The Negative Stigma of Absinthe Analyzed by Qualitative Surveys in
Transcription
The Negative Stigma of Absinthe Analyzed by Qualitative Surveys in
The Negative Stigma of Absinthe Analyzed by Qualitative Surveys in Paris, France and East Lansing, Michigan, U.S.A By: Jason Mashni and Justin Thomas LBS 145 Cell and Molecular Biology Wednesday 7:30-10:30pm Katherine Diller and Joseph Maleszewski Due: August 31, 2007 Abstract Written by Justin Thomas Revised by Jason Mashni Absinthe is a bitter tasting green colored spirit drink. For nearly two centuries, a cultural stigma has surrounded the beverage that the liquor causes severe psychological side effects when consumed. This was contributed by the rising popularity absinthe accrued in the past, as it found its way into the art circle and inspired notable artists such as Vincent van Gogh and Pablo Picasso (Smith, 2006). Through research, absinthe’s adverse effects, other then those caused by alcohol intoxication, such as hallucinations have been refuted (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). The purpose for this research was to determine the views of Americans and Europeans on the effects absinthe has on the human body and to determine if a stigma continues to exist in the modern age. This research focused on determining the social views of absinthe both in the present day. The present stigma of absinthe was researched by surveying 59 Americans at Lyman Briggs College, East Lansing, Michigan and 41 Europeans at Cite Universitaire, Paris, France. A broad questionnaire asked individuals, age 17-30, for their opinion on the effects absinthe has on the human body and if the individual had experienced or knew of another person who experienced non-alcohol related psychological symptoms associated with absinthe consumption. Several similarities continue to exist today, as it is obvious by survey analysis that absinthe is still viewed as a drink that inhibits a person’s mental capacity and causes abnormal physiological attributes to the brain. The total results show that 69 percent and 75 percent of students, know what absinthe is at Lyman Briggs College (LBC) and Cite Universitaire, respectively. A majority of people at Cite Universitaire and LBC think absinthe causes psychological side effects, 79.5 percent and 71.4 percent respectively. This contradicts the evidence by past research that absinthe does not cause any mental incapacities, other than those caused by normal alcohol intoxication; hence a stigma continues to exist in the modern age. 2 Introduction Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas Absinthe, a green colored spirit-drink made with variety of alcohol concentrations, was first created in Switzerland in the late eighteenth century. The beverage spread throughout Europe by the late nineteenth century due to its popularity with homecoming soldiers from the Wars of French Algeria and because of the increasing price of wine (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Absinthe, also known as the “green fairy”, spread to all social levels. The drink became so popular as to warrant a time of the day called the “green hour” or “l’heure verte” (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Absinthe has many unique preparations. Amongst some of the most popular is the traditional preparation in which a dilution with water is conducted to reduce the bitter taste of absinthe (Strang et al, 1999). The water is poured over a perforated spoon with sugar that is placed on top of a glass with cold absinthe. The addition of water makes the non-water soluble ingredients, mainly from anise and fennel, to come out of the solution and give the drink a cloudy appearance (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Traditionally this is called the louche effect. The dilution of water for this traditional preparation is done to taste. Absinthe also has roots in the art and intellectual circle of recent iconic figures. Painters such as Edgar Degas, Vincent van Gogh, Pablo Picasso, Edoard Manet, and Henri de Toulouse-Lautrec all were rumored to be influenced by absinthe (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Absinthe was used to stir creativity into the minds of many artists and intellectuals. The drink literally would inspire artists to include absinthe in their masterpieces, as seen in Edgar Degas’ masterpiece Au Café du l’Absinthe (1876), currently on display at Musee d’Orsay in Paris, France. The continual use of absinthe was believed to cause “absinthism.” This syndrome had symptoms such as hallucinations, depression, blindness, convulsions, and mental deterioration (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Many blame absinthe for Vincent van Gogh’s many mental disorders and early death at the age of 37 (Smith, 2006 and Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Due to the adverse health effects, absinthe reputation and popularity suffered. Bans throughout most of Europe and in the United States were enacted in the early 1900’s (Weisbord et al, 1997). The ingredient blamed for absinthism is an ancient 3 medicinal plant called Artemisia absinthium, also known as wormwood (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Wormwood gives absinthe its distinct green appearance and bitter taste (Behrens et al, 2004). The principle components of wormwood are _-thujone, _- thujone and absinthin (Höld et al, 2000). _-thujone and _- thujone are believed to be the toxic chemicals while absinthin affects the taste (Höld et al, 2000 and Behrens et al, 2004). Researchers believed that thujone acts similarly to tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the active ingredient in marijuana that causes hallucinations, due to the structural similarities, but this theory could not be proven in experiments (Meschler et al, 1999). Other research shows that _-thujone acts to block the _-aminobutyric acid (GABA) type A receptor which effects the central nervous system (Höld et al, 2000). This is the main reason for concern when dealing with products that contain thujone. Researchers believe that absinthism can be attributed to chronic alcohol intoxication (Strang et al, 1999). Inferior ingredients or alcohol added in the processing of absinthe could have also caused the observed health effects (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). In a recent medical case, wormwood was noted as the cause of acute death of a 31 year old male who ingested extreme amounts of the toxic chemical purchased over the internet (Weisbord et al, 1997). Although absinthe was not officially blamed as the cause of death, its association with wormwood negatively impacted its public image throughout the world. The ban on absinthe was lifted in the early 1990’s for all the countries in the European Union (EU) by the Council Derective 88/388/EEC (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). A maximum level of thujone content was set at 35mg/kg. Since the ban was lifted there has been a revival in the popularity of absinthe throughout Europe. Currently there are over a hundred brands of absinthe being produced (Strang et al, 1999). A review of absinthe brought four studies on absinthe brands together and concluded that 95 percent of researched brands do no exceed the thujone EU maximum limit, and 55 percent only contained 2mg/kg thujone or less (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Historically, the amount of thujone in absinthe has been hard to determine. It has been estimated to be as high as 260mg/kg although it was pointed out this may have been overestimated because of insufficient analytical methods (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Studies conducted on absinthe dating from 1904 show thujone concentrations of less than .001 mg/kg (Lachenmeier et 4 al, 2006) and absinthe made from historic recipes contained less then the current thujone limit of 35mg/kg (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). A review on absinthe presented research by Kröner et al showing that drinking absinthe with the EU limit of thujone results in only a high blood alcohol concentration and no adverse effects from thujone (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). It was concluded that if the EU thujone limit is obeyed the hallucinogenic symptoms of absinthe could be neglected (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Researchers hold the view that drinking absinthe with significantly higher amounts of thujone content then legal limit of 35mg/kg will not cause the drinker to ingest dangerous amounts of the chemical (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). The bitter taste of absinthe reduces the risk of harmful effects because the taste receptor hTAS2R14 responds to the bitter compounds in absinthe and cautions against ingesting toxic amounts (Behrens et al, 2004). Research also shows that ethanol acts to increase the reactivity of the GABAA receptor which offsets some of the danger caused by _thujone (Höld et al, 2000). So even though the GABAA receptor is inhibited by _-thujone, the concentration of thujone and the presence of alcohol in absinthe offset the possible health effects caused by thujone. The overall conclusion on absinthe based off previous research is that absinthe does not pose as serious psychological or negative health effects, other than those of typical alcohol consumption, as initially believed (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). The current research on absinthe and thujone supports that the absinthe is no more harmful that regular high proof liquor. Even so a stigma seems to exist about the harmful effects, particularly psychological hallucinations, that absinthe can cause. This stigma is portrayed in modern movies such as Alfie, Eurotrip, From Hell, Moulin Rouge and many others (Smith, 2006). The drinking of absinthe is often characterized by the presence of a hallucinated green fairy. These movies portray and encourage an uninformed negative view that is held about absinthe. The purpose of this research was to compare and contrast the differences in beliefs between America and Europe surrounding absinthe to determine if a stigma presently exists. The first objective was done by way of a qualitative survey of young adults between the ages of 18-30. Fifty-nine Americans at Lyman Briggs College, Michigan State University in East Lansing, Michigan and 41 Europeans at Cite 5 Universitaire in Paris, France answered a simple 17 question yes/no survey on absinthe. The surveys were analyzed for any trends or comparisons between groups and responses. These surveys were helpful to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both the United States and Europe, and in turn provided statistics to compare between the two. It was predicted that both countries would hold a negative view of absinthe that surpasses that of alcohol, although the United States citizens would hold a harsher view then European citizens. In connection with this prediction, it was forecast that Americans would assume absinthe causes psychological side effects in association with its ban in the United States, whereas Europeans would be more informed on the drink and not hold this view. Methods Written by Justin Thomas Revised by Jason Mashni Absinthe Survey Fifty nine and 41 anonymous individuals participated in a simple seventeen question absinthe survey from Lyman Briggs College at Michigan State University and Cite Universitaire in Paris, France respectively. The following criteria were used in choosing individuals for the survey: a) Each individual had to be between 17-30 years of age b) Either sex was allowed to participate in the survey c) Each individual was chosen randomly while at Cite Universitaire in Paris d) Each individual was chosen randomly while at Lyman Briggs College at Michigan State University in East Lansing, Michigan e) Each individual had to waive the right to have their information freely used in the analysis of this survey The survey was handed out randomly to 59 Americans after traveling to Paris and to 41 individuals in Paris. The responses from the survey were categorized based on their answer based on whether the subject had or had not ever tried or heard of absinthe. The information was qualitatively analyzed to determine if there were any trends or differences while comparing the responses from American and Europeans. The following 6 is an example of the survey given to each individual. The survey was translated into French for the individuals who responded in Paris, France. Absinthe Survey 1. Have you ever heard of the high proof liquor absinthe? Yes No 2. Do you think consuming large amount of absinthe can cause psychological delusions, paranoia, or hallucinations? Yes No 3. Have you ever consumed absinthe? (If no, skip to question 7) Yes No 4. If yes, have you ever experienced any of the symptoms to the consumption of absinthe? Yes No 5. If yes, do you think those psychological symptoms were caused by consuming absinthe? Yes No 6. Did you consume any drugs or mind altering substances while drinking absinthe? Yes No 7. Did you know of anyone who has consumed absinthe? (If no, skip to question 11) Yes No 8. Do you know of anyone who has experienced any of the symptoms mentioned above while consuming absinthe? Yes No 9. Have any of your peers ever experienced any of the possible psychological effects associated with absinthe Yes 10. Did your peers any drugs or mind altering substances while drinking absinthe? Yes No 11. Would you approve of your peers consuming absinthe? Yes No 12. Are you a permanent resident of Europe? Yes No 13. Are you a permanent resident of France? Yes No 14. Are you a permanent resident of the United States? Yes No No 15. What is your sex? M 16. What is your age? ________ 17. Are you currently a student at this university? Yes 7 F No Results Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas By using a survey (Figure 1) the public views on absinthe was quantified to allow for easier comparison of the present stigma relating to the liquor. Forty-one students from La Cite Internationale Universitaire De Paris were surveyed along with 59 students from Lyman Briggs College, a sub-college of Michigan State University. The total results from the responses show that 69 percent of both Americans and Parisian students know what Absinthe is, along with 75 percent who believe that absinthe can cause psychological delusions, paranoia, or hallucinations (Figure 2 & 3 and Table 1). The results of American students (Figure 4 & 5 and Table 2) were compared with results from Parisian students (Figure 6 & 7 and Table 2). More students from Cite Universitaire had heard of Absinthe then students from LBC with 75.6 percent and 64.4 percent of students respectively. On the other hand, 79.5 percent students from Cite Universitaire believe that absinthe causes hallucinations compared to 71.4 percent of students from LBC. Other data shows that less Paris students would approve of their peers consuming absinthe compared to American students with 33.3 percent and 44.4 percent of students respectively. Also, 26.8 percent of LBC students have consumed absinthe with 22.2 percent of those students reportedly experiencing psychological symptoms. Comparatively, 21.4 percent of Cite Universitaire students had tried absinthe with 50 percent of those students reportedly experiencing psychological symptoms of some kind. All responses from each question was recorded and compared along with an age distribution (Figure 8). Discussion Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas The purpose of this experiment was to research the stigma behind absinthe. The authors traveled to Paris, France during the summer of 2007 where absinthe was once one of the most popular alcoholic beverages in the early 20th century (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). For the last two centuries since the advent of absinthe, the liquor has always been 8 surrounded by controversy. The recurrent use of absinthe was believed to cause absinthism, which resulted in hallucinations, depression, blindness, convulsions, and mental deterioration (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). The ingredient in absinthe that is to blame for such medical symptoms is wormwood (Figure 9). This medicinal plant is characteristic of absinthe’s green color. The main component of wormwood that is believed to cause physiological health effects is _-thujone and _- thujone (Höld et al, 2000). At one point, absinthe had been banned in many countries in Europe and North America because of its possible negative health effects. Today, regulations have limited the amount of wormwood in absinthe, in response many countries have allowed it to return to production since 1990. However, absinthe still remains banned in the United States partly because of its wormwood content and also because of the negative views absinthe holds (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). Research in the field of absinthe supports the belief that the current day absinthe has no potential to cause the said negative health effects (Lachenmeier et al, 2005). Research on historic recipes of absinthe and studies done on old bottles of absinthe both show that the _-thujone and _- thujone content would not have been in high enough concentration to cause the negative side effects described (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). That does not mean that nobody experienced hallucinations or paranoia while consuming absinthe as it is predicted that these health effects could have been brought about by other causes such as inferior products or mixing of the alcohol with other chemicals (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). This may even be the reason why Vincent van Gogh was said to experience large effects from absinthe as he was around fumes from paint and other substances (Smith, 2006). We believe that even though past research refutes that absinthe causes effects such as hallucinations and paranoia that many people would still carry a negative view of absinthe. The research behind this experiment was to compare the European and American views of absinthe and to determine whether there actually is a negative stigma surrounding absinthe. A survey was randomly distributed at the Lyman Briggs College at Michigan State University and at Europeans from Cite University in their dining halls. The survey was geared towards finding out if an individual had ever tried or knew someone who had tried absinthe (Figure 1). The subsequent questions asked if they had 9 ever experienced or knew of someone who may have experienced the negative health symptoms that are associated with absinthe consumption. The survey was analyzed once all the responses were gathered. It was hypothesized that Americans will display a more negative view towards absinthe. This is because absinthe has been banned in the United States for several decades. The only way individuals will know whether or not absinthe can cause negative health effects is by word of mouth, traveling out of the country, or by obtaining the substance illegally. Thus it was predicted that the American responses will be based of the refuted myths that surround the liquor. Europeans, however, have had the ban of absinthe lifted for about the last two decades, so it was believed that a majority of Europeans over the age of 18 will have tried the beverage. Therefore, most individuals should know that the current absinthe liquors do not contain enough wormwood to cause hallucinations or other negative psychological health effects (Lachenmeier et al, 2005) The data we found does not support our hypotheses and in fact shows the opposite to some extent. Support that a stigma concerning absinthe does exist can be seen by looking at the total responses from the surveys (Figure 2 & 3). These results show that 75 percent of the people who knew about absinthe still believe it causes hallucinations and other associated conditions. This supports our reason for carrying out this research, in that there is a stigma surrounding absinthe and that it would be interesting to see how the stigma differs in two areas with different laws on absinthe. The main results come from the comparisons that can be drawn from the data of each group of students. It was found that 75.6 percent of responses from Cite Universitaire had heard of Absinthe compared to 64.4 percent of students from LBC (Figure 4 & 5). This statistic supports our hypothesis. This can be attributed to the simple fact that absinthe is legal in the European Union, while it is still illegal in the USA. Therefore, European residents would be more likely to be exposed to absinthe. Interestingly, 79.5 percent students from Cite Universitaire believe that absinthe causes hallucinations compared to 71.4 percent of students from LBC (Figure 6 & 7). Along with the other findings that show 33.3 percent of Paris students would approve of their peers consuming absinthe while 44.4 percent of American students would approve. 10 These findings refute our hypothesis that American would view absinthe harsher because of the fact that it’s illegal in America. We believe that these findings can possibly be attributed to a few reasons. One reason maybe because our research shows that even though absinthe is legal in the European Union a higher percentage of American students still had consumed it. The results showed that 26.8 percent of LBC students had tried absinthe while 21.4 percent of Cite Universitaire students have consumed absinthe. This statistic shows that the American students are more familiar with the effects of absinthe. Notably, this statistic does not support was we previously believed, that more Europeans would have tired absinthe. Another reason for the stigma found by the European students was the way that absinthe was marketed in liquor stores. All the liquor stores that we entered the owner would describe absinthe as having hallucinogenic effects. In fact, in one store called Quinta in Amsterdam the owner said that the absinthe he was selling had THC in it. Some stores marketed their absinthe as having the highest thujone content allowed, even though it has been found that concentrations even above the thujone limit would yield no adverse effects (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). This data does have some errors that have to be taken into account. First, as with any survey, the randomness of the survey comes into question. We surveyed people at one time in each of the schools dining areas. By doing this we may have targeted more of the population in each school that lives in the student housing and not other students that live elsewhere. Second, the power of our surveys is not very strong. We had originally aimed for at least 50 surveys for each school, but found that many of the surveys from Cite were from people who were not students so their surveys were discarded. Also, the directions on the survey were not followed by the survey takers for a good amount of the surveys. For example, when the question said “If no, skip to question 7” many people did not skip to seven. Either way, the surveys were recorded as the student filled it out. This may have affected the outcome of some of the survey results for the questions that were aimed for a specific group (for example, only the people who have tried absinthe). Last, as neither author is sufficient in translating French the translation of our original English survey may have been translated incorrectly, or may have been interpreted differently as some phrases cannot be translated word for word. 11 This may have been a factor in how each sample understood the question and in turn filled out their surveys. This research shows that a stigma still follows absinthe in today culture in both America and Europe. It also shows that even though absinthe is legal in the European Union that it still isn’t able to shed its past. For future research we suggest that similar studies would be repeated in other schools or universities throughout Europe and America. That way more data can be collected and better trends and correlations can be found. Second, more research could be done to determine why the Europeans view absinthe more negatively then Americans. This research not only sheds light on the views of absinthe, but can be used in a broader context on research on all social stigmas. Research can be used to better understand why this stigma still continues to live on. In a world where science and research is supposed to hold some sort of power why isn’t the public responding to the findings? The stigma behind absinthe mirrors the growth of a lot of stigmas, in that it began in the wake of controversy, and gained public recognition and survived even through the opposition of some force. In the case of absinthe that opposition has come in the form of science and research that opposed the previously believed facts. In any case, it is clear that the ghost of absinthe has followed in through the ages and still lives on in the hearts of both Americans and Europeans today. 12 References Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas Behrens, M., A. Brockhoff, C. Kuhn, B. Bufe, M. Winnig, and W. Meyerhof. 2004. The human taste receptor hTAS2R14 responds to a variety of different bitter compounds. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 319(2): 479-485 Höld, K., N. Sirisoma, T. Ikeda, T. Narahashi, and J. Casida. 2000. _-Thujone (the active component of absinthe: _-aminobutyric acid type A receptor modulation and metabolic detoxification. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences 97(8): 3826-3831 Lachenmeier, D. 2007. Assessing the authenticity of absinthe using sensory evaluation and HPTLC analysis of the bitter principle of absinthin. Food Research International 40:167-175 Lachenmeier, D., J. Emmert, T. Kuballa, and G. Sartor. 2005. Thujone-cause of absinthism? Forensic Science International 158(1): 1-8 Lachenmeier, D., S. Walch, S. Padosch, and L. Kröner. 2006. Absinthe - a review. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 46:365-377 Meschler, J., and A. Howlett. 1999. Thujone exhibits low affinity for cannabinoid receptors but fails to evoke cannabinimetic responses. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 62(3): 473-480 [online] http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/97/8/3826 Smith, P. 2006. Absinthe attacks. Practical Neurology 6:376-381 Strang, J., W. Arnold, and T. Peters. 1999. Absinthe: what’s your poison? British Medical Journal 319:1590-1592 [online] http://www.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/319/7225/1590 Tanigake, A., Y. Miyanaga, T. Nakamura, E. Tsuji, K. Matsuyama, M. Kunitomo, and T. Uchida. 2003. The bitterness intensity of chlarithromycin evaluated by a taste sensor. Chemical Pharmacology Bulletin 51(11): 1241-1245 Weisbord, S., J. Soule, and L. Kimmel. 1997. Poison on the line- acute renal failure caused by oil of wormwood purchased through the internet. The New England Journal of Medicine 337:825-827 [online] http://content.nejm.org.proxy2.cl.msu.edu:2047/cgi/content/short/337/12/825 13 Figures Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas Absinthe Survey 1. Have you ever heard of the high proof liquor absinthe? Yes No 2. Do you think consuming large amount of absinthe can cause psychological delusions, paranoia, or hallucinations? Yes No 3. Have you ever consumed absinthe? (If no, skip to question 7) Yes No 4. If yes, have you ever experienced any of the symptoms to the consumption of absinthe? Yes No 5. If yes, do you think those psychological symptoms were caused by consuming absinthe? Yes No 6. Did you consume any drugs or mind altering substances while drinking absinthe? Yes No 7. Did you know of anyone who has consumed absinthe? (If no, skip to question 11) Yes No 8. Do you know of anyone who has experienced any of the symptoms mentioned above while consuming absinthe? Yes No 9. Have any of your peers ever experienced any of the possible psychological effects associated with absinthe Yes No 10. Did your peers any drugs or mind altering substances while drinking absinthe? Yes No 11. Would you approve of your peers consuming absinthe? Yes No 12. Are you a permanent resident of Europe? Yes No 13. Are you a permanent resident of France? Yes No 14. Are you a permanent resident of the United States? Yes No 15. What is your sex? M 16. What is your age? F ________ 17. Are you currently a student at this university? Yes No Figure 1: Absinthe Survey. This is the survey that was given out to American and European students to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. A French version of this survey was used to give to students in La Cite Internationale Universitaire De Paris which was translated by Ellen Santos. 14 Total Responses 90 80 # of Responses 70 60 50 Yes 40 No 30 20 10 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 2: Total Response from Absinthe Survey. A survey was given out to a 59 Americans and 41 Europeans to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. This graph shows the total responses from both and European and American students. This combined graph was used to determine trends with all the responses. The student pool was composed of 45 males and 52 females ranging from the age of 17 to 30. 15 Total Responses by Percentage 100% # of Responses 80% 60% No Yes 40% 20% 0% 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 3: Total Responses Given by Percent. The results from both American and European students were combined and graphed according to the percent of each response. This combined graph was used to determine trends with all the responses by using percents. The student pool was composed of 45 males and 52 females ranging from the age of 17 to 30. Forty-one students were from Cite Universitaire and 59 students were from LBC. 16 Lyman Briggs College 70 # of Responses 60 50 40 Yes No 30 20 10 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 4: American Response from Absinthe Survey. A survey was given out to American and European students from a Lyman Briggs College and Cite Universitaire respectively to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. This graph contains the total results from just the 59 American students. This data was analyzed for trends, and it was also used to compare to European data. 17 Lyman Briggs College 100% 90% 80% Percent 70% 60% 50% No 40% Yes 30% 20% 10% 0% 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 5: American Response Given by Percent. A survey was given out to American and European students from a Lyman Briggs College and Cite Universitaire respectively to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. This graph contains the total results from just the 59 American students given in the percentage of responses. These percentages were used to compare to European percentages to make up for the differing amount of students surveyed in each area. 18 Cite Universitaire 45 40 # of Responses 35 30 25 Yes 20 No 15 10 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 6: European Response from Absinthe Survey. A survey was given out to American and European students from a Lyman Briggs College and Cite Universitaire respectively to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. This graph contains the total results from just the 41 European students. This data was analyzed for trends, and it was also used to compare to European data. 19 Cite Universitaire by Percent 100% Percent 80% 60% No 40% Yes 20% 0% 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Question Number Figure 7 European Response Given by Percent. A survey was given out to American and European students from a Lyman Briggs College and Cite Universitaire respectively to try to determine the stigma surrounding absinthe in both areas. This graph contains the total results from just the 41 European students given in the percentage of responses. These percentages were used to compare to American percentages to make up for the difference of surveyed students. 20 Age Distribution 25 # of People 20 15 Cite 10 LBC 5 0 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 Age 25 26 27 28 29 30 Figure 8: Age Distribution for Both Universities. In the survey given, sex was recorded. These two graphs represent the distribution of ages for both the students surveyed in Lyman Briggs College and Cite Universitaire. For LBC the range was from 17 to 21 and the average age was 18.34. For Cite the range was from 18 to 30 and the average age was 23.81. 21 Figure 9: The Main Components of Wormwood. _-thujone, _-thujone, and absinthin are the main components of wormwood. _-thujone and _-thujone are thought to be the cause of the “absinthism” which symptoms include hallucinations, depression, blindness, convulsions, and mental deterioration. Studies show _-thujone is approximately 2.5 times more toxic then _-thujone (Lachenmeier et al, 2006). All three chemicals give absinthe its distinctive bitter taste (Lachenmeier, 2007). 22 Tables Written by Jason Mashni Revised by Justin Thomas Table 1: Total survey results and percentages of responses. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Total Responses Yes No 69 31 66 22 24 74 8 18 8 8 2 22 53 43 19 54 13 62 3 66 38 58 26 55 10 82 59 41 %Yes 69.0 75.0 24.5 30.8 50.0 8.3 55.2 26.0 17.3 4.3 39.6 32.1 10.9 59.0 %No 31.0 25.0 75.5 69.2 50.0 91.7 44.8 74.0 82.7 95.7 60.4 67.9 89.1 41.0 23 Table 2: Survey results broken into American and European responses and percentages of responses. Males Females 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 17 Cite Universitaire 15 26 Yes 31 31 9 4 4 1 24 9 4 2 14 26 10 0 41 No 10 8 33 4 4 6 17 30 37 36 28 5 31 41 0 % Yes 75.6 79.5 21.4 50.0 50.0 14.3 58.5 23.1 9.8 5.3 33.3 83.9 24.4 0.0 100.0 % No 24.4 20.5 78.6 50.0 50.0 85.7 41.5 76.9 90.2 94.7 66.7 16.1 75.6 100.0 0.0 24 Lyman Briggs 30 28 Yes 38 35 15 4 4 1 29 10 9 1 24 0 0 59 59 No 21 14 41 14 4 16 26 24 25 30 30 50 51 0 0 % Yes 64.4 71.4 26.8 22.2 50.0 5.9 52.7 29.4 26.5 3.2 44.4 0.0 0.0 100.0 100.0 % No 35.6 28.6 73.2 77.8 50.0 94.1 47.3 70.6 73.5 96.8 55.6 100.0 100.0 0.0 0.0 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35
Similar documents
Myth, reality and absinthe
According to Margaria, the con- By the early 20th century, absinthe producers were selling their effects of absinthe is flimsy, does vulsive ED50 of thujone in rats is brands on the basis of health...
More informationAbsinthism - Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy
Misleading advertisements of aphrodisiac or psychotropic effects of absinthe try to re-establish absinthe's former reputation. In distinction from commercially manufactured absinthes with limited t...
More informationAbsinthe - Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Absinthe originated in the canton of Neuchâtel in Switzerland. It achieved great popularity as an alcoholic drink in late 19th and early 20th century France, particularly among Parisian artists a...
More information