and Journal on Developmental Disabilities
Transcription
and Journal on Developmental Disabilities
Journal on Developmental Disabilities 2012 vol.18 n.2 Le journal sur les handicaps du développement La publications officielle de l’association ontarienne sur les handicaps du développement The official publication of the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities Special joint issue: International Journal of Down Syndrome (formerly DSQ) and Journal on Developmental Disabilities vol.18, n.2 2012 Editorial Policy Journal on Developmental Disabilities / Le journal sur les handicaps du développement published by the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities, Toronto, ON ISSN: 118-9136 © Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities The Journal on Developmental Disabilities is published three times yearly by the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities (OADD). The Association has established the following objective for the journal: To foster and promote thoughtful and critical dialogue about relevant issues in the field of developmental disabilities, including those broader social issues that impact on persons with developmental disabilities. Content Each issue features a selection of research, conceptual, informational, and editorial papers. An issue may have a central theme. Reviews of relevant books, movies, websites, software, and other resources are welcomed, as are letters to the editor. In this way we strive to collectively enrich our understanding of issues, encourage stimulating debate among those working in the field, and improve services. Down Syndrome International is a supporter of the International Journal of Developmental Disabilities (formerly Down Syndrome Quarterly) Language Submissions are invited in either French or English and, if accepted, will be printed in the language of submission with an abstract in the other language. Submissions must be free of any gender and ethic bias. People-first language (e.g., persons with developmental disabilities) must be used throughout the manuscript (including figures and tables). Submissions must also use the non-possessive form in eponyms (e.g., Down Syndrome not Down’s Syndrome). Editorial Process Each submission will undergo a peer review by two or more reviewers with relevant expertise. The authors of the manuscripts will not be identified to the reviewers. The reviewers will recommend for or against publication and provide their reasons. Reviewers are asked to judge the manuscript on several criteria including its contribution to increasing our knowledge and clarity of communication. The reviewers are asked to write in a positive and constructive manner to help the authors improve their work, if necessary. Reviewers are asked to complete the reviews within two weeks using electronic communication. Final Decision The Editor-in-Chief, or guest editor, reserves the final decision regarding publication of a submission. To promote the publication of theme issues, the Editor reserves the right to decide in which issue to publish the article if it has been accepted. Copyright Design: Ted Myerscough Layout: Leah Gryfe Cover Image by Angela Gillis Marker on paper Angela Gillis attends the Manuela Dalla-Nora Program at Tycos Centre through Vita Community Living Services/Mens Sana. She has graced the program with her unique quirky style and her love of the arts for 25 years. Angie loves to spend hours on her artwork and is meticulous in the placement of lines and selection of colour that she uses, and always includes her signature within the art itself. Her art reflects her life in that moment. Manuscripts should not be submitted if previously published elsewhere or under review unless explicitly requested by the Editor. Once published, the article becomes the property of OADD. If an author wishes to re-publish materials originally published in the Journal on Developmental Disabilities, written permission is required from OADD. To re-publish previously published material in this journal, written permission must be obtained from the person or organization holding the copyright to the original material. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the individual authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities / L’association ontarienne sur les handicaps du développement. The Journal on Developmental Disabilities greatly appreciates the support shown by the following in sponsoring this issue: community living huronia Community Living Huronia 339 Olive Street Midland, ON L4R 2R4 www.clhmidland.on.ca 160 Springhurst Avenue, Suite 300 Toronto, Ontario M6K 1C2 www.core-toronto.ca 1409 Sperling Avenue, Burnaby, BC V5B 4J8 Phone: (604) 444-3773 Fax: (604) 431-9248 Charitable Donation No.: 89890 7266 RR0001 June 14, 2012 Chief Editorial Board Journal On Developmental Disabilities Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities 2 Surrey Place Toronto, ON M5S 2C2 Dear Members of Chief Editorial Board, We are proud to collaborate with the Journal on Developmental Disabilities on the first joint issue of JoDD and the new International Journal of Down syndrome (formerly Down Syndrome Quarterly). The Down Syndrome Quarterly journal was first published in March of 1996, by then Editor in Chief, Samuel J. Thios Ph.D. with the support of Denison University, The Nisonger Center and Ohio State University, and ran for seven years, until 2002. The Down Syndrome Research Foundation was honoured when asked to take over publication of the journal in 2005, and in 2006 we produced the next issue, a special issue covering topics from the World Down Syndrome Congress which was held in Vancouver in August of 2006. Since then Down Syndrome Research Foundation has published 12 issues with the most recent being the double issue in 2011. Our thanks to the Editorial Board and Staff of JoDD, and to DSQ Co-Editors Dr. Jake Burack and Dr. Roy Brown for their efforts in assisting with this joint publication. Yours sincerely, Dawn A. McKenna Executive Director Langdon Down Centre 2A Langdon Park Teddington TW11 9PS United Kingdom The B Board off D Directors The oard o irectors OADD OADD 2 urrey Place Place 2 SSurrey Toronto, ON Toronto, ON M5S C2 M5S 22C2 CANADA CANADA oadd@oadd.org oadd@oadd.org 17 July 2012 To our colleagues and friends at the Journal on Developmental Disabilities, Down Syndrome International is very grateful to the Journal on Developmental Disabilities for co-‐sponsoring this special issue of the International Journal of Down Syndrome. For us it is the first issue under the Journal's new name, formerly Down Syndrome Quarterly. We appreciate the co-‐operation as well as your enthusiastic support and interest in Down syndrome and your recognition of the need to make clear research and applied implications arising from this important area. Knowledge transfer, particularly in developing countries, is one of our missions, as well as passing on critical research to all families with children with Down syndrome who are increasingly being accepted into the world community. This joint issue will be made available to all participants at the World Down Syndrome Congress to be held in Cape Town, South Africa from 15-‐17 August 2012. Thank you for all your interest, support and practical help in making this happen! Yours Sincerely PENNY ROBERTSON O.A.M CHAIR OF TRUSTEES – DOWN SYNDROME INTERNATIONAL Reg Charity No. 1091843 T: +44 (0)20 8614 5124 Reg Company No. 4327941 E: ccontact@ds-int.org ontact@ds-‐int.org W: www.ds-int.org www.ds-‐int.org Table of Contents Introduction to International Journal of Down Syndrome, Volume 1, Issue 1 and Journal on Developmental Disabilities, Volume 18, Issue 2 Roy I. Brown, and Jacob A. (Jake) Burack . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii Volume 16, Number 3, 2010 Articles Education and Cognition Chief Editorial Board Maire Percy, Ann Fudge Schormans, Lynn Martin, Jonathan Weiss Assistant Editors Carly McMorris, Melody Ashworth Media Editor Esther Ignagni Editor Emeritus Ivan Brown OADD Board Representative Joe Persaud Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome: A Preliminary Investigation of Specific Teaching Strategies Lorraine Gaunt, Karen B. Moni, and Anne Jobling . . . . . . . . . . 8 The Impact of Verbal and Nonverbal Development on Executive Function in Down Syndrome and Williams Syndrome Oriane Landry, Natalie Russo, Tammy Dawkins, Philip D. Zelazo, and Jacob A. Burack . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 Families A Study of Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome Paul M. Ajuwon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 Relations of Parental Perceptions to the Behavioural Characteristics of Adolescents with Down Syndrome Meghan M. Burke, Marisa H. Fisher, and Robert M. Hodapp . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 The Middle Years and Beyond: Transitions and Families of Adults with Down Syndrome Nancy S. Jokinen, Matthew P. Janicki, Mary Hogan, and Lawrence T. Force . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Health Production Manager Jennifer Shaw Design Ted Myerscough Production and Layout Leah Gryfe High Plasma Homocysteine and Low Serum Folate Levels Induced by Antiepileptic Drugs in Down Syndrome Antonio Siniscalchi, Giovambattista De Sarro, Simona Loizzo, and Luca Gallelli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 Puberty, Menstruation and Pain in Australian and Irish Females with Down Syndrome Margaret R. Kyrkou, Rebecca O’Rourke, Joan P. Murphy, Verity M. Bottroff, Dennis S. Smith, Hilary M. C. V. Hoey, and Roy I. Brown . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 Book Review book review: Encore Edie by Annabel Lyon Susan Petko . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 Editorial Board Roy I. Brown, PhD University of Victoria University of New England, Australia Jacob A. Burack, PhD McGill University Management Co-ordinator Dawn McKenna Down Syndrome Research Foundation Burnaby, British Columbia Associate Editors Education Genetics Language/Literacy Medicine/Neurology Perception/Cognition Social Development Monica Cuskelly, PhD University of Queensland Cheryl Wellington, PhD University of British Columbia Elizabeth Kay-Raining Bird, PhDIra Lott, MD Dalhousie University University of California, Irvine Matthew Heath, PhDDeborah Fidler, PhD University of Western Ontario Colorado State University Editorial Board Armando Bertone, PhD McGill University Anne Jobling, PhD University of Queensland David W. Evans, PhD Bucknell University Joanne Nye, PhD University of Bristol Rhonda Faragher, PhD Australian Catholic University Natalie Russo, PhD Syracuse University Tara Flanagan, PhD McGill University Louis Schmidt, PhD McMaster University Jesus Florez, PhD University of Cantabria Naznin Virji-Babul, PhD University of British Columbia Robert Haslam, MD Emeritus, University of Toronto Daniel Weeks, PhD University of Lethbridge Robert M. Hodapp, PhD Vanderbilt University Jennifer Wishart, PhD University of Edinburgh Grace Iarocci, PhD Simon Fraser University Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Roy I. Brown, Jacob A. (Jake) Burack Introduction to International Journal of Down Syndrome, Volume 1, Issue 1 and Journal on Developmental Disabilities, Volume 18, Issue 2 We are very pleased to have edited this collaborative issue between the International Journal of Down Syndrome (IJDS), formerly Down Syndrome Quarterly, and the Journal on Developmental Disabilities (JoDD), that is being published to mark the 11th World Down Syndrome Congress in Cape Town, South Africa on August 15–17, 2012. This special issue includes original research reports and research reviews from basic science to professional practice by scholars from around the world. The articles, which cover the lifespan, have been grouped into sections on education and cognition, family life, and health. We hope that they, and this special issue, will contribute meaningfully to the literature on Down syndrome in particular and developmental disabilities in general, and will be a memorable and informative souvenir from the World Congress for parents and professionals alike. Correspondence royibrown@shaw.ca We would like to thank the chief editors of JoDD (listed in the Table of Contents) for their generous support, advice, and practical help in developing this joint project. Jennifer Shaw and Leah Gryfe worked with us patiently and supportively in producing the special issue. We also wish to acknowledge the assistance of Vanessa dos Santos, National Executive Director, Down Syndrome South Africa for arranging distribution of the special issue to the participants of the World Congress and the support of other trustees of Down Syndrome International (DSI), including Penny Robertson, Chair of Trustees, and Balbir Singh, Past President. Best wishes to all the attendees for a successful and informative congress. Co-Editors Roy I. Brown, PhD University of Victoria, Canada University of New England, Australia Jacob A. (Jake) Burack, PhD McGill University, Canada NOTE: A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/index. php?page=753 after August 15, 2012. Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome: A Preliminary Investigation of Specific Teaching Strategies Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Lorraine Gaunt, Karen B. Moni, Anne Jobling School of Education The University of Queensland, Brisbane St Lucia, QLD, Australia Abstract This article reports the implementation of a specific teaching program (STP) to improve the numeracy skills (place value) of a cohort of young adults with Down syndrome. The STP was based on a range of teaching strategies that research had shown were effective across various groups who have difficulties with numeracy learning. The participants were selected from a post school literacy program at the University of Queensland (Moni & Jobling, 2000). The twelve participants in the program were assessed using the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995) and divided into three groups based on these profiles. The group chosen to participate in the STP showed the prerequisite skills in number recognition and counting needed to learn place value but had no prior knowledge of the concept. The other groups participated in a series of game sessions and did not receive the STP. The data comprised pre and post testing, field notes, classroom observations and family histories. The five participants in the STP showed improvements in their skill acquisition, including developing some understanding of place value. Correspondence l.gaunt@uq.edu.au Our findings demonstrate that individuals with Down syndrome can learn basic numeracy concepts but due to the small sample size more research is required. There is a commonly held educational perception that not everyone can learn numeracy skills. Elliot and Garnett (1994) expressed concern about the acceptance of this perception in society, especially among parents, as this negativity would never be tolerated about literacy; nevertheless, this attitude has persisted in texts for special educators (Massey, Noll & Stephenson, 1994; Rosenberg, Westling & McLeskey, 2011) where it has been suggested that numeracy for some students, including those with intellectual impairment, should be confined to developing functional skills for everyday life. These attitudes persist due to the limited knowledge both about the numeracy abilities of adults with Down syndrome and how these abilities can be developed over time. In addition, there is limited information on the effect of specific teaching strategies on this development (Leonard-Giesen, 2009). Other research has suggested that the development of mathematical understanding relies on effective pedagogy based on knowledge of how students develop an understanding of mathematical concepts rather than on traditional teaching which is often mostly teacher centred (Lowrie, Bobis, & Mulligan, 2008). With this orientation towards how students learn, the characteristics of learners with Down syndrome, such as the degree of intellectual impairment, can be taken into account (Selikowski, 1997). Students with Down syndrome benefit from being taught abstract mathematical concepts with practice and variation using concrete materials over time (Buckley, 2007). Added to their intellectual impairment, learners with Down syndrome may also have sensory impairments (vision and hearing) that could contribute to their generalized language learning delay and affect their numeracy learning (Hammond & Millis, 1996; Lorenz, 1998; Tiens, 1999). Correlations between language learning delays and numerical ability have been found by Nye, Clibbens and Bird (1995) while Buckley (2007) and Horstmeier (2004) stated that generally their numeracy skills were approximately two years behind their literacy skills. The Development of Research in Aspects of Numeracy and Down Syndrome There have been limited studies of numerical abilities of individuals with Down syndrome. Studies by Brown and Deloache (1978); Cornwall (1974); Gelman (1982) and Gelman and Cohen (1988) suggested that children with Down syndrome showed no understanding of the principles of counting, and merely repeated what they had learned by rote. However, studies by Caycho, Gunn and Siegal (1991), Nye, Clibbens and Bird (1995), Porter (1999) and Nye, Fluck and Buckley (2001), have suggested that children with Down syndrome can learn to count, but their ability to learn these skills is related to their language development skills. Bird and Buckley (2002) also suggested that students with Down syndrome still failed to reach a competent level of numeracy skills even though they had reached a competent level in their literacy skills. Other researchers have found that children with Down syndrome are capable of learning v . 18 n .2 9 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome to complete addition tasks by using the strategy counting all (Buckley & Sachs, 1987; Irwin, 1991). Faragher and Brown (2005) found that adults with Down syndrome learned numeracy skills effectively when they were taught within the context of the everyday situations in which the skills were used. Turner, Alborz and Gayle (2008) discussed factors that may influence the development of literacy and numeracy skills in young people with Down syndrome from primary to secondary and post school settings and found that mainstream schooling had a positive effect on academic attainment of these young people. Thus it may be, as some researchers have found, that deficits in numeracy “more likely reflects a lack of teaching rather than a lack of ability” (Tiens, 1999, p. 5). Porter (1999) shared this view with Bird and Buckley (1994) arguing that “…with good teaching there is no reason to predict a level that cannot be surpassed…” (p. 65). A challenge also came from Shepperdson (1994) who concluded from their study of the reading and numeracy abilities of teenagers and young adults with Down syndrome who were born in the sixties and seventies, that “…if they are taught, individuals can learn.” (p. 101). This article reports on the development, implementation and evaluation of a STP designed to develop the numeracy skills, specifically the concept of place value, of a group of young adults with Down syndrome. The STP was designed to meet their identified learning needs in numeracy by using the strategies of explicit teaching, repeated practice and the use of concrete materials. Games sessions were used to help the participants practise what they had learnt. Aims The aims of this study were to explore the current body of knowledge and research about the numerical abilities of individuals with Down syndrome and to develop, implement and evaluate a program to teach a basic number concept, place value, to a group of young adults with Down syndrome. The study drew on three bodies of knowledge: the development of numeracy skills in typically developing children; knowledge of effective teaching strategies for students with learning difficulties; and knowledge of teaching and learning for individuals with Down syndrome. 10 Gaunt et al. This study addressed three questions: 1. What are the numeracy abilities of a group of young adults with Down syndrome? 2. What kind of specific teaching program would meet the needs of the individual students? 3. How effective were the strategies of explicit teaching, repeated practice and use of concrete materials and games in teaching the targeted students? Materials and Methods The key concept of understanding of place value in the numeracy development for the partici pants was investigated. This study used the four specific teaching strategies of explicit teaching, repeated practice and use of concrete materials and games in teaching the targeted participants. The four strategies were embedded in the STP. A single subject case study design was used to investigate the effectiveness of explicit teaching of place value concepts to a group of young adults with Down syndrome. The purpose of single subject research is to discover the effects of some type of intervention on an individual (McCormick, 1995). This design model can demonstrate the effectiveness of an intervention better than many statistical approaches because the subject’s achievements can be directly attributed to the intervention. In this study, each of the participants formed an individual case study (Gay, 1996). As only five participants were selected and targeted for the STP, it was decided that the most useful information would be achieved following this case study model as this would involve in depth analysis of the achievements of each student. To select participants for the study and to develop the STP at an appropriate level within the numerical development of the participants, the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995) were administered as a pre test to the full cohort of students attending a post school literacy programs for young adults (Moni & Jobling, 2000). Individual profiles of each student were developed from the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995) and these were used to match the learner’s profiles to the STP. During both assessment and teaching sessions, qualitative data in the form of observational notes were also collected. These data were used to enhance the data collected from the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995). Participants The twelve participants (4 females and 8 males) for this study were young adults who have Down syndrome (aged 17 years, 2 months to 21 years, 2 months) and had completed or were completing their last year of secondary schooling. Procedure The procedure for this study is presented in two parts. The first part comprises the assessment to place participants into different groups for intervention, and the second part describes the development and implementation of the STP and its related teaching strategies. Assessment and Group Selection of Participants Whilst most teachers may not have access to these assessments, in designing any intervention, teachers need to recognise the limitations demonstrated by individuals with Down syndrome in their receptive and expressive language skills. In this research study, these skills were formally assessed to guide the development of the STP. Assessment Tools Two standardised assessment tools were used: (1) the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition (PPVT-III, Dunn, Dunn, & Williams, 1997), and (2) the Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT, Williams, 1997). The PPVT-III (Dunn et al., 1997) measures receptive vocabulary attainment, and is a screening test of verbal ability. The age equivalent score for receptive language was used in this study because an understanding of the participant’s receptive language is essential for JoDD teachers to know when planning the language content of a teaching project. The EVT (Williams, 1997) measures expressive vocabulary knowledge. The age equivalent score for expressive language was used in this study because the participant’s responses to questions rely on their expressive language and it is important to understand an individual’s strengths and weaknesses in this area before a teaching program can be implemented. As well as investigating the participants’ language abilities, it was necessary to understand the numerical abilities of the participants before developing the STP. A number of standardized tests are available which give age equivalent or grade level equivalent scores but as Westwood (1997) argues: …standardized tests are of rather limited value for programming purposes since they do not yield a comprehensive picture of a student’s broad range of knowledge and skills. (p. 170) It was, therefore, decided to use a tool in which individual performance is presented as a profile of numeracy skills rather than a normative score as this provides a broader range of information related to the participants’ numeracy knowledge and skills. The Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995) is an extensive individual assessment tool and has two separate sub-tests, one for numeration and one for computation. The numeration test compiles a profile for each aspect of numeration: the use of materials, language and symbols and being able to move between them; the understanding of place value, sequencing, comparison and counting. It contains subsets for single digit numbers, two digit numbers and up to seven digit numbers. Only the first two sections of the numeration test were used in this study: single digit numbers and two digit numbers. Booker (1995) claims that “these tests not only profile an individual’s understanding and skill but also provide the basis for developing an individually tailored program to consolidate this knowledge as a foundation for using and applying mathematics.” (p. i of manual). It was on the basis of this profile that the participants were separated into groups for this study. v . 18 n .2 11 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome Lastly, the personal background data, including family and educational history as well as major health problems were collected using the files kept on the students who had participated in the DSRP longitudinal study (Jobling & Cuskelly, 1998). Results of Assessments As can be seen from Table 1, the PPVT-III scores ranged from 4 years, 2 months to 11 years, 3 months and the EVT from 3 years, 10 months to 6 years, 8 months. These results align with previous research that demonstrates that receptive language is more developed than expressive language among persons with Down syndrome (Horstmeier, 2004) and support the correlation between language development and numerical attainment reported by Rahman (2005). In the context of developing the STP, this meant that the researcher needed to be mindful of the language used in explanations of numeracy concepts and needed to check for understanding of the participants as they may not necessarily have had the means to ask for assistance if they did not understand a concept. Table 2 shows the data from the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995). The different sections of the test are shown across the table and the total possible score for each section is given in brackets. Group A is represented by the first 5 students listed, Group B by the next three students italicized and Group C the last four students shaded darkest. The table shows the number of correct responses each student achieved in each section. The first six sections of the test deal with the ability to recognise numbers and manipulate them using language (saying, reading and writing numbers in words), symbols (numbers) and materials (base 10 materials and paddle pop sticks to represent numbers). The other sections of the test investigate students’ abilities to understand the concept of place value, sequencing numbers, comparing the size of numbers, counting (both forwards and backwards and by ones, twos, tens, etc.) and the ability of students to round numbers to the nearest 10, 100, etc. Participants were divided into three groups on the basis of results from The Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker, 1995). Those selected for Group A had good counting and number rec- 12 Gaunt et al. Table 1. Participant Summary EVT Range PPVTIII Mean PPVTIII Range 17y 2m to 20y 9m 7y 5m 5y 8m to 8y 7m 5y 6m 3y 10m to 6y 20y 2m 18y11m to 21y 2m 8y 9m 6y 11m to 11y 3m 6y 5y 10m to 6y 8m 19y 3m 18y 9m to 19y 11m 6y 4y 2m to 8y 7m 4y 9m 4y 1m to 5y 10m Group Participants CA Mean A Julie David Tom Edward Kylie 19y 2m B Nancy Peter Jim C John Kelly Jack Pat CA Range ognition skills. It can be seen from the scores in the counting column reported in Table 2 that most participants in Group A demonstrated an ability to count. The questions the participants got incorrect were counting in tens and counting backwards. One participant scored one and another scored zero; however, both participants demonstrated their ability to count in other questions on the test and it was considered appropriate to include these two students in the target Group. Those selected in this group had mastered counting skills but had no understanding of place value as can be seen from Table 2, where all the participants in Group A, except Tom, scored zero on place value. Tom was able to answer some questions on place value. However, he made no distinction between the ones and the tens in the materials he used although he managed to arrive at the correct answer in some of the simple questions. Hence, the target Group had the prerequisite skills to learn about place value, but no existing concept of place value. The participants who were selected to participate in Group B had both good counting and number recognition skills as well as the beginnings of an understanding of the concept of place value (refer to Table 2). Their scores across the first eight sections of the test were higher than those of Group A. However, they were unable to apply this knowledge to skills such as counting by tens beyond 100 or increasing or decreasing a given number by ten. This is evidenced in their similar EVT Mean scores to Group A on the comparison and counting sections of the test. These participants were not selected as the target Group for the STP as they had some concept of place value. The participants in Group C did not have a strong knowledge of counting nor number recognition (refer to Table 2). Many of the members of this group did not score in many sections of the test. They would sometimes miss numbers in the count string or not show one-to-one correspondence when counting. It was determined that these participants did not have the necessary prerequisite skills to progress onto learning about place value concepts and thus were also not selected as the target Group for this study. Development and Implementation of the Specific Teaching Project (STP) Development of the STP The STP to teach place value was developed using the participants’ skill levels and was based on assessment results to find the participant’s level of learning. (see Tables 1 and 2). From the literature, explicit teaching strategies (Maccini, Mulcahy, & Wilson, 2007; Mercer, Jordan & Miller, 1996; Munro, 2000; Smith & Gellar, 2004) were employed with repeated practice (Impecoven-Lind & Foegen, 2010; Pagliano & Gillies, 2009, Westwood, 1997; JoDD v . 18 n .2 3 3 2 3 5 3 2 1 1 0 1 Edward Julie Kylie Tom Nancy Peter Jim Kelly Jack John Patrick 1 0 0 0 3 4 4 2 1 2 1 2 0 1 0 0 4 4 3 2 3 1 1 1 3 0 0 0 4 4 4 4 1 2 2 2 The number in brackets shows the total number of questions in each section. 3 David Name 2 0 0 1 1 4 4 4 4 2 2 1 2 1 1 1 2 4 4 4 3 2 1 2 Materials to Language to Materials to Symbols to Symbols to Language to Language Materials Symbols Materials Language Symbols (5)1 (4) (4) (4) (4) (4) Table 2. Individual Test Results for Booker Profiles of Mathematics 0 0 0 1 4 0 4 3 0 0 0 0 Place Value (4) 3 2 0 0 7 6 8 5 6 1 3 3 Sequence (10) 1 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 1 0 0 1 Comparison (3) 0 0 0 0 2 3 2 2 0 2 2 1 Counting (4) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 Rounding (1) Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome 13 14 Gaunt et al. van Kraayenoord & Elkins, 2009). The use of concrete materials (Booker, 2000a; Booker & Windsor, 2010; Booker, Bond, Sparrow & Swan, 2010; Mercer, Jordan & Miller, 1996; Westwood, 1997), and the use of games (Booker, 2000b; Booker et al. 2010; Westwood, 1997; Westwood, 2004) were employed in the STP. To ensure participants were given the best opportunities to learn, short instructional periods in a familiar setting (Ashman & Merrotsy, 2009) were considered appropriate. The sequential stages of the STP and the teaching and game sessions are outlined below in Table 3. There were nine weekly sessions of specific place value teaching for Group A and nine games sessions for all participants. After nine weeks, this was followed by the post test again using the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (1995). Table 3. Stages of the STP Stage of Project Pre Tests Table 4 shows an outline of the concepts taught each week, the resources and strategies used in each explicit teaching session. The program focussed on repetitive and concrete teaching methods. During each teaching session, the participants received explicit instruction on place value concepts. They used concrete materials (base ten blocks), various worksheets from Silbey (1989) and received individual tutoring in learning these concepts. Sessions were developed sequentially, reflecting the considered opinions of researchers that the nature of learning numeracy is sequential (Booker et al. 2010; Munro, 2000; van Kraayenoord & Elkins, 2009). Games Sessions All participants (Table 1) took part in a games session on Friday morning. Booker (2000b) suggested that games are an effective teaching tool and can both enhance the learning that has pre- All participants were assessed using the Booker Profiles of Mathematics prior to being organised into groups for the STP Teaching There were nine teaching Sessions: sessions in all. Group A Each session was 70 minutes in Only total, 40 minutes before morning tea and 30 minutes after. Participants in Group A were divided into two smaller groups: three attended Wednesday and two attended Thursday mornings. Implementation of the STP Each morning the teaching session was divided into two by a morning tea break. The time frame was designed to take into account difficulties with sustained attention as identified by Rosenberg, Westling and McLeskey (2011) and Ashman and Merrotsy (2009). In addition, it was assumed the learners would have better concentration in the morning. In the first 40 minute session of the morning new material was introduced (Goodman, 1996) and then consolidated in the second 30 minute session. Description Games Sessions There were nine game sessions in all. All five participants in Group A participated in the games session for the first 50 minutes on Friday mornings. After morning tea, the other two Groups, B and C, each attended a 50-minute games session. Post Tests All participants in Groups A, B and C were assessed using the Booker Profiles of Mathematics one week after the teaching sessions were complete. viously been taught in the classroom and be used to introduce new concepts to be learnt. Moreover, as stated by Booker (2000b) “they contribute to the development of knowledge while having a positive influence on the affective or emotional component of learning situations.” (p. 1). Thus Groups A, B and C attended the games sessions to ascertain if there were any benefits to their numeracy ability as measured by the Booker Profiles of Mathematics. JoDD 15 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome Table 4. Development and Sequence of Strategies Week Concepts Taught 1 Recognizing numbers 1–20 in words and numbers. Recognizing the date in words and numbers 2 Recognizing numbers from 1–100 using materials Games Strategies Number recognition games: •Teacher directed Number play discussion of each concept to engage students prior knowledge Counting games e.g., Snakes and ladders 3 Repeat week 2 Bingo: One more/less 4 Counting Board: counting in 10s starting from any number Bingo: Ten more 5 Review of counting in tens from any number. Computer games Finding ten more or less than a number 6 Review of previous concepts Ten less bingo Ordering numbers Introduction of hundreds Using MAB and dice, make largest number: throw dice three times then using ones, tens and hundreds make largest number possible. 8 Three digit numbers Card Game: make largest number from three chosen cards; extra points for stating ten more than number. 9 Review of two and three digit numbers. Assessment Ideas for the games were developed from The Maths Game, Booker (2000b). During each games session, the participants played games specifically designed to reinforce the concepts taught to Group A during the previous lessons that week. For example, when participants were learning to increase or decrease numbers by ten, the game played was 10 more bingo. When a number was called out, the participants had to find the number on their v . 18 n .2 •Explicit teaching of concepts followed by teacher directed activities followed by independent practice •Regular review activities throughout the program Introduction of decreasing numbers by ten 7 •Repetitive activities: e.g., the date was written on the board each session and discussion about different ways of writing the date and what the numbers meant was the first activity of each lesson. •Games sessions linked to previous content taught card, which was ten more than the number called out. If the concept of the game was too difficult for participants in Group B or C, it was modified. For example, when playing 10 more bingo, Group C had difficulty and the game was changed to 1 more bingo. Prizes were occasionally given to the winners but mostly the participants enjoyed playing the game without requiring extrinsic rewards. 16 Gaunt et al. of Numeration, one half of the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker 1995). The other half of the assessment instrument is the Booker Profiles of Computation. This section of the test was not administered as this research concentrated on the numerical concept of place value, a fundamental skill to all other aspects of numeracy (Booker et al., 2010; van Kraayenoord & Elkins, 2009; Zevenbergen, Dole & Wright, 2004). In each classroom session of the STP, anecdotal observational notes were recorded by the first author to determine the individual learning characteristics of the Group A participants. Results The results are presented first as a group analysis and second as selected individual case studies due to the small number of participants in each group. The comparison of pre and post test means (Table 5) show that some individuals in all Groups showed improvement in different sections of the test (improved scores are italicized and in larger font). The target group (Group A) showed improvements across all areas of the test except section two (language to materials) where the scores remained the same, and section 11 (rounding) which was not covered in the STP. Groups B and C, which only received the games sessions, did show some improvements in test scores, but these improvements were more varied and not as consistent as Group A. Group Results The results of pre and post test scores on the Booker Profiles of Mathematics (Booker 1995) are presented in Table 5. The table lists all of the Assessment Tasks (left hand side) participants completed both before and after the intervention of the STP and Games sessions. The number in the brackets indicates the maximum possible score for that section. Full details of each task can be found in Booker (1995). For example, the assessment task on place value, which is the focus of this study, explores participants’ understanding of ones, tens, hundreds, etc. in different numbers. The eleven sections of the assessment tasks administered comprise the Booker Profiles The results for Group A showed an improvement across all areas of the assessment tasks that were covered in the STP except for section two (language to materials) where the average score was maintained but not improved. Improvement was shown in the place value section where the mean Table 5. Mean Score Per participant On Assessment Tasks Group A Assessment Tasks Pre Test Group B Post Test Pre Test Group C Post Test Pre Test Post Test 1. Materials to Language (5) 2.60 3.20 3.30 5.00 0.75 2.00 2. Language to Materials (4) 1.60 1.60 3.60 3.60 0.25 0.75 3. Materials to Symbols (4) 1.60 3.20 3.60 3.00 0.25 0.75 4. Symbols to Materials (4) 2.00 2.60 4.00 4.00 0.75 1.00 5. Symbols to Language (4) 2.40 2.80 3.00 4.00 0.75 0.75 6. Language to Symbols (4) 2.00 2.80 3.30 3.30 1.25 1.25 7. Place Value (4) 0.40 2.00 2.60 3.60 0.25 0.00 8. Sequence (10) 3.60 4.00 7.30 7.60 1.50 2.75 9. Comparison (3) 0.40 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.25 0.25 10.Counting (4) 1.00 1.40 2.30 2.30 0.00 0.50 11.Rounding (1) 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 The number in brackets shows the total number of questions in each section. JoDD improved from a score of 0.40/4.00 to 2.00/4.00. Other improvements include section one , materials to language from 2.60 to 3.20; section three, materials to symbols from 1.60 to 3.20 and section six language to symbols where students average scores improved from 2.00 to 2.80. The place value scores also improved for Group B from 2.60/4.00 to 3.60/4.00. This group also showed improvements in three other areas. In section one (materials to language) there was improvement from 3.30 to 5.00, in section five (symbols to language) from 3.00 to 4.00 and in section eight (sequencing) from 7.30 to 7.60. Although it appears that Group B scored better in each section than the target group (Group A), it needs to be remembered that Group B began this project with higher scores across the board (see Table 4 and Table 2) and participants already had the beginning of an understanding of place value. Comparing the improved scores of Group B with those of Group C, it is evident that Group C scores were not as high as Group B; however, they still showed some improvements across more areas than Group B. Group C improved in six sections of the test. Section one (materials to language), average sores improved from 0.75 to 2.00, in section two and three (language to materials and materials to symbols), from 0.25 to 0.75, section four (symbols to materials) from 0.75 to 1.00, section eight (sequencing) from 1.50 to 2.75 and section 10 (counting) from 0.00 to 0.50. Group C was the only group that did not show any improvements in place value. Individual Case Study Profiles Table 6 showed the individual scores of Group A participants. Sections where the individuals have improved have been italicized in larger numbers. To highlight the results, three individual case study profiles have been included. The three profiles have been chosen to highlight some of the particular achievements of the students in the STP. Tom The reason Tom was chosen to be profiled was because he was confident and correct in class but did not perform as well on the assessment v . 18 n .2 17 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome for the study as would be expected from his achievement in class. Background Information and Classroom Observations Tom was 19 years, one month old and his receptive language was an age equivalency of 7 years, 3 months while his expressive language was an age equivalency of 6 years. Tom always worked eagerly in class completing classroom activities keenly and successfully. He showed a good understanding of what he was doing and would frequently complete exercises quickly and correctly. However, when it came to assessment tasks on the post test, which he had previously completed successfully in class, he was unable to recall his newly learnt skills. Skills Achieved As can be seen in Table 6, Tom did show some improvement in sequencing (section eight), where he scored five in the pre test and six in the post test. There was also improvement in section six, language to symbols, where Tom scored three in the pre test and four (100%) in the post test. In section seven (place value) Tom scored two on the pre test and three on the post test. In some of the 29 questions relating to place value and the ability to recognise and manipulate numbers on the post test (the first seven sections of the test), Tom showed an awareness of the concept even though the answer was incorrect e.g., when shown the number 47 constructed from the base ten materials, Tom said “four tens and seven ones…makes eleven,” and when making 87 from the base ten materials, he made the number correctly and then said that it was 15 (the sum of eight and seven). Edward Edward was chosen to profile as he showed the most improvement of all of the participants in Group A. Background information and Classroom Observations Edward was aged 20 years and his receptive language was an age equivalency of 8 years, 1 month while his expressive language was an age equivalency of 5 years, 11 months. Edward 2 3 3 3 5 2 3 3 3 (post) Edward (pre) (post) Julie (pre) (post) Kylie (pre) (post) Tom (pre) (post) 1 2 4 1 2 2 1 1 1 2 1 2 3 3 4 1 4 1 4 1 3 3 2 1 4 2 2 2 2 2 The number in brackets shows the total number of questions in each section. 3 David (pre) Name 4 4 4 4 2 2 3 2 1 1 4 3 2 2 3 2 4 1 2 2 Materials to Language to Materials to Symbols to Symbols to Language to materials language Symbols Materials Language Symbols (4) (5)2 (4) (4) (4) (4) Table 6. Group A Pre Test and Post Test scores 3 2 3 0 0 0 4 0 0 0 Place Value (4) 6 5 6 6 3 1 4 3 2 3 Sequence (10) 1 1 0 1 0 0 1 0 1 1 Comparison (3) 2 2 2 0 1 2 1 2 1 1 Counting (4) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 Rounding (1) 18 Gaunt et al. JoDD always worked quietly and was very keen to please. He always tried to complete tasks, even new ones. He would frequently repeat mistakes and would write an incorrect answer rather than leave it blank if he didn’t understand but did not get upset about having his work corrected. He was very pleased with himself when he learnt new skills. Skills Achieved Table 6 revealed that Edward showed improvement in areas involving place value. In section three (materials to symbols) Edward’s score improved from one on the pre test to four (100%) on the post test. Sections six and seven also showed improvement with his score improving from one to four (100%) on section six and from zero to three (maximum score was four) on section seven. He also showed some improvement in sections five (symbols to language) and eight (sequence) where his score improved from two out of four to three and from three out of ten to four, respectively. There was a slight improvement in section nine (comparison) where Edward scored one correct response in the post test after he scored zero on the pre test. In section 10 (counting, which includes counting backwards), Edward’s score improved from zero on the pre test to one out of five on the post test. In the 29 questions involving place value concepts (the first seven sections of the assessments), Edward achieved nine correct responses on the pre test and 20 correct responses on the post test. His comparison skills in recognizing greatest and least numbers, section nine of the assessment, also changed slightly on the posttest. Edward achieved one out of three correct on the post-test where he had not achieved any correct responses in the pre test. Kylie Kylie showed good improvements in some areas tested and was a very keen and interested student. Background Information and Classroom Observations Kylie was aged 20 years, 9 months and her receptive language age equivalency was 5 years, 8 months while her expressive language age equivalency was 5 years, 11 months. Typically, v . 18 n .2 19 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome Kylie was pleased to participate in class activities particularly if she understood the work. If the work was difficult, she would often work very slowly and wait for the answers to be put on the board, or try and copy from other students. She was happy to ask for assistance most times and would work very hard once she understood the concept and was a keen games participant who was not really interested in prizes. Skills Achieved As can be seen in Table 6, Kylie improved in the place value skills of manipulating the base ten materials to read and construct numbers (sections one to four), during the nine-week implementation of the STP. Her scores improved from five out of nineteen on the pre test to twelve correct on the post test. She also applied her knowledge of place value to develop an understanding of the number of ones and tens in a number (section seven). Kylie achieved three out of four correct responses on the post test where she had failed to score any correct responses in the pre test. There was also improvement in section 10, counting, where Kylie’s score improved from zero out of five to two out of five. During the pre test, Kylie answered 12 out of 29 questions involving the concept of place value correctly (sections one to seven). One of these was a picture of 37 using three bundles of ten paddle pop sticks and seven individual sticks. She achieved the correct answer by counting each stick separately not by counting the three bundles of tens as tens. During the post-test, Kylie answered 20 of the 29 questions correctly (including determining the number 37 by counting the tens and ones). She had much greater success when she manipulated the base ten materials rather than reading materials already constructed for her. Prior to the implementation of the STP, Kylie showed no signs of any knowledge of place value but after the program, she showed an awareness of ones and tens. Discussion The results demonstrate that these individuals with Down syndrome can learn the numeracy skill of place value with a program based on specific teaching, repeated practice, the use of concrete materials and games. This has been demonstrated through the improvement in performance 20 Gaunt et al. across almost all areas of the assessment tasks of students in Group A. Although Groups B and C also showed improvements, they were less consistent and less widespread than Group A. A discussion of the results follows. The concept of place value was the main focus of the STP and the increase in results for Group A participants support the value of the STP and demonstrate that individuals with Down syndrome, are able to learn basic numeracy skills if they are taught appropriately. Neither Group B nor C showed as comprehensive a pattern of improvement as Group A which received both the STP and the games sessions. The lack of improvement of Group A participants in section two of the test (language to materials) could be explained due to the short teaching period of the STP. To be able to manipulate the materials following a verbal prompt is a more abstract skill and hence more difficult than to start with the materials that you can then manipulate. Further instruction here was required. Group B did show some improvements in some areas of the assessment; however, Group B started at a higher level of understanding than Groups A or C and already had a basic understanding of place value. The participation of Group B in the games sessions could have triggered links with previous knowledge in each area. Previous research has shown that linking new mathematical concepts with known concepts allows for a deeper understanding of new knowledge (Booker et al., 2010; Van de Walle, 2007). As these participants had completed schooling, participation in the games sessions would have assisted this group in remembering knowledge that they may have forgotten when they completed the pre-test and the games could also have linked with their prior knowledge and developed their understanding further. If Group B had participated in the STP as well as the games sessions, a greater improvement in results would have been expected. Group C had lower scores across the board on the pre test and did not demonstrate an understanding in many areas of fundamental numeracy skill. For example, they did not display knowledge of a stable counting string and had difficulty manipulating materials and symbols to represent numbers. Hence, Group C had lim- ited existing knowledge from which to develop further skills. The games developed for the STP frequently had to be adjusted for the level of learning required by this group. For example, when playing 10 more or less bingo, the game was adjusted for Group C to be 1 more bingo. Hence although some improvements were shown by participants in Group C which can be attributed to the games sessions they participated in, improvements were not expected to be as great as for the other two Groups. A targeted intervention program for this group of students could have shown a greater improvement in numeracy skills across the areas tested. Group C was the only group that did not show improvements in place value from the pre test to the post test (the average score decreased from 0.25 to 0). This group were not specifically taught the skill of place value, only receiving the games session. Participants did not have the known concepts to link new mathematical concepts and develop a deeper understanding of new knowledge (Booker et al., 2010, Van de Walle, 2007). Hence, games alone could not be relied upon to teach new concepts. Games can enhance students learning but individuals with Down syndrome benefit from explicit teaching of numeracy concepts. The results demonstrated that the games sessions did assist the participants in the development of some numeracy skills but that this improvement in understanding was not as great as for those students who had received a targeted, direct teaching program to develop those skills. Games can enhance numeracy development, but specific teaching of concepts is still required. Conclusion The data question the perceptions that numeracy education for individuals with Down syndrome should be confined to functional skills and concurs with findings of Bird and Buckley (1994) and Bochner, Outhred and Pieterse (2001) that numeracy achievements can be achieved with appropriate teaching. As the results have demonstrated, improvement was shown by participants in all three groups but particularly participants in the target Group A, who received the STP and the games sessions. Although the improvements JoDD 21 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome were sometimes variable, the results show the importance of individualized targeted instruction. Furthermore, the results display the benefits of direct instruction with repeated practice and the use of concrete materials in conjunction with targeted and relevant games in the teaching of basic numerical skills. There were two limitations in this study. First, the instruction provided through the STP was for a limited time, and second, there were a small number of participants in this study. Both of these issues could be addressed by further replication with greater sample sizes. Even though there were limitations, this study has demonstrated that with specifically directed teaching strategies, individuals with Down syndrome are able to show improvements in numeracy skill (place value) acquisition, an awareness of place value, an improvement in the individual’s ability to complete tasks based on place value and increased ability to manipulate numbers and materials to solve problems. This article presented the results of the STP that used teaching strategies considered to be effective in improving the numeracy abilities of individuals with Down syndrome. From the presented individual case studies, the additional information gathered from personal background data and classroom observation complemented the assessment results and illustrated the progress of the individual participants in Group A. These data have shown that even with such a short program of instruction, the strategies of explicit teaching, repeated practice and the use of concrete materials and games were effective in improving the numeracy skills of the participants in the target skill of “place value.” As Shepperdson (1994) stated, “… if they are taught, individuals can learn.” (p. 101). Acknowledgments The authors would like to acknowledge the participation and co-operation of the young people with Down syndrome who attended the LATCH-ON program and their families. Without your continued support and participation in research, progress in the understanding of the educational needs of people with Down syndrome would not continue. v . 18 n .2 Key Messages From This Article People with Down syndrome: You should never allow people to tell that you are not capable of learning because with the right help you can achieve many things. Professionals: Explicit teaching strategies combined with the use of games can enhance the learning of place value concepts. To help young people with Down syndrome learn place value concepts, teachers must: • Determine their current level of understanding • Target instruction to individual needs • Directly teach concepts and strategies Policymakers: Policy to ensure that young people with Down syndrome continue to receive access to quality academic education throughout school and beyond is necessary to promote the idea of lifelong learning. References Ashman, A., & Merrotsy, P. (2009). Diversity and educational environments. In A. Ashman & J. Elkins (Eds.), Education for inclusion and diversity (pp. 57–89). Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Education Australia. Bird, G., & Buckley, S. (1994). Meeting the educational needs of children with Down syndrome: A handbook for teachers. University of Portsmouth, U.K. Bird, G., & Buckley, S. (2002). Number skills for teenagers with Down syndrome (11–16 years). Down syndrome Issues and Information 2002. Retrieved from http://www.down-syndrome.org/ doi/?10.3104/9781903806166 Bochner, S., Outhred, L., & Pieterse, M. (2001). A study of functional literacy skills in young adults with Down syndrome. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 48(1), 67–90. Booker, G. (1995). Booker profiles of mathematics. Victoria: The Australian Council for Educational Research. 22 Gaunt et al. Booker, G. (2000a). Difficulties in mathematics: Errors, origins and implications. CrossSection, July 12(2), 14–22. Booker, G. (2000b). The maths game. Wellington: NZCER. Booker, G., Bond, D., Sparrow, L., & Swan, P. (2010). Teaching primary mathematics. Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Education Australia. Booker, G., & Windsor, W. (2010). Developing algebraic thinking: Using problem solving to build from number and geometry in the primary school to the ideas that underpin algebra in high school and beyond. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 8(2010), 411–419. Brown, A., & Deloache, J. (1978). Skills, plans and self-regulation. In R. S. Seigler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 3–55). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Buckley, S. (2007). Teaching numeracy. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 12(1), 11–14. Buckley, S., & Sachs, B. (1987). The adolescent with Down syndrome: Life for the teenager and for the family. UK: Portsmouth Polytechnic. Caycho, L. Gunn, P., & Siegal, M. (1991). Counting by children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95(5), 575–583. Cornwall, A. (1974). Development of language, abstraction and numerical concepts in Down’s syndrome children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 79, 179–190. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Williams, K. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Elliot, P., & Garnett, C. (1994). Mathematics power for all. In C. A. Thornton & N. S. Bley (Eds.), Windows of opportunity: Mathematics for students with special needs (pp. 3–17) Virginia: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Faragher, R., & Brown, R. I. (2005). Numeracy for adults with Down syndrome: It’s a matter of quality of life. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49(10), 761–765. Gay, L. R. (1996). Educational research: Competencies for analysis and application. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Gelman, R. (1982). Basic numerical abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 181–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gelman, R., & Cohen, M. (1988). Qualitative differences in the way Down’s syndrome and normal children solve a novel counting problem. In L. Nadel (Ed.). The psychobiology of Down’s syndrome (pp. 51–99). Cambridge: MIT/Bradford Press. Goodman, J. (1996). Early Intervention with delayed children: Policies and pedagogy. In B. Stratford & P. Gunn (Eds.), New approaches to Down syndrome (pp. 206–225). London: Cassell. Hammond, C., & Millis, E. (1996) Ocular findings in children. In B. Stratford & P. Gunn (Eds.), New approaches to Down syndrome (pp. 122–135). London: Cassell. Horstmeier, D. (2004). Teaching math to people with Down syndrome and other hands-on learners. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House, Inc. Impecoven-Lind, L., & Foegen, A. (2010). Teaching algebra to students with learning difficulties. Intervention in School and Clinic, 46(1), 31–37. doi:10.1177/1053451210369520 Irwin, K. (1991). Teaching children with Down syndrome to add by counting on. Education and Treatment of Children, 14(2), 128–141. Jobling, M., & Cuskelly, M. (1998). Twenty years and going strong: The Down syndrome research project at the Fred and Eleanor Schonell Special Education Research Centre. International Journal of Development, Disability and Education, 45, 245–247. Leonard-Giesen, S. (2009). Parents as teachers of preschool children: Supports, barriers and routines. Dissertation for Doctor of Education. University of California Lorenz, S. (1998). Children with Down syndrome: A guide for teachers and learning support assistants in mainstream primary and secondary schools. London: D. Fulton Publishers. Lowrie, T., Bobis, J., & Mulligan, J. (2008). Mathematics for children: Challenging children to think mathematically. Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Education Australia. Maccini, P., Mulcahy, C. A., & Wilson, M. G. (2007). A follow-up of mathematics interventions for secondary students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22(1), 58–74. JoDD Massey, A., Noll, M. B., & Stephenson, J. (1994). Spatial sense and competitive-employment options for students with mental retardation. In C. A. Thornton & N. S. Bley (Ed.). Windows of opportunity: Mathematics for students with special needs (pp. 353–365). Virginia: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. McCormick, S. (1995). What is single subject experimental research? In S. Neuman and S. McCormick (Eds.), Single subject experimental research: Applications for literacy (pp. 1–32). Delaware: International Reading Association. Mercer, C., Jordan, L., & Miller, S. (1996). Constructivist maths instruction for diverse learners. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 147–156. Moni, K., & Jobling, A. (2000). LATCH-ON: A literacy course for young adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 44, 40–49. Munro, J. (2000). Practical teaching strategies in numeracy for children with learning difficulties, Books 1–5. Melbourne: Down Syndrome Association of Victoria and the Mathematical Association of Victoria. Nye, J., Clibbens, J., & Bird, G. (1995). Numerical ability, general ability and language in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 3(3), 92–102. Nye, J. Fluck, M., & Buckley, S. (2001). Counting and cardinal understanding in children with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7(2), 68–78. Pagliano, P., & Gillies, R. (2009). Curriculum, adjustments, and adaptations. In A. Ashman & J. Elkins (Eds.), Education for inclusion and diversity (pp. 201–234). Australia: Pearson Education. Porter, J. (1999). Learning to count: A difficult task? Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 6(2), 85–94. Rahman, A. (2005). Numerical ability in children with fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Rosenberg, M., Westling, D., & McLeskey, J. (2011). Special education for today’s teachers: An introduction. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc. Selikowitz, M. (1997). Down syndrome: The facts. Oxford: Oxford University Press. v . 18 n .2 23 Developing Numeracy in Young Adults with Down Syndrome Shepperdson, B. (1994). Attainments in reading and number of teenagers and young adults with Down’s syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: Research and Practice, 2(3), 97–101. Silbey, R. (1989). Place Value. USA: Frank Schaffer Publications Smith, K., & Gellar, C. (2004). Essential principles of effective mathematics instruction: Methods to reach all students. Preventing School Failure, 48(4), 22–29. Tiens, B. (1999). Effective teaching strategies for successful inclusion: A focus on Down syndrome. Calgary, AB: PREP Program. Turner, S. Alborz, A., & Gayle, V. (2008). Predictors of academic attainments of young people with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(5), 380–392. doi:10.1111/j.13652788.2007.01038.x Van de Walle, J. (2007). Elementary and middle school mathematics: Teaching developmentally. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc. van Kraayenoord, C. E., & Elkins J., (2009) Literacies and numeracy. In A. Ashman & J. Elkins (Eds.), Education for inclusion and diversity (pp. 235–270). Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Education Australia. Westwood, P. (1997). Commonsense methods for children with special needs. London & New York: Routledge. Westwood, P. (2004). Numeracy and learning difficulties: Approaches to teaching and assessment. London: David Fulton Publishers. Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Zevenbergen, R., Dole, S., & Wright, R. (2004). Teaching mathematics in primary schools. Crows Nest, NSW: Allen & Unwin. Note: At time of first publication of this article, minor changes were pending. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/ index.php?page=754 after August 15, 2012. The Impact of Verbal and Nonverbal Development on Executive Function in Down Syndrome and Williams Syndrome Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Oriane Landry,1 Natalie Russo,2 Tammy Dawkins,3 Philip D. Zelazo,4 Jacob A. Burack3 Psychology Department, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, Halifax, NS 1 Syracuse University, Department of Psychology, Syracuse, NY Abstract Using Vygotsky’s verbal mediation framework, we hypothesised that verbal development would be more strongly associated with executive function than non-verbal development in populations with developmental disabilities with distinct verbal/non-verbal profiles. We used correlational analyses to explore the developmental relationship between verbal and non-verbal development and the executive function components of cognitive flexibility and working memory among persons with Down syndrome and those with Williams syndrome. We found that verbal development was uniquely correlated with cognitive flexibility and working memory in both groups. We conclude that verbal development is a better predictor of both cognitive flexibility and working memory independent of non-verbal development in persons with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. 2 McGill University, Department of Educational Psychology, Montreal, QC 3 University of Minnesota, Institute of Child Development, Minneapolis, MN 4 Correspondence oriane.landry@gmail.com Keywords Down syndrome, Williams syndrome, cognitive flexibility, working memory, verbal development Language development is considered by many to provide the building blocks upon which other cognitive skills can grow (Luria, 1961; Luria & Wertsch, 1981; Vygotsky, 1962). According to Vygotsky and Luria, verbalization serves to focus attention and mediate the voluntary control of behaviour, which can be operationalized by tasks measuring executive functions (EF). In typical development, support for the developmental relationship between language and EF is found in both correlational analyses and experimental studies (Joseph et al., 2005; Kirkham et al., 2003). Disentangling the roles of different aspects of cognition in the development of EF is difficult with typically developing children, because by definition, verbal IQ and non-verbal IQ develop in parallel; that is how we define typical. However, populations such as persons with Down syndrome (DS) and those with Williams syndrome (WS) provide particularly compelling opportunities to examine the relationship between language development and EF because they present opposing profiles of verbal and non-verbal IQ, despite relatively similar full scale IQs. This will allow us to tease apart the relative contribution of verbal and non-verbal skill development in the development of EF. Accordingly, we compared the relationship among the EF tasks of Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS; Frye et al., 1995) and Self-Ordered Pointing (SOP; Archibald & Kerns, 1999; Petrides & Milner, 1982), and measures of verbal and non-verbal development between persons with DS and WS. The Role of Language in Executive Function Evidence from preschool aged children suggests an important role of language in the development of EF, although the co-contribution of non-verbal development has not been fully addressed. For example, the DCCS (Frye et al., 1995) is a sorting task in which children are asked to sort first by one dimension, either colour or shape, and then to change strategies and sort by the other dimension using the same set of cards. The key feature of a task such as this is that the rules are mutually exclusive and incompatible; on any given trial, the test card matches one target card on colour, and the other target card on shape. Typical 3-year-olds can successfully sort by one dimension (preswitch) but perseverate on the initial sorting rule when the sorting rule is changed (postswitch), regardless of which rule is presented first. However, typical 4-year-olds can switch rule sets and typical 5-year-olds can switch rapidly between rules (Frye et al., 1995; Zelazo & Frye, 1997). The ability to switch between incompatible rule sets is referred to as cognitive flexibility or set-shifting; it involves reasoning according to two contradicting pairs of rules, forcing the participants to, at first, think according to a certain rule, but to switch their mind set in order to follow a different rule if they want to succeed on a subsequent part of the task (Frye et al., 1995). The failure to switch mental sets leads to perseverative errors (Zelazo & Müller, 2002). Performance on the DCCS appears to be verbally mediated as labelling cards according to the relevant dimension improves performance among typically developing children. The majority of 3-year-olds fail the post-switch trials (Zelazo et al., 2003), but when children are prompted to label the cards themselves, the proportion of children sorting correctly on postswitch cards increases (Kirkham et al., 2003). Labelling the relevant dimension also improves cognitive flexibility on a separate task for preschool children, the Flexible Item Selection Task (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001). Vygotsky and Luria would argue that verbal representation of the relevant dimension focuses the child’s attention on the relevant attribute, guiding them to represent the problem in a new way. v . 18 n .2 25 Development & Executive Function Another EF task that appears to be verbally mediated is the Self-Ordered Pointing task (SOP; Petrides & Milner, 1982), a working memory task originally developed for use with adult neurological patients and modified for use with children. This task is thought to assess the capacity to initiate a sequence of responses, retain the responses, and monitor the consequences of behaviour (Petrides & Milner, 1982). The task involves participants` self-directed selection of items in an array such that all items are selected once and only once. Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee, and Zelazo (2005) adapted the task to make it appropriate for preschool children and found that performance improved with age between 3- and 5-year-olds. The 3-year-olds were able to successfully perform the task with an average of 4.5 items, while the 5-year-olds could successfully perform the task with an average of 6.5 items. Thus, working memory span appears to increase throughout childhood, with significant improvement during the preschool years. The SOP is assumed to be verbally mediated because the task is considerably easier when the objects can be verbalized. Joseph et al. (2005) presented a group of typically developing children (mean age 8 years) with two versions of a SOP task, one containing concrete namable objects, and a second containing abstract nonnamable objects. They found that the typically developing children committed fewer errors on the version with the concrete objects. This supports the verbal mediation model, which suggests that performance is facilitated by an internal dialogue. We hypothesized that this internal dialogue would facilitate performance on EF tasks among individuals with DS and WS, resulting in EF performance commensurate with language development more so than non-verbal skills. Among persons with DS, language skills are an area of relative weakness, however Pennington et al. (2003) reported that children with DS did not differ in their performance on EF tasks relative to typically developing children with similar vocabulary scores. Thus, EF skills are on par with vocabulary development in DS. Among persons with WS, language skills are an area of relative strength. Hoffman et al. (2003) concluded that persons with WS have intact 26 Landry et al. executive processes because they use similar methods as verbal mental age matched typically developing persons to solve puzzles. Hoffman et al further suggested that visual-spatial difficulties may impede performance among persons with WS on EF tasks that are primarily visual-spatial in presentation. Vicari et al. (2001) tested 12 low functioning children with WS on the Tower of London, an EF task of visualspatial planning, and found performance to be poorer than that of MA matched typically developing children, however details about verbal versus performance IQ of the participants were not provided. While poor visual-spatial skills may put individuals with WS at a disadvantage when tested with visual-spatial based materials, in accordance with the verbal mediation theory (Luria, 1961; Luria & Wertsch, 1981; Vygotsky, 1962), we would expect that among persons with DS and WS, individuals with more advanced verbal development will also show more advanced EF skills. While verbal development is not the same as verbal mediation, the verbal mediation model predicts that verbal development should be associated with EF abilities more so than non-verbal development. Current Study The aim of this study was to explore the differential roles of verbal and non-verbal cognitive development in the acquisition of EF abilities among children with DS and WS. We used the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 1997), a standardized measure of receptive vocabulary, as our estimate of verbal development and the Leiter International Performance Scale (Roid & Miller, 1997), a standardized measure of non-verbal intelligence, as our estimate of non-verbal development. We administered two tasks of EF, a test of cognitive flexibility and a test of working memory. In order to measure cognitive flexibility, we administered the DCCS (Frye et al., 1995) and to measure working memory, we administered the SOP (Petrides & Milner, 1982). In accordance with the verbal mediation model, we predicted that for both the participants with DS and those with WS that performance on the EF tasks would be more related to verbal development than to non-verbal development. Method Participants The participants included 11 persons with DS and 14 persons with WS. Most of the participants were functioning in the moderate mental retardation range, however one participant with WS achieved IQ scores in the borderline to normal range. Nine of the participants with DS were recruited from a special education school and two were recruited from a service agency for persons with intellectual disabilities. The participants with WS were recruited at a National Williams syndrome conference. The ethics boards of the university and schools involved approved the study. The chronological ages (CA) and mental age (MA) equivalents of the participants are provided in months in Table 1. Intellectual Development Measures Verbal development. The Peabody Picture Voc abu lary Test – Third Edition (PPVT; Dunn & Dunn, 1997) was used to measure verbal development. This test is a measure of one word receptive language in which respondents are required to point to a picture from a set of four that best represents a word spoken by the experimenter. For example, the child is presented with four pictures (a cat, a spoon, a crib and a dog) and is asked to “point to the picture of the spoon.” The PPVT is commonly used with persons with developmental disabilities and is especially appropriate for those with particular difficulties in expressive language. Raw scores were converted into mental age equivalents. Non-verbal development. The Leiter Inter national Performance Scale-Revised (Leiter-R; Roid & Miller, 1997) was used to measure non-verbal development. The Leiter-R is a brief measure of non-verbal intelligence that includes 4 subtests of reasoning and visualization appropriate for individuals between the ages of 2 through 20 years of age. As the test relies on non-verbal cues to convey the instructions, requires no verbal responses and has no time limitations, it is especially appropriate for administration to individuals with developmental disabilities. Raw scores were converted into mental age equivalents. JoDD 27 Development & Executive Function Table 1. Chronological Ages (CA), Verbal Mental Age (VMA) Estimates, and Non-Verbal Mental Age (NVMA) Estimates of Participants, in Months Participant Characteristics CA VMA NVMA DS01 112 50 48 DS02 154 56 54 DS03 141 12 38 DS04 176 45 60 DS05 231 36 48 DS06 228 12 42 DS07 157 55 55 DS08 162 21 47 DS09 214 58 67 DS10 255 64 74 DS11 105 54 NA MEAN 175.9 42.1 53.3 49.8 19.0 11.2 SD WS02 120 103 110 WS06 590 153 89 WS07 154 90 94 WS09 102 52 58 WS10 73 53 48 WS11 142 NA 71 WS12 138 58 62 WS13 63 34 NA WS14 81 58 NA WS15 NA 122 NA WS18 102 NA 61 WS21 NA 29 NA MEAN 156.5 75.2 74.1 SD 155.3 40.4 21.3 v . 18 n .2 28 Landry et al. Executive Function Tasks Dimensional Change Card Sort. The DCCS (Frye et al., 1995) is a card-sorting task with three levels of difficulty. These levels are called preswitch, post-switch, and complex. Participants were presented with target cards of a red boat and a blue rabbit that were affixed to two trays. The cards that the participants were given to sort included those with a blue boat and those with a red rabbit. These cards are shown in Figure 1. Figure 1. Target (top) and test (bottom) cards used in the DCCS. Dark shading represents blue and light shading represents red. Dark arrow represents a correct “colour” sort, while the dashed arrow represents a correct “shape” sort. In the pre-switch phase, the participants were introduced to either the colour or the shape game, counterbalanced across participants. For example, in the colour game, the participants were told, “We are going to play the colour game. In the colour game, all the red ones go here,” as the experimenter pointed to the tray with the red boat, “and all the blue ones go here,” as the experimenter pointed to the tray with the blue rabbit. The experimenter demonstrated with one card of each colour, and then the test phase began. The participants were reminded of the rules before each card was turned over. This phase consisted of six cards. Participants who sorted five of six cards correctly were told that they would now play a different game and were administered the post-switch phase. Participants who were unable to sort five of six cards correct- ly were told the game was over and proceeded to the next task. In the post-switch phase, participants who initially played the colour game were told, “we are not going to play the colour game any more, now we are going to play the shape game. In the shape game all the rabbits go here and all the boats go here.” The experimenter then administered the shape game in the same manner as the colour game had been presented in the pre-switch phase. The participants who correctly sorted five of six cards in the post-switch phase progressed to the complex phase. These participants were shown a new set of cards in which half of the cards had black borders, and the remainder were identical to those used in the pre-and post-switch phases. The new rules were explained and demonstrated to the participant. For example, “if the card has a black border, then we play the colour game, and if it has no black border then we play the shape game. Remember, in the colour game, all the red ones go here and the blue ones go here, and in the shape game all the boats go here and all the rabbits go here.” This phase consisted of 12 cards. Before each card was played, the participant was reminded of the rule that “if the card has a black border, then we play the colour game, and if it has no black border then we play the shape game”. The rules concerning the black border were counterbalanced across participants. Across all the phases, a maximum score of three points was possible. One point was assigned for passing each of the pre-switch and post-switch phases, and one point for correctly sorting 9 of 12 cards during the complex phase. Self-Ordered Pointing. This task is an adaptation of Milner and Petrides’ (1982) original task, simplified for younger children. In this task, the participants were presented with drawings of objects arranged in a matrix on a 21.6 cm x 27.9 cm page in a binder. The participants were instructed to choose a picture on each page, and when the page was turned to choose a different picture from the same group of objects that were displayed in different locations. For each level, this continued until the participant pointed to every object in the array, so that three pages were seen for the level with three objects and nine for the level with nine objects. An incorrect response was scored when the participant pointed to an object that had been pointed to on a previous page in that level. An JoDD 29 Development & Executive Function example of a three-object display is presented in Figure 2. In this scenario, if the participant pointed to the train on the first page, then pointing to either the rabbit or the crayons would be correct on the second page. On the third page, the remaining item from the rabbit or crayons that was not pointed to on the second page would be the correct response. Objects were not repeated across sets. The task involved nine levels, each increasing the number of items in the array by one, starting with two items and ending with ten. When the participants successfully completed a level, they continued to the next one. If the participants committed an error, they were given a second chance at that level with a second set of objects. The task continued until the participant failed both sets of pictures at a particular level or passed all nine levels. The score on the task reflected the highest level that was passed. The number of items in each level is one more than the level (e.g., level five contains six items, level nine contains 10 items). Procedure Most participants were able to complete the cognitive measures in one session and the EF tasks in a second session on another day. Each session lasted approximately 45 minutes. On the first session, participants were administered the PPVT-III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) and the Leiter-R (Roid & Miller, 1997) in counterbalanced order. The EF tasks were presented in counterbalanced order during the second session. A third session was included if necessary. Results Not all of the children in each of the groups completed both the two cognitive measures and the two EF tasks. One participant with DS did not complete the Leiter-R. One participant with WS did not complete the DCCS, four did not complete the PPVT, and six did not complete the Leiter-R. Sample sizes thus varied across groups as well as across analyses. Performance on the DCCS and SOP tasks is provided in Table 2. The mean DCCS score for participants with DS suggests many passed preswitch but failed post-switch, while more particv . 18 n .2 Figure 2. Sample three-item array from the SOP. The child is required to point to one item in the first array, a different item in the second array, and the remaining item in the third array. 30 Landry et al. Table 2. Performance on Executive Function Tasks for Participants with Down Syndrome and Williams Syndrome DCCS SOP Sample Size Mean Standard Deviation Sample Size Mean Standard Deviation DS 11 1.18 0.75 11 3.36 2.38 WS 13 1.92 0.95 14 2.93 1.59 ipants with WS passed post-switch. On the SOP, participants with DS had on average a working memory span of 3–4 items, while participants with WS had a working memory span of 2–3 items, although the average for both groups is close to 3. Comparisons were not made between groups as the groups were not matched for MA. significance were used to maximize power and compensate for the small sample sizes of some of the groups. Chronological age was not correlated with performance on the EF tasks; the correlation coefficients ranged from .05 to .34 and were influenced by one particularly older participant with WS. In an attempt to examine the relationship between measures of cognitive development and performance on executive function tasks, a series of correlational analyses were conducted. Given a priori assumptions that all correlations would be positive, one-tailed tests of Correlation matrices for mental ages and EF tasks for each group are presented in Table 3. Significant correlations were noted for the participants with DS for all measures. In the group with WS, the PPVT was significantly correlated with both EF measures, but the Leiter was not Table 3. Intercorrelations Between Measures of Verbal and Non-Verbal Development and Measures of Executive Function for Participants with Down Syndrome and Williams Syndrome LEITER DCCS Down Syndrome PPVT LEITER DCCS SOP r .838** .876** .693** n 10 11 11 r .800** .663* n 10 10 r .799** n 11 Williams Syndrome PPVT LEITER DCCS r .720 .683* .584* n 6 10 10 r .614 .220 n 8 8 r .286 n 13 * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (1-tailed). ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (1-tailed). JoDD 31 Development & Executive Function significantly correlated with either EF measure. Scatterplots showing these relationships are presented in Figure 3. Next, partial correlations were used to examine the unique association between the measures of verbal and non-verbal development and the measures of EF for participants with DS. The group with WS were excluded from this analysis because no bivariate correlation was found between Leiter and the measures of EF. Again, one-tailed tests were used due to a priori assumptions that all correlations would be positive. When controlling for performance on the Leiter, there remained a significant association between the DCCS and PPVT among participants with DS, r (7) = .75, p = .01. When controlling for performance on the PPVT, no association was found between the DCCS and Leiter among the participants with DS, r (7) = .15, ns. When controlling for performance on the Leiter, no association was found between SOP and PPVT for the participants with DS, r (7) = .40, ns. When controlling for performance on the PPVT, no association was found between SOP and Leiter for the participants with DS, r (7) = .16, ns. Discussion The relationships among measures of verbal and non-verbal development and EF components of cognitive flexibility and working memory for persons with DS and WS were examined. In accordance with the verbal mediation model of Vygotsky and Luria, we expected that verbal development would be associated with performance for both executive function tasks for both groups of participants. This hypothesis was supported. In both groups, verbal development was significantly correlated with both measures of EF, but non-verbal development was also correlated with both measures of EF among the participants with DS. Partial correlations showed that the association between cognitive flexibility and verbal development was robust even when controlling for the nonverbal development, but no partial correlations with working memory were significant. These findings can be contrasted to those of Hongwanishkul et al. (2005) who administered the same tasks to a group of 98 three to five year olds. They reported that verbal MA was significantly correlated with both the DCCS Verbal Mental Age by DCCS DS Verbal Mental Age by SOP 9 WS 2 SOP Score DCCS 3 1 0 0 20 40 3 0 20 Nonverbal Mental Age by DCCS DS WS 2 1 0 0 20 40 60 80 Nonverbal Mental Age (Months) 40 60 80 100 Verbal Mental Age 100 DS 140 WS 6 3 0 0 20 40 60 80 100 Nonverbal Mental Age (Months) Figure 3. Scatterplots of verbal mental age and non-verbal mental age by DCCS and SOP scores for participants with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome v . 18 n .2 120 Nonverbal Mental Age by SOP 9 SOP Score DCCS 3 WS 6 0 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 Verbal Mental Age DS 120 32 Landry et al. (.44) and SOP (.28) tasks after CA was partialled out, but that after partialling out chronological age, performance mental age was only correlated with DCCS (.46). The similarity in correlation coefficients between both measures of intellectual functioning and the DCCS support our contention that verbal and non-verbal intelligence cannot be easily parsed in typically developing children. Likewise, our data suggest a stronger association between EF and verbal relative to non-verbal intelligence. Cognitive flexibility was positively associated with verbal abilities among the participants with DS even after controlling for non-verbal abilities, whereas non-verbal abilities were no longer correlated after controlling for verbal abilities. Among the participants with WS, verbal but not non-verbal development was associated with cognitive flexibility. These findings support the special role of verbal development in the development of cognitive flexibility. Working memory was also associated with the verbal abilities of both groups. In contrast, nonverbal abilities were only correlated with working memory among the participants with DS, although neither verbal nor non-verbal abilities were significantly correlated with working memory after controlling for the other. The finding that verbal, and not non-verbal, abilities were associated with performance on both EF tasks among both the participants with DS and those with WS is relevant to theories of verbal mediation, as they display contrasting patterns of strength and weakness with respect to verbal and nonverbal development. The finding that performance was associated with verbal development both for children for whom language is a relative strength (children with WS) and for who it is a relative weakness (children with DS) lends support to the notion that cognitive flexibility and working memory are verbally mediated, even among persons with mental retardation of differing aetiologies. Further, experimental findings of verbally mediated performance for typically developing children (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001; Kirkham et al., 2003) suggest that, despite intellectual impairments, the developmental principles under operation for the typically developing children appear to be applicable to both persons with Down syndrome and those with Williams syndrome. Further research is needed on the relationship between verbal and nonverbal development and EF measures in both typically and atypically developing groups. The numbers of participant in the two groups in this study were small and fell within a restricted mental age range. In addition, the broader variability in CA and MA ranges represented among the participants with WS than among participants with DS, might be partly responsible for the group differences in the correlations. Future studies could include more tasks that are appropriate to assess achievement from a wider range of mental ages, and could also be focused on the relationship between language development and additional measures of cognitive flexibility and working memory. Our failure to record gender or sociodemographic data of our participants should also be corrected in future reports of studies. Preliminary evidence for the primacy role of language development in cognitive flexibility and working memory was provided. However, we only used one measure of verbal ability, the PPVT, which is widely used to estimate children’s language level but is only a measure of children’s receptive vocabulary. Further research should also include more comprehensive language measures that provide both a more global assessment of children’s language development as well as the ability to break down language development to determine if any aspects are more or less pertinent to verbally mediating behaviour. The findings reported by Jacques and Zelazo (2001), Kirkham et al. (2003), and Zelazo et al. (2003), as well as those described by Luria (1961), demonstrate that typically developing children who have not yet incorporated speech into their problem solving can learn the strategy from adults. This could be useful to develop training programs for children with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome, who might have the necessary language skills but have not spontaneously developed the verbal mediation strategy. Key Messages From This Article Professionals: For practical skills like problem solving and planning, the language skills of the client should be considered in setting developmentally appropriate goals. Educators may want to focus on language skills in early years. Policymakers: Individuals with developmental disabilities have a wide range of skills and skill levels and deserve opportunities to maximize their skills. JoDD 33 Development & Executive Function Acknowledgements The authors thank the participants in the study, who include students from Summit School (Montreal), clients of the West Island Readaptation Center (Montreal), and attendees at the annual Williams Syndrome Conference. We thank Nicki Saros and Rhoda Root for their help in recruiting the participants with Down syndrome. The work on this project was funded an operating grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council to Jacob A. Burack. References Archibald, S. J., & Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and description of new tests of executive function in children. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 115–129. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527. Hoffman, J. E., Landau, B., & Pagani, B. (2003). Spatial breakdown in spatial construction: Evidence from eye fixations in children with Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 260–301. Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644. Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 573–591. Joseph, R. M., Steele, S. D., Meyer, E., & TagerFlusberg, H. (2005). Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: Failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1400–1411. Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L. M., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449–467. Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York: Liveright. Luria, A. R., & Wertsch, J. V. (1981). Language and cognition. Washington, DC: V.H. Winston. v . 18 n .2 Pennington, B. F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., & Nadel, L. (2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: Evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Development, 74, 75–93. Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontaland temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249–262. Roid, G., & Miller, L. J. (1997). The Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised: Examiners manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting. Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. New York: Oxford University Press. Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 103–112. Russo, N3, Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 77–86. Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2001). Procedural learning deficit in children with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 39, 665–677. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press. Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1997). Cognitive complexity and control: A theory of the development of deliberate reasoning and intentional action. In M. Stamenov (Ed.), Language structure, discourse, and the access to consciousness (pp. 113–153). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., et al. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, vii–137. NOTE: A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www. oadd.org/index.php?page=755 after August 15, 2012. A Study of Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome Abstract Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Author Paul M. Ajuwon Department of Counseling, Leadership, & Special Education, Missouri State University, Springfield, MO Correspondence The Family Quality of Life Survey (FQOLS) (Brown et al., 2006), was administered to a group of 31 families who have children with Down syndrome enrolled at a community-based centre in Nigeria. Ideas on how families perceive services for their children, and the extent to which families are coping, are discussed. Suggestions are made for effective policy and practice and for enhanced understanding of disabilities by the society. This research project focuses on an exploration of quality of life of families that have children with Down syndrome (DS) in Nigeria, using the Family Quality of Life Survey (Brown et al., 2006). Although this instrument has been widely applied to the measurement of family satisfaction and well-being in some Western countries, the utilisation of the instrument is just now beginning to be employed not only as a research method, but also as an approach to understanding disabilities and family dynamics in a developing country like Nigeria (Ajuwon & Brown, 2012). In the study to be reported, the FQOLS was utilized to assess several domains and dimensions reflecting the major components and characteristics of family quality of life of 31 families who have children at the Down Syndrome Foundation of Nigeria (DSFN). The DSFN, established in 2001, is a privately-operated community-based centre in Lagos, Nigeria. It is the only centre of its kind in Lagos for educating learners, most of whom have been diagnosed with DS (see http:// www.dsanigeria.org). This study aims to provide a thematic analysis of the qualitative comments supporting the quantitatively measured domains and dimensions of the survey and to explore their relationships to one another. paulajuwon@ missouristate.edu Method Keywords Instrumentation families; quality of life; Down syndrome; satisfaction; well-being; Nigeria The FQOLS is a comprehensive tool for examining family quality of life, and is used to gather both quantitative and qualitative information. The survey tool consists of a section requesting details about the family, and nine domains: Health of the Family, Financial Well-Being, Family Relationships, Support from Others, Support from DisabilityRelated Services, Influence of Values, Careers/Preparing for Careers, Leisure and Recreation, and Community/Civic Interaction. Each domain contains questions related to six key dimensions: Importance, Opportunities, Initiat ive, Attainment, Stability, and Satis faction. The questionnaire has now been used in research in 20 countries. For a detailed description of the components of the survey, see Brown et al. (2006); Brown, Anand, Fung, Isaacs, and Baum (2003); Isaacs, Brown, Brown, et al. (2007); and Werner, Edwards, Baum, Brown, Brown, and Isaacs (2009). Procedures and Respondents The researcher obtained formal approval from his university in the United States to conduct the study. On his arrival in Nigeria, the researcher convened a meeting with the management of the Down Syndrome Foundation of Nigeria. Approval was obtained to invite willing parents to the centre for a meeting at which the nature and scope of the study were explained, and one participant from each household was subsequently recruited for the project. These participants represented families with diverse socioeconomic and educational backgrounds. Since English is the lingua franca in Nigeria, study participants had no difficulty in understanding and/or answering the questionnaire items. A clerical worker at the centre was identified and trained to assist with the administration of the instrument. The survey was completed by the volunteer respondents in small groups at the centre over a five-day period. From a standpoint of transportation and organizational efficiency, it was more effective to have the parents complete the survey when they brought their children to the centre in the morning, and/or returned them home in the afternoon. A total of 31 out of 45 parents having a family member with DS completed the survey. Respondents were nearly evenly split between male and female (15 vs. 16). The majority of male respondents (8) were the father of the DS family member. Likewise, most female respondents were the mother of the DS family member. Seventy-four percent (23) of the 31 respondents reported belonging to an immediate family with two parents. Characteristics of Household Members and Caregiving Responsibilities In one family, there was a pair of 8-year-old twin girls with DS, bringing the total number of DS family members in the study to 32. More v . 18 n .2 35 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome than half of the DS family members were male (56%). Their ages ranged from 1 to 40; 56% were minors under the age of 18. Only one DS family member was not living at home, specifically, a 24 year-old female. With regard to caregiving roles, most families (29 or 94%) included the biological mother of the family member with DS. A majority of these mothers (83%) both lived in the home and were caregivers. They ranged in age from 28 to 72, with a mean of 46.7. Most families (23 or 74%) also included the biological father of the DS family member, 65% of whom both lived in the home and were caregivers. Their age range was 35 to 69, with a mean of 48.6. Thirty-five percent of respondents reported the presence of one or more siblings of the DS family member living at home and acting as caregivers. These 22 siblings ranged in age from 7 to 35. The mean age of the 10 male siblings was 21.1, while the mean age of the 12 female siblings was 23.25. Involvement in family life appeared to be distributed across all family members. Respondents were asked which family member(s) was the most involved with the daily life of the DS family member. Mothers (alone) were named most frequently (18 or 58%), followed by a combination of parents and/or other family members (12 or 39%); only one father (alone) was named. With regard to the amount of personal responsibility that the respondent had for the family member with DS, responses were almost evenly divided into thirds as follows: 32% (10) indicated they had either “‘less’ or ‘about the amount of’ responsibility that I like,” 32% (10) indicated they had “more responsibility than I would like,” and 36% (11) indicated they had “much more responsibility than I would like.” Results The following results are responses from the family respondents. As such they are their perceptions of the quality of life of their family. Others may have differing perceptions, but since the primary carer is making the responses, they are likely to be views which play an important role in driving the family behaviours and actions. 36 Ajuwon Developmental and Health Issues Associated with Down Syndrome As in other countries, the well-being of many Nigerian children and adults with DS is negatively impacted by multiple developmental and health issues. Table 1 summarizes the open-ended comments made by the respondents when asked to describe the physical, mental, or behavioural conditions that they had identified. Over three-fourths of respondents cited speech or language difficulties and delays, including receptive and expressive language, mutism, and incoherent speech as primary concerns to providing effective care. Behavioural problems involving poor coordination, restlessness, inattention, and inability to follow directions were discussed by 29%, followed by developmental delays related to physical growth, feeding, toileting, and cognition by 19%. Fewer remarks were made regarding physical and mental health problems. Overall, the open-ended comments from respondents to this last question, together with the question “Is there anything else you would like to tell us about your family member(s) with an intellectual or developmental disability?” yielded a variety of interesting insights. For example, ten respondents indicated that their DS family member’s well-being was inhibited by poor social skills and a tendency to social timidity in new situations. These children with DS often required constant parental supervision to interact with the wider community. Conversely, two parents noted that their experiences were enhanced for themselves and their child with DS because she/he had a generally happy disposition or a desire to learn new things. Daily Caregiving Experience with a Family Member with Down Syndrome Over a third of respondents (12) identified caregiver burden as the primary stressor in their life. The daily burden was physically and financially overpowering for these caregivers and their families, because for most families “home care assistance” was unavailable. This daily burden resulted in loss of other normative activities for caregivers – from social activities to loss of employment and independence for mothers, who were most often the primary caregiver. Six respondents expressed specific concerns about the future well-being of their DS family member. These concerns were rooted in a lack of services that kept the adult child with the family and dependent on the family. For parents, the caregiver burden increased with age because disability-specific services were even less accessible when their child transitioned to adulthood. At the same time, individuals with DS had very limited access to vocational education and self-help training that would promote even basic independence in adulthood. In one of the interview sessions, an exasperated mother of a teenage daughter with DS remarked thus: “I’m not sure what will become of my daughter now that she is getting to that age, when other teen- Table 1. Summary of Qualitative Descriptions of Physical, Mental, or Behavioural Condition Condition N Percent Speech or language difficulties and delays1 24 78.4 Behavioural problems 9 29.0 6 19.4 4 12.9 2 6.5 2 Developmental delays 3 Physical health problems Mental health problems 3 4 5 1 2 4 5 Receptive and expressive language, mutism, and incoherent speech Poor coordination, restlessness, inattention, inability to follow directions Physical growth, feeding, toileting, cognition Heart condition, poor vision and/or hearing, chronic digestive problems Mood or anxiety disorders (including schizophrenia) and anger JoDD age girls without disability are doing some type of work, or engaging in social activities with boys, and looking ahead to possible relationship and marriage. Frankly speaking, I do not know how to handle the situation, and I am not going to be around forever to look after her!” A number of caregivers (41.9%) indicated that personal and family attitudes enhanced their caregiving experience. These attitudes were captured in three broad themes: first, caregiving was a family commitment; second, they accepted the DS family member’s condition and the primacy of the caregiving role; and third, their family member with DS brought a special joy and love to their entire family, in spite of negative experiences they sometimes encounter in society. Health Care Issues for Affected Families Adequate health care is a luxury for most Nigerians, and barriers to health care services were broadly themed as: affordability, accessibility, and public attitude. It was revealed by most families (54.8%) during group interviews that affordability was the primary issue. Factors included the poor state of the Nigerian economy, low personal and family incomes, the high cost of good private health care, the high cost of healthy food, and the need for ongoing care for some individuals with DS. Respondents linked the high cost of medical care to potential health risks. Since medical care and prescription drugs were unaffordable for at least 50% of participants, many families relied on unregulated herbal remedies from traditional healers for many ailments. These products and practices come with unknown risks and side effects. First, traditional healing practices may not be effective with developmental conditions like Down syndrome and associated behaviours (although some herbalists are known to lay claim to such a “remedy”). Second, unregulated herbal medicines may have unknown side effects, especially when prescribed for special needs populations. Financial issues were identified in a broader context, that is, financial need and long work hours generally impeded families from pursuing healthy lifestyle choices like regular exer- v . 18 n .2 37 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome cise. Respondents indicated access to basic health care services was problematic for most families and most problematic for persons with a developmental disability. Key inhibitors were systemic: scarcity of publicly funded health services, unaffordable private care, absence of governmental support for low-income families, and poor economic conditions. Consequently, public health facilities were under-funded, poorly equipped, and lacked specialists having disability-specific skills or knowledge. This point was reinforced by one male participant, thus: “When our baby son with DS was born in the public hospital, the doctors and nurses were not of much help. We spent our meager savings on searching for a “cure” from traditional healers. Luckily, through the DSFN, the Kanu Heart Foundation provided us with financial support to go to India for three months where successful open heart surgery was done. Today, our baby is doing well physically, although he still requires help in feeding and other social skills.” Feeling neglected was a major theme expressed by respondents. An underfunded public health care system, along with poor attitudes of health professionals towards persons with disabilities, little or no early intervention, and poor dissemination of knowledge to patients were key issues raised by respondents. One frustrated mother of 8-year-old twin daughters with DS angrily stated: “When I was carrying the pregnancy, the hospital staff either did not do proper pre-natal testing or failed to inform me of their conditions. If I had been told, I would have chosen to terminate the pregnancy. Six months after the twins were delivered, my husband ran away, and I’ve since been left alone to look after them. But, I owe a lot to the DSFN where the girls are now enrolled and where I also work as a teacher.” Still, there is increasing public awareness of health care needs and a growing expectation of access to services in Nigeria. This shift in public attitude and health knowledge is driven by media access, mainly radio and television, but also newspapers and magazines. This is leading to increased levels of frustration because families cannot access modern services they know will improve their health and wellbeing, because of the high cost of obtaining such health-related services. This declining situation has led some observers to believe that 38 Ajuwon Nigeria may be unable to meet the health-related Millennium Development Goals (MDGs) by 2015, which mandates each country to spend 15 percent of their yearly national budget on health. Nigeria currently spends less than seven percent annually (http://www.nigerianbestforum.com/generaltopics/?p=85074). Respondents also described enhancers and inhibitors to health care access. Geographic location, along with personal behaviours and attitudes, were prominent themes that respondents identified as enhancing health outcomes. In spite of concerns about transportation and cost, residents of large urban centres had much better access to care. Personal behaviours were the primary means for most respondents to maintain health. They indicated that eating well or improving their diet, getting regular exercise, improving their knowledge of what is required to maintain good health, and trying to access appropriate services were major contributors to well-being. Personal attitudes strongly linked to positive health behaviours included having an optimistic outlook on daily life; contentment with life situation; and a desire for better health. These attitudes were often integrated into their faith and daily religious practices. Financial Well-Being of Families Three-quarters of participants expressed dissatisfaction with galloping inflation, general insecurity in their communities, the widespread degradation of the environment occasioned by traffic congestion, toxic wastes, air pollution, frequent power outages, constant noise from generating plants, and incessant flooding of urban centres that lack poor drainage systems. They cited ineffective social support programmes for persons with disabilities and their families. Almost all respondents commented on lack of access to basic amenities and infrastructures for good quality of life. It was obvious that chronic daily struggle to survive when raising a child with a disability intensified with the poor state of the economy. Slightly over two-thirds of respondents (67.7%) indicated a declining financial position. Some experienced long-term unemployment; many were underemployed and worked long hours to keep their jobs. Chronic unemployment was also a reality for many young adults with no disability who were graduates of post-secondary institutions and still living at home several years after graduation. Three prominent themes emerged that related to poor future financial outcomes for families. First, family financial well-being was negatively impacted by inflation that dominates the economy. Second, aging and retirement issues were of concern for older caregivers. Many indicated poor earnings over their life course would be compounded by poor-quality pensions that are not paid on a regular schedule. Retirement would mean less income and less stable income, but not less caregiving responsibility. Third, a majority of respondents (80.6%) lamented the pervasive corruption at national, state, and local levels. Such corruption was seen as contributing immensely to the high levels of social and economic instability in the country (Dike, 2008; Sulaiman, 2008). However, some respondents indicated their financial situation was improving. A few reported good financial management and good-performing investments, while others indicated job promotion or new business opportunities had enhanced family finances. Generally, respondents indicated that employed young adult children with no disability still living at home enhanced the financial wellbeing of the family. Some indicated membership in cooperative societies enabled them to access additional resources for meeting needs. As with many other aspects of family life in Nigeria, respondents indicated personal effort and faith, which, along with a positive attitude that things will get better, was central to current and future well-being. Building and Maintaining Family Relationships Ninety percent of respondents described strong, positive social and emotional relationships with nuclear and extended family members. These relationships were enhanced in families that followed traditional, cultural values and actively practised their religious faith. Caregivers often received important emotional and instrumental support from adult children, JoDD 39 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome extended family, and their religious community. Satisfaction with family relationships was multi-dimensional. Some caregivers associated family satisfaction with personal growth and the ability to access training and continuing education. Most suggested satisfaction with family was related to mature personal attitudes. For older caregivers, family attitudes that valued the elderly and sought out their advice on important matters greatly added to satisfaction in this domain. On the other hand, satisfaction with family relationships was inhibited by chronic stressors related to urban life, financial hardship, and long hours of work that reduced contact between members. For some caregivers, satisfaction decreased when they had employed adult children struggling to make ends meet in their own families and unable to offer support to their siblings with DS or to aging caregiving parents. These employed adult children were employed in jobs that paid considerably less than their qualifications; yet, a great proportion of their monthly income was spent on transportation to and from worksites. Accessing Community Support Most participants (58%) indicated access to community supports was largely inhibited by a lack of economic resources, along with the time burden of daily household duties and child care responsibilities. Additional caregiving for the family member with DS exacerbated this social isolation. Some identified work responsibilities and the lack of encouragement from others as key factors that limited access to community supports. Three respondents mentioned self-imposed isolation because they feared those outside their religion or immediate family. The general economic turmoil in the country contributed to social isolation of some participants. Poverty, lack of job security, and a daily struggle to survive can create an environment in which people do not support others outside their immediate family. This is especially true in large urban areas, where the influence of Western society has challenged complex social relationships present in traditional Nigerian cultural roles and eroded traditional family values. Satisfaction with community support was enhanced by individual desire to socialize and ability to achieve balance between work, personal and domestic needs. Respondents globally indicated that receiving emotional and instrumental support, the presence of religious faith and traditional family values, and the growing independence of the DS family member improved satisfaction in this domain. Caregivers noted that community visibility – engaging in community activities with their affected child – resulted in improved social awareness and acceptance of disability. One parent described her experience as … “Interaction with the community can go a long way towards showing them that we love our daughter with a disability. This will be a way to tell others in a similar situation to accept their offspring who may have a similar disability.” In turn, improved community understanding of disability resulted in greater social acceptance of persons with disabilities and their families as integral members of the community. In the words of one caregiver … “The best approach to getting the community informed about disability is for the families of the disabled to continue to involve their children in community-based programmes. That way, the public will gradually become more enlightened about their abilities and needs.” Accessing Support Services Caregivers identified three specific categories of disability-related support services. These are summarized thus: A.Professional services: 1. Speech and language therapy, occupational therapy, and audiology services 2. Behaviour therapy 3. Specialized pediatric services and medical services for persons with disabilities 4. Sexuality training for adolescents and young adults with DS B. Community-based services: 1. Vocational training, including adapted computer programmes 2. Advocacy training for parents v . 18 n .2 40 Ajuwon 3. Nutritional services/counselling for family 4. Psychological counselling and self-help skills training for individuals with DS 5. Adapted recreation activities 6. Specialized transportation C. Home-based services: 1. Specialized health care services 2. Social workers 3. Respite care and related home support services The major barriers to support services are summarized as being the following: the high cost for private school and specialized services and training; the limited number of governmentsupported schools for the disabled that are not well-staffed or equipped; and poor economic conditions that eroded family incomes and prevented them from purchasing private services. It was noted by a small number of participants (19.3%) that access to these services was enhanced by: sufficient personal income to purchase private services; proximity to private schools and services; and the presence of philanthropists or organizations funding special education and rehabilitation programmes. However, for a number of caregivers (12.9%), access to advanced education or communitybased services decreased even further with transition to adulthood, and there was no vocational training for the disabled adult population. Service access was further inhibited by lack of finances for some parents to purchase their own car and the poor public transportation systems in some neighbourhoods. This situation has forced two parents to resort to using the “Okada” (the local motor cycle) to bring their child with DS to and from the Centre each day. Underlying the access problem was a lack of national interest or policy to develop a transportation-friendly programme, especially in urban centres of the country. As one parent noted: “The government would be improving the quality of training if it can set up a special transportation programme for children and youth who are disabled. Such a service would greatly relieve the burden of parents who spend several hours in the traffic each day as they bring their child to and from school.” Influence of Values Personal and community world views were strongly influenced by formal religious values, strong religious faith, and traditional cultural values. Caregivers noted that traditional superstitions associated with any form of disability were problematic and must be replaced with a modern understanding of disability. They identified religious and cultural leaders as the key to this shift in knowledge and practice. While many leaders did not promote disability awareness in their everyday dialog and actions, places of worship among Moslems, Christians, and traditional believers were viewed as appropriate venues to educate the broader public on disability-related issues, given the extreme religiosity of the people. It was suggested that professionally trained special educators and counsellors should work closely with such religious leaders to increase their knowledge and awareness, as well as enhance the integration into religious activities of members with disabilities. Indeed, religious values can be seen to have strong protective effects for individuals and families, reducing the negative impacts of daily life stressors and challenges associated with raising a child with a disability. The influence of religious beliefs and traditional cultural values can therefore be translated into greater personal and family worth and dignity and a sense that each person is valued for who they are, with special “compassion and love for the disabled.” Career Development Respondents indicated that career development was influenced by personal and structural factors. Individuals needed an enthusiastic attitude to learn, along with specific educational goals that focused on completing higher education or vocational training opportunities. One family succinctly characterized this philosophy thus: “Education is the passport to prosperity.” In the majority of cases (52%), of participants stated that access to scholarships for education was crucial to success. However, such scholarships are becoming limited, due to reduced education funds at both federal and state levels. To compound the situation, parents were frustrated with frequent strikes by lecturers in government-owned post-secondary institutions JoDD 41 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome agitating for better learning climate, as well as improved salaries. Indeed, such instability in higher education imposed serious limitations on opportunities for most students, and this in turn negatively impacted their career prospects. In addition to the high cost of education, it was observed that the curricula in the post-secondary institutions failed to inculcate in students the knowledge and skills needed for selfemployment. The outcome was labour instability with high unemployment rates in general, and for new graduates in particular. There were even fewer work opportunities for older workers and individuals with any identifiable disability. Some employed family members observed that their employers made little or no provision for advanced training on the job. Other inhibitors to career development included the depressed economy and the absence of effective policy to support education, training, and employment. Leisure and Recreation Pursuits In the home, joint leisure and recreation for families with DS focused on watching TV and movies together, listening to music, and reading. Other home-based pursuits included entertaining visitors and cooking. Two-thirds of respondents commented they lack the funds to purchase computer and computer games for their children with ID. In addition, these parents decried their inability to teach basic specialized computer literacy skills. Yet they feel that access to home-based computers and toys can supplement the school learning activities of their children with DS, and promote interaction with siblings and friends. Thirty-nine percent of caregivers described the poor availability and high cost of organized recreational activities in large urban areas. Lack of structured activities for individuals with any form of disability was problematic for all families and reflected the low value placed on disability-related services by society and the government. Low personal and family incomes and the high cost of public transportation further impeded family participation in many formal activities. For some families, caregiving burden and general family demands left no time for leisure and recreation. Community Interaction Respondents indicated that much of their community interaction involved faith based activities – participating in choirs, or doing volunteer activities. They also relied on social interactions with extended family members. Many families were members of the Centre, and service clubs like the YWCA. It was noted by some participants that the Centre periodically organizes community-based events that positively promote the aims of the centre. Such activities included: walkathons, running, and soccer matches. Analysis of Domains and Dimensions A majority of families (85%) indicated their community activities were centred on religiousbased events. However, a small percentage of respondents (19.3%) also spoke of going on family vacations, visiting friends and extended family members in the village, shopping, picnics, and outdoor sports as favorite pastimes. Table 2 provides the means and standard deviations of the Family Quality of Life Survey measures by the six dimensions (Importance, Opportunities, Initiative, Attainment, Stability, and Satisfaction) within each of the nine domains (Health of the family, Financial wellbeing, Family relationships, Support from others, Support from services, Influence of values, Careers or preparing for careers, Leisure and recreation, and Community interaction). In addition, the last two columns display the mean and standard deviation for each Domain Score, that is, the arithmetic mean of the six dimension measures within each domain. Most caregivers (81%) expressed an understanding that leisure pursuits contributed to physical and emotional well-being. They indicated that their leisure choices were based on activities that helped the family to relax, but inclusivity was a factor and the activity was selected to include the member with DS. On average, families gave the least importance to the domains of Support from others (mean response on a scale of 1 to 5=3.55) and Leisure and recreation (3.81), and the most importance to financial well-being (4.87) and Health of family (4.90). The fewest opportunities were associated with Support from services (1.84) v . 18 n .2 42 Ajuwon Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations of FQOLS Responses by Six Dimensions and Nine Domains Importance Oppor tunities Initiative Attainment Stability Satisfaction Domain Score Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Health of family 4.90 0.40 2.06 0.89 3.61 1.28 3.55 1.03 3.65 0.98 3.32 1.14 3.52 0.56 Financial well-being 4.87 0.34 2.39 0.84 4.32 0.94 3.16 1.10 3.48 1.24 3.06 1.18 3.55 0.66 Family relationships 4.65 0.80 4.39 0.62 4.68 0.65 4.48 0.85 4.19 0.91 4.29 1.04 4.45 0.59 Support from others 3.55 1.21 2.33 0.88 2.68 1.17 2.35 0.76 3.19 0.54 3.00 0.89 2.83 0.59 Support from services 4.61 0.62 1.84 0.82 3.97 1.20 2.10 0.94 3.10 0.79 2.84 1.10 3.08 0.54 Influence of values 4.61 0.67 4.13 0.96 4.45 0.77 4.48 0.85 4.23 0.84 4.06 0.96 4.33 0.70 Careers/ preparing for careers 4.77 0.50 2.63 1.19 4.43 0.82 3.86 1.16 3.48 1.06 3.29 0.94 3.75 0.67 Leisure and recreation 3.81 0.98 2.90 0.94 3.52 0.93 3.45 0.96 3.55 0.89 3.29 1.04 3.42 0.76 Community/ civic interaction 3.84 1.00 3.35 0.92 3.35 1.08 3.23 0.92 3.45 0.81 3.52 1.03 3.46 0.77 and Health of family (2.06), while the most opportunities were associated with Influence of values (4.13) and Family relationships (4.39). The least initiative, on average, was taken by families with respect to Support from others (2.68) and Community or civic interaction (3.35), and the most initiative was taken with respect to Influence of values (4.45) and Family relationships (4.68). Support from services (2.10) and Support from others (2.35) were rated the lowest domains for attainment, while Family relationships (4.48) and Influence of values (4.48) were rated the highest. Support from services (3.10) and Support from others (3.19) were perceived to be the least stable domains, and Family relationships (4.19) and Influence of values (4.23) were perceived to be the most stable domains. The least satisfaction was associated with the domains Support from services (2.84) and Support from others (3.00), while the most satisfaction was associated with Influence of values (4.06) and Family relationships (4.29). The bar chart in Figure 1 graphically displays the mean and standard deviation for each of the nine computed Domain Scores in descending order by mean value. As shown, the two highest means are for Family relationships (4.45) and Influence of values (4.33), while the two lowest means are for Support from services (3.08) and Support from others (2.83). Discussion Living with a child with disability in the Nigerian society can have profound effects on the entire family dynamics. In this study, respondents expressed their perspectives as JoDD 43 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome 5.00 Likert Scale Value 4.00 3.00 4.45 4.33 3.75 3.55 3.52 3.46 3.42 3.08 2.00 2.83 1.00 Influence of values Family relationships Financial well-being Careers Community Interaction Health of family Support from services Leisure and recreation Support from others Figure 1. Means and standard deviations of nine FQOLS domain scores they provide meaningfully for their children with DS. Table 2 and Figure 1 data are reasonably comparable with other studies (Brown, 2010; Cagron, Schmidt, & Brown, 2011; Clark, Brown, & Karrapaya, 2012; Werner, Edwards, Baum, et al., 2009). As Brown (2010) points out in a comparison of data from different countries using the same survey in terms of satisfaction and attainment dimensions, for example, the Nigerian data (Ajuwon & Brown, 2012) shows the lowest rating in Support from Others, and Support from Services and, again in terms of consistency with other countries, Family Relations and Health of Family were rated highly, and this is also true of the sub-set of Down data in the present study. However, there are differences, and in the present study values were consistently high on all dimensions which, as pointed out by Brown (2010), may have reflected the importance of religious and spiritual beliefs. Although there were often lev- v . 18 n .2 els of variation between higher and lower economic countries, similar domain patterns tend to exist though complicated by family attitudes and societal values (see Brown, Hong, Shearer, Wang, & Wang, 2011). Discussion also needs to reflect on the variations within domains and dimensions within a country. An average rating below 4 for any domain or dimension suggests that many of the families perceived themselves as having low family scores. Where the scores are below 3, most families fell in this negative range. For example, all of the dimensions associated with the domains covering Support from Others and Support from Services represent aspects that require particular attention both in planning and practical support (Brown, Hong, Shearer, Wang, & Wang, 2011; Wang & Brown, 2009), and this issue is further illustrated in the qualitative information above. 44 Ajuwon It appears that only one other FQOL study has been carried out with families having children with Down syndrome as a separate group (Brown, MacAdam-Crisp, Wang, & Iarocci, 2006). Again, the pattern of results appears reasonably comparable to the present study. However, it would be interesting to know whether in the current study Down families gained higher domain scores in otherwise vulnerable areas in other groups of families where there are multiple disabilities and/or behavioural challenges. It seems possible that social attitudes and environmental opportunities may be important variables in this context. Some of the qualitative comments point to the implications of not having in place concrete special education, health and social policies. This gap in service has impeded overall development and community integration of their children, leading to a decline in most family members’ life satisfaction and well-being. One participant noted: “This is not the kind of lifestyle I wish for members of my household or anyone for that matter … Nigeria has abundant natural resources that can be used to benefit the masses … yet most of its citizens live in poverty because of corruption on the part of some government officials. These officials visit the advanced countries where they see good social programmes in place. But they refuse to see anything good in bringing such practical social ideas to our own land.” Nonetheless, a handful of participants took their son or daughter with DS with them to places of worship, social gatherings, or to the market place. However, teasing of children with DS and accompanying family members when in the public like the market-place or even the neighbourhood water bore-hole was not an isolated event. A number of participants mentioned social isolation because other people avoided them in these public places. A few respondents described denial of retail services because they brought their child with DS to the market-place. Thus, based on such prevailing myths, a vendor of foodstuffs or shoes may view selling her/his merchandise to a buyer accompanied by a child with DS as illluck, especially if the transaction was the first to occur that day. Similarly, a commercial bus operator may disallow passengers with DS the opportunity to ride in the bus, because of the discomfort the presence of the child with DS might cause the passengers. Even at church, one mother observed the uneasiness of other members the first time she went to service with her daughter with DS. The mother noted: “After service that day, I met with the Pastor to talk about my daughter, and the need to include aspects of the condition in future sermons … and as time progressed, and my daughter also began to show improved behaviours, I could see changes in the attitudes and behaviours of the people.” Most Nigerian parents recognized that these negative attitudes stem from superstitions that are rooted in traditional beliefs about disability in some communities (Okoh, 1987), a pressing issue in a number of other countries (Brown, 2010). These superstitions point towards better education about disabilities, and also indicate that positive changes can be made with family members who are willing to assume advocacy roles. From the current FQOLS study, levels of unmet needs were found in relation to health, financial well-being, disability-specific support, careers, and community interaction, and this resulted in caregiver burden for most participants. The daily care situation often prevented family members (especially mothers) from independently engaging in social activities. Excessive care burden resulted in long-term effects of chronic stress on the health status of other family members. Thus, several respondents were concerned about their own future well-being and that of their adult family members with DS. But the positive attitudes of some respondents continue to inspire them to seek training for their children, in spite of the high costs of transportation, health care, and special education from privately operated schools. These families feel that providing meaningful special education to their children with DS is a worthwhile investment, because they view caregiving as a family commitment, and a way to show special joy and love for their children with DS. Some parents of children with DS provide care well in excess of that reported by other parents. They spend several hours on basic care of their children with DS in the evenings and/or week- JoDD 45 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome ends, and unable to access respite care services. This situation ultimately interferes with the ability of caregivers to work and contribute to the household income and/or provide quality care for other siblings, and to aged grandparents who live with them. and their children with DS should expand their focus to include a broader spectrum of the disability groups and their families. A number of currently-employed respondents commented that the skyrocketing inflation was impeding their ability to purchase their own housing and vehicles. One major concern was the families’ constant struggle with high tuition fees for their other children enrolled in privately run tertiary institutions. Furthermore, some participants were dissatisfied that their trained family members could not secure employment after graduation from institutions of higher learning. These educated older siblings without disabilities continued to live at home, unable to augment the household income, further inhibiting the families’ financial capacity to access quality specialized resources. Clearly, these situations severely limited choices for the families and their sons or daughters with DS. It is obvious that unless adequate social protections are established for these families and their children with disabilities, these families will continue to experience declining life satisfaction and wellbeing as their children grow up. In light of the multiple needs expressed by families interviewed, and who also completed the Family Quality of Life survey, the following specific recommendations are made: Limitations of the Project and Recommendations for Future Service One limitation of the current project relates to the samples selection. As previously noted, participants in this study were drawn from the Down Syndrome Foundation of Nigeria, a small centre in the city of Lagos. It should be emphasized that study participants consisted of mothers and fathers who had children with DS at the Centre; hence, the views expressed on family quality of life were those of the participants, and not necessarily those of the rest of the household. As such, the opinions of other family members should be explored in any future research. Although the participants demonstrated sufficient awareness of the impact of DS on the family structure, a larger sample of families from the rest of the country would have provided a more representative data. Future research efforts aimed at exploring Nigerian families v . 18 n .2 Recommendations 1. The DSFN should establish a variety of professional services that focus on developing speech and language skills, appropriate behaviours, specialized pediatric and health care services, and sexuality training of adolescents with DS. Provisions must also be made for in-home assistance including counselling of parents and the siblings of the child with DS. 2. The need for community-based vocational services and computer literacy training should be in the forefront of the Centres’ curriculum planning efforts, especially for young adults. The focus should be to capitalize on indigenous customs that will be inclusive of the individuals with DS, and their families at school, home, and community events. This process should involve the utilization of local resources and manpower to sufficiently prepare trainees with DS for employment and community integration, in the public or private sector, or even as selfemployed individuals. 3. Although participants in this study are a small fraction of the total number of parents of children with disabilities in Nigeria, they represent an exemplary group of advocates who are seeking for their children quality education that will guarantee for them a functional and independent living. In order to achieve this, these parents have demanded respite care to allow them to receive a break from care-giving. This is justifiable because raising a child with a disability is challenging, particularly in a society that has no social safety nets (Brown, Ajuwon, Wang, & Vahakuopus, 2009). As several parents have alluded to in this study, there are many demands placed on families in the process of adjusting to their children with DS, not the 46 Ajuwon least of which are: the number of appointments for medical and support services, challenges of learning how to provide and/ or adapt home supports, and working with school personnel. These families are often over-stressed as they try to juggle medical appointments during work hours and care for the children with a disability and their siblings. Parents are tired and depressed, yet they continue to sacrifice their own health and well-being to meet care-giving obligations of their children. 4. Respondents commented positively on their religious values and how they participate with their daughter or son in activities organized by their worship centres. In this regard, religious centres should capitalize on this and assume a major role in fostering greater acceptance of and respect for persons with disabilities and their families. This view is supported by a number of other studies that point out that meeting child and parent needs, particularly spiritual needs, helps some parents deal with their challenges (Kober, 2011). There are many ways leaders in these places of worship can accomplish this. They can give sermons that highlight the dignity and worth of people, including children, youth, and adults with disabilities. A mosque, a church or a shrine can sponsor the education of a child with DS. They can sponsor educational field-trips or sporting events like the Special Olympics. They can construct classrooms at these special schools, or even fund a child’s medical treatment that is often beyond the means of the family. The ultimate goal would be to identify with, and provide moral support for, programmes appropriate for the chronological age of the special needs learners. 5. The themes describing family experiences that emerged from the participants’ comments in this study indicate a strong need for the development of government disability policies, as well as greater initiatives to train family members or care-givers. In this regard, federal, state, and local authorities must set aside sufficient funds each year to implement new programmes. However, care must be taken to ensure that such programmes are planned and implemented within the community, so members become more cognizant of the difficulties that children with disabilities face, and are empowered to challenge the stereotypes held by the public towards people with disabilities. In conclusion, these recommendations will provide mutual support and joint action in advocating for the families and their offspring with DS that are aimed at improving their overall quality of life. Acknowledgments The author is grateful to Professor Roy I. Brown for his support and invaluable ideas and guidance in expanding the discussion section of this manuscript. I would also like to express my gratitude to Robert Downie and Rebecca Stallings for their advice and the statistical analyses contained in this manuscript. References Ajuwon, P. M., & Brown, I. (2011). Family quality of life in Nigeria. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 56(1), 61–70. Brown, I. (2010). Family quality of life: A comparison of trends in eight countries. In V. Prasher (Ed.), Contemporary Issues in Intellectual Disabilities (pp. 255–264). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. Brown, I., Ajuwon, P. M., Wang, M., & Vahakuopus, S. (2008). Is social inclusion always a good idea? Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 14(2), 5–12. Brown, I., Anand, S., Fung, W. L. A., Isaacs, B., & Baum, N. (2003). Family quality of life: Canadian results from an international study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 15(3), 207–230. Brown, I., Brown, R. I., Baum, N., Isaacs, B. J., Myerscough, T., Neikrug, S., Roth, D., Shearer, J., & Wang, M. (2006). Family quality of life survey: Main caregivers of people with intellectual or developmental disabilities. Toronto, ON, Canada: Surrey Place Centre. JoDD Brown, R. I., Hong, K., Shearer, J., Wang, M., & Wang, S. (2011). Family quality of life in several countries: Results and discussion of satisfaction in families where there is a child with a disability. In R. Kober (Ed.), Enhancing the quality of life of people with intellectual disability: From theory to practice (pp. 377–398). Dordrecht, SH, Netherlands: Springer. Brown, R. I., MacAdam-Crisp, J., Wang, M., & Iarocci, G. (2006). Family quality of life when there is a child with a developmental disability. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(4), 238–246. Cagron, B., Schmidt, M., & Brown, I. (2011). Assessment of the quality of life in families with children who have intellectual and developmental disabilities in Slovenia. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55(1), 1164–1175. Clark, M., Brown, R., & Karrapaya, R. (2012). An initial look at the quality of life of Malaysian families that include children with disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 56(1), 45–60. Dike, V. E. (2008). Corruption in Nigeria: A new paradigm for effective control. Retrieved from http://www.africaeconomicanalysis.org/ articles/gen/corruptiondikehtm.html Down Syndrome Foundation of Nigeria. Retrieved from http://dsanigeria.org Family Quality of Life Survey. Retrieved from http://www.surreyplace.on.ca/Educationand-Research/research-and-evaluation/ Pages/International-Family-Quality-ofLifeProject.aspx Isaacs, B. J., Brown, I., Brown, R. I., Baum, N., Myerscough, T., Neikrug, S., Roth, D., Shearer, J., & Wang, M. (2007). The international family quality of life project: Goals and description of a survey tool. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 4(3), 177–185. v . 18 n .2 47 Nigerian Families Who Have a Family Member with Down Syndrome Kober, R. (2011). Enhancing the quality of life of people with intellectual disabilities: From theory to practice. Dordrecht, SH, Netherlands: Springer. Nigeria Misses Africa’s Target for Health Budget. Retrieved from http:// www.nigerianbestforum.com/ generaltopics/?p=85074 Okoh, G. (1987). Mental handicap in Nigeria: A study of attitudes in Bendel State. Mental Handicap, 15(1), 35–36. Sulaiman, S. (2008, April 12). Rich nation, poor citizens: The missing links for increasing output and alleviating poverty in Nigeria. Africa Economic Analysis. Retrieved from http://www.africaeconomicanalysis.org/ authors/30/Sa%E2%80%99idu-Sulaiman Wang, M., & Brown, R. I. (2009). Family quality of life: A framework for policy and social service provisions to support families of children with disabilities. Journal of Family Social Work, 12(2), 144–167. Werner, S., Edwards, M., Baum, N., Brown, L., Brown, R. I., & Isaacs, B. J. (2009). Family quality of life among families with a member who has an intellectual disability: An exploratory examination of key domains and dimensions of the revised FQOL survey. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53(6), 501–511. Note: At time of first publication of this article, minor changes were pending. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/ index.php?page=756 after August 15, 2012. Relations of Parental Perceptions to the Behavioural Characteristics of Adolescents with Down Syndrome Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Meghan M. Burke,1 Marisa H. Fisher,2 Robert M. Hodapp1 Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Nashville, TN 1 University of Massachusetts, Boston Center for Social Development and Education, Boston, MA 2 Correspondence meghan.m.burke @vanderbilt.edu Keywords Down syndrome, adolescence, coping, transition, maladaptive behaviours Abstract This study examined how the behaviours of individuals with Down syndrome relate to parent functioning during the adolescent years. Measures of personality, intelligence, adaptive, and maladaptive behaviour were collected for 42 adolescents with Down syndrome and related to parental depression, ways of coping, worries about the future, and positive perceptions. Across the adolescent years, most parents continued to feel rewarded by their adolescents with Down syndrome; adolescents who displayed more positive personality characteristics had parents who felt more rewarded by their children. Conversely, those who displayed higher levels of internalizing (but not externalizing) problem behaviours had parents who were significantly more worried about their adolescents’ futures. Implications are discussed. Many studies of families of individuals with Down syndrome (DS) have identified a “Down syndrome advantage” (Hodapp, Ly, Fidler, & Ricci, 2001; Seltzer & Ryff, 1994). This perspective holds that, compared to families of children with other intellectual and developmental disabilities, families of individuals with DS cope better. Compared to these other families, then, families of individuals with DS tend to be warmer (Mink, Nihira, & Meyers, 1983), mothers experience less stress (Hodapp, Ricci, Ly, & Fidler, 2003), fathers report close relationships (Hornby, 1995), and siblings report increased amounts of positive benefits (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). While the “Down syndrome advantage” does seem to exist, the reasons for its existence continue to be debated. Some argue that closer family dynamics may relate to the personality phenotype of the child. Specifically, children with DS often have fairly low levels of behaviour problems, more positive personalities (Dykens & Kasari, 1997; Kasari, Mundy, Yirmiya, & Sigman, 1990), and, compared to individuals with other types of intellectual disabilities, often have less difficult temperaments (Kasari & Sigman, 1997). Conversely, advantages for families may be due primarily to demographic characteristics. On average, parents of children with DS tend to be older (Urbano & Hodapp, 2007), to hold more senior job positions (Corrice & Glidden, 2009), and to be more affluent (Grosse, 2010). In some studies, controlling for such demographic variables reduced the parental rewards of raising children with DS (Corrice & Glidden, 2009). To date, most studies have been conducted with families of younger children with DS (Hodapp, 2007), with little focus 49 Adolescents with Down Syndrome on adolescents. But just as for individuals without disabilities, adolescence is a difficult time for many individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Moreover, across the adolescent years, age may be related to subtle increases in internalizing behaviours for many individuals with DS (Dykens & Kasari, 1997; Dykens, Shah, Sagun, Beck, & King, 2002). Even if, when compared to those without DS, adolescents with DS increase in internalizing behaviours across adolescence, we do not yet know how internalizing behaviours affect others. Do, for example, the increased maladaptive behaviours of adolescents with DS affect parental reactions and their concerns about their child’s transition to adulthood? Alternatively, certain (mostly unchanging) demographic characteristics may continue to explain the “Down syndrome advantage” across adolescence for individuals with DS. This study related parent perceptions to the adolescents’ personality, behaviours, and IQ. Specifically, this study cross-sectionally examined: (1) the personality, demographic, and behavioural characteristics of adolescents with DS; (2) parental coping styles, feelings of rewards, positive perceptions, and worries for the future; and, (3) the connections among the adolescent’s characteristics and parental responses, positive perceptions, and transition concerns. Method Participants This study included 42 adolescents with DS (26 M; 16 F) and their parents (3 M; 39 F). Most participants were White (90.2%), and the remaining participants were AfricanAmerican. Adolescents averaged 15.12 years of age (SD = 2.82) with a range from 11–21 years. All adolescents were in the mild to moderate range of intellectual disability, with a mean IQ of 45.93 (SD = 6.77; range from 40 to 61). The mean standard score for verbal knowledge was 48.93 (SD = 9.38) and for non-verbal knowledge was 50.05 (SD = 9.78). Of those who were still in school, most adolescents (74.2%) were educated in a general education setting, while the remainder attended self-contained special education classes. Over half (62.1%) had repeated v . 18 n .2 a grade in school and parents reported that their adolescents had an average of 1.81 friends (SD = 1.13). The mean age of the mothers was 48.18 (SD = 5.94) and of fathers was 50.36 (SD = 6.67). Mothers worked an average of 21.49 hours (SD = 19.6) outside of the home and fathers 42.31 hours (SD = 15.32). Family size averaged 2.83 (SD = 1.0) children and, in 73.1% of families, the mother was married to the father of the adolescent. Measures Adolescents were administered the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990). A psychometric measure for individuals aged 4–90 years. The K-BIT includes a composite IQ as well as standard scores for verbal and for non-verbal knowledge. The K-BIT has been used with persons with intellectual disabilities (Dykens, Rosner, & Ly, 2001). Parents also completed two packets of questionnaires, one of adolescent-related measures and another of parent-related measures. Adolescent-Related Measures The Wishart Scale (Wishart & Johnston, 1990) consists of 23 commonly-mentioned personality characteristics of individuals with DS. Parents rated each personality trait on a 5-point Likert scale, with possible scores ranging from 23 to 115 (higher scores denote individuals who are more outgoing and sociable). For this sample, Cronbach’s alpha equaled .832. Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, 1991) includes 112 items concerning maladaptive behaviours, each rated from 0 to 2 (0 = not present; 1 = somewhat or sometimes true; 2 = very true or often). The CBCL consists of two broad-band factors (Internalizing, Externalizing), which are composed of eight narrow-band factors. Higher scores indicate more maladaptive behaviours. A reliable and valid measure of maladaptive behaviour in children and adolescents, the CBCL has been used with individuals with intellectual disabilities (Dykens & Kasari, 1997). 50 Burke et al. The Reiss Profiles of Fundamental Goals and Motivational Sensitivities for Persons with Mental Retardation (Reiss Profiles) (Reiss & Havercamp, 1998) assesses motivational strengths and styles of individuals with intellectual disabilities. Respondents rated the adolescent on 100 statements along a 5-point scale (1 – strongly disagree, not at all characteristic of person to 5 – strongly agree, definitely characteristic). Although the Reiss Profiles have 15 factors, our interests were primarily in factors relating to five positive and social constructs (independence – desire to make own decisions; curiosity – desire to learn; help others – desire to help friends; morality – desire to behave in accordance with a code of conduct; and social contact – the desire to be in the company of others). Parent-Related Measures The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) measures symptoms associated with depression (e.g., sadness, guilt, self-dislike, crying, insomnia). Respondents rated the severity in which they experience each symptom (0 to 3). Ranging from 0 to 63, scores above 10 fall within the clinical range. Positive Perceptions in Families Questionnaire (PPFQ) assesses how rewarded the parent feels about having a child with a disability. Six statements are rated on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree). Statements include: “I enjoy and feel satisfaction in my role as a parent of a child with a disability”; “Being a special parent has strengthened my marriage and our family”; and “I feel like I have my priorities straight because I am a special parent.” Ranging from 6 to 24, higher scores indicate that parents feel more rewarded (alpha = .887). Kansas Inventory of Parental PerceptionsPositive Contributions Section (KIPP-PC) (Behr, Murphy, & Summers, 1992) assesses how much the parent feels the adolescent with a disability positively contributes to the family. Respondents rated 41 statements on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 4 = strongly agree). In this study, we used seven (of nine) KIPP-PC subscales: (1) Learning through experience with special problems (e.g., “My child is responsible for my increased awareness of people with special needs”; alpha = .679); (2) Happiness and fulfillment (e.g., “Because of my child, I have many unexpected pleasures”; alpha = .673); (3) Personal strength and family closeness (e.g., “Because of my child, our family has become closer”; alpha = .856); (4) Personal growth and maturity (e.g., “My child is the reason why I am a more responsible person”; alpha = .833); (5) Awareness of future issues (e.g., “I consider my child to be what makes me realize the importance of planning for my family’s future”; alpha = .616); (6) Expanded social network (e.g., “My child is why I met some of my best friends”; alpha = .815); and (7) Career or job growth (e.g., “The presence of my child is an inspiration to improve my job skills”; alpha = .613). COPE Questionnaire (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989) is a 53-item index used to assess different coping styles. Respondents indicate how much they use a particular behaviour when they experience stressful events in their lives (1 = I usually don’t do this at all to 4 = I usually do this a lot). Lyne and Roger (2000) identified three coping factors: (1) Rational-Active Coping (accept that a stressful situation occurred; “I try to come up with a strategy about what to do”); (2) Emotional Coping (express feelings and seek emotional support; “I try to get emotional support from friends and relatives”); and, (3) Avoidance-Helplessness Coping (behavioural disengagement and denial; “I act as though it hasn’t even happened”). Scores range from 18 to 72 for Rational-Active Coping (alpha = .838), from 8 to 32 for Emotional Coping (.869), and from 11 to 44 for Avoidance-Helplessness Coping (.757). Transition Daily Rewards and Worries Questionnaire (TDRWQ) (Glidden & Jobe, 2007) is a 28-item questionnaire measuring parental rewards and worries as their adolescents transition to adulthood. Respondents indicated their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating more rewards and fewer worries. Questionnaire factors include: Positive Future Orientation (general feelings about the child’s future; “I am optimistic about my child’s future”; alpha = .682); Community Resources (feelings toward the child’s job preparation and independent living; “I believe that there are a lot of resources available in my child’s community”; .672); and Financial Independence (perception of the child’s likelihood of becoming financially self-supporting; “I worry that my child will never be self-supporting”; .704). An additional domain, Family JoDD 51 Adolescents with Down Syndrome Relationship with Siblings, was not used in this study, and a final domain, Family Relations, was not used because its component items did not form a single scale (alpha = -.353). Procedure Participants were recruited through DS support groups in Tennessee and Illinois. Interested parents either brought their adolescent with DS to the research lab or the researchers traveled to a location where the assessments could be completed (e.g., home; hotel conference room). Prior to the visit, parents were mailed a packet of adolescent-related measures to complete; during the visit, a research assistant conducted the K-BIT with the adolescent, while parents completed the parent-related measures. Results Characteristics of Adolescents with Down Syndrome On average, parents rated these adolescents as having fairly social, outgoing personalities and relatively low levels of behaviour problems. Wishart Scale scores averaged 91.20 (SD = 9.98), significantly above the scale’s mid-point (i.e., 69), t (40) = 8.47, p < .001. Similarly, most adolescents did not present many behavioural problems. On CBCL internalizing behaviours, 87.2% of adolescents scored in the normal range and 12.8% in the clinical range. Among specific narrow-band factors, thought problems showed the highest percentage of adolescents (23%) in the clinical range. Similarly for externalizing behaviours, 92.3% of adolescents scored in the normal range and 7.7% scored in the clinical range. For total behaviour problems as well, 84.6% scored in the normal range and 15.4% in the clinical range. No significant relations emerged between the participant’s age or IQ and personality or maladaptive behaviours. Characteristics of Parents While adolescents with DS generally showed few problems, the reactions and perceptions of their parents were more mixed. On the positive side, parents held generally positive perceptions about raising an adolescent with v . 18 n .2 Down syndrome. Compared to parents of children with various disabilities who answered about the positive contributions in the original KIPP-PC study (Behr et al., 1992), parents of these adolescents with DS scored higher on all but two of the 7 KIPP-PC sub-scales. Thus, whereas the original norming group scored slightly higher on the KIPP-PC’s Happiness and Fulfillment domain, t(1302) = -3.59, p < .01, and no group differences emerged on Personal Growth and Maturity, t(1302) = 0.94, ns, parents of these adolescents with Down syndrome scored higher than did the norming group on Learning Through Experience with Special Problems, t(1302) = 3.33, p < .01; Personal Strength and Family Closeness, t(1302) = 1.98, p < .05; Awareness of Future Issues, t(1302) = 2.19, p < .05; Expanded Social Network, t(1302) = 4.91, p < .001; and Career or Job Growth, t(1302) = 6.68, p < .001. Second, most parents showed high levels of Rational Coping (considered the best type of coping) and relatively low levels of Avoidant Coping. Compared to a mean on the COPE’s Rational-Coping Domain of 33.29 (SD = 8.74) for Lyne and Roger’s (2000) community-based sample, parents of these adolescents with DS scored 55.75 (SD = 6.57), t(519) = 16.02, p < .001. Conversely, compared to a community-sample average of 33.70 (SD = 4.74) on the COPE’s Avoidance-Helplessness Coping domain, parents of these adolescents scored much lower, at 15.78 (SD = 3.78), t(550) = 30.53, p < .001. No differences emerged between the two groups on the COPE’s Emotional Coping factor, nor were there significant correlations in the DS group among the three coping styles. Parents displaying higher levels of Rational-Active Coping felt more rewarded (PPFQ) and perceived their child as contributing more positively to their family within areas such as Learning Through Experience with Special Problems, Personal Strength and Family Closeness, and Awareness of Future Issues (see Table 1). Third, parents’ depression scores averaged significantly below the cutoff for mild depression, t(38) = -6.87, p < .001; several parents, however, experienced higher levels of depressive symptoms. Overall, the sample’s BDI averaged 6.17 (SD = 7.12), with a range of 0 to 35. While most parents (71.1%) were not depressed, nine (23.7%) were mild/moderately depressed (scores 52 Burke et al. Table 1. Means of Parental Perceptions of Rewards and Contributions and Transitions, and correlations with Depression and Coping Styles M (SD) PPFQ (Rewards) BDI Rational Active Coping Style 19.90 (3.62) -.45** .34* Learning Through Experience with Special Problems 3.53 (0.46) -.01 .54** Happiness and Fulfillment 3.10 (0.57) -.12 .31 Personal Strength and Family Closeness 3.23 (0.58) -.34* .49** Awareness of Future Issues 3.11 (0.64) -.04 .43** Personal Growth and Maturity 2.74 (0.68) -.28 .29 Expanded Social Network 3.00 (0.65) -.26 .24 Career or Job Growth 2.96 (0.51) -.18 .40* Positive Future Orientation 3.08 (0.72) -.10 -.37* Financial Independence 2.27 (0.93) -.22 .04 Community Resources 2.40 (0.71) .10 -.07 KIPP-PC (Positive Family Contribution) TDRWQ (Transition Worries/Rewards) between 10.2 and 15.1) and two (5.2%) showed moderate-to-severe depressive symptoms (scores = 20.4 and 35.3). While no significant relations occurred between parental depression and parental perceptions of the child’s positive contributions (r’s of BDI to KIPP-PC factors = -.34 to -.01, ns), parental depression scores were negatively related to parental feelings of adolescent rewardingness (BDI to PPFQ). See Table 1. Finally, parents of adolescents with DS characterized certain transitions as rewards or worries. Parents felt most concerned about their adolescent’s future financial independence (2.27; SD = .93) and community resources (2.40; SD = .71). Relations Between Parent and Adolescent Variables Demographic and functional skills. No significant correlations emerged between the adolescent’s age, gender, or IQ score and parental depression (BDI), coping styles (3 COPE factors), rewardingness (PPFQ), perceptions of adolescent’s positive contributions (KIPP-PC), or transition rewards or worries (TDRWQ). Adolescent Behaviour Problems. Parents’ depression scores were positively related to the adolescent’s Internalizing behaviours and, to a lesser extent, to total behaviour problems (see Table 2). Parental BDI scores were also related to the CBCL narrow-band domains of Thought Problems, r = .59, p < .01, Withdrawn/ Depressed, r = .50, p < .01, Social Problems, r = .43, p < .01, and Somatic Complaints, r = .37, p < .05. Consistent relations also emerged between the adolescent’s higher amounts of internalizing problems and parental worries about the adolescent’s future. Specifically, those parents who judged their adolescents to have more Internalizing problems rated themselves lower on TDRWQ Positive Future Orientation. Such parental concerns about the child’s future were significantly related to all Internalizing narrow-band domains (r’s from -.56, with Somatic Complaints, to -.42, with Thought Problems; p’s < .02). Negative correla- JoDD 53 Adolescents with Down Syndrome Table 2. Correlations between the adolescent’s level of behaviour problems and parent depression, feelings of reward, feelings of adolescent contribution, and transition worries or rewards Internalizing Problems Externalizing Problems Total Problems BDI .425** .130 .437** PPFQ .038 -.129 -.116 Learning through experience .175 .042 .088 Happiness and fulfillment .018 -.063 -.055 Strength and family closeness .029 -.015 -.035 Awareness about future issues .070 .067 .042 Personal growth and maturity .034 -.005 -.029 Expanded social network -.117 .017 -.124 Career/job growth .065 .103 .065 Positive Orientation -.618** -.160 -.395* Community Resources -.088 .145 -.021 Financial Independence -.373* -.297 -.397* KIPP-PC TDRWQ tions were also found between the adolescent Internalizing domain (and overall problems) and Financial Independence (see Table 2). No relations emerged between parental scores on the TDRWQ and the adolescent’s level of Externalizing problems. Personality. Parental perceptions were related to higher levels of positive—but not of negative— personality characteristics of the adolescent with DS. Specifically, the degree to which parents perceived their child as a source of reward (PPFQ) increased for parents who rated their adolescent higher on independence, curiosity, morality, and social contact. Parents reported less depression when their adolescents were more moral and independent. Similarly, as Table 3 shows, parents who rated their child higher on many of the more positive personality characteristics also viewed their adolescent as contributing more to their family’s happiness/fulfillment, strength, learning, closeness, growth, expanded social network, and even career/job growth (KIPPPC). Few correlations emerged between parental perceptions and any of the negative Reiss v . 18 n .2 factors; nor were there consistent correlations of either positive or negative adolescent personality characteristics and worries or rewards related to transition. There were also no relations between parent depression, reward, or family contributions and parents’ ratings of their child on the Wishart Scales (r’s = -.26 to .30, ns). Discussion Although for many decades individuals with Down syndrome and their families have been the subject of study, we continue to know little about how adolescent characteristics relate to parental reactions. As such, this study provides additional information about adolescents with DS, their parents, and the interplay between characteristics of adolescents with DS and their parents in relation to coping styles, transition, and positive effects. This study produced four main findings. First, as a group, adolescents with DS remained friendly and cheerful during their teenage 54 Burke et al. Table 3. Correlations Between Positive Reiss Personality Factors and Parent Depression, Feelings of Reward, and Perceptions of the Adolescent’s Positive Contribution to the Family Independence Curiosity Help Others Morality Social Contact BDI -.381* -.333* -.089 -.349* -.261 PPFQ .432** .422** .256 .406** .391* .483** .366* .352* .220 .369* Happiness/Fulfillment .346* .440** .294 .385* .383* Strength/Family Closeness .551** .540** .292 .363* .397* Awareness about Future Issues .570** .485** .397* .413** .354* Growth/Maturity .408** .485** .351* .417** .345* Expanded Social Network .315* .265 .082 .104 .376* Career/Job Growth .514** .406** .391* .440** .410** KIPP-PC Learning Through Experience years. While adolescence is often a difficult time for parents and their children, most parents of adolescents with DS continue to view these children as happy, affectionate, and with few internalizing or externalizing maladaptive behaviours. While internalizing problems may become more common as individuals with DS get older (Dykens, et al., 2002; Stores, Stores, Fellows, & Buckley, 1998; Tonge & Einfeld, 2003), these problems were not yet pervasive or of clinical significance in this sample. Second, we noted the importance of active, rational coping for these parents. Even compared to Lyne and Roger’s (2000) community group, parents of adolescents with DS more often endorsed a style of coping that was active and rational, less often one that relied on avoidance. Levels of rational coping, in turn, related to feelings of reward, both in relation to parenting the adolescent and from the benefits that parents perceived that child as bringing to the family. Prior studies have also noted that an active coping style relates to parents’ feelings of reward and positive contributions (Mak & Ho, 2007), and that avoidant coping styles were not related to positive perceptions. Hastings and Taunt (2002) suggested that a focus on posi- tive perceptions as coping resources could be used as an intervention to help families adapt to raising a child with disabilities. Those parents with more positive perceptions could also help parents who are struggling, in that contact with parents who have a positive attitude may make other parents’ attitudes more positive, thus helping them cope more effectively. Third, we noticed an intriguing pattern of relations between child negative and positive behaviours and parental perceptions-reactions. Thus, even though adolescent levels of internalizing problems were low, did not relate to age, and few participants showed clinical levels of CBCL Internalizing problems, such problems were worrisome to parents. Indeed, adolescent levels of Internalizing problems were related to increased parental depression, and greater worries (versus rewards) concerning their adolescents’ positive future outcomes and financial independence. Conversely, parents feel that they and their families benefit when their children are more independent, social, moral and curious. Such connections varied slightly with different measures, but—just as parents are worried about internalizing or “inwardness” of their children— JoDD they seem rewarded when the child shows more positive personality characteristics. Fourth, several expected relations did not occur. In contrast to the large (over 200) cross-sectional sample noted in Dykens et al. (2002), we did not not find increases in CBCL Internalizing problems with increased adolescent age. Thus, even though parents worried about the inwardness of their children, levels did not seem to increase over age. Most likely such changes were more subtle and hard to detect with our smaller sample. In addition, few connections emerged between CBCL Externalizing problems and parental reactions of any type. From these data, then, Internalizing problems most affect parents; indeed, even the hint of a slightly less sociable, more inward-directed personality in their children seemed to influence parents. While it offers many insights, this study nevertheless has certain limitations. Specifically, our sample was mostly comprised of highly educated, White parents; as such, these results may not be generalizable to families from different cultural and racial backgrounds. As a crosssectional examination, we also provided only a snapshot of families of adolescents with DS; we thus cannot determine how parental perceptions and coping styles may have changed over time. Finally, with the exception of the K-BIT IQ test, parents served as the reporters of the child’s personality-maladaptive behaviours and of their own reactions. Still, this study constitutes a first step in describing families of adolescents with DS. Although research is needed using multiple time points, reporters, and contrast groups, this study sheds light on both adolescents with DS and their parents. Ultimately, if our goal is to support parents to help their adolescents, we need to understand more fully the characteristics of individuals with DS; of parental coping, depression, and rewards; and the connections between offspring and parental characteristics over the adolescent years. v . 18 n .2 55 Adolescents with Down Syndrome References Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry, Burlington, VT. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, C. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression InventoryII. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Behr, S. K., Murphy, D. L., & Summers, J. A. (1992). User’s Manual: Kansas Inventory of Parental Perceptions (KIPP). Beach Center on Families and Disability: Lawrence, KS. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283. Corrice, A. M., & Glidden, L. M. (2009). The Down syndrome advantage: Fact or fiction? American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114, 254–268. Dykens, E. M., & Kasari, C. (1997). Maladaptive behavior in children with Prader-Willi syndrome, Down syndrome, and non-specific mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 228–237. Dykens, E. M., Rosner, B. A., & Ly, T. M. (2001). Drawings by individuals with Williams syndrome: Are people different from shapes? American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 94–107. Dykens, E. M., Shah, B., Sagun, J., Beck, T., & King, B. H. (2002). Maladaptive behavior in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 484–492. Glidden, L. M., & Jobe, B. M. (2007). Measuring parental daily rewards and worries in the transition to adulthood. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 275–288. Grosse, S. D. (2010). Sociodemographic characteristics of families of children with Down syndrome and the economic impacts of child disability on families. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 39, 257–294. Hastings, R. P., & Taunt, H. M. (2002). Positive perceptions in families of children with developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 116–127. 56 Burke et al. Hodapp, R. M., Ly, T. M., Fidler, D. J., & Ricci, L. A. (2001). Less stress, more rewarding: Parenting children with Down syndrome. Parenting: Science & Practice, 1, 317–337. Hodapp, R. M., Ricci, L. A., Ly, T. M., & Fidler, D. J. (2003). The effects of the child with Down syndrome on maternal stress. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 137–151. Hodapp, R. M. (2007). Families of persons with Down syndrome: New perspectives, findings, and research and service needs. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 13, 279–287. Hodapp, R. M., & Urbano, R. C. (2007). Adult siblings of individuals with Down syndrome: Findings from a large-scale American survey. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 1018–1029. Hornby, G. (1996). Fathers’ views of the effects on their families of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Family Studies, 4, 103–117. Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1997). Linking parental perceptions to interactions in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental disorders, 27, 39–57. Kasari, C., Mundy, P., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1990). Affect and attention in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95, 55–67. Kaufman A. S., & Kaufman N. L. (1990) Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Lyne, K., & Roger, D. (2000). A psychometric re-assessment of the COPE questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 321–335. Mak, W. W. S., & Ho, G. S. M. (2007). Caregiving perceptions of Chinese mothers of children with intellectual disability in Hong Kong. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 20, 145–156. Mink, I., Nihira, C., & Meyers, C. (1983). Taxonomy of family life styles: Independent homes with TMR children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 87, 484–497. Reiss, S., & Havercamp, M. (1998). Toward a comprehensive assessment of fundamental motivation: Factor structure of the Reiss Profiles. Psychological Assessment, 10, 97–106. Seltzer, M. M., & Ryff, C. D. (1994). Parenting across the life span: The normative and nonnormative cases. Life Span Development and Behavior, 12, 1–40. Stores, R., Stores, G., Fellows, B., & Buckley, S. (1998). Daytime behavior problems and maternal stress in children with Down’s syndrome, their siblings, and non-intellectually disabled and other intellectually disabled peers. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 228–237. Tonge, B. J., & Einfeld, S. L. (2003). Psychopathology and intellectual disability: The Australian child to adult longitudinal study. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 26, 61–91. Urbano, R. C., & Hodapp, R. M. (2007). Divorce in families of children with Down syndrome: A population-based study. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 261–274. Wishart, J. G., & Johnston, F. (1990). The effects of experience on attribution of a stereotyped personality to children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 34, 409–420. Note: At time of first publication of this article, minor changes were pending. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/ index.php?page=757 after August 15, 2012. JoDD The Middle Years and Beyond: Transitions and Families of Adults with Down Syndrome Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Nancy S. Jokinen,1 Matthew P. Janicki,2 Mary Hogan,3 Lawrence T. Force4 1 University of Northern British Columbia, Prince George, BC 2 University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL 3 Eliot, ME 4 Mt. Saint Mary College, Newburgh, NY Correspondence jokinenn@unbc.ca Keywords aging, Down syndrome, families, intellectual disability, transition Abstract Normally expected transitions connect the various periods of life. Often these transitions are prompted by life events that require adaptation to a changed circumstance and may challenge both individual and family quality of life. Such transitions may be planful (proactive) or demand (reactive). Little, however, has been written about the nature of such transitions and how they specifically affect older-aged families of adults with Down syndrome. Such families are often predominate lifelong carers of adults with Down syndrome. Drawing on research and experience, the authors examined three transition points from a family perspective. Each of these points of change requires that people adapt and may lead to various outcomes, including at times outcomes that are unexpected, stressful, and challenging. The three points of transition examined include moving away from the parental home, changes occurring within a residential service (e.g., staff changes, relocations), and the reactions to the onset and course of dementia. Vignettes and quotes illustrate the complexities of these transitions and show that, even with planful management, often such transitions can go awry and produce unpredictable outcomes. Worldwide population profiles indicate that life expectancy is increasing, particularly in the more affluent nations. This “longevity shift” has broad implications for the various segments of societies, including older individuals with disabilities, their family members, and the nature of programs within the aging and disability networks. As people continue to experience an extension in lifespan, theoretical models, practice interventions, and service delivery systems will need to be cognizant of the intersection between age, disability, and disease. Nowhere is this more relevant than to the expanding longevity of adults with Down syndrome (Percy, Summers, & Lovering, 2011). Societies have deeply rooted notions about independence, self-reliance, family responsibilities, and of the role of government policies that may help shape support provided to individuals within a range of dependent populations, including people with disabilities and older persons, and their families. Among societies, the need for and availability of services and supports varies significantly. While many developed countries have produced diverse systems of social services in attempts to meet needs, other countries have few if any formal social services. Many countries and their governments are also struggling with economic woes while attempting to meet the demands of an aging population. In the meantime, 58 Jokinen et al. there is a growing recognition of the important role and place of family caregiving. Indeed, across the life span, family members predominately provide support for most people (young or old, with Down syndrome or other disabilities) (Braddock, Hemp, & Rizzolo, 2008). The individualized nature and context of familial caregiving (e.g., individual attributes, culture, location, and policies that support or impede family care), however, are necessary considerations in designing effective interventions to support families in their many and varied roles (Dilworth-Anderson, Hilliard, Williams, & Palmer, 2011). Transitions are basically a movement through various life stages or notable life-course events that create some level of insecurity and require individuals to adapt (Golan, 1981, as cited in Ray, Bernard, & Phillips, 2009, p. 99). With increasing age, transitions can be expected to be marked by progressively more complex and socially significant life events that may include a change in residence, dealing with acute ill health or the onset of chronic conditions, and loss or death of family and friends. Processes associated with such transitions may be planful (proactive) or demand (reactive). Planful transitions often have more positive outcomes, since the stage for change has been set and the actors are prepared. Such transitions may include finding a new home, changing work, being drawn into a new circle of friends, and taking on new life responsibilities. Demand transitions may have unpredictable or adverse outcomes, since the transitional change is often precipitous or generally unexpected. Such transitions may include being injured or acquiring an unexpected disease or condition, having to relocate precipitously or without much warning, and the withdrawal of familiar supports. All such transitional events impact individuals as well as their families – requiring adjustments to relationships, routines, and long held roles and responsibilities. Many older-aged adults and families apparently have a preference to “age in place” -- to maintain their current living arrangements for as long as possible (Jokinen, 2008). However, as individuals age, support may be required in order to maintain current living arrangements and families are often forced to consider alternative arrangements. While older-aged adults and their fami- lies are a diverse group with varying needs, many manage such transition periods without seeking supports from formal services. However, supports obtained through social connections, individual self-care habits, and psycho-social resources can ease some transitions. Individuals and/or their families may, therefore, only come forward requesting assistance when a situation becomes problematic (Denton & Kusch, 2006). Despite the growing body of evidence on family caregiving, however, there is still much to learn about family dynamics and involvement in various periods of transition (e.g., who becomes involved, range of tasks, differing needs) (Gitlin & Wolff, 2011). With respect to middle-aged and older adults with Down syndrome, life expectancy has increased substantially since the mid 1950s. At birth, the current cohort of middle-aged and older adults with Down syndrome (those in their fifties and older) was often not expected to survive much beyond early adulthood (Bittles & Glassen, 2004). Yet with advances in social and health care, many adults with Down syndrome are now living to ages previously unseen (Janicki, Dalton, Henderson, & Davidson, 1999; Percy et al., 2011). Although most do not attain the longevity averages for men and women in the general population, some middle-aged and older adults with Down syndrome may outlive their parents, siblings, or other close relatives. These adults were born in an era when professionals often suggested to parents that they institutionalize their sons or daughters, in part because community supports were limited at the time. Nonetheless, many parents chose to raise their child with Down syndrome within the family alongside their other children. Commonly, in those early years, families faced and endured negative public attitudes and opinions about Down syndrome. Seeking out mutual support and solutions to the dilemmas they faced, some of these families pioneered the first community services for persons with Down syndrome and other disabilities. Now, these same families are older-aged and experiencing inevitable age-related challenges and transitions. Generally, the literature on transitions and adults with Down syndrome is somewhat divided. One focus has been on younger-aged adults transitioning from educational to adult JoDD services and employment (see as examples Bouck, 2012; Pueschel, 2006), and another focus has been on the health problems or decline in health of older adults (e.g., Torr, Strydom, Patti, & Jokinen, 2010), or neuropathologies, (e.g., Janicki, Zendell, & DeHaven, 2010). Patti, Amble and Flory (2005) reported that adults with Down syndrome aged 50 to 59 experienced more life events compared to similarly aged adults with other intellectual disabilities. These life events included changes in living circumstance and/or day activity or program routines; death of, or separation from, a parent, friend, or roommate; and the onset of medical problems. Additionally, adults aging with Down syndrome were reportedly more likely to be in jeopardy of referral to a nursing home in later life – which raised the question what impact such relocations have on the individual (Patti, Amble, & Flory, 2010). Lunsky and Elserafi (2011) found that specific life events experienced by persons with intellectual disability were associated with increased hospital emergency department visits in reaction to a crisis. Furthermore, life events that prompt transitions likely have adverse impact on adults with intellectual disability and services may be able to control some events (e.g., staff changes and relocations), thereby mitigating emotional distress (Hulbert-Williams & Hastings, 2008). Grant, Nolan and Keady (2003) acknowledged that individuals and families access various services throughout life and the impact of these transitions is not well understood. A dominant theme in the literature on older-aged families of adults with intellectual disability, including those with Down syndrome, is the need for parents (and families) to establish future plans. This is often viewed as a means to ease transitions and avoid the co-occurrence of two significant adverse life events (the death of a parent and need for relocation). While various planning models have been developed (see as examples Baxley, Janicki, McCallion, & Zendell, 2005; Bigby, 2004), commonly these planning processes highlight the needs of the adult who has a disability and promote a move from the family home prior to the incapacity of the main carer, usually the parent. Yet, although there is an underlying assumption that alternative living arrangements may be readily available, parents often lack information about housing options and few studies have reported fam- v . 18 n .2 59 Transitions and Older-Aged Families ily perspectives on this type of move (Gilbert, Lankshear, & Petersen, 2008). The literature also points to a reluctance for older-aged parents to preplan for transitions or engage disability services (Bigby, 2004), yet planning processes seem to give little attention to the family as a whole with limited regard for the impact such plans have on other family members (Jokinen, 2008). Miettinen (2012) suggested that government policies and reduced social service funding have both direct and indirect influences on family decisions about the future. Limited access to housing alternatives and the quality of services provided seem to reinforce and prolong family caregiving potentially to the detriment of parents and their adult offspring. It is within this context that we assess three transitions often encountered by older adults with Down syndrome and their families; some are typical and some are atypical. First, we consider the circumstances and perceptions of relocation when adults with Down syndrome move from the parental home where they have co-resided since birth. Next, we consider the transitions associated with changes in residential services (i.e., staff turnover and/or moving homes). Lastly, we consider transitions associated with the onset and progression of dementia. For each of these transitions, we highlight family carer perspectives. The vignettes1 and quotes used illustrate circumstances suggestive of strategies that may ease transitions and better support both individuals and their families, and portray some unexpected outcomes even with parental or carer involvement. Moving From the Parental Home Parents and their adult sons or daughters with Down syndrome often wrestle with the decision as to whether to arrange separate living arrangements or continue co-residing. These decisions are influenced by a number of factors 1 The first two vignettes are drawn from published research (Jokinen, 2008) and the author extends appreciation to the families who participated in that research and their willingness to share their stories. The third vignette is drawn from a published article reflecting family perspectives (Hogan, 2010). All names used in the vignettes are pseudonyms. 60 Jokinen et al. including familial values and beliefs, finances, opportunities, parental retirement plans, and the health status of either the parent or their offspring. In the following discussion, family perceptions are highlighted regarding planned moves, and two examples of how plans may also change are considered. Planned Moves Vignette 1 offers a brief overview of the situation of Larry, a man with Down syndrome in his 40s (Jokinen, 2008). Larry’s parents made plans for him to move out of the family home and were able to successfully realize their plans. They became actively engaged with a local provider organization so as to better understand the services available prior to the move and so that they could take advantage of a residential Vignette #1: A siblings’ story (Larry) Dan and George are middle-aged brothers; both have their own families and work full time. Their brother, Larry, aged 41, has Down syndrome and requires significant support for activities of daily living and lives within a 24/7 residential service. Prior to and following his move out of the family home, both parents became actively involved on the agency’s board of directors and were familiar with the services provided. After the move, Larry visited with his parents frequently and was included in family get-togethers. Both parents are now deceased and the brothers expressed appreciation for all that their parents had done in arranging Larry’s living circumstance. They feel they now carry on with something their parents wanted. While, admittedly, the brothers do not see each other often, they involve Larry in family gatherings and support him as needed (e.g., with medical appointments). As to the future, the brothers would like things to remain as they now are. They are confident in the residential service and deal with issues as they arise. option when one became available. An opening occurred in a supervised apartment living setting and the parents and Larry decided to take it. Larry moved to his new home, but all was not as smooth as it appeared on the surface. One of Larry’s brothers confided that “My mother, I don’t know, hesitated for a good number of years because she [was a] typical mother, ‘no one can look after my son as well as I can’. That was tough for her, for him to move out on his own. And she was over there every night of the week to make sure supper was cooked properly.” The daily visits by Larry’s mother reportedly reduced over time as her comfort level grew with the staff support provided. Nonetheless, she and her husband maintained regular weekly support and visits. Parental support ended only with the deaths of both parents. In other families, when parents do not initiate future residential plans for their adult son or daughter with whom they co-reside, siblings sometimes influence decisions and planning (Jokinen, 2008). One sibling, when reflecting on such a situation within her family, noted, “We prompted my mom to start considering putting [my sister with Down syndrome] somewhere because my mom was getting [old]… she was 70 or something then, and we thought maybe that would be good and to do it while my mom was still alive so she could see that it worked.” The sister reported that after some further family conversations with her mother, she accepted the idea that her daughter with Down syndrome move. When an opportunity for a residential option -- in this case a small group home -- became available, the sister with Down syndrome, with the encouragement of her siblings, moved from her mother’s home. Following the move, however, for unknown reasons the service provider reportedly was not supportive of family visits and this resulted in some tense relations between the family and service provider. The reflections of yet another sibling regarding her brother’s move away from the parental home also offers some insights into what might ease or hamper the transition of moving from the parental home (Hogan, 2010). The sibling noted that when her brother moved out of the home, he left behind a familiar neighbourhood as well as other residents and business owners with whom he had interacted over many years as a part of his daily routines. Consequently, after he moved into an agency group home, he experi- JoDD enced a sense of loss and his absence in his old community was noted. This loss of involvement in one’s neighbourhood and the inherent social relationships has generally received little, if any, attention, in the literature. The sister noted that once he relocated to the group home, her brother routinely returned home to spend weekends with his mother. In some respects, continued family contact is desirable and can ease transition while avoiding a sense of abandonment and abrupt separation from a valued community. On the other hand, such visits back to the family home may also inhibit being fully included in social activities with new housemates, slow down the development of new friendships, and hinder establishing an identity within the new neighbourhood. While routine familial support may increase, at least during the initial stages of a move, over time familial contact and support typically seems to settle into a new or adjusted pattern (Jokinen, 2008). An individual’s daily routines and responsibilities also likely differ between a family and new residential circumstance; whether this creates additional challenges needs to be considered. Brown and Brown (2003) referred to everyday routines that make life enjoyable, and to the need to balance established routines with new experiences that may or may not be adopted into daily life. Routines are an important aspect of both individual and family life (Jokinen, 2008; Knox & Bigby, 2007) and a fuller understanding of them can better ease transitions (Zisberg, Young, Schepp, & Zysberg, 2007). One lesson from these situations is that when necessity dictates a transition from the family home for an adult with Down syndrome, it should be individually tailored and designed to accommodate and manage those aspects of daily life that are important both to the individual making the move as well as the family. Change in Plans Despite having initiated plans for alternative living arrangements, sometimes an unexpected medical or similar significant crisis experienced by either the parent or adult with Down syndrome trumps a smooth transition. For example, a mother in her early 70s made an application for residential supports on behalf of her 40ishaged daughter Julie who had Down syndrome (Jokinen, 2008). The mother, being proactive, v . 18 n .2 61 Transitions and Older-Aged Families had registered her daughter with a local agency for residential options offered. After being on the wait list for almost ten years, an emergency relocation was required as the mother had been seriously injured and required hospitalization as well as an extended period of convalescence. The mother confided that “…if I hadn’t broken my hip, more than likely my daughter would still be at home.” While the emergency relocation turned into a permanent separate living arrangement for Julie, the transition was apparently fraught with disappointments, emotional upheaval, and challenges for both mother and daughter. Following emergency respite services, arrangements were made for Julie to move temporarily into a basement bedroom in a house rented by several adults with intellectual disability supported by a service provider. Julie had to adjust to both unfamiliar staff helping her and housemates whom she had never met. The mother reported that Julie’s sleep patterns changed, she was waking and dressing in the middle of the night, and then later falling asleep on the living room couch. On the mother’s first visit to her daughter’s new residence when she was able to walk, she also found that the home’s housekeeping standards did not meet her expectations (she found cobwebs, dirty carpets, soiled bedding, and the like). This issue was not fully resolved at that particular home, although housekeeping standards were better, at the mother’s insistence, in a subsequent home Julie moved to and lived in. The lesson here is even though the mother was planful in getting her daughter on a list for a residence service – in preparation of the transition to moving from the parental home – she lost control of the process when she was injured and her daughter required emergency relocation. Although emergency relocations are not reportedly commonplace (Bigby, 2004; Jokinen, 2008), extensive residential wait lists across many jurisdictions that now exist may increase such situations and or place increased demands on other family members. Furthermore, standards within residential services seem to vary and parents as well as other family members may need to be prepared to advocate for reasonable living conditions. Another example of a family situation offers a different circumstance that can alter planning for an alternative living option. Sometimes, parents and their offspring make decisions to live separately, only to have a medical crisis experienced by the adult with Down syn- 62 Jokinen et al. drome stymie plans and reinforce continued co-residency. One mother related that she had helped their daughter Helen to make plans to move away from the parental home (Jokinen, 2008). The mother related that: “Well, there was a time when she wanted to go to an apartment on her own. And we were all for [it]. And of course that fall, when this was in the making, she got sick with diabetes. She just, she went into a coma and I got her to the hospital…So since then she hasn’t wanted to be anyway else but home.” The mother related that Helen barely survived the medical crisis and the aftermath required adjusting to living with a chronic condition (in this case, brittle diabetes). The plans for Helen to live independently were abandoned. She continues living with her aging parents, now in their late 80s, with nominal services, and the family foresees her co-residing with her sister and brother-in-law in the future. practices within agencies or the results of external forces (Hulbert-Williams & Hastings, 2008). Vignette 2 illustrates staff turnover and location moves within residential services as a period of transition for older-aged family members. Older parents and their adult children with Down syndrome who make decisions to coreside for as long as is possible need information and support to ensure that inevitable future transitions are well managed. The degree to which families have discussed matters and have made plans varies (Jokinen, 2008), yet this is a critical task and warrants serious conversation within the family. Cultural and ethnic beliefs also play a role in shaping attitudes towards aging and disability (Hogg, Lucchino, Wang, Janicki, & Group, 2000) and can influence an older-aged family’s caregiving experiences, expectations, and plans for the future. There is an urgent need to better understand transitions as they occur in conjunction with adult sibling co-resident arrangements. This information will aid family planning and help discern pros, cons, and pitfalls of future sibling co-residency in order to adequately plan for the needs of all family members (Jokinen, 2008). Vignette #2: Even well-intended family help can go awry (Mark) Changes Occurring in Residential Services Many older-aged families of adults with Down syndrome have extensive experience with disability services, often spanning decades, and with the changes that occur within a residential service that lead to transitions for both the adult with Down syndrome and the family. Some of these changes may be initiated by administrative Changes in Staff Disability organizations have long struggled with staff recruitment and retention (Hall & Hall, 2002). Numerous changes in direct care staff and their supervisors can disrupt relationships and impact the supports provided (Jokinen, 2008). In the second vignette, Mark’s mother remarked that over the years “hundreds of people” have come and gone with respect to providing supports for her son. She commented on the staff changes that had occurred and In one particular service, adults with intellectual disabilities rented accommodations and the agency provided support staff. Mark, a middle-age man with Down syndrome, had been living in a number of such accommodations since he left home. Mark’s mother, however, always took note of where Mark lived and under what conditions. She noticed that landlords often gave notice for tenants to move and rental premises were not always properly maintained. To overcome these problems, Mark’s mother purchased a duplex. She lived in one part of the house and her son along with three male housemates moved into the other half with part time support from residential staff. The men became well known by neighbours, local businesses and a church community in the area. With an organizational change, management decided to move the men across town to different rental homes. No discussions were apparently held with the families and the men lost contact with their neighbourhood friends and acquaintances. JoDD the relations with various professionals, volunteers, and students that inevitably ended: “Like their whole lives are up and down.... different people working with them coming into their lives for a short time and going.” Sometimes, she said, people who moved on would try to keep in touch with Mark, but eventually these relations always dwindled away. Likewise, in the third vignette, Peter’s sister noted that her brother would become emotionally attached to “his staff” and generally they too would become attached to him. Often it was traumatic when staff turnover occurred and there was a limited understanding of why they had left. His family, like others, tried to help Peter stay connected to some of these people; however, most times, it was to no avail. Staff changes impact family-service relations as well as the continuity and quality of supports. New staff often has a steep learning curve in getting to know an individual’s abilities, his or her history, and the nature of the family relations (Hogan, 2010). One mother noted, “You know we’ve had 10 or 11 [supervisors] from the time that house opened... and I said [to the agency] I don’t know who I’m supposed to call… half the time I never even met the [supervisor]” (Jokinen, 2008). Such changes in staff can lead to administrative problems, as well as personal difficulties for the person and their family. Often, with multiple staff turnover, there can be a generalized loss of knowledge about an individual, such as behavioural and medical history. This loss of knowledge may well contribute to medical disparities and aggravate health conditions, and possibly spur the onset of adjustment problems that are then characterized as “problematic” behaviours. These staffing difficulties may pose significant challenges for parents and other relatives involved with the adult with Down syndrome who face these transitions with trepidations, having experienced the consequences many times over. Parents and adult siblings acknowledge staff turnover as a reality and this may reinforce the notion that families are the one constant in their relative’s life. Yet families may also have ideas to minimize the impact of staff turnover and should be consulted. For example, when staff changes are unavoidable, family members have suggested that agencies keep the number of staff moved around to a minimum and retain in place at least two or three key person- v . 18 n .2 63 Transitions and Older-Aged Families nel who have worked with their relative. In doing so, familiar staff can ease the transition for the individual, their family, and the new staff coming onboard (Jokinen, 2008). Relocations Within Residential Services In the second vignette, Mark’s mother also raised several concerns about relocations within residential services. Although the solution to the situation Mark faced was functional and productive, in the end it was undermined by the decision of the agency (providing key supports) to change Mark’s living situation. Relocations within a residential service occur and are influenced by a number of factors, including whether the disability organization owns the residential properties or, as in this vignette, provides support staff for people who are renters. In the latter, long term security and upkeep of premises may depend upon landlords concerned with business opportunities who are not particularly sympathetic to disability or aging issues. Relocation may also be prompted by a restructuring of the organization, governmental budgeting vagaries, legislative initiatives, the dynamics of housemate relations, and changes in health status that require support beyond the capabilities of the current site. While some agencies have explicit policies, others lack guidelines about residential moves (Webber, Bowers, & McKenzie-Green, 2010). Nonetheless, as for anyone, changing residences and moving is a major life event and, when people move into a new home and new environment, adjustments are required by the individual, his or her family, staff, and those others residing at the new location. In an attempt to reduce the likelihood of adverse relocations and to provide long term housing security, some parents have purchased homes where their sons or daughters and others might live with specialized supports. We recall another mother who suggested, “It’s a sure way of having your child not moved every two or three years… if you want them closer to home, you can make an effort to find a house that you can buy in the locality where you want it.” Referring to her own situation, she said of her daughter: “She’s seven minutes away from me.” However, the dual role responsibilities of parent and landlord are likely to be challenging and stressful. Decisions about who will live there, what the 64 Jokinen et al. home will offer in terms of furnishings and household goods, upkeep, and negotiations with a residential services provider to secure support are a few such details that need to be worked out (Jokinen, 2008). Further, while the option of purchasing a separate residence may be beyond the financial means of many families (Canada Mortgage and Housing Corporation, 2006, 2010), today’s extensive wait times for residential services means that there is ever more demand on families to be self-reliant. Changes Due to Onset and Progression of Dementia Worldwide, the impact of age-related decline and the onset and progression of neuropathologies, such as Alzheimer disease and related dementias, on individuals and family carers is well recognized (World Health Organization, 2012). These events perhaps pose the last significant transition event in the lifespan, save for death. When adults with intellectual disability and their families are affected by this transition, it becomes more important that families and other carers are knowledgeable and prepared to deal with the challenges that are to come (Factor, Heller, & Janicki, 2012; National Task Group on Intellectual Disabilities and Dementia Practices, 2012). Many families of adults with Down syndrome are aware of the increased risk of Alzheimer disease associated with Down syndrome and, with increased age, there is a genuine concern about what this elevated risk poses for the future. In this final section, we discuss transitions occurring as a result of the onset and progression of dementia. Vignette 3, drawn from Hogan (2010), is illustrative of some of the issues faced by families with respect to this transition. Parents and other family members face increasing challenges when their relative with intellectual disability evidences cognitive decline or behavioural change as a result of cognitive impairment or dementia. In one of few studies reporting family perspectives, Janicki, Zendell, and De Haven (2010) found many parents had a profound commitment and preference to keep and care for their son or daughter with Down syndrome at home. Their study noted that some parents also claimed that there was nowhere else for their son or daughter affected by dementia to go, or that the family was resistant to moving him or her. Decisions in response to progressive decline experienced by their sons or daughters were difficult and often considered to be influenced by a number of factors. Many parents report that they would seek help from a doctor or other clinician, look for personal care assistance, and or seek the support of a sibling. They Vignette #3: Frustrations with a lack of knowledge and information (Peter) Four years after the diagnosis of Alzheimer disease and 15 months before our brother Peter’s death, he exhibited more dramatic changes in behaviour, personality and level of functioning. After exploring several nursing homes, we became convinced that he needed to stay with people who knew him. Safety risks for both him and the staff became a major factor and our only option was to request a transfer to a 12-person group home for severely handicapped and medically fragile people. The transition was very traumatic for our brother who was very confused and further agitated by the change. We needed to play a major role in the transition to make sure that the new staff bonded with him and to help them understand his needs. They had never had a resident with Alzheimer disease and, with little specific training on Alzheimer disease, needed a great deal of support and encouragement with difficult behaviours, such as changed sleep patterns. In hindsight, our family realized that none of us had fully understood the types of changes that would occur as Alzheimer disease progressed. We, as a family, were not mentally or emotionally prepared for what unfolded or for the rate of decline. Our brother was 49 years old when died of complications related to aspiration pneumonia seven months after the transition to the new group home. JoDD suggested specific strategies to cope and manage things as they arose. These included acquiring support from an agency’s treatment team, use of medications, and guidance from other carers who have had similar experience. When it came to finally facing the difficult decision of whether to seek help to arrange for another residential option, the parents noted that continued at-home care as dementia progressed was influenced by various factors. These factors included the medical needs of their offspring, the impact caregiving was having on their personal health, barriers in the home environment, safety issues, the level of support provided by other family members, and outside services available to meet needs. A critical aspect in the decision-making process was where the adult offspring would most likely go if he or she moved out of the family home. The carers speculated, if such a move was made, their son or daughter would likely go live with another family member or go to a group home while some indicated a preference for a long term care facility (i.e., a dementia care unit or nursing home). However, a prevailing theme was uncertainty; the parents were unsure where their son or daughter might end up, and this uncertainty was a major stressor with which they had to contend (Janicki et al., 2010). In the situation of Peter, his sister noted that while her brother experienced progressive Alzheimer disease and remained living in his original group home, most staff at the home lacked training in understanding the progressive nature of the disease and in managing the new behaviours and diminishing abilities. Yet, at the same time, the staff remained committed to his care and tried hard to meet his needs. The lack of, and need for, staff training specific to dementia care is well recognized (National Task Group on Intellectual Disabilities and Dementia Practices, 2012; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2012) as is often staff’s hesitancy to relinquish supporting the person and have him or her move to another location (McCarron, McCallion, FaheyMcCarthy, Connaire, & Dunn-Lane, 2010). In Vignette 3, while additional support staff was scheduled, the ultimate challenge became the move to another group home during the later stages of Alzheimer disease (the home chosen was not a dementia-capable home, but one that provided for adults with severe impairments). While the intention was to have old and new staff work together for a planned transition, the v . 18 n .2 65 Transitions and Older-Aged Families changeover was complicated and difficult for all concerned. Eventually, the new staff learned to care for Peter, both physically and emotionally and to communicate with his family – and the family reciprocated. As the sister reported, “It was the best we could do.” In circumstances such as this, education, training, and communication are crucial to dispel misunderstandings about the move, the disease, its symptoms, and care requirements. The lesson taken from these types of transition experiences is that catastrophic and other changes in function, whether cognitive or physical, pose special challenges for families. Dementia poses a realization for many parents that supportive caregiving will become more, and not less, pronounced and demanding with their own advancing age. Both the parents as they age and their offspring, as dementia takes its toll, will find continued care more taxing and force personal and family examination of transitions and compel conversation on what to do next. Older age will always necessitate transition planning, but the demands presented by catastrophic change such as that brought on by Alzheimer disease may lead to this conversation taking place sooner than anticipated. Discussion In this paper, we have attempted to address some particular lifespan transitions not previously raised and examined. Admittedly, this examination is cursory and based on summative and anecdotal data, but it does begin to examine family situations and perspectives as changes are encountered due to transitions that evolve organically or forced by crisis. We observed that this part of the lifespan potentially offers multiple examples of both planful and demand transitions. We conclude that planful transitions are always less stressful and often lead to more positive and predictable outcomes and remain largely under parental and family control. Demand transitions, on the other hand, often lead to unpredictable outcomes and may result in control being relinquished to others outside the family. What we observed from our examination is that, even in situations where families observe natural pathways to decision-making and attempt to control and produce reliable outcomes, these may go awry due to factors 66 Jokinen et al. beyond their control. What we have learned from this exercise is that it is helpful to examine such transition situations, evaluate the options and alternatives, and follow in more detail those factors that facilitate or impede them. What can also be gleaned from this exercise is the knowledge that the natural course of the lifespan can be interrupted by transitions. Sometimes these transitions are positive events and help lead to advantageous outcomes. Sometimes these transitions are calumnious and lead to adverse outcomes. Loss of staff has the potential to impact transitions and the level of care; communication or the lack thereof can interfere with the best made transition plans; and staff training needs to be examined as a possible means to ease transitions, especially in relation to diagnosis of a progressive condition. Families and those staff and organizations functioning in aid to families will be better equipped if they understand the dynamics inherent in these transitions and conjure up ways to constructively address them. Discussions of nuanced older age in the arena of intellectual disabilities are relatively new and, as of yet, a little explored frontier. With the extended and more normal longevity of adults with Down syndrome, any and all inquiries into this new frontier are most welcome and will be helpful in aiding families with planful transitions. References Baxley, D., Janicki, M. P., McCallion, P., & Zendell, A. (2005). Aiding older caregivers of persons with intellectual and developmental disabilities: A tool kit for state and local aging agencies. Retrieved from http://www. uic.edu/orgs/rrtcamr/Aiding_older_ caregivers.pdf Bigby, C. (2004). Ageing with a lifelong disability: A guide to practice, program and policy issues for human services professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Bittles, A., & Glassen, E. (2004). Clinical, social, and ethical implications of changing life expectancy in Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46, 282–286. Bouck, E. C. (2012). Secondary students with moderate/severe intellectual disability: Considerations of curriculum and postschool outcomes from the National Longitudinal Transition Study-2. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. doi:10.1111/ j.1365-2788.2011.01517.x Braddock, D., Hemp, R., & Rizzolo, M. C. (2008). The state of the states in developmental disabilities: 2008. Washington, DC: American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. Brown, I., & Brown, R. I. (2003). Quality of life and disability: An approach for community practitioners. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Canada Mortgage and Housing Corporation (2006, 2010). Research highlights: Housing for adults with intellectual disabilities. Retrieved from https://www03.cmhc-schl.gc.ca/ catalog/productDetail.cfm?lang=en&cat=4 7&itm=5&sere=2&start=1&stfl=INTELLEC TUAL%20DISABILITIES&fr=1336837859582 Denton, M., & Kusch, K. (2006). Well-being throughout the senior years: An issues paper on key events and transitions in later life. Retrieved from http://socserv. socsci.mcmaster.ca/sedap/p/sedap165.pdf Dilworth-Anderson, P., Hilliard, T. S., Williams, S., & Palmer, M. H. (2011). A contextual conceptualization on transitions of care for older persons: Shaping the direction of care. Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics, 31, 1–14. Factor, A., Heller, T., & Janicki, M. (2012). Bridging the aging and developmental disabilities service networks: Challenges and best practices. Chicago, IL: Institute on Disability and Human Development, University of Illinois at Chicago. Gilbert, A., Lankshear, G., & Petersen, A. (2008). Older family-carers’ views on the future accommodation needs of relatives who have an intellectual disability. International Journal of Social Welfare, 17(1), 54–64. Gitlin, L. N., & Wolff, J. (2011). Family involvement in care transitions of older adults: What do we know and where do we go from here? Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics, 31, 31–64. Grant, G., Nolan, M., & Keady, J. (2003). Supporting families over the life course: Mapping temporality. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47(4–5), 342–351. Hall, P., & Hall, N. (2002). Hiring and retaining direct-care staff: After fifty years of research, what do we know? Mental Retardation, 40(3), 201–211. Hogan, M. (2010). Remembering Bill. American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD) Gerontology Division Newsletter, December, 3–9. JoDD Hogg, J., Lucchino, R., Wang, K., Janicki, M. P., & Group, W. (2000). Healthy ageing: Adults with intellectual disabilities: Ageing & social policy. Geneva: World Health Organization. Hulbert-Williams, L., & Hastings, R. P. (2008). Life events as a risk factor for psychological problems in individuals with intellectual disabilities: A critical review. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(11), 883–895. Janicki, M. P., Dalton, A. J., Henderson, C. M., & Davidson, P. W. (1999). Mortality and morbidity among older adults with intellectual disability: Health services considerations. Disability and Rehabilitation, 21, 284–294. Janicki, M., Zendell, A., & DeHaven, K. (2010). Coping with dementia and older families of adults with down syndrome. Dementia, 9(3), 391–407. Jokinen, N. S. M. (2008). Family quality of life in the context of aging and intellectual disability. PhD dissertation, University of Calgary (Canada). Publication No. AAT NR44403. Knox, M., & Bigby, C. (2007). Moving towards midlife care as negotiated family business: Accounts of people with intellectual disabilities and their families “Just getting along with their lives together.” International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 54(3), 287–304. Lunsky, Y., & Elserafi, J. (2011). Life events and emergency department visits in response to crisis in individuals with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55(7), 714–718. McCarron, M., McCallion, P., Fahey-McCarthy, E., Connaire, K., & Dunn-Lane, J. (2010). Supporting persons with Down syndrome and advanced dementia: Challenges and care concerns. Dementia, 9(2), 285–298. Miettinen, S. (2012). Family care of adults with intellectual disabilities: Analysis of Finnish policies and practices. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 9(9), 1–9. National Task Group on Intellectual Disabilities and Dementia Practices. (2012). ‘My Thinker’s Not Working’: A national strategy for enabling adults with intellectual disabilities affected by dementia to remain in their community and receive quality supports. Retrieved from www. aadmd.org/ntg/thinker v . 18 n .2 67 Transitions and Older-Aged Families Patti, P., Amble, K., & Flory, M. (2005). Life events in older adults with intellectual disabilities: Differences between adults with and without Down syndrome. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 2(2), 149–155. Patti, P., Amble, K., & Flory, M. (2010). Placement, relocation and end of life issues in aging adults with and without Down’s syndrome: A retrospective study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(6), 538–546. Percy, M., Summers, J., & Lovering, J. (2011). Down syndrome: Characteristics and health issues. In I. Brown & M. Percy (Eds.), Developmental disabilities in Ontario (3rd ed.) (pp. 267–292). Toronto, ON: Ontario Association on Developmnetal Disabilities. Pueschel, S. (Ed.). (2006). Adults with Down syndrome. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Ray, M., Bernard, M., & Phillips, J. (2009). Critical issues in social work with older people. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. Torr, J., Strydom, A., Patti, P., & Jokinen, N. (2010). Aging in Down syndrome: Morbidity and mortality. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 7(1), 70–81. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2012). National plan to address Alzheimer’s disease. Washington, DC: Author. Webber, R., Bowers, B., & McKenzie-Green, B. (2010). Staff responses to age-related health changes in people with an intellectual disability in group homes. Disability & Society, 25(6), 657–671. World Health Organization. (2012). Dementia: A public health priority. Geneva: Author. Zisberg, A., Young, H., Schepp, K., & Zysberg, L. (2007). A concept analysis of routine: Relevance to nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 57(4), 442–453. Note: At time of first publication of this article, minor changes were pending. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/ index.php?page=758 after August 15, 2012. language specific to this note indicated in blue. High Plasma Homocysteine and Low Serum Folate Levels Induced by Antiepileptic Drugs in Down Syndrome Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Antonio Siniscalchi,1 Giovambattista De Sarro,2 Simona Loizzo,3 Luca Gallelli2 Department of Neuroscience, Neurology Division, “Annunziata” Hospital, Cosenza, Italy 1 Department of Health Science, School of Medicine, University of Catanzaro, Clinical Pharmacology Unit, Mater Domini University Hospital, Catanzaro, Italy 2 Dentistry Division, “Annunziata” Hospital, Cosenza, Italy 3 Correspondence anto.siniscalchi@libero.it Keywords hyper-homocysteinemia, Down syndrome, epilepsy, folate, vitamin B12 Abstract Clinical and epidemiological studies suggested an association between hyper-homocysteinemia (Hyper-Hcy) and cerebrovascular disease. Experimental studies showed potential proconvulsant activity of Hcy, with several drugs commonly used to treat patients affected by neurological disorders also able to modify plasma Hcy levels. We assessed the effect of long-term AED treatment on plasma Hcy levels in patients with Down syndrome (DS) and epilepsy. We also evaluated the relationship between the plasma Hcy levels, and folic acid or vitamin B12. We enrolled 15 patients in the Down syndrome with epilepsy group (DSEp, 12 men and 3 women, mean age 22 ± 12.5 years old) and 15 patients in the Down syndrome without epilepsy group (DSControls, 12 men and 3 women, mean age 20 ± 13.7 years old). In the DSEp group the most common form of epilepsy was simple partial epilepsy, while the most common AED used was valproic acid. Plasma Hcy levels were significantly higher (P < 0.01) in the DSEp group compared with the DSControl group. Significant differences (P < 0.01) between DSEp and DSControls were also observed in serum concentrations of folic acid, but not in serum levels of vitamin B12. In conclusion, our observations suggest that treatment with AEDs in DSEp patients induces an increase in plasma Hcy levels and a significant decrease in serum folic acid, therefore supplementation with vitamins may be useful in order to obtain normal plasma Hcy values and reduce the risk of both cardiovascular and neurological diseases Homocysteine (Hcy) is a thiol-containing amino acid that is formed by the demethylation of methionine, an essential amino acid derived from the animal proteins introduced with the diet (Selhub, Jacques, Wilson, Rush, & Rosenberg, 1993; Siniscalchi et al., 2005). It is metabolized through two major pathways: methylation and trans-sulfuration that are dependent on several cofactors. In particular, the methylation of Hcy to methionine requires folate and vitamin B12, whilst the trans-sulfuration to cysteine requires vitamin B6 (Siniscalchi, 2004; Siniscalchi et al., 2005; Siniscalchi, Gallelli, Mercuri, Fererri Ibbadu, & De Sarro, 2006). The recycling of folate cofactors is dependent on vitamin B6 and B2; vitamin B2 is necessary to activate vitamin B6 to pyridoxal 5-phosphate (PLP) (Siniscalchi, 2004; Siniscalchi et al., 2005; Siniscalchi et al., 2006). Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs in Down Syndrome Clinical and epidemiological studies suggested an association between hyper-homocysteinemia (Hyper-Hcy), occlusive arterial vascular diseases (D’Angelo and Selhub, 1997; Perri, 1999; Refsum, Ueland, Nygard, & Vollset, 1998; Siniscalchi, 2004) and venous thromboembolism (den Heijer et al., 1996; 1998). Hyper-Hcy represents both a reaction to acute illness and an important risk factor for cardiovascular mortality in patients with a history of myocardial infarction, stroke, angina pectoris, diabetes, or hypertension (Boysen, Brander, Christensen, Gideon, & Truelsen, 2003; Vollset et al., 2001). Experimental studies have shown a potential proconvulsant activity of Hcy (Siniscalchi 2004) with several drugs commonly used to treat patients affected by neurological disorders also able to modify plasma Hcy levels. Clinical studies have reported that patients treated with old and new AEDs showed increased plasma Hcy levels (Belcastro et al., 2010; Linnebank et al., 2011; Schwaninger et al. 1999; Siniscalchi et al., 2005) Moreover, folate deficiency occurs in some epileptic patients treated with AEDs such as valproate (VA) and carbamazepine (CBZ), and this effect may induce Hyper-Hcy (Kishi, Fujita, Eguchi, & Ueda, 1997). Therefore, vitamin B plays a role in altering plasma Hcy levels during AEDs treatment (Siniscalchi 2004; Siniscalchi et al.,2005) In this paper, we assessed the effect of longterm AEDs treatment on plasma Hcy levels patients with Down syndrome and epilepsy. We also evaluated the relationship between the plasma Hcy levels, and serum levels of folic acid or vitamin B12. Patients and Methods Study Design We undertook an open label, control-group, single centre study on patients with Down syndrome and seizures treated with AEDs and enrolled at the Cosenza Hospital. This study was approved by the Researchers’ Ethics Committee and was conducted in accordance with the v . 18 n .2 69 Declaration of Helsinki and the Guideline for Good Clinical Practice; all parents of participants provided written informed consent. Patients eligible for the study were male or female, aged from 18 years up to 35 years, with primary epilepsy and prescribed AEDs (DSEp group). Patients with the following conditions were excluded: allergy to AEDs, progressive serious medical conditions (i.e., cancer or AIDS), renal diseases (serum creatinine concentration more than 1.2 times the upper limit of the normal range according to the central laboratory definition reference values), liver dysfunctions (serum alanine transaminase or aspartate transaminase concentrations more than 1.5 times the upper limit of the normal range according to the central laboratory definition reference values) and drugs known to increase the plasma Hcy levels. Patients with Down syndrome and without epilepsy were also enrolled in our study and represented our control-group (DSControl group). End Points The primary end point was the effects of AEDs on plasma Hcy levels. Moreover, co-endpoints included the relationship between both the plasma Hcy levels and the vitamin B12 or folic acid levels. Evaluation of Hcy, Vitamin B12 and Folic Acid Levels For plasma Hcy levels evaluation, blood samples collected in heparin were immediately stored in fresh ice (T = 0°C) and then centrifuged (T = 18°C, 1500 g/min for 10 min). Plasma was take through a polyurethane pipette, put in a polyurethane tube and analyzed with high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC-UV) in agreement with literature data (Amarnath, Amarnath, Amarnath, Valentine, & Valentine, 2003; Kuo, Still, Cale, & McDowell, 1997). Serum levels of folic acid and vitamin B12 were analyzed by chemiluminescence with the AxSYM instrument in agreement with literature data (Bamonti et al., 2010). 70 Siniscalchi Statistical Analysis ic acid, with a mean duration of treatment of 9.7 ± 3.0 years. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS. We evaluated the effects of drugs on the plasma concentration of Hcy, vitamin B12 and folic acid, but it is not easy to define a clinically significant change and therefore it is not possible to obtain a power calculation. Consequently, these results should be considered exploratory. The Student’s t test was used to evaluate the change between groups. For all comparisons, differences were considered significant at p < 0.05. Results During the study we enrolled 15 patients in the DSEp-group (12 men and 3 women, mean age 22 ± 12.5 years old) and 15 patients in the DSControl-group (12 men and 3 women, mean age 20 ± 13.7 years old). In the DSEp-group the most common form of epilepsy was simple partial epilepsy (Table 1), and the most common AED used was valpro- Plasma Hcy levels were significantly higher (p < 0.01), in the DSEp group compared with the DSControl group (Table 3). Significant differences (p < 0.01) between the DSEp group and DSC group were also observed in the serum concentrations of folic acid, but not in serum levels of vitamin B12. Discussion In this study we evaluated the effects of AEDs on plasma Hcy levels and serum vitamine B12 and folic acid levels in patients with Down syndrome (DS) with and without epilepsy. Table 1. Epilepsy Manifestations in DSEp-Group Simply Partial Generalized Epilepsy Epilepsy 7 Complex Partial Epilepsy 5 3 Table 2. Antiepileptic Treatment of Patients with Down Syndrome and Epilepsy (DSEp-Group) Valproic acid Carbamazepine Phenobarbital Treatment 5 mono-therapy 2 poly-therapy 4 mono-therapy 1 poly-therapy 3 mono-therapy Mean time of treatment (years) 9.7 ± 3.0 10.1 ± 2.5 17.5 ± 3.2 Table 3. Concentrations of Homocysteine in Plasma, and Vitamin B12 and Folate in Serum. Values are Expressed as Mean ± Standard Deviation. Homocysteine (normal values 5–12 µmol/L) Vitamin B12 (normal values 145–914 pg/mL) Folic acid (normal values 3–20 ng/mL) DSControl Group DSEp Group P Value 8.2+2.0 14.8 ± 1.8 0.000 359.1 ± 35.1 354.6 ± 40.1 0.746 17.9 ± 1.0 9.3 ± 1.5 0.000 JoDD Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs in Down Syndrome Our data demonstrate that patients in DSControl-group show normal levels of folate and Hcy, while long-term treatment with AEDs in patients with DSEp may increase the plasma Hcy levels and this effect is related to a decrease in serum levels of folic acid. These data are in agreement with previous studies which demonstrated that patients with epilepsy treated with AEDs showed low serum folate and elevated plasma homocysteine (Belcastro et al., 2010; Linnebank et al., 2011; Schwaninger et al., 1999; Siniscalchi et al., 2005). Although the mechanisms by which AEDs induce a decrease in serum folate levels are unclear, it has been hypothesized that AEDs may induce a decrease in intestinal absorption of folate, or an induction of liver enzymes (Siniscalchi et al., 2005). The conclusions from previous studies on plasma levels of vitamin B12 in patients with epilepsy are controversial. Researchers have variously described not only a decrease (Refsum et al., 1998), as well an increase in concentrations of vitamin B12 (den Heijer, Blom, Gerrits, Rosendaal, & Haak, 1995), but also reported normal levels of vitamin B12 (Van Beynum, Smeitink, den Heijer, te Poele Pothoff, & Blom, 1999). Recently, Linnebank et al., (2012) in a prospective monocentre study documented that the mean plasma levels of vitamin B6 were not significantly different between AED-treated, the untreated patients and the controls. Thus further studies are needed to better analyze the relationship between AEDs and vitamins. Plasma Hcy levels represent both an atherogenic and a thrombogenic risk factor in the development of cerebrovascular manifestations. This could be important in patients with DS who have been documented to be more susceptible at the development of cardiovascular (Irving and Chaudari, 2012; Raina, McGrath, & Gunn, 2011) and neurological diseases (Lott & Dierssen 2010). In patients with epilepsy, chronic treatment with AEDs induced an increase in plasma Hcy levels that represents a risk factor for the development of cerebrovascular diseases (Karabiber et al., 2003). v . 18 n .2 71 Patients with epilepsy receiving long-term AED therapy showed an alteration in circulatory markers of vascular risk that may contribute to a rapid increase of the atherosclerotic process (Chuang et al., 2012). With regard to a potential proconvulsant activity of Hcy, it has been documented that up to 20% of patients with homozygous cystathionine ß-synthase deficiency manifested seizures, therefore an increase in plasma Hcy levels (usually 50–200 µmol/L) may be able to induce epilepsy (Mudd et al., 1985). In agreement, recent investigations in experimental animal models, showed that systemic administration of higher doses of Hcy are able to induce seizures (Marangos et al., 1990; Mares, Folbergrova, Langmeier, Haugvicova, & Kubova, 1997). However, at the moment it has not yet been demonstrated in humans whether lower levels of Hyper-Hcy (15–20 µmol/L), are able to induce epilepsy. Different mechanisms have been proposed to explain the convulsing properties of Hcy. The L-isomer of Hcy and its oxidized product, homocysteic acid, acts as an agonist at the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-type glutamate receptor sites (Lipton et al., 1997; Schwarz, Zhou, Katki, & Rodbard, 1990) and at metabotropic glutamate receptors (Shi et al., 2003). Another hypothesis suggests that Hcy could increase the adenosine uptake able to reduce the seizure threshold (McIlwain & Poll 1985). Thus Hcy may also increase seizure frequency in patients treated with AEDs. Therefore, the decrease of plasma Hcy levels could lead to a decrease of seizure activity. Clinical studies (Apeland et al., 2002; Siniscalchi et al., 2005) reported that the supplementation with folate, vitamin B12 and B6 in patients with Hyper-Hcy induced by AEDs decreasesd the plasma Hcy levels and influenced endothelial activation, showing that the B group of vitamins have a vascular-protective effect in patients administered AEDs. Previously it has been suggested that in men and women treated with AEDs, supplementation with folic acid tablets (1 mg each) or as part of a multivitamin supplement could be useful in order to normalize the Hcy plasma levels (Morrell et al., 2002). Fillon-Emeryet et al. (2004) showed in adults with trisomy 21 that Hcy levels were decreased 72 Siniscalchi by folic acid administration. Therefore, supplementation with this vitamin may also be able to reduce plasma Hcy levels in patients with DS and epilepsy treated with AEDs. However, based on our data showing that DS patients without epilepsy don’t show an increase in Hcy plasma levels or a decrease in folate levels, we hypothesized that folic acid may be used in patients with DS and epilepsy treated with AEDs even if a deficit of folate has not been reported. In conclusion, our observations suggest that treatment with AEDs in patients with DS and epilepsy induces an increase in plasma Hcy levels and a significant decrease in serum folic acid. Therefore supplementation with this vitamin may be useful for achieving normal plasma Hcy values and consequently reduce the potential for the development of both cardiovascular and neurological. Key Messages From This Article • Patients with DS show normal levels of Hcy and folate. • AEDs treatment induced in patients with DS and seizures an increase in Hcy plasma levels with a reduction in folate serum levels. • The increased levels of Hcy could represent a risk factor for cardiovascular and neurological diseases in patients usually more susceptible at these manifestations. • The addition of folate through diet or vitamin supplements could normalize the levels of Hcy. Acknowledgements The authors acknowledge to support the collection of data from this study the Groups of Progetto DAMA (Disabled Advanced Medical Assistance) dell’Azienda Ospedaliera di Cosenza. The authors have no conflicts of interest that are directly relevant to the contents of this manuscript. References Amarnath, K., Amarnath, V., Amarnath, K., Valentine, H. L., & Valentine, W. M (2003). A specific HPLC-UV method for the determination of cysteine and related aminothiols in biological samples. Talanta, 60, 1229–1238. Apeland, T., Mansoor, M., Pentieva, K., McNulty, H., Seljeflot, I., & Strandjord, R.E. (2002). The effect of B-vitamins on hyperhomocysteinemia in patients in antiepileptic drugs. Epilepsy Research, 51, 237–247. Bamonti, F., Moscato, G. A., Novembrino, C., Gregori, D., Novi, C., De Giuseppe, R., et al. (2010). Determination of serum holotranscobalamin concentrations with the AxSYM active B12 assay: Cut-off point evaluation in the clinical laboratory. Clinical Chemistry Laboratory Medicine, 48, 249–253. Belcastro, V., Striano, P., Gorgone, G., Costa, C., Ciampa, C., Caccamo, D., et al. (2010). Hyperhomocysteinemia in epileptic patients on new antiepileptic drugs. Epilepsia, 51, 274–279. Boysen, G., Brander, T., Christensen, H., Gideon, R., & Truelsen, T. (2003). Homocysteine and risk of recurrent stroke. Stroke, 34, 1258–1261. Chuang, Y. C., Chuang, H. Y., Lin, T. K., Chang, C. C., Lu, C. H., Chang, W. N., et al. (2012). Effects of long-term antiepileptic drug monotherapy on vascular risk factors and atherosclerosis. Epilepsia, 53, 120–128. D’Angelo, A., & Selhub, J. (1997). Homocysteine and thrombotic disease. Blood, 90, 1–11. den Heijer, M., Blom, H. J., Gerrits, W. B. J., Rosendaal, F. R., & Haak, H. L. (1995). Is hyperhomocysteinemia a risk factor for recurrent venous thrombosis? Lancet, 345, 882–885. den Heijer, M., Koster, T., Blom, H. J., Bos, G. M. J., Briet, E., Reitsma, P. H., et al. (1996). Hyperhomocysteinemia as a risk factor for deep vein thrombosis. New England Journal of Medicine, 334, 759–762. den Heijer, M., Rosendaal, F. R., Blom, H. J., Gerrits, W. B. J., & Bos, G. M. J. (1998). Hyperhomocysteinemia and venous thrombosis: A meta-analysis. Thrombosis Haemostasis 80, 874–877. JoDD Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs in Down Syndrome Fillon-Emery, N., Chango, A., Mircher, C., Barbé, F., Bléhaut, H., Herbeth. B., et al. (2004). Homocysteine concentrations in adults with trisomy 21: Effect of B vitamins and genetic polymorphisms. American Journal Clinical Nutrition, 80, 1551–1557. Irving, C .A., & Chaudhari, M. P. (2012). Cardiovascular abnormalities in Down’s syndrome: Spectrum, management and survival over 22 years. Archives Disease Children, 97, 326–330. Karabiber, H., Sonmzgoz, E., Ozerol, E., Yakinci, C., Otlu, B., & Yologlu, S. (2003). Effects of valproate and carbamazepine on serum levels of homocysteine, vitamin B12 and folic acid. Brain and Development 25, 113–115. Kishi, T., Fujita, N., Eguchi, T., & Ueda, K. (1997). Mechanism for reduction of serum folate by antiepileptic drugs during prolonged therapy. Journal Neurological Sciences, 145, 109–112. Kuo, K., Still, R., Cale, S., & McDowell, I. (1997). Standardization (external and internal) of HPLC assay for plasma homocysteine. Clinical Chemistry, 43, 1653–1655. Linnebank, M., Moskau, S., Semmler, A., Widman, G., Stoffel-Wagner, B., Weller, M., et al. (2011). Antiepileptic drugs interact with folate and vitamin B12 serum levels. Annals Neurology 69, 352–359. Linnebank, M., Moskau, S., Semmler, A., Widman, G., Weller, M., Kallweit, U., et al. (2012). Antiepileptic drugs and vitamin B6 plasma levels in adult patients. Epilepsy Research, Mar 29. Lipton, S. A., Kim, W. K., Choi, Y. B., Kumar, S., D’Emilia, D. M., Rayudu, P. V., et al. (1997). Neurotoxicity associated with dual actions of homocysteine at the N-methyld-aspartate receptor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94, 5923–5928. Lott, I. T., & Dierssen, M. (2010). Cognitive deficits and associated neurological complications in individuals with Down’s syndrome. Lancet Neurology, 9, 623–633 Marangos, P., Loftus, T., Wiesner, J., Lowe, T., Rossi, E., Browne, C. E., et al. (1990). Adenosinergic modulation of homocysteine-induced seizures in mice. Epilepsia, 31, 239–246. v . 18 n .2 73 Mares, P., Folbergrova, J., Langmeier, M., Haugvicova, R., & Kubova, H. (1997). Convulsant action of d,l homocysteic acid and its stereoisomers in immature rats. Epilepsia, 38, 767–776. McIlwain, H., & Poll, J. (1985). Interaction between adenosine generated endogenously in neocortical tissue, and homocysteine and its thiolactone. Neurochemistry International, 7, 103–110. Morrell, M. J. (2002). Folic acid and epilepsy. Epilepsy Currents, 2, 31–34. Mudd, S., Skovby, F., Levy, H., Pettigrew, K. D., Wilcken, B., Pyeritz, R. E., et al. (1985). The natural history of homocystinuria due to cystathionine ß-synthase deficiency. American Journal Human Genetics, 37, 1–31. Perri, I. J. (1999). Homocysteine, hypertension and stroke. Journal Human Hypertension, 13, 289–293. Raina, T., McGrath, E., & Gunn, J. (2011). Myocardial infarction in a patient with down syndrome: A case report and review of the literature. Clinical Cardiology, 34, 87–89. Refsum, H., Ueland, P. M., Nygard, O., & Vollset, S. E. (1998). Homocysteine and cardiovascular disease. Annual Review Medicine, 49, 31–62. Schwaninger, M., Ringleb, P., Winter, R., Kohl, B., Fiehn, W., Rieser, P. A., et al. (1999). Elevated plasma concentrations of homocysteine in antiepileptic drug treatment. Epilepsia, 40, 345–350. Schwarz, S., Zhou, G., Katki, A., & Rodbard, D. (1990). L-Homocysteate stimulates (3H)MK-801 biding to the phencyclidine recognition site and is thus an agonist for the N-methyl-D-aspartate-operated cation channel. Neuroscience, 37, 193–200. Selhub, J., Jacques, P. F., Wilson, P. W. S., Rush, D., & Rosenberg, I. H. (1993). Vitamin status and intake as primary determinants of homocysteinemia in an elderly population. Journal of the American Medical Association, 270, 2693–2698. Shi, Q. I., Savage, J. E., Hufeisen, S. J., Rauser, L., Grajkowska, E., Ernsberger, P., et al. (2003). L-Homocysteine sulfinic acid and other acidic homocysteine derivates are potent and selective metabotropic glutamate receptor agonists. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 305, 131–142. 74 Siniscalchi Siniscalchi, A., Gallelli, L., Mercuri, N. B., Fererri Ibbadu, G., & De Sarro, G. (2006). Role of lifestyle factors on plasma homocysteine levels in Parkinson’s disease patients treated with levodopa. Nutritional Neuroscience, 9, 11–16. Siniscalchi, A., Mancuso, F., Gallelli, L., Ferreri Ibbadu, G., Mercuri, N. B., & De Sarro, G. (2005). Increase in plasma homocysteine levels induced by drug treatments in neurologic patients. Pharmacological Research 52, 367–375. Siniscalchi, A. (2004). Hyperhomocysteinemia in neurologic diseases. Recenti Progressi Medicina, 95, 371–375. Van Beynum, I. M., Smeitink, J. A., den Heijer, M., te Poele Pothoff, M. T., & Blom, H. J. (1999). Hyperhomocysteinemia: A risk factor for ischemic stroke in children. Circulation, 99, 2070–2072. Vollset, S. E., Refsum, H., Tverdal, A., Nygard, O., Nordrehaug, J. E., Tell, G. S., et al. (2001). Plasma total homocysteine and cardiovascular and noncardiovascular mortality: The Hordalans Homocysteine study. American Journal Clinical Nutrition, 74, 130–136. NOTE: A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www. oadd.org/index.php?page=759 after August 15, 2012. JoDD Puberty, Menstruation and Pain in Australian and Irish Females with Down Syndrome Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Authors Margaret. R.Kyrkou,¹ Rebecca O’Rourke², Joan P. Murphy2 Verity M. Bottroff,¹ Dennis S. Smith,¹ Hilary M. C. V. Hoey,² Roy Brown1,3 The Flinders University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia 1 Department of Paediatrics, University of Dublin, Trinity College, National Children’s Hospital, Dublin, Ireland 2 University of New England, Armidale, NSW, Australia 3 Correspondence margaret.kyrkou@ flinders.edu.au Keywords Down syndrome, puberty, menstruation, menstrual pain, pain Abstract Females with Down syndrome (DS) have generally been considered a homogeneous group, equated with females in the general population in terms of puberty, menstruation and ability to indicate pain. However, parents who related more closely to females with DS were reporting otherwise, indicating that these aspects of development in females with DS are still poorly understood. Information was sought from parents, and where parents chose, females with DS in Australia by means of a questionnaire collecting cross-sectional data. With permission from the principal researcher, the same questionnaire was used when interviewing females with DS and their parents (predominantly mothers) in Ireland, the only question changes being the addition of demographic questions regarding age of parents at conception, and the female’s karyotype. The results are analysed and discussed with practical recommendations provided. Note: At time of first publication this article was in press. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/index.php?page=760 after August 15, 2012. book review: Encore Edie by Annabel Lyon Volume 18, Number 2, 2012 Author Susan Petko NOTE: At time of first publication an introduction to the author was not available. A fully edited version of this article can be found on the OADD website at http://www.oadd.org/ index.php?page=761 after August 15, 2012 I have just read Encore Edie by Annabel Lyon. This book is about Edie who is 13. It is about how things are now Edie is at high school. Edie is a very interesting girl who has a great love for Shakespeare and her family too. She is having a hard time making friends and finding her place at school. The book is also about her and her cousin Merry who has Down syndrome and how Edie is worried what other kids will think. I enjoyed reading the book and I liked the storyline and the characters. But I did not like the way Edie described how Merry looked. Edie said “Merry has something called Down syndrome, which makes her short and pudgy, with tiny ears and stubby fingers and eyes like pods and worst of all, it makes her slow.” I’ve had Down syndrome since I was born and I know people who have Down syndrome and even though we look different we are the same as anyone. That description was poor but I like the character of Merry. Merry was 14 year old and to Merry Edie is her favourite cousin. Correspondence spetko@rogers.com I think that the author, Annabel Lyon knows what Down syndrome is but doesn’t understand or feel what it is like to have Down syndrome. I think that the way Annabel showed Merry was positive because Merry did do a lot with the other characters in the book that she met. I think that the other characters, like Regan and Sam liked Merry and they treated her the same as anyone. + Edie went to the drama club and came up with the idea of putting on King Lear as a musical. She was put in charge. Edie’s parents wanted Edie to put Merry in the play because they wanted her to have a chance to do something with Edie. So Edie tried to put Merry in charge of the music and that didn’t work. Merry was at all the rehearsals and eventually Edie discovered that Merry knew all the lines for all the characters. When Edie had an argument with Raj who was playing the fool, he just wouldn’t do the part the way Edie thought he should, so she gave the role to Merry. But that did not work out well. “I realize now – what it must have looked like everyone thinks I was making fun of Merry. I realized the moment they started to Boo, they thought I was trying to get everyone to laugh at her, by putting her onstage and making her the fool. They didn’t realize that she could actually do the part, that it wasn’t just a grotesque practical joke. That, I realize, is what I have to explain that if we all just give her a chance, she can actually do the part.” Edie wasn’t thinking of Merry as someone who has Down syndrome but as person who can actually can play the part. Someone reading the book would be impressed by that. Edie did a good job by putting Merry in the play. There are lot of other interesting things that happen in the book. The characters in the book change and grow. In the end of the book Edie came to realize that she can be friends with Merry as well as her cousin. It is a good story and a good book and I would personally recommend this book because of the characters. You get to know a lot of characters and one of them especially, who just happens to have Downs syndrome. Lyon, A. (2011). Encore Edie. Puffin Books, 192 pp., ISBN 978‑0143177418 v . 18 n .2 77 Encore Edie Editorial Policy Journal on Developmental Disabilities / Le journal sur les handicaps du développement published by the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities, Toronto, ON ISSN: 118-9136 © Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities The Journal on Developmental Disabilities is published three times yearly by the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities (OADD). The Association has established the following objective for the journal: To foster and promote thoughtful and critical dialogue about relevant issues in the field of developmental disabilities, including those broader social issues that impact on persons with developmental disabilities. Content Each issue features a selection of research, conceptual, informational, and editorial papers. An issue may have a central theme. Reviews of relevant books, movies, websites, software, and other resources are welcomed, as are letters to the editor. In this way we strive to collectively enrich our understanding of issues, encourage stimulating debate among those working in the field, and improve services. Down Syndrome International is a supporter of the International Journal of Developmental Disabilities (formerly Down Syndrome Quarterly) Language Submissions are invited in either French or English and, if accepted, will be printed in the language of submission with an abstract in the other language. Submissions must be free of any gender and ethic bias. People-first language (e.g., persons with developmental disabilities) must be used throughout the manuscript (including figures and tables). Submissions must also use the non-possessive form in eponyms (e.g., Down Syndrome not Down’s Syndrome). Editorial Process Each submission will undergo a peer review by two or more reviewers with relevant expertise. The authors of the manuscripts will not be identified to the reviewers. The reviewers will recommend for or against publication and provide their reasons. Reviewers are asked to judge the manuscript on several criteria including its contribution to increasing our knowledge and clarity of communication. The reviewers are asked to write in a positive and constructive manner to help the authors improve their work, if necessary. Reviewers are asked to complete the reviews within two weeks using electronic communication. Final Decision The Editor-in-Chief, or guest editor, reserves the final decision regarding publication of a submission. To promote the publication of theme issues, the Editor reserves the right to decide in which issue to publish the article if it has been accepted. Copyright Design: Ted Myerscough Layout: Leah Gryfe Cover Image by Angela Gillis Marker on paper Angela Gillis attends the Manuela Dalla-Nora Program at Tycos Centre through Vita Community Living Services/Mens Sana. She has graced the program with her unique quirky style and her love of the arts for 25 years. Angie loves to spend hours on her artwork and is meticulous in the placement of lines and selection of colour that she uses, and always includes her signature within the art itself. Her art reflects her life in that moment. Manuscripts should not be submitted if previously published elsewhere or under review unless explicitly requested by the Editor. Once published, the article becomes the property of OADD. If an author wishes to re-publish materials originally published in the Journal on Developmental Disabilities, written permission is required from OADD. To re-publish previously published material in this journal, written permission must be obtained from the person or organization holding the copyright to the original material. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the individual authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities / L’association ontarienne sur les handicaps du développement.