptees entered their families directly from orphanage settings. Adopting directly from institutional settings in Eastern Europe is the typical route for adoptive families of these children. Depending on their age at adoption and the events in their early history, children may live in orphanages from several months to many years. Some of the children in the present study had lived their months and 59 months old. Males were 20% more likely than females to be adopted at five years old or older. Female n % 10 24 25 61 4 10 2 5 41 100 Male n % 2 7 17 59 9 31 1 3 29 100 whole lives in an orphanage before they were adopted. Just over one-third of the adoptees in this study were diagnosed with emotional, psychological, or behavioral problems after they arrived in the United States and were examined by U. S. doctors or other professionals. From comments respondents shared, the author concluded that more than a few of these adoptees should have been classified as having special needs before they were offered for adoption but were not presented as such. The designation of a child as likely to have special needs prior to his/her adoption is important for several reasons. First, preadoptive parents could make a more informed decision about whether or not to adopt the referral of a child or children IF they know in advance about special needs, especially about pre-natal exposure to alcohol, issues with attachment and behavioral problems observed before adoption. Second, unless their needs are physical and correctable through surgery and/or medication, special needs children require more and longer-lasting post-adoption services and a greater investment of parents’ time, energy, and financial resources. Third, 61 the risks of parental burnout and possible disruption of the adoption are higher. The study reported here offers social and behavioral scientists and professionals in medical and related fields a unique, in-depth glimpse into how 70 adoptees from populations at-risk for special needs are doing in their American families, how their adoptive parents perceived each child’s adoption outcome, and how some have dealt with the long-term post-adoption, challenges they encountered. Although parents rated three out of four of their child/ren’s adoptions as successful, it is apparent to the author that rating some of these children’s adoption outcomes as successful was due more to the respondents’ extraordinary creativity and pro-activity in doing their own research about their child’s issues and in searching for needed post-adoption services and less to the children having an optimal start and an easy post-adoption period for them and their families. Most people desire, and expect, to adopt a healthy child. Many seek infants or young toddlers. All expect to adopt a child who will fit into their family after a reasonably-brief period of adjustment. In three quarters of the adoptions in this study participants got what they expected. Participants who adopted children under 18 months old-- all of whom reported very successful adoption outcomes for these children-- appear to have gotten what they expected. Other research on adoptees from Russia has reported age to be a relevant factor in behavior problems, especially during adolescence (Hawk & McCall, 2011) and in parents’ ratings of deficits in their schoolage children’s executive functioning, including working memory and control of inhibi- 62 tions when compared to children adopted at younger than 18 months old ( Merz & McCall, 2011). Children adopted before they were 18 months old reportedly showed fewer behavioral problems in adolescence than children adopted at 18 months or older (Hawk & McCall, 2011). Interestingly, Merz et al. (2011) found that differences in executive functioning did not appear in pre-school age children and were not related to prematurity. Overall, these studies support the findings reported in this article. Age can mask dysfunctional pre-adoption experiences. What traumas did adoptees experience in his her birth home and/or in orphanages prior to being adopted? Older children come to their adoptive families with an already-formed personality and a pre-adoption history. Adoptees of all ages also come with genetic predispositions and risk factors. The high rate of alcoholism and domestic violence in Russia are examples of major risk factors which affect a child’s pre- and post-natal development and early experiences in a family. To lead to intimate inter-personal relationships later in life, a secure attachment with a parent or other consistently-attentive caregiver must occur within the first seven months of a child’s life (Stout, 2005: 131). How likely is a secure, early attachment to have occurred for older children with troubled pre-adoption histories and even for infants who have spent their whole lives in orphanages? The reasonable answer is not very likely. Because such secure attachments are not likely in the preadoption phase, it is not surprising that attachment and behavioral issues were most likely to reported in the adoptions perceived as in this study. Another variable to consider is the child’s desire to be adopted. Children who want to be adopted will have an easier time of attaching to their adoptive parents. This statement rises the question of how young is young enough to ask a child if s/he wishes to be adopted by non-biological relatives and move to a country far away from his/her birth country. This author suggest that children as young as three years old ought to be asked, counseled, and given an informed say in the decision to be adopted either outside or inside their birth country. The positive adoption outcome reported in this study for adoptees 10 years or older is both hopeful and should be explored further through subsequent research. One reason for this outcome may be that older children in this study somehow had a better understanding of, and appreciation for, how families actually function. They might have established positive attachments in their birth families or possibly with other caretakers during their formative months and these attachments may have enabled them to establish future attachments with their adoptive parent(s). The older children may have been waiting for their chance to be adopted into a loving family and were open to the opportunity when it came along. Exploring other variables which may be associated with respondents’ perceptions of adoption outcomes can also provide insights into factors related to a child’s adoption outcome. Based on previously-cited questionnaire surveys (See Essley and Perilstein, 1998; Claus & Baxter, 1997; Price, 2000), one would expect most parents’ perceptions of the adoption outcome for the child/ren they adopted to be concentrated at the positive end of the continuum. The data reported here support that conclusion overall. However, one in four adoptees was categorized as having either somewhat or very problematic adoption outcomes. For the families of these children, completing the adoption was the beginning rather than the end of their struggle to be parents. Even if one in four is a rough estimate, when extrapolated to the more than 93 thousand Eastern European children adopted by U. S. families, the numbers of adoptees and adoptive families that may fall into this category is staggering. Since most previous research had focused on adoption outcome success, the study reported here is especially insightful in the situations where respondents perceived the adoption outcome of their child/children as problematic and as significantly challenging family well-being. In such situations, the preadoptive information adoptive parents were given was minimal and/or inaccurate beyond any obvious physical problems the child had at the time. When one considers a child’s “health,” however, physical health is only one dimension of “global health.” Moreover, many physical health conditions are treatable through surgery and/or medication. Mental, emotional and behavioral health issues, however, are often more challenging to diagnose and to treat effectively. Such conditions may also not manifest themselves right away. Although there is not a lot of variation in adoption outcome reported by respondents, relatively speaking, there is more variation than what is typically reported in surveys conducted by adoption agencies. Although adoption outcome is not exactly the same as satisfaction with the adoption, the two vari- 63 ables are correlated. Trained professionals, including Ronald Federici (1998), an adoptive parent of five Romanian-born adoptees and a neuropsychologist who diagnoses and works with special needs adoptees from Eastern Europe, have expressed skepticism of the highly-positive results reported in surveys conducted by adoption agencies-- viewing them as over-estimated and self-serving for the agencies. Many children adopted from Eastern European countries have now reached adolescence and young adulthood. Therefore, it is important to ask how they and their families are doing. One way we can get glimpses into various stages of their children’s lives is by asking them as well as their adoptive parents. Important milestones on which to focus include completion of educational goals, ability to find and sustain work obligations, successful development of long-term relationships and genuine intimacy and trust with a potential or actual spouse/partner, and where they are in terms of their thinking and behavior regarding parenthood. With this goal in mind, the author encourages social scientists and others to develop cross-disciplinary investigations and dialogues about adoption as a vibrant sub-field of family studies. This sub-field should involve doing more high-quality, cross-disciplinary longitudinal research on adoptees and their adoptive families at various stages in the life cycle of the children and their families. Such research could provide a more solid foundation for addressing policy issues and appropriate advocacy on behalf of the diversity of adoptees and adoptive families we have in contemporary societies currently. 64 References Claus, D. & Baxter, S. (1997). Post-adoption survey of Russian and Eastern European children: conducted by Rainbow House International. Retrieved June 24, 2011 from http:// www.rhi.org/Anews/SurveyResults1997. html. Essley, M. & Perilstein, L. Adoptions from Eastern European orphanages overwhelmingly successful. (March, 1998). Retrieved June 24, 2011 from http://www.cradlehope. org/surv/html/Survey Findings Federici, R. (1998). Help for Hopeless Child. A Guide for Families. Alexandria, VA: Federici & Associates. Gindis, B. (1998). Navigating uncharted waters: School psychologists working with internationally adopted post-institutionalized children. COMMUNIQUÉ [Part 1], 27(1) 6-9 and [Part 2], 27(2), 20-23G. Goldberg, R. (1997). Adopting Romanian children: Making choices, taking risks. Marriage and Family Review. Vol. 25, Nr 1/2, pp. 79-98. Goldberg, R. (2001). The social construction of adoptive families: A follow-up study of adopting Romanian children.” International Review of Sociology. Vol. 11, Nr 1, pp. 89-101. Groze, V. (1996). Successful Adoptive Families: A Longitudinal Study of Special Needs Adoption. Westport, CT: Praeger. Groze, V. & Ileana, D. (1996). A followup study of adopted children from Romania.” Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal. Vol. 13, Nr 6, pp. 541-565. Hawk, B. N. & McCall, R. B. (2011). Specific extreme behaviors of post-institutionalized Russian adoptees.” Developmental Psychology. Vol. 47, Nr 3, pp. 732-738. Johnson, MD., D. (1997). Adopting an institutionalized child: What are the risks?” The Post, Issue #9 (January‑February), pp. 1‑3. Kreider, R. & Cohen, P. N. (2009). Disability among internationally adopted children in the United States. Pediatric, Vol. 124, pp. 1311-1318. McGuinness, T. M., & Pallansch, L. (2007). Problem behaviors of children adopted from the former Soviet Union. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. (May/June), pp. 171-79. Merz, E. & McCall, R. (2011). Parent ratings of executive functioning in children adopted from psychosocially depriving institutions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Vol. 52, Nr 5, pp. 537-546. Miller, MD., L. C., Chan, W., Tirella, L. & Perrin, E. (2009). Outcomes of children adopted from Eastern Europe. International Journal of Behavorial Development. SAGE Journals Online. Retrieved 17 Nov. 2010. <http://jbd.sagepub.com/content/33/4/289. refs.html>. Miller, MD., L. C. & Adamec, C. (2004). Handbook of International Adoption Medicine: A Guide for Physicians, Parents, and Providers. Cary, NC: Oxford University Press. Miller, MD., L. C., Kiernan, M. T., Mathers, M. I. & Klein‑Gitelman, M. (1995). Developmental and nutritional status of internationally adopted children, Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. Vol. 149, Nr 1, pp. 40‑44. Ruggiero, J. A. (2007). Eastern European Adoption: Policies, Practice and Strategies for Change. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Ruggiero, J. A. & Johnson, K. (2009). Implications of recent research on Eastern European adoption for social work practice, The Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal. Vol. 26, Nr 5, pp. 485-504. Stout, M. (2005). The Sociopath Next Door. New York: Broadway Books. U. S. C. I. S. Immigrants admitted as orphans by sex, age, and region and selected country of birth. Table 15 for Fiscal Year 1993-2002; Table 10 for Fiscal Year 20032004; Table 12 for Fiscal Year 2005-2013. Website address for fiscal year 2013 statistics is http://www.dhs.gov/yearbook-immigration-statistics-2013-lawful-permanent-residents. 65 NOTES The statistics on Russian adoptees to the U. S. were prepared by the author from statistics provided by the U. S. C. I. S. Source: Immigrants Admitted as Orphans by Sex, Age, and Region and Selected Country of Birth. Table 15 for Fiscal Year 1993-2002; Table 10 for Fiscal Year 2003-2004; and Table 12 for Fiscal Year 2005-2013. 1 The other Eastern European countries from which U. S. citizens have adopted include Albania, Armenia, Belarus, Bulgaria, Estonia, Hungary, Kazahkstan, Latvia, Lithuania, Moldo va, Poland, and the Republic of Georgia. 2 To target as diverse a sample as possible, the author a) sent a description of the research project to two large Eastern European adoptive parents groups whose administrators agreed to advertise a call for participation among their members, b) recruited respondents through adoptive parents who were leaders of, or involved in, adoptive parent support groups in various parts of the United States, c) developed a web-page describing the research, and d) asked adopters who had 3 participated in an earlier survey conducted by the author to refer other adoptive parents they thought would be interested in participating in the interview study. Because of privacy issues and the sensitive nature of adoption, it was not possible to obtain a true, national-level random sample of either adoptive parents or international adoptees from a country or groups of countries. There is no existing nation-wide list of adopt- 66 ers from which to choose a random sample. To date, there have also been no large-scale, national samples of U. S. citizens who adopted children from countries with potentially notable numbers of children with, or at-risk for, special needs and available for international adoption. Because this sample of respondents is non-random, statistical tests and measures which require random samples are not used in the analysis of the data. Rather, the author uses percentages in this largely qualitative analysis. 4 PILOT STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSIVE TEACHING ON YOUNG ADOPTED CHILDREN AND THEIR PARENTS: A COMPARISON OF TWO LEVELS OF TREATMENT INTENSITY Gerald MAHONEY1 Sunghee NAM2 Frida PERALES3 Abstract Objective: The purpose of this pilot study was to examine the feasibility of a parent-mediated developmental intervention called Responsive Teaching at enhancing maternal responsiveness and addressing the development and social emotional functioning of young adopted children. Methods: Twenty eight parent-child dyads in which all children had been adopted and were under six years of age were randomly assigned to two treatment intensity groups: 3 and 6 months. Each group received weekly individual Responsive Teaching sessions in which parents were coached to use Responsive Interaction strategies to enhance their interactions with their children during daily routine activities. Results: Mothers in both the 3 and 6 month intervention groups made significant increases in responsiveness, while their children made improvements in their development and social emotional functioning. Treatment group differences in children’s development and social emotional functioning were not significant; but maternal responsiveness ratings at the 6 month observation were significantly higher for mothers in the 6 versus the 3 month group. Intervention effects were not associated with children’s age or time living with parents, but international adoptees made greater improvements than domestic adoptees. Conclusions. Responsive Teaching appears to be an effective intervention for encouraging parents to increase their responsive interactions with their young adopted children whether they receive 3 or 6 months of intervention services. 1 Ph.D, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio E-mail: gim14@case.edu 2 Ph.D, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio 3 Ph.D, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio 67 Implications for Practice: Responsive Teaching is a relationship focused intervention that has the potential to help reduce the high incidence of developmental and social emotional problems commonly observed among young adopted children. Key words: social emotional functioning; young adopted child; Responsive Teaching; responsive interaction strategies; mother responsiveness; international adoptees; domestic adoptees. The development and social emotional functioning as well as academic performance of the majority of adopted children is comparable to their non-adoptive peers (Bimmel, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2003; Stams, Juffer, van IJzendoorn & Hoksenbergen, 2005; Keyes, Sharma, Elkins, Iacono &McGue, 2008). Nevertheless, adopted children are more likely than nonadoptive children to experience a number of developmental challenges including developmental delays, low IQ, deficits in communication (van IJzendoorn, Juffer & Poelhuis, 2005), as well as disorders in self-regulation and other social emotional conditions (Bramble, Radel, & Blumberg, 2007; Juffer & van IJzendoorn, 2005; Keyes, et. al., 2008). In addition, a disproportionately high percentage of adopted children receive special education (Welsh, Viana, Petrill, Mathias, 2007) and mental health services (Sharma, et. al., 2008) and have low levels of academic achievement (van IJzendoorn, et.al., 2005). Although it is difficult to compare the prevalence of these problems among domestic and international adoptees, some maintain that the extremely adverse pre-adoption histories of many international adoptees place them at greater risk for these problems than domestic adoptees (e.g., Wiersbicki, 1993; Welsh, et. al., 2007). Reasons for the developmental and social emotional problems associated with adoption 68 are complex. However, the most frequently cited reason is the difficulty that adopted children have in establishing a secure attachment with their parents (Howe, 2003; Handley-Derry, 1997; Marcovitch, Goldberg, Gold, Washington, Wasson, Krelwich, 1997; Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, 2005). Attachment problems have been attributed to a number of factors including the age children are adopted, the number of placements and disrupted attachment relationships children experience, the quality and consistency of care during pre-adoptive placements, as well as unresolved health issues, inadequate nutrition and prenatal exposure to drugs and alcohol (O’Connor & Rutter, 2000; Stams, et. al., 2000) . In addition, the limited experience many adopted parents have raising children is thought to increase the stress they experience managing their adopted children’s unique social-emotional behaviors, thus exacerbating their children’s attachment challenges (Edelman & Connolly, 1986; Levy-Shiff, Goldschmidt & Har-Even, 1991). Despite the high rate of attachment problems among adopted children, it is unlikely that all of their developmental challenges can be attributed to attachment. Given the rates of attachment problems reported for adopted children (e.g., Juffer, et. al., 2005; Howe, 2003) compared to their incidence of developmental and social-emotional prob- lems (Keyes, et. al., 2008), many adopted children undoubtedly develop appropriate attachments yet still manifest developmental delays or social-emotional challenges. Such problems may be associated with the inherent characteristics of these children both with respect to their capacity for developmental growth as well as their temperament style and other challenging social behaviors (c.f., Wierzbicki, 1993). Yet numerous studies of nonadoptive children suggest that even when parent-child attachments are not problematic, children’s development and social emotional functioning are not simply a reflection of their inherent vulnerabilities, but are also affected by the degree to which their parents engage in highly responsive interactions with them (Mahoney & Nam, 2011). For example, while disabilities such as Down syndrome or autism severely impact the communication skills of these children, the level of communication competence these children acquire is greatly influenced by their parents’ level of responsiveness (Mahoney, 1988; Siller & Sigman, 2002; 2008). Early interventions that have been designed to either address adopted children’s social emotional challenges or prevent such problems from occurring have tended to focus on enhancing maternal sensitivity (i.e., responsiveness) to promote secure attachment. Results from these interventions have generally been favorable at enhancing maternal sensitivity and children’s attachment (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003), and some have reported improvements in children’s play and social competence as well (van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, 2005). Most of these interventions have been brief, presumably because research suggests that shorter interventions are more effective than longer ones (van IJzendoorn, et. al, 2005). In addition, most attachment interventions have been conducted with children who are in their first year of life, raising the question about their effectiveness with older children (van den Dries, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, BakermansKranenburg, 2009). While attachment-based interventions offer a promising method for addressing some of the social emotional problems of adopted children, their focus on attachment as the major target of intervention may not be appealing to parents who are not challenged by relationship or attachment problems even though they may be concerned about their children’s development and risks for future problems. However, these parents may be more enthusiastic about interventions which either focus on the child development issues that are of concern to them and/or emphasize preventing their children from acquiring such problems. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the feasibility of a general developmental intervention called Responsive Teaching [RT (Mahoney & MacDonald, 2007)] as a means of enhancing the quality of parent-child relationships as well as addressing the developmental and social emotional issues that adoptive children are experiencing. RT is a parent mediated intervention designed to enhance children’s cognitive, communication and social emotional functioning. Similar to attachment based interventions RT encourages parents to engage in highly responsive interactions. This occurs by coaching parents to use Responsive Interaction (RI) strategies 69 during play and other routine activities with their children. RI strategies are suggestions that help parents modify their interactive behavior such that it reflects each of five components of responsive interaction: Contingency – “Respond immediately to little behaviors”; Reciprocity – “Take one turn and wait”; Affect – “Interact for fun”; Match – “Do what my child can do’; and Non-Directiveness – “Follow my child’s lead”. One of the major features of RT is that RI strategies are not used to change parents’ interactive style, but rather to help their children increase their use of the pivotal developmental behaviors which are purported to be the foundations for developmental learning. Thus, to address parents’ concerns about their children’s cognitive development, RT may encourage parents’ use of RI strategies to promote their children’s “social play”, “exploration”, or “practice”. If parents’ concern is communication, RI strategies may be recommended to promote children’s “joint attention” or “intentionality”. For social emotional concerns, parents might be asked to use RI strategies to promote their children’s “trust”, “cooperation” or “self-regulation”. By emphasizing that the purpose of intervention is to change the child as versus the parents, RT attempts to reduce implications that parents are to blame for their children’s problems. Four studies have been reported with children with developmental delays or disabilities which support the effectiveness of RT (Mahoney & Perales, 2003; 2005; Karaaslan, Diken & Mahoney, 2013; Karaaslan & Mahoney, 2013). The children who participated in these studies ranged from six months to almost five years. In each of these studies, RT 70 was conducted during individualized parentchild sessions either once or twice a week for periods ranging from 4 to 12 months. Results from all studies indicated that RT resulted in significant improvements in mothers’ responsiveness; three reported improvements in children’s cognitive and communication development (Mahoney & Perales, 2005; Karaaslan, et. al., 2013; Karaaslan & Mahoney, 2913); and two reported social emotional improvements (Mahoney & Perales, 2003; 2005). In general, the age children began receiving RT did not affect intervention outcomes. In addition, intervention effectiveness was not associated with children’s diagnoses, although social emotional improvements were observed primarily for children experiencing problems in this domain (Mahoney & Perales, 2003; 2005). In this study, adopted children and their parents were randomly assigned to one of two RT treatment intensity groups: three or six months. Each of these groups was scheduled to receive one RT session each week. The purpose was to determine whether these different levels of treatment intensity would be associated with changes in parents’ style of interaction and psychosocial functioning, as well as children’s development and social emotional functioning. In addition, we were interested in exploring how intervention effects would be associated with the children’s age, time with their parents, and status as a domestic or international adoptee. Methods Sample This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Case Reserve Western University. Subjects were recruited through program flyers that were distributed to medical clinics and social agencies that provided services to adopted children and their families. These flyers described the project as a “preventive intervention” and did not require that parents had concerns about their children’s current development or social emotional functioning. A total of 35 parent-child dyads signed an approved subject consent form to participate, and the final sample consisted of 28 dyads. Subjects excluded from the final sample either discontinued participation (n=4) or failed to complete follow-up assessments (N=3). The demographic characteristics of subjects excluded from the study were not significantly different from those of the final sample. As indicated on Table 1, the average age for mothers was 41.2 years and 45.7 years for fathers. The majority were married (90%) and white, non-Hispanic (90%). Sixty eight percent of the mothers were college graduates and 71.5% were working either full or part time. Families had an average of 2.6 children and most (78.5%) had annual incomes greater than $60,000. Table 1. Demographic characteristics of parents and children 3 Month 6 month (n= 14) (n=14) Variable % Characteristics of Parents Age mother Age Father Number of Other Children Marital Status (% Married) Race White (Non-Hispanic) Black Education High School Post-Secondary College Graduate M (SD) % 41.9 (4.6) 43.0 (4.2) 2.1 (2.6) 93% 79% 21% 0.53a 1.15a 1.38 86% 100% 7% 29% 0% 29% 64% 71% 14% 29% Part-Time 57% 43% 7% 21% 71% M (SD) 40.5 (5.2) 48.4 (16.9) 1.1 (1.4) Mother Employment Full-Time Family Income $20-40,000 $40- 60,000 Over $60,000 Statistics 7% 7% 86% 3.36b 1.08b 3.61b 1.18b 71 Characteristics of Children Age at Intervention Onset Age at Adoption Gender (% Males) % International Adoption % Resided in Orphanage % Foster care Race White (Non-Hispanic) African American Hispanic Asian Other a 72 21% 64% 57% 33% 32.2 (16.1) 14.4 (13.2) 36% 29% 0 29% 7% 43% 71% 57% 67% 36% 0 29% 21% 14% 37.8 (16.9) 11.6 (12.6) 0.80a 0.32a 1.44a 0.15a 0.00a 1.05b 8.48b ANOVA; b Chi Square Children’s mean age was 35 months at the for either 3 or 6 months. Sessions were con- start of intervention and 32% were boys. On average children were adopted at 13 months and had lived with their parents for 22 months. The children were racially diverse, including Caucasian (36%), African-American (14.5%), Hispanic (14.5%) and Asian (25%). Sixty seven percent were international adoptees and 57% had resided in orphanages. A trickle process randomization procedure was used to assign subjects to treatment intensity groups. The only significant group difference (See Table 1) was children’s race: the three-month group included more African-American and the six-month group more Hispanic children. At the onset of intervention treatment groups did not differ in terms of mothers’ interactive style (Table 2), parenting stress (Table 3), as well as children’s development (Table 4) and social emotional functioning (Table 5). Procedures Subjects received weekly parentchild intervention sessions based upon the RT curriculum (Mahoney & MacDonald, 2007) ducted by RT certified interventionists and were provided either in family’s homes or at a center based facility. Each session lasted approximately 60 minutes. RT is organized around 16 pivotal behaviors that are used to enhance children’s cognitive, communication and social emotional functioning (see www.Responsiveteaching. org for a more detailed description). During each session interventionists discussed the pivotal behaviors that were the objectives for the child; introduced one or two RI strategies to promote these behaviors; demonstrated the strategies; coached parents in their use the strategies: and developed a plan for parent follow through. Data Collection Parent-child observations were collected at baseline, 3 and 6 months. All other child and family assessments were collected at baseline and 12 months. Because some children were older than 42 months at pre- assessment, two different tests were used to assess developmental ages (DA): the Bayley Scales of Infant Development and the Battelle Developmental Inventory. DAs were converted to ratio developmental quotients (e.g. DA/CA x 100). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd Edition (Bayley, 1993) were used to assess children less than 42 months. The Bayley is considered to be one of the best measures of early general development. Average interrater reliabilities are .88, and test-retest reliabilities exceed .90. Predictive validity studies indicate that 2-year old Bayley scores are highly associated with preschool IQ scores. The Battelle Developmental Inventory (Newborg, Stock, Wnek, Guidbaldi, & Svinicki, 1984) was used to assess children who were 42 months or older. The Battelle is an individually administered assessment for children up to 8 years of age. It measures five developmental domains: personal-social, adaptive, motor, communication, and cognition. It was standardized on a nationally representative sample of children and has high test-retest and interrater reliability. Battelle developmental quotients are highly correlated with the Bayley developmental indices (rs = .81 to .90) (Newborg, et. al., 1984). The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, 2nd Edition (Sparrow, Cicchetti & Balla, 2005) was also used to assess children’s communication. This parent report instrument yields estimates of children’s functioning across four domains: Communication; Daily Living; Socialization; and Adaptive Behavior. Correlations between the Vineland and other adaptive behavior and intelligence tests range from .40 to.70 (Sparrow, et. al., 2005). Two scales were used to assess children’s social emotional functioning: the Child Behavior Checklist (1½-5) (CBCL) and the Temperament and Atypical Behavior Scale (TABS). The Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1.5 to 5 years [CBCL/1.5–5 (Achenbach &Rescorla, 2000)] is a revision of the 1992 checklist for children age 2–3 years [CBCL/2–3 (Achenbach, 1992)] and was normed on a national sample of children. It yields three normative subscales scores: Internalizing, Externalizing, and Total Problems. The Temperament and Atypical Behavior Scale (TABS) assesses problem behaviors of children between one to six years of age. This parent respondent instrument assesses four factors: Detached, Hypersensitivity/activity, Under-reactive, and Dysregulated. The corrected split-half reliability for the TABS is .95 for children with disabilities. Children and mothers were video recorded playing together for seven-minutes with a standard set of developmentally appropriate toys. Mothers’ style of interaction was rated from these videotapes with the Maternal Behavior Rating Scale (MBRS) (Mahoney, Powell, & Finger, 1986; Mahoney, 1992). The MBRS is a twelve item scale that assesses four interactive dimensions: Responsiveness, Affect, Achievement Orientation, and Defectiveness’. Research indicates that the MBRS assesses parenting characteristics associated with children’s development; that ratings on the scale are stable over time for parents not involved in parenting interventions (Mahoney & Bella, 1998); and that it is sensitive to interactive changes promoted through parent mediated interventions (Mahoney & Powell, 1988; Mahoney, Boyce, Fewell, Spiker, & Wheeden, 1998). The Parenting Stress Inventory-Short Form [PSI (Abidin, 1995 - 3rd Ed.)] was ad- 73 ministered to assess the psychological status of mothers. The PSI is a 36 item self-report questionnaire that has three subscales (parental distress, dysfunctional parent-child interaction and difficult child) and a Total Stress Index. Internal consistency for the Total Stress Index is .91, and for the subscales are: (a) .87 for Parental Distress; (b) .80 for Dysfunctional Parent-Child Interaction; and (c) .85 for Difficult Child (Abidin, 1995). Responsive Teaching Intervention Service Logs were used to document the services that each family received. After each intervention session, service providers completed logs indicating: (a) the strategies discussed during each intervention session; (b) Intervention Topics; (c) follow-up activities mothers were asked to implement at home; and (d) mothers’ follow-through with suggestions from the previous week. Coding and Reliability of the Maternal Behavior Rating Scale. Each mother-child video was coded independently by raters who had received 40 hours of training and had attained 80% agreement within one point on a five-point Likert scale. Pre- and post- intervention observations were coded at the same time to avoid rating drift. Observations were randomly sorted so that pre- and post- observations were counterbalanced and were not coded consecutively for any dyad. A second rater coded 20% of all observations to assess reliability. Interrater reliability as estimated with the Spearman correlation was .81. Raters attained 72% exact agreement and 99% agreement within one scale point. Results: Comparisons of Treatment Groups For the following Treatment Group comparisons, a repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to examine the effects of Time (Intervention) and Time x Groups to compare intervention changes for the two groups . ANOVAs were used to explore these effects for individual scales or subscales. Intervention Effects on Mothers` Interactive Style. MBRS ratings from baseline, 3 and 6 months are presented in Table 2. At the beginning of intervention, mothers had average ratings on MBRS subscales that clustered near the midpoint. Table 2. Intervention Changes in Mother’s Style of Interaction 3 Month Group Variables Pre M 6 Month Group 3 SD M SD 6 Pre 3 M SD M SD M SD F F (Time X Partial (Time) Treatment Eta2 6 M SD Maternal Behavior Rating Scale 0.49 .42 Responsivea 2.9 0.5 3.3 0.7 3.0 0.5 3.1 0.7 3.7 0.6 3.6 0.7 6.21** 0.55 .19 Affecta 2.9 0.3 3.0 0.5 2.9 0.5 2.9 0.4 3.2 0.3 3.1 0.5 1.65 0.34 .06 Achievement/ 2.9 0.5 2.7 0.3 2.6 0.4 2.7 0.5 2.5 0.3 Directivea 2.5 0.4 2.44 0.30 .09 a 74 2.51* MBRS Subscales: *p<0.05, **p<0.01 Results for the MANOVA were significant for Time (p<. 05) but not for Time x Groups (p>.05). Univariate analyses indicated that the effects of Time were significant for responsiveness (p < .01) but not the other two subscales. Eta square indicated that the effect size for responsiveness was in the large range. Post hoc comparisons indicated that for both groups responsiveness was significantly greater at 3 months than at baseline (t = 4.01, p < .001) while differences between responsiveness at baseline and 6 months were not significant (t = 1.95, p > .05). A between subjects ANOVA was used to further explore group differences in responsiveness at 3 and 6 months, controlling responsiveness at baseline. Results indicated that group differences were not significant at 3 months (F = 1.62, p > .05) but were significant at 6 months (F = 6.03, p < .05). As depicted on Figure 1, responsiveness declined from three to six months for mothers in the 3 month group but remained stable for the 6 month group. Intervention Effects on Mother’s Psychological Status. Table 3 presents Parenting Stress Index scores. While mean total stress scores were within the normal range for both groups at baseline and 12 months, 22% of the sample had clinically significant scores at baseline and only 9% had clinical scores at the 12 month follow-up. Results from the MANOVA indicated that the effects for Time and Time X Group were not significant (ps> .05). However, univariate analyses indicated significant Time effects on two subscales, parental distress and dysfunctional parent-child interaction, as well as for total stress (ps < .05) indicating decreases in parenting stress. Table 3. Intervention changes in mothers’ psychosocial functioning 3 Month Variables Pre M 6 Months Post SD M SD Pre M SD Post M F F Time X Time Treatment SD Parenting Stress IndexPSI Partial Eta2 Time 2.01 1.32 .24 Distress 24.7 7.0 21.0 4.2 24.8 8.2 23.4 4.8 4.07* 0.86 .16 PC 20.8 8.0 17.2 4.8 22.4 6.9 20.1 6.2 6.41* 0.30 .23 Diff child 24.6 8.8 24.7 7.1 30.9 6.7 27.3 8.3 1.53 1.57 .07 Total Stress 70.1 21.8 62.8 12.8 78.1 16.1 70.8 16.7 5.41* 0.01 .21 CES-D 9.2 8.5 0.12 .13 P<0.05; **P<0.01, * *** 6.6 2.9 11.5 8.5 8.5 5.0 2.96 P<0.001 75 Intervention Effects on Child Development. Table 4 reports pre- post scores on both Bayley/Battelle Developmental Quotients (DQ) and the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. On both scales children were in the low average range at baseline and increased to the average range at the 12 month observation. Twenty one percent of the children had Bayley/Battelle Developmental Quotients that were 75 or lower at baseline and only 5% had scores in this range at the at 12 months. Similar percentages of children had DQs t 75 or lower at baseline and 12 months on the Vineland. Analyses of Bayley/Battelle DQs indicated a significant effect for Time (p < .01) indicating that 12 month DQs were greater than baseline DQs. However, the Time X Treatment effect (p > .05) was not significant. Analyses of the Vineland also indicated significant effects for Time (p < .01) but not Time X Treatment (p > .05). Univariate analysis indicated that communication, daily living and adaptive behavior DQs were higher at 12 months than at baseline (ps < .01). Eta squares for both instruments indicated large effect sizes for intervention changes over time. Table 4. Intervention Changes in Child Development Variables Developmental Quotienta Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale 3 Month M SD M 6 Months SD M 93.4 14.1 101.4 12.8 89.8 SD M SD 20.2 96.8 19.9 F (Time) F (Time X Partial Treatment Eta2 7.71** 0.34 .28 3.82* 0.46 .40 Communi84.4 13.5 99.8 cation 14.7 85.6 19.5 92.5 15.8 13.71*** 1.81 .35 Daily Living 80.5 9.6 14.1 77.2 15.0 83.5 10.5 12.32** 0.20 .32 Social 84.2 10.9 88.9 8.7 81.9 14.9 86.4 11.8 2.25 0.72 .08 Adaptive behavior 79.9 10.9 92.7 13.8 78.4 17.1 85.2 14.0 10.68** 0.98 .29 a 89.3 Ratio Developmental Quotient computed from Bayley or Battelle developmental ages; P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001 * Intervention Effects on Children’s Social Emotional Functioning. Table 5 presents prepost measures of children’s social emotional functioning both for the CBCL and the TABS. Average scores on both instruments were within the normal range for both instruments at baseline and 12 months. However, while 21% of the children had Total TABS scores 76 that were in the clinical range at baseline, only 8% had clinical scores at 12 months. The overall MANOVA for the CBCL yielded significant effects for Time (p < .001) but not for Time X Treatment (p > .05). Univariate analyses indicated that 12 month scores were significantly lower on each of three subtests (ps < .001). Intervention effect sizes were in the large range on all measures, indicating substantial improvement in socialemotional functioning. Results for the TABS paralleled those for the CBCL. The overall MANOVA indicated a significant effect for Time (p < .01) but not Time X Treatment (p > .05). However, univariate analyses indicated that Time effects were significant for only two of the four subscales: Hypersensitivity/Activity (p < .001) and Dysregulation (p < .05). Pre- post differences were also significant for the Total TABS score (p < .001). Intervention effect sizes on the TABS were in the moderate range. Factors Associated with RT Intervention Effects Table 5. Intervention Changes in Children’s Social Emotional Functioning F Time Partial 3 Months 6 Months F X Eta2 Variables Pre Post Pre Post Time Treat Time ment M SD M SD M SD M SD CBCLa Internalizing Externalizing Total Problems TABS b Detach Hypersensitivity Underreactive Dysregulation TABS Total a 14.40*** 1.39 .64 49.9 12.4 43.5 11.6 55.4 10.0 46.4 9.5 46.24*** 1.25 .64 52.7 10.6 44.6 11.6 54.9 9.4 48.7 6.7 21.54*** 0.39 .45 52.4 11.4 44.1 11.4 56.1 9.7 48.0 7.4 32.92*** 0.01 .56 1.03 0.11 .61 .10 39.1 21.9 45.6 16.3 5.63** 41.6 16.3 45.9 16.0 2.56 42.8 16.0 48.0 11.7 37.3 11.9 50.9 7.2 14.66*** 2.95 .40 47.6 12.1 50.8 10.6 49.5 7.6 53.1 3.3 2.96 0.01 .12 42.6 16.4 47.3 11.7 41.5 15.4 51.2 5.3 6.97* 0.84 .24 84.3 28.0 95.0 19.9 79.7 22.1 99.2 16.6 13.05*** Child Behavior Checklist; b Temperament and Atypical Behavior Scale; Two sets of hierarchical regression analyses were used to explore factors associated with intervention outcomes. The first set explored the effects of children’s age at adoption and the amount of time they lived with their adoptive parents; while the second set explored how children’s status as a domestic versus international adoptee was associated with intervention outcomes. Both sets of analyses were conducted for each of the dependent variables reported in the previous section. For each regression model the depen- * 1.09 P<0.05; **P<0.01, .37 P<0.00 *** dent variable (e.g., Time 2 Outcome) was examined with a hierarchical regression model. The first step included the Outcome at Time 1. The second set included the variables of interest: age at adoption and time with parents for the first set of analyses; international vs. domestic adoptee for the second set of analysis. Results from the first set of analyses indicated that neither children’s age at adoption nor time with adoptive parents was associated with any of the dependent variables. The 77 one exception was that Vineland Communication DQs at Time 2 were negatively associated with the amount of time children lived with their adoptive parents (Beta = -.36; t = 2.04, p < .05). The second set of analyses indicated that intervention changes on most variables, including mothers’ interaction style, parenting stress, and children’s social emotional functioning as assessed both by the TABS and CBCL were unrelated to children’s status as an international or domestic adoptee. However as reported on Table 6 international adoptees made greater developmental improvements on the Bayley/Battelle and Vineland than domestic adoptees. This effect was significant for every developmental measure with the exception of the Vineland Daily Living Scale. Table 6. Relationship of domestic versus international adoption status to child development outcomes Dependent Variable Model Beta T Value DevelopmenDevelopmental tal Quotient T1 Quotient T2a .730 4.77 .000 .53** .343 2.52 .021 .61** .539 3.27 .003 .21** .408 2.77 . 011 .41** .487 4.81 .009 .21** .299 2.91 . 007 .27** .598 3.80 .001 .33** .283 1.88 . 072 .39** .510 3.02 .006 23** .453 3.04 . .005 .47*** Adoption Status Vineland Communica- Vineland Communication tion T1 T2b Adoption Status1 Vineland Social Vineland Social T1 T2 b Adoption Status1 Vineland DaiVineland Daily ly Living ing T1 T2 b Liv- Adoption Status1 Vineland Vineland Adaptive BeAdaptive BehaviorT1 havior T2 b Adoption Status1 Significance R2 R2 Change .08 .20 .06 .06 .24 Bayley/Battelle Ratio Developmental Quotients; b Vineland Developmental Quotients; 1Adoption Status: 1= Domestic, 2= International *p < .1; ** p < .05; *** p<.01 a 78 Discussion This pilot investigation was designed to assess the feasibility of Responsive Teaching as developmental intervention for young adopted children and their parents. Because there was no control group, the degree to which the intervention outcomes were greater than might have occurred with parents and children who received no intervention services cannot be determined. However, the results generally paralleled those reported from more rigorous evaluations of RT with other populations of parents and young children. Most notable were improvements in mothers’ responsiveness as well as children’s development and social emotional functioning, each of which resulted in large effect sizes similar to what has been reported in previous RT evaluations. In addition there was a small decrease in parenting stress which had not been reported in previous studies. This study was designed to explore three issues that have practical implications for providing early interventions services to young adopted children and families. In particular, we examined how factors such as treatment intensity as well as the age of children at the onset of intervention and/or the amount of time they had lived with their parents might affect intervention outcomes. We had expected that intervention outcomes would be greater for parents and children who participated longer in intervention. Yet, similar to findings from the attachment based intervention literature (van IJzendoorn, et. al., 2005) nearly all intervention effects appeared to be as robust for low versus high treatment intensity groups. The one exception was intervention changes in responsive- ness. Although both groups made comparable improvements in responsiveness which plateaued at the three-month observation, low treatment intensity mothers decreased their responsiveness from three to six months while high- treatment intensity mothers maintained their level of responsiveness. In so far as maternal responsiveness is causally related to children’s development, it was surprising that group differences in responsiveness did not affect children’s developmental outcomes. Perhaps, the drift in responsiveness for low treatment intensity mothers is a factor that has long-term as versus short term implications. Although short term attachment interventions have produced changes in parenting sensitivity that have sustained from 12 to 18 months, long-term follow-up studies have reported that these early effects do not sustain over time (van IJzendoorn, et. al., 2005). Thus while low treatment intensity interventions may produce short-term child outcomes that are comparable to those observed in more intensive interventions as reported in this study, more intensive, or longer term, interventions may reinforce mothers’ responsiveness thus having a greater probability of enhanced child effects over time. In addition, while attachment based interventions have been reported to be less effective for children younger than 12 months versus older children (van den Dries, et. al., 2009), no age effects were observed in this study. This failure to find age effects may be associated with the fact that our sample consisted predominately of children older than 12 months of age as well as the fact that measures of attachment which may be highly sensitive 79 to age effects were not used as outcome measures. Nonetheless, the child intervention effects observed in this study were quite robust and clinically significant, mitigating concerns about age effects that were not identified in this study. We had expected that the less time children lived with their parents the less likely their parents would have developed habitual patterns of interacting with them. As a result, we reasoned that parents of newly adopted children regardless of their children’s age would be more amenable to suggestions to modify their style of interaction than parents who had lived longer with their children. However, we found no evidence to support this. RT appeared to be as effective with parents of newly adopted children as with parents whose children who had live with them for even more than two years. Finally, we had expected that international adoptees would benefit more from participation in the intervention than adopted children. This had been reported in previous attachment based intervention studies (Klein-Velderman, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, & van IJzendoorn, 2006) and was thought to be associated with international adoptees having more negative emotions (Belsky, 2005). Although international adoptees made greater developmental improvements than domestic adoptees in this study, the lack of group differences in children’s social emotional functioning suggests that this had little to do with international adoptees having more negative emotions than domestic adoptees. Rather this effect appeared to be associated with the fact that although group differences in communication skills were not significant at the 80 onset of intervention, many of the international adoptees in this sample had been recently adopted and had limited English skills at the onset of intervention. This may have accounted for their making greater progress in their communication development than domestic adoptees, and may have also contributed their advantages on other developmental assessments as well. In conclusion, results from this study suggest that RT is a feasible intervention both for promoting maternal responsiveness as well as enhancing the development and social emotional functioning of young adopted children. However, because of the quasi-experimental research design as well as the small sample used for this evaluation, results from this study can only be interpreted as “promising” and clearly do not support the efficacy of RT. Nonetheless, because results observed in this study were robust and clinically significant as well as similar to findings from more rigorous evaluations of RT, there is a strong need to examine how these results would hold up with more rigorous research designs that include larger and more diverse samples of parents and adopted children. Both the high and low intensity treatment models that were evaluated in this study were more intense than typically provided in attachment-based interventions. Yet, the finding that the three month intervention was nearly as effective as the six month intervention has major practical implications, especially since public funding for post-adoption services is limited. However, on a more cautionary note, results indicating that parents who received 3 versus 6 months of intervention began to decline in responsiveness 3 months after their intervention was completed is a concern that needs to be addressed. Given the likelihood that parental responsiveness is the main factor affecting children’s development and social emotional functioning, this finding points to the need of RT, and perhaps other attachment-based interventions, to develop mechanisms for reinforcing parental responsiveness after intervention services have ended. Finally, the widespread use of intervention models in community based practice is partly dependent upon the capacity of these models to address the needs of the populations of parents and children that are typically encountered. Children can be adopted at any age, not just under 12 months of age; many parents do not seek developmental services until long after they have adopted their child; and most agencies serve a mix of domestic and international adoptees. Results from this analysis indicated that none of these factors were associated with intervention outcomes for parents or children. In addition, as also occurs in practice, several of the children in this study had developmental, social emotional and quite possibly attachment problems at the onset of intervention, but none of these conditions appeared to influence the effectiveness of this intervention. In general, the results from this pilot investigation underscore the viability of RT as community based option for providing developmental service to preschool adoptees of all ages and ability levels regardless of the amount of time they have lived with their parents and whether they are international or domestic adoptees. References Abidin, R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index, 3rd Edition. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Achenbach, T.M., & Rescorla, L.A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd edition). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Bagnato, S., Neisworth, J., Salvia, J., Hunt, F. (1999). Temperament and Atypical Behavior Scale. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H.; Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195-215. Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2003). Problem behavior of internationally adopted adolescents: A review & meta-analysis. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 22, 64–77. Bramlett, M.D., Radel, L.F., & Blumberg, S.J. (2007). The health and well-being of adopted children. Pediatrics, 119, S54-S60. Edelmann, RJ. and Connolly, KJ. (1986). Psychological aspects of infertility. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 59, 209219. 81 Gindis, B. (2005). Cognitive, language and educational issues of children adopted from overseas orphanages. Journal of Cognitive Education & Psychology, 4, 290–315. Howe, David, D. (2003). Disorders of attachment in adopted and fostered children: Recognition and treatment. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 369-387. Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A meta-analysis. JAMA – The Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2501−2515. Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A meta-analysis. JAMA – The Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2501−2515. Juffer, F., Hoksbergen, R. A. C., RiksenWalraven, J. M. A., & Kohnstamm, G. A. (1997). Early intervention in adoptive families: Supporting maternal sensitive responsiveness, infant–mother attachment, and infant competence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,38, 1039–105. Karaslan, O. Diken, I., & Mahoney, G. (2013). A randomized control study of Responsive Teaching with young Turkish Children and their mothers. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 33, 18-27. Karaslan, O. & Mahoney, G. (2013). Effectiveness of Responsive Teaching with children with Down syndrome. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 51, 458–469. 82 Klein-Velderman, M., BakermansKranenburg, M. J Juffer, F., & van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2006). Effects of attachmentbased interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant attachment: Differential susceptibility of highly reactive infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 266–274. Keyes, M.A., Sharma, A., Elkins, I.J., Iacono, W.G. & McGue, M. (2008). The mental health of US adolescents adopted in infancy. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 162, 419-423. Levy-Shiff, R., Goldschmidt, I., Har-Even, D. (1991) Transition to parenthood in adoptive families. Developmental Psychology, 27, 131–140. Mahoney, G. (1988). Communication patterns between mothers and developmentally delayed infants. First Language, 8, 157-172. Mahoney, G. (1999). Maternal Behavior Rating Scale (Revised). Tallmadge, Ohio: Family Child Learning Center. Mahoney, G. & Bella, J. (1998). An examination of the effects of family-centered early intervention on child and family outcomes. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18, 83-94. Mahoney, G., Boyce, G., Fewell, R., Spiker, D. & Wheeden, C.A. (1998). The relationship of parent-child interaction to the effectiveness of early intervention services for at-risk children and children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18, 5-17. Mahoney, G., Boyce, G., Fewell, R. R., Spiker, D., & Wheeden, C. A. (1998). The relationship of parent-child interaction to the effectiveness of early intervention services for at-risk children and children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18 (1), 5-17. Mahoney, G., & MacDonald, J. (2007). Autism and developmental delays in young children: The Responsive Teaching curriculum for parents and professionals. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Mahoney, G. & Perales, F. (2005). A comparison of the impact of relationship-focused intervention on young children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders and other disabilities. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(2), 77-85. Mahoney, G. & Perales, F. (2003). Using relationship-focused intervention to enhance the social-emotional functioning of young children with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23 (2), 77-89. Mahoney, G., & Powell, A. (1988). Modifying parent-child interaction: Enhancing the development of handicapped children. Journal of Special Education, 22, 82-96. Mahoney, G., Powell, A. & Finger, I. (1986). The Maternal Behavior Rating Scale. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 6, 44-56. Mahoney, G., & Wheeden, C. A. (1998). Effects of teacher style on the engagement of preschool aged children with special learning needs. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 2, 293–315. Marcovitch, S., Goldberg, S., Gold, A., Washington, J., Wasson, C, Krekewich, K., & Handley-Derry, M. (1997). Determinants of behavioural problems in Romanian children adopted in Ontario. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 17-31. Newborg, J., Stock, J. R., Wnek, L., Guidubaldi, J., & Svinicki ,J. (1984). The Battelle Developmental Inventory. Allen, TX: DLM/Teaching Resources. O’Connor, T. G., & Rutter, M. (2000). Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 703–711. Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 32, 77–89. Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1691–1704. Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales - 2nd Edition manual. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc. 83 Stams, G. J. J. M., Juffer, F., Rispens, J., & Hoksbergen, R. A. C. (2000). The development and adjustment of 7-year old children adopted in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 1025–1037. Stams, G. J., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Hoksbergen, R. A. C. (2001). Attachment based intervention in adoptive families in infancy and children’s development at age 7: Two follow-up studies. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 159–180. van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., & Poelhuis, C. W. K. (2005). Adoption and cognitive development: A meta-analytic comparison of adopted and nonadopted children’s IQ and school performance. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 301–316. van IJzendoorn, M. H., BakermansKranenburg, M. J., & Juffer, F. (2005). Why less is more: From the dodo bird verdict to 84 evidence-based interventions on sensitivity and early attachments. In L. J. Berlin, Y. Ziv, L. Amaya-Jackson & M. T. Greenberg (Eds.), Enhancing early attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy (pp. 297– 312). New York, NY: Guilford. van den Dries, L. Juffer, F. van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J. (2009). Fostering security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 410-421 Welsh, J., Andres G., Viana A., Petrill, S, Mathias, M. (2007). Interventions for Internationally Adopted Children and Families: A Review of the Literature. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 24, 285-311. Wierzbicki, M. (1993). Psychological adjustment of adoptees: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 447– 454. RESILIENT LIVES AND AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL SUGGESTIONS ITALIAN NATIONAL TRAINING PROCESS IN THE FIELD OF INTERCOUNTRY ADOPTION Giorgio MACARIO1 Abstract This report will consider the crossroad between two virtuous factors. On one hand, the multi-factorial and interdisciplinary approach that increasingly characterizes the concept of resilience today. On the other hand the contributions of autobiographical method that enhances the history of the individual in the family and in different contexts where they belong, contributing to the spread of the ‘turning narrative’ in society and emphasizing the value of writing. The area is referring to the training for inter-country adoptions in Italy, continuously active since 2001, and we will try to find a shared area between resilient lives and autobiographical suggestions, that tell us about the person but at the same time helps us to understand the society that surrounds it. Keywords: autobiography, resilience, writing, international adoptions, narratives. 1 Trainer and psychosociologist, (ITALY). E-mail: macario.g@gmail.com 85 1 Introduction. about adoptions, resilience and autobiography “A scarecrow tries not to think about. It’s too painful to build an inner world full of atrocious performances. You will suffer less when you have wood in place of the heart and straw under the hat. Is enough, however, that a scarecrow encounters a man alive that infuse a soul, because it is, again, tempted by the pain of living. (...) Then, the scarecrow begins to speak again and sometimes even write his own autobiographical chimera. “ (Cyrulnik, 2009)2 The identification of a plural category defined by Boris Cyrulnik of ‘hidden children’, which opens in-depth analysis of the world of adoption as non-traumatic factor, represents the starting point of a interdisciplinary reflection. The subject of resilience, or better the resilient lives, is extremely important for the world of adoptions and, particularly, for inter-country adoptions. Cyrulnik confirms this approach when he says: “The adoption is not a trauma: indeed, it is a meeting that allows evolution resilient.” (Cyrulnik, 2009) On the other hand, the autobiographical suggestions, always present in the adoptive paths, are spreading in a growing number of contexts, which apply an autobiographical approach3, (Regione Piemonte-ARAI (2009) oriented to the construction of a autobiographical method. The confirmation of the possibility that adoptive experience could usefully contribute to implementation of the autobiographThe review of the book of Boris Cyrulnik (2009), by the author, is available at: http://www.lua.it/index.php?option=com_content&tas k=view&id=1423&Itemid=77 3 Among the various significant initiatives on this subject that have been made throughout the country: -the project of Adoption Center of ASL Province Milan 1 on the value of the adoptive tell stories (“C’era una volta” - Seminar on 22 October 2008); -the regional project of Regione Piemonte-ARAI “Racconto di me: il valore della narrazione fra genitori e figli adottivi” 2 86 ical method comes from the most national expert autobiography and a leading expert at the European level, Duccio Demetrio. Demetrio, in his speech at the European Conference in 2010,4 identifies the adoption as autobiographical transition and, in this regard specific that: “...the representation of the adoptive event, can better be defined thanks to narrative attentions, or better, narrative, since adoption, and not only that, it is a story generating life stories to meet, re-examine, re-organize and forget too.” (Demetrio, 2013) 2 Writing, autobiography, resilience focused on adoption Comparison with our own adoptive history begins in the early days, of course, but it does not end with the attainment of adulthood. Adulthood involves, as also indicates Marco Chistolini, a re-signification of its own adoptive history gaining greater self-awareness, particularly with respect to their origins (Chistolini, 2010). The search for information on the origins, has became during last years the center of the attention in Italy5 Pregliasco, 2013), and the subject of recent significant legal pronouncements6, makes possible to take action on the part of the adopted person inquiring about his past and possibly establish a contact with the Commission for intercountry adoption, Resilience and autobiographical approach in intercountry adoptions’, Florence, 8-9 june 2010. 5 In addition to the recent specialized training conducted by the Commission for intercountry adoption in 2012, entitled “L’accesso alle informazioni sulle origini nelle adozioni internazionali: Aspetti giuridici, psicosociali, relazionali.”, deserves to be mentioned the most recent book of the Istituto degli Innocenti: Pregliasco, R. (2013) (a cura di), Alla ricerca delle proprie origini. Carocci Editore). 6 See the recent pronouncement of the Constitutional Court, judgment of 22 November 18, 2013, n. 278 declaring the unconstitutionality of article 28, paragraph 7, of the Law of 4 May 1983, no. 184, in the part that does not allow the court to consult again the mother who has refused to recognize the child, even many years after. 4 biological parents. In Italy, there are very few people active in this kind of search but the possibility of being able to start has to be considering a factor of resilience. The autobiography, on the other hand, is considered as an experience of re-identification, and Demetrio tells us “The autobiographical writing is nothing more than the re-writing of our history; it goes back into the scene, we again becomes, somehow, protagonists and characters of our life, we are back into the game as if you go up on a stage.” (Demetrio, 2010) The autobiographical writing is considered a useful tool to be used in the field of adoption, however, not only with adults (Mazzonis, 2010). The autobiographical writing is also considered a clinical tool in an autobiographical perspective but different from clinical therapeutic perspective (Demetrio, 2008). Adoption generates multiple autobiographical stories, but they are just the different possibilities of writing that Demetrio enhances when he says: “There is always something more in a script.” (Demetrio, 2013). Firstly, writing is a valuable opportunity for the author to find himself considering explicit and implicit areas influenced by the unconscious. Second, are those who have responsibilities for education, parenting, guidance or treatment that can identify more signs of comfort or discomfort of the narrator: the motivation to writing, or running away from this, allow a better understanding of the levels of self-consideration expressed by the adopted person. If we consider the writing a sort of additional adoptive parent, we can define an experience in itself potentially ‘resilient’, which helps us to resist the wounds of life. Recognizing the central importance of the contribution of Cyrulnik about adoption as a possibility of a resilient evolution, Demetrio underlines that writing can facilitate the emergence in the adoptee of an ‘internal biographer’. The internal biographer, in fact, may be central for the mental health of the adoptive person because places himself at the center as a major character of the story, a ‘clerk’ at the service of its author. This specific factor,-characterized at the same time by storytelling and writing- is not resilient only because it makes the person self-sufficient. It becomes central because it amplifies the role of the ‘facilitator of storytelling’ (which could be defined as ‘external biographer’), both for the parent that attend together with the adoptee at his everyday life, and for the operator that assists and supports its growth path. However, the author who has been adopted and who writes about himself trying to observe better, provides the best narrative synthesis that can help to understand the close interconnection between the stated various factors. Here is what Daniele Callini tells us in his book entitled “44 steps” (Callini, 2006): “The rigor and method I’ve learned from Dad Franco. (...) The speed and instinct decision-making are a resource of Mamma Cristina (adoptive parents). (...) I have no doubt saying that I have assimilated the courage of my mother. Moreover it requires an enormous courage to give birth to a child that you do not want or you are not able to keep. The tenacity, strength and endurance, I think I have received them from my blood father. Even if I do not know who he is, I feel that these resources are a gift from him.” (Macario, 2010) A summary ‘masterful’, certainly resilient. 87 3 Resilience and training of operators for intercountry adoptions “A good education for resilience requires the establishment of programs and projects that facilitate the child, in his family and in the community, building solidarity networks and the opening of a space capable of facilitating a positive development. The prospect of integration between the various professionals underlies the assumption that considers the person as a whole not parceled depending on the context or on the historical moment in which it appears.” (Cyrulnik, Malaguti, 2005). This passage, taken from the introduction to the cited volume, edited by Cyrulnik and Malaguti, can significantly represent at least two of the milestones of the training activities for inter-country adoptions, made in the last 15 years in Italy by the Commission for Intercountry Adoptions in collaboration with the Istituto degli Innocenti in Florence.7 The formation activities organized for the operators involved in international adoptions in Italy (200 to 300 for each year) has always sought to combine the best reading of the changing environment and tools that can be used for an enhancement of the ‘natural powers’ of adoptive couples, adoptive parents and adopted children of the same time. As it says Franca Olivetti Manoukian: “Everyone who lives and coexists in a social context is asked to share the guidelines which this discomfort should be addressed with. Operators are more competent than others simply because they have available elements The author was responsible for scientific and training activities, launched in 2001 and which continues until now. The documentation of the formative work carried out is contained in 8 volumes of the series Studies and Research (numbers 1, 4, 7, 10, 15, 17, 18, 20) for free download at the website of the CAI: http://www.commissioneadozioni.it/it/bibliografia/ studi-e-ricerche.aspx 7 88 of knowledge and experience for identifying and for guiding how to treat the discomfort.” (Olivetti Manoukian, 2011). The perspective of empowerment not only connected to the professional aspects but also to educational parenting, has in a cross concern a lot of topics discussed during many years. Between this topic: intercultural aspects, specificity of adolescence, scholastic inclusion in the post-adoption - where ample space is given to the possible interpretations of the ‘new resilient development ‘ theorized by Cyrulnik (Favaro, 2012) - adoptive sib ships, professional and self-help groups to the assessment of the couple and special needs. Regarding the assessment of the couples -to give just one example, very significant and in line with other European studies - (Palacios, 2013) it was possible to deepen the evolution from a predominantly evaluative approach to an accompanying perspective during the entire adoption process. On the other hand, the integration between the different professional knowledge has always been a leitmotif of in-depth training, characterized as inter-professional training (psychologists, social workers, judges, the three most consistent professionalism), but also interdisciplinary (psychological, social, anthropological and legal) and inter-organizational (teams adoptions of public services, the authorized agencies, the juvenile courts and prosecutor’s offices, the organizations). The effort for empathizing with the problems of the adoptive couples and of the adopted children, and the subsequent identification of additional factors to promote resilience was then even more significant during the training experiences made in Eastern Eu- rope in 2004-2005 (Macario, 2005), and during joint training Brazil-Italy in 2009-2010 (Macario, 2011). A further authoritative confirmation of the central inter-professional aspects in the world of adoptions comes also from two Authors inspired to the systemic approach who state: “... the more you support the training for all those people (judges, social workers, counselors) who approach the families ‘dare to take’, the more it contributes to social welfare”( Scabini, Cigoli, 2010). The European Conference of 2010 on adoptions and resilience, mentioned above, has led to synthetize many other elements considered during the subsequent years. Along with the autobiographical suggestions more often we would like to understand the centrality of the resilient lives in order to better support the different contexts of growth of the adopted children. The multifactorial and interdisciplinary approach that characterizes resilience is becoming even more important inside the considerations of Elena Malaguti, who has analyzed the process of resilience not only in reference to the individual but also to groups (family, community. .), that, in particular difficult moments, must be able to put in place resources for reorganizing in a positive way his own path and life plan, according to an evolutionary approach. Resilience is spontaneous, but it implies stress to be activated, becoming an opportunity for personal and social evolution. There can be several theoretical models: cognitivebehavioral, medical, eco-systemic and clinical. It remains “a complex object that is by its nature not predictable, chaotic and does not meet the natural laws” that “does not work according to linear patterns but in a systemic and dynamic way,” “as a function of the interactions and transitions occurring stable between a person in permanent development and its environment.”8 The Lectio Magistralis of Beràstegui Pedro-Viejo (2013), finally, starting from the studies and research on inter-country adoption in the European context, has made a significant contribution to deepening the three fundamental concepts of risk, resilience and recovery. Some authors have questioned whether the adoption itself may represent a ‘risk factor’ for the problems of adaptation for a child already vulnerable by abandonment.9 (Beràstegui Pedro-Viejo, Gomez-Bengoechea, 2006; Schofield, Beek, 2013). The research, however, –Berastegui stated- shows that adopted children often exhibit behavioral problems, cognitive, social and educational, to a greater extent than their peers not adopted, but if you compare them with their peers institutionalized or other living situations of hardship the comparison is reversed into opposite parameters.10 (Brodzinsky, Palacios, 2011). Facing the risks and trauma in childhood, we can highlight the significant differences between protection (when there is no harm in situations of risk), reversibility or recovery (recovers the normal path), resilience (with the resumption of development path Malaguti, E., Research on resilience and prospects for inclusive education. Adoption and beyond, Abstract intervention at European Conference - Florence 8-9 June 2010. 9 For further information on post-adoption, cf. [19]. For further information on the theories of attachment applied to the field foster, of custody and residency, cf. [20]. 10 Very similar to the conclusions of two of the leading experts of adoptions worldwide (David Brodzinsky and Jesus Palacios) in their fundamental book on research and practices. Cf. ChildOnEurope, 2008) 8 89 that keeps track of the suffered pain) and executive functions (despite the problems, the child reaches a certain functionality in everyday life). As specific insights, the three themes explored in the national training during the previous year - the school integration in the post-adoption, the adoption of sibling groups and the specificity of adolescents in adoptionwere developed with a specific focus on resilience and autobiographical approach. 4Searching a shared area In conclusion, I would use a methodological cue usefully applied in the analysis of educational models to verify the assonance or the dissonance between ‘education’ and ‘therapy’. There is no doubt, in fact, that there are characteristics of the specific educational and there are others definitely attributable to the therapeutic area. It is also true that there is a common area where the identified characteristics apply both to education and therapy. And yet, some other features and tools that have a priority in the educational effectiveness sometimes facilitate therapeutic outcomes, while the contrary is true even when therapeutic interventions mainly involve non-negligible changes in education. The intention is not so much to make long lists, but to keep in mind, on a qualitative level, that there is a resilient approach (section 3rd) and autobiographical approach (section 2nd). Both of these fields of study and research have led to a particularly close relationship with the practice and to identify specific target -in this case the adoptive context- that can help to operate in a limited area, and then to compare those different fields of intervention. Alongside the adoptive area, for example, 90 it’s possible to cite the hospitality of residential boys and girls. An interesting reflection is the one developed by Andrea Canevaro on educating the resilience that weaves in various ways also autobiographical references (Canevaro, 2008). These are some references to the elements necessarily unsaturated and deepened, located in the shared area: • The EMPOWERMENT, like exploitation the potential of the individuals, certainly within their contexts; • The SELF-EMPOWERMENT, which allows the expression of new desires, opinions and possibilities; • The NARRATION, as a tool that gives meaning to personal paths (for the construction of their own path in life, to the search for origins, for overcoming traumatic experiences, etc.). • The CENTRALITY OF INDIVIDUALS, such as re-centering on the individual (both the adopted child as well as the adoptive parent) in the network of relationships and social context (current membership and, possibly, of origin); • The SELF-TRAINING, like equidistant perspective between self-centered and dependency, ‘vitality’ factor, the ability to capture the support and advice they need.11 I’ll entrust the closing, in line with the spirit of this contribution, to an adoptive parent a bit special, Leo Ortolani, creator of Ratman, a well-known Italian comic book character, which describes a path adopted that last nearly a decade. Some of the items shown are from a first systematization edited by the author in the section ‘The role of Training’ contained in the Guidelines on post-adoption services. Cf [23]. 11 Ortolani says, describing the flight to Colombia to get to know the two girls Lucy and Johanna: “And then I feel that I still do a lot of things that I did not just in time, I had to prepare myself better, that everything is going well suddenly, that 2001 was yesterday and you cannot do things so fast, I still need a bit ‘of time, a month, a few days, it cannot happen now, not me, not so, do not .... Passengers are requested to fasten their seat belts (...) we are landing at the airport in Cali.” (Ortolani, 2011). The effectiveness autobiographical-narrative interpreted in an humoristic - resilient way. References Beràstegui Pedro-Viejo, A., GomezBengoechea, B., (2006). Los retos de la postadopcion: balance y perspectivas., Universidad Pontificia Comillas. Beràstegui Pedro-Viejo, A., (2013). Il rischio, la ripresa e la resilienza nell’adozione internazionale: le lezioni della ricerca europea., in G. Macario, (a cura di), I percorsi formativi nelle adozioni internazionali. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 20, Istituto degli Innocenti, pp. 250-264. Brodzinsky, D.M., Palacios, J.(2011). Lavorare nell’adozione. Franco Angeli. (Edizione originale – Brodzinsky, D.M., Palacios, J.(2005), Psychological issues in adoption. Research and practice, Praeger Publisher. Callini, D., (2006). 44 passi. Tempo al Libro, pp. 125-126. Canevaro, A., (2008). I diritti dei bambini e delle bambine e abitare l’apprendimento. in G. Macario, Dall’istituto alla casa., Istituto degli Innocenti-Carocci Editore. ChildOnEurope (2008). Guidelines on Post-adoption Services, Istituto degli Innocenti. Chistolini, M., (2010). L’età adulta e la risignificazione della propria storia adottiva. In CIAI (a cura di Chistolini M. e Raymondi M.), Figli adottivi crescono, Franco Angeli Editore, pp. 104-115. Cyrulnik, B. (2009). Autobiografia di uno spaventapasseri. Raffaello Cortina Editore, p. 211. Cyrulnik B., Malaguti E.(2005) (a cura di). Costruire la resilienza. Erikson,, pp. 9-10. Demetrio, D. (2013). Scrivere l’adozione: resilienza e transizioni esistenziali in G. Macario. (a cura di), I percorsi formativi nelle adozioni internazionali. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 20, Istituto degli Innocenti, pp. 240-249. Demetrio, D., (2010). La rielaborazione della propria storia in età adulta: la scrittura autobiografica. in CIAI , op. cit., pp. 195-206. Demetrio, D. (2008). La scrittura clinica. Raffaello Cortina Editore. Favaro, G., (2012). Con cura, competenza, amore. L’inserimento scolastico dei bambini adottati, in G. Macario, I percorsi formativi 91 del 2009 nelle adozioni internazionali. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 17, Istituto degli Innocenti, pp. 27-44. Macario. G. (2005) (a cura di). L’operatore oltre frontiera. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 4, Istituto degli Innocenti. Macario. G. (2011) (a cura di). L’Italia e il Brasile per il benessere dell’infanzia nelle adozioni internazionali. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 15, Istituto degli Innocenti. Macario, G., I tempi dell’attesa: una sfida formativa per la qualità. in Macario. G. (2010) (a cura di), La qualità dell’attesa nell’adozione internazionale. Collana Studi e Ricerche della Commissione per le adozioni internazionali n. 10, Istituto degli Innocenti, pp. 3-18. Mazzonis, G., (2010). Il confronto con la propria storia in adolescenza. in CIAI , op. cit., pp. 85-103. Olivetti Manoukian, F. (2011). Ma il lavoro sociale che lavoro è? in Animazione Sociale n. 255, pp. 23-35. 92 Ortolani, L., (2011). Due figlie e altri feroci animali. Diario di un’adozione internazionale. Sperling & Kupfer, pagg. 18-19. Palacios, J., (2013). Manuale degli interventi professionali nell’adozione internazionale. Regione Emilia Romagna Quaderno n. 29. Edizione originale spagnola – Gobierno de Espana, Ministerio de Education, politica social Y deporte, J. Palacios, Manual para intervenciones profesionales en adopcion internacional, MEPSYD. Pregliasco, R. (2013) (a cura di). Alla ricerca delle proprie origini. Carocci Editore. Regione Piemonte-ARAI (2009). I quaderni del genitore adottivo n. 1, “Vite da raccontarsi”. Scabini, E., Cigoli, V.(2010). Il legame adottivo: una forma radicale di genitorialità, in R. Rosnati, (a cura di), Il legame adottivo. Contributi internazionali per la ricerca e l’intervento, Unicopli, pp. 17-34. Schofield, G., Beek, M., (2013). Adozione affido accoglienza. Raffaello Cortina Editore Edizione originale: Schofield, G., Beek, M., (2006), Attachment Handbook for Foster Care and Adoption. BAAF. TODAY’S CHILDREN ARE TOMORROW’S PARENTS INSTRUCTIONS FOR AUTHORS Short description of the journal The Journal Today’s Children are Tomorrow’s Parents (TCTP) started in Romania, in 1999. The journal is an useful resource of information for professionals working in the childhood area. Each issue of the Journal is based on a specific topic concerning the prevention of any kind of violence against the child. After more than 15 years of appearance, TCTP journal, arrived at 37-38 issue, included in the international data bases EBSCO, Index Copernicus, Norwegian Social Science Data (NSD), is bringing into the author’s attention few recommendations. Types of contributions: Papers will be considered providing that they have not previously been published or admitted simultaneously elsewhere for publication. Original, Theoretical, and Empirical Contributions: The paper should conform the APA (Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association) standards, with a legible abstracts of 100 to 280 words). Furthermore the paper should include a clear introductory statement of purpose; historical review when desirable; description of method and scope of observations; full presentation of results; brief comment/discussion on the significance of the findings and any correlation with others in the literature; section on speculation and relevance or implications; summary in brief which may include discussion. Section of references is required. Brief Communications: Shorter articles of 5 to 7 pages (abstracts and/or references optional). Articles on Clinical Practice: Authors should provide an account of previous clinical theory in an organized and up-to-date manner distinct from the clinical case material. Further, the clinical case material should occupy no more than a third of the paper. The first third should include only relevant background theory, while the final third should aim to discuss the descriptive presentation of the clinical case material against the background of existing theories and/or modifications needed to accommodate the clinical material. Invited Reviews: Plans for proposed reviews and invited in draft outline in the first instance. The editors will commission reviews on specific topics. Reviews submitted without invitation or prior approval will be returned. Submission Requirements: All submissions should include an abstract, and ordinarily be 15 to 20 pages in length, though occasionally longer papers are considered. In order to facilitate blind peer review, authors are encouraged to prepare a cover sheet that includes identifying details not included in the manuscript which will be sent out for review, less the cover sheet. E-mail submission to the Editor is preferred; please send an electronic copy of your manuscript to: anamuntean25@yahoo.com. All correspondence, including notification of the Editor-in-Chief’s decision and requests for revision takes place by e-mail. Manuscripts may be submitted in Romanian, French, or English and the author will be informed about the languages of the publica- 93 tion. The Romanian Authors will be asked for providing the translation into the language of publication. Authors are responsible for obtaining written permission from copyright owners to reprint any previously published material included in their article. The editors reserve the right to refuse any manuscript submitted, whether by invitation or otherwise, and to make suggestions and modifications before publication. Submitted papers must be in final form when submitted; manuscripts will be returned for reworking or retyping that do not conform to required style and format. Abstract: A structured abstract (objective, method, results, conclusions, practical implications) should not exceed 360 words in length covering the main factual points is required. Use complete sentences, and spell out acronyms at first mention. References: Style and formatting of bibliographic citations in the text and the reference section must adhere to the guidelines of APA (Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association). The Journal uses an alphabetical style rather than a numeric style both in the text and bibliography. No abbreviations of journal titles or use of et al. is permitted in the bibliography. Tables/Figures: Cite each table/figure clearly in text. Tables should be arranged one to a page with a self – contained title that is understandable without reference to the text. Figures should be computer generated one per page, with legends. Tables and figures should be submitted as separate files named Tables of Figures. Citations in text: Citations in text must match reference citations exactly. Groups of citations with the 94 text must be in alphabetical order within the group. List all authors the first time a work is cited unless there are six or more authors. In bibliography use et al. after the sixth author’s name and initial to indicate the remaining authors of the article. In text list the first author et al year (Jones et al., 2009) for first and subsequent citations. For citations with three or more authors, list all authors the first time the work is cited, then author et al. for citations thereafter. When two authors list both and do so every time reference are cited. If two or more citations with the same authors for the same year, list as many authors as needed to differentiate citations followed by et al. If two or more citations with exactly the same authors in exactly the same order for exactly the same year, use 2009a, 2009b etc. to clearly link text citations to correct reference citation. If reference is within parentheses, use ampersand. If not within parentheses, use and. Citations in the references: The use of et al. is not permitted in the reference section under any circumstances. Abbreviations of journal titles is not permitted. Please write out completely. Citations should be in correct alphabetical order. Watch punctuation closely, particularly in strings of authors (and initials), and in journal volumes, issues, and page numbers. Examples of citations in Reference section: Journal articles: Egeland, B. (2009). Taking stock: Childhood emotional and developmental psychopathology. Child Abuse and Neglect. Vol. 33, Nr 1, pp. 22-27 Authored books: Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlsson, E A. & Collins, W. A. (2005). The Development of the Person. The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaption from Birth to Adulthood. New York: The Guilford Press. Edited books: George, C. & Solomon, J. (2008). Attachment and caregiving behavioral system. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications.Second edition. New York: Guilford Press. Chapters in books: Cicchetti, D. & Valentino; K. (2006). An Ecological-Transactional Perspective on Child Maltreatment: Failure of the Average Expectable Environment and Its Influence on Child development I D. Cicchetti & D.,J. Cohen (Eds.). Developmental Psychopathology. Volume 3: Risk, Disorder and Adaptation. Second Edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. On-line citations: Ascione, F. R. (2001). Animal abuse and youth violence. Juvenile Justice Bulletin. Washington, DC; Departement of Justice. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Retrieved September 26, 2003 from http://www.ojjdp.ncjrs.org Presentations at conferences: Lundén, K. (2007). To identify children at risk for maltreatment Paper presented at the Second International Forum on Psychological safety, Resilience and Trauma, September 2007, Timisoara, Romania. 95 96 Parteneri: Organizator : Partener media: 25-26 septembrie 2014, București ADOPŢIA, O NOUĂ PERSPECTIVĂ Conferința națională pentru adopție