As a PDF file - E
Transcription
As a PDF file - E
Semantic Fluency in Mild and Moderate Alzheimer’s Disease Seija Pekkala 2004 Department of Phonetics University of Helsinki P.O. Box 35 (Vironkatu 1B) 00014 University of Helsinki ISSN 0357-5217 ISBN 952-10-1645-0 (paperback) ISBN 952-10-1646-9 (PDF, http://ethesis.helsinki.fi/) Hakapaino Oy, Helsinki 2004 Copyright Seija Pekkala 2004 To my parents ABSTRACT Semantic Fluency in Mild and Moderate Alzheimer’s Disease Seija Pekkala University of Helsinki, FIN Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by an impairment of the semantic memory responsible for processing meaning-related knowledge. This study was aimed at examining how Finnish-speaking healthy elderly subjects (n = 30) and mildly (n = 20) and moderately (n = 20) demented AD patients utilize semantic knowledge to perform a semantic fluency task, a method of studying semantic memory. In this task subjects are typically given 60 seconds to generate words belonging to the semantic category of animals. Successful task performance requires fast retrieval of subcategory exemplars in clusters (e.g., farm animals: ‘cow’, ‘horse’, ‘sheep’) and switching between subcategories (e.g., pets, water animals, birds, rodents). In this study, the scope of the task was extended to cover various noun and verb categories. The results indicated that, compared with normal controls, both mildly and moderately demented AD patients showed reduced word production, limited clustering and switching, narrowed semantic space, and an increase in errors, particularly perseverations. However, the size of the clusters, the proportion of clustered words, and the frequency and prototypicality of words remained relatively similar across the subject groups. Although the moderately demented patients showed a poorer overall performance than the mildly demented patients in the individual categories, the error analysis appeared unaffected by the severity of AD. The results indicate a semantically rather coherent performance but less specific, effective, and flexible functioning of the semantic memory in mild and moderate AD patients. The findings are discussed in relation to recent theories of word production and semantic representation. Semantic fluency, clustering, switching, semantic category, nouns, verbs, Alzheimer’s disease Contents Acknowledgements ................................................................................................ i List of Tables ........................................................................................................ iii List of Figures ....................................................................................................... iv Abbreviations ......................................................................................................... v 1 Introduction ...................................................................................................... 1 2 Alzheimer’s disease ........................................................................................... 5 2.1 Etiology and pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease .......................................... 7 2.2 Clinical findings of Alzheimer’s disease ......................................................... 8 2.3 Staging the severity of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease ............................... 12 2.4 Semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease ................................................ 16 2.4.1 Impaired knowledge of the meaning representation ........................... 17 2.4.2 Impaired naming ................................................................................. 18 3 Frameworks of semantic knowledge ............................................................. 23 3.1 Principles of categorization ........................................................................... 26 3.1.1 Categorization of objects ..................................................................... 28 3.1.2 Subcategories and hierarchical structure of nouns .............................. 32 3.2 Feature-based models as accounts of the semantic representation of nouns . 34 3.3 Accounts of semantic representation of verbs ............................................... 39 3.3.1 Categorization of actions ..................................................................... 41 3.3.2 Subcategories and hierarchical structure of verbs ............................... 43 3.3.3 Feature-based models as accounts of the semantic representation of verbs ........................................................................ 47 3.3.4 Scripts as the account of semantic representation of verbs ........................................................................ 49 4 Spoken word production ................................................................................ 51 4.1 Theories of spoken word production ............................................................. 51 4.2 Two-stage interactive activation models ........................................................ 53 5 Semantic fluency performance in elderly adults and Alzheimer’s patients....................................................... 55 5.1 Word production during the semantic fluency task ....................................... 56 5.2 Clustering and switching ............................................................................... 66 5.3 Activation of different associations and semantic dimensions ...................... 68 5.4 Error analysis ................................................................................................. 70 5.4.1 Intrusions ............................................................................................. 71 5.4.2 Perseverations ...................................................................................... 73 5.5 Performance in different semantic categories ................................................ 74 6 Causes of the semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease........................................................................................ 77 6.1 Breakdown and loss of semantic structures ................................................... 77 6.2 Impaired processing ....................................................................................... 79 6.3 A multifactorial deficit ................................................................................... 80 7 Aims of the study ............................................................................................ 83 8 Method ............................................................................................................. 85 8.1 Subjects .......................................................................................................... 85 8.2 Method ........................................................................................................... 86 8.2.1 Procedure of the semantic fluency tasks ............................................. 86 8.2.2 Analysis of the overall performance on the semantic fluency tasks .... 87 8.2.3 Clustering rules ................................................................................... 89 8.2.4 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the semantic fluency tasks ........................................................................................ 90 8.2.5 Inter-rater judgements ......................................................................... 92 8.2.6 Control tasks ........................................................................................ 93 8.2.7 Statistical analysis ............................................................................... 94 9 Results .............................................................................................................. 97 9.1 Overall performance on the noun fluency tasks ............................................ 97 9.1.1 Number of correct nouns ..................................................................... 99 9.1.2 Clustering and switching ................................................................... 100 9.1.3 Summary of the results and discussion ............................................. 103 9.2 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the noun fluency tasks .......... 109 9.2.1 Proportion of correct nouns ............................................................... 109 9.2.2 Proportion of intrusions and perseverations ...................................... 109 9.2.3 Clustering strategies .......................................................................... 112 9.2.4 Number and variety of different semantic subcategories .................. 114 9.2.5 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the nouns produced ...... 118 9.2.6 Summary of the results and discussion ............................................. 120 9.3 Overall performance on the verb fluency tasks ........................................... 128 9.3.1 Number of correct verbs .................................................................... 128 9.3.2 Clustering and switching ................................................................... 131 9.3.3 Summary of the results and discussion ............................................. 133 9.4 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the verb fluency tasks ........... 135 9.4.1 Proportion of correct verbs ................................................................ 136 9.4.2 Proportion of intrusions and perseverations ...................................... 138 9.4.3 Number and variety of different semantic subcategories .................. 140 9.4.4 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the verbs produced ....... 144 9.4.5 Summary of the results and discussion ............................................. 144 9.5 Summary of the overall semantic fluency performance .............................. 152 9.6 Performance on the control tasks................................................................. 154 9.6.1 Performance on the control tasks requiring verbal responses ........... 154 9.6.2 Performance on the control tasks requiring non-verbal responses .... 155 9.6.3 Correlations among scores on the semantic tasks ............................. 156 9.6.4 Discussion on the semantic tasks ...................................................... 157 10 General discussion ...................................................................................... 161 10.1 Semantic fluency performance in mild and moderate Alzheimer’s disease162 10.1.1 Decreased semantic fluency performance ....................................... 162 10.1.2 Errors as indicators of impaired semantic memory functioning ..... 165 10.1.3 Causes of the semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease ............ 167 10.2 Methodological considerations of the study .............................................. 171 10.2.1 Subjects ........................................................................................... 171 10.2.2 Considerations of the semantic fluency task ................................... 173 10.2.3 Limitations of the study ................................................................... 177 10.3 Clinical implications .................................................................................. 179 10.4 Implications for further study .................................................................... 180 11 Conclusions ................................................................................................. 183 References ......................................................................................................... 187 Appendix 1A. Cluster division for the noun categories .................................... 225 Appendix 1B. Cluster division for the verb categories ...................................... 228 Appendix 2 A sample of prototypicality ratings of the words produced for the semantic fluency tasks ...................................................................... 231 Appendix 3 A sample of frequency ratings of the words produced for the semantic fluency tasks ...................................................................... 233 Appendix 4A. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group : clothes .................................. 235 Appendix 4B. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: vegetables ............................. 236 Appendix 4C. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: vehicles ................................. 237 Appendix 4D. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: animals .................................. 238 Appendix 4E. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: preparing food....................... 239 Appendix 4F. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: playing sports ........................ 240 Appendix 4G. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: construction .......................... 241 Appendix 4H. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: cleaning up ............................ 242 Appendix 5. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the noun fluency tasks .................................................................................. 243 Appendix 6. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the verb fluency tasks ................................................................................... 246 Appendix 7. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the control tasks .... 249 i Acknowledgements This study was originally inspired by the learning experiences I gained in the early 1990s when writing my Master’s thesis about the semantic breakdown found to take place in Alzheimer’s disease. I am very grateful to Professor Matti Lehtihalmes, who first encouraged me to continue studying the field I had found fascinating, for giving me good advice and support during these years. I am very thankful to Dr. Timo Erkinjuntti for enabling this study to be carried out at the Department of Neurology, University Hospital of the University of Helsinki. I also want to thank Raija Ahlfors and Toini Nukari for their co-operation in many practical arrangements that needed to be taken care of during data collection. My warmest thanks are due to my supervisors. I want to thank Professor Anu Klippi for her valuable comments on this manuscript and for her patience and encouragement throughout this study. I would also like to cordially thank Dr. Minna Laakso for her interest in my work and helpful advice during the later phases of this dissertation. I would like to express my sincerest thanks and greatest appreciation to Dr. Inga-Britt Persson who so willingly shared her knowledge of semantics and spread her enthusiasm for it. I will always remember the long hours of brainstorming, the vivid flow of ideas and intense discussions, which helped me understand many theoretical issues and combine theory and practice. I would especially like to thank Anu Airola for her friendship and significant linguistic contribution in co-analysing the data of this study. I want to thank Professor Anneli Pajunen, Dr. Marja-Liisa Helasvuo, and Dr. Tiina Onikki-Rantajääskö for advising me on many questions requiring expertise in linguistics and, in particular, the Finnish language. I am very grateful to Dr. Kimmo Vehkalahti and Pekka Lahti-Nuuttila for all their help and guidance with the statistical matters of this study. I want to thank the statisticians Marjatta Mankinen, Leena Pussinen, and Leena Kuukasjärvi at the University of Oulu for their contribution when clearing up the final statistical issues that needed to be resolved in order to complete this thesis. I wish to express my most sincere gratitude and appreciation to Professor Loraine K. Obler, The City University of New York, and Professor Matti Laine, Åbo Akademi, the pre-examiners of this dissertation, for their very constructive criticism and good suggestions for improving the manuscript. I am very grateful to Roderick Dixon for carefully revising the English of the manuscript. I cordially thank my colleagues and fellow-students Dr. Helena Heimo and Dr. Eira Jansson-Verkasalo for sharing with me the ups and downs common to all graduate students. I would also like to thank Jenni Holappa and Riitta Nauha, as well as Minna Vanhala and Outi Kaleva, for exchanging ideas about the semantic fluency task which turned out to be our common interest. I want to thank my ii colleagues at the Department of Phonetics, University of Helsinki, for their encouraging attitude towards this study. I would like to express my gratitude to Dr. Anna-Maija Korpijaakko-Huuhka for her support throughout all these years. Furthermore, my thanks go to my new co-workers at the Department of Finnish, Saami and Logopedics, University of Oulu, for all the help I received when finishing this dissertation. I am extremely grateful to Jarmo Herkman, Lic.Psych, for his friendship, support and invaluable help with a number of stumbling blocks that I came across when working on this thesis. Without his positive attitude and great sense of humor, many things would have become insurmountable obstacles. I am deeply grateful to all the participants who voluntarily made an invaluable contribution to this study. I want to thank them for giving me the great opportunity to learn about semantics and, in particular, to find out how Alzheimer’s disease affects the semantic memory that is so fundamental to our linguistic abilities. I also wish to thank the caregivers for their co-operation and positive attitude towards this study. I am privileged and happy to have many friends around me who form a very important cornerstone of my life. I am very grateful to them for helping me in various ways during this study. I also thank them for always being there for me when I needed them most. I want to express my special thanks to Kirsti Eskelinen and Tuomo Härkönen, Matti Hämäläinen, Maarit Koivurova, Jaana Lamminperä, Kari Lehtonen, Kirsi, Reijo and Ronja Leino, Riitta-Leena and Mikko Manninen and their children, Elizabeth Milliman and Ernie Taylor, Nökö Mähönen, Sari Salmisuo, Tatu Ulvila, and Marja Vuorinen. I am also very grateful to my family and relatives who have unceasingly encouraged me during all these long years of study. I want to thank my cousin Helinä Vanhala for helping me code the data and sort out journal articles needed for this study. Many thanks are due to my sister Kaisa and my brother-in-law Timo Leinonen for all the care and help they have given me. I wish to thank my niece Sofia and my nephews Ville-Petteri and Tuomas for bringing great joy into my life. I owe my sincerest thanks to my parents, Lempi and Helge Pekkala, to whom this dissertation is dedicated, for their understanding and willingness to support me not only in my studies, but also throughout my life. When working on this dissertation, I had a chance to participate in the activities of the Langnet, Finnish National Graduate School for Language Studies, which I gratefully acknowledge. I wish to thank the Finnish Cultural Foundation, Finnish Konkordia Fund, Vetenskapsstiftelse för Kvinnor, and the University of Helsinki for the financial support which made this study possible. Seija Pekkala Helsinki, December 2003 iii List of Tables Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 12 Table 13 Table 14 Table 15 Table 16 Table 17 Table 18 Table 19 Table 20 Table 21 Table 22 Table 23 Table 24 Page Criteria of probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 6 Clinical findings in different stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 9 Progress of language deficits and communication changes in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 11 Studies on the semantic fluency in normal control subjects (NC) and patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 60 Demographic features of the subject groups 87 Scoring of clustering and switching on the semantic fluency tasks 88 Total number of words and number of correct nouns produced in the noun fluency tasks 98 Number of correct nouns produced by the male and female participants in the NC group 100 Clustering and switching in the noun fluency tasks 101 Data on the animal fluency task performed by normal control subjects (NC) and Alzheimer’s patients (AD) in different studies 107 Proportion of correct words, intrusions, and perseverations in the noun fluency tasks 110 Number of semantically related and unrelated intrusions produced in the noun fluency tasks 111 Clustering strategies in the noun fluency tasks 113 Number of different subcategories produced for the semantic categories in the noun fluency tasks 115 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the nouns produced 119 Total number of words and number of correct verbs produced in the verb fluency tasks 129 Number of correct verbs produced by the male and female participants in the NC group 130 Clustering and switching in the verb fluency tasks 132 Word forms produced in the verb fluency tasks 136 Proportion of correct words, intrusions, and perseverations in the verb fluency tasks 137 Number of semantically related and unrelated intrusions produced in the verb fluency tasks 139 Number of different subcategories produced for the semantic categories in the verb fluency tasks 141 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the verbs produced 145 Summary of the comparison of the semantic fluency performance between the subject groups 153 iv Table 25 Table 26 Table 27 Performance of the subject groups on the control tasks requiring verbal responses Performance of the subject groups on the control tasks requiring non-verbal responses Spearman rank-order correlation coefficients (ρ) between the correct responses of the semantic fluency tasks and the control tasks in the subject groups 155 156 157 List of Figures Page Figure 1 Figure 2 Figure 3 Figure 4 Figure 5 Figure 6 Figure 7 Figure 8 The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of clothes in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of vegetables in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of vehicles in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of animals in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of preparing food in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of playing sports in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of construction in different subject groups The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of cleaning up in different subject groups 116 116 117 117 142 142 143 143 v Abbreviations AD ANT BNT miAD MMSE moAD NC Alzheimer’s disease Action Naming Test Boston Naming Test Alzheimer’s patients with mild dementia Mini Mental State Examination test Alzheimer’s patients with moderate dementia Normal control subjects vi 1 Introduction Although increased knowledge and wisdom are believed to come with age, scientific evidence has indicated that a variety of mental processes decline with advancing age. Signs of cognitive aging include a decrease in the speed of performing mental operations and limited working memory processes, such as retrieving and storing information. Finding words and retrieving names of people and places may cause trouble for elderly people, as well as remembering details belonging to past episodes (Craik 2000; Park 2000). Sensory functions (e.g., vision and hearing), which are fundamental to cognitive abilities, seem to decline, and focusing attention on target information and inhibiting attention to irrelevant material may become difficult (Park 2000). Aging does not always take place normally. A faster and more severe decline in multiple cognitive functions is found in dementia, which is a common clinical syndrome among elderly people. It was estimated that in 2000 approximately 30, 000 people suffered from mild dementia and 80, 000 from moderate or severe dementia in Finland (Viramo & Sulkava 2001). As the population ages, the number of people with dementia increases, which makes varying demands on the society in terms of planning appropriate assessment and management and providing services for those in need. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia affecting a person’s behavior, personality, and social skills. AD has also been characterized by a progressive decline in a wide range of cognitive functions, with memory problems being one of the earliest, as well as the most disabling, of the cognitive deficits (Nebes, Martin & Horn 1984; Martin, Browers, Cox & Fedio 1985). In addition to a defective episodic memory, responsible for maintaining recent experiences, there is evidence that also the part of long-term memory called semantic memory, responsible for storing meaning-related knowledge, is particularly impaired in AD (e.g., Warrington 1975; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Martin & Fedio 1983; Nebes 1989; Hodges, Salmon & Butters 1990; Huff, Corkin & Growdon 1986; Chan, Butters, Paulsen, Salmon, Swenson & Maloney 1993). The role of semantic memory is crucial for communication and language processing, including word production and comprehension, which have been found to be impaired in AD (Nebes 1989). The language processing deficits are known to be frequent and to take place at an 2 Introduction early phase of the disease (e.g., Appell, Kertesz & Fisman 1982; Huff et al. 1986; Nebes, Brady & Huff 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990). However, all aspects of language function do not appear to be equally impaired in AD. The most impaired domain of language seems to be semantics and pragmatics, while phonology and syntax are relatively well preserved (Nebes et al. 1989). The semantic fluency task, in which a subject is asked to produce words for a particular semantic category (e.g., animals) in a certain period of time, is a method widely used by speech pathologists and neuropsychologists to investigate retrieval of words from semantic memory. The task is considered to be a very simple, fast, and sensitive clinical task that provides useful information about the functioning of the semantic processes and the status of the semantic representations (Binetti et al. 1995). The performance of the subjects is usually analyzed by counting correct responses and errors, such as category violations and repetitions of words. There seems to be a strategy commonly applied by the subjects to perform the task that involves a cycle of clustering semantically related words (e.g., farm animals) and switching to another subcategory (e.g., birds, fish, exotic animals, etc.; Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980; Laine 1989:4-5, 20-25; Troyer, Moscovitch & Winocur 1997). There is evidence indicating that AD patients tend not to perform the task in the way normal elderly control subjects do. The semantic fluency performance of the AD patients is characterized by a reduction in word production and an increase in errors, as well as a limited use of clustering and switching (e.g., Beatty, Testa, English & Winn 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach & Freedman 1998; Tröster et al. 1998). The impaired semantic fluency performance in AD is attributed to disruptions in the processing and/or organization of semantic memory (e.g., Warrington 1975; Martin & Fedio 1983; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). Although an extensive literature documents the performance deficits of patients with AD on the semantic fluency task, the previous studies seem to have some limitations, which served as the motivation for the present study. First, only very few studies have provided detailed theoretical descriptions of the cognitive processes (e.g., categorization, word production, clustering, and switching) necessary for performing the task, and the “semantics” of the contents of the responses has not attracted as much attention as it deserves. Furthermore, the nature and organization of the knowledge contained in semantic memory have usually been defined in very vague and general terms. Second, the more detailed analyses of semantic fluency performance, in general, have been restricted solely to the semantic category of animals or the things obtainable in a supermarket. Recent theoretical research (e.g., Moss, Tyler & Devlin 2002) suggests that different semantic categories have distinctive representations, which is why generalizations of the subjects’ performance should not be made on the basis of one or two semantic categories. Third, very little investigation has been done on the effects of the severity of dementia on semantic fluency performance. Semantic deficits being one of the hallmarks of AD, it is worthwhile knowing about the nature of the progressive changes, which also includes semantic fluency behavior. Such knowledge can be used when assessing the phase Introduction 3 of the disease a patient is in. It may also be useful for differential diagnosis. Fourth, reports on how the semantic fluency task can be extended to involve the processing of other grammatical classes, especially verbs, are virtually nonexistent. Implications of the contents and structure of semantic memory have so far been predominantly made based on findings obtained from testing with nouns, which may be easier to study but which do not represent the whole spectrum of lexical-semantic information contained in semantic memory. Because AD has been found to result in deterioration of the semantic structure of nouns, it can be presumed that other word classes carrying meaning-related information are also impacted by the disease. The present study attempts to address these issues. Typical of the field of logopedics, searching for answers in the present study required a multidisciplinary approach to the phenomenon, which turned out to be quite a challenge. Although the semantic fluency task is a common method in logopedics, the findings obtained from the task have mostly been interpreted in the traditions of cognitive psychology and neuropsychology. These fields of study tend to focus on explaining the functions and organization of memory, and to cover one of the most fundamental semantic processes underlying the semantic fluency performance, categorization. In order to explain the nature and format of semantic representation, the notions of the current connectionist theories were also considered. To account for the other essential lexical process involved in the semantic fluency task, word production, it was necessary to focus on current theories on lexical retrieval in psycho- and neurolinguistics. As a consequence of bringing together different traditions and conventions, matching the definitions for various concepts was difficult. In particular, defining the scope of semantic memory and the mental lexicon was cumbersome and using the terms object vs. noun and action vs. verb to refer to the mental representations and to the lexemes was at issue throughout the study. When referring to different lexemes in the text, single quotation marks are used to make the distinction (e.g., ‘sheep’, ‘cut’). Examples of semantic features (e.g., ‘has-a-tail’) and parts of scripts (e.g., ‘standing-in-a-line’) are also marked. When considering the structure of this dissertation, a matter worth mentioning is that, in general, most of the research concerning semantic processing and representation has so far been focused on nouns, and there is still relatively little experimentally obtained knowledge about how verbs may be semantically represented and processed. Thus, in this study, it may appear that the issues concerning verbs are discussed at a shallower level compared to those concerning nouns. However, despite the diverse traditions, the different terminology in the fields, and the less explored field of the semantic representation of verbs in relation to that of nouns, an attempt is made to provide an understandable synthesis of these approaches to explain the semantic fluency performance. In addition to investigating the abovementioned theoretical issues, the purpose of this study was to provide a systematic and detailed analysis about the way healthy elderly control subjects and AD patients with mild and moderate dementia performed the semantic fluency task. In the present study, the subjects were asked to produce 4 Introduction nouns for several different categories including categories of clothes, vegetables, vehicles, and animals. An attempt was made to apply the task to also cover verb production. The subjects were asked to produce concrete verbs for the categories of preparing food, playing sports, construction, and cleaning. Evaluation of the semantic fluency performance included measuring the total and correct output for each category, as well as the clustering of semantically closely related words and switching between the semantic subcategories. An error analysis was conducted to determine the proportion of outside-category words (intrusions) and repetition of previously generated words (perseverations) produced during the tasks. The nature of the semantic dimensions activated for different semantic categories by the subject groups was examined. The results are discussed in relation to other studies on semantic fluency tasks as well as to some control tasks designed to measure semantic processing. The results are also discussed in light of the current theories on semantic representation and word production. 2 Alzheimer’s disease Alzheimer’s disease (AD) was named after a German neuropsychiatrist, Professor Aloys Alzheimer (1864-1915), who was the first to discover symptoms of advancing difficulty in memory and language functions and disorientation in his patient, Auguste D (Alzheimer 1907; Maurer, Volk & Gerbaldo 1997; see also Maurer, Ihl & Frölich 1993:1-4). The symptoms progressed to severe dementia and caused the patient to die in a few years after their appearance. Alzheimer related the symptoms to the changes found in Auguste D’s brain at autopsy: atrophy of the brain, senile plaques, and neurofibrillary changes which later were considered to be the characteristic neuropathological findings of the disease. AD appears to be the most common form of dementia, a common clinical syndrome that progressively affects multiple cognitive functions, such as memory functions, language skills, praxis, visuospatial perception, and executive functions, and causes a remarkable restriction of social and occupational competence among elderly people (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association 1994). AD accounts for 65-70% of the moderately or severely demented patients while vascular dementia, the second most common cause of dementia, is evident in 15-20% of the cases. Other rarer diseases (e.g., Lewy’s body, frontotemporal dementia, Pick’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, Hakola’s disease, Huntington’s disease, Creutzfeldt-Jacob’s disease) and factors such as disturbances in cerebral blood flow, brain injury, brain tumors, infection, drugs, and toxins have also been reported to result in a broad dysfunction of the brain typical of dementia (Erkinjuntti 2001; Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Maurer et al. 1993:5-14; Fratiglioni et al. 2000; Viramo & Sulkava 2001). The criteria for the clinical definition of AD are introduced both in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association 1994) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Organization 1993), and by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Associations (NINCDS-ADRDA; McKhann et al. 1984). The NINCDS-ADRDA criteria divide AD into definite, probable, and possible levels of diagnostic certainty. The criteria of probable AD can be found in Table 1. These criteria are the most 6 Alzheimer’s disease Table 1. Criteria of probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Clinical picture Findings supporting AD clinically diagnosed dementia, confirmed by neuropsychological examination deterioration of cognitive functions (aphasia, apraxia, agnosia) disorder in at least two cognitive functions deteriorating dysfunction of memory and cognition normal level of arousal Findings applicable to AD, unless other etiology is found periods of plateau deteriorating skills in daily living, changes in behavior depression, sleeplessness, incontinence, delusions, hallucinations family history sexual disorders normal spinal fluids loss of weight normal EEG or nonspecific changes tension, myoclonus, difficulty in walking brain atrophy in CT/MRI epileptic seizures age of onset between 40-90 years no general or brain diseases that could explain the dysfunction of memory and cognition normal age-related CT/MRI findings (Modified from Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001:138) broadly used and their reliability is estimated to be 85-90% when clinically diagnosing probable AD (Cummings, Vinters, Cole & Khachaturian 1998; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). In addition to the clinical picture and neuropsychological tests, laboratory tests, imaging studies (e.g., computer tomography, CT; magnetic resonance imaging, MRI; positron emission topography, PET; single photon emission computed tomography, SPECT), as well as neurophysiological studies (EEG), are used to diagnose probable AD (Maurer et al. 1993:21-50; Bauer 1994:66-76; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). During the lifetime of a patient with AD, a brain biopsy may support the clinical findings of AD, but the definitive diagnosis is made on the basis of neuropathological findings at autopsy (Cummings et al. 1998; Alafuzoff 2001). AD is an age-related and irreversible disease with an insidious onset and slow and stable progression (e.g., Maurer et al. 1993:5; Pirttilä and Erkinjuntti 2001). The most prominent symptom is the progressive dementia. AD results in a memory loss, behavior and personality changes, and a decline in intellectual, professional, social, and everyday functions. Different forms have been called presenile AD (onset before 65 years) and senile AD (onset after 65 years), but today they are considered the same disease (Sulkava 1982; Sulkava & Amberla 1982; Sulkava 1983:7-8; Erkinjuntti 1988:13; Sulkava, Erkinjuntti & Palo 1989:42-44; Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001). According to Pirttilä and Erkinjuntti (2001), the neuropathology and the clinical symptomatology associated with AD are, with some rarely occurring variants of AD excluded, similar in both groups of patients despite differences in the age of onset, etiology of the disease, or gender. The course of the disease and the rate of decline vary from person to person. In most people with AD, the first symptoms seem to appear between 60 and 70 years of age. The age at which AD is most often Alzheimer’s disease 7 diagnosed is 70-80 years (Baumann, Tienari & Haltia 1999). The average life expectancy after the diagnosis of AD is 10 years, but it can vary from 2 to 16 years (Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). The prevalence (the number of people with the disease at any one time) of AD is estimated to be 0.5% among people under 65 years, 1% among people aged 65-70 years, and 30% among those older than 85 years (Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). In Europe, the prevalence of clinically diagnosed AD is estimated to be 4.4% among people older than 65 years, the proportion being a little lower among younger people and higher among females than males, especially among very old individuals (Lobo et al. 2000). The incidence (the number of new cases) of AD among people older than 75 is 10/1000 and among people over 90 63/1000 in Western and Northern Europe, and 47/1000 in Southern Europe. In all age groups, females are more vulnerable to AD than males (Fratiglioni et al. 2000; Viramo & Sulkava 2001). 2.1 Etiology and pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease Two types of AD exist: familial AD (FAD) and sporadic AD. FAD has an early onset, usually before 60 years of age, and it is known to be caused by mutations in three genes located in three different chromosomes (chromosomes 1, 14, and 21; Bauer 1994:54-56; Cummings et al. 1998; Lehtovirta 2001). The mutations are inherited in an autosomal dominant mode of transmission, and they account for less than 10% of patients with AD. Most patients with AD have dementia syndromes that have developed later in life and are sporadic in nature. Several risk factors have been identified or are likely to cause sporadic AD, such as advanced age, female gender, lower intelligence, lower educational level, depression, small head size, advanced age of the mother at subject’s birth, Down syndrome, history of head trauma, hyperthyreosis, vascular dysfunction of the brain, inflammatory processes, high cholesterol, viruses, toxins, and yet undetermined environmental influences (e.g., Maurer et al. 1993:69-74; Cummings et al. 1998; Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Soininen 2001). The neuropathology typical of AD may start to develop even 20-30 years before the appearance of the first symptoms. Histopathologically, the accumulation of the phosphorylized tau protein and the formation of neurofibrillary tangles in the neurons and the accumulation of toxic beta-amyloid in the extracellular neuritic plaques are likely to destroy neurons in AD (Maurer et al. 1993:51-67; Bauer 1994:3049; Cummings et al. 1998; Baumann et al. 1999; Alafuzoff 2001; Partanen, Mäntylä, Erkinjuntti & Rinne 2001; Tienari & Haltia 2001). Other changes include presence of amyloid in the small blood vessels (amyloid angiopathy), granulovacuolar degeneration, and loss of synapses and neurons. Several systems of neurotransmitters can be impacted, especially damage to the cholinergic transmitters in the basal ganglia. Microscopically, a global loss of neurons is found in the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex located in the medial temporal lobe. The pathways that connect 8 Alzheimer’s disease the hippocampus and the entorhinal region with the rest of the cortex and the cholinergic system in the basal forebrain—the neural systems that subserve learning, memory, attention, and behavior—are vulnerable to AD. Later, the process of atrophy spreads to the neocortex of the temporal lobe, followed by a central and cortical atrophy. Macroscopically, the brains of the AD patients show atrophy of the cerebral gyri and associated widening of the sulci, dilatation of the lateral and third ventricles, and a decrease in weight. The areas most severely involved in the atrophic process are the medial temporal lobe, especially the entorhinal cortex and the hippocampus. 2.2 Clinical findings of Alzheimer’s disease Alzheimer (1907; see also Maurer et al. 1997) observed that Auguste D had problems in many cognitive areas and communication, such as memory function, praxia, speech production and comprehension, conversational skills, reading, and writing. While her articulation seemed to be normal, she tended to forget recently presented material easily, to produce semantic substitutions for words, to repeat her speech, and to use incomplete sentences and out-of-context utterances. In 1907, Alzheimer documented the most typical cognitive deficits for the disease that was later to be named after him. After Alzheimer’s first observations, it has been discovered that the clinical findings of AD are numerous and diverse. Abnormal neurological symptoms are few and rare during the early stages of AD, but they are likely to appear towards the more advanced stages (see Table 2). The most typical neurological findings of AD seem to be slowness of motor movements, extrapyramidal signs (e.g., hypomimia, hypokinesia, rigidity, posture and gait abnormalities), primitive reflexes (e.g., suck and grasp reflexes), as well as involuntary and apractic movements (Bauer 1994:1415; Stern et al. 1996). Behavioral and psychiatric symptoms are present in AD in the form of inappropriate verbal bursts, physical aggression, agitation, irritability, restlessness, and difficulty in sleeping (Burns, Jacoby & Levy 1990a, b, c, d; Rubin, Kinscherf & Morris 1993; Bauer 1994:15-16; Mega, Cummings, Fiorello & Gornbein 1996; Saarela, Koponen, Erkinjuntti, Alhainen & Viramo 1997; Soininen 1997; Vataja 2001; see Table 2). Suspiciousness, paranoid thoughts, visual and auditory hallucinations, and delusional misidentification of people and events are common (Burns et al. 1990a, b; Binetti et al. 1993). Apathy, slowness of functions, and lack of initiative are found (Saarela et al. 1997), as well as mood changes, carelessness, eating disorders, and disturbed sexual functions (Burns et al. 1990d; Soininen 1997). Depression is often associated with the phase when indications of cognitive decline and memory dysfunction are identified, as well as with the neurochemical changes in the neurotransmitters, such as deficiency of noradrelanin and serotonin (Burns et al. 1990c; Saarela et al. 1997; Vataja 2001). Alzheimer’s disease 9 Table 2. Clinical findings in different stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Early AD MMSE 26-30 Cognitive skills Functional ability Behavior deterioration of learning slowness and uncertainty at work stress difficulty in coping with new and demanding situations low-spiritedness impaired retrieval of recently learned names impaired skills in foreign languages Somatic symptoms exhaustion reduced activity in hobbies Mild AD MMSE 18-26 difficulty in following slowing down of executive conversation reduction in reading functions apathy loss of weight withdrawal slowing of motor functions agitation apraxia withdrawal from complicated hobbies and deterioration of judgement interests difficulty in planning and problem solving housekeeping tasks deterioration of anxiety increased forgetfulness decrease in initiation of action depression delusions jealousy difficulty in handling difficulty in finding words finances and shopping difficulty in taking difficulty in calculating responsibility for own medication concentration decrease in working skills use of memory aids Moderate AD MMSE 10-22 poor memory for recent events impaired instrumental activities (ADL) delusions loss of weight hallucinations apraxia difficulties in speech production impaired cooking skills paranoia difficulty in dressing appropriately psychomotor restlessness extrapyramidal symptoms: rigidity, slowness, inexpressiveness of face good physical condition impaired conversational skills losing things difficulty in perception wandering getting lost disorientation sleep disorder need to be reminded of things depression loss of insight dyspraxia visuospatial difficulties poor concentration Severe AD MMSE 0-12 poor memory functions poor speech production, echolalia very poor speech comprehension inability to concentrate disorientation severe apraxia requires assistance in managing basic functions (ADL) impaired driving skills apathy relatively well preserved personality and social skills behavior problems restlessness requires much help with basic ADL functions: behavior problems dressing, personal hygiene, outbursts going to the toilet, eating agitation incontinence apraxia extrapyramidal symptoms poor walking (festinating) sleep disorder primitive reflexes: grasp, snout and suck reflexes depression low blood pressure deviant motor behavior (Modified from Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001:333 and Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001:132134); ADL = Activities of Daily Living; MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination (Folstein et al. 1975) 10 Alzheimer’s disease AD seems to affect a wide range of cognitive functions that deteriorate slowly and selectively (Almkvist & Bäckman 1993; Pulliainen & Kuikka 1998). The speed of the deterioration varies inter-individually (Braak & Braak 1991). The profile of the cognitive decline is more severe and more biased to memory dysfunction in AD than in the normal aging process (Soininen & Hänninen 1999). The most typical and early feature seems to be memory dysfunction, especially in the domain of episodic memory, leading to a loss of memory of recent things and experiences (Huff et al. 1987; Almkvist 1996; Soininen & Hänninen 1999). Primary (short-term) memory, as well as semantic and procedural memory, are likely to be better preserved during the early phases of the disease (Morris 1994; Almkvist 1996; Soininen & Hänninen 1999). However, AD may cause global amnesia in its more advanced stages (Almkvist 1996). Disorders also tend to appear in verbal abilities, visuospatial functions, attention, and executive functions. Furthermore, deficits in sensory-motor performance, agnosia (disorder of recognition), apraxia (disorder of skilled movements), and acalculia (disorder of arithmetic skills) are likely to occur in AD (Travniczek-Marterer, Danielczyk, Simanyi & Fischer 1993; Bauer 1994:12; Almkvist 1996, Carlomagno et al. 1999). Language and communication disorders also seem to be among the most outstanding and early symptoms in AD, and the deficits tend to change during the process of the disease (Obler & Albert 1981; Obler 1983; Bayles 1982; Martin & Fedio 1983; Seltzer & Sherwin 1983; Cummings, Benson, Hill & Read 1985; Smith, Murdoch & Chenery 1989; Bayles, Boone, Tomoeda, Slauson & Kaszniak 1989; Bayles & Tomoeda 1991; Kempler 1995). The findings on language impairment seem to be heterogeneous: More severe language impairment has been found in early-onset than in late-onset AD (Seltzer & Sherwin 1983). Contrarily, Bayles (1991) found that late onset AD patients were more impaired than patients with an early onset of the disease. However, according to more recent investigations, the younger and the older onset patients seemed to have somewhat similar language impairment and the degree of language impairment was highly correlated with the severity of the dementia, regardless of the age of onset (Cummings et al. 1985; Selnes, Carson, Rovner & Gordon 1988; Murdoch, Chenery, Wilks & Boyle 1987). Furthermore, some studies have reported gender differences among AD patients’ language skills, with female AD patients performing significantly worse on language production tasks than male AD patients (Henderson & Buckwalter 1994; Buckwalter et al. 1996). The findings of a more recent longitudinal study, however, have shown that the decline in language function and other domains of cognitive function in AD may not vary by gender (Hebert et al. 2000). The language and communication problems in AD are largely confined to two aspects of language; semantics and pragmatics, with preservation of phonology and syntax (see Table 3). These problems may be expressed as impaired speech production and comprehension, deteriorated reading and writing abilities, and poor conversational skills (e.g., Schwartz, Marin & Saffran 1979; Appell et al. 1982; Bayles 1982; Cummings et al. 1985; Kempler, Curtiss & Jackson 1987; Murdoch et Alzheimer’s disease 11 Table 3. Progress of language deficits and commincation changes in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Pragmatics Symptoms in mild AD Symptoms in moderate AD Symptoms in severe AD preserved conversational skills compromised conversational skills: poor topic maintenance and use of reference, knows when to talk, responds to questions but has lost sensitivity to conversational partners, repeats ideas, forgets topic, talks about the past or trivia, lacks ideas, uses stereotypical utterances (“ How are you?” ) poor conversational skills: lack of coherence, poor maintenance of eye contact and turn-taking, few ideas, meaningless and bizarre utterances, severe word finding deficit some difficulty in giving instructions and storytelling pronominal referencing may cause confusion expresses needs of clarification and confirmation difficulty in understanding humor, analogies, sarcasm, metaphors, and abstract expressions initiation of conversation may be inappropriate and unsuccessful perseveration mutism in final stage vague, incomplete, and irrelevant responses lacks self-correction of speech errors difficulty in accounting for the situational context can digress from topic generating series of meaningful sentences may fail vague, incomplete, and irrelevant responses uses compliments and expressions of appreciation Semantics deteriorated word fluency and word finding poor word fluency and confrontation naming uses circumlocutions, gesture, or diminished vocabulary associated words to compensate uses circumlocutions, unrelated for word finding difficulties word substitutions, and empty compromised comprehension of speech abstract and/or complex impaired comprehension of concepts cause-effect (sequential) relationships Syntax no errors generally grammatical mistakes, simplified syntax difficulty in comprehending complex structures paraphasia, echolalia, palilalia poor comprehension very limited vocabulary jargon grammar generally preserved incomplete sentences poor comprehension of syntactic structures Phonology normal articulation, pitch, volume, and speaking rate phonological and articulatory impairments may occur (e.g., false starts, paraphasias, articulatory difficulty) Reading difficulty in reading for comprehension, intact mechanics for reading aloud compromised coherent and well- severely impaired formed reading, preserved mechanical reading skills Writing difficulty in generating spontaneous written language, intact mechanics for writing compromised coherent and well- severely impaired formed writing, preserved mechanical writing skills more frequent errors, repetition of nonsense sounds (After Huff 1988; Fromm & Holland 1989; Kempler 1995; Ripich & Ziol 1998:476; Croot et al. 2000, and Orange & Ryan 2000) 12 Alzheimer’s disease al. 1987; Horner, Heyman, Dawson & Rogers 1988; Fromm & Holland 1989; Rapcsak, Arthur, Bliklen & Rubens 1989; Mentis, Briggs-Whittaker & Gramigna 1995; Kempler 1995; Ripich & Ziol 1998; Luzzatti, Laiacona & Agazzi 2003). Morpho-syntactic abilities are relatively well preserved (Obler & Albert 1981; Murdoch et al. 1987; Smith, Chenery & Murdoch 1989; Kempler 1995; cf. Obler & Gjerlow 1999:94-95, 97-99) even in the moderate and late stages (Bayles 1982; Kempler et al. 1987), even though the conveyance of meaningful information may be poor (Appell et al. 1982; Murdoch et al. 1987). Phonologic deficits and motor speech deficits (e.g., poor control of phonation, dysarthria) may be selectively present in early and moderate stages (Croot, Hodges, Xuereb & Patterson 2000), but they are usually not observed until the latest stages of the disease (Kempler 1995). The impairment of semantic processing and functional communication can have a devastating effect on the communicational skills of AD patients (Huff et al. 1986; Fromm & Holland 1989). Failing attention, memory dysfunction, anomia, and more general pragmatic deficits may contribute to the discourse problems in AD or AD may selectively affect specific discourse knowledge, that is, knowledge of the rules and the type of information used in a discourse (Fromm & Holland 1989; Kempler 1995; Watson, Chenery & Carter 1999). The findings concerning the deficits in language and communication abilities are presented in Table 3 in conjunction with staging the severity of dementia in AD (see 2.3). A more detailed description on the semantic difficulties found in AD is given in 2.4. 2.3 Staging the severity of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease Short tests for clinical use have been developed to assess the cognitive and functional capacity and social skills and to stage the severity of dementia of patients. They do not replace a broad neuropsychological examination, but they give an estimation of the cognitive decline and severity of dementia, and they can be used to follow the progression of the disease (Alhainen & Rosenvall 2001; Hänninen & Pulliainen 2001). Such tests include the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS; Reisberg, Ferris, De Leon & Crook 1982), Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR; Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben & Martin 1982), and the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein & McHugh 1975; see also Erkinjuntti, Rinne, Alhainen & Soininen 2001:578-579). The MMSE is the method applied to define the severity of dementia in this study. The MMSE can be used to evaluate orientation, concentration, memory, language functions, and perception. It is a valid and reliable method to evaluate the cognitive decline of the elderly (Rantakrans 1996:22-24; Hänninen & Pulliainen 2001), but it is not very effective in detecting the very mild cognitive decline in early AD (Hänninen et al. 1999). The MMSE score correlates with the age and educational level. In studies conducted in Finland, it was found that the younger and more Alzheimer’s disease 13 educated tended to perform better in the test (Ylikoski et al. 1992; Rantakrans 1996:16-17), even with an apparent dysfunction of memory (Hänninen & Pulliainen 2001). Social class was also found to affect the MMSE score (Ylikoski et al. 1992; Rantakrans 1996:16-17, 25). The clinical findings typical of different stages of dementia in AD are gathered above in Table 2 in which the MMSE scores refer to the stage of dementia severity. In Table 3, in which the findings of language and communication deficits are presented, the staging of the disorders is based on several types of ratings found in the literature. Preclinical, very early, and mild stage of Alzheimer’s disease In the preclinical phase of AD, when the clinical diagnosis has not yet been confirmed, very slight deterioration in attention, episodic memory, verbal abstractions, and visuospatial construction can be identified fitting the criteria of Mild Cognitive Impairment (Ylikoski et al. 1999; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). The changes seem to correspond to the pathological neurofibrillary changes that take place in the transentorhinal and entorhinal cortex in the temporal lobe (Braak & Braak 1991, 1996; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). In the very early, clinically defined stage of AD, the cognitive impairment is still mild but more obvious, and the patient may be aware of his or her forgetfulness and difficulty in learning (Pulliainen & Kuikka 1998; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001; see Table 2). The most important symptom of early AD is memory dysfunction (Almkvist & Bäckman 1993; Petersen, Smith, Ivnik, Kokmen & Tangalos 1994; Locascio, Growdon & Corkin 1995; Soininen 1997). Above all, episodic memory is likely to be impaired and to involve limited capacity in both encoding and retrieval of information (Masur, Sliwinski, Lipton, Blau & Crystal 1994), which leads to an inability to learn new things, to remember recent events and experiences, and to a compromised performance in memory tests, such as story recall, learning pairs and series of words, word retrieval, and word fluency (Petersen et al. 1994; Locascio et al. 1995; Herlitz, Hill, Fratiglioni & Backman 1995; Kempler 1995; Pulliainen & Kuikka 1998; see 2.4, chap. 5). Planning and executing new and complex actions may be impaired, causing slowness and uncertainty in work and hobbies (Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). The cognitive decline seems to correspond to the limbic stages in which the entorhinal region is severely destroyed and neurofibrillary changes appear in the hippocampus and the adjoining limbic area (Braak & Braak 1991, 1996; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). In the mild stage of AD, a more severe decline in neurological and intellectual functions seems to take place (Almkvist & Bäckman1993; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). Sensory functions, especially auditory discrimination, olfactory functions, and tactile stimulation may be affected. Attention (focus, sustain, and attentional shift), psychomotor speed, and executive functions (planning, flexibility, and monitoring) seem to be impaired, especially when more complicated mental tasks are at 14 Alzheimer’s disease hand. Decline in the short-term memory functions for verbal and visuospatial information can be manifested during the mild stage of AD, but the semantic memory and the procedural memory tend to stay more preserved until the later stages of the disease (Almkvist & Bäckman 1993; Pulliainen & Kuikka 1998; cf. 2.4). Visuospatial (constructional) functions may also be impaired. Apraxia may hinder motor functioning (Travniczek-Marterer et al. 1993). Difficulty in concentration and orientation may appear, as well as psychiatric symptoms (e.g., depression, irritation; Pulliainen & Kuikka 1998; Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). In the mild stage of AD, functional abilities and social functions usually become restricted but the patient manages to live independently. Studies have also found that in the mild stage of AD, conversational skills are mainly preserved, but some patients may have difficulty with conversational initiation, storytelling, bringing up and maintaining a topic, and following the course of a conversation (Obler & Albert 1981; Orange, Lubinski & Higginbotham 1996; Ripich & Ziol 1998; see Table 3). Prosody, articulation, and rate of speech are relatively well preserved (Appell et al. 1982; Hier, Hagenlocker & Shindler 1985; Huff 1988; Orange & Ryan 2000; cf. Croot et al. 2000). Some patients may show reduction in their verbal abilities, especially in word retrieval (Obler & Albert 1981; Huff et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1989; Kempler 1995; Bucks, Singh, Cuerden & Wilcock 2000; see 2.4), despite their verbose output (Appell et al. 1982). Semantically empty words (e.g., ‘thing’, ‘stuff’, ‘do’, and deictic terms), circumlocutions, excessive use of pronouns, gestures, and semantic paraphasias are used to overcome the word-finding problems in order to maintain the fluency of the conversation (Appell et al. 1982; Obler & Albert 1981; Nicholas, Obler, Albert & Helm-Estabrooks 1985; Kempler 1995; Ripich & Ziol 1998; Bucks et al. 2000; Croot et al. 2000; Orange & Ryan 2000). Comprehension of concrete and simple, well structured language is preserved, but understanding abstract language, such as humor, analogies, sarcasm, and metaphors, or complex grammatical structures may be difficult (Emery 1988; Fromm & Holland 1989; Kontiola, Laaksonen, Sulkava & Erkinjuntti 1990; Kempler 1995; Grossman et al. 1996; Ripich & Ziol 1998; cf. Papagno 2001). Difficulty in reading comprehension and creative writing may appear, although mechanical writing and reading skills are relatively well preserved (Kempler 1995; see also Platel et al. 1993; Luzzatti et al. 2003). The changes in cognitive abilities during the mild stage of AD likely correspond to the isocortical (neocortical) stages at which the entorhinal, hippocampal, and limbic regions are severely damaged (Braak & Braak 1991, 1996; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). Excessive pathological changes may have taken place in the association areas of prefrontal and temporo-parietal cortices. Furthermore, the pathways connecting the different parts of the brain are likely to be affected, especially the cholinergic tract (Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001). Alzheimer’s disease 15 Moderate and severe stage of Alzheimer’s disease In the moderate stage of AD, short-term memory is poor and patients tend to lose things and repeatedly ask the same questions (Soininen 1997; Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001; see Table 2.). Memory aids do not facilitate coping with every-day functions. Orientation to time and places is poor. Agnosia, apraxia, and deterioration of executive functions become evident, followed by an inability to perform activities of daily living. In the moderate and severe stages of AD, most of the medial temporal lobe appears to be destroyed and the neocortical pathology increases in the prefrontal and parietal areas. The increased severity of the neuronal damage is followed by an increase in impairments in several cognitive functions, which reduces the patients’ ability to live independently. In the moderate stage, AD patients have severe difficulty in language production and comprehension (see Table 3.). The word-finding problems worsen, the vocabulary diminishes, and the amount of empty speech, circumlocutions, and semantic paraphasias replacing content words increases (Obler & Albert 1981; Bayles 1982; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Hier et al. 1985; Nicholas et al. 1985; Ripich & Ziol 1998). The patients’ ability to participate in a discourse is usually poor due to pragmatic deficits, inattention, word-finding difficulties, or memory deficits (Kempler 1995; Mentis et al. 1995; Orange et al. 1996; Ripich & Ziol 1998). Turn-taking, responding to questions, and using stereotypical, socially ritualized utterances (e.g.. greetings and leave takings) may occur successfully, but the content of speech may be vague and disordered. People with dementia may frequently repeat the same words or ideas, and they may easily forget the topic and talk about irrelevant topics (Bayles 1982; Nicholas et al. 1985; Ripich & Ziol 1998; Croot et al. 2000). Self-correction of speech errors or inappropriateness takes place less frequently. Their ability to follow conversation is poor. Withdrawal from social situations in which communication is demanded may occur (Bayles 1982; Mentis et al. 1995; Orange et al. 1996). The use of grammatically complex sentence structures seems to remain relatively preserved (Obler & Albert 1981; Hier et al. 1985). Instead, comprehension of more complex language is likely to be deteriorated (Emery 1988; Fromm & Holland 1989; Kontiola et al. 1990), and difficulty in grasping the meaning of common words may appear, which can show up as confusion in using words, phonemic and semantic paraphasias, and as a lack of ability to differentiate between semantically related words (Obler & Albert 1981; Huff 1988). Coherent and well-formed writing is compromised, as well as reading for comprehension (Huff 1988; Horner et al. 1988; Bayles et al. 1989; Kempler 1995), while mechanical writing and reading skills are preserved (Cummings, Houlihan & Hill 1986; Huff 1988; Rapcsak et al. 1989). In the severe stage of AD, the impairment of memory and language functions is grave and perception, orientation and praxis are very poor (Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001; see Table 2.). Behavioral problems, apathy, and restlessness are common symptoms. Neurological symptoms, such as apraxia and extrapyramidal disorders, are very common. The patients need help and guidance 16 Alzheimer’s disease in all basic functions in their everyday life. They are likely to develop other illnesses and infections. Most commonly, due to problems in swallowing, people with AD die of pneumonia (Juva, Valvanne & Voutilainen 2001). Conversational abilities in the severe stage are poor (Appell et al. 1982; Obler & Albert 1981; Obler 1983; Kempler 1995; Ripich & Ziol 1998; see Table 3.). The patient’s speech production may be incoherent, contain meaningless and absurd utterances and consist of only a few ideas and a few words, due to poor vocabulary. Phonemic and semantic paraphasias exist, but phonological errors seldom violate the phonotactic constraints of the language. Echolalia (repetition of others) and palilalia (repetition of self) may appear. The patient has difficulty in eye contact and turn-taking in a conversation. Language comprehension, especially comprehending grammatical structures, is very limited. In the very advanced stage of AD, language production and comprehension are very limited (Appell et al. 1982; Obler & Albert 1981; Bayles 1982; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Kempler 1995; Ripizh & Ziol 1998). Verbal expressions are very few and likely to be bizarre and uninterpretable, due to paraphasias and dysarthria. Some patients may be mute. Language comprehension is impaired in all modalities and may be limited only to very few, if any, concrete words, or the patient may understand something from the emotional content or the speaker’s gestures. 2.4 Semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease It is commonly thought that AD patients have a progressive semantic memory impairment characterized by an inability to distinguish among words belonging to the same semantic category, and a difficulty in producing names for them (e.g., Huff et al. 1986; Kertesz, Appell & Fisman 1986; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Hodges & Patterson 1995). There seems to be a wealth of findings both supporting and opposing the notion (for a review, see Nebes 1989, 1992). However, strong evidence of this decline has been obtained from AD patients’ performance on tasks tapping lexical semantic processing, particularly on noun and verb recognition and naming tasks, as well as on the semantic fluency tasks, in which different aspects of semantic knowledge are called into play (e.g., Martin & Fedio 1983; Bowles, Obler & Albert 1987; Hodges, Salmon & Butters 1992; Grossman, Mickanin, Onishi & Hughes 1996; see chap. 5). In word recognition tasks, the AD patients were found to be prone to select semantically related foils instead of correct targets (Huff et al. 1986; see also Martin & Fedio 1983; Diesfeldt 1989; Hodges & Patterson 1995). Some studies have provided information about the consistent and general pattern of semantic impairment present in AD by a detailed item-to-item analysis of various semantic tasks in and across different sensory modalities (Huff et al. 1986; Chertkow, Bub & Seidenberg 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1992; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Laine, Vuorinen & Rinne 1997; cf. Diesfeldt 1989). Alzheimer’s disease 17 2.4.1 Impaired knowledge of the meaning representation Patients with AD have been reported to have difficulty in retaining different kinds of semantic features (i.e., information concerning the superordinate category as well as physical and functional features) that make up the meaning of words (see 3.1.2, 3.2). The semantic features are thought to be essential for, for example, encoding of information, categorizing and differentiating closely related semantic items, such as members of the same semantic category. Therefore, impaired processing of semantic features may be responsible for many types of semantic dysfunctions, such as word finding difficulties and impaired maintenance of the associations between items in semantic memory. However, studies that have found normal semantic representation in AD have also been reported (e.g., Nebes et al. 1984, 1989; Nebes & Halligan 1996; Grober, Buschke, Kawas & Fuld 1985; Smith, Murdoch et al. 1989; Bayles, Tomoeda & Trosset 1990; Cronin-Golomb, Keane, Kokodis, Corkin & Growdon 1992; see 6.2). Many studies, in which different semantic tasks (e.g., card sorting, category membership judging) have been used to study the functioning of semantic memory, have indicated that AD patients are progressively impaired in their ability to use semantic information, the highest superordinate level information being more robust to degeneration than the basic level and subcategory level information (Warrington 1975; Schwartz et al. 1979; Martin & Fedio 1983; Martin et al. 1985; Huff et al. 1986; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Chertkow et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992; Hodges et al. 1992; Tippett, McAuliffe & Farah 1995; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; cf. Diesfeldt 1985; Smith et al. 1989; Bayles et al. 1990; Funnell 1995; Bell, Chenery & Ingram 2000; see 3.1.2). However, Laatu, Portin, Revonsuo, Tuisku, and Rinne (1997; see also Hodges, Patterson, Graham & Dawson 1996) found that, relative to the normal control subjects, their Finnish-speaking mildto-moderate AD patients were significantly impaired in comprehending the structural hierarchies of concrete words, even at the superordinate level. The impairment appeared when the AD patients were first asked to correct a hierarchy in which words were misplaced at different levels of abstraction and then to construct semantic hierarchies of written labels representing different hierarchical sub- and superordinate categories (e.g., food was divided to three lower level items (fruit, rootcrop, vegetable), each of which was further divided to two subordinate level items (‘lemon’ and ‘pear’, ‘carrot’ and ‘turnip’, ‘tomato’ and ‘cucumber’)). Supporting findings on impaired superordinate knowledge were presented by Grossman, D’Esposito et al. (1996) who concluded that AD patients were impaired in their superordinate membership judgments when acceptance or rejection between category members and foils was required. There is ample evidence to support the notion that the subordinate features of nouns are vulnerable to damage in AD. It has been indicated that AD patients tend to have difficulty in grasping semantic relations between a noun and its defining structural and functional features (Hodges et al. 1992, 1996; Chan et al. 1993; Laine, 18 Alzheimer’s disease Vuorinen et al. 1997; Laatu et al. 1997, Laatu 1999), as well as in determining the salience of features to a specific item (Grober et al. 1985; Abeysinghe, Bayles & Trosset 1990; Laatu et al. 1997). As a consequence, the boundaries of different semantic items may become obscure. For example, in the study of Hodges et al. (1992, 1996) and Laatu et al. (1997), words were defined by semantically related features belonging to a neighboring word from the same semantic category or by semantically unrelated features. Grober et al. (1985) suggested that a reduction in the weight of the semantic features contained by words may take place in AD, possibly leading to a change in the organization of semantic information (see 3.2, 10.1.3). Contrasting findings have also been obtained from noun-feature verification studies with the AD patients performing as accurately as the normal control subjects, leading the authors to presume a normal organization of the semantic memory in AD (Johnson, Hermann & Bonilla 1995; Smith, Faust, Beeman, Kennedy & Perry 1995). As far as verb processing is concerned, equivalent difficulties to noun processing have been presented by Grossman, Mickanin et al. (1996), who found that while the healthy control subjects were able to understand and make associative judgments about the semantic relations of verbs denoting motion, cognition, and perception, the AD patients evidenced misunderstanding and anomalous associations of the semantic relations between these verbs. Many studies using the method of semantic priming tasks provided findings supporting the notion of an intact semantic memory in AD. In these tasks, the time was measured for a subject to process a semantically related (e.g., ‘doctor’ - ‘nurse’), an unrelated (e.g., ‘pepper’ - ‘goat’) or a neutral (e.g., ‘blank’ - ‘baby’) stimulus word or picture of an object occurring prior to the target word or a picture of an object to be named or recognized. AD patients performed well on the priming tasks and showed shorter response times when the word to be named or recognized was preceded by a semantically related rather than a semantically unrelated or a neutral prime. AD patients showed a tendency to have a normal spread of activation along a normally structured semantic network, and a sensitivity to the semantic relations between words, as well as to the knowledge of semantic attributes making up the semantic representation of words (Nebes et al. 1984, 1989; Nebes & Halligan 1996; Albert & Milberg 1989). However, more recent studies have indicated that AD patients may express abnormal responses in the semantic priming tasks with both noun and verb stimuli, implying a semantic memory degradation in AD (Ober & Shenaut 1988; Chertkow et al. 1989; Knight 1996; Bushell & Martin 1997; Bell, Chenery & Ingram 2001). For example, Bushell and Martin’s study indicated that AD patients failed to show priming for semantically related verbs denoting different types of motions (e.g., ‘go’-‘come’, ‘tremble’-‘shake’). 2.4.2 Impaired naming AD patients tend to perform worse than normal control subjects when producing names for both objects and actions in confrontation naming tests such as the Boston Naming Test (BNT; Kaplan, Goodglass & Weintraub 1983) and the Action Naming Alzheimer’s disease 19 Test (ANT; Obler & Albert 1979 in Bowles et al. 1987). AD patients are also impaired in the semantic fluency task in which nouns are produced according to a category constraint in a certain period of time (see Table 4). There is a wealth of controversial results obtained from AD patients’ performance in object naming and semantic fluency with categories of nouns. Moreover, the number of reports on their performance in action naming is still very limited and published studies on the semantic fluency task with verb categories are lacking. Anomia, a disorder of producing or thinking of an appropriate word for an object or an action, is a prominent feature in AD (Kirshner, Webb & Kelly 1984; Kertesz et al. 1986; Bowles et al. 1987; Nebes 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Bayles & Tomoeda 1991; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; Cappa et al. 1998; Bucks et al. 2000). Difficulty in naming tend to appear at very early phases of the disease (Appell et al. 1982; Kirshner et al. 1984; Huff et al. 1986; Flicker, Ferris, Crook & Bartus 1987; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Goldstein et al. 1996; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; cf. Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Faber-Langendoen et al. 1988; Hodges et al. 1996) and severity of dementia tends to correlate with naming success (Bowles et al. 1987; Robinson, Grossman, White-Devine & D’Esposito 1996; WhiteDevine et al. 1996; Cappa et al. 1998; Williamson, Adair, Raymer & Heilman 1998; Kim & Thompson 2001). Naming ability may deteriorate rapidly in some AD patients, as indicated by follow-up studies on naming of nouns (Kertesz et al. 1986; Hodges et al. 1990; Beatty, Salmon, Testa, Hanisch & Tröster 2000). Some studies evidenced that semantic cueing appeared ineffective to prompt correct naming in AD patients (Obler & Albert 1981; Chertkow & Bub 1990). Normal healthy elderly tend to name high and low frequency words relatively equally, whereas AD patients’ accuracy of naming low frequency words is remarkably affected (Kirshner et al. 1984; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Miller Sommers & Pierce 1990; Goldstein, Green, Presley & Green 1992; Williamson et al. 1998; Kim & Thompson 2001). Other factors, such as familiarity of the to-be-named target, as well as its imageability and typicality, have also been shown to affect the naming performance both among normal elderly and people with AD, with the higher familiarity, imageability, and category typicality corresponding to more accurate naming (Gainotti, Di Betta & Silveri 1996; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Bird, Lambon Ralph, Patterson & Hodges 2000). The naming ability of the AD patients may also be affected by word length but not until later in the course of the disease (Kirshner et al. 1984). Furthermore, the semantic category, distinguishing between living and nonliving objects, is likely to have an impact on naming in AD. However, the findings are contradictory. According to some studies, the semantic information corresponding to living entities may be better preserved in AD than that of nonliving entities (Gainotti et al. 1996; see 3.1.2, 3.2), while some other studies hold the opposite view (Whatmough et al. 2003). The ability to name words according to their syntactic category may remain unaffected late in AD. This was demonstrated by AD patients’ relatively well preserved ability to name nouns and verbs with paradigmatic responses consisting of 20 Alzheimer’s disease semantically related responses without violating the grammatical class boundaries (Gewirth, Shindler & Hier 1984; see also Astell & Harley 1998). However, the findings concerning the difficulty of producing words for the grammatical classes of nouns and verbs seem to vary: some studies found the naming of nouns to be more difficult than that of verbs in the AD group (Cappa et al. 1998; Williamson et al. 1998; Fung et al. 2001), while some others have observed the opposite pattern (Robinson et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1995, 1996; Kim & Thompson 2001). The errors produced by normal control subjects and AD patients for both noun and verb naming tasks tend to consist of semantically related errors, omissions, and “don’t know” responses (Huff 1988; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Smith et al. 1989; Bayles et al. 1990; Goldstein et al.1992; Nicholas, Obler, Au & Albert 1996; White-Devine et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 1996; Williamson et al. 1998), as well as perceptual errors (Kirshner et al. 1984; Bowles et al. 1987; Goldstein et al. 1992; Williamson et al. 1998). AD patients tend to describe the contextual and functional features of objects and actions more often than control subjects (Obler & Albert 1981; Martin & Fedio 1983; Bayles et al. 1990; Miller Sommers & Pierce 1990; Robinson et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1996; Astell & Harley 1998; Williamson et al. 1998). As the disease advances, the error rate keeps increasing, and different types of erroneous responses occur, such as circumcolutions (tangentially related, nonspecific labels for the target), nonwords (anomalous combinations of morphemes), unrelated words (a response without semantic, phonological, or perceptual similarity to the target), utterances with empty syntax (i.e., noninformative responses), and less logical, even bizarre responses (Obler & Albert 1981; Bowles et al. 1987; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Smith et al. 1989; Bayles et al. 1990; WhiteDevine et al. 1996). Phonologically related errors may also emerge for both types of tasks (Williamson et al. 1998), but their proportion was found to be relatively low among AD patients (Robinson et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1996; cf. Croot et al. 2000). As far as the naming of nouns is concerned, the typical word errors are semantically related errors, such as superordinate category labels (e.g., ‘bird’ for ‘pelican’; Martin & Fedio 1983; Hodges, Salmon & Butters 1991; White-Devine et al. 1996; cf. Bayles et al. 1990; Astell & Harley 1998) and co-ordinates of the same semantic category (e.g., ‘goat’ for ‘camel’; Martin & Fedio 1983; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Miller Sommers & Pierce 1990; Hodges et al. 1991; Astell & Harley 1998). Errors specific to action naming are likely to involve very general responses (Bowles et al. 1987) and naming the parts of the objects in the picture without naming the action (Williamson et al. 1998). Nevertheless, it has also been observed that AD patients’ ability to produce verbs with a varying number of arguments tends to be preserved (Kim & Thompson 2001). All in all, there is a great but quite controversial body of research, some of which provide evidence of the existence of semantic impairment in AD while others argue for an intact semantic memory where only the access procedures are affected (see chap. 6). One reason for the discrepancy in these results may be the lack of Alzheimer’s disease 21 consistency among the methods applied. The semantic tasks tapping the functioning of the semantic memory seem to show a great variability across the studies. Even though the tasks may have been similar, the test material (e.g., words in the naming task) has differed. The type and level of detail in qualitative analyses varies between studies, which makes them less comparable to each other. Different, even opposing, findings of the integrity of semantic memory may also be due to some of the tasks requiring more effort in processing than others (see the discussion in Nebes 1989, 1992). Moreover, the methods used in selecting the participants for the studies and grading the severity of dementia of the AD patients vary, which adds up to the difficulty in comparing the studies with each other. However, it is generally believed that AD patients seem to have difficulty in retaining and using semantic information at different levels of abstraction, which is reflected in their impaired performance on several lexical-semantic tasks requiring a fine-grained differentiation between semantically related items. 22 Alzheimer’s disease 3 Frameworks of semantic knowledge Semantic knowledge, one of the basic components of the language faculty, has mainly been investigated in four disciplines: in psycholinguistics and cognitive psychology, where the research concerns the language of neurologically healthy persons, and in neurolinguistics and cognitive neuropsychology where the study deals with neurologically impaired individuals. The disciplines are interrelated. Psycholinguistic and cognitive psychological findings provide information by means of which semantic disturbances may be identified and analyzed. Neurolinguistic and cognitive neuropsychological evidence may, on the other hand, shed light upon the architecture or processing of the healthy system. The reason is that damaged systems may work more transparently and therefore possibly reveal aspects of the semantic knowledge structure and its processing in general (Persson 1995:16-17; Obler & Gjerlow 1999:112). Due to the great interest in finding out how the brain works, and the new technological devices available, the number of studies on lexical-semantic impairment in different types of brain damage has increased dramatically; the number of studies concerning the semantic fluency performance of AD patients alone can be counted in the dozens. Although the descriptions of the theoretical background of the semantic fluency studies are usually at a very general level, these studies seem to be primarily based on the notions of cognitive psychology and psycholinguistics, and more recently, to a greater extent, on the notions of cognitive neuropsychology and neurolinguistics. However, the major focus of these studies has been on memory and executive functions, leaving the neurolinguistic perspective less explored. Cognitive neuropsychology is an approach which “… seeks to explain the patterns of impaired and intact cognitive performance [such as thinking, reading, writing, speaking, recognising, and remembering] seen in brain-injured patients in terms of damage to one or more of the components of a theory or model of normal cognitive functioning and, conversely, to draw conclusions about normal, intact cognitive processes from the observed disorders.” (Ellis & Young 1988:23, brackets added; see also Shallice 1988:3-37; Coltheart 2001; Selnes 2001). Neurolinguistic research, on the other hand, attempts to shed light on the fundamental components of the human language system mainly by studying damaged language systems and to model understanding of the complex processes of language (Westbury 1998; Obler 24 Frameworks of semantic knowledge & Gjerlow 1999:1-2, 7-8, 12). Thus, the scope of study in cognitive neuropsychology is broader, whereas neurolinguistic research is concentrated on language functions. However, with regard to studying languages, these fields of study do not exclude one another. Rather, the approaches overlap to a great extent (e.g., Willmes 1998; Obler & Gjerlow 1999:2-3, 156-168). The overlap notwithstanding, the terminology concerning the semantic knowledge applied by these traditions tends to differ, which may appear confusing to a reader when interpreting what types of information are considered integrated in the semantic system of the words (see Nickels 2001). For example, the terms essential for the present study, semantic memory and mental lexicon (dictionary), are both used for the semantic entity central to processing words, but they originate from different scientific traditions, the former from cognitive (neuro)psychology and the latter from psycho- and neurolinguistics (see below). Another pair of terms that may cause confusion is object and noun, the former of which is used to denote a (concrete) entity in the real world and the latter of which is used to denote the linguistic unit, that is, the word that refers to that entity. Respectively, action is used to denote an action and an event that takes place in the real word, whereas the linguistic term verb is used to refer to that action or event. Semantic memory In cognitive (neuro)psychology, as well as in psycho- and neurolinguistics, the person’s internal, conceptual knowledge of the meanings of words is called the semantic representation (Ellis & Young 1988:114-115). A commonly accepted view is that part of the long-term memory system, the semantic memory, is responsible for the permanent storage of this meaning-related knowledge (e.g., Hodges 2000; Balota, Dolan & Duchek 2000; Tulving 2000; Schacter, Wagner & Buckner 2000). Tulving (1972) first defined semantic memory as a mental thesaurus which contained generalized and organized information about words and their meaning, as well as relations between the words, facts, and concepts, but which lacked information about the autobiographical episodes and events and their temporo-spatial relations which were considered part of the episodic memory. Later on, it was concluded that episodic knowledge and information derived from autobiographical events and experiences overlapped the contents of the semantic memory and that information originating from both memory systems contributed to the semantic representation of words (Tulving 1983; see the discussion in Sartori, Coltheart, Miozzo & Job 1994 and Graham, Lambon Ralph & Hodges 1997). In the Oxford Handbook of Memory, semantic memory was recently defined by Hodges (2000:442) as a “… permanent store of representational knowledge including facts, concepts, and words and their meaning—for example, knowing the meaning of the word panda, that Paris is the capital of France, and that the boiling point of water is 100° C, and so on”. Semantic memory is claimed to be necessary for the use of language because the knowledge and beliefs about the world that people acquire, possess, and use is critically dependent upon semantic processing (Schacter & Tulving 1994:28; Tulving 2000). Frameworks of semantic knowledge 25 Although the definition of semantic memory is widely agreed upon, there are several divergent theories as to the structure of semantic memory. The most influential theories are in favour of the hypothesis of multiple, modality-specific semantic systems (e.g., Warrington 1975; Warrington & Shallice 1984; Shallice 1988, esp. chap. 12; Powell & Davidoff 1995), or the hypothesis of a unitary semantic system (e.g., Riddoch, Humphreys, Coltheart & Funnell 1988; Caramazza, Hillis, Rapp & Romani 1990; Farah & McClelland 1991; Sheridan & Humphreys 1993; Klimesch 1994:171-172; Tyler, Moss, Durrant-Peatfield & Levy 2000; Tyler, Russell, Fadili & Moss 2001). Another issue worth mentioning is that the human memory system is not restricted to the semantic and the episodic memory system, but has been divided more specifically into various subsystems on the basis of human behavior and cognition (see e.g., Tulving 1983, 2000; Schacter and Tulving 1994; Schacter et al. 2000; Squire 1987:151-174; 1994:204; Knowlton 1997; Squire & Knowlton 2000). Because these themes are beyond the scope of this thesis, they are not dealt with in more detail. Mental lexicon The mental lexicon, its architecture and functioning in language production and comprehension, constitutes a pivotal area of research in psycho- and neurolinguistics (Schreuder & Flores D’Arcais 1989). The semantic layer of the mental lexicon is part of semantic memory and it contains information about the word’s meaning, that is, the set of conceptual conditions that must be selected by the word (e.g., the meaning of ‘eat’ is “to ingest for nourishment or pleasure”; see Levelt 1989:182) and encyclopaedic (real-world) knowledge (Lakoff 1987b:168-171, 182, 206; Schroeder & Flores D’Arcais 1989; Persson 1995:29-30). The mental lexicon also hosts the syntactic properties of word, containing information about the grammatical category and the syntactic arguments it can take, the word’s morphological specification, as well as the phonological composition, and the ortography of the word (Levelt 1989:182; Aitchison 1994:10-14; Hagoort 1998; Pulvermüller 1999). The role of the mental lexicon is to participate in language processing by mediating between the form and the meaning of the word (Levelt 1989:182; Hagoort 1998; see 4.1). This requires that the mental lexicon contains information about a large number of words (Hagoort 1998). The lexicon of an adult Finnish speaker consists of about 100, 000 words (Niemi & Laine 1994), which can be inflected in 15 semanticsyntactic cases (Hakulinen 1979:97). Karlsson and Koskenniemi (1985) calculated that a Finnish noun has about 2000 and a verb as many as 12 000 - 15 000 possible different forms. Adding the derivatives and compound words, one word stem may have an innumerable amount of lexical forms (Niemi & Laine 1994). Thus, the mental lexicon is both large and complex (Aitchison 1994:15). The classical view of linguistic semantics is not suited for the study of meaning as a cognitive-psychological entity, that is, as a subdomain of memory. The classical notion of linguistic semantics holds that the semantic representation of a word is describable by means of sets of binary contrastive features (e.g., ‘woman’ described 26 Frameworks of semantic knowledge as +HUMAN, -MALE, +ADULT; e.g., Lyons 1977:317-335). In such definitions, various kinds of information that humans gain about the referent from their realworld experience (e.g., typical but not logically necessary properties, for example that women typically are mothers) are not considered part of the semantic representation (see Lakoff 1987b:74-76). In contrast, the most widely used definitions of semantic memory and the semantic layer of the mental lexicon seem to overlap in their notion about the semantic representation of a word by considering the lexical meaning of the word, encyclopaedic information, and autobiographical events as part of the semantic representation. The encyclopaedic knowledge mainly concerns various kinds of factual information of the referents’ physical properties, what it is typically used for (functional features), its typical location (thematic features), etc. (see Persson 1995:29-30; see also 3.2, 3.3.3). Thus, the meaning of ‘panda’ may contain the following information: a panda is an animal, it belongs to the subspecies of bears, it has a black and white coat, its natural habitat is in China, it is mainly herbivorous and it likes to eat bamboo and leaves, I have seen a panda at the zoo, etc. In this study, this notion of the meaning of a word is applied. Subsequently, the terms ‘semantic memory’ and ‘the semantic layer of the mental lexicon’ are used to refer to the semantic system responsible for the meaning representations. The term ‘mental lexicon’ is used when referring to the system responsible for combining semantic, grammatical, and phonological information for word production (see chap. 4). 3.1 Principles of categorization The human mind seems to organize the phenomena of the real world in categories, such as persons, objects, events, actions, states, emotions, times, places, directions, and manners (Lakoff 1987b:6; Levelt 1989:74; Nelson 1996:111). The entities and classes of items in the real world are likely to be represented mentally in a semanticconceptual format and expressed linguistically as words (e.g., nouns and verbs). The human cognitive system, however, does not mirror the real world transparently, but identification and classification of the real-world entities and actions are based on the cognitive-perceptual system’s ability to selectively encode and store relevant and purposeful information and to ignore irrelevant information (Persson 1995:2829, 80-84; Heit 1997). The prerequisite for categorization is to recognize similarities and differences in sensory-functional features regarding concrete objects and actions between an input to be processed and the information in storage, which is fundamental to the human thought processes (Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Fivush 1987; Persson 1995:58, 99; Nelson 1996:223; Hahn & Chater 1997; Smith & Jonides 2000). The semantic-conceptual representation can be formed by virtue of visual and auditory perception and by virtue of touching, feeling, smelling, and tasting (Miller & JohnsonLaird 1976:583-618; Fellbaum 1998b:8; Goldstone & Barsalou 1998; Saffran & Sholl 1999; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Vinson & Vigliocco 2002; for a developmental view, see e.g., Nelson 1996; Gentner & Medina 1998), as well as performing and Frameworks of semantic knowledge 27 observing actions (Huttenlocher, Smiley & Charney 1983; Engelkamp, Zimmer & Denis 1989; Persson 1995:72, 59; Pulvermüller 1999). Vision is one of the most important sensory modalities in perception-based concept formation: it plays a central role in, for example, figure-ground distinction, detection of movement, and identifying shape, contour, size, texture, and colour of objects, as well as categorization and part-whole analysis of objects (Engelkamp 1975; Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Marshall, Pring, Chiat & Robson 1996; Persson 1995:72-77; Pulvermüller 1999; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Riddoch & Humphreys 2001). Moreover, the system also enables processing of the causal forces underlying similar properties between instances (Keil, Smith, Simons & Levin 1998) and the ways instances interact with each other (Tversky & Hemenway 1983, 1984; Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Fivush 1987; Markman 1987; Nelson 1996:232-248, 252). Consequently, categorization is based on prior knowledge structures which facilitate the selection of relevant features of the input and which are reused in the integration of new and old information (Persson 1995:61-62; Heit 1997; Kersten & Billman 1997). After having perceived the incoming information and having mapped it into the pre-existing knowledge structures, the system makes a decision about the category membership (Lamberts 1997). The process of similarity detection, which takes place in categorization, is thought to take place automatically and subconsciously by some researchers (Neisser 1987; Persson 1995:58-59; see also Nelson 1996:111), whereas others believe that a more controlled and conscious working of the cognitive system is needed (Smith, Patalano & Jonides 1998; Sloman & Rips 1998). Categorization enables human perception, reduces the amount of information to be processed to manageable proportions, and allows inferences and predictions about imperceptible or additional properties and future occurrences. Categorization also enables memory functions and thought processes, as well as makes communication labels more economical through the usage of general category labels (Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Lakoff 1987b:5-6; Heit 1997; Kersten & Billman 1997; Smith & Jonides 2000; cf. Small, Hart, Nguyen & Gordon 1995; Small 1997). Categorization seems to underlie any representational knowledge. Thus, information stored in the semantic memory by the means of categorization serves as the basis for semantic processing, including the performance on the semantic fluency task. The kind of semantic knowledge that is extracted by sensory and sensor-motor systems from the external world can be called semantic-perceptual features or exogenous features (i.e., physical, functional, and thematic information contained by basic-level nouns, subordinate nouns with concrete reference, proper names, and onomatopoeic items; Persson 1995:20, 70-90, Smolensky 1986; see also JohnsonLaird 1987; Bird, Howard et al. 2000). Semantic features that are not derived directly from perceptual or kinesthetic information, but have more language-based knowledge, can be called endogenous features because they make more use of system-internal linguistic than perceptual-semantic features (i.e., some adjectives and specific subclasses of verbs and superordinate nouns). Some of these features have a perceptual 28 Frameworks of semantic knowledge back-up and they are called perceptually inferred endogenous features meaning that their content is acquired from semantic-perceptual structures which are combined with linguistic features. Purely endogenous features are totally derived from languagebased information and they lack semantic-perceptual backup (e.g., abstract nouns, polysemous verbs, mental verbs, relational verbs, and adverbs). The nature of semantic representation of concrete nouns is dealt with in more detail in 3.2. Categorization of actions and the nature of semantic representation of verbs are discussed in more detail in 3.3. 3.1.1 Categorization of objects The classic theory of concept structure and category formation, dating from the times of Aristotle’s (384-322 BC) philosophical inspirations, considers categories as representing a summary of all of its members (e.g., all birds) rather than describing subsets or exemplars of the particular category. Different words and word classes can be formed and distinguished according to the logical combination of necessary and sufficient features that fit and apply to all the cases of the category. Every member of a category shares the necessary features with other members, and sufficient (defining) features allow the membership in a given category. Categories have a hierarchical structure, in which lower-level items inherit their defining features from the higher level. The boundaries of the categories are clear-cut and only two degrees of membership are permitted, member and non-member of a category (see the discussion in Labov 1973; Smith & Medin 1981:22-60; Medin & Smith 1984; Lakoff 1987b:12-57; Komatsu 1992; Klimesch 1994:75-79; Taylor 1995:21-37). The view described above, which is established mainly in theoretical semantics, has been refuted by experimental studies. It has been shown that the semantic features of words are likely to be of different importance (e.g., McClelland, Rumelhart & Hinton 1986; Persson 1995:80-84; see 3.2) and that the meaning of words (e.g., polysemous words) may vary due to the influence of their context (Barsalou 1982; Lakoff 1987b:74-76; Persson 1995:30, 87, 109-113; Smith & Samuelson 1997; Sloman & Rips 1998). Categories can also include atypical cases, for example an object that does not look or act like a bird (e.g., an object lacking the ability to fly or sing) can be still considered one (Rosch 1975; Smith & Samuelson 1997). Items in the common categories (e.g., fruit, furniture, and vehicles) may be cross-classified across different categories to serve a special goal or purpose (Barsalou 1982, 1983; Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Hampton 1998). For example, an apple may belong to fruit, things to take on a picnic, things that could fall on your head, and so on (Barsalou 1983). Further, the experimental studies have also indicated that the boundaries of categories are likely to be fuzzy rather than rigid and clear-cut. For example, it is difficult to determine the boundaries between a cup, a bowl, and a mug (Labov 1973). Furthermore, Barsalou (1982, 1983) showed that new semantic categories could be formed on the basis of representations established earlier (see below for goal-derived and ad hoc categories). Frameworks of semantic knowledge 29 Recent studies of cognitive neuroscience have provided support for a multiple-procedure view according to which different processes are used in concept formation and categorization (Smith & Medin 1981:170-175; Medin & Smith 1984; Small et al. 1995; Sloman & Rips 1998; Keil et al. 1998; Smith et al. 1998; Gentner & Medina 1998; see also Barsalou 1982, 1983, 1987; Schwartz & Reisberg 1991:366405). Various strategies of categorization may be applied to the same items at different stages of a child’s development (e.g., Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Nelson 1996:232236). Furthermore, multiple strategies may be used simultaneously but they may have different neural bases, and they seem to be qualitatively different from one another (Smith & Medin 1981:174-175; Hahn & Chater 1998; Smith et al. 1998; Koivisto & Laine 1999; Smith and Jonides 2000). Some of the strategies may involve analytical and controlled processing, as well as strategic and selective attending to and weighting of the features of the object, whereas others require more holistic and automatic processing with less load on the working memory (Smith et al. 1998; Sloman & Rips 1998). The different processes involve classification strategies such as classification by rule, prototype, or exemplar. Classification by rule holds that categories are represented by a set of pre-existing mental decision rules that an object has to satisfy in order to be classified as a member of a given category (e.g., Heit 1997; Lamberts 1997; Hahn & Chater 1998; Smith et al. 1998; see also Keil et al. 1998). For example, the rule for the category of cars may include that the object possesses the following features: it has four wheels and an engine, and it can be driven on the road. Classification by prototype requires retrieval of prototypes (the most typical members or members with the most inter-correlated features of a category, e.g., a robin) or clusters of inter-correlated features of various categories (e.g., ‘has-feathers’, ‘haswings’, ‘is-able-to-fly’) from the long-term memory, systematic comparison of features, and selection of that category whose prototype is most similar to the test object. Items that are similar to the prototype tend to be classified most readily as a member of a category, while items that are further from the prototype are less likely to be included as a category member (Rosch 1975, 1978; Rosch and Mervis 1975; Smith & Medin 1981:61-101; Smith et al. 1998; see also Storms & De Boeck 1997). On the other hand, classification by exemplar is a view holding that categories are represented by their exemplars rather than by a rule or an abstract summary of features (Smith & Medin 1981:143; Storms & De Boeck 1997). This procedure includes retrieval of prestored knowledge as a set of various subsets (e.g., a robin and a sparrow) or specific instances (the pet canary “Tweety”) that are similar to the test object, comparison between the object and the exemplars, and selection of that category whose retrieved exemplars are most similar to the object (Smith & Medin 1981:143161; Smith et al. 1998; Smith & Jonides 2000; Heit 1997; Lamberts 1997; Storms & De Boeck 1997). According to the connectionist view, the categorical organization in the semantic memory is understood as an emergent property based on processing the sensory-functional and associative feature information about objects (Klimesch 30 Frameworks of semantic knowledge 1994:144-145, 153; Small et al. 1995; Small 1997; Persson 1995:63; Gonnerman, Andersen, Devlin, Kempler & Seidenberg 1997; Garrard & Hodges 1999). Categorization can be described as detecting the saliency of and similarity between incoming and existing semantic information. The processing of features causes activation to spread through various connections in the network of features and finally to settle on the target pattern of features (see 3.2). The more familiar the combination of features of the word, the higher frequency of occurrence of the word, or the greater the overlap among the features to be matched, the faster the word can be categorized. Unfamiliar words are identified and classified with less strength and slower integration of features. Flexible and dynamic combination of these features allowed by the cognitive system may bring about a change in the previously existing organization of semantic information, an appearance of a whole new category, and even a disappearance of some entities of knowledge (Hinton, McClelland & Rumelhart 1986; McClelland, Rumelhart et al. 1986; Persson 1995:60-61, 64-67; Small 1997). Flexibility in feature combination also lies beneath the use of different words in terms of crossing category boundaries (e.g., a knife is both a weapon and a kitchen utensil), as well as differences found in semantic representations among different people and the individual change of classification principles over time (Small et al. 1995; see also Barsalou 1983, 1987). A somewhat different approach to knowledge representation and categorization focuses not only on how objects are perceived, but also on how they are functionally, spatially, and temporally related to each other in activities and routines (Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Fivush 1987; Nelson 1996:232-248; see also Schank & Abelson 1977; see 3.3.4). Infants, as well as adults, often seem to group things around themselves on the basis of a common function of objects which tend to occur in the same position or “slot” in a given routine and structure of daily events (e.g., food to eat at lunch), and which can be substituted by other functionally similar items (e.g., spaghetti, salad). These “slot filler categories” (e.g., food to eat at breakfast, food to eat at lunch) seem to be typical of the organization of early semantic memory and they may serve as a basis for the development of hierarchical taxonomic categories (e.g., food and clothes), into which these event based subcategories can be combined later during the child’s development at the age of 7 or 8 (Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Nelson 1996:232-236; cf. Keil et al. 1998). The associations between the items in the slot-filler categories can be explained by substitutability rather than by similarity among the items (Nelson 1996:235). Categorization can also be based on contiguity or a thematic relation in which objects involved temporally and spatially in the same events and routines can be related to each other (e.g., spaghetti and plate are part of the lunch event; Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Nelson 1996:234). Additional evidence concerning the principle of non-taxonomic categorization was produced by Barsalou (1982, 1983; see also Smith & Samuelson 1997) who observed that people could construct new semantic categories in order to achieve different goals. The goal-derived categories may include such categories as things Frameworks of semantic knowledge 31 to eat on a diet and things to take on a camping trip. The ad hoc categories, on the other hand, tend to consist of such categories as birthday presents and things that can be used to hold a door open, and so on. These categories are composed of crossclassified items. They appear to be less familiar, less central to cultural knowledge, and less established in the semantic memory than the common taxonomic categories, due to not having as strong a correlational structure of semantic features among the items (see 3.2). Nevertheless, once constructed and frequently used, goal-derived and ad hoc categories may be well established as a common semantic representation, which enables a rapid activation of information. Barsalou (1983, 1987) also noted that, as well as common categories, goal-derived and ad hoc categories tended to have a graded structure with more and less typical items which, however, seemed to vary considerably in different contexts and cultures (see 3.1.2). Barsalou noticed that different people seemed to classify objects differently, and that people tended to change their classification over time. Relationships between different manners of categorization may be dynamic, complementary, and constructive, resulting in interacting planes of knowledge organization (Nelson 1996:235). Evidence to support this notion has been brought forward, for example, by Keil et al. (1998) who indicated that even young children at the age of five were able to use typicality and similarity-based judgements, as well as explanatory procedures based on rules and causal principles, in order to classify animals and machines. Furthermore, amnesic patients were observed to perform poorly on tasks that required retrieval of exemplars from the long-term memory store that in their case had been impaired. However, at the same time they performed better on tasks requiring the use of prototype and rule-based categorization which did not seem to stress long-term memory (Smith et al. 1998 and the references therein). Further, the study of Koivisto and Laine (1999) indicated that in normal conditions, the right hemisphere seemed to process lexical-semantic knowledge in the prototype manner when previously learned words were encoded and the left hemisphere processed lexical-semantic information analytically by features and was called in to play when new and less known words were encoded (cf. Medin & Smith 1984). Contrary to the classical view in linguistic semantics, according to which a concept or a word can be defined by necessary and sufficient features, supporters of the psycho- and neurolinguistic, as well as the cognitive psychological and cognitive neuropsychological, accounts assume that a set of necessary and sufficient features is not able to define the members of a category or the category boundaries (Aitchison 1994:43-45, Taylor 1995:40-41; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:22) or to mirror the psychological reality (Rosch 1975; Rosch & Mervis 1975). Rather, it is emphasized that there is no fixed, basic meaning for all words or clear boundaries between all categories, but they have fuzzy edges so that new entities and experiences can be easily associated with a category (Labov 1973; Rosch 1975, 1978; McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; Aitchison 1994:39-41; Taylor 1995:53; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:16-20, 29, 38; Hampton 1998; cf. Persson 1995:85 for words with 32 Frameworks of semantic knowledge fixed core-meanings). Categorization is also influenced by contextual and cultural factors (Barsalou 1983; Aitchison 1994:92; Taylor 1995:40-41; Lakoff 1987b:7476; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:49-52; Hampton 1998), as a consequence of which the semantic representations are likely to differ between individuals and between different cultures. 3.1.2 Subcategories and hierarchical structure of nouns Nouns, generally speaking, can be divided into common (e.g., ‘furniture’) and proper nouns (e.g., ‘Mount Everest’) according to their semantic contents and referential purposes (Levelt 1989:196-197; Karlsson 1998:191). Common nouns specify several semantic-conceptual properties that need to be activated in order for a word to be identified (see 3.2). Proper nouns mainly merely point to a specific referent in the world. Common nouns can be further subdivided into count nouns (e.g., ‘dog’) and mass nouns (e.g., ‘water’). Count nouns usually refer to entities that may be countable, whereas mass nouns refer to substances. In some languages, such as German and Spanish, nouns can also be divided according to grammatical gender (Crystal 1987:93). Dixon (1991:76-77) divided nouns into nouns with a concrete reference (e.g., ‘horse’), an abstract reference (e.g., ‘time’), nouns denoting states and properties (e.g., ‘joy’), activities (some basic nouns e.g., ‘war’, and nouns derived from verbs, e.g., ‘decision’), and speech acts (‘question’). In this thesis, the main focus concerning nouns is on nouns with a concrete reference. The semantic knowledge concerning nouns that refer to concrete objects can be organized into two broad domains, living and non-living items, which are also referred to as biological, animate, or natural categories vs. artifacts, inanimate, or man-made categories. These domains can further be divided into distinct semantic categories (e.g., Keil 1989:25-57; Karlsson 1998:191; Tyler et al. 2000). However, there are varying notions of how the semantic-conceptual knowledge contained by the semantic memory can correspond to the different semantic domains and categories of concrete objects (see 3.1.1, 3.2). The domain-specific knowledge hypothesis assumes that evolutionarily, it is important for a human’s survival that specialized innate neural mechanisms for recognizing and understanding such categories as animals, plant life (fruit and vegetables), conspecifics, and perhaps artifacts (e.g. tools), are developed and represented in specific regions of the brain (Caramazza & Shelton 1998; Caramazza 2000; Shelton & Caramazza 2001; Santos & Caramazza 2002; for a critical view, see Tyler & Moss 2001; Devlin et al. 2002). However, most of the recent semantic theories hold the view that the division into living and nonliving categories is based on the shared properties of the items, and further distinguished as different semantic categories (e.g., birds and fruit vs. vehicles and tools) due to a unique set of clustered semantic features, which are not shared by other categories of the domain (Farah & McClelland 1991; Gonnerman et al. 1997, Small 1997; Moss et al. 2002; Garrard, Lambon Ralph, Hodges & Patterson 2001; McRae & Cree 2002; see also Warrington & Shallice 1984; Warrington & McCarthy 1983, Frameworks of semantic knowledge 33 1987). Semantic features and their constellations in different semantic categories are discussed in more detail subsequently in 3.2. Categories of both living and non-living items form hierarchies of different levels of specificity which are tightly interconnected in the semantic memory and which can be relied on in order to be able to generalize the semantic knowledge productively (Persson 1995:92-94). Categories can be vertically divided into the superordinate (e.g., animals, furniture), basic (dogs, chairs), and subordinate level (e.g., Dalmatians, kitchen chairs), according to their level of abstraction (Smith, Shoben & Rips 1974; Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson & Boyes-Bream 1976; Tversky & Hemenway 1984). Rosch et al. strongly claimed that the internal structure of the categories is horizontally considered graded, meaning that categories tend to have more central or more prototypical and more marginal members (e.g., a robin vs. an ostrich; see 3.1.1). The better or more typical of a category a member is, the more features it has in common with other members of the category and the faster it can be identified as a member of a particular category. Conversely, the marginal, “poor” members share only few attributes with other members of the category (Rosch 1975, 1978; Rosch & Mervis 1975; see also chap. 1 in Ungerer & Schmid 1996; cf. Barsalou 1982, 1983; Lakoff 1987a, b). Categories at the superordinate level are fairly large, very distinctive from each other (e.g., animals vs. furniture), and they contain general information that can be applied to the whole category. Nouns denoting categories at the superordinate level (e.g., ‘furniture’, ‘vehicle’) have a rather sparse semantic feature-structure compared to the nouns at the basic and subordinate levels (Smith et al. 1974; Rosch et al. 1976; Tversky & Hemenway 1984, Persson 1995:93). The semantic features of the superordinates tend to be abstract and functional, lacking a direct visual or motor base. Nevertheless, some superordinate nouns seem to embody perceptual information regarding the referents’ shape structure (e.g., many animals have four legs; Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Schreuder & Flores D’Arcais 1989; Persson 1995:93). Therefore, some superordinates denoting biological categories may function as basic-level terms (e.g., ‘bird’) and do not form a clear three-level hierarchy (Rosch et al. 1976; Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Persson 1995:93). Furthermore, nouns such as ‘thing’, and ‘stuff’, form the uppermost level of the noun hierarchy, called the super-superordinate level (Persson 1995:94). These super-superordinates may denote almost any kind of concrete and abstract referents, which often become specified in a particular context. While the superordinate nouns (e.g., ‘tool’) contain very generalized information, summarized from different categories at the basic-level, the semantic structure of the super-superordinate nouns is very sparse, schematic, and derived from the superordinates. In other words, the structure of the superordinate nouns embodies perceptually inferred endogenous features, whereas the structure of the super-superordinate nouns is purely endogenous, that is, lacking a perceptual back-up (Persson 1995:71, 94). 34 Frameworks of semantic knowledge The basic level nouns (e.g., ‘dog’, ‘cat’, ‘car’, ‘bus’, ‘chair’, ‘sofa’) denote cognitively the most important categories and they constitute the cornerstone of different taxonomies. Categories at this level are very familiar, informative, and distinctive from each other. For example, if one knows that the object in question is a dog, one can better predict more about its appearance (e.g., it has four legs, it has fur) and behavior (e.g., it barks and eats meat) than if one only knows that it is a living thing. At the basic level, categories tend to be mutually exclusive, owing to the breaks in their correlational structure of features in the environment (e.g., one knows that dogs are different from the other categories at the same level, such as cats, horses, cows, and other mammals; see 3.2). The basic level nouns are perceptually transparent, that is, their semantic structure closely overlaps the perceptual characteristics of the referents. The structure of the subordinate nouns does not tend to differ significantly from the basic level nouns in their general physical (e.g., shape, parts) or functional features. However, subordinates can provide more information for a more accurate disambiguation because they often contain many specific details (e.g., sensory-perceptual features) in addition to attributes that overlap with other subordinates (e.g. ‘Dalmatian’, ‘German shepherd’ and other breeds of dogs; Rosch & Mervis 1975; Rosch et al. 1976; Rosch 1978; Smith et al. 1974; Tversky & Hemenway 1983, 1984, Persson 1995:84-85, 92-95; see also Ungerer & Schmid 1996: chap. 2 and Murphy & Lassaline 1997). Thus, the structure of the basic-level and subordinate nouns is exogenous, that is, perceptually encoded (Persson 1995:70-71). 3.2 Feature-based models as accounts of the semantic representation of nouns In the philosophical tradition called associationism, originating in the 17th century, it was thought that the human knowledge of the world was built by accumulated sensory information and ideas, which became linked on the basis of temporal contiguity and repetition. More precisely, when two or more experienced entities occurred repeatedly, associative links were established between them and a new idea was compounded. In this way, the knowledge structure was believed to form a huge associative network. The notion that memory can be represented as a vast associative network in which episodes and facts are connected to each other has been very influential in cognitive psychology. It has had a strong impact also on the modern theories of memory (e.g., connectionism) in which knowledge is represented as a dynamic, effective, and interconnected network (Schwartz & Reisberg 1991:5-7; 406-427; see also Pulvermüller 1999). Another notion about semantic organization, shared by many theories and supported by experimental evidence, is that knowledge is represented in different hierarchical structures, as discussed previously (e.g., Collins & Quillian 1969; Warrington 1975; Warrington & McCarthy 1983; Klimesch 1994:71, 81; Persson 1995:92-94). Frameworks of semantic knowledge 35 Theories of semantic representation are divided into two broader groups according to the notion they hold about the format of the semantic knowledge. According to the atomistic view, words are regarded as indivisible conceptual wholes (i.e., without an internal structure) that form an associative network between each other (Collins & Loftus 1975; Watkins & Gardiner 1979; Roelofs 1992; Levelt 1999a, 2001; Levelt, Schriefers et al. 1999; see discussion in Persson 1995:57, 101). Alternatively, the semantic representation of the words can be defined by decomposing the meaning into smaller units (e.g., Smith et al. 1974; Miller & Johnson-Laird 1976; Allport 1985; Stemberger 1985; Dell 1986; Dell & O’Seaghda 1992; Persson 1995:57-61). These latter theories, commonly called decompositional or feature-based theories, can be divided further into two distinct approaches by the way the information is stored in the semantic memory. According to the hypothesis of multiple copies, words are supposed to consist of a rather fixed set of wordspecific semantic features, which are stored one-to-one for each lexical item. In the distributed accounts, rather than being stored over and over again for all relevant words, the semantic features common to many different words are shared by the words in question (see the discussion in Allport 1985; Stemberger 1985:136-145; Hinton, McClelland et al. 1986; Persson 1995:57, 118-119; Pulvermüller 1999; Tyler et al. 2000). Thus, for example the feature ‘has-a-mane’ is represented only once in the semantic memory and it is shared by, for example, a lion and a horse (Garrard & Hodges 1999). Nouns form the largest group of words in the Finnish lexicon (Saukkonen, Haipus, Niemikorpi & Sulkala 1979:8-21; Karlsson 1998:132). The early productive vocabulary of children seems to consist mainly of concrete nouns, whereas verbs and other word classes appear later in their active use of words (Guasti 2002:8081, Reyna 1987; cf. Tomasello 1992:9-10). The advantage of concrete nouns over verbs in a child’s early vocabulary may lie in the way in which the meaning of some concrete nouns can be fixed by relying on a word-to-world mapping procedure, whereby the word is transparently mapped onto the sensory and motor features of an object to which it refers (Persson 1995:29, 84; Guasti 2002:81). A concrete noun tends to have a conceptually and semantically independent structure, meaning that its existence at a given moment does not depend on any other object or on its participation in an interaction (e.g., nouns such as ‘knife’ and ‘tool’ can be understood independently of other structures; Schank 1972; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Langacker 1991:14; Persson 1995:96). Verbs, on the other hand, have a dependent conceptualsemantic structure, meaning that they are closely related to other words (see 3.3). The classical view has been adopted in many experimental feature models, according to which the semantic structure of a word denoting a concrete object can be explained by decomposing its meaning into smaller units. These elements have been called by different terms, such as properties (Collins & Quillian 1969), semantic features (Smith et al. 1974; Smith & Medin 1981), and attributes (Rosch 1975; for a review on feature-based models, see Chang 1986 and Komatsu 1992). In the connectionist models, the semantic representation is not a static or fixed cluster of 36 Frameworks of semantic knowledge features but rather a dynamic feature conglomerate. It is believed that the item’s final constellation is tuned, and thus determined, by the verbal content of the item. In this sense, the semantic representation is not believed to consist of semantic features as such but of a pattern of activation across semantic microfeatures (McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; see also Persson 1995:57, Tyler et al. 2000). The microfeatures do not necessarily have a linguistic correspondence but are subsymbolic and unconscious elements, which may act connected to each other flexibly and at various levels of the semantic processing system (e.g., Rumelhart, Hinton & McClelland 1986; Persson 1995:57-61; Schyns & Rodet 1997). Though the feature-approach is empirically supported, critical views have been raised (e.g., Levelt 1989:201, 1999a; Roelofs 1992; Aitchison 1994:39-50; Harley 2001:284-288). In this study, terms such as property, semantic feature, and attribute that refer to the decomposition of the word meaning are used interchangeably, and no distinction is made among them. Semantic features Semantic representation is believed to consist of physical, functional, and thematic information. Some researchers seem to share the view that different kinds of semantic information are housed in topographically distinct regions in the brain and that there is a relationship between the manners in which the information is acquired and the format in which it is stored (e.g., Warrington 1975; Warrington & McCarthy 1983, 1987; Warrington & Shallice 1984; Allport 1985; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller, Mohr & Schleichert 1999; Pulvermüller, Härle & Hummel 2001; Saffran & Scholl 1999). Some other researchers stress that semantic information is likely to be represented in an undifferentiated cortical network (Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; Tyler et al. 2001). Although there are plenty of theories and they seem to stress different aspects of semantic representation, they need not be mutually exclusive. Perceptually encoded physical features are thought to be essential components of meaning carrying information of the shape (form and part-structure), texture, size, colour, taste, smell, etc. of the objects (Persson 1995:73-76; Tyler et al. 2000). Semantic-perceptual processing can be mediated by several sensory modalities (e.g., vision, sound, smell, taste, touch), by sensations of subject-internal states (e.g., pain and cold), and by sensorimotor (kinesthetic) percepts (Miller & JohnsonLaird 1976:583-618; Persson 1995:72; Goldstone & Barsalou 1998; Saffran & Sholl 1999; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Vinson & Vigliocco 2002; see 3.1). The role of the functional features, which concerns the manner and the rules (cause and effect) by which objects move or interact with the environment or how humans move their bodies when manipulating objects, is considered important in semantic representation (Marshall, Chiat, Robson & Pring 1996; Persson 1995:7779; Bird, Howard et al. 2000, 2001; Tyler et al. 2000; see also Labov 1973; Rosch et al. 1976, and Tversky & Hemenway 1984). For example, the semantic representation of a telephone is not only about what it looks and feels like, but about the Frameworks of semantic knowledge 37 sensory-perceptual features being associated with the specific functional information about what a telephone does and is used for, and what significance it has in the environment (Allport 1985; Tyler et al. 2000). Functional features can be encoded by imagining or observing actions performed by others, or through the motor modality involving self-enactment or simulated self-action (Persson 1995:77; see also Engelkamp et al. 1989). As far as the basic-level objects are concerned (e.g., a chair), the functional features seem to refer to actions that are regularly and typically (canonically) associated with the object (a chair is for sitting rather than painting or buying; Persson 1995:77). Thematic features (also called contextual or associative features), such as spatial locations and causal and interactional relationships between the objects in a scene (who does what to whom, etc.), as well as cultural experiences, may be attached to the representation of words (Barsalou 1982, 1983, 1987; Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Nelson 1996:234). The perceptual and motor systems, as well as emotional impressions experienced in connection with objects and words, are involved in mediating thematic information (Persson 1995:79). Many objects share thematic contexts, that is, thematic features often fit many different words (e.g., books and pencils can be found on shelves or in stores and offices). The information of referents not gained through direct perceptual or motor experiences, such as facts of a referent learned from books (e.g., lions live in Africa and they are often called “The king of the jungle”), is also part of the semantic representation (Tyler et al. 2000). Such information is called the encyclopaedic knowledge. Shared and distinctive features In the more recent connectionist semantic theories, it is thought that the semantic features making up the semantic representation of a word involve distributed or shared features and distinctive features. Shared features tend to be features common to many semantically related words (cf., the multiple copy hypothesis), whereas distinctive features may be activated only for one or a few members in a category thus enabling differentiation among co-ordinates (Persson 1995:118-124; Gonnerman et al. 1997; McRae, de Sa & Seidenberg 1997; Devlin, Gonnerman, Andersen & Seidenberg 1998; Tyler & Moss 2001; cf. Rips, Shoben & Smith 1973; Smith et al. 1974). Most lexical items, such as nouns, seem to form semantic categories of various kinds of similarity in meaning (see 3.1.2). On a distributed account, semantically similar items share a varying number of features (the greater the similarity, the higher the number of shared features), and their semantic representations thus have overlapping zones for shared features. As a consequence of overlap, a shared feature, when activated, spreads activation not only to the semantic representation of the target, but also to representations having features in common with the target. Because successful selection requires that activation settle on one representation, the semantically related items need to be deactivated, and the target-specific, distinctive features must be activated in addition to the shared features (Persson 38 Frameworks of semantic knowledge 1995:118-124). For example, lions and tigers share such features as ‘has-4-legs’, ‘has-fur’, and ‘has-a-tail’, but only tigers have the feature ‘has-stripes’, by which they can be differentiated from lions (Gonnerman et al. 1997). The features that co-occur in a systematic fashion can predict another (e.g., things that have legs typically also have ears and eyes and are able to move) and support each other with mutual activation. These features are called correlated features (e.g., Rosch et al. 1976; Rosch 1978; Keil 1989:154-155; Caramazza et al. 1990; Klimesch 1994:135; Persson 1995:118-124; Vandenberghe, Price, Wise, Josephs & Frackowiak 1996; Gonnerman et al. 1997; McRae et al. 1997; Keil et al. 1998; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 2001; Bird, Howard et al. 2000, 2001; Caramazza 2000; Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; see also Smith & Medin 1981). The shared and distinctive features include physical and functional features, as well as thematic information. The relative importance of the features may vary. Some features (i.e., physical and functional features) are likely to be more discriminative of their semantic structures than other features (i.e., thematic features) thus helping the disambiguation and selection of items take place. This is called the principle of weighting (Farah & McClelland 1991; Persson 1995:72-84; Gonnerman 1997; Bird, Howard et al. 2000, 2001; Tyler & Moss 2001; see also Rosch et al. 1976). Thematic features being shared by many items may not be very discriminative and, therefore, they are likely to be more lightly weighted than sensory-perceptual and functional features, having, therefore, less impact on the selection of items (Persson 1995:80-82). It is not yet clear if thematic features form correlated patterns with other semantic features (Tyler et al. 2000). The degree of correlation of features tends to be higher in the living than in the non-living categories (Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002; Garrard et al. 2001). Living entities (most typically, animals) share many perceptual features that frequently co-occur and thus are strongly correlated. Their distinctive features, which distinguish one category member from another, tend to be weakly correlated with other semantic features (e.g., tiger: ‘has-stripes’, lion: ‘has-mane’). Unlike artifacts, the distinctive features of the living things are not usually closely related to a specific type of function with the environment (cf. Persson 1995:81 for the biological categories that are strongly associated with functions: donkeys for transporting, horses for riding). Rather, the shared perceptual features closely interact with their biological functions (e.g., ‘has-wings’&‘flies’; ‘has-eyes’&‘sees’; ‘haslegs’&‘walks’, etc.) that are typical of the category and shared by other category members (Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002). Artifacts tend to have fewer shared but more distinctive properties than the members of the living domains. There seems to be a strong relationship between the distinctive physical (perceptual) form and the specific function of the object (e.g., ‘has-blade’&‘cuts’). Artifacts are mutually discriminative, that is, their semantic representation tends to have a unique function that is associated with distinctive perceptual features. For example, a knife, a spoon, and a fork all have a different shape and a different function. The features denoting form and function in artifacts tend to form more strongly Frameworks of semantic knowledge 39 connected clusters than those in the category of living entities (Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Persson 1995:80-81; Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002; see also Labov 1973; cf., Garrard et al. 2001). Basing on the varying patterns of feature conglomerations and the degree of feature correlation, the semantic representations for the categories and the category members seem to have different internal structures (Tyler et al. 2000; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002; cf. Pulvermüller et al. 2001). For example, animals tend to have many shared, correlated features with relatively few distinctive features, whereas fruit and vegetables, although they are semantically closely related, tend to have fewer shared and fewer and poorly correlated distinctive features. Respectively, vehicles, which are all used for transportation, seem to have a higher ratio of shared features but less distinctive features than, for example, tools. Tools seemingly have more distinctive functional than shared properties for the purpose of their usage (e.g., for cutting: ‘has-a-sharp-edge’; for raking: ‘has-long-tines’). Strongly correlated sets of features tend to be heavily weighted and thus they are likely to be more resistant to damage than features with weaker correlation. Consequently, distinctive features that are weakly correlated for living things tend to be more vulnerable to damage than the highly correlated shared features that are protected by mutual correlations. Therefore, as a result of damage to the vulnerable distinctive features of living things, naming pictures and matching words to pictures may cause problems, whereas sorting items into semantic categories is preserved due to shared semantic features. The man-made objects seem to be more robust against damage than the living entities because their feature correlation is formed between distinctive perceptual feature denoting form and functional features (Tyler & Moss 2001; Tyler et al. 2000; Devlin et al. 2002; Moss et al. 2002; see 9.2.6, 10.1.3). 3.3 Accounts of semantic representation of verbs Although the proportion of verbs in the lexicon seems to be smaller than that of nouns, at least in Finnish (Saukkonen, Haipus, Niemikorpi & Sulkala 1979:9-11) and English (Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Aitchison 1994:110), verbs probably are the most important, as well as perhaps the most complex lexical category of a language. The importance of verbs lies, for example, in their pivotal position in clauses because they seem to motivate the other clausal members and thus specify the clausal structure (Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Pulman 1983:107; Engelkamp 1975, 1986; Reyna 1987; Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Aitchison 1994:111; Persson 1995:96-97; Wayland, Berndt & Sandson 1996; see also Pajunen 1999:14-15). Verbs are thus in close interaction with many other lexical categories, such as nouns (see 3.2), prepositions (e.g., ‘to’), and adverbs (e.g., ‘out’; Persson 1995:111). In Finnish, which is an agglutinative language, verbs are likely to influence the inflectional forms in which the other, nominal members of the sentence occur (e.g., Pajunen 1999:16). For example, the Finnish words in the sentence “The girl’s books fell off the table onto the floor” are inflected in the following way: 40 Frameworks of semantic knowledge Tytön girl-GEN kirjat book-PL putosivat pöydältä lattialle. fell-PST-PL table-ABL floor-ALL There is also evidence to indicate that at least some verbs tend to be closely connected to other verbs. For example, Garrett (1992, see also Huttenlocher & Lui 1979) showed that normal speakers often substituted target verbs by verbs with an opposite meaning (e.g., ‘start’ - ‘stop’), co-ordinate verbs from the same semantic domain (e.g., ‘drink’ – ‘eat’, ‘watch’ – ‘listen’) that selected the same or semantically related arguments, or by functionally close verbs displaying an entailment of the actions (e.g., ‘answer’ - ‘dial’, ‘eat’ - ‘cook’). These patterns have also been seen in slips for other lexical categories, e.g., nouns (‘table’ for ‘chair’), adjectives (‘hot’ for ‘cold’), and prepositions (‘bottom’ for ‘top’), which demonstrate that lexical items tend to be interconnected (Aitchison 1994:16-27, 85-88; Persson 1995:38, 123). Furthermore, information on aspect and tense, which relate situations to time, is also closely associated to the semantic representation of verbs (see e.g., Persson 1995:104-107; see also Frawley 1992: chap. 7 and 8; Saeed 1997:114-124, and Cruse 2000:274-279). In conclusion, the data seem to demonstrate that lexical items tend to be strongly interconnected (see Aitchison 1994:15). Semantic roles Verbs are conceptually-semantically dependent structures. For example, the verb ‘walk’ implies the presence of an entity performing the movement, the verb ‘give’, on the other hand, involves by necessity three components – a giving part, a transferred entity, and a receiving part. These entities, part of the semantic structure of verbs, are described in terms of arguments and semantic roles, also called thematic roles (Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Reyna 1987; Levelt 1989:90-94; Pinker 1989:165246; Aitchison 1994:117-121; Persson 1995:96-104; Collina, Marangolo & Tabossi 2001; for a theoretical view, see Frawley 1992:197-249; Saeed 1997:139-155; Pajunen 1999:23-37; Cruse 2000:281-284). When a verb is activated, its semantic roles are activated as well, and a frame for a whole clause is given (Shapiro, Zurif & Grimshaw 1987; Persson 1995:96-97; Wayland et al. 1997). Although some verbs may refer to concrete objects, and action can be encoded in visual and sensory-motor form (Engelkamp 1975; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Helstrup 1989; Pulvermüller 1999), semantic roles of verbs are pieces of knowledge that represent gradually achieved and highly generalized information extracted from, for example, motion, manner of movement, spatial configuration, causation, and the different relations in which entities (e.g., noun-referent) act or are involved when partaking in an event (Persson 1995:96-98; see 3.3.1). Semantic roles (e.g., Agent, Patient, Mover, Source, Goal, Instrument, Theme, etc.) are a relatively restricted set, which can be used and suited for several verbs (see McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; Levelt 1989:90; see also Frawley 1002:197). Concerning their semantic role structure, some verbs tend to be more complex and abstract than others (e.g., relational verbs Frameworks of semantic knowledge 41 vs. verbs of involvement, Persson 1995:99; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996; Thompson, Lange, Schneider & Shapiro 1997; Kemmerer & Tranel 2000; Collina et al. 2001; see 3.3.2). Generally speaking, semantic roles can be filled with a variety of lexical items (e.g., ‘I’/’you’/’David’/’Helen’/’the child’/’the dog’ jumped). The specific meaning of many verbs tends to impose semantic restrictions on the items in role positions. In the example given above, only living entities of certain types have the ability to jump, meaning that the semantic role function as the subject to the verb is filled with an item representing a living creature with this particular ability. The role position of some other verbs is closely connected to a restricted set of nouns (see the discussion in Persson 1995:96-104; Bastiaanse 1991; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996; Wayland et al. 1996; see 3.3.2). Semantic roles are purely semantic entities that are sometimes not even formally but implicitly realized (Persson 1995:99). For example, in a sentence “He ate”, the semantic role of Patient (specifying a passive participant who experiences the effects of an action) is part of the verb’s semantic structure, as well as ‘food’ representing the role of Theme (specifying the entity being eaten), which is implicitly represented. Specific verbs, such as ‘hammer’ and ‘sweep’ implicitly encode the semantic role Instrument (labelling a tool or an instrument used by an agent in an event; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Persson 1995:99; cf. Behrend 1990; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996; Kemmerer & Tranel 2000). 3.3.1 Categorization of actions So far little is known about the principles and procedures of how actions are categorized, relative to the procedures of classifying objects (see 3.1.1). However, categorization of actions is likely to occur on the basis of semantic roles, which form the contents of verbs (Persson 1995:99; Kersten & Billman 1997). Kersten and Billman presented experimental findings on animated events supporting assumptions that the principles of the correlational structure of semantic feature representation underlying object categorization and category learning may also apply to categorization of events (see 3.3.3). Their study showed that finding rich and systematic correlation among semantic features (e.g., agent path, state change, manner of motion, and environment) caused better learning than when different features varied independently of one another (e.g., path and manner of motion were considered independent organizing principles; cf. Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Persson 1995:101). A rich correlational structure can be seen in the semantic representation of specific verbs implying the use of an instrument, such as ‘hammer’ and ‘saw’, which specify not only the use of a particular instrument but also particular actions and results. For example, the verb ‘saw’ implies not only the use of a saw, but also a particular physical motion associated to sawing, as well as the result of the affected object being cut (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Behrend 1990; Persson 1995:100; Kersten & Billman 1997). According to Persson (1995:101), these verbs seem to combine more than one semantic role. 42 Frameworks of semantic knowledge Evidence to support the notion that semantic roles underlie category learning and classification can be found from the studies of developmental psychology: the most basic and developmentally earliest acquired form of activity is motion (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; cf. Behrend 1990), which involves movement of the whole body (e.g., ‘dance’) and movements of different parts of the body, such as hands and legs (e.g., ‘applaud’, ‘kick’; Persson 1995:100; see also Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al., 2000, 2001). Encoding motion verbs tends to have a strong perceptual basis by means of vision and sensory-motor experiences (Miller & Johnson-Laird 1976:527; Engelkamp 1975, 1986; Engelkamp et al. 1989; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Persson 1995:77). Patterns of movement are acquired through repetition and practice (Engelkamp 1986). Subsequently, after children’s ability to note similarities in movements between themselves and others has developed, they are able to understand causality first on the basis of their own sensory-motor self-experience (by 20 months of age) and only later (at about 26 months) in other people’s movements and change of state (Huttenlocher et al. 1983). Thus, fully mature categorization of action requires not only information derived from an individual’s own sensory-motor experiences, but also the knowledge to understand that there exists a parallel in the behavior and intentions (causes) between oneself and others carrying out different types of action, as well as knowledge of goal-directed motives underlying actions (e.g., a child realizes there is a similar motivation and patterns of movement between his or her and another person’s actions when going to a sink to wash up; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Persson 1995:102). Consequently, the semantic role of Mover specifying physical movement is likely to be among the first to organize information, followed later by Agent that specifies causality of actions. The role of Instrument specifying the instrument used in action causing a result or a change of state may also have a crucial part in encoding and classifying information relatively early on in a child’s development (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Behrend 1990). In the process of participatory interaction and while observing others taking actions, children learn to interpret actions taking place in the socially and culturally arranged world around them in terms of causal and spatial-temporal relations between participating elements (e.g., actors, actions and objects) and relations between certain actions leading to certain goals (Fivush & Slackman 1986; Nelson 1986; Slackman, Hudson & Fivush 1986; Nelson 1996:5-8, 93-97). Daily routines (e.g., taking a bath, going to bed, having lunch, etc.) and different types of episodes and events (e.g., going to a birthday party or a restaurant) gradually develop into a generalized event representation. In these representations, the structure of an event, as well as objects and persons usually participating in the particular event, are translated into a structure of a common event category (also called a schema or a script) containing shared information and structural similarities (Fivush & Slackman 1986; Slackman et al. 1986; Nelson 1996:96, 111, 154-155; see Schank & Abelson 1977; for a more detailed description of scripts, see 3.3.4). Children at the age of three may have well-developed event representations for familiar routine events (Fivush & Slackman Frameworks of semantic knowledge 43 1986; Nelson & Gruendel 1986; Nelson 1996:17). The event representations change with age and experience from simple to more complex and decontextualized scripts, which may reflect a growing awareness of event categories and provide the basis for more abstract temporal, causal, and taxonomic relationships (Slackman et al. 1986). The generalized knowledge contained by a script may serve as a dynamic, creative, and effective memory system in identification, encoding, and categorizing incoming information, as well as in learning new things and organizing information in the memory (Schank 1982:23-25, 80-90, 167-171; Rumelhart, Smolensky, McClelland & Hinton 1986; Smolensky 1986; Persson 1995:62; Funnell 2001). Thus, taken that the semantic roles underlie categorization of actions, highly generalized scripts imply participation of a conglomerate of semantic roles in conjunction with each other (Persson 1995:61-62, 101-102), and a very rich and systematic correlations between them (Kersten & Billman 1997). 3.3.2 Subcategories and hierarchical structure of verbs In psycholinguistically oriented approaches, the foundation of the verb distinction is based on how the human semantic-perceptual system is able to extract information and encode the functional features contained in verbs from interaction with the world (see 3.1). For example, the motor-functional information embodied in different types of verbs denoting, for example, various kinds of actions (e.g., ‘cut’, ‘walk’) tends to originate in visual encoding (imaginary or observed actions) and sensorymotor experience (self-enactment; Engelkamp 1975, 1986; Engelkamp et al. 1989; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Persson 1995:77; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 2001; Cacciari & Levorato 2000; Vinson & Vigliocco 2002). However, all actionrelated associations do not involve the motor modality (Pulvermüller 1999). For example, verbs denoting processes (e.g., ‘trickle’, ‘splinter’) seem to lack encoding via motor activity (Engelkamp 1986; see Pajunen 1999:51-53), and verbs denoting relationships between the participants of an event encode information that has no direct link to perception, but is very abstract in nature (Williams & Canter 1987; Persson 1995:101-102; Marshall, Chiat et al. 1996). In this thesis, most of the discussion concerns concrete actions. Concrete action verbs can be defined as referring to intentional behaviors in which an initiator carries out physical movements or motor programs alone or in relation to other persons or objects (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Engelkamp 1986; cf. division of verbs into verbs of involvement and true relation by Persson 1995:96104). Action may be motivated by causality, for example, by causing change or result (e.g., ‘open’), by making contact (e.g., ‘touch’, ‘hug’, ‘pet’), or by moving in a particular way (e.g., ‘dance’, ‘jump’; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Persson 1995:99100; cf. Behrend 1990). Actions, carried out as various physical movements, may have a sequential and temporal structure (e.g., opening implies certain arm movements, such as lifting, pulling or pushing, as well as a certain amount of force, and eating involves biting, chewing, and swallowing; Miller & Johnson-Laird 1976:549; 44 Frameworks of semantic knowledge Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Engelkamp 1986; Persson 1995:100; Pulvermüller et al. 2001). The outcomes of movements can be realized, for example, as a change of state, an initiator’s movement, the initiator’s contact with an object, the object’s movement, a change in location, etc. (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; see also Kemmerer & Tranel 2000). Recent neurophysiological studies by Pulvermüller et al. (2000, 2001) have provided evidence that actions carried out by different parts of the body, such as legs/feet, arms/hands, and face/mouth, activate different regions in the motor cortex and at different rate implying that a psychologically plausible distinction can thus be made between the semantic representation of verbs (e.g., ‘kick’, ‘lift’, and ‘smile’; see also Warrington 1975; Warrington & McCarthy 1983, 1987; Warrington & Shallice 1984; Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Allport 1985; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 1999). Persson (1995:96-104, 196-197) presented a notion that a psychologically plausible subdivision of verbs could be made between verbs symbolizing involvement of some entity in an event (e.g., ‘applaud’ or ‘jump’) and verbs symbolizing relational structures (e.g., one loves somebody, or one gives something to somebody; see also Saffran, Schwartz & Marin 1980; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996, 1998; Bastiaanse & Jonkers 1998; Thompson et al. 1997; Kemmerer & Tranel 2000). Verbs of involvement tend to be concrete verbs containing one semantic role, Mover, that acts out the particular movement implied by the functional features of actions (e.g., clapping of hands when applauding or moving of legs when walking; Persson 1995:77-79, 96-104). While being associated with concrete verbs denoting physical involvement (cf. Agent that brings about a change), which requires movements of the whole body or parts of the body, as well as visual perception, the semantic information conveyed by Mover is likely to contain perceptually inferred semantic features which are not directly derived from perception but built upon it, thus having a perceptual back-up (Persson 1995:102; see also Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 2001). These verbs are also called actor-inherent verbs to emphasize the role of the entity in action (Saffran et al. 1980; see also Huttenlocher et al. 1983). Verbs containing a true relation, on the other hand, presuppose a semantic structure of two or several roles. The meaning of such verbs (e.g., ‘chase’) contains a synthesis of different roles implied by a particular verb. Thus, the meaning of relational verbs cannot be inferred from the perceptual nature of a single entity in the relation, but from the roles participating in conjunction to make up the semantic content of the verb (Persson 1995:101-102). The division between verbs of physical involvement and relational verbs was suggested as a plausible division also by the study of Saffran et al. (1980) who observed that agrammatic aphasic subjects expressed particular difficulty in accessing relational rather than actor-inherent verbs. However, instead of showing any strength towards generability at the group level, the study of Kemmerer and Tranel (2000; see also Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996, 1998; Thompson et al. 1997; Kim & Thompson 2000) indicated that the ability of some brain-damaged subjects to retrieve action verbs with one vs. multiple roles varied individually. Frameworks of semantic knowledge 45 Another psychologically relevant distinction between verbs can be based on specific verbs (e.g., ‘adopt’, ‘sweep’) and generic verbs (e.g., ‘make’, ‘put’; Engelkamp 1975; Persson 1995:102-104; Kim & Thompson 2001; see also Breedin, Saffran & Schwartz 1998 for “heavy” and “light” verbs). The meanings of some specific verbs seem to consist of strong perceptual components, due to their close associations with specific concrete nouns. Some of these verbs are closely related to basic-level nouns (e.g., ‘sweep’ – ‘broom’), as a consequence of which their semantic representations contain semantic-perceptual features. Some other verbs are closely related to more abstract superordinate nouns (e.g., ‘eat’ – ‘food’), the reason why their semantic representations include perceptually inferred features (Engelkamp 1975; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Behrend 1990; Persson 1995:85,101-102; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996; Kerstin & Billman 1997). A noun can be considered an aspect of the meaning of a particular verb. This is indicated, for example, when a noun response is given to a verb in a free association task, and that noun is a prototypic example of one of the arguments of that verb (e.g., ‘knife’ in response to ‘cut’; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979). The semantic-perceptual and perceptually inferred features of the nouns may function as a route to specify the verb (Persson 1995:102-103, 208-209; cf. Engelkamp 1975; Davidoff & Masterson 1996). One subset of verbs with a close association to certain nouns can be called instrument verbs because they designate the tool or the instrument being used, as well as the action being performed or the result being accomplished by the action (e.g., ‘sweep’, ‘hammer’, ‘mop’; Behrend 1990; Bastiaanse 1991; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996, 1998; Kemmerer & Tranel 2000; see 3.3.1). The basic meaning of generic verbs (also called general purpose verbs), such as ‘put’, ‘go’, ‘give’, ‘make’, ‘come’, is schematic in a sense that they denote a certain relational structure of roles between entities participating in an action (e.g., ‘come’) as physical movement between two poles which manifests the semantic role structure of Mover - Source (specifying the point of origin) - Goal (specifying the point of arrival). Unlike specific verbs, generic verbs are very frequent, polysemous verbs (i.e., they have many meanings), and they can easily be modified and shaded by the semantic content of items they occur with, such as fillers of the semantic roles (Reyna 1987; Persson 1995:103; Berndt, Haendiges, Mitchum & Sandson 1997; Breedin et al. 1998). Since they are flexible in meaning, generic verbs have fewer contextual constraints than specific verbs and they can thus be used in a broad range of situations, and they may allow for several interpretations. Generic verbs contain purely endogenous information that lacks a direct connection to perception (Persson 1995:102-103). These verbs also tend to be low in imageability (Bird, Lambon Ralph et al. 2000). Empirical evidence has shown the relevancy of the distinction between specific and generic verbs, for example, in studies in which agrammatic speakers were found to produce specific (heavy) verbs easier than general (light) verbs by Persson (1995:209), Breedin et al. (1998) and Kim and Thompson (2001; cf. Berndt et al. 1997; Bastiaanse & Jonkers 1998; Kemmerer & Tranel 2000). Evidence supporting the division was also obtained from patients with AD 46 Frameworks of semantic knowledge by Kim and Thompson (2001) who found that AD patients produced general verbs better than specific verbs (see 9.4, 9.4.5, 10.1.3). As with nouns, a graded structure (i.e., some verbs are considered better or more typical examples of verb categories than others) can also be found at least among some verbs. For example, to ‘murder’ can be considered as a better example of ‘killing’ than ‘execute’, and ‘stride’ a better example of ‘walking’ than ‘limp’ (Pulman 1983:110-133; see also Coleman & Kay 1981; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:100, 191). In contrast to nouns, however, verbs do not seem to form a strict and neat hierarchical structure with many levels of abstraction. Instead, most verbs have a shallow, bushy organization (Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Pajunen 1998, 2001:60-61). All superordinate nodes are not branched into more specific variations and there are no well-defined levels of structure. In such a bushy structure, there typically are only a couple of hierarchy levels and the verbs of a particular level are grouped in several small, parallel groups. In very flat structures, such as the structure of cognition verbs (‘anticipate’, ‘think’, ‘intend’, ‘suppose’, and ‘know’), verbs do not form a specific hierarchy but are grouped together as co-hyponyms (e.g., ‘think’, ‘consider’, ‘ponder’ and ‘suppose’, ‘suspect’, ‘imagine’, ‘hope’; Pajunen 2001:60-63). The shallower the hierarchical structure, the weaker the representation of the verb class (Pajunen 1999:20). The members of the same level of abstraction do not seem to form a very firm network and there are few semantically similar verbs that can be used as variations expressing certain actions or events (Pajunen 1998). On the other hand, not all verbs can be grouped under a single top verb in a semantic field. Motion verbs, for example, tend to have two top nodes (‘move’, ‘make a movement’) and (‘move’, ‘travel’, ‘displace’) and the verbs of possession can have three verbs as top nodes (‘give’, ‘transfer’), (‘take’, ‘receive’), and (‘have’, ‘hold’; Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:71-72, 81; see also Pulman 1983:107). Some of the verbs, however, can form taxonomies that show a specific level at which there are more verbs than at other levels, and where most of the verbs cluster and have richer semantic relations to their superordinate verb than between verbs on other levels in the same hierarchy (Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:80-81; cf. 3.1.2). For example, the taxonomy for speech act verbs may stem from the highest level ‘communicate’ to the next lower level that contains relatively few verbs, such as ‘talk’ and ‘write’. The verb ‘talk’, however, seems to have many hyponyms, such as ‘babble’, ‘mumble’, ‘slur’, ‘murmur’, and ‘chatter’. The level below the most richly lexicalized one has few verbs, which tend to be compounded from their superordinate and a noun (such as ‘telecommunicate’). When descending the verb hierarchy, semantic elaboration of different semantic components (e.g., manner, cause, speed, intensity, purpose, and volition) and sometimes nouns denoting, for example, instruments or materials, is needed (Pulman 1983:111-112; Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:80). The variety of nouns that the verbs at a given level can take as possible arguments diminishes, implying a function of the increasing elaboration and meaning specificity of the verbs (Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:80-81; Pajunen 2001:35; see also Pulman 1983:107-136; Ungerer Frameworks of semantic knowledge 47 & Schmid 1996:102-103). For example, the semantic role of the verbs ‘communicate’ and ‘talk’ corresponding to the subject can be filled with either nouns referring to figures, pictures, or humans, whereas the corresponding role of the verbs ‘fib’ or ‘perjure themselves’ is restricted to nouns denoting only human beings (Fellbaum 1998a:81). Thus, upper parts of the hierarchy tend to have more general meaning, lower parts more specific meaning. In Finnish, verbs at the lowest level tend to be very descriptive in nature (e.g., ‘hölkyttää’ / ‘jog’; Pajunen 1998, 2001:52-54; see also Ungerer & Schmid 1996:101-104). Miller and Fellbaum (1991; Fellbaum 1998a:79) merged the different kinds of semantic dimensions that distinguished a verb hyponym from its superordinate into a manner relation, called troponymy. Troponymy represents a particular kind of entailment, where verb pairs are temporally coextensive and are related by manner relation (Fellbaum 1998a:80). Thus, to ‘amble’ is to walk in a particular manner, to ‘hammer’ is to hit in a particular manner, and to ‘tape’ is to fasten in a particular manner. Troponymy is the most frequently found relationship among verbs and a very productive process for coining new words in a language (Miller & Fellbaum 1991). 3.3.3 Feature-based models as accounts of the semantic representation of verbs The account that word meanings can be decomposed into different types of semantic units can also be applied to the semantic representation of verbs. Some theories maintain that there are certain basic actions (e.g., go, get) that can act as the core of other verbs (e.g., ‘run’, ‘grab’; Breedin et al. 1998; see also Schank 1972). Some other accounts share the view that the meaning of verbs denoting actions is composed of different semantic features (e.g., Miller & Johnson-Laird 1976: chap. 7, esp. p. 525; Allport 1985; McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; Persson 1995:77-78, 85, 96-104; Marshall, Chiat et al. 1996; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 2001). Bird, Howard et al. concluded from their simulation studies that the semantic representation of animate nouns, inanimate nouns, and action verbs tended to form a continuum from more sensory (visual) and less functional (motor) features to less sensory and more functional features in such a way that there is a heavier weighting of sensory features for animate nouns designating living entities, less sensory but more functional for inanimate nouns designating artifacts, and more functional weighting for verbs designating concrete actions (see also Marshall, Chiat et al. 1996; Marshall, Pring, Chiat & Robson 1996; Bird, Howard et al. 2001; cf. Shapiro & Caramazza 2001a, b). A somewhat similar distinction was made by Pulvermüller (1999; see also Pulvermüller et al. 2001; Vinson & Vigliocco 2002) who divided different words into “action” and “vision” words, according to their type of featural composition and their topographical representation in the brain rather than on the basis of their lexical categories (e.g., verbs referring to body movements were assigned as action words, concrete nouns such as animal names were assigned as vision words, and names referring to tools were assigned as both). 48 Frameworks of semantic knowledge Connectionist accounts share the view that functional information, symbolizing patterns of movement, is part of the semantic representation of concrete verbs denoting physical involvement (e.g., ‘squeeze’, ‘jump’), which can be derived perceptually from multimodal stimuli (see 3.1, 3.2). However, in these accounts, it is emphasized that visual and sensory-motor information are correlated. Subsequently, combinations of (somato)sensory-motor information can be established to represent these actions in the brain (Persson 1995:77-78; Pulvermüller 1999; Pulvermüller et al. 2001). According to Persson (1995:79), functional features, however, do not stand for the whole semantic representation of verbs denoting concrete action alone, but as integrated with the semantic roles that specify the entities partaking in an action, they form the structure of the semantic representation of these verbs. For example, the functional features of concrete actions (e.g., the pattern of movements of legs associated with running or the particular movements of one’s arm when sawing) and the role of someone performing the action (Mover or Agent) are integrated in the verbs ‘run’ and ‘saw’ (Persson 1995:99-100). In connectionist modelling, such as the Parallel Distributed Lexical Processing (PDLP; see Persson 1995), the semantic roles are considered decomposed and flexible structures, or features, that may be shared (distributed), overlapped, and integrated with each other (see also McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; Persson 1995:101; Pulman 1983:109; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Behrend 1990; Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:71-72, 81; Kersten & Billman 1997; Vinson & Vigliocco 2002; 3.2, 3.3.1). This notion is not in agreement with the established view, according to which semantic roles are semantic primitives, which lack internal structure, and which are mutually exclusive, meaning that only one semantic role occupies one verb at a certain time (see the discussion in McClelland & Kawamoto 1986 and Persson 1995:101; cf. Frawley 1992: chap. 5 and Saeed 1997: chap. 6). Instead, it is emphasized that the human memory system allows for a flexible integration of features and that different routes can be utilized in processing a semantic entity. For example, specific verbs (e.g., ‘sweep’) can be activated by strongly established connections to certain basic-level or superordinate nouns (e.g. ‘broom’ - ‘sweep’; McClelland & Kawamoto 1986; Persson 1995:34, 78-79, 103; see 3.3.2). Accordingly, semantic roles such as Agent (specifying an active participant and an instigator of an action) and Patient (specifying a passive participant who experiences the effects of an action) can be simultaneously represented (McClelland & Kawamoto 1986). For example, in the sentence “The boy moved” the boy is both the instigator and the participant of the action. Respectively, Agent and Mover (specifying an entity carrying out any type of movement) can be simultaneously activated but differently weighted in different verbs (Persson 1995:77, 100-101). For example, in the concrete causative verb ‘cut’, the role of the Mover is considered less weighted than the role of the Agent, whereas in the concrete, noncausative action verb ‘walk’, the role of the Mover is highly emphasized. In relational verbs, semantic roles work in conjunction with each other making up the content of the verbs (see 3.3.2). Frameworks of semantic knowledge 49 3.3.4 Scripts as the account of semantic representation of verbs Script theories seem to provide a broad semantic framework for explaining the semantic representation of objects and actions, the complex relationship between the participants, and the spatial, temporal, and socio-cultural circumstances presented in an event (Schank & Abelson 1977:36-68; Schank 1982; Nelson 1986; Fivush 1987; Grafman et al. 1991; Nelson 1996; see 3.1.1). Although there may yet be little neuropsychological evidence to support the existence of scripts as knowledge representation (Funnell 2001; cf. Weingartner, Grafman, Boutelle, Kaye & Martin 1983; Grafman et al. 1991), plausible findings from cognitive and developmental psychology supporting their existence can be found (e.g., Bower, Black & Turner 1979; Galambos 1983; Fivush 1987; Nelson 1996). A script (also called a schema, a scene, a frame, and an activity) is a high-level, usually highly generalized and thus very abstract, knowledge structure of a rather typical or stereotyped episode (e.g., going to the doctor, washing a car, or eating at a restaurant), which does not correspond to, or have a basis in, any single episode as such but is formed gradually of pieces of knowledge comprising the most prototypical or regular parts of recurring episodes (Schank & Abelson 1977:41; Graesser 1978; Bower et al. 1979; Galambos 1983; Nelson 1986; Persson 1995:62; Funnell 2001). A person may learn about scripts by personal experience and by reading about them, by seeing them done, or by being told about them by others (Schank & Abelson 1977:36-46; Bower et al. 1979; Schank 1982:3-12, 23-24; Galambos 1983; Fivush & Slackman 1986; Fivush 1987). Thus, scripts contain both episodic (autobiographic) and semantic information (Fivush & Slackman 1986; Hudson 1986; Nelson 1996:174-177; cf. Tulving 1972, 1983, 2000; Schacter & Tulving 1994). Even very young children may have general, spatiallytemporally organized knowledge about routine and familiar events, such as eating and going to bed (Fivush 1987; Nelson 1996:232-240; see also 3.1.2). The internal structure of scripts appears to contain vertical and horizontal connections between their components, similar to category representations (for a comparison between categories and scripts, see Barsalou & Sewell 1985). The vertical connections are hierarchical part-whole relationships between higher level components and lower level components; for example, the restaurant script may involve a component such as ‘ordering-food’ which serves as a superordinate to a subordinate component ‘customer-picks-up-a-menu’. The horizontal connections between the parts of a script can be defined by structural characteristics, such as sequential and temporal order between actions, causality, spatial relations, centrality, distinctiveness, and frequency of performing an action as a part of a script (Schank & Abelson 1977:30-35, 38, 42-46; Graesser 1978; Bower et al. 1979; Galambos 1983; Barsalou & Sewell 1985; Fivush & Slackman 1986; Rifkin 1984 (in Lucariello & Rifkin 1986); Nelson & Gruendel 1986; Slackman et al. 1986; Grafman et al. 1991; Nakamura, Kleiber & Kim 1992; Nelson 1996:232-232). It has been indicated that temporal properties and causal relationships in a script specify the order in which 50 Frameworks of semantic knowledge certain actions take place; which actions occur early in the sequence, which happen later (e.g., ordering a meal is temporally and causally related to getting and eating it; Galambos 1983, Barsalou & Sewell 1985; Nakamura et al. 1992). In scripts, actions seem also to be organized by centrality, meaning that some actions seem to be more important to the performance of a script than others (e.g., it is more essential for a script going on vacation to ‘decide-on-a-place’ than ‘stopthe-newspaper’; Galambos 1983; Nelson & Gruendel 1986; see also Barsalou & Sewell 1985; Grafman et al. 1991; cf. 3.1.2). The centrality of an action may have an influence on how easily it is activated for use and how easily a script can be comprehended (Galambos 1983). In scripts, the relationships between adjacent actions should cause the encoding of one to prime the other. In addition, the prime should spread beyond its adjacent actions to further actions, although the amount of priming should decrease with distance (Barsalou & Sewell 1985; for schemata and sequential thought processes in connectionist models, see e.g., Rumelhart, Smolensky et al. 1986). The structural characteristics of a script can be defined by how distinctive an action is for the script (Galambos 1983; Grafman et al. 1991; cf. 3.2). Some highly distinctive actions tend to occur in only one particular script (e.g., ‘get-a-camera’ is specific for the script of taking a photograph), whereas some other actions may be part of a number of different scripts (e.g., ‘standing-in-a-line’ is part of various scripts, such as shopping for groceries and going to the movies). Thus, these actions are low in distinctiveness and do not seem to distinguish among various scripts. Actions of a script can also be specified by how frequently they are included when a script is performed. Some actions in a script are almost always performed (‘get-a-towel’ in the script of washing your hair), while some others may be less often included in a script (e.g., ‘get-a-tripod’ in taking a photograph). 4 Spoken word production In normal speech, about two to five words per second are retrieved from the mental lexicon that contains tens of thousands items (Levelt 1989:199; Levelt et al. 1999; Niemi & Laine 1994). Retrieval of words is thus a high-speed, fairly automatic and effortless process (Levelt 1989:2, 20-22), as far as frequent and familiar words are concerned (Persson 1995:32; Harley 1998; Astell & Harley 1998). Infrequent and less familiar words, as well as deficits in the language system, tend to reduce the degree of automatic processing, and may require attentional control in the form of, for example, a more conscious search for particular words (Persson 1995:32; Astell & Harley 1996). Controlled processing is always needed when the speaker intentionally chooses his or her perspective to compose a message including any relevant information (Levelt 1989:20; 1999a; Levelt et al. 1999). Automatic processing can run simultaneously at multiple levels in parallel, whereas operations, in which controlled processing is needed, seem to take place in a serial order (Levelt 1989:2, 2022, 28; Persson 1995:32-33, 48-50). Although word production is a high-speed process, the rate of errors of lexical selection is merely in the range of one or two per thousand (Garnham, Shillcock, Brown, Mill & Cutler 1981; Levelt et al. 1999; Levelt 2001). Among normal speakers, the rate of errors is likely to increase due to attentional lapses and under high and stressful processing demands (Levelt 1989:487). According to Dell (1986), the semantic-lexical organization, in which semantically and phonologically related items are easily activated, seems to some extent to be prone to speech errors. The system probably tolerates errors because of its productive and flexible nature in terms of allowing novel combinations of items, through which new words can be coined in a language. Word production is sustained by a system of cortical and subcortical regions, primarily across the left hemisphere of the brain (for a review on the neural architecture underlying word production, see Price, Indefrey & Van Turennout 1999). 4.1 Theories of spoken word production Generally speaking, word production follows the following pattern (Levelt et al. 1999a; Price et al. 1999). First, the speaker has to decide on what kind of information to express, whereby he or she selects the semantically most appropriate lexical 52 Spoken word production concept (i.e., concept for which there exists a word in the target language; e.g., ‘table’) among several other semantically related and activated alternatives (e.g., ‘furniture’, ‘chair’). Subsequently, the lexical item’s syntactic properties (e.g., grammatical class and, in some languages, gender) and morpho-phonological word form (/teibl/) are encoded, followed by syllabification with associated phonemes (/tei//bl/). Finally, the chosen phonemes are translated into a phonetic plan and articulated. During and after the process, the system provides the speaker feedback (internal speech) to monitor the output for corrections. Spoken word production can be approached from several perspectives that have their roots in different research traditions (see the review in Levelt 1999a; Price et al. 1999; Nickels 2001). Among the most current models of naming, there are chronometric models that account for distributions of reaction times in word production (e.g., Levelt 1989, 1999a, b, 2001; Levelt et al. 1991b, 1999; Roelofs 1992), as well as models that account for the distributions of spontaneous or induced speech errors (e.g., Stemberger 1985; Dell 1986; Dell & O’Seaghdha 1991, 1992; Martin, Dell, Saffran & Schwartz 1994; Persson 1995; Dell, Schwartz, Martin, Saffran & Gagnon 1997; Foygel & Dell 2000). The models of both traditions explain the knowledge domain as a network of associative links, and the same underlying processes of word production in terms of activation spreading in the network. Both accounts hold that two stages, lexical selection and phonological encoding, are the crucial phases of successful word production. Despite the similarities, the approaches seem to differ in many details, such as the relationship between meaning and form retrieval and the architecture of the semantic representation. However, the research traditions have begun to merge in recent years (Levelt 1999a). According to the discrete two-stage word production model, developed by Levelt et al. (see the reviews in Levelt 1999a, b, 2001; Levelt et al. 1999; see also Roelofs 1992), the conceptual-semantic representations of words is described as individual “wholes” which are non-decomposed chunks of information. The meaning of a word is defined by the strength and number of links there are between the semantically related lexical concepts, such as super- and subordinates or co-ordinates of the same semantic category. The model allows bi-directional spreading of activation at the conceptual-semantic stage among the competitive candidates of the lexical concepts (e.g., ‘sheep’, ‘goat’, ‘llama’ or ‘choose (x, y)’, ‘elect (x, y)’, ’select (x, y)’), and between the conceptual and the lemma stage, during which the grammatical information of the lexical concepts is specified (Levelt 1999a, b, 2001; Levelt et al. 1999). However, there is only uni-directional, feed-forward spread of activation further down from the lemma level to the phonological level, during which the selected item’s morpho-phonological form is established. Consequently, the word’s phonological form does not contribute to the semantic specification of the word (see the discussion in Persson 1995:51-54, 125; Laine & Martin 1996; Harley 1998; Levelt 1999a). Spoken word production 53 There is evidence, however, to indicate that prior to the lexical selection, feedback from phonological information flows to the level of semantic specification. For example, Martin, Weisberg and Saffran (1989; see also Persson 1995:52-53, 127-130) showed that words sharing both semantic and phonological similarity were vulnerable to substitutions. In the semantic fluency task, in which semantically related words are to be named, words sharing both semantic and phonological features tend to be produced (e.g., ‘pantteri’, ‘panda’, ‘puuma’/ ‘panther’, ‘panda’, ‘puma’ in Laine 1989:22 or ‘pomme’, ‘poir’ / ‘apple’, ‘pear’ in Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; see 5.3). Furthermore, there is evidence to assume that semantic representations can be decomposed into different types of features (e.g., Smith et al. 1974; Barsalou et al. 1982, 1983; Dell 1986). Taking these arguments into account, and considering a theory’s possibilities to explain errors occurring during word production, the twostage interactive activation models appear to be architectures of choice in word production studies, including the present study (see 10.1.3). 4.2 Two-stage interactive activation models Similarly to the discrete two-stage model, the interactive, connectionist two-stage word production models (e.g., Dell 1986; Dell & O’Seaghdha 1991, 1992; Martin et al. 1994; Persson 1995; Dell et al. 1997; Foygel & Dell 2000), distinguishes meaning vs. form retrieval in word production. However, the semantic representations of words are decomposed into semantic features (see 3.2). The models are serial in the sense that at onset, the semantic activation is followed by phonological activation, and the output is likely to follow a certain order defined by the grammatical, morphophonological, and articulatory regularities. Above all, however, the models are interactive, because the network allows a simultaneous and bi-directional excitatory spread of activation in and across the levels during the word formation process. As a consequence, the system allows interaction between semantic and phonological levels, leading to an overlap of different functional stages during the word retrieval. The crucial difference between the models concerns the extent of the activation between the semantic and lexical level and the point of inhibition of the semantic competitors (see the discussion in Persson 1995:127). During the semantic feature encoding, as soon as fragments of the targetrelated information have become activated by an external stimulus (e.g., an object), the semantic memory system starts encoding (Persson 1995:64-67). In a well working system, the semantic features start exciting each other until a whole set of features becomes simultaneously and quite automatically excited. According to the model of Dell et al. (Dell 1986; Dell & O’Seaghdha 1991, 1992; Martin et al. 1994; Dell et al. 1997; Foygel & Dell 2000), activation from all the excited semantic features spreads to the lexical (lemma) network to prime the target (e.g., ‘cat’), semantically related nodes (e.g., ‘dog’), as well as semantically and phonologically related nodes (e.g., ‘rat’), from which activation is fed back to the semantic network to reinforce the 54 Spoken word production activated semantic features. Activation also continues to spread forward to the phonological network where both the primed and related segments and phonemes, such as onsets /d/, /f/, /k/, /l/, /m/, and /r/, vowels /ae/, /o/, and codas /g/, /t/ are triggered. Activation is fed back to the lexical network, which also receives reverberating feedback from the activated semantic features, leading all the related lexical nodes to be more or less activated (e.g., ‘fog’, ‘log’, ‘dog’, ‘cat’, ‘mat’, ‘rat’) and the lexical selection to take place. At this point, the node with the strongest activation gets selected, phonologically and phonetically encoded, and articulated as a word. In the Parallel Distributed Lexical Processing model (Persson 1995: 64-67, 125-130), the semantic structure of the target word becomes rapidly built-up and disambiguated during the semantic feature encoding, requiring that the target structure gains most activation, and that the activation settles on the target, stops flickering, and ceases triggering features of less relevant non-targets, whereby the activation of the semantic competitors decays. Information from only the target semantic entity spreads down to the morpho-phonological level of information, exciting the word’s initial syllabic and segmential information. Subsequently, activation from these early morpho-phonological probes (i.e., after the word onset) spreads back to the semantic level, where the semantic feature encoding is then completed. Once the entire semantic feature pattern has become selected, activation spreads back down to the morpho-phonological level, when the rest of the morpho-phonological information is encoded and the word’s form is completed. Thus, the semantic information supports the encoding of the word’s final form and the successful word production. The purpose of early inhibition of non-targets is to avoid noise and to reduce interference and errors in the system (see also Levelt 1999; Levelt et al. 1991, 1999). Relative to other word production models, the advantage of multiple activation at all three levels in the interactive models can be considered in their potential to explain various types of speech errors occurring in and between the different phases of word processing (Dell 1986; Martin et al. 1994; Schwartz, Saffran, Bloch & Dell 1994; Laine & Martin 1996; Dell, Burger & Svec 1997; Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997; Foygel & Dell 2000; Persson 1995; see 10.1.3). On the other hand, the relevance of the models that are based on speech error analysis in describing normal word production, in which the error rate is very low, has been questioned by proponents of, for example, chronometric models (Levelt et al. 1991a). A newer version of the interactive word production model presented by Foygel and Dell (2000; see also Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997) assumes that the semantic-lexical connections accounting for the meaning component for one, and the lexical-phonological connections accounting for the form component for the other, are different functional processes, each of which can be damaged independently of each other, for example, in aphasia. 5 Semantic fluency performance in elderly adults and Alzheimer’s patients A generative naming task, also known as the verbal fluency task, is a commonly used experimental method to investigate spoken word production. More specifically, it is used to measure mental processing speed (Lafosse et al. 1997), the organization and function of the lexical-semantic systems, as well as the ability of the speaker to rapidly access these systems (Joanette & Goulet 1986; Huff 1988; Hodges & Patterson 1995). Verbal fluency has been explored, for example, in the developing (Lucariello, Kyratzis & Nelson 1992; Cohen, Morgan, Vaughn, Riccio & Hall 1999; Riva, Nichelli & Devoti 2000; Kaleva & Vanhala 2001) and ageing brain (Crossley, D’Arcy & Rawson 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Capitani, Laiacona & Barbarotto 1999), as well as in psychiatric diseases, such as schizophrenia (Allen, Liddle & Frith 1993; Crowe 1996). The verbal fluency task has been a very popular method for investigating the change in cognitive functioning in various neurological conditions, such as aphasia (e.g., Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Holappa & Nauha 2000), multiple sclerosis (MS; Tröster et al. 1998), and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS; Abrahams, Leigh, Harvey, Vythelingum, Grisé & Goldstein 2000). It is also commonly used in studying the effects of some dementing diseases on cognitive processing, such as Parkinson’s disease (PD), Huntington’s disease (HD; e.g., Randolph, Braun, Goldberg & Chase 1993; Tröster et al. 1998), and Alzheimer’s disease, which has been the main focus of a wealth of studies (see 5.1). Fluency tasks are thus in wide linguistic and neuropsychological use, both in clinical and experimental work (Capitani et al. 1999). They are included in the standardized language assessment batteries as part of the evaluation of the language production abilities, for example, the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE; Goodglass & Kaplan 1972) and the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesz 1982), and incorporated into some dementia screening batteries (e.g., Mattis 1976). In the verbal fluency task, a person is asked to orally generate as many words as possible according to a cue or specified rules in a limited time range, the period most typically being 60 seconds. There are several kinds of verbal fluency tasks: The phonemic (letter) fluency task involves words that begin with a specific letter, such as ‘A’, ‘C’ ‘F’, ‘L’, ‘P’, or ‘S’ (e.g., Rosen 1980; Laine 1989:4-5, 20-21; Capitani, 56 Semantic fluency Laiacona & Basso 1998). In the semantic fluency task, which is the focus of the present study, subjects are asked to generate words that belong to a certain semantic category, such as animals and vegetables (e.g., Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:20-22; Troyer et al. 1997; Capitani et al. 1999). The task may also be executed non-verbally by drawing the category items (Mickanin, Grossman, Onishi, Auriacombe & Clark 1994). Furthermore, another form of the semantic fluency task, the supermarket fluency task, involves items found in a supermarket (e.g., Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober, Dronkers, Koss, Delis & Friedland 1986). In the phonemic fluency task, phonological processing is investigated (Rosen 1980; Capitani et al. 1998), whereas the semantic fluency tasks are used to investigate the retrieval from, and the organization of, the semantic memory (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Monsch et al. 1992; 1994; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Crowe 1998). In particular, the semantic fluency task is widely used to reveal word finding problems and deteriorated cognitive processes associated with word retrieval (Ruff, Light, Parker & Levin 1997). In comparison with other fluency tasks, the semantic fluency task has appeared to be more sensitive than the phonemic fluency task with regard to revealing difficulty in word finding and differentiating patients with AD from healthy elderly normal control subjects (Monsch et al. 1992; Monsch et al. 1994; Weingartner, Kawas, Rawlings & Shapiro 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Goldstein et al. 1996; Carew, Lamar, Cloud, Grossman & Libon 1997; Crossley et al. 1997). However, opposite findings have been reported by Bayles, Trosset, Tomoeda, Montgomery, and Wilson (1993) and Suhr and Jones (1998) who reported on the AD patients’ worse phonemic fluency task performance relative to that on the semantic fluency task. Furthermore, AD patients have been shown to perform better on the supermarket fluency task for which words are available from several different semantic categories (e.g., fruit, vegetables, pastry, dairy products, etc.; Monsch et al.1992), as well as on a task tapping non-semantic drawing of designs (Mickanin et al. 1994) than on the traditional semantic fluency task (e.g., animals). Differences in performing various types of fluency tasks may be caused by the specific aspects of language they are tapping (e.g., phonological vs. semantic utilization), and by different neural substrates (e.g., verbal vs. nonverbal generation; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997). 5.1 Word production during the semantic fluency task In spontaneous speech, word production is guided by semantic and syntactic information contained by words, syntactic frame (e.g., a clause with different slots the words fill in a certain order), as well as by contextual cues (Dell 1986). Picture naming, on the other hand, is primarily supported by a picture or by a drawing of an object or action. In retrieving a name for a picture, the very first stage of the naming Semantic fluency 57 process is the computation of the visual representation of, for example, an object, followed by categorization of the object as a car, table, etc., after which the information is subjected to syntactic, morphological, and phonological encoding (Levelt 1989:222-234; see chap. 4). The semantic fluency task is different in the sense that a retrieval cue or a semantic probe is verbally given that defines the semantic category (e.g., vehicle, furniture) that needs to be first identified before word production can be initiated (e.g., Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:22, Troyer et al. 1997; Capitani et al. 1999). The semantic fluency task is a multifactorial, complex task that involves several psycho-linguistic components. Categorization (Troyer 2000; see 3.1), availability of intact semantic representations, and the ability to break down a semantic category into subordinate categories and category members (Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:1819; Binetti et al. 1995; Pasquier, Lebert, Grymonprez & Petit 1995; see 3.1.1, 3.1.2) are crucial for performing the task. Flexible functioning of the whole mental lexicon is required in the form of semantic feature selection, lemma retrieval, and morphophonological encoding, as well as articulation, and self-monitoring (Laine 1989:5; Price et al. 1999; see chap. 4). Performing the task is not constrained by syntactic structure or discourse planning, as far as production of nouns is concerned, but a semantically and/or phonologically guided search for appropriate responses is required (Laine 1989:5, 14, 18, 22). For an optimal performance, the role of working memory and executive functions, in particular, are underscored (Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:5; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Auriacombe et al. 1993; Rosen & Engle 1997; Ruff et al. 1997; Abrahams et al. 2000; Cohen & Stanczak 2000; Bayles 2003). Performing the semantic fluency task requires the functioning of working memory which provides temporary storage and processing of information necessary for complex cognitive tasks (Baddeley, Logie, Bressi, Della Sala & Spinnler 1986; Baddeley, Bressi, Della Sala, Logie & Spinnler 1991, Baddeley 1992; Gathercole & Baddeley 1993:1-23; see also Morris 1994; Rosen & Engle; Zec et al. 1999; Abrahams et al. 2000; Rende, Ramsberger & Miyake 2002; Bayles 2003). In particular, the role of the central executive component of the working memory to rapidly initiate a systematic, effective, and attentional search for subcategories and words through the semantic memory and to flexibly change semantic subcategories seems to be essential when performing the task (see Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Laine 1985, 1989:5, 13-14, 18-19, 22-25; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Allen et al. 1993; Della Sala, Lorenzi, Spinner & Zuffi 1993; Randolph et al. 1993; Monsch et al. 1994; Pasquier et al. 1995; Rosen & Engle 1997; Ruff et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Zec et al. 1999). Working memory is also responsible for monitoring the word retrieval processes to avoid repeating previously retrieved words and breaking the rules set for the fluency task (Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:5; Auriacombe et al. 1993; Della Sala et al. 1993; Pasquier et al. 1995; Rosen & Engle 1997; Ruff et al. 1997; see also Levelt 1989:463-467, 1999a, b). Semantic fluency performance may also 58 Semantic fluency be influenced by nonverbal factors, such as imagery and ability to generate visual images (Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Mickanin et al. 1994; Rende et al. 2002; see also Baddeley et al. 1986, 1992; Gathercole & Baddeley 1993:17-22). Word production and factors affecting semantic fluency performance Traditionally, semantic fluency tasks are analyzed by counting the sum of correct responses for each semantic category and by comparing the scores among different subjects (Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Crowe 1998). Evidence on the semantic fluency performance of healthy elderly adults representing different cultures or languages (see Table 4) revealed a relatively wide range in the number of the words that were produced for different categories. For example, in the semantic fluency task with naming words for the category of animals (for 60 seconds), which is the most often studied semantic category, on the average 14.1 words were produced by an Italian-speaking group of subjects (Binetti et al. 1995), 14.2 and 17.8 words by two Canadian English groups (Crossley et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 1997), 19.3 words by an American English group (Chertkow & Bub 1990; see also Hodges et al. 1992), 22.4 words by a Finnish group (Kontiola et al. 1990), and 26 words by a French group (Pasquier et al. 1995), the subjects’ mean age being between 65 and 74 years. Performance on the semantic fluency task also shows great heterogeneity among AD patients (see Table 4). There exists a number of studies indicating that AD patients in different phases of the disease generated significantly fewer words in the semantic fluency task than normal control subjects across several semantic categories (e.g., Rosen 1980; Appell et al. 1982; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster, Salmon, McCullough & Butters 1989; Mickanin et al. 1994; Binetti et al.1995; Crossley et al. 1997; cf. Fischer, Gatterer, Marterer & Danielczyk 1988). In the animal fluency task, a marked impairment in the ability of AD patients to generate exemplars for that category was discovered. Chertkow and Bub (1990) noticed that AD patients listed overall only 40% as many animal names as the controls, the proportion being 51% in the study by Cronin-Golomb et al. (1992). Studies in which the animal fluency task was employed and the severity of dementia was assessed with the MMSE (see Table 4), significantly more words were produced by normal control subjects (e.g., 14.1 words in Binetti et al. 1995, 17.5 words in Hodges & Patterson 1995, and 17.7 words in Carew et al. 1997) relative to patients with minimal dementia (14.0 words in Hodges & Patterson 1995), and patients with mild dementia (6.7 words in Carew et al. 1997, 8.3 words in Binetti et al. 1995, and 9.5 words in Hodges & Patterson 1995). Respectively, the performance of the moderately demented subjects (4.5 words in Hodges & Patterson 1995), and the severely demented subjects (3.5 words in Binetti et al. 1995) were also significantly poorer than that of healthy elderly control subjects. It was also reported that AD patients with very mild dementia tended to perform better than mildly demented subjects, who in turn produced significantly more words than moderately (Mickanin et al. 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Crossley et al. 1997) and severely Semantic fluency 59 demented AD subjects (Binetti et al. 1995). In the study of Randolph et al. (1993), the outcome of the AD patients was poorer even in the cued version of the task in which retrieval cues in terms of subcategories (e.g., pets, farm animals, jungle and water animals) were provided. It was pointed out in some studies that common words were retrieved before uncommon ones by most of the normal control subjects and AD patients, and that they tended to be recalled early in the retrieval period (Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986). It was also shown that, on average, AD patients produced words in a significantly higher level of prototypicality (Beatty et al. 2000) and mean lexical frequency (Weingartner et al. 1993; Binetti et al. 1995) than normal control subjects during the course of the whole semantic fluency task. Some other studies, however, failed to observe such differences (Ober et al. 1986; Chan et al. 1993; Goldstein et al. 1996; see also Binetti et al. 1995). Rosen (1980; see also Butters, Granholm, Salmon, Grant & Wolfe 1987) found that, in the animal fluency task (60 s), healthy control subjects, as well as mildly and moderately-to-severely demented AD subjects, retrieved most of the words during the first 15-second interval. The normal control subjects produced more words than the mild and the severe AD groups during the first interval, as did the mildly demented AD patients relative to the moderately-to-severely demented AD patients. During the second interval, the only difference in the number of words emerged between the normal controls and the moderately-to-severely demented AD subjects. In a similar setting, Ober et al. (1986) found that the rate of production of correct responses differed among the groups, the asymptote occurring earliest for the moderate-to severe AD subjects, then for the mild AD subjects, and last for the healthy controls. The semantic fluency task was shown be significantly affected by advancing age among normal control subjects (Tröster et al. 1989; Crossley et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer 2000; Capitani et al. 1999). Older participants were found to produce fewer words than younger participants (Butters et al. 1987; Tomer et Levin 1993; Tröster et al. 1989; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer 2000). In the animal fluency task, Troyer et al. (1997) observed that both younger and older participants formed clusters consisting of two semantically related words belonging to the same subcategory of animals (e.g., ‘dog’, ‘wolf’), but the older subjects were able to generate fewer animal names and to activate fewer semantic dimensions for subcategories in the animal category, implying a less effective retrieval strategy among the older participants. Among AD patients, age failed to explain reductions in verbal fluency (Huff et al.1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997). In a one year follow-up study conducted by Hodges et al. (1990), it was observed that the rapid decline in the AD patients’ performance on measures of semantic memory could not be explained by ageing because age-matched normal controls showed no decline in fluency performance over the course of the study. Rather than advancing age, neurogenerative effects of AD were assumed to cause the decline in semantic fluency (Huff et al.1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997). animals supermarket items a. clothes Rosen 1980 Martin & Fedio 1983 Diesfeldt 1985 (Dutch) vehicles, vegetables, tools, clothes + a. colors b. animals c. towns d. fruit supermarket items a. animals b. vehicles c. fruit d. body parts e. tools f. furniture g. clothing Huff et al. 1986 Hart et al. 1988 Tröster et al. 1989 Chertkow & Bub 1990 b. fruit 3 min + Semantic category Study (language, other than English) 10 20 12 18 24 11 10 73.0 70.4 (6.23) 69.8 (6.2) 62.0 83.0 (4.1) 61.5 83.6 NC NC n mean age (SD) MMSE a. 16.1 b. 12.0 c. 12.0 d. 21.2 e. 13.2 f. 15.1 g. 18.7 19.35 (0.8) a. 13.1 (2.1) b. 8.0 (3.3) c. 19.9 (5.0) d. 14.5 (3.8) 52.6 (9.6) b. 16.9 (4.7) a. 25.4 (6.7) 22.7 11.3 NC mean number of words (SD) 10 MMSE 17.3 miAD 20 moAD 20 15 15 miAD 12 moAD 10 seAD 1 66 moAD 44 seAD 22 14 miAD 10 mo-seAD 10 AD n severity of dementia# MMSE 76.3 72.2 (8.1) 70.6 (6.7) 68.3 (4.0) 65.3 80.5 (6.4) 58.2 85.2 84.8 AD mean age (SD) a. 5.7 * b. 5.0 * c. 5.3 * d. 8.8 * e. 4.2 * f. 3.1 * g. 6.5 * 12.55 (3.97) * 8.95 (3.07) * a. 7.1 (2.7) * b. 7.3 (2.9) * c. 4.7 (2.6) * d. 6.2 (1.9) * 23.9 (16.0) * b. 4.2 (2.6)* a. 5.5 (4.0)* 11.4 * 8.4 * 1.7 * AD mean number of words (SD) semantic impairment (loss) and semantic search, affected by attention problems, motivation and inefficient strategies, no visual deficits disruption in the structure of semantic knowledge impaired access to lexical information loss of specific information required to distinguish among members of a category, associated with difficulty in naming -impaired knowledge of more specific attributes of concepts / retrieval -weakened thought and search strategy -impaired short-term memory capacity -slowness of search through long-term store -increased arousal level specific disruption in the organization of semantic knowledge (loss or access) and relative preservation of broader categorical information impaired retrieval Hypothesized cause of the impairment Table 4. Studies on the semantic fluency in normal control subjects (NC) and patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 60 Semantic fluency a. animals b. birds c. water creatures d. dogs e. houshold items f. vehicles g. musical instr. h. boats a. animals, fruit, and vegetables 3 min + Hodges et al. 1992 Monsch et al. 1992 Monsch et al. 1994 a. living categories (animals, birds, water creatures) + Rosser & Hodges 1994 animals, fruit/ vegetables 3 min + b. man made categories (household objects, vehicles, musical instruments) animals Chan et al. 1993 b. supermarket items animals Kontiola et al. 1990 (Finnish) Table 4 (continued) 72.3 (5.8) 65.3 (9.1) 44 25 24 70.0 (10.6) 69 (8.4) 70.0 (6.2) 53 71.2 (7.9) MMSE 28.8 (1.1) 26 86 48.9 (8.4) b. 55.1 (8.5) a. 57.4 (12.7) 19.25 (5.4) b. 22.8 (4.7) a. 48.4 (9.8) a. 19.7 (4.5) b. 14.1 (4.5) c. 13.0 (3.6) d. 10.2 (4.1) e. 19.8 (5.7) f. 13.9 (4.8) g. 14.0 (3.8) h. 11.6 (3.4) 22.41 (5.5) 44 10 46 89 MMSE 18.0 (5.0) 22 33 71.5 (6.5) 67.2 (9.9) 71 (7.7) 72.1 (6.6) 69.5 (5.4) 70.0 (10.4) 21.6 (8.2) * b. 32.5 (9.6) * a. 31.1 (6.1) * 10.7 (4.37) * b. 8.4 (4.9) * a. 16.2 (9.6) * a. 9.9 (4.1) * b. 5.4 (2.8) * c. 4.4 (2.6) * d. 3.2 (2.0) * e. 9.1 (3.8) * f. 6.9 (3.0) * g. 6.5 (2.6) * h. 4.4 (2.9) * 9.5 (4.4) * Table 4 continues breakdown in the structure of the semantic knowledge breakdown of semantic knowledge breakdown in the structure of semantic knowledge deterioration of the semantic knowledge system semantic breakdown caused by storage degradation; differential impairment across the hierarchy of semantic knowledge (preserved superordinate knowledge but impaired lower level knowledge) general intellectual impairment and memory disorder Semantic fluency 61 animals Binetti et al. 1995 (Italian) e. househ. items f. vehicles g. musical instr. h. boats Hodges & Patterson 1995 a. animals b. birds c. water animals d. dogs Semantic category Study (language, other than English) Table 4 (continued) 24 MMSE 29.1 (1.0) 35 MMSE 28.0 (1.8) NC n 69.7 (7.8) 67.4 (7.3) NC mean age (SD) e. 17.4 (3.4) f. 11.7 (2.6) g. 15.3 (3.4) h. 10.8 (3.3) a. 17.5 (3.9) b. 15.9 (4.5) c. 14.0 (6.7) d. 10.8 (3.4) 14.1 (5.4) NC mean number of words (SD) e. 10.0 (4.5) * f. 6.9 (2.7) * g. 7.1 (3.9) * h. 4.4 (3.2) * e. 3.3 (2.9) * f. 3.1 (1.9) * g. 2.1 (2.4) * h. 1.4 (1.8) * moAD a. 4.5 (3.5) * b. 3.1 (2.8) * c. 1.3 (1.8) * d. 1.2 (1.3) * miAD 63.4 (8.8) moAD 18 MMSE 10.0 (4.6) a. 9.5 (4.3) * b. 6.7 (3.6) * c. 4.9 (4.6) * d. 4.7 (2.2) * e. 11.1 (4.1) * f. 8.2 (3.9) * g. 8.0 (1.7) * h. 6.3 (2.9) * 67.0 (8.3) miAD 17 MMSE 20.9 (1.7) a. 14.0 (4.1) * b. 7.9 (3.1) * c. 6.7 (3.9) * d. 5.8 (2.2) * 3.5 (2.4) * 8.3 (3.6) * AD mean number of words (SD) minAD 72.2 (7.2) 71.3 (9.8) seAD 30 MMSE 12.3 (2.6) minAD 17 MMSE 25.6 (1.8) 69.7 (7.8) AD mean age (SD) miAD 40 MMSE 21.4 (2.7) AD n severity of dementia # task difficulty, a central semantic deficit damage to subordinate categories / degradation of semantic knowledge Hypothesized cause of the impairment 62 Semantic fluency animals food, 30s vegetables, 30s + animals animals animals, fruit/vegetables tools/kitchen utensils + animals animals Beatty et al. 1997 Monsch et al. 1997 (Swiss-German) Crossley et al. 1997 (English and French) Carew et al. 1997 Suhr & Jones 1998 Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998 Tröster et al. 1998 30 38 25 31 MMSE 28.8 (1.1) 635 50 MMSE 28.4 (1.4) 38 MMSE 28.7 (1.6) 10 70.8 (7.0) 73.8 (6.2) 67.9 (12.9) 17.8 (4.1) 17.9 (4.2) 50.9 (11.9) 17.7 (4.7) 13.8 (4.0) 78.8 (6.8)+ 76.4 (6.6) 22.1 (5.0) 18.0 (4.9) 26 71.9 (7.2) 73.7 (8.7) 66.8 (8.1) 30 23 31 40 MMSE 22.2 (3.0) 154 miAD 46 moAD 108 50 MMSE 24.2 (3.1) 35 MMSE 18.5 (5.3) 10 MMSE 23.1 (5.2) 69.7 (5.9) 70.3 (8.4) 75.5 (5.6) 76.8 (5.9) 82.5 (5.9)+ 72.4 (7.1) 76.5 (8.2) 68.2 (8.9) 7.6 (4.6) * 8.3 (4.2) * 30.1 (13.7) * 6.7 (2.5) * 9.5 (3.5) * 8.1 (3.5) * 13.6 (4.1) * 7.4 (4.7) * 17 * inefficiency of access to lexical and semantic memory stores impoverished semantic memory or deficient search processes within semantic memory none overall dissolution of semantic knowledge (loss or degradation) impaired semantic knowledge / limited capacity store impaired structures of semantic knowledge degraded structure of the semantic memory; deterioration of mechanisms that govern initiation of search for appropriate subcategories dysfunction of one or several complex processes (e.g., loss of semantic knowledge, inadequate activation of the concept, reduced retrieval, lack of systematic strategy, reduced short-term memory capacity) Note. MMSE = average scores on the Mini Mental State Examination (Folstein et al. 1975). +scores summed. # = severity of dementia in AD: minAD = minimal, miAD = mild, moAD = moderate, mo-seAD = moderate to severe, se = severe. * AD patients performed significantly worse than control subjects. animals Pasquier et al. 1995 (French) Table 4 (continued) Semantic fluency 63 64 Semantic fluency Education was found to be a sensitive factor in semantic fluency performance, affecting the increase in performance the more educated the normal elderly subjects were (Crossley et al. 1997; Capitani et al. 1999; Troyer 2000; cf. Ratcliff, Ganguli, Chandra, Sharma, Belle, Seaberg & Pandav 1998 for the relatively high scores of the uneducated and illiterate Haryanvi speakers). In AD, education did not covary with semantic fluency performance (Rosen 1980; Crossley et al. 1997). Monsch et al. (1992) reported that female participants fared significantly better than male participants when generating words for the semantic fluency task in which animals, fruit and vegetables were used as the semantic categories. The gender effect was found both in the group of normal healthy elderly adults, as well as in the group of AD patients. In the same vein, Capitani et al. (1999) found that gender had a significant effect on the fluency performance but only in the categories of fruit and tools. In their study, male participants produced significantly fewer words for fruit but significantly more words for tools than female participants. Capitani et al. explained that the gender effect could be due to male and female subjects’ different experiences, habits, and occupation influencing the recall of words rather than either gender having a larger semantic store per se. In contrast, Troyer (2000) found no difference between male and female participants in the animal and supermarket fluency performance. Likewise, Crossley et al. (1997) noticed that the semantic fluency performance of AD patients on animals was gender-insensitive. The performance of the AD patients on the semantic fluency task appears to be significantly correlated with severity of dementia (Huff et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Crossley et al. 1997), regardless of the methods used to evaluate severity of dementia and the semantic categories employed. Higher scores on the MMSE measuring the severity of dementia were found to be associated with more correct responses in the task (Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997; cf. Shuttleworth & Huber 1988). Hodges and Patterson (1995) demonstrated a significant difference in the semantic fluency performance between AD patients with minimal (MMSE = 25.6), mild (MMSE = 20.9), and moderate (MMSE = 10.0) dementia. However, their study also indicated that AD patients with minimal and mild dementia were difficult to distinguish from each other when very specific semantic categories, such as birds, dogs, vehicles, and musical instruments were used. Furthermore, Hodges and Patterson (1995; see also Della Sala et al. 1993) found great heterogeneity in the performances of the AD patients, especially in the mildest cases. A significant group difference in the semantic fluency performance was found also between mild (MMSE = 23.7) and moderate (MMSE = 17.5) AD patients (Mickanin et al. 1994). Semantic fluency task has previously been mainly used to study the semantic representations of nouns, whereas the semantic representations of verbs have so far remained almost unexplored by this particular method. Two studies have employed an action fluency task in PD patients with and without dementia, as well as normal control subjects, who were asked to produce verbs under the instruction of “differ- Semantic fluency 65 ent things people can do” (Piatt, Fields, Paolo, Koller & Tröster 1999; Piatt, Fields, Paolo & Tröster 1999). A variant of the semantic fluency task, however, has been used by asking AD patients to generate script-like events to study their access to semantic memory and its knowledge representation system, as well as its degradation (for a description of scripts, see 3.3.4). Weingartner et al. (1983) reported that AD patients were impaired both in the traditional category fluency task with four-footed animals and vegetables and in sequencing activities of a complex event script. AD patients had difficulty in arranging the subevents of a script of eating in a restaurant in an appropriate temporal order. Their order errors tended to occur particularly on those events that were temporally close to each other rather than far apart. When asked to produce activities to the question “What are the things you would do after getting up in the morning?”, AD patients generated significantly fewer and more frequent types of activities than normal control subjects. Based on their findings, Weingartner et al. concluded that AD patients’ access to semantic memory was impaired and that they, therefore, had great difficulty in organizing and encoding semantic information (see chap. 6). Grafman et al. (1991) replicated the study and confirmed the finding that some activities produced by AD patients for the script ”all the things that you do when you get up in the morning till you leave the house or have lunch” fell outside the temporal boundaries of the script or were inappropriate (e.g., ‘going-shopping’). Furthermore, some of the script events were inappropriately repeated. Grafman et al. concluded that, either due to a structural degeneration or a processing problem, AD patients presented a breakdown in script production. The traditional scoring of the semantic fluency task, that is, counting the sum of correct responses has been claimed to be insufficient to distinguish fluency performance among different dementia groups (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998) or focally lesioned patients with different lesion loci (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander & Stuss 1998), and it may not necessarily reveal whether subjects’ problems lie in the slowed production, the retrieval deficit, or the degradation of the semantic memory (Raskin, Sliwinski & Bodor 1992; Della Sala et al. 1993; see chap. 6). Merely looking at the number of correct responses in the semantic fluency task may not be enough to highlight the semantic representations of the words or the word retrieval processes (Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Troyer et al. 1997). It is important also to analyze and compare the qualitative aspects of semantic retrieval performance among different subject groups, for example, the strategies of exploiting the semantic field (Laine 1989:18-19, 22-23; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998), the rate of responding (Rosen 1980; Laine 1989:22-24), the nature of correct responses in terms of semantic associations (Allen et al. 1993; Binetti et al. 1995; Roberts & Le Dorze 1997), and the type of errors made during the task (Ober et al. 1986; Laine 1989:22; Bayles et al. 1993; Binetti et al. 1995; Pasquier et al. 1995; Carew et al. 1997; Suhr & Jones 1998; Tröster et al. 1998). 66 Semantic fluency 5.2 Clustering and switching Performing the semantic fluency task does not occur at random, but in an organized fashion (Laine 1989:18-19, 22-25). Successful retrieval of words belonging to different semantic categories depends on systematic and efficient retrieval strategies, as well as on intactness of the information to be retrieved from the semantic memory (Martin & Fedio 1983; Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Butters et al. 1987; Laine 1989:5; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al.1990; Monsch et al. 1992, 1994; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Binetti et al. 1995; Pasquier et al. 1995). Although the semantic fluency task is widely used in the assessment of language functions, there are only a few studies with a more thorough description on the strategies involved during the word retrieval. However, knowledge concerning those strategies is limited to the production of nouns, mainly to the category of animals and items available in a supermarket. First, an access to the intact semantic memory or the semantic layer of the mental lexicon is required. More specifically, access to the information corresponding to the semantic categories, as well as to the semantic features defining the category items, is necessary for performing the task (Schwartz et a. 1979; Rosen 1980; Martin & Fedio 1983; Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Randolph et al. 1993; Price et al. 1999; Troyer 2000). Second, a prototype (a typical item) or a very frequently occurring word of the category is likely to be first activated (e.g., ‘cat’ for the category of animals), from which activation automatically spreads to closely related semantic neighbours (e.g., ‘dog’; Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Hodges et al. 1990; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Crowe 1998; see chap. 4). Subsequently, after using the most prototypical cases, a more active search of category items is likely to be initiated, and an entrance into subcategories takes place (e.g., farm animals, birds, sea animals; Rosen 1980; Laine 1989:18; Crowe 1998). An ability to generate an effective strategy, for example, an inner visualization of a search set, such as a wardrobe or a dress shop for clothes, and a fruit desk or a greengrocer shop for fruit, plays a role in retrieval and use of semantic information (Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Randolph et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Capitani et al. 1999; Rende et al. 2002; see also Baddeley et al. 1986, 1992; Gathercole & Baddeley 1993:17-22).). A systematic strategy reduces the likelihood of repetitions to emerge during the word retrieval (Diesfeldt 1985; Pasquier et al. 1995). Gruenewald and Lockhead (1980) were among the first to introduce a twostage component model of the semantic fluency performance that was later operationalized by other researchers (e.g., Laine 1989; Troyer et al. 1997; Mayr & Kliegl 2000). For one, performing the task involves switching, that is, an effective search for different semantic fields or subcategories and a change from one semantic subcategory to another. For the other, clustering includes production of words in the subcategories once they are identified (Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980; Laine 1989:13-14, 18-19; Troyer et al. 1997). The search is not only for individual words per se but also for a collection or cluster of words associatively or semantically Semantic fluency 67 related to the particular semantic field (Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980; Rosen 1980; Martin & Fedio 1983; Chan et al. 1993; Troyer et al. 1997). According to Troyer et al. (1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer 2000; see also Grunewald & Lockhead 1980; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000; Rich, Troyer, Bylsma & Brandt 1999), clustering and switching are dissociable components of semantic fluency performance. They consider clustering to be semantic processing related to the temporal lobe functioning, whereas switching involves executive functions, such as flexibility in shifting from one subcategory to another that is processed in the frontal regions of the brain. On the other hand, according to Mayr and Kliegl (2000; see also Mayr 2002), the whole semantic fluency performance, including clustering and switching, is predominantly semantic processing and executive processes are likely to be accompanied in each act of word retrieval, not just during between-cluster switches. Clustering and switching determine the number of total words produced for the task. Thus, larger cluster size would be associated with less switching, and vice versa. Hence, an optimal fluency performance requires a balance between clustering and switching (Laine 1989:14, 19; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer 2000), although the ability to produce clusters of semantically meaningful responses as such is independent of the ability to search and shift from one cluster to another (Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980; Carew et al. 1997). During the task, words are not produced evenly in time but in spurts (Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980). Most of the words are produced during the first 15 seconds, after which the word retrieval slows down and more atypical and infrequently occurring words are retrieved (Rosen 1980; Ober et al. 1986; Butters et al. 1987; Laine 1989:29; Crowe 1998). Generating words in a certain subcategory should take place rather automatically and for as long as a relatively fast pace can be maintained, after which a new search and a switch to another subcategory should occur (Laine 1989:19). Switching from one subcategory to another elicits longer pauses between produced words than retrieval in a subcategory does (Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980, Laine 1989:23-24; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer 2000; Carew et al. 1997). The study of Troyer et al. (1997) indicated that younger Canadian English-speaking adults (mean age 22.3 years), when producing approximately 21.8 (SD = 5.7) animal names, switched on average 10.6 (SD = 3.5) times between the subcategories and produced a mean of two words per cluster, whereas the older adults with the same background (mean age 73.3 years) produced approximately 17.8 (SD = 4.2) animal names, switched 8.5 (SD = 2.3) times between the subcategories, and formed clusters of the same size as the younger participants (see 5.1). The way AD patients perform on the semantic fluency task seems to differ from the way normal participants execute the task. AD patients were reported to produce less and smaller clusters than normal control subjects (Rosen 1980; Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; 1998; Binetti et al. 1995, Carew et al. 1997, Troyer et al. 1998b; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000). In the supermarket fluency task, Martin and Fedio (1983), Ober et al. (1986), Tröster et al. (1989), and 68 Semantic fluency Beatty et al. (2000) found that those AD patients who produced the smallest number of items also generated the fewest words per subcategory. Tröster et al. (1989; see Tröster et al. 1998) indicated that both mild AD and moderate AD subjects clustered less words in subcategories than normal control subjects, but, as expected, the performance of mild AD patients was less severely defected than that of moderate AD patients. Ober et al. (1986) reported similar findings on the supermarket fluency task between normal control subject group, the group of mild AD patients, and the group of moderate-to-severe AD patients. In the animal fluency task, Binetti et al. (1995) discovered that among Italianspeaking subjects, the total number of clusters was significantly decreased in the group of moderately-to-severely demented AD patients, compared with the normal control subjects and patients with mild AD. In their study, 86% of the normal controls, 58% of the mild AD group, and 24% of the severe AD group used the strategy of clustering together semantically closely related words when executing the task. However, all the subject groups formed clusters of equal size of approximately four words. Contrasting findings have been reported by Troyer, Moscovitch. Winocur, Leach et al. (1998), Tröster et al. (1998), and Beatty et al. (1997, 2000). They observed that the cluster size differed significantly between the group of AD patients (between one and two words) and normal control subjects (between two and three words). In the supermarket fluency task, Beatty et al. (2000) reported that AD patients switched less often between subcategories than normal control subjects. The findings obtained also from the animal fluency task indicated that AD patients switched less often between animal subcategories than normal control subjects (Tröster 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Beatty 1997, 2000). For example, Troyer et al. reported 5.1 (SD = 2.9) switches per 8.3 (SD = 4.2) animal labels in their group of mild AD patients, whereas healthy control subjects switched 8.3 (SD = 2.4) times per 17.9 (SD = 4.2) words. Respectively, Tröster et al. reported 3.8 (SD = 2.9) switches per 7.6 (4.6) correct words in their AD group, and 7.6 (SD = 2.6) switches per 17.8 (SD = 4.1) correct words in the group of normal control subjects. 5.3 Activation of different associations and semantic dimensions In addition to analyzing the productivity in the semantic fluency task, that is, the subjects’ ability to systematically generate items in semantic subcategories in the test category, the content of the responses can also be analyzed. The semantic representation of words can be operationalized according to the type and the degree to which words are organized in the semantic space (Robert & Le Dorze 1994, 1997; Binetti et al. 1995; Troyer et al. 1997). The number of subcategory labels may be used to measure the extent to which subjects organize their responses around subgroups such as types of wild animals or farm animals (Laine 1989:24-25; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994, 1997; Binetti et al. 1995). The size of clusters and the percentage of words in clusters are measures of the ability to activate words in the semantic sub- Semantic fluency 69 categories and the strength of associative links in the semantic-lexical network (Roberts & Le Dorze 1994, 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Rich et al. 1999). Associations between words have been defined by various criteria in different studies. Words belonging to the same semantic subgroup sharing physical features (e.g., ‘tiger’ – ‘lion’), functional characteristics (e.g., ‘bus’ – ‘train’), or items that are frequently associated with each other by thematic relations (e.g., ‘cheese’ – ‘crackers’) may form an association (Laine 1989:22; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Binetti et al. 1995; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; see 3.2). Sometimes words are generated by their phonological similarity (e.g., ‘pantteri’, ‘panda’, ‘puuma’/ ‘panther’, ‘panda’, ‘puma’ in Laine 1989:22 or ‘pomme’, ‘poir’ / ‘apple’, ‘pear’ in Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; see also Rosen 1980; Troyer et al. 1997; Kaleva & Vanhala 2001; see 4.1, 4.2) or in alphabetical order (Tröster et al. 1989). The criteria for the number of words that form a plausible association rather than a random combination of words vary among the studies. In some studies, a string of two successively generated words sharing semantic similarity was considered a minimum for a cluster of words (Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch. Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000; Tröster et al. 1998; Rich et al. 1999), whereas in some other studies, the cluster needed to consist of at least three subsequent words (Laine 1989:22-23; Binetti et al. 1995; Roberts & Le Dorze 1997; Holappa & Nauha 2000; Kaleva & Vanhala 2001). Research indicates that normal controls tend to generate clusters of words from more subcategories than AD patients (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Binetti et al. 1995; Beatty et al. 2000). Tröster et al. (1989; see also Beatty et al. 2000) observed that normal control subjects produced significantly more subcategories of supermarket items (e.g., fruit, vegetables, meats, fish, bread, dairy products, household items) than AD patients, but that there was no difference in the number of categories between mild AD and moderate AD patients. In the study of Binetti et al. (1995), normal control subjects showed a great variability in the types of clusters and produced several different subcategories in the animal fluency task. AD patients were more likely to produce clusters that mainly involved farm animals, whereas other types of clusters were produced only to a very limited extent. Rosen’s (1980) study on the animal fluency task gives support to the findings that subcategory entrance may be already impaired in mild AD, while retrieval of prototypes is affected little or not at all. Furthermore, retrieval of prototypes was likely to be impaired and entrance to the subcategories was hardly possible for moderately-to-severely demented AD patients. Hodges et al. (1992) reported a difference in the number of words produced from different hierarchical levels between normal control subjects and AD patients. Both groups generated the greatest number of basic level words belonging to broader 70 Semantic fluency superordinate categories (animals and household items), and both used fewer items from the subcategory level of the hierarchy (types of boats and breeds of dogs; see 3.1.2). However, the AD patients retrieved only half of the words produced by the normal control group for the superordinate categories and only a third of the words for the subcategories. In the studies of Martin and Fedio (1983) and Tröster et al. (1989), it was found that AD patients tended to give superordinates rather than names of particular items of the subcategories. An overrepresentation of superordinates characterized the performance of mild and moderate AD patients (Tröster et al. 1989; see also Beatty et al. 2000), whereas the names of the categories were rarely used in the group of normal control subjects (Martin & Fedio 1983). In the study of Chan et al. (1993), cognitive maps were created from a pool of words produced in a semantic fluency task, in which the semantic distance among animal names was measured. For example, animals with high conjunction frequency (e.g., ‘cat’ and ‘dog’, ‘lion’ and ‘tiger’, ‘zebra’ and ‘giraffe’) were considered closely linked, whereas animals that had less in common (e.g., ‘bear’ and ‘cow’, ‘cat’ and ‘sheep’) were far apart on the map. The cognitive maps turned out dissimilar between normal control subjects and AD patients. New but abnormal associations between words were formed. For instance, AD patients’ dimensions of domesticity and size of animals contained anomalies (e.g., ‘bear’ was considered a domestic animal, and small animals were considered big, and vice versa). Further, associations between closely related items (e.g., ‘cat’ and ‘dog’) were weaker. The authors concluded that the general semantic structure appeared to be breaking down in AD, although scattered intact clusters were still observed (see chap. 6.). The results reported by Chan et al. implied a significant qualitative deterioration in the relative saliency of semantic features and in the semantic space in the category of animals. Carew et al. (1997) also reported findings on the change of strength of fine-grained semantic associations between responses in the animal fluency task. In their study, AD patients did not produce contiguous responses that consisted of very specific, distinctive features. Instead of producing words sharing a very close perceptual relationship, they tended to produce words that shared more general and thematic features, such as living environment. 5.4 Error analysis The qualitative analysis of the semantic fluency performance calls for a closer look at the correct responses, as well as the errors that are produced during the task (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Butters et al. 1987; Tröster et al. 1989; Crowe 1992; Bayles et al. 1993; Troyer et al. 1997; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000). The purpose of the error analysis is to find patterns that can hint about the possible factors interfering the successful task performance (Joanette, Goulét & Le Dorze 1988). Participants that are less productive at the semantic fluency task are prone to both make errors and produce fewer correct words (Roberts & Le Dorze 1994). The most frequent errors encountered in the task are intrusions (an inappropriate new response Semantic fluency 71 outside the semantic category boundaries; e.g., ‘chair’ for vegetables) and perseverations (recurrence of a previous response, e.g., ‘cat’, ‘dog’, ‘cat’). Also phonemic errors (responses that begin with the same sound), as well as generic errors (response covering a very general category of response, e.g., ‘farm animals’) may occur during the task but to a lesser extent than intrusions and perseverations (Ober et al. 1986; Butters et al. 1987; Bayles et al. 1993). The performances of both normal control subjects and AD patients contain these errors (e.g., Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Rosser & Hodges 1994). The erroneous responses of AD patients in the semantic fluency task have been analysed in a number of studies. However, they appear to paint a somewhat contradictory picture. For example, Ober et al. (1986) noticed that the performance of normal control subjects, relative to mild and moderate-to-severe AD patients, was characterized by a higher proportion of correct responses and a very low proportion of different types of errors in the fluency task employing animals and fruit. The performance of the AD patients was characterized by significantly fewer correct responses and more intrusions, perseverations, and variants of the category items (e.g., ‘kitten’ after having said ‘cat’). On the other hand, the findings of Carew et al. (1997) on the animal fluency task showed no differences in the number of perseverations, intrusions, non-specific errors, or phonemic errors between the normal control group and the AD patients. The number of errors made by individual participants remained low in their study. In the same vein, Binetti et al. (1995; see also Fischer et al. 1988) found that in the animal fluency task, the control subjects and the mild AD patients showed no phonemic errors, intrusions, or perseverative errors, and only a few instances of these error types were observed in the moderate-to-severe AD group. Binetti et al. proposed that the type and number of errors in the task were not very informative variables in distinguishing the performance of controls from AD patients, especially in the case of patients with mild dementia. 5.4.1 Intrusions Intrusions, inappropriate items violating the category boundaries (e.g., ‘shoe’ for furniture), seem to be very rare in the semantic fluency production of normal adults. Suhr and Jones (1998) found that intrusions were non-existent among younger healthy adults (mean age 44.1, SD = 10.1) and that their proportion was very low (0.9%, SD = 1.9) among the errors made by older normal adults (mean age 67.9, SD = 12.9). Similar findings have been reported by Diesfeldt (1985), Ober et al. (1986), Tröster et al. (1989), and Binetti et al. (1995). However, the longitudinal study conducted by Beatty et al. (2000) indicated that an increase in the proportion of intrusions took place along with advancing dementia in some AD patients. Findings concerning the occurrence of intrusions among the AD patients are somewhat mixed. Nevertheless, most of the studies seem to report a relatively low number of intrusions compared to other errors among the AD patients. 72 Semantic fluency In the fluency task naming vegetables and tools, Mickanin et al. (1994) found that intrusions were more common among AD patients than among normal control subjects. In their study, 9 of 22 AD patients, relative to 1 of 22 normal control subjects, made violations in their semantic fluency performance in 10% or more of their total responses. However, most of the category violations were semantically related to the target category and never violated the living/non-living nature of the target category. In the supermarket fluency task, Tröster et al. (1989; see also Beatty et al. 2000) discovered that the proportion of intrusions (i.e., items unobtainable in a supermarket) was significantly higher in the moderate AD group (2.0%, SD = 5.0) than in the group of older normal control subjects, who were free of category violations. Contradictory findings were reported by Binetti et al. (1995) who found that intrusions were absent among the Italian-speaking mild AD patients in the animal fluency task, and that even moderate-to-severe AD patients produced only very few intrusions. Similar results has been reported by Ober et al. (1986) who noticed that only one of nine moderate-to-severe AD subjects gave two non-category responses in the semantic fluency task employing supermarket items. Among the studies conducted with English-speaking subjects, Rosser and Hodges (1994) observed that in the category fluency task with animals, birds and water creatures, household objects, vehicles, and musical instruments, the proportion of intrusions was not significantly different between a group of control subjects (0.7%) and a group of AD patients (2.2%). In a similar vein, Suhr and Jones (1998) found no differences in the proportion of intrusions between groups of normal control subjects (0.9%, SD = 1.9) and patients with AD (0.3%, SD = 1.2), when categories of animals, fruit, vegetables, tools, and kitchen utensils were used. Diesfeldt (1985) also reported on the rarity of intrusions among Dutch-speaking AD patients. Intrusions produced in the category of clothing consisted of accessories with articles of clothing, such as ‘ornaments’ or ‘lady’s bag’. For the category of fruit, sometimes vegetables were recalled, such as ‘potatoes’ or ‘carrots’. The proportion of intrusions for clothing in the group of normal control subjects was 1.8%, the proportion being 1.9% in the AD group. For fruit, the proportion of intrusions was 4.4% in the normal control group and 3.3% in the AD group. Nevertheless, in spite of the emergence of intrusions in both subject groups, the subjects seemed to adhere remarkably well to the search set and displayed a preservation of the knowledge concerning superordinate categories and their boundaries even in more advanced cases of AD. Diesfeldt’s findings thus implied that at least in the categories of clothes and fruit, production of intrusions may to a certain extent be a normal pattern among normal elderly control subjects, possibly indicating some fuzziness of the category boundaries (see 9.2.6, 10.1.2, 10.1.3). Semantic fluency 73 5.4.2 Perseverations The number of perseverations (i.e., any continuation or recurrence of an earlier response) produced among healthy elderly adults in the animal fluency task has been reported to be non-existent (Binetti et al. 1995) or very low (Carew et al. 1997). The study of Ramage, Bayles, Helm-Estabrooks, and Cruz (1999) indicated that although perseverations took place in 21 of their 60 normal healthy subjects (aged 20-35 and 60-75 years), a combined score of the semantic and phonemic fluency performance yielded an average rate of perseverations as low as 1% of all the responses. Their study did not show age or gender effects or their interaction on the frequency of perseverations. The rate of perseverations reported in Gruenewald and Lockhead’s (1980) study with young university students was about the same, 1.6% (range 0 – 7%) of the responses on words belonging to animals, birds, foods, or cold foods. Similar findings were published by Suhr and Jones (1998; see also Butters et al. 1987; Bayles et al. 1993) who reported the rate of perseverations as being 1.1% (SD = 1.5) for the younger healthy adults (mean age 44.1 years, SD = 10.4) and 1.4% (SD = 2.4) for the older healthy adults (mean age 67.9 years, SD = 12.9) when producing animals, fruit/vegetables, and tools/kitchen utensils. However, findings that advancing age may increase the frequency of perseverations have been reported. Tröster et al. (1989) observed that elderly adults (mean age 70.4 years, SD = 6.2) perseverated significantly more often (3.0% of the responses, SD = 4.0) than middle-aged adults (1.0% of the responses, SD = 2.0; mean age 50.8 years, SD = 8.6) in the semantic fluency task with supermarket items. Perseverations in the fluency tasks among normal elderly people thus seem to be rare but they may occur more often with increasing age. On the other hand, Gruenewald and Lockhead (1980) considered perseverations as a normal phenomenon among the healthy adults in the fluency task and explained their occurrence by the activation of overlapping semantic fields that causes the same words to activate in different semantic contexts (see also Barsalou 1982, 1983; Persson 1995:30, 42). Perseverations are a recognized sign of AD patients’ semantic fluency performance (Bayles et al. 1993; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000) and other task performances requiring lexical-semantic processing (e.g., Bayles, Tomoeda, Kaszniak, Stern & Eagans 1985; Hodges et al. 1992; Lamar et al. 1997). However, studies on perseveration in the semantic fluency task seem to give mixed findings about their frequency of occurrence among patients with AD, as well as among healthy control subjects. Rosser and Hodges (1994; see also Hodges et al. 1992) reported that the rate of perseverations in AD patients (7.7%) across living (animals, birds, water creatures) and non-living categories (household objects, vehicles, musical instruments) was significantly higher than that of normal control subjects (1.4%). In a similar vein, Suhr and Jones (1998) found that AD patients made a significantly higher percentage (6.3%, SD= 9.1) of perseverative errors than healthy controls (1.4%, SD = 2.4) when naming animals, fruit/vegetables, and tools/kitchen utensils. Tröster et al. (1989; see also Beatty et al. 2000) noticed that both mild AD patients 74 Semantic fluency (9.0%, SD=12.0) and moderate AD patients (7.0%, SD = 10.0) tended to perseverate more than the older healthy control subjects (3.0%, SD = 0.04) in the supermarket task. Moreover, Ober et al. (1986) found a similar pattern in the fluency task tapping animal and fruit naming. However, perseverations have not been reported in all studies. Butters et al. (1987) did not observe higher rates of perseverations in AD patients than in normal control subjects in the animal fluency task. Also Carew et al. (1997) study indicated that mild AD patients (MMSE = 22.2, SD = 3.0) made very few perseverations when generating words for the category of animals. With regard to the effects of dementia severity on the rate of perseverations, the results seem to be somewhat contradictory. Ober et al. (1986) found no difference in the proportion of perseverations between mild and moderate-to-severe AD patients when they produced words belonging to animals and fruit. Similar findings were reported in the study of Binetti et al. (1995) who observed that mild AD patients (MMSE = 21.4, SD = 2.7) showed no perseverative behavior at all and that even moderate-to-severe AD patients (MMSE = 12.3, SD = 2.6) produced only a few perseverative responses when generating animal names. On the other hand, Ober et al. (1986) noticed that mild AD patients tended to perseverate significantly less often than moderate-to-severe AD patients in the semantic fluency task with supermarket items. Using the same task, however, Tröster et al. (1989) found that the ratio of perseverations to the total output was the same between patients with mild AD (9.0%, SD = 12.0) and moderate AD patients (7%, SD = 10.0). In comparison to other patient groups with dementia (e.g., HD and PD), several studies indicated that AD patients produced significantly more perseverations over several semantic categories (Bayles et al. 1993; Hodges et al. 1992; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Suhr & Jones 1998). 5.5 Performance in different semantic categories The number of semantic categories used in the semantic fluency tasks varies among studies from one to several different categories (see Table 4). In most cases, however, the category of animals has been the category of choice. In the study of Chertkow and Bub (1990), a wide variation appeared in the exemplars generated for eight different semantic categories in the group of normal control subjects. Their average performance varied between 12 and 21 items per category (mean 14.9 items per category), while AD patients’ average production across the categories varied between 3 and 9 words (mean 5.5 items per category). The nature of the semantic categories seems to have an effect on the semantic fluency performance of the normal control subjects and AD patients (Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Bayles et al. 1989, 1993; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Capitani et al. 1999). Different semantic categories may have distinctive representations in the cognitive system and in the brain (Caramazza and Shelton 1998; Moss et al. 2002; see 9.2.6, 10.1.1). Some studies showed that normal control subjects produced significantly more words for the living than the man-made categories (Rosser & Hodges Semantic fluency 75 1994; Binetti et al. 1999). For example, Binetti et al. indicated that healthy control subjects generated more words for the category of animals than for the category of tools. Nevertheless, also in the domain of living things differences were found by Bayles et al. (1989) who observed that healthy elderly participants found the category of animals easier than the category of vegetables or fruit. There are some reports on the dissociation between knowledge of animate and inanimate things in the semantic fluency task also among AD patients, but these findings are somewhat inconcistent (cf. Chertkow & Bub 1990; Gainotti et al. 1996; Moss et al. 2002). However, Cronin-Golomb et al. (1992) and Rosser and Hodges (1994) indicated that AD group generated fewer responses in the living categories than in the manmade categories, whereas the opposite was true for normal control subjects (see Table 4). The differences in the performances between the semantic categories described above may also be explained by some categories being more familiar to the speakers than others. People often tend to have a very subjective and imperfect knowledge of categories and the category boundaries which may vary with the number of factors, such as specialized knowledge and life experience (Aitchison 1994:39-50; Taylor 1994:72-75, 79, 242; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:14-20; Azuma et al. 1997; Roberts & Le Dorze 1997). Furthermore, different categories tend to include a varying number of members, some categories having more exemplars than others (Diesfeldt 1985; Bayles et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Azuma et al. 1997; Crowe 1998; Binetti et al. 1999). Diesfeldt (1985) and Crowe’s (1998) experiments showed that the larger and more general a semantic category (e.g., animals or articles of clothing), the more items were generated for the task, and the smaller and rarer a category (e.g., fruit and precious stones), the fewer items were produced. Nevertheless, Crowe found a similar pattern in the production of large and small categories in terms of a significant decrease in both the number and the frequency of the words as a function of time. Cronin-Golomb et al. (1992) noted that the pattern of performance across several semantic categories was similar between healthy control subjects and AD patients in the sense that those categories that elicited the fewest items (e.g., birds, furniture, vehicles, and insects) and the most items (e.g., parts of human body, clothes, animals, and vegetables) were the same for both subject groups. The study of Chertkow and Bub (1990) seems to confirm this finding (see Table 4). 76 Semantic fluency 6 Causes of the semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease The nature and the cause of the impairment in the semantic system in AD have been widely discussed and several different interpretations have been proposed to explain the cause of the deficit (for a review, see Nebes 1989, 1992; see also Table 4). There seem to be three main accounts for the cause of the semantic impairment. First, a breakdown or a loss of information in the representations in the semantic system may underlie the deficit (a storage deficit). Second, the deficit may be due to a failure in the procedures called upon to retrieve and exploit the relatively well preserved semantic representations (an access deficit). Third, there are some researchers who account for a multifactorial deficit underlying the impaired semantic processing, in which both the degradation of semantic structure and the impaired retrieval of information contribute to the impairment. The storage versus access deficit has been hotly debated, not only in studies concerning AD, but also other neurological disorders (e.g., aphasia), and it is an ongoing controversy in the literature (e.g., Shallice 1988:279-286; Caramazza et al. 1990; Chertkow, Bub & Caplan 1992; Rapp & Caramazza 1993; Carew 1997; Crowe 1998; Hagoort 1998; see also Persson 1995:67-69). 6.1 Breakdown and loss of semantic structures As far as the semantic fluency task is concerned, certain findings have been interpreted to possibly reflect a significant breakdown in the integrity of the structure of the semantic memory or a loss of semantic information taking place in AD. For example, smaller cluster size (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur & Leach 1998), abnormal clusters, aberrant semantic distance between the produced words (Chan et al. 1993; Carew et al. 1997), decrease in switching between subcategories (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur & Leach 1998), and smaller number of subcategories (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Binetti et al. 1995) have been considered signs of the semantic breakdown. Semantic category violations (e.g., Ober et al. 1986; Mickanin et al. 1994), decreased use of 78 Causes of semantic impairment words at the basic and subordinate levels, and increased use of superordinate category labels (Martin & Fedio 1983; Tröster et al. 1989; Hodges et al. 1992; Carew et al. 1997; Beatty et al. 2000) were also interpreted as signs of semantic degradation. Findings that retrieval cues did not aid word retrieval during a semantic fluency task (Hodges et al. 1992; Randolph et al. 1993) and that both verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks brought about an impaired performance among AD patients (Mickanin et al. 1994) have also been interpreted along the same lines. Furthermore, support for the breakdown hypothesis was provided by studies in which AD patients were found to have greater difficulty in semantic than letter fluency tasks (Hodges et al. 1990; Monsch et al. 1992, 1994, 1997; Weingartner et al. 1993; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Mickanin et al. 1994; Goldstein et al. 1996; Crossley et al. 1997). Breakdown of the semantic memory may occur early in the course of the disease (Rosser & Hodges 1994; Monsch et al. 1997), and it is likely to be systematic and progressive in nature (Hodges et al. 1990, 1992). It may involve a bottom-up deterioration of the hierarchical structure of semantic knowledge, leading to an appearance of the loss of items at the basic level and subordinate level categories (e.g., type of boats and breed of dogs) and causing difficulty in any kind of semantic functioning, such as naming. Meanwhile, the superordinate items (e.g., household items and animals) may remain intact and their labels are available for naming (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1990; 1992; Tippett et al. 1995). More specifically, the disruption may be caused by a loss or a degradation of the weights and connections of the very specific defining features (e.g., physical and functional) by which the meaning of the words at the lower hierarchical levels are determined, identified, and distinguished from semantically related items. Semantic information at the superordinate levels is likely to be better preserved because it is general and more sparse, shared by all or most members of the category, frequently used, and learned early (Warrington 1975; Martin & Fedio 1983; Grober et al. 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Chan et al. 1993; Monsch et al. 1992, 1994; Binetti et al. 1995; Tippett et al. 1995; Goldstein et al. 1996; Carew et al. 1997; Harley 1998; Moss et al. 2002; see also 3.1.2, 3.2). The loss or disruption of semantic features may lead to a loss of some subcategories, the deactivation of which can be seen as an unsuccessful performance in the word generation task in the form of limited number of different types of clusters, such as production of solely farm animals in the animal fluency task (Binetti et al. 1995; see also Allen et al. 1993) or difficulty in switching between the subcategories (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). The breakdown theory has been supported by various types of findings on other lexical-semantic tasks (see 2.4). One of the most remarkable findings in favor of the breakdown hypothesis originates from studies in which item-to-item correspondence of errors or correlations between performances over various tasks and different modalities have been found (Huff et al. 1986; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1992; Mickanin et al. 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997). Furthermore, correlation between the poor confrontation naming and the poor Causes of semantic impairment 79 semantic fluency performances (Martin & Fedio 1983; Huff et al. 1986; Flicker et al. 1987; Diesfeldt 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1992; Randolph et al. 1993), unhelpful semantic cueing (Chertkow & Bub 1990), and semantic naming errors produced in both noun and verb confrontation naming tasks (Martin & Fedio 1983; Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Bayles et al. 1990; Miller Sommers & Pierce 1990; Hodges et al. 1991; Robinson et al. 1996; Astell & Harley 1998; Williamson et al. 1998) have been labeled as symptoms of the semantic breakdown. Confusion in choosing the right target among the semantic foils in word-picture matching tasks (Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Hodges et al. 1992) and impaired sorting of pictures into semantic categories (Hodges et al. 1992) have also been interpreted as semantic degradation. More severe difficulty in naming animate than inanimate items has been held as clear evidence of the semantic deficit (Gainotti et al. 1996). Moreover, the findings that AD patients were impaired in ranking semantic features, generated inappropriate features when defining words, and showed impaired determination of relationships between semantic features, has been interpreted as supporing the hypothesis of the semantic memory breakdown in AD (Grober et al. 1985; Abeysinghe et al. 1990; Hodges et al. 1992, 1996; Grossman, D’Esposito et al. 1996; Laatu et al. 1997; Laatu 1999; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997). 6.2 Impaired processing The semantic fluency task is considered to be a rather complex task requiring a directed search of the semantic memory. Clustering and switching are considered necessary for the systematic search through the semantic categories and for the retrieval of words from various subcategories (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; see 5.1, 5.2). Therefore, reduction in the total output (Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Tröster et al. 1998), smaller cluster size, and reduced switching (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Tröster et al. 1998) have also been interpreted as signs of an impaired search and processing of semantic information. Also the finding that the same semantic categories elicited the most vs. the fewest words in both the control subjects and the AD patients led some researchers to hold that the organization of the semantic categories was intact in AD (Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992). Deficient search processes rather than breakdown or loss of information in the semantic memory were considered a more likely cause of the reduced semantic fluency performance in AD. However, because AD patients tended to perform the phonemic fluency task significantly better than the semantic fluency task, the view that deteriorated search processes alone would explain the impaired semantic fluency performance might not quite hold (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). Support for the account that reduced processing of semantic information was responsible for the impaired semantic performance was provided by data obtained from other tasks measuring semantic memory functions. For example, AD patients were observed to be aware of the category membership of the tested nouns (Smith, 80 Causes of semantic impairment Murdoch et al. 1989; Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992), and to be able to recognize both nouns and verbs in a word-picture matching task (Diesfeldt 1985; White-Devine et al. 1996). In fact, AD patients were observed to recognize pictures referring to nouns that they were not able to name in the confrontation naming task (Smith, Murdoch et al. 1989). As far as nouns are concerned, evidence was given to show that all or at least part of their semantic feature knowledge was retained but that it was not available for appropriate use in AD (Grober et al. 1985; Bayles et al. 1990). Furthermore, normal ranking of semantic features and category exemplars with different degrees of typicality was shown by AD patients (Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992; Johnson et al. 1995). Moreover, a normal pattern of responding in some of the priming tasks was interpreted as a sign of a normally functioning semantic memory system in AD (Nebes et al. 1984, 1989; Nebes & Halligan 1996; Albert & Milberg 1989). The difficulty in picture-based word retrieval may at least partly be caused by a deficit in visual processing (Rochford 1971; Appell et al. 1982; Martin and Fedio 1983; Kirshner et al. 1984; Flicker et al. 1987; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Nicholas et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 1996; cf. Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; Huff et al. 1986; Goldstein et al. 1992; Silveri & Leggio 1996; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; Astell & Harley 1998; see the discussion in Harley 1998). The errors in naming may also be a consequence of a deficit at the levels of spoken word production (see chap. 4; 10.1.3). It was suggested that an impaired spread of activation among semantically related items (Diesfeldt 1985; Nebes 1989) and an inability to use the semantic features that distinguish related items from one another (Diesfeldt 1985) may lead to difficulty in selecting amongst possible candidates (Bowles et al. 1987). Difficulty in the interactive connections between the semantic and the lexical level of information during word production, difficulty in lemma retrieval, and an impaired access to the phonological level of word production may also affect naming (Kirshner et al. 1984; Huff 1988; Chertkow & Bub 1992; White-Devine et al. 1995; Nicholas et al. 1996; Astell & Harley 1996, 1998; Harley 1998; see also Smith, Murdoch et al. 1989). Evidence supporting the retrieval deficit arises also from studies indicating that AD patients had more difficulty with low-frequency words than high-frequency words (Kirshner et al. 1984; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Miller Sommers & Pierce 1990; see 2.4.2) and that the naming performance of the AD patients was facilitated by phonological cueing (Martin & Fedio 1983). 6.3 A multifactorial deficit Several researchers seem to hold that a combination of the storage and access deficit may underlie the semantic disorder in AD. In other words, the degradation of the semantic memory and the impaired lexical-semantic retrieval may account for the semantic difficulties found in AD (Schwartz et al. 1979; Martin & Fedio 1983; Kirshner et al. 1984; Martin et al. 1985; Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Huff, Mack, Mahlmann & Greenberg 1988; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Monsch et al. 1994; Mickanin et al. 1994; Laatu et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1995, 1996; Troyer, Causes of semantic impairment 81 Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). Troyer and her colleagues assumed that the smaller cluster size of the AD patients was caused by their disability to identify semantic subcategories, and that the decreased switching reflected either the semantic impairment or the deficient search processes in the semantic memory (see also Binetti et al. 1995). After all, as Auriacombe et al. (1993:188) speculated, “…it is difficult to assert with confidence whether there is difficulty with lexical retrieval during spontaneous category naming, however, because the characteristics of the target mental domain that underlies the retrieval cannot be specified in detail”. In addition to the possible causes mentioned above, several other complex processes may be responsible for the reduction of word fluency in AD (Hodges et al. 1992; Pasquier et al. 1995). Findings reported in the literature, such as executive and attentional problems (Diesfeldt 1985; Mickanin et al. 1994; Ober et al. 1986; Crossley et al. 1997; Ruff et al. 1997; Rende et al. 2002), disability to generate imaginary representations of a search set (e.g., wardrobe or fruit desk; Diesfeldt 1985), as well as reduced processing speed, seem to refer to a deficit in the working memory system which was found to be present in AD (Baddeley et al. 1986, 1991; Morris 1994; Bayles 2003; see also Diesfeldt 1985; Kopelman 1994; Mickanin et al. 1994; Pasquier et al.1995; Ruff et al. 1997). Furthermore, limited general processing capacity (Huff et al. 1986; Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992; Crossley et al. 1997), lack of motivation (Diesfeldt 1985), and the difficulty of the task (Nebes et al. 1984; Nebes 1989; Cronin-Golomb et al. 1992; Hodges & Patterson 1995) may also decrease the output of the AD patients in the semantic fluency task. 82 Causes of semantic impairment 7 Aims of the study Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by an impairment of multiple memory-related systems, including deterioration of the language-specific semantic memory. The semantic (category) fluency task has been used extensively as a method of studying the semantic memory impairment in AD (e.g., Monsch et al. 1992; Binetti et al. 1995; Goldstein et al. 1996; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). The wide clinical use of the task is most probably based on its sensitivity to deficits and its effectiveness in revealing information about a subject’s semantic processing and word retrieval in a short time. In this task, participants are asked to generate words belonging to different semantic categories, such as animals and supermarket items, in a certain period of time (usually 60 seconds). According to previous studies, AD patients do not perform the task in the way normal elderly subjects do. Their overall production of words is lower and they seem unable to use an effective strategy to generate semantically related words as clusters in the subcategories (e.g., farm animals: ‘cow’, ‘horse’, ‘sheep’, ‘pig’), or to switch among subcategories (e.g., from farm animals to wild animals; Beatty et al. 1997, 2000; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). During the task, AD patients also tend to produce more errors than healthy control subjects, such as perseverations and outside-category intrusions (e.g., Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Mickanin et al. 1994; Beatty et al.1997, 2000). There are plenty of studies in which a semantic fluency task has been used as a method of investigating the semantic memory functions in AD patients (see Table 4). However, detailed reports on how Finnish-speaking AD patients perform the task are scarce. In general, studies seem to differ in terms of operationalizing the task, some of them having used only one or two semantic categories to measure word production or having used more general and fewer parameters than others. Furthermore, in many studies, the theoretical background concerning the fundamentals for performing the task, for example, the semantic organization of words and the processes responsible for word production seems to be somewhat vaguely discussed. So far, very little is known about AD patients’ ability to generate verbs in a fluency task in which different kinds of verb categories are used as semantic stimuli. 84 Aims of the study The general aim of this dissertation is to provide information on the patterns of how mildly and moderately demented Finnish-speaking AD patients utilize information in semantic memory in order to perform on the semantic fluency task in which noun (object) and verb (action) categories are used as constraints for spontaneous word production. The objective of the study is to compare the performances of mild and moderate AD patients with each other and with the performance of the healthy elderly adults. The specific research questions are: 1. How does the overall performance of mildly and moderately demented AD patients compare to that of normal control subjects on the semantic fluency task in which different noun categories are used to elicit word production? Can the performance of the AD patients be characterized by a reduction in noun production and an impaired ability to use a strategic search for semantically related nouns (i.e., to sample nouns in clusters and to switch between subcategories) as stated in earlier studies? Are the performances of mildly and moderately demented AD patients different? 2. How does the content of the performance of mildly and moderately demented AD patients compare to normal control subjects on the noun fluency task? Can the performance of the AD patients be characterized by a tendency to violate the boundaries of semantic categories (i.e., to produce intrusions) and to repeat (i.e., to perseverate) previously produced nouns? Do mildly and moderately demented AD patients show an inability to generate nouns from different semantic subcategories and a tendency to use more frequent and prototypical nouns, as indicated in previous studies? Is there a difference between the performance of mildly and moderately demented AD patients? 3. How does the overall performance of mildly and moderately demented AD patients compare to that of normal control subjects on the semantic fluency task in which different verb categories are used to elicit word production? Can the performance of the AD patients be characterized by a reduction in verb production and an impaired ability to use a strategic search for semantically related verbs (i.e., to sample verbs in clusters and to switch between subcategories), as was previously found in their semantic fluency performance in noun categories? Are the performances of mildly and moderately demented AD patients different? 4. How does the content of the performance of mildly and moderately demented AD patients compare to normal control subjects on the verb fluency task? Can the performance of the AD patients be characterized by a tendency to violate the boundaries of semantic categories (i.e., to produce intrusions) and to repeat (i.e., to perseverate) previously produced verbs? Do mildly and moderately demented AD patients show an inability to generate verbs from different semantic subcategories and a tendency to use more frequent and prototypical verbs? Does the content of the performance on the verb fluency task differ between mildly and moderately demented AD patients? 8 Method 8.1 Subjects The subjects consisted of 20 mildly and 20 moderately demented AD patients and 30 healthy elderly control subjects (see Table 5). The AD patients came from the Department of Neurology (Memory Clinic) of the Helsinki University Central Hospital. The diagnosis of the probable AD was confirmed by neurological examination, consistent with National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disease-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorderes Association criteria (NINCDADRDA criteria; McKhann et al. 1984). The dementia severity was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al. 1975). The Alzheimer’s patients scoring 20-27 points of the total 30 points on the MMSE formed the group of mildly demented subjects (miAD), and the patients scoring 12-19 points formed the group of moderately demented subjects (moAD). The miAD group consisted of 12 female and 8 male participants whose mean age was 65.0 years and their mean MMSE score was 23.5 points (see Table 5). The moAD group included 15 female and 5 male subjects. Their mean age was 67.4 years and the mean MMSE score was 15.9 points. The miAD group had attended school for approximately 10.5 years and the moAD group for 10.1 years. The normal control subjects, matched for age and educational level to the AD subjects, were 16 male and 14 female volunteers from the pool of participants of the Helsinki Aging Brain Study which was started in 1989 with the aim of studying the neurological and cognitive status of a random sample of persons aged 55, 60, 65, 70, 75, and 80 years of age living in the city of Helsinki (see Ylikoski 2000). All the control subjects had previously undergone a complete neurological and neuropsychological examination. They did not have neurological or psychiatric diseases and they did not suffer from alcohol or drug abuse or use medication that could have affected their cognitive performance. The mean age of the control subjects was 66.7 years and they had attended school for approximately 9.7 years. The average score of the control subjects on the MMSE was 28.9 points (see Table 5). 86 Method There was no difference in the age, gender, or educational level among the subject groups (see Table 5). The MMSE score of the NC group was significantly higher than that of the miAD group (U = 0.5, p < .001) and the moAD group (U = 0.0, p < .001). The MMSE scores differed significantly also between the miAD group and the moAD group (U = 0.0, p < .001; for a more detailed description of the statistical tests, see 8.2.7). Examination of the AD patients began in 1994, first by conducting a pilot study. The data collection was completed in 1997. The AD patients were tested either at the Helsinki University Central Hospital or at their homes or care units, depending on their transportation facilities. All the control subjects were tested at the Helsinki University Central Hospital. All subjects were examined individually in as quiet and undisturbed a setting as possible. All the participants or their caregivers gave their informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Helsinki University Central Hospital. 8.2 Method 8.2.1 Procedure of the semantic fluency tasks For the semantic fluency task, all subjects were given 60 seconds to generate as many words as possible belonging to a given semantic category. Eight different semantic categories were given in the following order: four noun categories (i.e., articles of clothing, vegetables, vehicles, animals) and four verb categories (i.e., preparing food, playing sports, construction, cleaning up). The reason these categories were chosen was to cover concrete, everyday categories of objects and actions. The nominal categories represented both animate and inanimate objects categories commonly used in other studies on semantic fluency performance (see Table 4). In order to avoid confusions and misunderstandings, the subjects practiced performing the task and produced examples for the categories of dishes (kitchen utensils) and gardening. During the practice, subjects were encouraged by the example of the examiner to produce the nominative singular forms for nouns (e.g., ‘kuppi’ / ’cup’, ’lautanen’ / ’plate’, ’kulho’ / ’bowl’, etc.) and the first infinitive form for the verbs (e.g., ‘kaivaa’ / ’dig’, ‘leikata’ / ’cut’, ‘istuttaa’ / ‘plant’, ‘kitkeä’ / ‘weed’, etc.), but no restrictions for the performance were given. The subjects were given the following instructions for performing the noun fluency tasks: “Please name as many nouns, that is, names of objects that belong to the category of XX (the name of the semantic category) as possible in one minute. You can start now.” For the verb production tasks, the instruction was as follows: “Please name as many verbs, that is, names of actions that belong to the category of YY (the name of the semantic category) as possible in one minute. You can start now.” The instruction was repeated after 30 seconds to each subject if production of responses had ceased or the subject appeared distracted. The task performance was timed by a stopwatch and all products Method 87 Table 5. Demographic features of the subject groups NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) H p-value Female / Male 14 / 16 12 / 8 15 / 5 3.994 # p = .136 Age M (SD) Mdn 66.7 (5.5) 66.0 65.0 (10.3) 64.5 67.4 (8.7) 64.5 0.962 p = .618 Education, years M (SD) Mdn 9.7 (3.3) 9.0 10.5 (3.7) 9.0 10.1 (3.6) 10.0 0.380 p = .827 MMSE M (SD) Mdn 28.9 (0.9) 29.0 23.5 (2.0) 23.5 *** 15.9 (2.4) 60.893 17.0 *** ¤¤¤ p < .001 Note. H = values on the Kruskal-Wallis test, # = chi square test (see Ranta, Rita & Kouki 1991:136.143). Pair-wise comparisons computed using the Mann-Whitney U test: *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤¤¤ = p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. were written down by the examiner. The examinations were taped to guarantee a verbatim transcription of the responses. 8.2.2 Analysis of the overall performance on the semantic fluency tasks In order to measure the overall productivity and the strategies of the performance of the NC group, the miAD group, and the moAD group on the semantic fluency tasks, and to compare their performance, the following parameters were used (see Table 6; examples of the complete protocol are provided in conjunction with the results, see also Appendix 4A-H). The total number of words (i.e., the sum of nouns and verbs produced for each category) was calculated, including repetitions and outside-category intrusions. Correct words (i.e., correct, unique responses) were calculated separately. The total number of clusters (i.e., groups of two or more successively produced words belonging to the same semantic subcategory or sharing the first phoneme (nouns only; see 8.2.3)) was calculated to measure the ability to use a strategic search for words. For the clustering rules, see Table 6 and Appendix 1A and 1B. The term subcategory was used to refer to any types of semantic groupings found in each category. 88 Method Table 6. Scoring of clustering and switching on the semantic fluency tasks A sample of produced nouns Clusters and switches cat, dog, hen, cow, horse, sheep, pig, goat lion, tiger bird hare, squirrel elephant, giraffe, monkey seal CLUSTER farm animals SWITCH and CLUSTER exotic animals SWITCH SWITCH and CLUSTER rodents SWITCH and CLUSTER exotic animals SWITCH Total number of words: 17 Number of clusters: 4 Words in clusters: 15/17 (88.2%) Number of switches: 5 Mean cluster size: 1.8 Counting the cluster size: 1 word: 2 * 0 = 0 2 words: 2 * 1 = 2 3 words: 1 * 2 = 2 8 words: 1 * 7 = 7 sum: 6 11 cluster size: 11/6 = 1.8 The mean cluster size was counted to measure the ability to access words in the subcategories or phonemic clusters (for the latter, nouns only). The cluster size for each category was counted by applying the protocol presented by Troyer et al. (1997; Troyer 2000) and Rich et al. (1999; see Table 6). A single word was marked with a cluster size of 0, two words with a cluster size of 1, three words with a cluster size of 2, and so on, errors and repetitions included. First, the number of the clusters of each cluster size was multiplied by the equivalent cluster size marker. Second, the products of the multiplication were added together, and third, the sum was divided by the total number of clusters, including single words. Successively repeated words (‘jänis’, ‘jänis’, ‘jänis’ / ‘hare’, ‘hare’, ‘hare’ or ‘keittää’, ’keittää’, ‘keittää’ / ‘boil’, ‘boil’, ‘boil’) were not counted as a cluster. The proportion of words in clusters was calculated to measure the coherence and efficiency of production. The formula for calculating the proportion was ((the number of the total words produced - the number of the single words) / the number of the total words) * 100. Number of switches (i.e., transitions between clusters), including single words, was calculated to measure the ability to initiate a search for a new subcategory or the first phoneme shared by many category members (for the latter, nouns only). Errors and repetitions were included, except for successively repeated words that were scored as one transition. Method 89 8.2.3 Clustering rules The division of the clusters was based on the observation that, in addition to using the semantic criteria to produce semantically closely related words together as a cluster, subjects also seemed to produce chains of words that began with the same phoneme (see also Laine 1989:22; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994; Kaleva & Vanhala 2001). Such phonemically similar strings of words appeared as separate clusters, as well as embedded in a cluster of semantically very closely related items. In order to find out whether phonemically similar words were used as a real strategy rather than produced at random, the occurrences of such word chains were analysed in closer detail. First, the appearance of all phonemic clusters in the real data was calculated. Subsequently, the number of these phonemic clusters was compared to ones in a randomized data set, in which the order of the word occurrence was randomized with Excel for Windows 95, version 7.0. For the noun categories, an average of 4.7 (SD = 2.6, Mdn = 4.0) strings of words sharing phonemic similarity were produced in the real data, and 3.8 (SD = 2.3, Mdn = 4.0) strings of words in the randomized data. The Wilcoxon matched-pair signed ranks test (see Ranta, Rita & Kouki 1991:14-218; Howell 1997:652-656; see 8.2.7) revealed a statistically highly significant difference between the real and the randomized data (Z = -3.281, p < .001). Based on this finding, clusters of nouns were divided into three types of clusters to describe the nature of the strategic search for words: The number of semantic clusters (words produced by using their semantic relatedness as the criterion, such as farm animals, pets, and zoological categories), the number of phonemic clusters (words generated by the resemblance of the first phoneme, such as ‘kissa’, ‘kettu’, ‘karhu’, ‘koira’, ‘kani’ / ‘cat’, ‘fox’, ‘bear’, ‘dog’, ‘rabbit’), and the number of mixed clusters (semantic in nature; combination of semantic and phonological strategy, e.g., ‘kissa’, ‘koira’, ‘kani’, ‘lammas’, ‘lehmä’, ‘hevonen’ / ‘cat’, ‘dog’, ‘rabbit’, ‘sheep’, ‘cow’, ‘horse’) were recorded separately and counted as mixed clusters. For the verb categories, only the successively produced verbs without arguments, but sharing the first phoneme in common (e.g., ‘maalata’, ‘mitata’ / ‘paint’, ‘measure’) were counted for the analysis. The average length of phonemic strings in the real data was 1.7 (SD = 1.6, Mdn = 1.5) and in the randomized data 1.5 (SD = 1.6, Mdn = 1.0). Because the Wilcoxon matched-pair signed ranks test did not reveal a statistically significant difference between the real and the randomized data (Z = -1.519, p = .129), the strings of phonemically close words that were produced for the verb categories were counted as semantic clusters only if they happened to share a close semantic relationship. The determination of the semantic clusters was done on the basis of a post hoc analysis in which all produced words were registered and classified. A myriad of semantic relations was introduced for all the eight categories. The cluster divisions are shown in Appendix 1A and 1B. 90 Method In case two clusters overlapped, the overlapping items were counted in both clusters (e.g., ‘kala’, ‘virtahepo’, ‘krokotiili’, ‘kenguru’, ‘tiikeri’, ‘leopardi’, ‘kirahvi’ / ‘fish’, ‘hippopotamus’, ‘crocodile’, ‘kangaroo’, ‘tiger’, ‘leopard’, ‘giraffe’). In the given example, the three first words formed a cluster of animals living in the water. However, because the two last words of the cluster could also be classified as exotic animals, they were considered to belong to two semantic clusters and two different subcategories. Respectively, in a string of verbs ‘tehdä taikina’, ‘paistaa’, ‘keittää’, ‘grillata’ / ‘make dough’, ‘bake/’fry’, ‘boil’, ‘grill’, the verb ‘paistaa’ referred to both baking and frying and was thus considered part of the actions belonging to baking and cooking. Only the larger, common subcategory was used when smaller clusters (maximum size of three words) were embedded in a bigger cluster, or clusters overlapped, but all items could correctly be counted as a common subcategory (e.g., ‘takki’, ‘ulsteri’, ‘turkki’, ‘hattu’, ‘pipo’, ‘myssy’, ‘kengät’, ‘saappaat’ / ‘coat’, ‘ulster’, ‘fur coat’, ‘hat’, ‘knit hat’, ‘cap’, ‘shoes’, ‘boots’ = outdoor clothes or ‘hiihtää’, ‘luistella’, ‘juosta’, ‘lumilautailla’ / ‘ski cross-country’, ‘skate’, ‘run’, ‘snowboard’ = winter sports). 8.2.4 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the semantic fluency tasks In order to analyze the quality of the performance, the intactness of the semantic categories, and the semantic nature of the responses, the following parameters were used: The proportion of correct and unique words was counted to measure the overall ability to activate and produce as many different but correct nouns and verbs as possible belonging to the given category. Variations of the items (e.g., ‘lehmä’ / ‘cow’, ‘vasikka’ / ‘calf’ and ‘sonni’ / ‘bull’) were not counted if produced by the same subject but compound words (e.g., ‘rekka-auto’ / ‘trailer truck’, ‘kilpa-auto’ / ‘racecar’, ‘pakettiauto’ / ‘van’) and superordinates (‘linnut’ / ‘birds’; ‘juokseminen’ / ‘running’) were considered correct responses. Repetitions (perseverations) and outside-category intrusions were excluded. The formula used for counting the proportion was (the total number of the words / the number of the correct and unique words) * 100. The following verb forms were accepted to refer to different types of action: specific verbs (infinitive and inflected forms and collocations; e.g., ‘leikata’ / ‘cut, ‘juoksee’ / ‘[s/he] runs’, ‘hakata matot’ / ‘beat carpets’), verb phrases (e.g., ‘ensin kaivetaan se alusta’ / ‘first the ground is dug’), deverbal forms (deverbal nouns, e.g., ‘käveleminen’ / ‘walking’, ‘pesu’ / ‘wash’), and general (default) verbs and their modifications such as ‘laittaa’ (‘make’, ‘do’, ‘put up’: ‘laittaa katto’ / ‘build the roof’, ‘laittaa tapetit’ / ‘wallpaper’), ’panna’ (‘put’, ‘set’: ‘panna laastia’ / ‘put mortar’, ‘panna ikkunalasit’ / ‘glazing’), and ‘tehdä’ (‘do’, ‘make’: ‘tehdä huopakatto’ / Method 91 ‘lay a felt roof’). Because the verb fluency task seemed to elicit single nouns for the verb categories, the number of these nouns was calculated. However, they were considered intrusions (see below). The proportions of the different verb forms and the separate nouns were calculated with the formula (the number of the verb forms or nouns / the total number of the verbs) * 100. The proportion of intrusions (i.e., outside-category words) was calculated to measure the intactness (coherence) of the semantic categories. Semantic intrusions (i.e., words from semantically related categories (‘omena’ / ‘apple’ produced for the category of vegetables, ‘pelata bridgeä’ / ‘play bridge’ produced for the category of playing sports), were counted separately from irrelevant intrusions (i.e., words from unrelated categories (‘valokuva’ / ‘photograph’ produced for the category of articles of clothing; ‘kiillottaa’ / ‘polish’ produced for the category of preparing food). If an intrusion was repeated, it was counted as a perseveration. The formula for the proportion of intrusions was (the number of the intrusions / the total number of the the words) * 100. As for the integrity measures of verb categories, the number of single nouns (i.e., nouns produced without a verb) was also calculated. Semantically adequate nouns related to the category were counted as semantic intrusions (e.g., ‘makaronilaatikko’ / ‘macaroni casserole’ for the category of preparing food; ‘naula’ / ‘nail’ for the category of construction; ‘rätti’ / ‘rag’ for the category of cleaning up), and semantically unrelated nouns as irrelevant intrusions (e.g., ‘valokuva’ / ‘photograph’ for the category of playing sports). The proportion of perseverations (i.e., repetition of a previously produced word), was counted to measure the coherence and fluency of the word production. Synonyms (e.g., ‘sukset’, ‘hiihtimet’ / ‘skis’ or ‘juosta aitajuoksua’, ‘aitoa’ / ‘hurdle’), which were very few in number, were scored as repetitions because they seemed to activate the same or almost the same concept. The formula for the proportion was (the number of the perseverations / the number of the total words) * 100. The number of different subcategories (i.e., semantic dimensions activated for cluster production) was counted to measure the subjects’ ability to activate the semantic space. The variety of different subcategories was taken into account by displaying a distribution of the most often used criteria for semantic clustering (see the cluster division criteria in Appendix 1A and 1B). Prototypicality of words, as well as frequency ratings of words, were scored in order to control for their effect on word production. Because norms for prototypicality and frequency ratings of spoken Finnish were nonexistent, all the words produced were controlled for prototypicality and frequency in two separate post hoc sessions. In one of the sessions, 14 healthy adults (7 females and 7 males) rated the prototypicality of the words given for the fluency tasks. In the other session, another 14 healthy adults (7 females and 7 males) rated the frequency of the occurrence of the words. The raters were recruited from the author’s circle of acquaintances. In the group of the prototypicality raters, the mean age was 34.8 92 Method years (SD = 4.5, Mdn = 37.0). Eight of the raters had a university degree and six were undergraduate students or had a lower educational level. In the group of the frequency raters, the mean age was 35.9 years (SD = 5.6, Mdn = 38.0). Nine of the raters had a university degree and five were undergraduate students or had a lower educational level. All the raters were native speakers of Finnish and none of them reported any difficulty in their language skills, reading, or writing. All the raters were given randomly ordered lists of words, adding up to 585 different nouns and 644 different forms for verbs that were produced for the semantic fluency tasks, and asked to indicate how good an example of a given category or how frequent in everyday use they felt the words were on a 7-point scale. One indicated a very poor example of the category or a very infrequent word and seven indicated a very good example or a very frequently used word. Four meant that the word fit the given category moderately well or that the word was of moderate frequency. The other numbers indicated the intermediate judgments. For these ratings, the method of Rosch (1975) was applied. On the basis of these ratings, a mean for each word’s prototypicality and frequency of use was calculated. Examples of the words with the highest, intermediate, and the lowest prototypicality and frequency ratings are presented in Appendices 2 and 3. Afterwards, the prototypicality and the frequency of the words generated by the NC group, the miAD group, and the moAD group were calculated according to these ratings. Each noun and verb form was scored, after which a mean score was calculated to represent the prototypicality and the frequency of the items in each semantic category for each individual subject. 8.2.5 Inter-rater judgements All protocols were scored twice by the author, the second scoring taking place one year after the first scoring. Every seventh (n = 10) of the recordings and transcriptions was checked and verified by an independent judge. One third of the subjects’ responses (n = 23; nine subjects from the NC group, seven from the miAD group, and seven from the moAD group) were rated for the clusterings, switches, p rseverations, and intrusions by a linguist who acted as an independent judge. For t is analysis, subjects’ productions were selected using random sampling so that all n un and verb samples came from different participants. Inter-rater reliabilities with p int-by-point agreement (PPA; Kazdin 1982:23-56) were calculated separately for n uns and verbs. The formula for counting the PPA ratio was as follows: PPA = (agreements for the trial / disagreements for the trial)*100 (Kazdin 1982:54). After that, all the cases with differing judgements were discussed and a ratio of discussion-toconsensus was calculated, as a result of which only the instances upon which an agreement was made were accepted for the final analysis and scoring. The ratio of the PPA for the nouns was as high as 89.1% and the ratio of the discussion-toconsensus reached 98.8%. For the verbs, the ratio of the PPA was 88.9% and the discussion-to-consensus reached 99.7%. Method 93 8.2.6 Control tasks After the semantic fluency task was performed, all subjects performed a battery of the following tasks to measure psycho-linguistic functioning, such as naming, comprehension, and recognition, as well as attention and short-term memory. The following tasks were designed by the author to measure the overall semantic performance of the subjects. Some of the tasks required verbal responses to the stimuli, while other tasks tapped non-verbal semantic performance. Each task type was executed for each word class, first for the nouns and later for the verbs. In these tasks, the following pool of words representing the semantic categories introduced in the semantic fluency task were used in different combinations: ‘pants’, ‘hat’, ‘shirt’, ‘coat’, ‘vest’, and ‘skirt’ for the category of clothes; ‘turnip’, ‘carrot’, ‘cabbage’, ‘pea’, ‘tomato’, and ‘cucumber’ for the category of vegetables; ‘plane’, ‘boat’, ‘train’, ‘car’, ‘bus’, and ‘bicycle’ for the category of vehicles; ‘horse’, ‘cat’, ‘cow’, ‘dog’, ‘pig’, and ‘sheep’ for the category of animals; ‘fry’, ‘bake’, ‘beat’, ‘grade’, ‘clean a fish’, and ‘peel’ for the category of preparing food; ‘run’, ‘jump’, ‘skate’, ‘cross-country skiing’, ‘swim’, and ‘stretch’ for the category of playing sports; ‘paint’, ‘saw’, ‘plane’, ‘drill’, ‘weld’, ‘hammer’ for the category of construction; ‘vacuum’, ‘wash a window’, ‘beat a carpet’, ‘dust’, ‘do dishes’, and ‘sweep’ for the category of cleaning. The frequency and prototypicality of the words involved in the tasks were not controlled. Originally, the tasks were not planned for the purpose of serving as the control tasks for the semantic fluency tasks, which explains the difference in the number and the different combinations of the items among the tasks. Picture naming tasks were conducted to measure the subjects’ confrontation naming abilities. Twenty photographs (5 from each noun and verb category) were presented in a fixed order in each task. The subjects were given 20 seconds to name each picture (see the BNT below). No cues were given to prompt naming. Category recognition tasks were performed in order to measure the subjects’ non-verbal ability to differentiate between the four semantic noun categories and the four semantic verb categories. During each task, subjects were shown four series of examples from each of the four semantic categories and asked to point out the picture representing the category named by the examiner. Each series consisted of a different set of category members, one member from each category. The task involved a total of 16 category responses (4 x 4). Recognition tasks of in-category members were performed to measure the subjects’ non-verbal ability to differentiate between nouns and verbs belonging to the same semantic category. For each category, subjects were shown three sets of six semantically related photographed objects and actions and asked to point out two pictures per set according to the instruction given by the examiner. The task involved recognition of 6 examples of each category, adding up to a total of 24 nouns and 24 verbs. 94 Method Serial word naming tasks were executed to measure naming and recognition of semantically related category members belonging to the noun and verb categories. For each category, subjects were shown a series of six semantically related photographs of objects and actions, and asked to name the items from left to right in the given order. The task involved naming a total of 24 nouns and 24 verbs. Card-sorting tasks were performed to measure the subjects’ non-verbal abilities to recognize and categorize semantically related pictures. In this task, subjects were given a pile of 20 randomly ordered pictures of objects and actions belonging to the semantic categories and asked to sort the cards out according to their semantic similarity. A maximum of three minutes was given for subjects to complete each card-sorting task. If the time limit was exceeded, subjects were asked to stop. The scoring of the performance on each subtask was conducted as follows: a set of two semantically related pictures occurring together earned one point, three pictures earned two points, and four pictures earned three points, respectively. Five points were given for a complete set of five correctly sorted pictures. A total of 20 points was given for a correct performance on both sorting tasks. To measure overall verbal functions, the Finnish test version of the Boston Naming Test (BNT; Laine, Koivuselkä-Sallinen, Hänninen & Niemi 1997) and the Token Test (short version; De Renzi & Faglioni 1978) were used. The Boston Naming Test has been widely used in assessing the word finding difficulties of normal older population and demented patients (Lezak 1995:537-538). In the test, subjects were presented with 60 black and white ink drawings of objects that they were asked to name, each in 20 seconds. When necessary, semantic and/or phonemic cues were provided to help word retrieval. The Token Test was assessed to measure auditory language comprehension. Originally, the test was designed to assess language comprehension difficulties among mild aphasic subjects (De Renzi & Vignolo 1962), but it has also been used to measure language comprehension skills also in dementia, including AD (e.g., Swihart, Panisset, Becker, Beyer & Boller 1989; Tomoeda, Bayles, Boone, Kaszniak & Slauson 1990). The test consists of 20 big and small tokens of squares and circles that come in five colors (red, yellow, green, white, and black). The subjects are asked to show and manipulate the tokens according to a total of 36 verbal instructions given by the examiner. In order to measure working (short-term) memory, the span of immediate verbal recall, and attentional capacity, the Digit Span Test (forward) from the Finnish version of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler 1986; see also Lezak 1995:356360) was administered. In this task, subjects were asked to repeat series of digits in the order they were given by the examiner. 8.2.7 Statistical analysis Prior to the statistical comparison of groups, the scores of the parameters in the semantic fluency task were examined for their distributions (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; see e.g., Ranta et al. 1991:150-154) and their homogenity of variance (Levene’s Method 95 test; see e.g., Howell 1997:198-199, 321-322). Because the shape of the distribution of very many variables described above deviated from the shape of the normal distribution and their variances were heterogeneous, nonparametric, distribution-free tests were chosen for the statistical analyses of the data (Ranta et al. 1991:223-227, 316-319; Howell 1997:645-647). Mean (M) and median (Mdn) were chosen for the measures of central tendency to describe the distribution of the data, and standard deviation (SD) to describe the amount of variation in the data. To assess whether a group difference existed in the performance among the control group and the AD groups, a Kruskall-Wallis one-way analysis of variance was computed (Ranta et al. 1991:322-325; Howell 1997:658-659). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons between the control group and each AD group, as well as between the miAD group and the moAD group were assessed using the Mann-Whitney U test (Ranta et al. 1991:195-202; Howell 1997:652). For the statistical measures in the subject groups, the Friedman test was used to compare more than three paired and dependent variables. The post hoc pair-wise comparisons were calculated in order to locate the differences in the sums of ranks computed by the Friedman test (Ranta et al. 1991: 329-332). The Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test was used to assess the differences between two dependent variables in the subject groups (Ranta et al. 1991:14-218; Howell 1997:652-656). The Spearman rank correlation coefficient was used to calculate the correlations between the correct responses on the semantic fluency tasks and the scores of the control tasks (Ranta et al. 1991:437442; Howell 1997:289-290). In all comparisons, an alpha level of 0.05 was used as the cutoff for the statistical significance. All the statistical data was processed using the program of Statistical Product and Service Solutions (SPSS 10.0 for Windows, 1999), except for the post hoc comparisons of Friedman’s test which were calculated manually. 96 Method 9 Results The results of this study are presented in six sections. In the first two sections, the results concern the overall fluency performance and the content of the responses given by the subjects in the noun fluency task, followed by the verb fluency results, respectively. Each section is completed with a summary and a discussion of the results. Later, the main results concerning the total noun and verb production are summarized. Finally, results on the performance on the control tasks are presented and related to the semantic fluency performance. Samples of the semantic fluency performance selected from each subject group and each semantic category, accompanied by full data analyses, are presented in Appendix 4A-H. For the convenience of the reader, the data on the post hoc pairwise analysis between the different subject groups is collected in detail in Appendices 5, 6, and 7 instead of being presented in the text. 9.1 Overall performance on the noun fluency tasks The overall performance across all four semantic categories, as well as the number of words produced for individual semantic noun categories, was significantly different among the groups (in all categories, p < .001; see Table 7). The post hoc pair-wise comparisons between the NC group and the AD groups demonstrated a remarkable decrease in word production in each category in the miAD group and the moAD group (in each, p < .001). The most severe reduction in word production was found in the moAD group, especially, who generated only half the number of words produced by the NC group. Also in comparison to the miAD group, the moAD group’s word production was significantly poorer (p < .01). The detailed description of the post hoc pair-wise analyses in the noun production tasks can be found in Appendix 5:1. The number of words produced for the individual categories varied from 14 to 19 words in the NC group and from 9 to 13 words in the miAD group, whereas the moAD group produced words in a more stable manner, 7 or 8 words per category. The pair-wise comparisons between the groups revealed that, in comparison to the 98 Results Table 7. Total number of words and number of correct nouns produced in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn Clothes 16.8 (4.3) 16.5 14.1 (6.8) 12.5 * Vegetables 13.6 (3.4) 13.5 Vehicles Variable H (df = 2) p-value 7.8 (4.7) 7.5 *** ¤¤¤ 28.119 p < .001 9.8 (4.4) 9.0 *** 7.0 (2.9) 7.0 *** ¤ 28.935 p < .001 14.0 (3.0) 14.0 11.1 (5.8) 10.0 *** 7.6 (4.9) 7.0 *** ¤ 25.487 p < .001 Animals 19.5 (4.7) 18.5 13.8 (6.3) 13.0 *** 8.2 (5.1) 7.5 *** ¤¤ 33.966 p < .001 All categories 63.8 (11.7) 61.5 48.7 (20.5) 44.0 *** 30.6 (14.7) 30.5 *** ¤¤ 36.969 p < .001 Clothes 15.7 (4.5) 15.5 12.2 (6.4) 11.5 ** 5.9 (3.9) 5.0 *** ¤¤¤ 33.592 p < .001 Vegetables 11.4 (3.2) 10.0 7.1 (3.6) 6.5 *** 4.5 (2.1) 4.0 *** ¤¤ 39.292 p < .001 Vehicles 13.3 (2.8) 13.0 8.9 (5.1) 8.0 *** 4.9 (2.9) 4.0 *** ¤¤ 40.683 p < .001 Animals 19.0 (4.7) 18.5 12.3 (5.7) 12.0 *** 6.3 (4.0) 5.5 *** ¤¤¤ 44.023 p < .001 All categories 59.4 (11.5) 58.0 40.5 (18.7) 39.5 *** 21.5 (11.4) 44.806 21.0 *** ¤¤¤ p < .001 Total number of words Number of correct nouns Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. Results 99 NC group, significant reduction in word generation occurred in both AD groups, and the moAD group’s performance was significantly poorer than that of the miAD group across all semantic categories (see Appendix 5:1). 9.1.1 Number of correct nouns Incorrect responses were found in all subject groups and in all semantic categories (see Table 7 and 11). The difference in the number of all correctly produced nouns between the three groups was highly significant (p < .001). There was a remarkable reduction in the total number of correctly produced nouns in the miAD group and the moAD group. The post hoc pair-wise analyses (see Appendix 5:2a) demonstrated that the NC group produced significantly more correct words in the task than the miAD group and the moAD group (in both, p < .001). When the AD groups were compared to each other, a significant difference was noticed in their performances, with the miAD group generating significantly more correct nouns in the whole task than the moAD group (p < .001). A similar pattern in the performance of the subject groups was found at the level of individual semantic categories. A significant difference emerged among the groups in the number of correct nouns throughout all categories (in each case, p < .001). Compared to the NC group, a significant reduction in the number of correct responses was found in both the miAD and the moAD group, and the miAD group generated significantly more correct words than the moAD group for all categories (see Appendix 5:2a). According to the mean frequencies in all subject groups, most correct words were produced for the category of animals, whereas the fewest correct responses were generated for the category of vegetables (see Table 7). The Friedman test indicated that there was a significant difference in the number of correct words produced for the different categories in each subject group (NC group: χ2 = 56.8 (df = 3), p < .001, miAD group: χ2 = 31.5 (df = 3), p < .001, and moAD group: χ2 = 10.1 (df = 3), p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise comparison revealed statistically significant differences in the number of correct responses between the semantic categories in the NC group and the miAD group. In the moAD group, however, the pair-wise analyses revealed no statistically significant differences between the categories, but some of the differences in the sums of ranks approached the significant level (see Appendix 5:2b). There was no statistically significant difference in the number of correct words produced for the whole task or the individual categories between the male and female participants in the NC group, in which the distribution of gender was fairly equal (16 men and 14 women; see Table 8). 100 Results Table 8. Number of correct nouns produced by the male and female participants in the NC group Male participants (n = 16) Female participants (n = 14) Category M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn Mann-Whitney U test Clothes 15.3 (4.4) 15.0 16.3 (4.7) 17.0 U = 94.5 p = .473, n.s. Vegetables 10.4 (2.4) 10.0 12.5 (3.8) 13.0 U = 76.5 p = .142, n.s. Vehicles 13.9 (2.4) 13.0 12.6 (3.1) 12.5 U = 85.5 p = .275, n.s. Animals 18.7 (3.7) 18.0 19.4 (5.9) 18.5 U = 111.5 p = .984, n.s. All categories 58.3 (9.2) 55.5 60.8 (13.9) 63.5 U = 102.0 p = .697, n.s. Note. n.s. = non significant. 9.1.2 Clustering and switching The subject groups indicated varying numbers of switches between the subcategories (e.g., from farm animals to birds) when performing the task (see Table 9). The total score for switching indicated a significant overall difference among the groups (p < .001). The post hoc analyses (see Appendix 5:3.) revealed that the NC group was able to more easily shift from one subcategory to another than either the miAD group or the moAD group, and that the miAD group was significantly better at switching than the moAD group (in each case, p < .001). When generating words in a category, the NC group switched five times on average from one subcategory to another, the miAD group switched from three to four times, while the moAD group was able to switch subcategories only twice in a category. In all subject groups, the number of switches remained fairly constant over the semantic categories. The post hoc pair-wise analysis revealed that the NC group switched subcategories significantly more often than the miAD group in all other categories but clothes (vegetables and animals p < .05, vehicles p < .001) and significantly more often than the moAD group in all semantic categories (in each category, p < .001). The miAD group showed a significantly better switching performance than the moAD group in all categories (clothes p < .001, vegetables and animals p < .01, vehicles p < .05). Results 101 Table 9. Clustering and switching in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Clothes 5.0 (1.9) 5.0 4.5 (1.9) 4.0 2.2 (1.9) 2.0 *** ¤¤¤ 19.562 p < .001 Vegetables 5.3 (1.8) 5.0 4.2 (2.5) 3.5 * 2.2 (2.1) 2.0 *** ¤¤ 23.274 p < .001 Vehicles 5.2 (1.3) 5.0 3.5 (1.9) 3.5 *** 2.7 (4.1) 1.5 *** ¤ 23.653 p < .001 Animals 4.9 (1.9) 5.0 3.6 (2.2) 3.0 * 1.8 (1.7) 2.0 *** ¤¤ 24.440 p < .001 All categories 20.4 (3.7) 20.0 15.7 (6.2) 14.5 *** 8.8 (7.1) 8.0 *** ¤¤¤ 32.408 p < .001 Clothes 4.9 (1.7) 5.0 3.9 (1.9) 4.0 * 1.9 (1.4) 2.0 *** ¤¤¤ 26.978 p < .001 Vegetables 4.1 (1.7) 5.0 2.9 (1.8) 3.0 ** 1.7 (1.2) 2.0 *** ¤ 23.581 p < .001 Vehicles 4.6 (1.4) 5.0 2.7 (1.3) 3.0 *** 1.6 (1.2) 1.5 *** ¤¤ 36.464 p < .001 Animals 5.0 (1.7) 4.0 3.5 (1.7) 3.0 ** 2.0 (1.4) 1.5 *** ¤¤ 29.509 p < .001 All categories 18.6 (3.8) 19.0 13.0 (5.1) 12.5 *** 7.2 (4.0) 6.5 *** ¤¤¤ 40.678 p < .001 Clothes 2.2 (1.0) 2.1 1.7 (0.9) 1.4 (*) 1.6 (1.6) 1.1 * 7.775 p < .05 Vegetables 1.4 (0.7) 1.3 1.1 (0.9) 0.9 1.6 (1.3) 1.5 3.214 p = .201, n.s. Vehicles 1.5 (0.6) 1.5 1.7 (1.6) 1.5 1.6 (1.7) 1.0 0.820 p = .664, n.s. Animals 2.8 (1.2) 2.8 2.3 (0.9) 2.6 2.6 (2.6) 1.8 3.367 p = .186, n.s. All categories 1.8 (0.4) 1.7 1.5 (0.4) 1.4 ** 1.5 (0.8) 1.4 * 8.430 p < .05 Clothes 93.0 (9.0) 94.0 85.8 (14.1) 89.2 * 73.5 (30.6) 85.4 ** 8.341 p < .05 Vegetables 82.7 (16.3) 85.2 71.9 (25.9) 71.4 81.5 (21.6) 88.2 2.523 p = .283, n.s. Vehicles 87.5 (14.0) 92.0 80.2 (21.5) 86.9 68.5 (38.1) 86.6 3.582 p = .167, n.s. Animals 95.3 (5.6) 95.3 92.6 (9.8) 100.00 83.1 (29.7) 94.4 1.086 p = 582, n.s. All categories 91.0 (4.6) 92.2 85.2 (6.7) 87.2 *** 78.5 (18.4) 81.9 ** 12.950 p < .01 Variable Number of switches Number of clusters Cluster size Nouns in clusters (%) Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. 102 Results The number of clusters of closely related nouns differed significantly among the subject groups, both at the level of overall performance across the categories (p < .001) and at the level of individual categories (in each category, p < .001; see Table 9). The mean number of the clusters varied from 4 to 5 clusters in the NC group and from 3 to 4 clusters in the miAD group. The moAD group produced approximately two clusters for each semantic category. The total number of clusters produced by the NC group was significantly higher than that produced by the two AD groups (in each, p < .001), as was the number of clusters in all categories (see Appendix 5:4). Furthermore, the miAD group produced significantly more clusters than the moAD both when the number of all clusters (p < .001) and the number of clusters in the single categories were analysed (clothes p < .001, vegetables p < .05, vehicles, and animals p < .01). The overall performance of the groups indicated a significant difference in the cluster size (p < .05; see Table 9). When comparing the overall size of the noun clusters, it was noticed that the NC group created significantly larger clusters than the miAD group (p < .01) and the moAD group (p < .05), whereas the clusters formed by the miAD group and the moAD group did not differ in size (see Appendix 5:5). Depending on the category, all groups produced clusters consisting of two to four words (i.e., cluster size between one and three). The category of vegetables seemed to bring about the smallest clusters, while the animals evoked the largest ones. At the level of individual semantic categories, however, the size of the clusters among the subject groups differed significantly only in the category of clothes (p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise analysis revealed that the moAD group formed significantly smaller clusters for the category of clothes than the NC group (p < .01) and that the difference in the cluster size between the NC group and the miAD group approached the statistically significant level (p = .073). The cluster size difference between the miAD group and the moAD group was not significant. Considering the coherence of the overall performance and the ability to use the strategy of clustering words, a difference among the subject groups emerged in the proportion of nouns in clusters (p < .01; see Table 9). The post hoc pair-wise analyses (see Appendix 5:6) revealed that the NC group clustered more nouns than the miAD group (p < .001) and the moAD group (p < .01), while the miAD group and the moAD group clustered nouns to the same extent. In all groups and all semantic categories, more than 70% of the words were clustered. However, for the category of clothes, a difference in the performance among the groups emerged (p < .05). The NC group clustered nouns more efficiently than the miAD group (p < .05) and the moAD group (p < .01) but the difference between the two AD groups did not reach the level of statistical significance. Results 103 9.1.3 Summary of the results and discussion The findings of this study indicated that relative to the NC group, the miAD group and the moAD group showed a significant reduction in production of responses for the semantic fluency task with noun categories, and that the miAD group was able to produce significantly more responses for the categories than the moAD group. The different semantic categories seemed to evoke different amounts of correct responses: in all subject groups, the category of animals provoked most words, whereas the category of vegetables brought about the fewest responses. All in all, the performance of the subject groups indicated that the semantic categories elicited varying numbers of responses in the NC group and the miAD group, whereas the moAD group showed very little variation in the number of words produced across the categories. Although both AD groups produced significantly fewer clusters and switches for the categories, they were able to generate nouns clustered together according to some shared properties. It was noticed that the performance of all subject groups contained erroneous responses, which is reported in more detail in 9.2.1 and discussed in 9.2.6. Overall performance on the noun fluency tasks When the number of correct and uniquely produced nouns was taken into account, a significant reduction in the word production was found in both of the AD groups relative to the NC group (see 9.1.1). The finding is consistent with several other studies, such as Rosen (1980), Tröster et al. (1989; 1998), Binetti et al. (1995), Chertkow and Bub (1990), Hodges and Patterson (1995), Crossley et al. (1997), and Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998; see Table 4). It was also noticed that the moAD group produced significantly fewer correct nouns than the miAD group, a finding in accordance with Bayles et al. (1993), Mickanin et al. (1994), Crossley et al. (1997), and Hodges and Patterson (1995). Unfortunately, as can be seen in Table 4, the studies cannot be directly compared to each other, for the following methodological reasons. The sizes of the subject groups differ, as do the tests used to assess the degree of dementia. The cut-off scores to indicate the stage of dementia also vary. Furthermore, in many studies, AD patients of different degrees of dementia were often considered one group or only patients with mild AD were involved in the studies. The semantic categories chosen for the studies are often dissimilar. In some studies, only one semantic category was used to tap the fluency performance, while in others data was collected from several semantic categories. Furthermore, instead of providing scores for individual semantic categories, the data in some cases was presented as a combined score over various categories. Unequal time limits (e.g., 60 or 90 s) were given for subjects to perform the fluency task. Finally, some studies tend not to provide the reader with sufficient basic frequency data, such as standard deviations. Nevertheless, when comparing the results obtained from different studies on the animal fluency task, which is the most often used form of the task, one can note 104 Results that the number of correctly produced words differs slightly among the studies (see Table 4). The healthy control subjects of the present study produced a mean of 19.0 (SD = 4.7) correct words, which is nearly the same as the number of words produced by English-speaking healthy controls with approximately the same mean age in the study of Chertkow and Bub (1990) and Chan et al. (1993). On the other hand, the number of correct responses given by the control subjects in the present study was greater than that of Italian-speaking subjects reported by Binetti et al. (1995) and that of English-speaking subjects reported by Hodges and Patterson (1995). However, French-speaking subjects in the study of Pasquier et al. (1995) produced more correct responses than the subjects in the present study. The healthy controls of the present study produced fewer correct words than their counterparts also in the study of Kontiola et al. (1990), which is another study conducted with Finnishspeaking elderly subjects providing data, for example, on the fluency task. When the performance of the patients with mild AD is compared to that from studies in which MMSE was used in assessing the degree of dementia, the mean of 12.3 correct words (SD = 5.7) produced by the miAD group in the present study seemed to be higher than that in the study of Binetti et al. (1995) and Hodges and Patterson (1995), and lower than in the study of Pasquier et al. (1995; see Table 4). There are very few comparable studies on patients with moderate AD. However, the moAD group of the present study produced approximately the same number of correct words (M = 6.3, SD = 4.0) as their counterparts in the study of Chertkow and Bub (1990), but more than those in the study of Hodges and Patterson (1995). When comparing the results obtained in the present study to those reported by Hodges and Patterson (1995; see also Chertkow & Bub 1990), it can be found that not only the animal category, but also the category of vehicles provoked more responses in all subject groups in the present study. The miAD group in the present study produced slightly more correct responses than the patients with minimal and mild degree of dementia in the study of Hodges and Patterson. The differences in the mean age between the groups are not likely to explain the finding (see Huff et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997). The method used for assessing the degree of dementia of the AD patients may not contribute to the differing performances either because the MMSE was used in both studies. A possible reason for the differences between the studies may be the way the cut-off points were set to indicate the degree of dementia. The cut-off point on the MMSE to denote the degree of minimal dementia (25.6, SD=1.8) may be too low in the study of Hodges and Patterson, as the MMSE scores in early AD usually range from 24 to 30 points and in mild AD from 18 to 26 points (Erkinjuntti, Rinne & Soininen 2001; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001; see Table 2). Alternatively, some of the patients of the miAD group in the present study may have a very mild (minimal) degree of dementia and, thus, high naming scores, and some of the AD patients in the Hodges and Patterson study could be assessed as moderately demented, which could explain their achieving lower scores on naming. The group of patients with moderate dementia in the Hodges and Patterson study seems also to involve patients whose degree of Results 105 dementia had already advanced to the severe stage of dementia, which may explain the group’s lower number of correct words in the animal and vehicle fluency tasks, relative to the scores of the moAD group in the present study. Concerning the number of correct words, it is not easy to exactly pinpoint the reasons underlying the differences among the studies. Partly, it may be a question of the number of semantic categories used in the studies. If only one category had been used, as was the case in the study of Kontiola et al. (1990) and Pasquier et al. (1995), the subjects might not have tired of listing different words after another and thus might have produced a high number of correct responses for the task. In this study, as well as in the study of Hodges and Patterson (1995), several semantic categories were involved implying that the subjects may have become fatigued during the course of testing. In the latter study, the category of animals appeared as the first category for the word production, whereas it was the last noun category introduced in this study. Nevertheless, the scores of the present study were still higher than those in the study of Hodges and Patterson between the groups of healthy elderly adults, the groups of mildly demented AD patients, and the groups of moderately demented AD patients. Using only one semantic category in the task does not seem to fully explain the better performance, reported by Binetti et al. (1995; cf. Crossley et al. 1997) who used only the category of animals for the task and reported a strikingly poorer number of correct animal words (M = 14.1, SD = 5.4) in the group of healthy control subjects compared to other studies in which either one or more semantic categories were administered. Differences in the languages and the cultural backgrounds of the subjects in the studies may partly explain the variability. However, a high number of correct responses was obtained by speakers with different backgrounds, that is, by French-speaking subjects as shown in the study of Pasquier et al. (1995), by Finnishspeaking subjects as indicated in the study of Kontiola et al. (1990) and in the present study, as well as by English-speaking subjects as reported by Hodges and Patterson (1995). By contrast, Italian-speaking subjects in the study of Binetti et al. (1995) did a lot worse than the French-speaking subjects. The gender of the subjects has been considered an important factor in explaining the semantic fluency performance (e.g., Monsch et al. 1992; Capitani et al. 1999; see 5.1). For example, Monsch et al. found that both in the normal control group and in the group of AD patients, female participants produced significantly more correct responses than male participants for such categories as fruit, vegetables, and animals. Capitani et al. showed that male subjects named significantly more words denoting tools than female subjects. Unfortunately, because both the miAD and the moAD group of the present study had more female than male subjects, the effect of gender on their semantic fluency performance could not be accomplished (see 9.1.1). However, the comparison between the male and female subjects in the NC group indicated that gender did not play a role in the semantic fluency performance of the subjects in any of the semantic noun categories. The finding is consistent with that of Crossley et al. (1997) and Troyer (2000), who found no gender 106 Results effect on the animal fluency task, and inconsistent with the findings of Monsch et al. (1992) and Capitani et al. (1999). Consequently, gender may not have played a significant role in the noun fluency performance in the AD groups. Clustering and switching Although the miAD group showed a significantly better ability to cluster nouns and to switch between the subcategories than the moAD group, the performance of both AD groups in clustering and switching was remarkably poorer than the NC group’s performance (see 9.1.2). These findings accord with those presented by Martin and Fedio (1983), Ober et al. (1986), Tröster et al. (1989, 1998), Binetti et al. (1995), Carew et al. (1997), Beatty et al. (1997, 2000), and Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998). Regardless of the reduction in the number of the words, and in the number of switches and clusters, all subject groups produced clusters of similar size (from two to four words) for all other categories but clothes, for which the moAD group produced smaller clusters than the NC group. This finding supports the findings of Binetti et al. (1995) who reported that the average cluster size in the animal fluency task was equal (approximately four words) when comparing the performance of the normal control subjects, the mild AD patients, and the moderate-to-severe AD patients. In the present study, the average number of nouns within a cluster in the animal fluency task approached four in the group of normal control subjects, three in the group of mild AD patients, and four in the group of moderate AD patients (see Table 9). Concerning cluster size, however, the finding is in contrast with that presented by Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998; see also Beatty et al. 1997, 2000), who observed that the normal control subjects produced significantly larger clusters (between two and three words) than the AD patients (between one and two words) in the animal fluency task (see Table 10). It is worth noting, however, that a general trend appeared in the present study indicating that the average cluster size calculated over all semantic categories was significantly smaller in each AD group than in the NC group. The way the size of the cluster was calculated cannot explain the differences between these studies. The study of Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998), as well as the present study, used a minimum of two subsequent words to form a cluster, ending up with contrasting findings (see Table 10). On the other hand, Binetti et al. (1995) considered a minimum of three words in a cluster and yet did not find a difference in the cluster size between the normal control group and the group of the AD patients. The difference cannot be well explained by the size of the subject groups or the mean age of these subjects. The difference in the mean age is only within a range of a few years among the studies, and age was found to be an insensitive factor affecting the semantic fluency performance in AD in several studies (Huff et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Crossley et al. 1997; see 5.1). Furthermore, Troyer et al. (1997; see also Troyer 2000) did not Results 107 Table 10. Data on the animal fluency task performed by normal control subjects (NC) and Alzheimer’s patients (AD) in different studies Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) Tröster et al. (1998) The present study NC AD NC AD NC miAD moAD Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn Number of subjects 38 23 30 30 30 20 20 Age 73.8 (6.2) 70.3 (8.4) 70.8 (7.0) 69.7 (5.9) 66.7 (5.5) 65.0 (10.3) 67.4 (8.7) 66.0 64.5 64.5 Education 12.6 (2.7) 13.0 (3.3) 13.9 (2.4) 13.0 (1.7) 9.7 (3.3) 9.0 Degree of dementia+ - 10.5 (3.7) 10.1 (3.6) 9.0 10.0 118.8 (13.1) 137.0 (4.3) 107.4 (13.4) 28.9 (0.9) 23.5 (2.0) 15.9 (2.4) mild moderate 29.0 23.5 * 17.0 * ¤ Number of correct 17.9 (4.2) 8.3 (4.2) * words 17.8 (4.1) 7.6 (4.6) * 19.0 (4.7) 12.3 (5.7) 6.3 (4.0) 18.5 12.0 * 5.5 * ¤ Number of switches 8.3 (2.4) 5.1 (2.9) * 7.6 (2.6) 3.8 (2.9) * 4.9 (1.9) 3.6 (2.2) 5.0 3.0 * 1.8 (1.7) 2.0 * ¤ Cluster size 1.1 (0.6) 0.6 (0.4) * 1.3 (0.5) 0.8 (0.6) * 2.8 (1.2) 2.3 (0.9) 2.8 2.6 2.6 (2.6) 1.8 Note. +For evaluating the degree of dementia, Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) was applied in the study of Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998), while Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used in the present study. * = a statistical difference between the AD group and the NC group in different studies. ¤ = a statistical difference between the miAD group and the moAD group in the present study. find a difference in the cluster size in the animal category naming task between younger (mean age 22.3 years, SD = 3.8) and older healthy adults (mean age 73.3, SD = 6.5; see 5.1). Moreover, the differences between the performances of the subjects cannot be explained by the level of education, which is another factor found not to affect the fluency performance in AD (Rosen 1980; Crossley et al. 1997; see 5.1). Besides, the subjects in the present study were the least educated but they produced more words and larger clusters on average than their counterparts in the other studies. 108 Results One way of explaining the differing findings may be the great number of perseverations produced by the AD subjects in the present study and their possible effect on the clustering performance in the AD groups (see 9.2.2, 9.2.6, 10.1.2, and 10.1.3). However, the studies are not comparable because Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998) did not provide a report on the occurrence of perseverations in their AD groups. In the present study, all repetitions of responses were included in the clusters, following the protocol of Troyer et al. (1997). Another alternative explanation for the differing findings may be the use of different methods in evaluating the severity of dementia and the criteria used in forming the subject groups. In the study of Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998), dementia was rated mild using the Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis 1988, in Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998), while in the study of Binetti et al. (1995) and the present study, the degree of dementia was evaluated using the MMSE (Folstein et al. 1975). Yet another possible factor contributing to the contrasting findings may be the different statistical methods used in the studies, this study having used non-parametric methods, while parametric methods were applied in the study of Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) and Tröster et al. (1998). When the proportion of all clustered words was taken into account, the subject groups showed a coherent pattern of performance by clustering more than 70% of all nouns. In general, the tendency to cluster nouns was seemingly not disturbed by the production of single words between the clusters in any subject group. However, when producing words for clothes, the miAD and the moAD group indicated an exceptional performance by clustering less and producing more single words than the NC group. The tendency of the miAD group to perseverate words and the tendency of the moAD group to produce words from outside the semantic category boundaries for the category of clothes (see 9.2.2) may explain why the proportion of words in clusters remained smaller in their groups than in the NC group. The high proportion of words within clusters also implies that the production of nouns was guided by a strategy of tying them together by some relation (see 9.1.2, 9.1.3, and 9.2.3). In conclusion, the word production during the noun fluency task, when counted as the number of total and correct words, switches, and clusters, was significantly diminished in both AD groups, the moAD group faring significantly worse than the miAD group. However, the performance of the AD groups appeared to be semantically as coherent as that of the NC group when the size of the clusters and the proportion of the words in the clusters were taken into account. Results 109 9.2 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the noun fluency tasks The qualitative analysis of the noun fluency task involved an error analysis in which the proportion of correct words, intrusions, and perseverations in each subject group and in each semantic category was determined. The strategies and the scope of semantic space used by the subject groups, as well as the degree of prototypicality and frequency of nouns produced for the task, were also analyzed. 9.2.1 Proportion of correct nouns When considering the overall performance, the proportion of all correctly produced nouns indicated a significant overall difference among the groups (p < .001; see Table 11). The NC group produced more than 90% of the nouns correctly (i.e., unique words belonging to the given semantic category), the proportion of correct responses being some 80% in the miAD group and 70% in the moAD group. The post hoc pair-wise analyses showed that these differences were statistically significant (NC vs. miAD, p < .01; NC vs. moAD, p < .001; miAD vs. moAD, p < .05; see Appendix 5:7). In all subject groups, the average frequencies indicated that the category of animals had the highest rate of correct responses, whereas the lowest rate was found in the category of vegetables (see also 9.1.1). A significant group difference emerged in the proportion of correct responses at the level of each semantic category (clothes, vehicles, and animals p < .001; vegetables p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise analysis indicated that the miAD group produced significantly more errors than the NC group for the categories of clothes (p < .05), as well as for the categories of vehicles and animals (in each, p < .01). When the NC group and the moAD group were compared, it was noticed that the moAD group produced more erroneous responses for all categories (clothes, vehicles, and animals p < .001; vegetables p < .01). The proportion of correct words in the categories remained the same between the AD groups. 9.2.2 Proportion of intrusions and perseverations Intrusions (i.e., words not belonging to the given semantic category) were produced by all subject groups (p > .05), but their total number remained fairly low in all groups (see Table 11). The performance of 17/30 subjects in the NC group, 12/ 20 subjects in the miAD group, and 14/20 subjects in the moAD group contained intrusions in at least one of the four semantic categories. According to the average frequencies, most of the intrusions emerged in the categories of clothes and vegetables, with the lowest rates observed in the category of animals. All subject groups seemed to produce intrusions in a similar vein for the individual categories, except for the category of clothes (p < .05), for which the moAD group generated significantly 110 Results Table 11. Proportion of correct words, intrusions, and perseverations in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Clothes 94.0 (12.0) 100.0 86.1 (13.5) 89.4 * 72.5 (27.3) 83.8 *** 15.552 p < .001 Vegetables 85.3 (17.2) 90.9 73.4 (26.9) 73.2 66.3 (22.2) 69.1 ** 8.142 p < .05 Vehicles 95.3 (6.1) 100.0 81.6 (17.7) 85.3 ** 69.2 (28.7) 76.4 *** 16.629 p < .001 Animals 97.3 (3.8) 100.0 89.9 (10.8) 91.6 ** 75.0 (27.4) 84.5 *** 15.074 p < .001 All categories 93.1 (6.9) 95.5 83.3 (13.7) 85.8 ** 71.1 (18.2) 71.7 *** ¤ 26.160 p < .001 Clothes 4.7 (10.7) 0.0 2.5 (6.1) 0.0 21.7 (29.7) 8.6 * ¤ 8.849 p < .05 Vegetables 9.8 (16.3) 0.0 14.8 (26.7) 0.0 9.3 (18.2) 0.0 0.754 p = .686, n.s. Vehicles 0.8 (2.1) 0.0 3.7 (9.6) 0.0 3.4 (9.4) 0.0 0.599 p = .741, n.s. Animals 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 0.5 (1.6) 0.0 4.0 (12.0) 0.0 4.483 p = .106, n.s. All categories 3.8 (4.9) 1.6 4.4 (6.6) 2.3 7.9 (7.7) 3.4 2.522 p = .283, n.s. Clothes 1.3 (3.2) 0.0 11.4 (12.9) 9.8 *** 5.8 (9.5) 0.0 14.220 p < .001 Vegetables 4.9 (8.0) 0.0 11.8 (14.6) 6.0 24.4 (24.4) 23.6 ** 9.863 p < .01 Vehicles 3.9 (6.4) 0.0 14.7 (16.2) 12.5 ** 27.4 (26.8) 19.1 *** 14.208 p < .001 Animals 2.7 (3.8) 0.0 9.6 (10.9) 7.2 * 21.0 (21.0) 15.5 *** 14.401 p < .001 All categories 3.1 (3.7) 1.8 12.3 (10.9) 10.8 *** 21.0 (15.0) 16.5 *** ¤ 27.678 p < .001 Variable Correct nouns (%) Intrusions (%) Perseverations (%) Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. Results 111 Table 12. Number of semantically related and unrelated intrusions produced in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) Type of intrusions M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn Semantically related intrusions 2.5 (3.3) 1.0 1.8 (2.7) 1.0 1.7 (2.1) 1.0 Semantically unrelated intrusions 0.03 (0.2) 0.0 0.3 (0.7) 0.0 0.5 (1.1) 0.0 Z-value+ p-value -3.610 p < .001 -3.114 p < .01 -1.944 p < .05 Note. +Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test: * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p <. 001 more intrusions than the NC group and the miAD group (in both cases, p < .05; see Appendix 5:8). Most of the intrusions were semantically related rather than unrelated to the given category in the NC group (p < .001) and the miAD group ( p < .01), as well as in the moAD group (p < .05; see Table 12). The intrusions produced for the category of clothes consisted mostly of words referring to other pieces of fabric, such as carpets, wall hangings, tablecloths, curtains, towels and bed linen. The outside-category words produced for the category of vegetables were in most cases different kinds of berries, fruit, and grains. In the category of vehicles, toys, working machinery, and animals other than beasts of burden were classified as intrusions. As for the category of animals, words without a specific referent (e.g., ‘buzzer’) were considered inappropriate words. Intrusions that had no semantic relationship to the given category seemed to be words that were activated by the test situation and the place of examination (e.g., ‘recorder’ referring to the tape-recorder on the table) or words from outside the test context (e.g., ‘smoked fish’ for clothes). Perseverations (i.e., repetition of previously produced words) were produced in all subject groups and in each category (see Table 11). Perseverations were generated at least once by 22/30 subjects in the NC group, 18/20 subjects in the miAD group, and 18/20 subjects in the moAD group. The average number of total perseverations in the NC group was 3.1% (SD = 3.7) and their number increased both in the miAD group (M = 12.3%, SD = 10.9) and in the moAD group (M = 21.0%, SD = 15.0). The total number of perseverations over the categories indicated a significant difference both among (p < .001) and between the subject groups (see Appendix 5:9). 112 Results There was a statistically significant difference in the tendency to perseverate words among the different subject groups in all semantic categories (clothes, vehicles, and animals p < .001; vegetables p < .01). The post hoc pair-wise analysis indicated that the miAD group produced significantly more perseverations than the NC group when naming items for the category of clothes (p < .001), vehicles (p < .01), and animals (p < .05). The moAD group produced significantly more perseverations than the NC group for the categories of vegetables (p < .01), vehicles, and animals (in each, p < .001). The two AD groups’ rates of perseverations did not differ in any single category. The number of perseverations seemed to fluctuate from one category to another in each subject group. In the moAD group, only a few perseverations were produced for the first category (i.e., clothes), after which their number increased considerably during the second category (i.e., vegetables) and remained high for the rest of the categories. 9.2.3 Clustering strategies As shown in Table 13, in all subject groups more than 60% of the clusters were produced using the pure semantic strategy (i.e., clusters formed on the basis of semantic relatedness without phonological resemblance between the words, e.g., ‘leijona’, ‘tiikeri’ / ‘lion’, ‘tiger’), followed by the proportion of approximately 30% of clusters formed applying the mixed strategy (i.e., clusters formed on the basis of semantic and phonological relatedness between the words, e.g., ‘kissa’, ‘koira’, ‘kani’ / ‘cat’, ‘dog’, ‘rabbit’ ), and some 5% of the clusters with the pure phonological strategy (i.e., clusters formed on the basis of phonological resemblance between the words, e.g., ‘sika’, ‘susi’ / ‘pig’, ‘wolf’). However, at the level of single semantic categories, the ratios of different strategies used by the subject groups varied to some extent. The mixed and the phonemic strategies were utilized to a similar extent in all subject groups in all semantic categories. The extent of the use of the semantic strategy among the subject groups was significantly different in the categories of clothes and animals (in each category, p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise analyses (see Appendix 5:10) indicated that the moAD group produced significantly more clusters of clothes than the NC group (p < .01) and the miAD group (p < .05) by applying the pure semantic strategy, whereas the difference between the NC group and the miAD group was not significant. Unlike the NC group and the miAD group, the moAD group tended not to utilize the phonemic resemblance between the words for the category of clothes. On the other hand, the moAD group produced purely semantic clusters for the animal category significantly less often than the NC group and the miAD group (in each, p < .05). Again, the difference between the NC group and the miAD group in the use of the semantic strategy did not reach statistical significance. The moAD group produced more than 50% of the animal words using the mixed strategy that involved a combination of semantic and phonemic relatedness between the words within the clusters. Results 113 Table 13. Clustering strategies in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 18) moAD (n = 15) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Clothes 74.9 (19.5) 77.5 74.2 (27.3) 80.0 90.7 (17.0) 100.00 ** ¤ 7.007 p < .05 Vegetables 63.7 (28.3) 69.0 57.2 (37.3) 63.3 50.0 (45.0) 66.7 0.760 p = .684, n.s. Vehicles 66.6 (23.0) 66.7 68.1 (35.5) 66.7 66.7 (41.2) 100.00 0.723 p = .697, n.s. Animals 60.2 (16.5) 66.7 58.1 (25.5) 66.7 31.8 (37.5) 0.0 * ¤ 6.240 p < .05 All categories 66.8 (8.5) 66.7 66.3 (16.2) 69.0 62.8 (17.2) 63.6 1.146 p = .564, n.s. Clothes 20.0 (18.2) 16.7 20.1 (28.2) 7.1 9.3 (17.0) 0.0 3.915 p = .141, n.s. Vegetables 28.1 (23.7) 25.0 33.9 (35.6) 26.7 46.7 (42.8) 33.3 1.514 p = .469, n.s. Vehicles 29.4 (21.2) 25.0 30.1 (36.3) 29.2 30.0 (41.9) 0.0 0.996 p = .608, n.s. Animals 31.3 (15.4) 33.3 38.7 (26.2) 33.3 58.8 (42.4) 50.0 4.313 p = .116, n.s. All categories 26.1 (9.29) 24.3 27.8 (18.3) 16.1 32.1 (15.1) 32.1 1.859 p = .395, n.s. Clothes 5.1 (9.8) 0.0 5.6 (13.1) 0.0 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 4.542 p = .103, n.s. Vegetables 8.2 (11.7) 0.0 8.9 (13.5) 0.0 3.3 (12.9) 0.0 3.704 p = .157, n.s. Vehicles 4.0 (8.3) 0.0 1.9 (7.9) 0.0 3.3 (12.9) 0.0 2.261 p = .323, n.s. Animals 8.5 (11.3) 0.0 3.2 (7.6) 0.0 9.4 (16.9) 0.0 2.642 p = .267, n.s. All categories 7.1 (5.3) 6.9 5.9 (5.7) 6.3 5.0 (10.6) 0.0 4.784 p = .091, n.s. Strategy Semantic strategy (%) Mixed strategy (%) Phonological strategy (%) Note. * = p < .05, ** p < .01 when NC vs. moAD and ¤ = p < .05 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. 114 Results 9.2.4 Number and variety of different semantic subcategories All four noun categories combined, the number of different subcategories activated by the NC group was approximately 15, whereas the miAD group was able to produce nouns from 10 and the moAD group from 5 different semantic subgroups (see Table 14). The total number of the semantic subcategories distinguished the subject groups overall (p < .001), and the pairwise differences were significant between all subject groups (between all, p < .001; see Appendix 5:11). For each category, the NC group was able to produce clusters of nouns from the mean of three to four different subcategories, but the variety of semantic subcategories narrowed down in the AD groups. The miAD was able to activate on average two or three different semantic subcategories, while the moAD group produced words from one or two subcategories. The number of semantic subcategories was significantly different among the subject groups in all semantic categories (p < .001). The post hoc analysis revealed that in each semantic category, a significant decrease in the number of subcategories took place in both AD groups compared to the NC group, and a significantly greater decrease occurred in the moAD group relative to the miAD group (see Appendix 5:11). When investigating the variety of semantic subcategories, it was observed that the distribution of the subcategories was rather similar between the NC group and the miAD group across all semantic categories (see Figures 1-4). The moAD group showed some reduction even in the use of the most common semantic subcategories. All subject groups tended to prefer clusters in which the words shared thematic properties (i.e., information of the contextual and spatial locations, and causal and interactional relationships between the objects in a scene, as well as cultural information) and functional properties (i.e., information concerning the manner and the rules by which the objects move or interact with the environment or how humans move when manipulating the objects) rather than combining words according to their taxonomic relatedness (i.e., by means of the hierarchy of the class inclusion; see 3.1.2 , 3.2). Physical features, such as part-whole analysis, were also used as a criterion for clustering by the participants, especially when words were produced for the category of clothes. Nevertheless, a lack of subcategories did not imply the subjects’ inability to produce items belonging to a particular subcategory per se. Rather, depending on the way clusters were formed, strings of subordinate nouns were embedded within larger thematic clusters (e.g., ‘caps’, ‘ski suits’, ‘fur hats’, ‘hats’, ‘ulster’, ‘summer coat’, ‘jacket’ for outdoor clothes) and some of the subcategories were represented as a single word outside a cluster. The same words were clustered by various semantic criteria by individual subjects (see Appendix 4A-4D). When producing words for the category of clothes, outdoor (e.g., ‘coat’, ‘hat’, ‘gloves’) and indoor clothing (e.g., ‘socks’, ‘pants’, ‘skirt’, ‘blouse’) were the most often produced subcategories in all subject groups (Figure 1; see also Appendix 4A). The third common subcategory for the NC group was different outfits (e.g., ‘evening dress’, ‘national costume’), whereas underclothes (‘undershirt’, ‘bra’, Results 115 Table 14. Number of different subcategories produced for the semantic categories in the noun fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) Category M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df=2) p-value Clothes 3.7 (1.2) 4.0 2.9 (1.3) 3.0 * 1.7 (1.1) 2.0 *** ¤¤ 23.866 p < .001 Vegetables 3.2 (1.3) 3.0 2.1 (1.0) 2.0 ** 1.4 (0.7) 1.0 *** ¤ 23.482 p < .001 Vehicles 4.0 (1.1) 4.0 2.6 (1.4) 3.0 *** 1.5 (1.2) 1.0 *** ¤ 32.347 p < .001 Animals 3.8 (1.0) 4.0 2.9 (1.2) 3.0 ** 1.5 (1.1) 1.0 *** ¤¤¤ 32.332 p < .001 All categories 14.7 (2.7) 15.0 10.4 (3.5) 10.0 *** 6.1 (3.4) 5.0 *** ¤¤¤ 43.130 p < .001 Note. * = p < .05, ** p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ p < .01, ¤¤¤ = p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. ‘pantihose’) were the third most often used subcategory in the group of miAD and moAD patients. In the moAD group, coats (e.g., ‘trenchcoat’, ‘fur coat’, ‘raincoat’) and footwear (e.g., ‘galoshes’, ‘boots’, ‘slippers’, ‘sports shoes’) were nonexistent as a distinct subcategory. The distribution of subcategories in the category of vegetables appeared somewhat dissimilar among the subject groups (Figure 2; see also Appendix 4B). When producing clusters of vegetables, the three most often utilized subcategories were the sprouts and greens (e.g., ‘cabbage’, ‘pumpkin’, ‘cauliflower’, ‘lettuce’), rootand tuberous vegetables (e.g., ‘potato’, ‘carrot’, ‘rutabaga’) and vegetables used for salads (e.g., ‘tomato’, ‘cucumber’, ‘lettuce’) in the NC group and the miAD group. The moAD group produced mostly words from the subcategories of sprouts and greens and root- and tuberous vegetables. The subcategory of herbs (e.g., ‘dill’, ‘parsley’), onions (e.g., ‘onion’, ‘garlic’), or vegetables used for salads did not belong to the repertoire of subcategories in the moAD group. 116 Results 1.4 NC Number of subcategories (M) 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 at co hi en ’s un cl de ot sh rw s ng s irt ea r ts tfi ou om w ou in td do oo or rc cl lo ot th he s es 0.0 Subcategories Figure 1. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of clothes in different subject groups. 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 es rri be es ic sp d rb s an ca bb ag es it fru he ro sp ro ut s gr an ee d ot ns an d ve tub ge er ta ou bl s es us v ed eg fo eta rs b al les ad s 0.0 Subcategories Figure 2. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of vegetables in different subject groups. Results 117 1.4 NC Number of subcategories (M) 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 m ea tra ns ns of po m rta as m us tio s cl n epo w ve er hi ed on -ro cles ad ve hi cl es bo at s an d sh ve ip hi s cl es on w he el s ve hi in cle w s in u te se rti d m ve e hi cl es on u w sed at er 0.0 Subcategories Figure 3. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of vehicles in different subject groups. 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 in se ct s ts pe s rd bi ts ro de n s al im w ild an an ot ic ex fa rm an i im m al al s s 0.0 Subcategories Figure 4. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of animals in different subject groups. 118 Results When the subject groups produced clusters of vehicles, they most often consisted of vehicles used in public transportation (‘bus’, ‘train’, ‘boat’, ‘plane’) and road traffic (‘bus’, ‘car’, ‘bike’, ‘motorcycle’), as well as of vehicles operated by muscle power (‘kickboard’, ‘kick sled’, ‘roller skates’, ‘a pair of roller skis’; Figure 3; see also Appendix 4C). Both the miAD and the moAD group lacked the subcategory of vehicles used during the winter (e.g., ‘kick sled’, ‘a pair of skis’, ‘snowmobile’, ‘reindeer sleigh’). Furthermore, the moAD group did not produce the subcategory of vehicles used on the water (e.g., ‘ship’, ‘canoe’, ‘boat’, ‘ferry’). However, a few items referring to winter vehicles (e.g., ‘a pair of skis’, ‘sleigh’) and water vehicles (e.g., ‘rowboat’, ‘motorboat’) were produced but they were embedded in other subcategories, such as vehicles operated by muscle power or vehicles referring to boats, or produced as single words outside a cluster. As for the category of animals, farm animals (e.g., ‘horse’, ‘cow’, ‘sheep’, ‘hen’, ‘dog’, ‘cat’), exotic or foreign animals (e.g., ‘whale’, ‘elephant’, ‘rhinoceros’), and wild animals (e.g., ‘hare’, ‘bear’, ‘wolf’, ‘fox’) were the three subcategories most often found in the NC and the miAD groups (Figure 4; see also Appendix 4D). In the group of moAD patients, the three most often used subcategories consisted of farm animals, wild animals, and pets (e.g., ‘cat’, ‘dog’). For both the miAD group and the moAD group, clusters of words denoting different types of fish (e.g., ‘pike’, ‘zander’, ‘perch’, ‘roach’) were non-existent as separate subcategories, unlike for the NC group. 9.2.5 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the nouns produced With regard to the total number of nouns produced for all the semantic categories, the degree of prototypicality did not differentiate between the subject groups (p > .05; see Table 15 and Appendix 2). The post hoc pair-wise analyses indicated that the words referring to vehicles were significantly more prototypical in the miAD group (p < .05) and the moAD group (p < .05) than in the NC group (see Appendix 5:12). For all other categories, the AD groups produced nouns with an equal degree of prototypicality as the NC group. The level of prototypicality of the words produced by the AD groups remained the same in all the semantic categories. The study indicated a significant difference among the subject groups in the overall frequency of nouns produced (p < .05; see Table 15 and Appendix 3), the moAD producing more commonly occurring words than the NC group (p < .01; see Appendix 5:13). A similar trend was found in the miAD group, as the difference between the miAD group and the NC group in the frequency ratings approached statistical significance (p < .06). The AD groups produced words with the same frequency of occurrence. At the level of individual semantic categories, words with the equal frequency of occurrence were produced for the category of clothes by all subject groups but with significantly different frequencies for vegetables and animals (in each, p < .05), Results 119 Table 15. Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the nouns produced NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Clothes 5.49 (.46) 5.51 5.41(.36) 5.41 5.12 (1.82) 5.70 1.218 p = 544, n.s. Vegetables 5.71 (.28) 5.74 5.68 (.32) 5.62 5.63 (.20) 5.64 3.538 p = .170, n.s. Vehicles 5.63 (.43) 5.65 5.98 (.51) 6.01 * 6.7 (.74) 6.06 * 8.603 p < .05 Animals 6.50 (.22) 6.60 6.60 (.31) 6.68 6.21 (1.49) 6.57 1.729 p = .421, n.s. All nouns 5.84 (0.19) 5.86 5.91 (0.26) 5.95 5.76 (0.59) 5.89 1.692 p = .429, n.s. Clothes 4.73 (.31) 4.67 4.78 (.33) 4.79 4.48 (1.62) 4.90 2.256 p = .324, n.s. Vegetables 4.56 (.27) 4.58 4.46 (1.15) 4.57 4.93 (.45) 4.91 ** 7.433 p < .05 Vehicles 4.63 (.30) 4.65 4.96 (.50) 5.00 * 5.14 (.59) 5.12 *** 14.117 p < .001 Animals 4.64 (.27) 4.54 4.86 (.39) 4.90 * 4.64 (1.23) 5.03 (*) 6.513 p < .05 All nouns 4.89 (0.18) 4.90 5.00 (0.35) 5.06 (*) 5.09 (0.35) 5.07 ** 8.777 p < .05 Variable Degree of prototypicality of nouns Degree of frequency of nouns Note. Judgements were made on a 7-point scale: 1 = a very poor example of a category / a very infrequent word, 7 = a very good example of a category / a very frequent word. (*) almost statistically significant, * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. 120 Results as well as for vehicles (p < .001). The post hoc pair-wise analyses revealed that, compared to the NC group, the miAD group produced more frequent words for the categories of vehicles and animals (in each, p < .05). The moAD group produced more frequent words than the NC group for the categories of vegetables (p < .01) and vehicles (p < .001). The frequencies of the words in the category of animals between the NC and the moAD group were nearly significant (p < .06). The AD groups produced words with the same frequency level in all semantic categories. 9.2.6 Summary of the results and discussion The analysis of the contents of the responses showed that each subject group, including the normal control subjects, occasionally erred when producing nouns for the semantic categories. Errors were not made by just a few individuals in each group but by many of them (see 9.2.2). Compared to the performance of the NC group, the proportion of erroneous responses in the miAD group was significantly higher in all categories except vegetables, whereas the moAD group made significantly more errors in all semantic categories. The finding of a remarkable increase in errors in the performance of the AD patients supports the findings of Ober et al. (1986) and Bayles et al. (1993) on the animal and fruit fluency task. The finding stands in contrast to the results of Binetti et al. (1995) and Carew et al. (1997), who reported that practically no errors occurred in the performance of healthy control subjects and mild and moderate-to-severe AD patients on the animal fluency task. In the present study, the proportion of errors did not differentiate between the miAD and the moAD group in any individual category, which is in accordance with the finding of Binetti et al. (1995). However, the total proportion of correct responses indicated that, throughout the whole task, the moAD group was significantly more likely to make errors than the miAD group. The present finding is consistent with the study of Bayles et al. (1993) who also discovered a relationship between the severity of dementia and the increase in the overall error rate. In this study, the errors were divided further into intrusions and perseverations. Such phonological errors, which would have made the interpretation of the target word impossible, were non-existent, and minor phonological changes in the output were not counted as errors. Very few intrusions were produced by all the subject groups, whereas the number of perseveration was significantly higher in the miAD and the moAD group than in the control group. Furthermore, the ability of the patients in the miAD and the moAD group to generate words from a varying set of semantic dimensions was significantly reduced relative to the subjects in the NC group. Intrusions Even though intrusions were made by many individual subjects, the proportion of intrusions remained very low in all subject groups and most of the intrusions were semantically related to the given category (see 9.2.2). Only in the category of clothes did the moAD group produce more nouns outside the category boundaries Results 121 than either the NC group or the miAD group. The finding that very few intrusions emerged in the fluency performance of both healthy adults and AD patients is in accordance with the results reported by Diesfeldt (1985), Rosser and Hodges (1994), Binetti et al. (1995), Carew et al. (1997), as well as Suhr and Jones (1998). These studies employed several different semantic categories (see Table 4). The findings of the present study, concerning the number of intrusions made by the moAD group for the category of clothes, also partly support those discovered by Ober et al. (1986) who reported higher proportions of intrusions among the mild and the moderate-tosevere AD patients relative to healthy control subjects in the combined fluency scores from animal and fruit fluency tasks. The present study also supports the finding of Tröster et al. (1989) and Beatty et al. (1997, 2000) who noticed that AD patients generated a higher proportion of intrusions in the supermarket fluency task than the control subjects. Tröster et al. also pointed out that intrusions were more typical of the performance in the group of moderately demented AD patients, which is in accordance with the present study. The finding implies that, depending on the category, both mildly and moderately demented AD patients tended to produce nouns according to the instruction and to stay within the limits of the category boundaries. This indicates relatively intact categorization processes in terms of successful convergence and disambiguation of appropriate semantic features corresponding to the given categories, which is a prerequisite for the production of category members in the semantic fluency task (see 3.1, 3.1.1, 3.1.2, and 3.2). Consequently, knowledge of category membership may have remained relatively intact for the AD patients, which lends support to the findings of Martin and Fedio (1983), Hodges et al. (1992), and Chertkow and Bub (1990), among others. Nevertheless, the intrusions produced by the moAD group for the category of clothes may have implications for assuming that the activation in the semantic memory does not always work well for patients with moderate AD (see Persson 1995:125-130, 132-135, 177-182; Astell & Harley 1996; Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997; see 10.1.3). This finding, on the other hand, also supports the ones made by Grossman et al. (1996), Laatu et al. (1997), and Laine, Vuorinen et al. (1997) who observed that AD patients had difficulty in apprehending and identifying superordinate category knowledge (see 2.4.1). Once off-category words were produced in this study, they mostly originated from semantically very closely related categories, such as accessories (e.g., ‘suspenders’) and linen for the category of clothes, berries, and fruit for the category of vegetables, and machines and tools for the category of vehicles. The semantically related intrusions produced for the category of animals were more general nouns without a specific referent, such as ‘buzzer’ referring to the sound of some flying insect(s) or ‘the snake of Eden’. The number of semantically unrelated errors was very low in all subject groups. The occurrence of intrusions can be explained in relation to the functioning of the semantic memory and/or the process of the semantic layer of the mental lexicon during spoken word production (see chap. 4). During word production, the activation 122 Results first spreads along the semantic network and simultaneously triggers features of both the target and the semantically related items, the activation of which may cause noise in the system. Due to this background activation, incorrect features may have higher activation levels than correct ones, or their decay rate may be increased, as a consequence of which they are more likely to get selected and thus replace the correct targets (Dell 1986; Persson 1995:35-39, 181; Foygel & Dell 2000). Alternatively, damage to or loss of the target features, or their connections to other features, may cause semantically related features to become more easily activated and selected for further processing (Hinton & Sejnowski 1986; Persson 1995:80-84, 132-135; Devlin et al. 1998; see 10.1.3). Selection of more general names for objects may reflect an impaired spreading of activation which cannot pick out the subordinate items by integrating their fine-grained semantic information, but settles for a more general target on the superordinate level which has a sparser set of features. Alternatively, the activation may settle for a nominal substitution that describes one strongly shared perceptual characteristic, for example, the sound of the target (see 3.2, and chap. 6; see also Barsalou 1982; Persson 1995:178). The present study lends support to the notion that due to a similar semanticpragmatic structure, some but not all of the category boundaries may appear fuzzy or to some extent overlapping even in healthy elderly adults (see e.g., Rosch et al. 1976; Rosch 1978; Lakoff 1987a, b; Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Aitchison 1994:39-50; see 3.1.1). For example, some of the machines and tools (e.g., ‘roller’) may have many shared features with vehicles (e.g., the big size, the ability to move, and the use for example on the road) but relatively few distinguishing features (Moss et al. 2002; see 3.2). Similarly, vegetables and fruit share semantic features denoting their physical appearance (e.g., colour, shape), function, and use (e.g., to grow, to nourish), as well as thematic information in terms of a common context of appearance in the environment (e.g., a garden, a greenhouse, a grocery store, or a market place), but they have very few distinguishing features, which may make them difficult to differentiate at the point of word selection. Chertkow and Bub (1990) reported that the healthy elderly control subjects, as well as AD patients, erred in differentiating fruit from vegetables. Nevertheless, the study of Vinson and Vigliocco (2002) showed that undergraduate students made a clear distinction between fruit and vegetables. On the other hand, Capitani et al. (1999) reported gender and age differences among the healthy control subjects in the fluency performance, in which female participants produced more fruit than male participants, and younger participants produced more words than older participants. In the present study, however, gender was not found to affect word production in any of the noun categories among the normal control subjects (see also Crossley et al. 1997 and Troyer 2000). Consequently, the tendency of some of the healthy elderly control subjects to produce intrusions might thus be explained by their advancing age making them less specific about the feature disambiguation of semantically related items, which can be shown as fuzziness at the borders of Results 123 certain semantic categories. However, Bayles et al. (1993) did not find a significant effect of age on the number or rate of intrusions in the animal or fruit fluency task. As far as artefacts made of a piece of cloth are concerned, they tend to share the physical structure in the form of the material (e.g., silk, cotton, wool), the fabric (cloth made by weaving, knitting, etc.), and their colour. However, the functional and thematic features, as well as some features denoting their physical appearance (e.g., shape), commonly divide these items into distinguishable groups, such as garments for the body (i.e., ‘clothes’ in English, ‘vaatteet’ in Finnish, e.g., ‘shirt’, ‘pants’, ‘coat’), things to decorate and protect the inside of the house (‘wall hanging’, ‘carpet’, ‘curtains’), and things to use for bedding (e.g., ‘blanket’, ‘sheets’) and personal hygiene (e.g., ‘towel’). This study indicated that, to some extent, all subject groups included semantically related items under the term ‘clothes’, which is consistent with the finding of Diesfeldt (1985) who observed that the category of clothes was prone to some fuzziness also among the healthy elderly adults. The present study showed that in the category of clothes, the correct selection of words in the moAD group might have been hampered by noise caused by semantically related items being simultaneously activated. The activation of features may have been guided by the strongly shared physical properties (e.g., the material the items were made of) instead of other features. Consequently, a changed or damaged pattern of semantic feature activation and integration, leading to difficulty in specifying the category boundary of clothes, may have taken place in the moAD group who produced significantly more incorrect words that denoted items semantically related to clothing (see 10.1.3). Disambiguating between the different categories made of cloth may be different from distinguishing other man-made categories in the sense that disambiguation may be even more heavily dependent on the thematic and functional features, which, in general, are likely to be more lightly weighted and thus less discriminative than features denoting physical properties (see Persson 1995:82; Tyler and Moss 2001). It is also possible that the phonological form of the word ‘vaatteet’ may have interfered with the feature selection. The selection of items may have been based on the shared stem ‘vaatteet’, such as ‘liinavaatteet’ (‘linen’), ‘vuodevaatteet’ (‘bedclothes’), etc., as a consequence of which semantically related words were produced (see 4.1, 5.3, and 10.1.3). The findings of the present study imply that all semantic categories may not be similarly exploitable during the semantic fluency task (see also 9.1.1). The semantic characteristics of the category of animals are likely to be so clear cut and robust against neural damage that semantically related intrusions hardly emerge, whereas categories such as vegetables and clothes seem to be more vulnerable to category violations, due to their few distinguishing features and their close relatedness to other semantic categories in terms of shared features (see Moss et al. 2002; see also 10.1.3). 124 Results Perseverations Concerning the production of perseverations, this study indicated that both AD groups repeated previously produced words more excessively than the control group (see 9.2.2). Production of perseverations in both AD groups, however, did not form a constant pattern across the semantic categories. Both AD groups produced more perseverations than the NC group for the category of vehicles and the category of animals. As far as the category of clothes was concerned, only the miAD group perseverated significantly more often than the NC group, whereas in the category of vegetables, it was the moAD group who perseverated significantly more frequently than the NC group. The finding that the AD patients made more perseverative errors than the normal control subjects supports the studies of Rosser and Hodges (1994), Beatty et al. (1997, 2000), and Suhr and Jones (1998) who used several different semantic categories such as supermarket items, animals, vehicles, musical instruments, fruit/vegetables, and tools/kitchen utensils (see Table 4). However, Butters et al. (1987), Binetti et al. (1995) and Carew et al. (1997) reported that perseverations seldom occurred among the normal control subjects or among the AD patients in the animal fluency task. The present study also indicated that there was no difference between the number of perseverations produced by the two AD groups with different degrees of dementia, which parallels the observations made by Tröster et al. (1989) on the supermarket fluency task. In the present study, the proportion of perseverations of all the produced words in the group of healthy control subjects varied between 1.3-4.9% across the semantic categories. In total, the proportion of all perseverations was 3.1% (SD = 1.8), which seems to be consistent with the study of Tröster et al. (1989), who reported the rate of perseverations among normal control subjects to be 3.0% (SD = 0.04) in the supermarket fluency task, that is somewhat higher than the 1.4% (SD = 2.4) reported in the study of Suhr and Jones (1998). The average proportion of perseverations varied between 9.6-14.7% across the categories in the mild AD group and between 5.8-27.4% in the moderate AD group. The total proportion of perseverations was 12.3% (SD = 10.9) for the mild and 21.0% (SD = 15.0) for the moderate AD group. The rates seem to be higher than AD patients’ average of 7.7% in the Rosser and Hodges’s (1994) study and 6.3% (SD = 9.1) in the Suhr and Jones’s (1998) study. The number of perseverations in the AD groups in the present study was also higher than in the study of Tröster et al. (1989; see also Ober et al. 1986) who observed that the rate of the perseverations in their mild AD group was 9.0% (SD = 12.0) and 7.0% (SD = 10.0) in their moderate AD group. All in all, this study showed that the word production system might not work well in AD. Instead of activating the present or preparing to activate or prime the future, the word production system of the AD patients seemed to “dwell on the past” and it was not able to turn off the activation of the words already produced in the fluency task (see Dell 1986; Dell, Burger et al. 1997). Generally, if the previously activated words do not decay, the activation gets stuck on particular patterns and the Results 125 system starts perseverating, consequently blocking the shifting from one processing pattern to another and interfering with the activation and the selection of other words (Dell 1986; Dell, Burger et al. 1997; Martin et al. 1994; Persson 1995:66; see the discussion in Laine 1989:75-80). As discussed above, the tendency of the AD patients to produce many perseverations can have an effect on the fluency performance that can be manifested as an increase in the number of clusters and the size of clusters in individual semantic categories. However, there was no general trend to support the notion. The role of the perseverations in word production is further discussed in 9.4.5 and 10.1.2. Strategies The high proportion of words in clusters (see Table 9; see also 9.1.3) implies that production of nouns was guided by a strategy of tying them together by some relation (semantic, phonological, or both). The subject groups tended to produce clusters using mostly the pure semantic strategy, that is, the activation of nouns was most often guided by shared semantic properties without the contribution of a phonological component. However, phonological similarity between the words also seemed to have an effect on word production. Some semantically related nouns frequently produced by the subjects seemed to share some phonological features (e.g., ‘kissa’, ‘koira’, ‘kukko’, ‘kana’ / ‘cat’, ‘dog’, ‘rooster’, ‘hen’ or ‘porkkana’, ‘peruna’, ‘punajuuri’ / ‘carrot’, ‘potato’, ‘beet’). The finding supports the study of Laine (1989:22), Bayles et al. (1989), Roberts and Le Dorze (1994), as well as Kaleva and Vanhala (2001). The pure phonemic strategy, in which the nouns did not share a close semantic relatedness, other than the category membership (e.g., ‘kissa’, ‘kirppu’, ‘kala’ / ‘cat’, ‘flea’, ‘fish’), was used relatively little by all subject groups. In other words, the performance of both the NC group and the AD groups during the semantic fluency task was guided by a spread of activation to semantically and phonologically related items in the mental lexicon. The finding supports the two-stage interactive activation models of word production (see 4.2). Semantic subcategories The present study indicated that the normal control subjects showed a more dynamic, flexible, integrative, and creative retrieval of semantic information by being able to activate more diverse semantic dimensions in production than either of the AD groups (see 9.2.4). The finding that AD patients produced fewer subcategories than the NC subjects is in accordance with the findings reported by Martin and Fedio (1983), Ober et al. (1986), Tröster et al. (1989), Binetti et al. (1995), and Beatty et al. (2000). Furthermore, a clear narrowing of the variety of semantic dimensions was found in the moAD group relative to the miAD group. This finding supports the study of Binetti et al. (1995) who observed that in the animal fluency task, mild AD patients were able to generate category exemplars for farm animals, wild animals, and birds, whereas moderate-to-severe AD patients produced words only for farm animals. In 126 Results contrast, Tröster et al. (1989) did not find any difference between patients with mild and moderate AD in their ability to generate subcategories in the supermarket fluency task. In all subject groups, the formation of clusters of nouns seemed primarly to be based on functional and thematic features between nouns rather than on physical features or hierarchical relations of taxonomic subordination (e.g., strict zoological or botanical relations), which were mainly used to guide word selection by the subjects in the NC group and to some extent by the subjects in the miAD group (see Figures 1 - 4). The semantic dimensions used by the subjects in the moAD group were restricted mainly to functional and thematic relations. Activating functional and thematic features (e.g., contextual and spatial features) seemed to guide activation and selection of words in all categories: clothes to wear daily indoors and outdoors or on certain occasions, vegetables that tend to occur together in a salad or at the dining table (e.g., ‘lettuce’, ‘cucumber’ and ‘tomato’) or that grow in, on the surface, or above the soil, vehicles used by the public in an urban environment or on the water, in the air, or on the roads, and animals to be found in the wilderness, at home or in a foreign country, on a farm, in a zoo, etc. Temporal features, such as the time of the year certain things are most likely to appear, were also used as the strategy to activate words for production. For example, clothes were clustered as winter and summer clothes and vehicles as ones that could be used in the snow or on the open water. Words denoting vehicles were at times also clustered according to the manner or initiator of the movement, that is, whether transportation was carried out by muscle power or by a motor. Physical features, such as part-whole analysis, were used when clustering words mainly for clothes by specifying the part of the body the item is used for (e.g., upper body, lower body, head, hands, feet). Other physical features, such as color or size, were very rarely used as a criterion for the cluster formation. In the moAD group, the lack of taxonomic subordinate clusters (e.g., words for different fish) may be an indication of a reduction of their ability to utilize the semantic information of nouns based on the fine-grained semantic analysis of the distinguishing features (e.g., physical features) among the items. This finding tentatively supports the study of Binetti et al. (1995) who suggested that a bottom-up deterioration of the semantic features may take place in AD, which may lead to a loss of, or a difficulty in disambiguating and integrating the defining features of words and, subsequently, to a difficulty in naming items in a subcategory (see also Grober et al. 1985; Hodges et al. 1992; Chan et al. 1993; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; Laatu et al. 1997; Laatu 1999; Moss et al. 2002; Whatmough & Chertkow 2002). On the other hand, the ability to integrate information according to the functional and thematic features may be easier or better preserved in AD. The functional and thematic information may allow cross-classification over different subcategories, and they may thus be more salient, easier, and over-represented for the AD patients (see Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Barsalou 1982, 1983; Lucariello et al. 1992; Persson 1995:178; Nelson 1996:230-248; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002; see also 3.1.2, and 3.2). Results 127 Reduction in the flexible use of semantic information, however, does not necessarily imply a breakdown of the structure of semantic memory or an inability of the AD patients to retrieve clusters of specific subordinate nouns. In the present study, the words produced by some of the subjects even with moderate dementia consisted of specific subordinate nouns which may have been either embedded in certain functional or thematic clusters, or were produced as a “strictly” taxonomic subordinate cluster (e.g., birds or fish), or as a single word. Hence, the thematic and functional clusters outnumbering the others may reflect the methodological solution to combine smaller clusters into bigger ones (see 8.2.3), as a consequence of which larger clusters may cover chains of words formed using various semantic dimensions. Thus, one should attempt to break down these clusters in order to analyze the true performance of the subjects in different semantic dimensions (see Chan et al. 1993; Carew et al. 1997). Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the nouns produced The qualitative analysis revealed that the subject groups tended to use words with equal prototypicality (see 9.2.5). Differences emerged only in the category of vehicles, for which both AD groups produced more prototypical words than the NC group. The present study thus only partially supports Beatty et al. (2000) who found that on the supermarket fluency task, AD patients generated a greater proportion of prototypical words than the normal control subjects (see also 5.1). The study indicated that the mean frequency of nouns produced by the AD groups was higher than that of the NC group in all other categories but the category of clothes. The finding implies that the words used by the AD patients tended mainly to be very commonly occurring words that were likely to have strongly connected patterns of feature integration and a low threshold of activation, which made them easily available for word production. The finding is consistent with Weingartner et al. (1993) and Binetti et al. (1995). On the other hand, the finding contrasts those made by Ober et al. (1986), Chan et al. (1993), and Goldstein et al. (1996) who did not find a difference in the word frequencies between healthy control subjects and patients with AD (see also 5.1). On the basis of the present study, it can be concluded that the higher frequency of nouns may have affected word production in the AD groups, whereas the degree of prototypicality did not contribute to their semantic fluency performance. It can also be speculated that the nature of the semantic category may have had some impact on the frequency and prototypicality of the nouns produced by the AD groups. For a more detailed discussion on the methodological issues, see 10.2.3. 128 Results 9.3 Overall performance on the verb fluency tasks The subject groups were statistically significantly different in their overall ability to produce responses for the verb generation task (p < .001; see Table 16). During the task, the NC group produced a total mean of 40 responses for the four semantic verb categories, while the miAD group was able to generate 33 and the moAD group only 19 responses. The post hoc pair-wise analyses revealed that, compared to the NC group, a significant reduction in the word production emerged in the miAD group (p < .01) and the moAD group (p < .001). The moAD group’s word production was significantly poorer than that of the miAD group (p < .01). More detailed data on the post hoc pair-wise analyses concerning the total verb production can be found in Appendix 6:1. When generating words for the individual verb categories, the NC group’s average performance varied between 8 and 12 responses, the miAD group’s between 7 and 9 responses, and the moAD group was able to give 4 to 5 responses per each category (see Table 16). The difference in word production was significant among the groups in all the semantic categories (in each category, p < .001). The miAD group produced fewer responses than the NC group only for the categories of playing sports and construction (in each, p < .01). The moAD group performed significantly poorer than the NC group and the miAD group in all verb categories (see Appendix 6:1). 9.3.1 Number of correct verbs Incorrect responses were found in all subject groups and in all semantic categories (see Table 16; see also 9.4.1, 9.4.2, and 9.4.5). There was a significant difference among the groups in the total number of all correctly produced verbs (p < .001). The post hoc pair-wise analyses indicated a remarkable reduction in the total number of correct verbs in the miAD group and the moAD group (in both, p < .001), compared to the NC group (see Appendix 6:2a). On the whole, the miAD group performed significantly better than the moAD group (p < .001). A reduction of correct words in the AD groups was found also at the level of individual categories. While the NC group produced 7 to 11 correct words per category, the miAD was able to produce about 6 to 7, and the moAD group about 2 to 3 correct verbs per category. A significant difference among the groups appeared in all semantic categories (in each, p < .001). The post hoc analyses revealed significant differences in the number of correct words between the NC group and the miAD group in all other categories but the category of cleaning (preparing food p < .05, playing sports and construction p < .001). For each semantic category, the moAD was able to generate significantly fewer correct verbs than the NC group and the miAD group (see Appendix 6:2a). According to the average frequencies, all subject groups produced most correct responses for the category of playing sports (see Table 16). The fewest correct Results 129 Table 16. Total number of words and number of correct verbs produced in the verb fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Preparing food 9.4 (2.9) 9.0 8.5 (5.2) 8.0 4.8 (3.7) 4.0 *** ¤ 16.073 p < .001 Playing sports 11.4 (3.3) 11.5 8.5 (3.8) 7.0 ** 5.0 (3.0) 4.0 *** ¤¤ 29.005 p < .001 Construction 10.6 (3.1) 10.5 7.8 (4.9) 7.0 ** 4.7 (3.6) 4.0 *** ¤ 24.744 p < .001 Cleaning 8.6 (2.5) 8.0 8.1 (4.0) 8.0 4.3 (3.4) 4.0 *** ¤¤ 21.858 p < .001 All categories 40.0 (8.7) 39.0 32.8 (16.8) 30.0 ** 18.8 (11.5) 15.5 *** ¤¤ 28.250 p < .001 Preparing food 8.6 (2.7) 8.0 6.6 (4.7) 6.0 * 2.6 (2.2) 2.0 *** ¤¤¤ 33.488 p < .001 Playing sports 10.9 (3.5) 11.0 6.9 (3.5) 6.0 *** 3.5 (2.9) 3.0 *** ¤¤ 34.822 p < .001 Construction 9.1 (3.2) 9.0 5.6 (3.8) 5.0 *** 3.1 (2.8) 2.0 *** ¤ 28.388 p < .001 Cleaning 7.5 (2.7) 7.0 6.5 (3.2) 6.0 3.5 (3.1) 2.5 *** ¤¤¤ 23.925 p < .001 All categories 36.0 (8.8) 35.5 25.5 (13.9) 21.0 *** 12.6 (8.7) 9.5 *** ¤¤¤ 36.118 p < .001 Total number of words Number of correct verbs Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. 130 Results Table 17. Number of correct verbs produced by the male and female participants in the NC group Male participants Female participants (n = 16) (n = 14) Category M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn Mann Whitney U test Preparing food 7.7 (1.7) 8.0 9.6 (3.3) 9.0 U = 81.5 p = .208, n.s. Playing sports 11.1 (3.4) 12.0 10.6 (3.6) 11.0 U = 97.0 p = .552, n.s. Construction 10.2 (2.9) 10.5 7.8 (3.2) 7.5 U = 63.0 p < .05 * Cleaning 6.3 (1.1) 6.0 8.9 (3.4) 8.5 U = 47.0 p < .01 ** All categories 35.3 (7.2) 35.0 36.9 (10.6) 36.5 U = 100.5 p = .637, n.s. Note: * = p < .05, ** = p < .01. n.s. = non significant. responses were produced for the category of cleaning in the NC group, for the category of construction in the miAD group, and for the category of preparing food in the moAD group. The Friedman test indicated that there was a statistically significant difference among the number of correct responses produced for the different semantic categories only in the NC group (χ2 = 20.7 (df = 3), p < .001). The post hoc pair-wise comparisons revealed a statistically significant difference in the number of correct responses between the category of playing sports and the category of cleaning (see Appendix 6:2b). There was a statistically significant difference between the male and female participants in the NC group in the number of correct words produced for the category of construction (p < .05) and the category of cleaning (p < .01; see Table 17). For the former, the male participants produced significantly more responses than the female participants, whereas for the latter, it was the female participants who produced significantly more correct verbs than the male participants. However, there was no statistically significant difference in the average mean number of correct verbs produced for all the four verb categories. Results 131 9.3.2 Clustering and switching A significant group difference emerged in the overall switching performance among the subject groups (p < .001; see Table 18). The NC group was remarkably more flexible at changing the subcategories during word production than the miAD group and the moAD group (in both, p < .001; see Appendix 6:3). Moreover, the miAD group was able to switch between the subcategories more often then the moAD group (p < .01). The NC group switched subcategories about 4 to 5 times in each category, the miAD group from 2 to 4 times, and the moAD group were able to shift only once or twice in a category. The miAD group switched significantly less frequently than the NC group when producing verbs for the categories of playing sports and construction (in both, p < .001). In any category, the switching performance of the moAD group was significantly poorer than that of the NC group (preparing food, playing sports, and construction p < .001, and cleaning p < .01) and the miAD (in all categories, p < .05). The NC group produced, on average, 10 clusters during the whole action fluency task, the miAD group 8, and the moAD group only 4 clusters (see Table 18). The total number of all clusters differed significantly among the subject groups (p < .001), as well as between the groups (NC vs. miAD, p < .01; NC vs. moAD and miAD vs. moAD, p < .001; see Appendix 6:4). The NC group produced from 2 to 3 clusters per verb category, the miAD group approximately 2, and the moAD group only 1 cluster for each category. Compared to the NC group, the miAD group produced significantly fewer clusters for the categories of playing sports and construction (in each, p < .05). The difference in the number of clusters produced by the groups for the category of preparing food was almost statistically significant (p = .078), whereas in the category of cleaning the number of clusters did not differentiate between the two groups. The moAD group formed significantly fewer clusters than the NC group and the miAD group for all categories (see Appendix 6:4). As far as the total performance was concerned, it was noticed that all subject groups produced clusters with the size of approximately 1, implying that the clusters contained mainly two semantically related verbs (see Table 18). At the level of individual verb categories, the NC group and the miAD group were able to produce verbs in clusters the size of which varied between 1 and 2 (i.e., containing 2 or 3 words). In the moAD group, the cluster size was smaller than 1 in all other categories but the category of construction. A group difference in the cluster size was found for the category of preparing food (p < .05), for which both the NC group and the miAD group formed clusters of the same size but significantly larger than the moAD group (NC vs. moAD, p < .01; miAD vs. moAD, p < .05; see Appendix 6:5). The cluster size differed among the groups also in the category of cleaning (p < .05), with the NC group producing significantly larger clusters than the moAD group (p < .01). The size of the clusters did not differ among the subject groups in the categories of playing sports and construction. 132 Results Table 18. Clustering and switching in the verb fluency tasks Category NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value 3.9 (1.5) 4.0 5.8 (2.6) 5.0 3.9 (1.6) 4.0 3.4 (2.1) 4.0 17.0 (4.8) 16.5 3.3 (2.7) 3.0 3.3 (2.0) 3.5 *** 2.3 (2.7) 2.0 *** 3.4 (2.3) 3.5 12.2 (7.3) 11.5 *** 2.0 (2.4) 1.5 *** ¤ 1.9 (1.6) 2.0 *** ¤ 1.1 (1.4) 1.0 *** ¤ 1.7 (2.2) 1.0 ** ¤ 6.6 (5.9) 6.5 *** ¤¤ 12.177 p < .01 29.810 p < .001 30.638 p < .001 9.168 p < .05 28.000 p < .001 2.6 (0.9) 2.0 2.9 (1.3) 3.0 2.7 (1.2) 3.0 2.1 (1.1) 2.0 10.2 (2.6) 10.0 2.1 (1.4) 2.0 (*) 2.0 (1.5) 1.5 * 2.0 (1.5) 2.0 * 1.8 (1.1) 2.0 7.8 (4.4) 7.0 ** 0.7 (0.9) 0.5 *** ¤¤¤ 1.1 (1.1) 1.0 *** ¤ 1.0 (1.0) 1.0 *** ¤ 1.1 (1.2) 1.0 *** ¤ 3.9 (2.7) 3.0 *** ¤¤¤ 27.884 p < .001 19.271 p < .001 20.059 p < .001 12.978 p < .001 32.118 p < .001 1.0 0.8 0.8 0.6 1.3 1.2 1.6 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.1 0.8 1.6 1.3 1.3 0.7 1.0 0.8 0.9 (1.7) 0.1 ** ¤ 0.6 (0.8) 0.4 1.7 (2.3) 1.0 0.7 (1.0) 0.3 ** 0.9 (1.0) 0.6 8.614 p < .05 4.648 p = .098, n.s. 1.417 p = .492, n.s. 6.759 p < .05 4.772 p = .092, n.s. 7.421 p < .05 3.186 p = .203, n.s. 4.314 p = .116, n.s. 3.258 p = .763, n.s. 5.011 p = .082, n.s. Switches Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning All categories Number of clusters Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning All categories Cluster size Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning All categories (0.5) (0.6) (0.8) (1.6) (0.3) (0.8) (1.1) (1.7) (1.5) (0.6) Verbs in clusters (%) Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning All categories 74.3 70.7 64.7 66.7 62.8 62.0 74.0 80.0 72.9 72.7 (16.7) (22.9) (14.8) (25.4) (9.7) 69.1 73.2 69.9 68.3 72.2 72.1 63.1 71.4 71.4 72.3 (29.4) (24.0) (19.4) (31.3) (14.7) 42.7 (38.6) 41.7 ** ¤ 48.8 (36.1) 61.9 69.6 (31.4) 66.7 57.1 (36.2) 58.3 60.1 (24.3) 59.7 Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. Results 133 When the proportion of clustered verbs was taken into account, the difference among the groups in the overall clustering performance across the four categories approached statistical significance (p = .082; see Table 18). On average, the NC group and the miAD group clustered 70% of all verbs. In the moAD group, the proportion of clustered verbs was approximately 60%. There was no significant difference among the subject groups in the extent of clustering verbs in the single categories of playing sports, construction, and cleaning. Instead, the groups differed in the category of preparing food (p < .05), for which the NC and the miAD group clustered words to a similar extent but significantly more often than the moAD group (NC vs. moAD, p < .01; miAD vs. moAD, p < .05; see Appendix 6:6). 9.3.3 Summary of the results and discussion The present study provided new information about how verbs were produced in the verb fluency tasks by normal control subjects, as well as by AD patients. Relative to the NC group, a significant reduction in the total number of responses and the total number of correct verbs was found in both AD groups. The moAD group showed a remarkable reduction in verb production, also when compared to the miAD group. Although the strategic use of clustering and switching was limited in number in both AD groups, they indicated a semantically coherent pattern of verb activation. Overall performance on the verb fluency tasks The total number of correct responses produced by the NC group for the different categories varied between 7 and 11 verbs. A significant difference appeared in the number of correct verbs generated for the semantic categories by the NC group. Most of them were produced for the category of playing sports, while the least responses were found in the category of cleaning (see 9.3.1). Such a variation in the semantic fluency performance between the categories was not found in the AD groups, both of which tended to generate correct words evenly for the different semantic categories. The verb production of the NC group seemed to be poorer than that of elderly control subjects in the studies of Piatt, Fields, Paolo, Koller, and Tröster (1999) and Piatt, Fields, Paolo, and Tröster (1999). In the former study, the healthy subject group (mean age 71.4 years, SD = 6.5) produced on average 17 (SD = 4.7) verbs and in the latter study (mean age of the subjects 72.9, SD = 7.5) approximately 15 (SD = 4.3) verbs for the verb fluency task (60 s). It is worth noting, however, that none of the tasks used by Piatt et al. was semantically restricted but the subjects were asked to generate as many single verbs as possible denoting different things people do. Given this constraint, the task used in their study was likely to activate words from a number of various semantic fields, whereas the categories given in the present study were restricted, which may explain the difference between the studies. 134 Results Different from the performance on the noun categories (see 9.1.1, 9.1.3), the semantic fluency performance of the NC group in some of the categories appeared to depend on the gender of the subjects: the female participants produced significantly more correct verbs for the category of cleaning than the male participants, whereas the opposite was true for the category of construction. The finding may be explained by the fact that some types of actions may be more familiar to females while some others are more associated with males, due to different life experiences, habits, occupations, etc. (see Monsch et al. 1992; Aitchison 1994:39-50; Taylor 1995:72-75, 79, 242; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:14-20; Azuma et al. 1997; Roberts & Le Dorze 1997; Capitani et al. 1999; see 5.1 and 5.5). However, when the average of the total output over all the verb categories was considered, the male and female subjects produced correct verbs to a similar extent (for nouns, see Crossley et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 2000). The gender effect was not controlled for the AD patients because of the unequal number of the male and female subjects in each AD group. Nevertheless, based on the finding obtained from the NC group, there may not be a general trend for the female participants to fare better on the verb fluency tasks than the male participants. Compared to the NC group, the miAD group showed a significant reduction in verb production only in two of the categories, playing sports and construction, but the moAD group produced significantly fewer words across all the semantic verb categories. Their performance in each semantic category was significantly poorer also when compared to the miAD group. The decreased verb production in the AD groups is in accordance with those obtained in studies involving the script generation task (a variation of the verb fluency task) conducted by Weingartner et al. (1983) and Grafman et al. (1991). In their studies, subjects were asked to produce a script of actions, which was likely to take place in a certain temporal-causal order (e.g., going to a restaurant; see 3.3.4, 5.1). Reduced verb production among AD patients was also found in verb confrontation naming in the studies of Bowles et al. (1987), Robinson et al. (1996), White-Devine et al. (1995, 1996), and Williamson et al. (1998; see also 2.4.2). One should note here that the control tasks of the present study, which included two confrontation naming tasks, indicated a significantly poorer naming of verbs in the AD groups compared to the NC group (see 9.6.1, 9.6.4). Clustering and switching This study also suggested that a systematic strategy for performing the semantic fluency task by producing various clusters of semantically related verbs and switching between semantic subcategories was, in general, less efficient in both AD groups than in the NC group. However, at the level of individual categories, the miAD group showed an ease of clustering and switching in two of the categories (preparing food and cleaning, see 9.3.2). The moAD group, in contrast, had significantly more difficulty in clustering and switching than the other groups throughout all the categories. The miAD group showed a better ability to cluster and switch than the moAD group. Results 135 The average cluster size appeared to be relatively small in all subject groups. The clusters between the subject groups were of the same size in all categories but two. For the categories of preparing food and cleaning, the moAD patients formed significantly smaller clusters than the NC group. Relative to the miAD group, the cluster size of the moAD group appeared to be smaller only in the category of cleaning. The number of perseverations made by the AD groups may have partly increased their cluster size (see Table 20, see also 9.4.2, 9.4.5, and 10.1.2). Nevertheless, even though the moAD group produced more perseverations for the category of preparing food, their cluster size remained smaller than that of the NC group. The small number of clusters and the small cluster size (approximately two words) generated by all subject groups for all semantic verb categories may be a reflection of the shallow and bushy hierarchical organization of the verbs (see Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Pajunen 1998, 2001:60-61; see also 3.3.2). Instead of having a well-defined, multi-layered structure the way many noun categories have, the superordinate verbs typically branch out to a couple of hierarchy levels where there are not many semantically similar verbs that can be used as variations to express certain actions or events (Pajunen 1998, 2001:60-63). As far as the performance was measured as the proportion of semantically related verbs in clusters, the subject groups performed almost identically across all the semantic categories, with no less than 60% of the words clustered. The finding implies that although the AD patients showed a remarkable decrease in the number of verbs, clusters, and switches, their ability to use a coherent strategy to produce verbs in a string on the basis of their close semantic relatedness was preserved. The only exception was made by the moAD group who had problems in clustering together verbs belonging to the category of preparing food. Instead of clustering verbs, they tended to list single verbs with no close semantic relationship to each other except for the category membership. They also tended to produce nouns that referred to different dishes, which were counted as errors and were not included in clusters (see 9.4.2). 9.4 Analysis of the contents of the responses on the verb fluency tasks In the verb fluency task, the subjects referred to different types of actions with various forms (see Table 19). There was no difference among the groups in their use of verb phrases or deverbal forms, whereas the extent to which general verbs and concrete nouns were used differentiated between the subject groups (in each, p < .05). The groups tended to differ also in the production of specific verb forms (p < .058). The NC group and the miAD group used mostly specific verb forms and deverbal forms when generating words for the verb categories, whereas verb phrases, general verbs, or single concrete nouns were relatively rarely used in these groups. In the moAD group, the use of specific verbs tended to decrease significantly (p < .05; 136 Results Table 19. Word forms produced in the verb fluency tasks Word form NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Specific verbs (%) 50.2 (37.3) 41.7 (36.8) 63.7 44.1 23.0 (28.2) 5.706 8.7 * p < 058 Verb phrases (%) 14.0 (19.1) 13.6 (14.0) 3.5 8.3 13.0 (13.7) 0.009 11.0 p = .956, n.s. Deverbal forms (%) 29.0 (33.8) 36.7 (37.2) 12.7 23.1 40.7 (29.8) 3.159 39.2 p = .206, n.s. General verbs (%) 2.1 (5.1) 0.0 4.9 (9.2) 0.0 9.3 (11.8) 5.1 ** 7.802 p < .05 Nouns in verb categories (%) 4.7 (9.2) 0.0 3.1 (7.0) 0.0 14.0 (15.2) 6.489 13.5 * ¤ p < .05 Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. see Appendix 6:7) while the use of the other types of verb forms increased. The moAD group produced significantly more general verb forms (e.g., ‘laittaa’, ‘panna’, ‘tehdä’ / ‘put’, ‘put’, ‘make’) than the NC group (p < .01), and they generated concrete nouns instead of verbs significantly more often than the NC group and the miAD group (in both, p < .05). 9.4.1 Proportion of correct verbs When performing the verb fluency task, all subject groups produced errors (see Table 20). Comparison of the combined scores indicated an overall group difference in the ratio of correctly produced verbs (p < .001). According to the post hoc pairwise analyses (see Appendix 6:8), the NC group produced significantly more correct responses to the verb fluency task than the miAD group (p < .01) and the moAD group (p < .001). The difference in the overall performance of the miAD group and the moAD group almost reached statistical significance (p < .07). The mean percentage of the NC group’s correct verb production per category varied between 86-95%, for the miAD group between 74-83%, and for the moAD Results 137 Table 20. Proportion of correct words, intrusions, and perseverations in the verb fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Preparing food 92.4 (10.5) 96.7 77.6 (18.7) 81.5 ** 59.3 (33.0) 61.1 *** 17.227 p < .001 Playing sports 94.4 (9.0) 100.00 82.1 (15.8) 85.4 *** 69.7 (32.5) 75.0 ** 14.462 p < .001 Construction 86.2 (22.6) 100.00 74.0 (21.9) 72.7 * 65.9 (34.4) 75.0 * 7.022 p < .05 Cleaning 87.5 (16.4) 91.9 82.7 (17.2) 88.2 78.4 (24.7) 90.0 1.402 p = .496, n.s. All categories 90.0 (9.9) 93.1 78.8 (14.3) 80.7 ** 67.5 (21.0) 65.2 *** (¤) 18.370 p < .001 Preparing food 0.0 0.0 7.7 (16.2) 0.0 ** 16.0 (26.4) 0.0 *** 12.561 p < .01 Playing sports 1.3 (6.9) 0.0 4.5 (7.4) 0.0 * 17.3 (25.9) 0.0 *** 13.710 p < .001 Construction 7.8 (17.8) 0.0 7.7 (17.6) 0.0 22.5 (37.3) 0.0 1.060 p = .589, n.s. Cleaning 6.4 (15.0) 0.0 4.6 (12.2) 0.0 8.3 (17.4) 0.0 0.516 p = .773, n.s. All categories 4.0 (8.9) 0.0 6.2 (8.9) 3.1 16.5 (15.1) 14.6 ** ¤ 9.545 p < .01 Preparing food 7.6 (10.5) 3.3 14.7 (14.9) 13.4 (*) 24.7 (27.5) 16.7 ** 8.250 p < .05 Playing sports 4.3 (6.7) 0.0 13.4 (12.9) 14.3 ** 13.0 (25.2) 0.0 7.349 p < .05 Construction 6.0 (13.9) 0.0 18.3 (16.7) 19.1 ** 11.6 (18.0) 0.0 9.731 p < .01 Cleaning 6.1 (7.3) 0.0 12.7 (14.6) 10.6 13.3 (21.9) 0.0 2.303 p = .316, n.s. All categories 6.0 (5.5) 4.2 15.0 (10.7) 13.6 *** 16.0 (14.4) 12.5 ** 17.260 p < .001 Variable Correct verbs (%) Intrusions (%) Perseverations (%) Note. (*) = almost statistically significant; * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. 138 Results group between 59-78%. A statistically significant difference emerged in the ratio of correct verbs among the subject groups in all other categories but cleaning (preparing food and playing sports p < .001; construction p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise analyses in these categories revealed that the NC group outnumbered the miAD and the moAD group in the number of correct responses, but the ratio of correct verbs was not significantly different between the miAD and the moAD group (see Appendix 6:8). However, the difference between the AD groups approached statistical significance in the category of preparing food (p = .087). 9.4.2 Proportion of intrusions and perseverations Some of the errors found on the verb fluency task were intrusions (i.e., verbs not belonging to the given semantic category or nouns), which were observed in all subject groups (see Table 20). Intrusions emerged in at least one semantic category in the performance of 13/30 subjects in the NC group, 11/20 subjects in the miAD group, and 14/20 subjects in the moAD group. The overall ratio of intrusions differed among the subject groups (p < .01), and the post hoc pair-wise analyses indicated that the moAD group produced significantly more intrusions than the NC group (p < .01) and the miAD group (p < .05), while the NC group and the miAD group produced intrusions to the same extent (see Appendix 6:9). The NC group violated the category boundaries in all other categories but the category of preparing food, whereas intrusions interfered in the performance of both AD groups in all categories. Most of the intrusions emerged in the category of construction in all subject groups. The miAD group produced the fewest intrusions for the category of playing sports and the moAD group for the category of cleaning. There was a significant difference among the subject groups in the proportion of intrusions in the categories of preparing food (p < .01) and playing sports (p < .001). Both the miAD group and the moAD group generated significantly more intrusions than the NC group in these categories (see Appendix 6:9), but the difference between the two AD groups in either category was non-significant. Rather than being unrelated, intrusions were more frequently semantically related to the given semantic category in the NC group and the moAD group (see Table 21). The difference in the occurrence of related and unrelated intrusions was statistically significant in each group (p < .01). In the miAD group, there was no difference between the number of related and unrelated intrusions. Intrusions in the category of preparing food consisted of things to prepare food of (e.g., ‘meat’) and dishes to be prepared (e.g., ‘porridge’), kitchen utensils (e.g.,‘stove’), or outsidecategory words (e.g., ‘talk’). Intrusions in the category of playing sports were words referring to sports equipment (e.g., ‘ball’), nouns referring to the location of playing sports in (e.g., ‘gym’), semantically related actions (‘play bridge’), and outsidecategory intrusions (e.g., ‘crochet’). In the category of construction, tools (e.g., ‘hammer’) as well as things to be built (e.g., ‘the stairs’), things to be attached to a construction (e.g., ‘window’) and construction materials (e.g., ‘cement’) were consid- Results 139 Table 21. Number of semantically related and unrelated intrusions produced in the verb fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) Type of intrusions M (SD) Md M (SD) Md M (SD) Md Semantically related intrusions 1.5 (2.9) 0.0 1.3 (2.1) 0.0 2.3 (2.4) 1.5 Semantically unrelated 0.03 (0.2) intrusions 0.0 0.9 (1.5) 0.0 0.4 (0.8) 0.0 Z-value+ p-value -.849 -2.655 p = .396, n.s. p < .01 ** -2.969 p < .01 ** Note. +Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test: ** = p < .01. n.s. = non significant ered intrusions. As for the category of cleaning, words referring to cleaning equipment (e.g., ‘rag’) and actions regarding decorating the inside of the house (e.g., ‘decorate’), as well as semantically unrelated verbs (e.g., ‘sleep’) were counted as intrusions. All subject groups produced perseverations while generating verbs (see Table 20). The performance of 24/30 subjects in the NC group, 18/20 subjects in the miAD group, and 17/20 subjects in the moAD group contained perseverations in at least in one of the categories. Comparison of the total number of perseveration indicated a significant difference among the subject groups (p < .001). Relative to the NC group, significantly more perseverations were made in the miAD group (p < .001) and the moAD group (p < .01), but there was no difference in the number of perseverations between the two AD groups (see Appendix 6:10). The NC group produced perseverations in a relatively stable manner across the semantic categories, while the miAD and the moAD group showed more variation between the categories. There was a statistically significant difference among the groups in the proportion of perseverations produced in all other categories but the category of cleaning (preparing food and playing sports p < .05, construction p < .01). According to the post hoc pair-wise analyses, the miAD group perseverated significantly more often than the NC group when generating words for the category of playing sports and construction (in each, p < .01). In the category of preparing food, the difference in the number of perseverations between the two groups approached statistical significance (p = .07). Compared to the NC group, the moAD group perseverated significantly more frequently only in the category of preparing food (p < .01), while the number of perseverations remained statistically the same 140 Results in the categories of playing sports and construction between these two subject groups. There was no statistically significant difference between the miAD group and the moAD group in producing perseverations for the categories of preparing food, playing sports or construction. 9.4.3 Number and variety of different semantic subcategories The combined score indicating the number of different semantic subcategories from which verbs were produced for the task differentiated the subject groups (p < .001; see Table 22). Compared to the NC group, the miAD group (p < .01) and the moAD group (p < .001) displayed a remarkable decrease in the use of subcategories, and the miAD group produced clusters of verbs from significantly more dimensions than the moAD group (p < .01; see Appendix 6:11). For each semantic category, the NC group generated clusters from approximately two different subcategories, whereas the miAD group’s clustered words originated from one to two subcategories, and the moAD group clustered words using only one subcategory. The number of semantic dimensions among the subject groups was significantly different in all verb categories (preparing food, playing sports, and construction p < .001, cleaning p < .01). The post hoc pair-wise tests indicated that the NC group produced words from a significantly larger semantic scope than the miAD and the moAD group in the category of preparing food, playing sports, and construction (see Appendix 6:11). In the category of cleaning, the NC group generated more subcategories than the moAD group (p < .001), the difference between the NC and the miAD group being non-significant. The miAD group outnumbered the moAD group in the number of semantic dimensions in all other categories (preparing food p < .001; construction and cleaning p < .05) but playing sports. The variety of semantic subcategories in the verb categories was dissimilar between the subject groups. The NC group produced various types of semantic clusters, relative to the miAD group and the moAD group (Figures 5-8; see also Appendix 4E-4H). Characteristic of the NC group was that verbs were clustered according to one main subcategory, followed by two to four other rather well represented subcategories for which verbs were divided in a more stable manner. In the miAD group, verbs were mainly clustered according to two main subcategories, the rest remaining less represented. In the categories of construction and cleaning, the miAD group lacked some of the semantic dimensions produced by the NC group. The moAD group showed a remarkable reduction in the range of semantic activation and produced most of the verbs using mainly one semantic criterion for each category. For the moAD group, more than one subcategory was very seldom represented for the categories of preparing food, construction, and cleaning. However, for the category of playing sports, all subject groups were able to activate several semantic subcategories. When producing verbs for the category of preparing food, verbs denoting cooking (e.g., ‘boil’, ‘fry’, ‘grill’) were most often produced in all subject groups, Results 141 Table 22. Number of different subcategories produced for the semantic categories in the verb fluency tasks NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) Category M (SD) Md M (SD) Md M (SD) Md H (df=2) p-value Preparing food 2.3 (0.9) 2.0 1.8 (1.0) 2.0 * 0.6 (0.8) 0.0 *** ¤¤¤ 27.95 p < .001 Playing sports 2.5 (1.2) 2.0 1.7 (1.2) 1.0 ** 1.1 (1.1) 1.0 *** 16.205 p < .001 Construction 2.5 (1.0) 2.5 1.7 (1.2) 1.5 * 1.0 (1.0) 1.0 *** ¤ 19.018 p < .001 Cleaning 2.0 (1.0) 2.0 1.7 (1.0) 2.0 1.0 (1.0) 1.0 *** ¤ 13.249 p < .01 All categories 9.2 (2.2) 9.0 6.8 (3.4) 5.5 ** 3.7 (2.6) 3.0 *** ¤¤ 30.568 p < .001 Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤ = p < .01, ¤¤¤ p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. followed by verbs referring to cleaning and preparing (e.g., ‘peel’, ‘wash’, ‘chop’) and seasoning (e.g., ‘salt’, ‘spice’; Figure 5; see also Appendix 4E). Clusters of specific verbs involving an instrument and referring to handling the ingredients (e.g., ‘cut’, ‘mix’, ‘whip’) and baking (e.g., ‘bake’, ‘knead the dough’, ‘flour’) were produced by all other groups but the moAD group for whom they did not exist as a dimension for eliciting clusters of verbs. The distributions of the semantic subgategories in the category of playing sports were more or less similar among the subject groups (Figure 6; see also Appendix 4F). In all groups, the most frequently used verbs were those referring to different types of sports in which the use of feet/legs was foregrounded (e.g., ‘run’, ‘walk’, ‘jump’). The second most often used subcategory was verbs referring to wrestling and boxing for the NC group, actions carried out in the water (e.g., ‘swim’, ‘butterfly’, ‘crawl’) for the miAD group, and types of games (‘play basketball’, ‘play football’) for the moAD group. Instrument verbs referring to winter sports (e.g., ‘ski’, ‘skate’, ‘snowboard’, ‘ski downhill’, ‘cross-country skiing’) and actions emphasizing the use of hands/arms (e.g., ‘throw’, ‘push’, ‘lift’) were also represented in all subject groups. 142 Results 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 on in g fo od ha nd se lin as g ba ki ng ea n pr ing ep a ar nd in g cl co ok i ng 0.0 Subcategories Figure 5. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of preparing food in different subject groups. 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 em p sp ha ha orts nd sis w s on ith an u th d si e ar ng m s es m ga rts po te rs in w sp o em rts ph wit us a h in sis the g bo le on xi gs ng an d w re st lin g 0.0 Subcategories Figure 6. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of playing sports in different subject groups. Results 143 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 e han su d rfa ling ce s k or w c si ba w or ki ng of th a th e bu fab ild ric in g k or kw w on oo br ic dw or ki ng 0.0 Subcategories Figure 7. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of construction in different subject groups. 1.4 Number of subcategories (M) NC 1.2 miAD 1.0 moAD 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 lin kin en g an car d eo ca f rp the et s ta cl ea n g pr ep ar in of c s to le an in g ai rin g w ay cl ea ni n g th e flo or 0.0 Subcategories Figure 8. The distribution and mean number of the most common subcategories of cleaning up in different subject groups. 144 Results In the category of construction, verbs describing the ways of handling wood (e.g., ‘carve’, ‘saw’) were the ones most often used in all subject groups (Figure 7; see also Appendix 4G). Verbs connected with masonry (e.g., ‘carry bricks’, ‘mix the mortar’, ‘mason’, ‘plaster’, ‘pour’) were the second most often used dimension in the NC group, but they were rather seldom produced as a cluster in the AD groups. Verbs referring to various types of action taking place in the foundation (e.g., ‘dig’, ‘lay the foundation’, ‘cover with boards’, ‘cast’, ‘tear down the boarding’) and on other parts of a building (e.g., ‘cover [with roofing], ‘insulate’, ‘cover with boards’) were rather well represented in the cluster production of the NC group and the miAD group, whereas the moAD group produced clusters of verbs only for actions taking place during the laying of the foundation. Respectively, the miAD group lacked clusters of verbs referring to actions done on the surface of the walls (e.g., ‘paint’, ‘tile’, ‘wallpaper’). The distributions of the most common semantic subcategories differed among the subject groups also in the category of cleaning (Figure 8; see also Appendix 4H). In all subject groups, clusters mostly involved verbs that referred to cleaning the floors (e.g., ‘vacuum’, ‘sweep’, ‘wipe’, ‘polish’, ‘wash’, ‘scrub’). In the NC group and the miAD group, the second best represented subcategory was verbs denoting airing (e.g., ‘shake’, ‘beat’, ‘air’), a subcategory that did not exist as a source of cluster production for the moAD group. Verbs referring to different ways of cleaning (‘scrub’, wash’, ‘wipe’) and taking care of the linen (e.g., ‘do the bed’, ‘change the sheets’) elicited some clusters of verbs in all subject groups. Clusters of verbs denoting getting ready to clean (e.g., ‘pour water into a bucket’, ‘get a vacuumcleaner’) existed only for the NC group. 9.4.4 Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the verbs produced There was no difference in the degree of prototypicality among the subject groups in any single verb category, not even when the analysis was based on the total number of verbs produced (Table 23). A statistically significant difference in the overall verb frequency among the groups was evident (p < .05). The post hoc pair-wise analyses revealed that the miAD group produced more frequent verbs than the moAD group (p < .05), whereas the frequencies of the verbs between the other groups remained the same (see Appendix 6:12). At the level of individual categories, all subject groups produced verbs at the same level of frequency. 9.4.5 Summary of the results and discussion The responses given for the verb fluency task consisted of five types of word forms: specific verb forms, phrasal structures, deverbal forms, and general verbs combined with different argument structures. Furthermore, single concrete nouns were also produced in all subject groups. The NC group and the miAD group gave similar Results 145 Table 23. Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the verbs produced Variable NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H p-value (df = 2) Degree of prototypicality of verbs Preparing food 5.13 (0.67) 5.12 (1.00) 4.35 (2.66) 0.532 5.28 5.16 5.55 p = .767, n.s. Playing sports 5.55 (0.52) 5.43 (0.52) 4.69 (2.27) 1.295 5.69 5.26 5.62 p = .523, n.s. Construction 4.99 (0.54) 4.78 (0.79) 4.50 (1.72) 1.598 5.13 4.74 4.89 p = .450, n.s. Cleaning 5.91 (0.41) 5.81 (0.60) 5.22 (1.98) 0.179 6.01 5.85 5.95 p = .914, n.s. All verbs 5.28 (0.26) 5.23 (0.42) 4.61 (1.1) 5.26 5.25 4.75 3.412 p = .182, n.s. 4.68 (0.32) 4.90 (0.67) 4.20 (2.42) 3.218 4.68 4.78 5.03 p = .200, n.s. Playing sports 4.56 (0.36) 4.59 (0.48) 4.05 (1.86) 0.082 4.56 4.59 4.79 p = .960, n.s. Construction 4.00 (0.29) 4.18 (0.56) 3.84 (1.49) 3.658 3.97 4.32 4.14 p = .161, n.s. Cleaning 4.50 (0.38) 4.62 (0.43) 3.91 (1.42) 4.829 4.58 4.76 4.29 p = .089, n.s. All verbs 4.44 (0.23) 4.60 (0.36) 3.96 (0.89) 5.828 4.43 4.51 4.16 ¤ p < .05 Degree of frequency of verbs Preparing food Note. Judgements were made on a 7-point scale: 1 = a very poor example of a category / a very infrequent word, 7 = a very good example of a catefory / a very frequent word. ¤ = p < .05 when miAD vs. moAD. n.s. = non significant. types of responses for the task, whereas the performance of the moAD group consisted of semantically more general responses. Most of the errors in the NC group and the miAD group were perseverations. In the moAD group, one half of the errors consisted of intrusions and the other of perseverations. It is worth noting that in each subject group, including the NC group, errors were not made by just a few participants, but a majority of them generated errors at least once during the task (see also 9.4.2, 9.4.5). Compared to the NC 146 Results group, both AD groups made significantly more errors in general and in all other individual categories but the category of cleaning. The error analysis was sensitive enough to differentiate between the two AD groups only at the level of overall performance. The qualitative analysis revealed a remarkable narrowing of the semantic scope of subcategories from which to generate verbs for the task in the AD groups. There was no difference between the groups in the degree of prototypicality of verbs, but the frequency ratings showed that, in general, the moAD group used verbs with significantly lower frequency of occurrence than the miAD group. Word forms The NC group and the miAD group seemed to name actions in a similar manner, most of the responses consisting of specific verbs and deverbal forms of verbs (see 9.4). The moAD group, however, showed a tendency to lose the degree of specificity when generating verbs for the task, favoring more general verbs and single concrete nouns to specific verbs. Thus, the responses given for the verb fluency task did not follow the examples given by the examiner for the practice category, that is, to use single verbs in the first infinitive (e.g., ‘kaivaa’ / ‘dig’, ‘istuttaa’ / ‘plant’). Although sentence-like phrases were not introduced or encouraged for production during the practice phase, some of the subjects in each group produced verbs which were embedded in phrasal structures. The tendency to produce whole phrases may reflect the conceptual-semantic dependency of the verbs on other words and the verbs’ central position which relates to nouns as their arguments and thus motivates the whole clausal structure for production (see Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Engelkamp 1975; Reyna 1987; Shapiro et al. 1987; Aitchison 1994:111; Persson 1995:96-97; Wayland et al. 1996; Pajunen 1999:14-15; see 3.3). In some confrontation naming tasks, such a response pattern may have been interpreted as a circumcolutory or a descriptive error that described a perceptual or a functional feature of the target (e.g., Robinson et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1996). In this study, however, the phrasal structures were considered appropriate responses because they designated the whole frame for the action, including the verb and its arguments, which can be considered part of the meaning structure of the verb (see 9.4.1). The use of deverbal forms of verbs is likely to display a normal pattern of nominalization common to any language, which does not essentially modify the conceptual-semantic content of the verb, as stated by Langacker (1991:25) and Vinson and Vigliocco (2002). Nominalization may contribute as a shift in the profile of the action by changing the focus from the process of the action to its nominal entity, for example, to an internal subject (e.g., ‘blender’), an internal object (e.g., ’choice’), an instrument (e.g., ‘probe’), a product (e.g., ‘painting’), or a location (e.g., ‘lounge’), to a single episode of a process (e.g., ‘make a throw’), or to an atemporal abstraction of the relationships between the participants of a process (e.g., ‘walking’; Langacker 1991:22-28; see also Saffran et al. 1980; Leino 1999:80, 89). Consequently, a noun- Results 147 like profile may have been taken for verbs and deverbal forms were thus listed in a noun-like manner as a continued pattern of response strategy, originating from the preceding noun fluency task. The use of nominalizations, which does not require the use of the whole rolestructure of the verb, has been interpreted as a way of agrammatic speakers of English to ease their verb production (Saffran et al. 1980). Possibly, the increasing number of nominalizations in the AD groups may be an indication of a need to ease the demands of the naming task, especially in the moAD group. However, the difference between the NC group and the moAD group did not reach the level of statistical significance in the proportion of deverbal forms. Therefore, the role of the nominalizations as a compensatory manner to ease word production in AD remains speculative in the present study. The tendency of the moderate AD patients to express different types of action by forming structures containing more general verbs (e.g., ‘panna’, ‘laittaa’, ‘tehdä’ / ‘put’, ‘make’, ‘do’) rather than specific verbs supports the study of Kim and Thompson (2001) who reported on the tendency of AD patients to use generic verb forms rather than more complex verbs in the confrontation naming task. For the AD patients, generic verbs, although they are relational and lacking a direct connection to perception, may be easier to retrieve than specific verbs because generic verbs are very high in frequency, polysemous, easy to modify semantically, and flexible to use in different contextual situations (see Reyna 1987; Persson 1995:103; Berndt et al. 1997; Breedin et al. 1998; Kim & Thompson 2001; see also 3.3.1). Specific verbs, on the other hand, are rich in functional and semantic role information. For example, verbs like ‘bake’, ‘mix’, and ‘sweep’ that were produced for the task consist of a conglomerate of semantic features that can be simultaneously activated. They involve such semantic roles as Mover to encode the physical movements of the body or a part of the body, Agent to encode the entity to bring about a change, Instrument to encode the involvement of a tool or an instrument with which to carry out the action, Patient to encode the entity being acted upon, and Result encoding the outcome of the action (Persson 1995:99-103; cf. Levelt 1989:90-94; see 3.3, and 3.3.3). The present study supports the finding of Persson (1995:101) who claimed that some of the semantic roles might overlap and constrain the effects and the interpretation of each other. As noted by her, the role of Mover may be more emphasized than Agent in verbs referring to different types of motion (e.g., ‘walk’), whereas Agent may be more fore-grounded in verbs encoding causative action than Mover (e.g., ‘cut’). The data of the present study may imply that the roles of Patient and Result may be overlapping in certain verbs. For example, ‘bake’ implicitly contains the underlying element of Patient, the entity being acted upon (i.e., the batter), and Result, the entity being changed during the action (i.e., the cake). Another aspect that may make specific verbs more difficult for the AD patients to retrieve is that, relative to generic verbs, a flexible modification of specific verbs is not possible 148 Results because they are closely integrated with specific nouns (e.g., ‘sweep’ entails ‘broom’; see Engelkamp 1975; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Persson 1995:102-193). It can be assumed that the occurrence of general purpose verbs in the performance of the moderately demented AD patients may be a sign of the activation to settle for information that is sparser in terms of semantic features which is thus semantically more available or less of an effort for them to be integrated and produced for an output than the information contained by specific verbs (see 3.3.1, 10.1.3). Thus, the selection of semantically related nouns for production may be explained by AD patients’ difficulty in integrating the semantic information of specific verbs, whereby nouns corresponding to the semantic roles of the verb structure may have become more activated and selected for the production. For example, instead of a specific cleaning verb (e.g., ‘wipe’), the subject may produce the Instrument (e.g., ‘rag’) designating the thing with which the action is carried out, which usually is not specified when the verb is produced but is indirectly implied by the verb (see Behrend 1990; Persson 1995:99). For a further discussion on the appearance of nouns among verb responses, see below under Intrusions and 10.1.3. The pattern of producing general verbs and nouns that are part of the semantic role structure suggests that the system tries to compensate for the non-availability of the more specific features by semantically more available features in AD. This bears certain similarities to the findings concerning the noun representation changes in AD patients, with a better availability of the general, superordinate knowledge relative to the more specific knowledge at the lower levels of the semantic hierarchy, which has been documented by a number of authors (e.g., Warrington 1975; Schwartz et al. 1979; Martin & Fedio 1983; Martin et al. 1985; Huff et al. 1986; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Chertkow et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1992, Hodges & Patterson 1995; Tippett et al. 1995; see 9.6). Thus, it seems as if a reduction in semantic specificity takes place in AD, especially at its moderate stage. For those patients, naming an action seems to be dependent more on the generic verbs and concrete nouns than on the specific verb forms. Intrusions The total number of intrusions (i.e., verbs outside the category boundaries and single nouns) was significantly higher in the moAD group than in the other groups (see 9.4.2). In the NC group and the moAD group, most of the intrusions tended to be verbs and nouns semantically related to the given category, whereas the miAD group produced both semantically related and unrelated intrusions to the same extent. Intrusions were produced selectively for the individual semantic categories by the NC group, whereas both AD groups tended to violate category boundaries. Compared to the NC group, the number of intrusions in the AD groups was significantly higher in the categories of preparing food and playing sports. As discussed earlier, most of the intrusions consisted of semantically related concrete nouns, such as dishes to be eaten and sports equipment. Concrete single nouns also appeared in other semantic Results 149 categories. Common to all categories, most of the nouns were names of different objects involved in the action. More specifically, they seemed to represent the semantic roles encapsulated in the verbs (see 3.3). For example, the semantic role Instrument was often used to designate things the action was carried out with (e.g., ‘hammer’, ‘rag’). The role Patient was involved in carrying the information of an entity to be acted upon or to be changed by the action (e.g., ‘meat’, ‘cement’). Result was used to designate the result or the product of the action (‘porridge’, ‘stairs’), and Location the site of the action (e.g., ‘stove’, ‘gym’). The finding that syntagmatic errors occurred in AD is in contrast to the study of Gewirth et al. (1984) who claimed that in the word association task, demented subjects were as sensitive to the grammatical class of the stimulus word as normal control subjects and that the frequency of syntagmatic responses was not likely to change significantly with increasing severity of dementia. In the present study, syntagmatic errors did not occur while words were produced for the noun fluency task. However, violations of the grammatical class of verbs were made, particularly in the moAD group. The finding may imply that the grammatical class of verbs is more vulnerable to a breakdown in AD or that naming actions is even more dependent on other word classes, especially nouns, for these patients than for normal elderly subjects. However, although most of the subjects of the dementia group in the study of Gewirth et al. had AD, the group involved other subjects with miscellaneous types of dementia (e.g., patients with multi-infarct dementia), which is why a straightforward comparison between the studies may be questionable. The deficit underlying the emergence of intrusions may involve the incorrect features having a higher level of activation or an increased decay rate whereby they are more likely to be selected for further processing (see 9.2.6, 10.1.2, 10.1.3). Also the process behind the production of semantically related concrete nouns instead of verbs may imply an impaired semantic feature integration of verbs. In order for a selection of an item to take place successfully, a convergence of a whole set of semantic features is required. Especially in the case of the subjects with moderate AD, the semantic system, after finding the encoding of the functional features and the semantic roles of a verb difficult or impossible, may compensate the unsuccessful feature integration by settling the spread of activation on perceptually richer, more transparent, more static, and more available features belonging to the nominal component of the verb’s semantic structure (e.g., ‘rag’; see Persson 1995:26, 84-86, 92-104, 146; Guasti 2002:81). Consequently, the nouns produced for the verb categories may have acted as substitutions for the specific verbs. The finding that semantically related concrete nouns occurred in verb categories supports the notion that verbs are semantically dependent structures and that the semantic representation of specific verbs includes nouns (Persson 1995:96-104; see 3.3 and 3.3.3). However, putting aside theoretical speculations about the process underlying the intrusions, it may also be possible that the occurrence of nouns in the verb categories may be an indication of the subjects having forgotten the task instruction (see Astell & Harley 2002). 150 Results The errors made by the AD patients in the semantic fluency task may bear a certain similarity to the semantically related and unrelated errors, as well as to the descriptive and part-whole naming errors reported to occur in verb confrontation naming tasks (White-Devine et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 1996; Williamson et al. 1998; see 2.4.2). These errors may imply difficulty in the integration of semantic features (see 10.1.3). Perseverations Perseverations were produced in all subject groups for all semantic categories (see 9.4.2). Overall, significantly more perseverations were produced in the miAD group and the moAD group than in the NC group. Surprisingly, the number of perseverations did not differentiate between the AD groups when the combined score was taken into account. Compared to the NC group, the category of preparing food seemed to provoke significantly more repetitions than other semantic categories in both the miAD group and the moAD group. Furthermore, the miAD group perseverated significantly more often than the NC group in the categories of playing sports and construction, while the moAD group perseverated to the same extent as the NC group. One of the reasons for the moAD group producing fewer perseverations may be their tendency to generate more intrusions than others, at least for some categories. Similar to the AD patients’ performance on the noun fluency task, their performance on the verb fluency task gives reason to assume that their word production system may not work well because perseverations tend to interfere with the activation of new responses (see 9.2.6, 10.1.2, 10.1.3). It can be speculated that the fewer number of perseverations in the moAD group relative to the miAD group may partly be due to semantically irrelevant features (i.e., intrusions) gaining even more activation than previously activated appropriate features. Another possibility may be that the self-monitoring system may function better for the miAD group than for the moAD group which prevents the miAD group from violating the semantic and grammatical category but not from selecting the previously activated patterns of features (see Diesfeldt 1985; Della Sala et al. 1993; Pasquier et al. 1995; see also Baddeley et al. 1986, 1991; Levelt 1989:463-467, 1999a, b; Rosen & Engle 1997). In earlier reports on the AD patients’ performance on the confrontation naming tasks with verbs, perseverations were not reported to take place (see e.g., Bowles et al. 1987; White-Devine et al. 1995, 1996; Robinson et al. 1996; Williamson et al. 1998). Thus, semantic fluency tasks may involve at least partly different semanticcognitive functions from confrontation naming tasks (see 9.6.1, 9.6.3, 9.6.4). Semantic subcategories The present study indicated that the normal control subjects were better able to activate varying subcategories to evoke verb production and thus showed a more dynamic, flexible, integrative, and creative use of the semantic information con- Results 151 tained by different types of concrete verbs than the patients with mild and moderate AD did (see 9.2.4, 9.2.6). The category of cleaning notwithstanding, a remarkable decrease in the number of available semantic subcategories was found in the miAD group. The most obvious narrowing of the dimensions was found in the moAD group that produced significantly fewer subcategories than the NC group throughout all verb categories and less than the miAD group for all other categories but the category of playing sports. Hence, the moAD group showed the most restricted potential for activating new semantic subcategories from which to draw verbs for production. Typical of the clusters in the categories of preparing food, construction, and cleaning was the appearance of verbs denoting agent-initiated, goal-directed action causing a change of state and/or a result as an outcome of the action, for example, verbs denoting different ways (i.e., troponyms) of cooking (‘boil’, ‘fry’, ‘grill’), bricklaying (e.g., ‘mason’, ‘plaster’, ‘pour’), and airing carpets (‘shake’, ‘beat’, ‘air’). Characteristic for the category of playing sports was the occurrence of verbs with a reference to motion involving the whole body (e.g., ‘wrestle’, ‘box’), body parts (e.g., ‘run’, ‘walk’, ‘jump’ and ‘throw’, ‘push’), and games involving more than one participant (e.g., ‘play basketball’, ‘play ice-hockey’). Common to all semantic categories, some of the clusters consisted of verbs the semantic content of which contained a concrete noun (e.g., a tool or an instrument) as a part of the verb structure, such as verbs denoting diminishing and handling the ingredients when preparing food (e.g., ‘chop, ‘mix’, ‘whip’), actions of sports (e.g., ‘skate’, ‘snowboard’, ‘downhill skiing’, ‘cross-country skiing’), as well as verbs referring to handling wood (e.g., ‘carve’, ‘saw’, ‘paint’) and cleaning the floor (e.g., ‘vacuum’, ‘sweep’, ‘polish’, ‘wash’, ‘scrub’). Furthermore, thematic information, such as temporal-contextual information, was encoded in the clusters (e.g., types of winter sports and particular targets or phases of construction a house). Common to all subject groups, clusters of verbs were also produced by ordering the actions in a sequential, temporal order in which they are likely to take place when preparing food, construction, or cleaning a house. For the category of playing sports, these script-like listings of actions were rarely given. In sum, the verb clusters were formed using thematicfunctional, troponymic, and temporal-causal relationship between verbs (see 3.3). Degree of prototypicality and frequency of the verbs produced The statistical analysis indicated that the subject groups produced words of the same degree of prototypicality for all four verb categories. However, it is worth noting that, throughout all the semantic categories, the ratings of the verbs indicated the highest prototypicality for the NC group and the lowest for the moAD group. As far as the frequencies of the verbs produced are concerned, all the subject groups produced verbs with the same level of frequency for all individual verb categories, but the overall frequency score indicated that the miAD group produced more words of higher mean frequency than the moAD group. 152 Results These findings are in contrast to the finding obtained from the prototypicality and frequency ratings on the noun fluency task (see 9.2.5, 9.2.6; cf. Ober et al. 1986; Chan et al. 1993; Weingartner et al. 1993; Binetti et al. 1995; Goldstein et al. 1996; Beatty et al. 2000). It is possible that, because the number of verbs produced by the moAD group remained very small, individual performances were likely to gain more weight and thus they may have skewed the outcome of the verb frequency ratings towards more seldom occurring verbs. For a further discussion on the methodology, see 10.2.2. 9.5 Summary of the overall semantic fluency performance Comparing the overall semantic fluency performance of the miAD group and the moAD group to the performance of the NC group on the noun and verb categories, it can be summed up that some of the parameters chosen for the study appeared to be more sensitive in differentiating between the subject groups than others. As indicated in Table 24, the parameters seemed to work better to differentiate between the subject groups in the noun than in the verb fluency tasks. As far as the noun categories are concerned, almost all parameters differentiated the miAD group and the moAD group from the NC group. Furthermore, the critical parameters were the same in these comparisons. This implies that relative to the performance of the healthy normal elderly adults, the semantic fluency performance of the mild and the moderate AD patients on the four noun categories was significantly impaired. Both AD groups produced nouns that were fewer in number, more frequent, and from a smaller number of semantic subcategories than the nouns produced by the NC group. Moreover, the switching and clustering performance of the AD groups was significantly poorer and the AD patients produced more errors, mainly perseverations, for the task. In the AD groups, the role of the perseverations may have an influence on the clustering performance in individual categories (see 9.2.2, 9.2.6). The parameters showing no significant difference between the groups imply that intrusions rarely took place and that the responses given for the task were of an equal level of prototypicality in all subject groups. Comparison between the miAD group and the moAD group indicated that the miAD group was significantly better than the moAD group at generating more total and correct responses, switches, clusters, and semantic subcategories for the noun categories. The overall clustering performance, however, indicated that although the number of clusters was significantly smaller in the moAD group, the cluster size and proportion of clustered nouns of the groups were similar. Both groups seemed to use nouns at the same level of prototypicality and frequency. Concerning the error analysis, the moAD group produced significantly more perseverations than the miAD group. The number of intrusions remained the same in all three groups. Results 153 Table 24. Summary of the comparison of the semantic fluency performance between the subject groups NC vs. miAD NC vs. moAD miAD vs. moAD NC vs. miAD NC vs. moAD miAD vs. moAD Variable Nouns Nouns Nouns Verbs Verbs Verbs Total number of words *** *** ** ** *** ** Number of correct words *** *** *** *** *** *** Number of switches *** *** *** *** *** ** Number of clusters *** *** *** ** *** *** Cluster size ** * n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Words in clusters *** ** n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Degree of prototypicality of words n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Frequency of words (*) ** n.s. n.s. n.s. * Proportion of correct words ** *** * ** *** (*) Propotion of intrusions n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. ** * Propotion of perseverations *** *** * *** ** n.s. Number of semantic subcategories *** *** *** ** *** ** Note. The comparisons are based on the combined average scores. (*) = almost statistically significant, * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001. n.s. = non significant Concerning the semantic fluency performance on the verb categories, the overall performance of the AD groups was significantly impaired relative to the NC group. The NC group produced significantly more total and correct responses, switches, and clusters on a larger scale of semantic dimensions than the miAD group and the moAD group. However, the cluster size and the proportion of clustered 154 Results verbs remained the same between the subject groups. The AD groups also produced verbs as prototypical and frequent as the NC group. The error analysis indicated that both AD groups made significantly more errors than the NC group. Both AD groups committed more perseverations than the NC group, but only the moAD group violated the boundaries of the semantic and the grammatical categories by producing significantly more intrusions than the NC group. The tendency of the AD groups to perseverate responses may cruicially affect the cluster formation and the overall performance on the tasks (see 9.4.2, 9.4.5). The miAD group produced significantly more total and correct responses from a wider variety of semantic dimensions, as well as more switches and clusters for the verb categories than the moAD group. Nevertheless, the size of the verb clusters and the proportion of clustered verbs was similar in these two groups. Furthermore, the verbs appeared to be of the same level of prototypicality but the verbs produced by the moAD group were of lower frequency than those produced by the miAD group. The error analysis indicated that the moAD group produced more intrusions than the miAD group, but these two groups did not differ in the number of perseverations produced for the verb categories. To sum up, the most sensitive parameters to differentiate the performance of the different subject groups in the present study appeared to be the number of total and correct responses, the number of clusters and switches, and the proportion of correct words, as well as the number of different semantic subcategories employed. As far as the error analysis is concerned, the proportion of perseverations seems also to be a sensitive parameter, especially when measuring semantic fluency performance on nouns and the performance differences between the NC group and the AD groups. 9.6 Performance on the control tasks The control tasks chosen for the present study consisted of tasks for which the participants were asked to give verbal responses (naming) and non-verbal responses (pointing to or sorting the targets). 9.6.1 Performance on the control tasks requiring verbal responses The overall group differences were significant in the semantic tasks requiring verbal responses and in the test tapping the functioning of the working (short-term) memory (p < .001, see Table 25). The miAD group performed significantly worse than the NC group on all tasks requiring the naming of verbs (p < .001; see Appendix 7). The miAD group named fewer picture objects in the BNT and in the serial naming task than the NC group (p < .001), but the groups did not differ in naming single photographed objects originating from the four semantic categories used in the semantic fluency task. Compared to the NC group and the miAD group, the moAD group Results 155 Table 25. Performance of the subject groups on the control tasks requiring verbal responses NC miAD moAD Control task M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value BNT 53.8 (3.7) 55.0 42.2 (12.3) 54.5 *** 33.8 (10.8) 34.0 *** ¤ 37.259 p < .001 Naming, nouns 19.8 (0.5) 20.0 19.0 (3.1) 20.0 17.4 (2.9) 18.0 *** ¤ 15.170 p < .001 Naming, verbs 18.3 (1.8) 19.0 16.3 (3.5) 17.0 *** 13.0 (3.5) 34.477 14.0 *** ¤¤¤ p < .001 Serial naming, nouns 24.0 (0.2) 24.0 22.8 (3.5) 24.0 *** 21.3 (3.7) 23.0 *** ¤ 23.278 p < .001 Serial naming, verbs 23.9 (0.3) 24.0 22.1 (3.0) 23.5 *** 18.3 (4.1) 19.5 ***¤¤¤ 41.383 p < .001 Digit span forward+ 5.2 (1.7) 5.0 3.8 (1.4) 4.0 ** 2.9 (1.1) 3.0 *** ¤ 25.904 p < .001 Note. BNT = Boston Naming Test (Laine, Koivuselkä-Sallinen et al. 1997). * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤¤ = p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. +interpreted with a licensed psychologist. performed significantly worse on every task (p < .01 or smaller). Relative to the NC group, the functioning of the short-term memory was significantly limited in the miAD group (p < .01) and the moAD group (p < .001), and the short-term memory span appeared to be significantly shorter in the moAD group than the miAD group (p < .05). 9.6.2 Performance on the control tasks requiring non-verbal responses Overall group differences were statistically significant in all tasks requiring nonverbal responses (p < .01 or smaller; Table 26). Relative to the NC group, the Token Test tapping comprehension and interpretation of verbal instructions was performed significantly worse by the miAD group and the moAD group (both, p < .001; see Appendix 7). The miAD group did not show signs of deteriorated recognition of categories or category exemplars, either of nouns or verbs. However, the miAD group failed to sort the cards of objects and actions according to their category membership (p < .001). The moAD group performed significantly poorer than the NC group on all semantic tasks (p < .05 or smaller), and worse than the miAD group on all other tasks (p < .05 or smaller) but the tasks of category recognition. 156 Results Table 26. Performance of the subject groups on the control tasks requiring non-verbal responses NC miAD moAD Control task M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn M (SD) Mdn H (df = 2) p-value Token-test 33.7 (2.1) 34.0 30.1 (3.7) 30.8 *** 21.1 (6.5) 24.0 *** ¤¤¤ 38.349 p < .001 Category recognition, nouns 16.0 (0.0) 16.0 16.0 (0.0) 16.0 15.4 (3.0) 16.0 * 9.746 p < .01 Category recognition, verbs 16.0 (0.0) 16.0 15.8 (0.7) 16.0 15.0 (3.6) 16.0 ** 11.278 p < .01 In-category recognition, nouns 23.9 (0.3) 24.0 23.6 (1.6) 24.0 22.8 (2.3) 23.5 *** ¤ 15.487 p < .001 In-category recognition, verbs 23.8 (0.5) 24.0 22.9 (2.5) 24.0 18.9 (4.9) 21.0 *** ¤¤¤ 37.786 p < .001 Card sorting, nouns 19.8 (0.5) 20.0 18.6 (1.6) 19.0 *** 13.9 (5.5) 15.0 *** ¤¤¤ 36.130 p < .001 Card sorting, verbs 19.7 (0.8) 20.0 16.3 (3.5) 18.0 *** 10.0 (5.3) 10.5 *** ¤¤¤ 47.594 p < .001 Note. * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001 when NC vs. miAD and NC vs. moAD, and ¤ = p < .05, ¤¤¤ = p < .001 when miAD vs. moAD. 9.6.3 Correlations among scores on the semantic tasks The number of correct nouns in the NC group correlated only with the Token Test (p < .01; see Table 27). In the miAD group, positive correlations were found between correct nouns and the BNT, the Token Test, and the naming of category related nouns (all, p < .01). For the moAD group, there was a high positive correlation between the number of correct nouns and almost all verbal and non-verbal tasks: Token Test and naming of category related nouns (p < .01), serial naming of nouns, category recognition, and in-category item recognition (all, p < .05). In the moAD group, the number of correct nouns also correlated highly with the digit span test (p < .05). The number of correct verbs was correlated in the NC group only with the incategory recognition of verbs (p < .05). For the miAD group, a significant positive correlation existed between the number of correct verbs and the main linguistic tests, the BNT (p < .01) and the Token Test (p < .05), as well as the digit span test (p < .01). For the moAD group, the correct verbs correlated highly with the Token Test (p < .01), the digit span test (p < .05), the task of serial naming of verbs (p < .05), the category recognition task (p < .05), and the card-sorting task (p < .05). Results 157 Table 27. Spearman rank-order correlation coefficients (ρ) between the correct responses of the semantic fluency tasks and the control tasks in the subject groups Noun fluency task Noun fluency task ρ ρ NC miAD (n = 30) (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) NC miAD (n = 30) (n = 20) moAD (n = 20) BNT .244 .615** .395 .259 .685** .255 Naming category related items .026 .631** .595** .173 .148 .248 Serial naming .183 .304 .547* .186 .205 .503* Digit span forward .246 .379 .538* .301 .480* .537* Token-test .506** .568** .622** .340* .450* .611** Category recognition (-) (-) .504* (-) -.023 .585** In-category recognition -.008 .302 .474* .439* .322 .357 Card sorting .081 .115 .227 -.205 .122 .532* Control task Verbal tasks Non-verbal tasks Note. n = 30 (NC): ρ > .339, p < .05 = *; ρ > .437, p < .01 = **. n = 20 (miAD, moAD): ρ > .444, p < .05 = *; ρ > .561, p < .01 = **. (-) = no variation. 9.6.4 Discussion on the semantic tasks The present study indicated that both AD groups had difficulty in tasks requiring verbal and non-verbal semantic processing and working memory functioning. The miAD group showed selectively impaired semantic abilities. They performed as well as the NC group on tasks that required recognition of noun and verb categories and category members, as well as naming of very familiar category-related photographed objects (i.e., clothes, vegetables, vehicles, and animals). However, their performance deteriorated on the BNT and the Token Test. They also fared worse than the NC group when naming single verbs and series of semantically related nouns and verbs that required simultaneous processing of various types of semanti- 158 Results cally related information. Relative to the NC group, the moAD group had remarkable difficulty in all types of semantic processing, including the easiest tasks. Comparing the performance between the AD groups, the miAD group fared significantly better only in the category recognition tasks. The findings of the present study give support to Hodges and Patterson (1995), Huff (1988), Mickanin et al. (1994), and Laine, Vuorinen et al. (1997) who observed that the semantic impairment in AD could be revealed by using both verbal and non-verbal tasks. On the basis of the control tasks, it can be concluded that both AD groups displayed impaired naming of concrete nouns and verbs. The finding is consistent with many other studies (e.g., Kirshner et al. 1984; Kertesz et al. 1986; Bowles et al. 1987; Nebes 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Nicholas et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 1996; White-Devine et al. 1996; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; Cappa et al. 1998; Tröster et al. 1998; Williamson et al. 1998; Kim & Thompson 2001; see 2.4.2). Naming of concrete objects may be better preserved in the miAD group than in the moAD group, whereas naming of concrete actions appeared to be impaired in both groups. Moreover, subjects in the moAD group tended to suffer from significantly more severe naming difficulties than subjects in the miAD group. Worsening of the naming difficulties thus seems to be associated with the severity of dementia, a finding in accordance with several others (e.g., Bayles & Tomoeda 1983; SkeltonRobinson & Jones 1984; Bowles et al. 1987; Shuttleworth & Huber 1988; Smith et al. 1989; Bayles et al. 1990; Hodges & Patterson 1995). The present study suggests that the information needed for recognizing semantic categories and in-category members of both nouns and verbs was likely to be intact in the miAD group but impaired in the moAD group. The finding opposes the general notion according to which superordinate information is likely to be relatively preserved in AD (e.g., Warrington 1975; Schwartz et al. 1979; Martin & Fedio 1983; Martin et al. 1985; Huff et al. 1986; Chertkow et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Tippett et al. 1995). There are other recent studies that have also indicated deteriorated information processing at the superordinate level in AD (Grossman, D’Esposito et al. 1996; Laatu et al. 1997; Laine, Vuorinen et al. 1997; see also Hodges et al. 1992; see 2.4.1). On the other hand, the finding that the more specific information needed for disambiguating category co-ordinates may be impaired in AD partly supports many previous studies (e.g., Warrington 1975; Schwartz et al. 1979; Martin & Fedio 1983; Martin et al. 1985; Huff et al. 1986; Chertkow et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Tippett et al. 1995; see 2.4.1). On the basis of the present study, however, it can be assumed that the impaired semantic processing at the super- and subordinate level may not involve all AD patients but is likely to be restricted to the moderate and later phases of the disease. In this sense, the present study supports the finding of Hodges and Patterson (1995) who indicated that the deterioration of processing category information progressed with advancing dementia. Nevertheless, indicated by the performance on the Token Test, comprehension of more complex language was impaired in both AD groups, a finding consistent with earlier studies (Swihart et al. 1989; Tomoeda et al. Results 159 1990). It should be borne in mind, however, that the Token Test might not just measure linguistic abilities (i.e., semantic and syntactic knowledge), but it probably involves other cognitive functions, such as working memory, attention, visuospatial, and praxis abilities that may be affected as well in AD and, therefore, contribute to the linguistic processing (see Rochon, Waters & Caplan 1994). The variety of performance found in the subject groups on the different tasks may be explained by some of the tasks being semantic-cognitively more demanding than others. For example, in the category recognition and the card-sorting tasks, which were meant to measure non-verbal category identification, the miAD group performed like the NC group on the former but considerably worse on the latter. The recognition tasks required identification of only one item from a set of unrelated items, whereas the card-sorting tasks required active and simultaneous processing of semantically related and unrelated items, as well as self-initiated executive functioning. A reduction in naming was not found in the miAD group when naming of single, concrete objects related to the four semantic categories was involved. However, when they were asked to name the same items as a set of closely related category members, which required rapid and flexible functioning of the whole mental lexicon, a remarkable reduction in their noun production took place. The finding that different experimental tasks may impose differing cognitive demands supports the notions discussed widely by Nebes (1989, 1992; see also Nebes et al. 1984, 1989; Bayles 2003). Nebes et al. noticed that AD patients were rather successful at tasks in which the semantic information was heavily constrained and guided by the task and the stimuli (e.g., in the priming conditions, see 2.4.1). At the same time, they were likely to have difficulty in tasks that required intentional search or manipulation of the semantic memory or making conscious decisions and judgements about semantic information (e.g., fluency tasks). Such semantic processing makes heavy demands on the capacity of the working memory (e.g., executive and attentional functions), which is known to deteriorate in AD (e.g., Diesfeldt 1985; Baddeley et al. 1986, 1991; Ober et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1989; Bayles 2003; Nebes 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Morris 1994; Pasquier et al. 1995; Rosen & Engle 1997). Consequently, a deficit in working memory functions, indicated by the significantly weakened performance on the digit span task in both AD groups, is likely to have had an effect on performing some of the control tasks (e.g., the Token Test) and the fluency task. For example, the emergence of perseverations may be explained as an impaired ability to monitor the output and to remember the previously produced words (see Rochon et al. 1994; Rosen & Engle 1997). Concerning the correlations between the correct responses on the fluency tasks and the control tasks, it was found that both the noun and the verb fluency performance of all subject groups correlated significantly with the Token Test. The finding is in line with Ober et al. (1986) and Swihart et al. (1989), as far as production of nouns is concerned. The significant correlation between the tasks may reflect the multi-faceted nature of the tasks, each of them requiring complex linguistic and non-linguistic processes (Rosen 1980; Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:5; Chertkow & 160 Results Bub 1990; Auriacombe et al. 1993; Rochon et al. 1994; Ruff et al. 1997; Cohen & Stanczak 2000). The correlation between the semantic fluency tasks and the BNT being significant only in the miAD group for both nouns and verbs remains somewhat puzzling, considering that a correlation between those tasks has previously been explained by both of them involving semantic retrieval performance (Ober et al. 1986; Randolph et al. 1993; cf. Bayles et al. 1989). However, these tasks may involve partly different semantic-cognitive processes, which may explain the lack of correlation between the tasks in the NC group. Although all correlations did not seem to form a regular and easily interpreted pattern, it was discovered that in the miAD group fluency performance on the noun categories was highly correlated with the verbal tasks, whereas in the moAD group, a significant association was found with tasks requiring both verbal and non-verbal responses. Somewhat similar relationships existed for each AD group between the verb fluency task and the control tasks tapping verb processing. The correlation between the fluency task and the digit span in both AD groups may indicate that their fluency performances were also influenced by impaired working memory function, as discussed above. On the basis of the fluency tasks, the control tasks, and the correlations between them, it can be concluded that semantic processing was impaired in both AD groups and that the impairment was likely to show a progressing pattern: In the miAD group, the impairment could be revealed by cognitively more demanding tasks, such as semantic fluency and naming tasks emphasizing verbal output, whereas in the moAD group, the semantic impairment could be indicated even by easier tasks, such as recognition tasks. However, it should be borne in mind that also nonlinguistic factors, such as deficit in working memory functions, probably had an impact on the semantic performance of the AD patients. Finally, on the basis of the fact that the miAD group performed significantly worse than the NC group on a greater number of tasks requiring the processing of verbs relative to those of nouns, it can be presumed that in AD, the ability to process semantic information contained by different verbs may be impaired earlier than information contained by concrete nouns. In other words, the semantic features making up verbs may be more vulnerable to damage caused by AD than the semantic features contained by concrete nouns. 10 General discussion The semantic fluency task has been considered an easy and useful task for clinicians to assess spoken word production and the organization and functioning of the lexical-semantic system in different subject groups (see chap. 5). The semantic fluency task, in fact, is very complex and attempts have been made to operationalize the task and reveal the underlying semantic-cognitive processes (i.e., clustering and switching) required for performing the task. However, the task is also related to very fundamental cognitive processes, such as categorization and word production, but very little attention has been paid to clarifying these processes in conjunction with the semantic fluency task. Furthermore, the way semantic information is represented and processed in semantic memory, which is one of the most fundamental aspects of the task, has very seldom been looked into in detail. The present study was an attempt to discuss these elements and shed light on how they may be related to performing the semantic fluency task with different types of semantic categories and grammatical classes. In addition to providing detailed information about how the semantic fluency task was performed by healthy elderly adults and AD patients with mild and moderate dementia on different noun and verb categories, the findings of the present study provided empirical support to some of the theoretical issues discussed above. The multiple and varying responses given for the semantic fluency tasks by individual participants in the present study may reflect the multifaceted principles of categorization, which underlies any semantic processes (see 3.1). As humans are able to use different processes to grasp the similarities and differences of objects and actions around them, semantic memory is likely to be built up of a vast amount of different types of encoded information of these entities and their relations. Physical and functional similarities, as well as thematic and temporal-causal relations underlying the cluster formation in the semantic fluency tasks, may reflect the way different entities and actions are categorized and related to each other in semantic memory. The ways clusters are formed may also be taken as support for the notion that the information contained by semantic memory can be described in terms of semantic features and semantic roles that are connected to each other, and that can very 162 General discussion flexibly be processed for different purposes of the language (see 3.2, 3.3.3). The processes taking place during the semantic fluency performance, such as integrating the semantically related information in the semantic memory and preparing it for spoken word production, can be well understood in light of the current connectionist interactive models (see 4.2). Furthermore, the errors taking place during the semantic fluency performance can be explained by those theories. 10.1 Semantic fluency performance in mild and moderate Alzheimer’s disease The general aim of the present study was to find out how mildly and moderately demented AD patients performed the semantic fluency task with noun and verb categories. The more specific objective of the study was to reveal AD patients’ word production strategies in the tasks, and to analyze the content of their responses via errors (i.e., intrusions and perseverations), types of semantic subcategories employed, and the average frequency and prototypicality of the words. 10.1.1 Decreased semantic fluency performance The findings of the present study indicated that the ability of the AD patients to dynamically and flexibly process and use the semantic information corresponding to a semantic category, to integrate related information as clusters of semantically or phonologically similar words, and to switch between different semantic dimensions to guarantee optimal word production, was reduced compared to the healthy elderly adults. The findings confirm those of several previous studies (Martin & Fedio 1983; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989, 1998; Binetti et al. 1995; Carew et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998). Unlike the earlier studies, the present study broadened the scope of the semantic fluency task and involved a detailed analysis to cover the performance not only on the category of animals, but also on other semantic categories of concrete nouns (i.e., clothes, vegetables, vehicles) and verbs (i.e., verbs denoting the actions of preparing food, playing sports, construction, and cleaning up). Although it was found that the AD patients produced significantly fewer responses than the normal control subjects, the performance of the subjects was, to some extent, affected by the nature of the semantic category. The present study provided information about how the severity of the dementia affected semantic fluency performance. It was found that, some semantic categories notwithstanding, the miAD patients were significantly poorer than the subject in the NC group but better than the moAD patients at processing the semantic-lexical information and producing words, clusters, and switches for both types of tasks. The finding that the fluency performance was affected by the severity of dementia is in accordance with earlier studies (e.g., Huff et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Crossley et al. 1997). General discussion 163 Despite the marked reduction in word production in both AD groups, it was discovered that the performance of all subject groups was semantically relatively coherent. The clusters produced for the tasks tended to be of equal size and the proportion of clustered words appeared to be similar in the subject groups. As far as clustering of nouns was concerned, the subject groups similarly utilized the semantic and phonological relatedness between the items for cluster formation. Furthermore, category violations were not produced regularly but in individual categories, mainly for verbs. Concerning cluster size, the finding of the present study supports that of Binetti et al. (1995) and opposes those presented by Beatty et al. (1997, 2000), Tröster et al. (1998), and Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. (1998) who claimed that the cluster size of the AD patients was significantly smaller than that of the healthy elderly adults. The prominent role of perseveration found in the performance of the AD patients in the present study, however, may explain the discrepancy between the findings and partly question the relevance of the method and the internal validity of these studies (see 10.1.2). The analyses of the contents of the clusters revealed that the AD groups showed a significantly reduced range of semantic subcategories for both noun and verb production. In the noun categories, the most relevant semantic dimensions for disambiguating the subpatterns of features for the cluster formation consisted mainly of thematic and functional features, that is, features denoting contextual, spatial, and temporal information of the items, as well as their functional purpose. Physical features denoting the appearance (e.g., parts, colour, or size) of objects or strictly taxonomic relations between words (e.g., birds, fish) emerged relatively rarely, mainly in the NC group and the miAD group. Thematic and functional features seemed to allow a cross-classification of different lexical items over the semantic subcategories, which may explain their high frequency of occurrence among the subject groups (e.g., Barsalou 1982, 1983; Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Lucariello et al. 1992; Nelson 1996:232-248; see 3.1.1). Relating object concepts on the basis of their physical features, that, in general, tends to require delicate differentiation between the feature patterns, may be more difficult for the AD patients than relating items according to common thematic and functional features. Physical (perceptual) features have been found to be more vulnerable to brain damage than functional features (Tyler et al. 2000), which may also contribute to the cluster formation in the semantic fluency task in AD. Although thematic features are supposed to be more lightly weighted and less discriminative and thus less effective as retrieval cues than functional and physical features, they may commonly fit several different lexical items and make cross-classification of items possible (see McClelland, Rumelhart et al. 1986; Persson 1995:79-80, 82; see 3.2). The present study indicated that the thematic features served well as integrating cues for cluster formation for all subject groups (see 9.2.4, 9.2.6). In fact, some of the thematic features may be very heavily weighted and well resistant to changes in semantic memory, because they are learnt first and early in childhood as the basis for category formation (Lucariello et al. 1992; see also Nelson 1996:232-248), origi- 164 General discussion nate from emotional and autobiographical experiences, and are culturally shared by many (Persson 1995:79; Funnell 2001). As far as the verb fluency task is concerned, the most foregrounded semantic roles that were used to activate semantic subgategories, and to guide activation further to more specific feature patterns, involved Instrument (denoting tools or instruments with which the action was carried out), Result (denoting the outcome or the result of the action), and Location (denoting spatial configurations of the action; see 9.4.3, 9.4.5). In the category of playing sports, the roles of Mover (denoting the entity carrying out any type of movement), as well as form-functional features denoting the part of the body doing the action, played an important role in disambiguating verbs for production. Sports verbs were also clustered on the basis of thematic features denoting temporal (seasonal) information implied by the verb. Sports verbs notwithstanding, verbs were also produced by applying the temporal-causal, sequential order (i.e., scripts, see 3.3.4), reflecting the order in which actions are likely to take place in the real world. Yet another way to cluster verbs was to integrate the semantic information sharing the manner in which actions are carried out (i.e., troponyms; Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Fellbaum 1998a:79; Marshall, Chiat et al. 1996; see 3.3.2). The analysis of the variety and distribution of the most common semantic subcategories showed that the performance of the AD patients lacked some of the semantic dimensions expressed by the control subjects, especially those that denoted different phases or parts of a script (see 9.4.3). Furthermore, the moderate AD patients produced fewer clusters of verbs denoting actions carried out with specific instruments. Instead, they tended to compensate for the specific verb forms by naming the semantic roles (e.g., Instrument) corresponding to the basic-level noun implied by the verb or by using general purpose verbs (e.g., ‘do’, ‘put’, ‘make’). Activation of words and different semantic dimensions for the verb categories seemed to be more limited than during the noun fluency task, which may reflect the smaller number of verbs than nouns in Finnish (Saukkonen et al. 1979:9-11) and a shallower semantic structure of the verb categories compared to the semantically rich organizational structure of the noun categories (Miller & Fellbaum 1991; Pajunen 1998; 2001:60-63). On the other hand, similar to some nouns, some of the verbs (e.g., ‘rinse’, ‘cut’, ‘lift’) appeared to cross over the boundaries of the semantic categories and to occur in different semantic contexts (e.g., ‘rinse’ was associated with the verbs of preparing food and cleaning up, ’cut’ with the verbs of preparing food and construction, and ‘lift’ with the verbs of playing sports and construction). Furthermore, the data of the present study seemed to support the notions that verbs may contain more than one semantic role (e.g., Mover and Agent in ‘walk’ and ‘cut’) and that verbs have strong connections to certain basic-level nouns that can be considered part of their semantic structure (see Engelkamp 1975; Huttenlocher & Lui 1979; Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Behrend 1990; Persson 1995:100, 102-104, 208-209; Jonkers & Bastiaanse 1996; Kersten & Billman 1997; see 3.3.2, 9.4.5). The data also showed that verbs were connected to each other by their close temporal-causal relationships (see 3.3.4, 9.4.5). General discussion 165 10.1.2 Errors as indicators of impaired semantic memory functioning The AD groups produced significantly more errors than the NC group, with the miAD group exhibiting a more accurate performance than the moAD group. The errors consisted mainly of semantically related intrusions and perseverations. The semantic categories seemed to have an impact on the accuracy of word production, some categories provoking more errors than others. However, despite the category of clothes, the error analysis appeared insufficiently sensitive to differentiate the AD groups from each other at the level of individual noun and verb categories, a finding parallel to that made by Ober et al. (1986) and Binetti et al. (1995). In general, the total number of intrusions in the noun categories remained relatively low, which is consistent with previous findings (Diesfeldt 1985; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Binetti et al. 1995, Carew et al. 1997; Suhr & Jones 1998). It was discovered that only in the category of clothes did a higher proportion of intrusions emerge and they were related to more advanced dementia, which is in accordance with earlier studies (Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Tröster et al. 1989; Beatty et al. 2000) and adds to the discussion about the nature of the semantic categories affecting the semantic performance in AD (Moss et al. 2002). Moreover, the present study indicated that intrusions could appear also in verb categories. Among those intrusions were concrete nouns referring to tools and instruments needed for different actions, which implied the occurrence of grammatical category violations in AD (i.e., syntagmatic errors), a finding in contrast with the results of Gewirth et al. (1984). One reason for the occurrence of intrusions during semantic fluency performance may be an unsuccessful feature integration in the semantic memory (see 9.2.6, 9.4.5, and 10.1.3). In particular, the present study revealed that very many perseverations were produced in both AD groups for both types of tasks. Even though the emergence of perseverations is well documented in some earlier studies (e.g., Tröster et al. 1989; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Suhr & Jones 1998), the nature and role of perseverations in the fluency performance of the AD patients has not been widely studied or discussed. The present study, the data of which originated from several semantic noun and verb categories, gives reason to assume that perseverations are a prominent feature of the semantic fluency performance among both mildly and moderately demented AD patients. Perseverations may, at least partly, be taken as a cause which diminishes the overall production of words, prevents the subject from forming clusters and easily changing the subcategories and dimensions to activate new words for the output, and affects the cluster size (see 9.2.2, 9.2.6, 9.4.2, 9.4.5, 9.5; see also Dell 1986; Laine 1989:75-80; Dell, Burger et al. 1997; Martin et al. 1994; Persson 1995:66). In other words, the variables, with which productivity, efficiency, and coherence of the word production are measured, may be affected by perseverations. In the present study, even though the subjects had to perform eight different semantic fluency tasks in a row, thus involving a heavy loading of the cognitive- 166 General discussion linguistic capacity, the proportion of perseverations did not accumulate in the last semantic categories (cf. Sandson & Albert 1987). The time pressure in the fluency tasks may have had an impact on the performance, as a consequence of which an adequate spread and decay of activation in the lexicon did not occur. Thus, previously produced words may have remained activated because they were not turned off properly by the self-inhibition mechanism, and the new items did not receive enough activation from the higher representations to compete successfully (see Dell 1986; Dell, Burger et al. 1997; cf. Schwartz et al. 1994). In a more relaxed setting, in which more time had been given for the activation to spread, the system might have edited out some of the perseverations. It can also be speculated that in AD perseverations may have appeared as a sign of an impoverished active vocabulary, due to semantic degradation or loss of semantic information, leading the subject to activate and repeat the same words and clusters over and over again (see 6.1). On the other hand, repetitions may have been an attempt to activate new routes or semantic dimensions to facilitate the retrieval of new words, as proposed also by Gruenewald and Lockhead (1980; see also Persson 1995:33-35). However, the present study, as well as that of Binetti et al. (1995), showed that activation of different semantic subcategories was difficult for the AD patients. Consequently, word retrieval from different semantic subcategories might have been hindered rather than facilitated by the increased number of perseverations in the AD patients. Generally speaking, perseverations may be of different kinds (e.g., Liepmann 1905:115-127; Luria 1965; Hudson 1968; Helmic & Berg 1976; Buckingham, Whitaker & Whitaker 1979; Sandson & Albert 1984, 1987; Bayles et al. 1985, 1993; Albert & Sandson 1986; Vilkki 1989; Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995a, b; Ramage et al. 1999). They may be produced when the failure to access a target in the lexicon triggers the activation of preceding targets that have been strongly primed due to related meaning and re-exited by spreading activation, thus raising the likelihood of their being retrieved. Factors such as reduced behavior regulation and planning (Luria 1965; Daigneault, Braun & Whitaker 1992), inability to change the mental set (Shindler, Caplan & Hier 1984), defective attention (Ober et al. 1986; Albert & Sandson 1986; Sandson & Albert 1987; Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995b), poor self-monitoring skills (Luria 1965; Shindler et al. 1984; Albert & Sandson 1986), and impaired inhibition of memory traces, and suppression of incorrect responses (Hudson 1968; Buckingham et al. 1979; Shindler et al. 1984; Sandson & Albert 1984; Bayles et al. 1985; Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995b) have been claimed to trigger perseverations. Damaged motor mechanism (Liepmann 1905; Luria 1965), general slowing of functions (Buckingham et al. 1979; see also Dell 1986), and paucity of ideas (Bayles et al. 1985) may also bring about perseverations. These symptoms seem to be familiar clinical findings of AD (see 2.2), and factors associated with performance on the semantic fluency task (5.1). Taken together, these disorders may refer to a dysfunction of working memory and its components which is found in AD patients (Baddeley et al. 1986, 1991; Baddeley 1992; Gathercole & General discussion 167 Baddeley 1993), and which may also contribute to their linguistic abilities and, consequently, the occurrence of perseverations (see Bayles 2003). The dysfunctions mentioned are related to changes in neural function that take place in AD and other neurological diseases (e.g., Luria 1965; Lees & Smith 1983; Sandson & Albert 1987; Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995b). The role of the frontal lobe lesions in the emergence of perseverations has been underscored by several authors (Luria 1965; Sandson & Albert 1987; Vilkki 1989; Shindler et al. 1984; Daigneault et al. 1992; see also Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995a), and it has also been related to fluency performance in the form of decreased switching (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998). Lesions to other regions of the brain, including left temporal and parietal areas, have also been associated with the appearance of perseverations (Buckingham et al. 1979; Albert & Sandson 1984; Sandson & Albert 1987; Vilkki 1989; see also Hotz & Helm-Estabrooks 1995a), also in AD (Bayles et al. 1985). The participation of temporal lobes is likely to be vital for clustering during the semantic fluency task (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Pihlajamäki et al. 2000). As far as the neuropathology of AD is concerned, the typical cortical lesion sites include the (pre)frontal and the temporo-parietal region, even in the mild stage of the disease (Braak & Braak 1991, 1996; Pirttilä & Erkinjuntti 2001; see 2.1, 2.3). Hence, more research on the occurrence of perseverations and their interaction with other variables of the fluency task, as well as other tasks measuring linguistic and non-linguistic skills, is needed before plausible conclusions can be made regarding their contribution to the word fluency performance. 10.1.3 Causes of the semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease The poor performance of the AD patients on the semantic fluency task and other tasks measuring the functioning of the semantic memory may be caused by deteriorated perceptual functions, loss of the semantic information, impaired access to the semantic information, or a combination of the last two causes (see chap. 6). The accounts of access and/or storage disorder, however, have been criticized for being too vague not only in dementia research, but also in aphasia research (see the discussion in Warrington 1975; Warrington & McCarthy 1983; Shallice 1988:274286; Rapp & Caramazza 1993; Persson 1995:43-44, 67-68; Harley 1998). There is a continuing controversy with regard to the criteria for a storage vs. access deficit. Furthermore, conclusions regarding the possible causes of the impairment may have been drawn on the basis of symptoms rather than taking a stand on the underlying semantic representation, its format and structure. This may also apply to some of the studies on the semantic fluency performance in AD, which seem to have concentrated on describing the overt behavior of the subjects in the task without taking an explicit position on the nature and functioning of the semantic memory or the semantic layer of the mental lexicon. Moreover, the findings obtained from the semantic fluency performance of the AD patients have not often been discussed in relation to word production theories. 168 General discussion An alternative approach to the access-storage issue is provided by connectionist models, according to which the semantic representation corresponds to dynamic and flexible patterns of activation across a conglomerate of different semantic microfeatures, which are triggered by the content of the input and processed quite automatically without a separate access-mechanism. The models also provide a direct access to the syntactic and phonological features of the item(s) in the form of spreading activation (e.g., Dell 1986; Dell & O’Seaghda 1991, 1992; McClelland, Rumelhart et al. 1986; Farah & McClelland 1991; Martin et al. 1994; Persson 1995:66-67; Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997; Foygel & Dell 2000; see 4.2). Nevertheless, the way the subjects produced responses (i.e., clusters and switches) on the semantic fluency tasks in the present study indicated that some sort of retrieval strategies were needed to perform the task, some of which probably required intentional search of semantic information while some others may have taken place quite automatically as a result of the activation spreading in the semantic network (see also Diesfeldt 1985; Laine 1989:5; Allen et al. 1993; Monsch et al. 1994; Binetti et al. 1995; Pasquier et al. 1995; Rosen & Engle 1997; Mayr & Kliegl 2000). As discussed earlier, some of the connectionist models assume that damage to the semantic microfeatures and/or the connections, noise-induction in the system, and changes in the rate at which activated information decays (Hinton et al. 1986; Hinton & Sejnowski 1986; Dell 1986; Smolensky 1986; Farah & McClelland 1991; Martin et al. 1994; Gonnerman et al. 1997; Devlin et al. 1998; Harley 1998; see 9.2.6, 9.4.5) may interfere with the performance on the semantic tasks. The notion is compatible with the finding that AD causes widespread damage to the brain and a loss of neurons (see 2.1), in that microfeatures are thought to work in a neuron-like fashion (e.g., Gonnerman et al. 1997; Harley 1998; Moss et al. 2002). Damage to semantic microfeatures may lead to a heterogeneous degradation in performance in AD, including naming, matching, and feature decisions (Harley 1998; Moss et al. 2002). Nevertheless, as far as word production is concerned, damaged or lost microfeatures may not affect only the functioning at the semantic level of the mental lexicon but also involve functioning at the lemma (lexical) and the phonological level, due to the interconnectivity between the levels (Harley 1998). Consequently, the performance of the subjects should not only reflect the functioning of the semantic memory or the semantic layer of the mental lexicon, but also the whole word production system. However, the interpretation of the loci of errors seems to depend on the theories. According to the two stage interactive model of word production introduced by Dell and his colleagues (see 4.2), semantic substitutions (i.e., semantically related intrusions produced for both noun and verb categories) are likely to occur during the lemma access while syntactic category violations (i.e., the occurrence of basic-level nouns among the verbs) probably take place during phonological access, because of its indifference to the information concerning the grammatical categories (Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997; see also Astell & Harley 1998; cf. Persson 1995:125-127, Bird, Howard et al. 2000). On the other hand, Persson (1995:125-130, 132-135, General discussion 169 177-182) speculated that damage to the semantic layer of the mental lexicon was most likely to be responsible for substitutions, although the post-semantic morphophonological processing may also have an effect on misguiding item selection. The reduced number of words produced in the AD groups may be interpreted as omissions resulting from suppression of connections between the semantic features or a total failure of activating and integrating semantic and phonological features corresponding to a lexical item (see Rumelhart, Hinton et al. 1986; Persson 1995:133-134, 159, 181; Laine & Martin 1996). Perseverations produced particularly by the AD patients may take place at the lemma level and their occurrence can be interpreted as a change in the rate at which activated information decays, whereby the prolonged activation of a previous item interferes with the selection of upcoming information (Dell 1986; Dell, Burger et al. 1997; cf. Foygel & Dell 2000; see 9.2.6, 9.4.5). Unrelated, irrelevant words, which were also produced but in a very low number by all subject groups in the present study, are supposed to occur due to activation from non-related or distant connections to the target during lemma access and/or during phonological access when the correct word, which was selected at the lemma level, is substituted by another word at the phonological level (Dell, Schwartz et al. 1997). The occurrence of intrusions in the noun categories may also reflect the greater vulnerability of some categories, or more specifically, some types of semantic feature patterns, to damage in the semantic system. It has been suggested that due to the constellations of different types of semantic features, feature correlations, and distinctive features in the semantic categories, the semantic categories may be differently sensitive to the damage in the semantic network (see Gonnerman et al. 1997; Devlin et al. 1998; Garrard et al. 2001; Tyler et al. 2001; Tyler & Moss 2001; Moss et al. 2002; McRae & Cree 2002; Whatmough et al. 2003; see also Tversky & Hemenway 1984; Persson 1995:80-81). The more strongly inter-correlated features the items share with each other, the more robust they seem to be against damage because they provide enough mutual activation for appropriate features to be selected. The more weakly correlated features, the less mutual activation there is in the system, and the more vulnerable features are to damage. The present finding that very few category violations were made in the category of animals and vehicles while a number of violations took place at the border of the category of vegetables (e.g., fruits and berries) and, in particular, the category of clothes (e.g., bed linen) partly supports the notion that categories tend to be differently affected to damage in AD (see Gainotti et al. 1996; Gonnerman et al. 1997; Devlin et al. 1998; Moss et al. 2002; Whatmough et al. 2003). However, category violations in the categories of vegetables and clothes were also found among the normal control subjects, raising issues such as the type of features that make the critical correlations upon which the members of the categories can be distinguished, the fuzziness of the category boundaries in general, and the role of aging, gender, education, and life experiences as contributors to the semantic representation of category-specific information (see e.g., Labov 1973; Rosch 1975, 1978; Smith & 170 General discussion Medin 1981:22-60; Medin & Smith 1984; Barsalou 1982, 1983; McClelland & Kawamoto 1986:278; Lakoff 1987a; Tröster et al. 1989; Aitchison 1994:39-41; Crossley et al. 1997; Hampton 1998; Capitani et al. 1999; see 3.1.1). Equivalent to nouns, verbs and verb categories are also likely to consist of different constellations of semantic features and their correlations (Huttenlocher et al. 1983; Behrend 1990; Persson 1995:96-104; Kersten & Billman 1997; see 3.3.2, 3.3.3), but connectionist simulations and empirical studies with AD patients on the consequences of damage to the verbs semantics are, to my knowledge, so far lacking. However, applying the notions presented by Persson (1995:96-104; 145-146, 149; see also Reyna 1987; Robinson et al. 1996; Bird, Howard et al. 2000), it can be speculated that verbs may be more vulnerable to damage than nouns, because verbs, in general, consist of a restricted set of semantic features which are perceptually inferred or language-dependent (endogenous) information rather than perceptually transparent (sensory, exogenous) information, and their semantic structure is dependent on other items across different grammatical classes (e.g., basic-level nouns; see 3.1, 3.3). Consequently, damage to the features and connections may complicate the integration of widely spread information contained by verbs in AD patients. Because verbs may have less shared, less densely intercorrelated features, and fewer distinctive features, they may be less robust against the effects of the damage in AD than nouns. Evidence for this was provided by the moAD group producing syntagmatic errors for the verb fluency task and both AD groups performing worse on the control tasks requiring processing of verbs relative to nouns. It can also be concluded that the semantic representation of verbs may be affected earlier during the course of AD than that of concrete nouns. As a consequence of a more sparse semantic structure, verbs tend to be lower in imageability (i.e., more difficult to produce a mental image for a word) than nouns and, therefore, they tend to be slower and more difficult to produce (see Bird, Howard et al. 2000). Moreover, imagining verbs is likely to be more difficult than imagining nouns because forming an image of verbs requires the integration of dynamic sequences of actions, whereas objects are static and thus better suited to imagery encoding (Engelkamp et al. 1989; see also Reyna 1987; Helstrup 1989; Persson 1995:161; Fung et al. 2001). Given that semantic fluency performance is influenced by imagery (Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Mickanin et al. 1994), the reduced semantic structure of verbs, as well as nouns, may lead to poorer skills in forming mental images for word production in AD. The difference between the semantic representation of nouns and verbs has also been explained by their being processed by differently distributed neural systems and their being stored in anatomically distinct locations (e.g., Damasio & Tranel 1993; Martin et al. 1995; Koenig & Lehmann 1996; Perani et al. 1999; Tranel, Adolphs, Damasio & Damasio 2001; cf. Pulvermüller et al. 1999; Bird, Howard et al. 2000; Tyler et al. 2001), which may also have an impact on the difference in the semantic fluency performance on nouns and verbs in AD. General discussion 171 With regard to both nouns and verbs, there may be a tendency among the AD patients to replace more specific, distinctive, semantic information by more general information that could be shared by many other lexical items. On the basis of the content of the clusters, it seemed as if the moAD group used more thematic relations to form semantic clusters rather than combining semantically related words according to their structural or functional similarity. Furthermore, the tendency of the moAD group to use less specific verbs and to favour more general and all-purpose verbs can be interpreted as a trend towards more general information (see 3.3.2, 9.4, 9.4.5). These findings tentatively lend support to the notions suggesting that distinctive semantic features in particular may be damaged or lost in AD (Diesfeldt 1985; Harley 1998; Moss et al. 2002), but not until the moderate stage of the disease. Credence to the notion may be given by the significantly poorer performance of the moAD group on all control tasks. However, as discussed earlier, the errors made by AD patients may imply that they might not only have difficulties in processing semantic information, but also the grammatical and phonological information of words. In addition to the impaired semantic representation of nouns and verbs and impaired word production, deterioration of other cognitive functions may be yet another plausible factor explaining the quantitative and qualitative changes in the performance of AD patients on the semantic tasks relative to the normal control subjects. Impaired working memory and, especially, executive functions affecting planning, initiating, and monitoring one’s performance, cognitive flexibility to shift mental sets, and so on, are likely to have a negative impact on the performance of the AD patients, including the semantic fluency task (Diesfeldt 1985; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Kopelman 1994; Morris 1994; Rosen & Engle 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; cf. Binetti et al. 1996). On the other hand, Astell and Harley (2002) suggested that it is due to a failure in metalinguistic skills that AD patients fail to keep in mind the demands of a particular task, and regulate and monitor the retrieval and organization of the relevant material. However, the relation between the executive processes and the functioning of the semantic memory and the relation of the metalinguistic processes to the linguistic processes seem to be unclear and require further study (Kopelman 1994; Astell & Harley 2002). 10.2 Methodological considerations of the study 10.2.1 Subjects The medical background of the subjects participating in the present study was carefully checked because all AD patients were recruited from the Department of Neurology of the Helsinki University Central Hospital and the healthy elderly control subjects had previously undergone a thorough examination to rule out any neurological symptoms (see chap. 2, 8.1). However, all subjects voluntarily participated in the study, which may bias the selection towards fitter, more capable, and more motivated subjects and thus restrict the generalizability of the study. 172 General discussion The sizes of the subject groups in this study were small but they represented the average, or even larger than average, size used in other semantic fluency studies (see Table 4). Although a statistical difference in the ratio of male/female subjects did not emerge among the subject groups, both AD groups had more female participants, and thus gender may have had some biasing effect on the semantic fluency performance (see Monsch et al. 1992; cf. Capitani et al. 1999; Hebert et al. 2000; Troyer 2000; see also 5.1). However, the interpretation should be taken with caution because, as indicated by the male and female participants in the NC group, the semantic fluency performance was affected by gender only in two of the eight semantic categories, the male participants faring better on the category of construction and the female participants on the category of cleaning. Furthermore, no general gender effect was found in the total noun or verb production of the NC group (see 9.1.1, 9.3.1). In the future, larger samples, groups with an equal number of participants, and groups with an equal number of male and female subjects would be optimal for a more reliable comparison of semantic fluency performance. In the present study, the mental status of all the participants was assessed and the division of the AD patients into the miAD group and the moAD group was made using the Mini Mental State Examination (Folstein et al. 1975), which has been found to be a suitable method for assessing the severity of dementia (Rantakrans 1996:22-24). The division into mild and moderate AD groups was well founded taking into consideration the statistically highly significant differences between the subject groups, and the tendency of the higher scores on the MMSE to be associated with a better fluency performance across the semantic categories. Similar findings on the relationship between the scores on the MMSE and the fluency task have been reported in several other studies (e.g., Bayles et al. 1993; Mickanin et al. 1994; Hodges & Patterson 1995; Crossley et al. 1997). It should be kept in mind, however, that a short test like the MMSE does not cover the whole scope of cognitive and functional capacity of the participants. Furthermore, although the MMSE was used for grouping the subjects according to their mental status, a great heterogeneity in the fluency performance across different parameters in the subject groups was found (see also Della Sala et al. 1993; Hodges & Patterson 1995). The heterogeneity in the fluency performance may be partly caused by such factors as different occupations and expertise in particular fields, as well as autobiographical experiences and habits of the subjects, which are likely to affect the structure of semantic representation (e.g., Rosch et al. 1976; Barsalou 1992; Aitchison 1994:39-50; Taylor 1994:72-75, 79, 242; Ungerer & Schmid 1996:14-20; Azuma et al. 1997; Capitani et al. 1999; see 5.5). It should also be taken into account that fatigue and test anxiety may have obscured the performance of some subjects (Roberts & Le Dorze 1994). General discussion 173 10.2.2 Considerations of the semantic fluency task The most common measure derived from the semantic fluency task is the number of correct responses generated for the task, which, however, has proved insufficient to uncover the processes and strategies exploited by the subjects to perform the task, to highlight the semantic relationships between the words, or to reveal the types of errors made by subjects during the task (Ober et al. 1986; Laine 1989:18-19, 22-23; Allen et al. 1993; Bayles et al. 1993; Roberts & Le Dorze 1994, 1997; Binetti et al. 1995; Pasquier et al. 1995; Carew et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Troyer 2000; Suhr & Jones 1998; Tröster et al. 1998). Attempts to operationalize and to describe the multifactorial semantic fluency performance have been made (e.g., Gruenewald & Lockhead 1980; Laine 1989; Troyer et al. 1997), but the precise nature of this task remains unclear and complex to explain probably because of the many overlapping processes involved (Chertkow & Bub 1990; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Mayr & Kliegl 2000). There appears to be no agreement among the researchers on the nature of the parameters or on the most influential measures to best cover fluency performance (e.g., Binetti et al. 1995; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Tröster 1998; Rich et al. 1999; Mayr 2002). Choosing the best possible parameters to describe the performance on the semantic fluency task is not very easy because parameters tend to depend on each other (see Laine 1989:14; Beatty et al. 1997; Troyer et al. 1997; Mayr 2002). However, the parameters used for example in the studies of Troyer et al. (1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Troyer 2000) and Tröster et al. (1998; e.g., the number of correct words produced for the task, the number of switches, and the mean cluster size) do not seem to be sufficient to highlight all the important aspects, such as the semantics, of the fluency performance. In addition to these parameters, the total number of words produced for the tasks should be given in order to compare the difference between the total and the correct output. Reporting the number of clusters should also be included because it makes possible a more transparent comparison between the switching and clustering performance of the subjects. An analysis of switching alone is not informative enough because it includes both clusters of words and single words that, as such, do not reveal the true clustering of words along some semantic or associative relation. Moreover, the mean cluster size may not be enough to cover the clustering performance or to reveal the coherence of the behavior in the task because it can be given on the basis of only one single cluster. Reporting the mean cluster size does not provide enough information about the overall tendency of the subject to cluster words as does reporting the proportion of clustered words. Nevertheless, taking into account the number of clusters alone does not differentiate between repeated subcategories and those used only once because subjects may keep producing words from the same subcategories over and over again (see also Laine 1989:25). Therefore, counting the number of different 174 General discussion subcategories from which clusters are formed may better reflect the ability of the subject to use the multifaceted information contained by the semantic memory. Finally, a more detailed error analysis, although it did not turn out to be sensitive enough to differentiate the subject groups in all semantic categories in the present study, should be provided. Taking into account the types and the proportion of errors produced by subjects may highlight the processes of word production and the robustness of the semantic categories involved in the task. Instead of considering clustering and switching as two separate processes, the former involving semantic processes mediated by the temporal lobe and the latter involving the executive processes associated to the frontal lobe (see Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al 1998; Troyer 2000; Tröster et al. 1998), the notion that both clustering and switching are part of the semantic processing is put forth by Mayr and Kligel (2000). They claimed that the semantic process involved the betweencluster and the in-cluster retrieval of words in the form of relatively automatic spreading of activation in the semantic network and that the executive control was likely to accompany each act of word retrieval in the form of, for example, updating the current search criterion and stopping or initiating retrieval processes (see also Ober et al. 1986; Bayles et al. 1989; Mayr 2002). Switching may thus involve an ability of the semantic system to activate and flexibly integrate patterns of features corresponding to different subcategories or semantic dimensions in the semantic network. As a consequence, a new feature set and its strong connections to other features, which correspond to the semantically related category co-ordinates or items belonging thematically or functionally together, are activated and elaborated for further processing in the mental lexicon and the articulatory apparatus. Overtly, the process can be viewed as the subject producing a cycle of clusters of semantically related words and switches to semantic subcategories for other sets of related words. In the present study, the mean cluster size and the proportion of words in the clusters were calculated in order to measure the activation, as well as the coherence and efficiency, of the production of semantically related words (cf. Beatty et al. 1997, 2000; Troyer et al. 1997, Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Tröster et al. 1998). As stated earlier, the cluster size and the proportion of words produced in clusters did not differ between the subject groups in very many categories (see 9.1.2, 9.3.2). Mainly, the differences were located between the NC group and the moAD group in the category of clothes, preparing food, and cleaning. Consequently, depending on the semantic category, the finding could be interpreted as the AD groups being as capable of activating items sharing highly inter-correlated features in semantic memory and having semantically as coherent and as efficient a process as the NC group in their semantic feature disambiguation and integration. Should this finding imply an intact and normal functioning of the semantic memory in AD, as suggested by Tröster et al. (1998) who claimed that a reduced cluster size was a sign either of impaired lexical and semantic memory or a difficulty of access to the memory stores? Taking into consideration the increased number of General discussion 175 perseverations in the performance of both the miAD and the moAD group throughout the categories, it may be possible that perseverations have an enlarging effect on the clustering performance (i.e., number of clusters, size of clusters, and proportion of clustered words). Thus, a straightforward interpretation of the integrity of or the ease of functioning of semantic memory in light of these parameters should not be taken before the location of the perseverations (i.e., in or between the clusters) is first controlled. Counting the perseverations as part of a cluster may, in general, jeopardize the internal validity of the study and obscure the results and interpretation of the performance of the AD patients. A separate registering of intact clusters or clusters from which perseverations have been ruled out and clusters containing perseverations would make the comparison between the performance of the AD patients and the normal control subjects more plausible. If the number of different subcategories exploited during the task was not registered, the reduced number of clusters and the relatively high proportion of clustered words found in both AD groups could be interpreted as normal but slowed performance on the semantic fluency task (see Laine 1989:25). However, the number of subcategories and semantic fields appeared to be a sensitive variable because it differentiated the subject groups and gave reasons to assume that the lexical-semantic processing did not work well in either AD group. Relative to the NC group, both AD groups produced words from significantly fewer subcategories for all noun categories and for all but one verb category, and a remarkable reduction of subcategory exploitation was found in the moAD group. Consequently, registering only the number of clusters or the proportion of clustered words may not be enough to account for semantic fluency performance, because the subjects may repeat the same few semantic subcategories and even the same clusters of items during the task. In order to control the re-occurrence of the same subcategories and words, as well as to control the variety in activating the semantic space, the number of different subcategories should be involved as one parameter. The clustering rules applied according to those described by Troyer et al. (1997; Troyer 2000; see also Rich et al. 1999) appeared to be somewhat unspecific (see also Kaleva & Vanhala 2001). For example, Troyer et al. did not provide information on how large the size of a small cluster was supposed to be in order for it to be embedded together with another small cluster. In the present study, small clusters of the maximum size of three words were embedded if a common denominator was found. Applying the clustering rules to find how verbs were produced for the task was at times complicated because verbs were produced in a script-like manner, the temporo-causal information guiding the production. Moreover, verbs were not only produced as one-word responses by the subjects, which were encouraged during the rehearsal category, but also as a variety of word forms, including phrases. All in all, to avoid too subjective decisions about ambiguous clusters and responses, it was necessary to have a sample of the data analyzed by an independent rater. Afterwards, the analyses were collected and all the ambiguous points discussed. 176 General discussion Using the absolute number of responses (e.g., the number of switches, clusters, and subcategories) instead of proportional scores to analyze the verbal fluency performance of the AD patients, and to compare the performances between different subject groups, may be questionable because any reduction in the total number of words generated in a category is likely to be associated with a reduced number of any of the parameters dependent on the total output (see e.g., Laine 1989:25; Binetti et al. 1995; Troyer et al. 1997; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. 1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach et al. 1998; Troyer 2000; Tröster et al. 1998). However, proportional scores may be uninformative and partly misleading because subjects with different numbers of total words and other parameters may come up with the same proportional scores, as stated also by Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander et al. (1998; see also Troyer 2000) and Rich et al. (1999). For example, a subject with a low total of 8 words and 4 switches in the animal fluency task would have a proportional score of 50% for the switches as would another subject with a high total of 16 words and 8 switches (Rich et al. 1999). Thus, the proportional scores being equal, they may hide information about the variation in the performances between the subject groups. Support for the decision to use the raw scores to describe the fluency performance can also be found in the study by Tröster et al. (1998), who attempted to analyze switching in proportion to total word output but came up with a conclusion that the proportional measure of switching did not give an appropriate picture of the fluency performance, because the subjects performed the task in different ways. For example, of the subjects with an equal number of words, those who employ the strategy of clustering necessarily make fewer switches than those who do not cluster words during the task. To enable comparison between the performances in different semantic categories for one, and to ensure the registering of major tendencies in the fluency performance for the other, the present study provided the fluency data both as separated for individual semantic categories and as averaged across the different categories. A combined score over the different semantic categories may reveal larger trends in the overall performance and offer more stability and reliability than the scores provided by a single category (Monsch et al. 1992, 1994, 1997). Providing only averaged results across the categories, however, may mask category-specific information, as discovered in the study of Capitani et al. (1999), in which the gender of the subjects was found to affect the naming of fruit and tools, with females faring better on the former and males on the latter. The data of the present study also indicated that specific categories differently affected the semantic fluency performance (see 9.1.1, 9.3.1). Presenting the fluency scores at the level of individual semantic categories is recommended not only to prevent the masking of information, but also because the categories tend to be different in their semantic structure, difficulty, size, and familiarity (e.g., Diesfeldt 1985; Ober et al. 1986; Hart et al. 1988; Bayles et al. 1989, 1993; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Hodges et al. 1992; Azuma et al. 1997; Crowe et al. 1998; Capitani et al. 1999; Mayr & Kliegl 2000; Moss et al. 2002; see 5.5). Furthermore, the categories General discussion 177 may be differently sensitive to the cognitive deficits in AD (e.g., Chertkow & Bub 1990; Silveri et al. 1991; Gonnerman et al. 1997; Moss et al. 2002). Consequently, if findings on the semantic fluency performance of the AD patients are based only on one semantic category (e.g., animals) they may lead to misleading conclusions about the effects of AD on the semantic memory and language (cf. Azuma et al. 1997). 10.2.3 Limitations of the study The conclusions drawn in this study are limited by several factors that should be kept in mind when generalizing the results. First of all, the theoretical notions and the experimental decisions taken, as well as the conclusions drawn in this study, concerned concrete nouns and verbs. Second, the study focused on discussing the semantics of the fluency task, leaving the role of working memory and executive functioning aside, although they have been found to affect semantic processing in AD (e.g., Bayles 2003). In the present study, more attention should have been paid to the selection and the order of presentation of the semantic categories. The verb categories should have a priori been controlled for gender insensitivity and selected to provide as wide a variety of different verb types as possible. In this study, goal-oriented verbs denoting results of an action were foregrounded. A category with a broad variety of verbs (e.g., what kind of hobbies do people have) might have replaced one of the three goal-oriented categories. The categories of preparing food and cleaning up could have been combined as a single category, household activities, whereby responses given for the category might have been more in number and from a wider range of activities (e.g., activities belonging to doing laundry, rearranging furniture, etc.). Similarly, the noun categories should also have been selected paying more attention to their semantic structure: either the category of clothes or the category of vehicles could have been replaced by a category with a richer structure of physical-functional features (e.g., tools and kitchen utensils) in order to determine whether possible damage to the distinguishing features found in AD could be reflected in the patients’ semantic fluency performance. The fixed order in which the semantic categories were presented to the subjects may have had an effect on the efficiency of performance towards the end of the task thus favoring the performance on the noun categories over the verb categories (see Hart et al. 1988). Therefore, it is recommended that in future studies, the order of the categories be systematically varied. The categories should have been controlled for difficulty and for the difficulty of noun vs. verb production (see Hart et al. 1988; Bayles et al. 1993; Azuma et al. 1997; Mayr & Kliegl 2000). Furthermore, to make comparison between the different retrieval modes (semantic and phonological) and to draw more reliable conclusions about the functioning of the different levels of the mental lexicon during word production, the phonemic fluency task should have been included in the study. 178 General discussion Because prototypicality and frequency ratings of the words were made by a small number of volunteers, conclusions about the effects of these factors on subjects’ word production should be taken with caution. Unfortunately, frequency corpuses of spoken language were not available in Finnish, and the existing corpuses were based on newspaper texts (e.g., Saukkonen et al. 1979; Laine & Virtanen 1999) and thus not appropriate for the use of the present purposes. Furthermore, they did not contain all the words produced by the subjects in this study. In order to have an objective control of the word frequencies in the future, a sample of words could be evaluated using the ratings of the corpuses of written Finnish. Considering the complex cognitive-linguistic processes involved in the semantic fluency task, the great variability between the performances of different subjects, and the very poor performance of some of the moderately demented subjects in this study, it is reasonable to think about the relevance of the task in experimental and clinical use. Although performing the task provides much information about how effectively and flexibly semantic information is processed and retrieved from semantic memory and how fluently words are produced, factors having an influence on performance seem to be many, intermingled, and difficult to interpret. Furthermore, the ability of a subject to perform the task may be sensitive to his or her mental agility and mood, which may sometimes cause an individual to fail in the task. In order to get a more reliable picture of the functioning of semantic memory by using the semantic fluency task, the task ought to be carried out by the same subjects, frequently, and regularly. The semantic fluency task should not be used alone to study semantic processing. In order to increase the reliability of the task, other tasks that also measure semantic functions should be employed. As discussed earlier, the outcome of the control tasks in the present study indicated that AD patients performed some of the tasks better than others. Nevertheless, they also indicated that the semantic functions of the AD patients were impaired compared to the healthy elders, and that semantic impairment was most obvious in the group of moderately demented AD patients. However, even though a number of other tasks were involved as control tasks that measured the overall semantic performance of the subjects in the present study, a more detailed qualitative analysis on the subjects’ performance on the control tasks would still be needed. Furthermore, the performance of the subjects on the semantic tasks should be assessed in relation to common neuropsychological tests, especially those measuring different memory functions. Finally, the present study, to the best of my knowledge, was the first attempt to broaden the use of the semantic fluency task and to investigate how the semantic information contained by verbs was utilized during word production by healthy elderly adults and patients with AD. Therefore, a replication of the study is required to validate the findings. General discussion 179 10.3 Clinical implications One of the starting points of this study was the need to find out more about the semantic factors underlying the semantic fluency task which is very commonly used in the clinical and experimental assessments of speech pathologists, among others. For the purpose of studying the linguistic functions of semantic memory and the speech production in patients with neural dysfunction, the theoretical issues discussed throughout the study provided an insight into the complex semantic structure of both concrete nouns and verbs, semantic representation being the prerequisite for naming also in the fluency tasks. This study provided information about how verbs may be represented semantically and about how they are produced in the semantic fluency task by healthy elderly adults and AD patients with different degrees of dementia. Furthermore, a wider analysis of the semantic fluency task was applied, including different semantic categories from two grammatical classes. An attempt was also made to integrate the semantics of the task and the current word production models. Semantic processing by healthy individual subjects can be very dynamic and flexible, resulting in a variety of combinations of words during the semantic fluency task. The present study gave guidelines for the quantitative and qualitative performance of the healthy elderly controls over several semantic categories that can later be used as the basis for comparing and assessing the performance of other subjects (e.g., subjects with different types of dementia or aphasia). The present study also provided information about the reduction and changes in the fluency performance among patients with mild and moderate AD, which was the other main objective of the study. The error analyses notwithstanding, the findings concerning the overall fluency performance (i.e., number of correct words, switches, and clusters) can be of use when staging the severity of dementia in AD. The study sketched a detailed method of analyzing the semantic fluency performance with both nouns and verbs. As pointed out before, the analysis should not be restricted to just counting total and correct responses, but it should also include the switching and clustering performance of the subjects. The number of different semantic subcategories used for the cluster formation, in particular, may be a sensitive parameter to reveal the subjects’ ability to use the vast amount of information contained by the semantic memory. In the present study, it was among the few parameters which was powerful enough to differentiate the performance of the control subjects and the patients with mild and moderate AD from each other on both noun and verb categories. Furthermore, although the error analysis did not distinguish between the subject groups, it may give an insight into the nature and the location of the deficits in the functioning of the mental lexicon. Considering the differences between semantic categories, it is recommended that more than one semantic category is used when the fluency task is applied to investigate the semantic memory of AD patients (see also Bayles et al. 1993). Therefore, the categories should be chosen with care. As far as nouns are concerned, 180 General discussion categories should be selected from both living and non-living domains, keeping in mind the differences in their semantic composition (see 3.1.2, 3.2). In order to control for hesitations or semantic degradation at the borders of the different categories, a category with a low probability of semantic intrusion (e.g., animals) and a category with a higher probability of semantic intrusion (e.g., vegetables, fruit, or clothes) may be included. Taking into account that semantic memory consists not just of information for concrete nouns and/or objects, it is recommended that the semantic fluency task be extended to cover other grammatical classes, such as verbs. Concerning the selection of verb categories, categories providing verbs that denote different types of action are worth considering (e.g., categories providing instrument verbs, goaldirected verbs, and motion verbs). For the reasons described previously, it is also recommended that the performance of the subjects be examined by each semantic category at a time, rather than averaging the performance by combining the test scores over several semantic categories. A summative score can be used to reveal larger trends in semantic functioning, with the reservation that significant information may be obscured (see the discussion in Capitani et al 1999; cf., Huff et al. 1986; Fischer et al. 1988; Diesfeldt 1989; Monsch et al. 1992, 1994, 1997; Bayles et al. 1993; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Suhr & Jones 1998). Because the semantic fluency task involves several cognitive processes and because the parameters derived from the task tend to be inter-dependent, other tasks measuring semantic memory and other cognitive functions should be included in the studies (see Chertkow & Bub 1990; Monsch et al. 1992). Conclusions about the semantic fluency performance should be drawn in conjunction with the observations obtainable from other semantic tasks, such as confrontation naming, definition, and recognition tasks. 10.4 Implications for further study In order to provide more reliable findings on the semantic fluency performance of healthy control subjects and AD patients with mild and moderate dementia, the limitations of the present study discussed earlier translate into essential needs for further study (see 10.2.3). First of all, the role of perseverations produced for the semantic fluency performance requires further investigation. In order to draw conclusions about the efficiency of the functioning of the semantic memory and the coherence of the performance during the task, the exact location of perseverations in the output of normal controls and AD subjects should be examined. More specifically, the emergence of the perseverations in and between clusters should be checked. Should more perseverations be found in clusters in AD groups than in the group of normal control subjects, it may explain why the cluster size remained the same among the subject groups in many semantic categories. Furthermore, the role of perseveration in the control tasks (e.g., confrontation naming task, serial naming task) needs to be taken General discussion 181 into consideration in more detail. In general, it would be very important to find out if there is a difference in the nature of the perseverations produced by AD subjects compared to ones made by healthy control subjects (see Sandson & Albert 1984, 1987; Albert & Sandson 1986; Bayles et al. 1993; Ramage et al. 1999). It would also be informative to know if there is a tendency for the moderate AD patients to produce more deviant perseverations relative to the mild AD patients. In the future, the emergence of perseverations in the semantic fluency task should be related to the performance in certain neuropsychological tests measuring executive processes (e.g., the Trail Making Test and the Stroop Test; see Lezak 1995:373-376, 381-384). The data of the present study provides an opportunity to deepen the qualitative analysis of the semantics of the responses given during the fluency tasks, as well as the control tasks, and to compare the findings among the subject groups. Because of the great variability in the performance in each subject group, the semantic fluency performance should be investigated at the level of the individual subject to get a profile of an individual’s performance over several categories. In order to get a more profound profile of an individual’s semantic performance, case studies can be extended to also cover the performance of the subject on different control tasks. In order to find out more about the nature of the associations between the noun responses, a closer look at the cluster formation is required. The embedded clusters should be broken down in order to better investigate the emergence of pure taxonomic and physical-functional associations vs. thematic associations in the subject groups. As far as verbs are concerned, a closer look at the responses given for the verb categories, especially the composition of the semantic roles contained by these verbs, would shed light on the semantic representation of verbs and add knowledge about the changes of the structure of the semantic memory that are likely to take place in AD. The occurrence of different verbs (e.g., instrument vs. non-instrument verbs) would provide further information about the types of verbs that are prone to emerge in the semantic fluency task. In the future, in order to get an insight into the strategies exploited for cluster formation, subjects could be asked to specify the way they produced the words during the tasks. Furthermore, the differences found in the performance of the male and female participants in the NC group in some of the verb categories gives reason to examine the effects of gender on the verb fluency performance of AD patients. Further investigation of the data collected for the present study provides an opportunity to participate in two ongoing discussions. Firstly, in order to examine the presence of the category-specific disorder that has been found to differently affect the semantic representations of living and non-living categories in AD, the data obtained from the semantic fluency tasks and the control tasks can be divided into living (i.e., vegetables and animals) and man-made entities (i.e., clothes and vehicles; see, e.g., Bayles et al. 1989; Chertkow & Bub 1990; Crown-Golomb et al. 1992; Rosser & Hodges 1994; Binetti et al. 1995; Gainotti et al. 1996; Moss et al. 2002; see also 3.1.2, 3.2, 5.5). Secondly, it is possible to analyze in more detail how nouns vs. verbs are processed in the subject groups in the different tasks. More 182 General discussion specifically, investigations can be expanded to uncover the patterns in which words are produced for inanimate categories vs. verb categories, both of which typically contain functional information (see Bird, Howard et al. 2000, 2001; Bird, Lambon Ralph et al. 2000; Shapiro & Caramazza 2001a, b; see also 3.3.3). The present study has also brought up several new aspects of the semantic fluency task that would be interesting and valuable to investigate. New data with larger groups of participants and a more balanced distribution of gender could be collected. A new set of categories could be used for which the factors such as varying size, familiarity, and semantic differences between the different noun and verb categories were better controlled (see Hart et al. 1988; Bayles et al. 1993; Azuma et al. 1997; Mayr & Kliegl 2000). In order to better control for the occurrence of intrusions, semantically closely related categories that are likely to provide category violations could be employed (e.g., vegetables vs. fruit or tools vs. kitchen utensils or vehicles). Moreover, AD patients’ behavior in the semantic fluency tasks could be contrasted with other types of fluency tasks. For example, the phonemic and the supermarket fluency task could be included, as well as tasks measuring production of adjectives (e.g., states of mind), abstract words (e.g., occupations), names (e.g., celebrities), and non-verbal images (see Mickanin et al. 1994). Further, it would be of interest to examine the fluency performance of those AD patients who have deteriorated language skills and those whose language abilities are better preserved, using both noun and verb fluency tasks (cf., Beatty et al. 2000). In order to investigate the effects of different dementing processes on the semantic representation of nouns and verbs, the semantic fluency performances of the AD patients could also be compared with patients having other types of dementia (e.g., fronto-temporal dementia, vascular dementia, etc.). A longitudinal study between the subject groups would reveal if there exists a systematic pattern of decline in the semantic information in the different dementing diseases. Moreover, having monovs. bilingual (e.g., Finnish vs. Finnish-Swedish) AD patients perform the task could shed light upon the effects of AD on different languages, as well as on the nature of the semantic associations produced for the task. The scope of the investigation could be broadened to cover the performance of normal elderly subjects and AD patients on the variation of the semantic fluency task in which subjects are asked to produce different types of action that belong to a particular script (e.g., going to the doctor or making coffee; see Weingartner et al. 1983; Grafman et al. 1991; 3.3.4, 5.1). It would be worthwhile to carry out a semantic analysis of the errors taking place and the strategies used to perform the task by normal elderly subjects and by AD patients, and to compare these errors and strategies to those found in the traditional semantic fluency tasks. Because both nouns and verbs may be produced for such a script and because they can be considered appropriate responses (see Lucariello & Rifkin 1986; Fivush 1987; Nelson 1996:231248), the effects of AD on the occurrence of those grammatical classes could be examined in more detail in a script-like framework. 11 Conclusions On the basis of the present study, the following conclusions can be made: 1. During the semantic fluency task with the noun categories (superordinates) as the category constraint, the miAD and the moDA groups showed a significant reduction in total and correct word production relative to the NC group. Both AD groups also showed a remarkable reduction in the extent to which clustering and switching were used as the strategy to prompt noun production. The severity of dementia significantly affected the overall semantic fluency performance, which was indicated by the miAD group producing a greater total number of nouns and more correct nouns, as well as more clusters and switches, than the moAD group. Despite the smaller number of clusters produced in the AD groups, the size of the clusters and the proportion of all clustered nouns tended to remain the same among the subject groups. The finding might imply that the exploitation of the subcategories, once activated, and the tendency to produce nouns according to their shared properties worked equally well and coherently in the subject groups. Nevertheless, the emergence of the perseverations in the fluency performance of the AD groups is likely to affect the interpretation of the finding (see conclusions 2 and 6). 2. The integrity of the semantic categories appeared to be relatively intact in all subject groups, indicated by very few category violations. Only in the category of clothes did the moAD group show a significant increase in the number of intrusions that may imply an altered or deficient pattern of semantic feature activation and integration. Instead, perseverations, the inability to deactivate previously produced responses, seemed to be a prominent feature characterizing the performance of both AD groups in virtually every semantic category. However, the error analysis did not appear very sensitive in differentiating between the miAD group and the moAD group in individual semantic categories. Only the combined proportion of perseverations produced across all semantic categories differentiated the two AD groups from each other. 184 Conclusions When forming clusters, all subject groups produced nouns not only according to their semantic associations, but also using their phonological relatedness. The miAD group tended to form clusters from a significantly more restricted set of semantic subcategories than the NC grorup, but from a significantly wider range than the moAD group. For the NC group and the miAD group, formation of the semantic clusters was based on physical, functional, and thematic features shared by nouns, whereas the clusters produced in the moAD group were mainly formed on the basis of less specific (physical) but more robust functional and thematic relatedness between nouns. Overall, both AD groups tended to use more frequently occurring nouns than the NC group, whereas the degree of prototypicality of the responses between the AD groups and the NC group remained the same in all other categories but vehicles. The degree of frequency and prototypicality of nouns did not differentiate between the two AD groups. 3. The miAD group produced a significantly smaller number of words than the NC group in the semantic fluency task with verb categories. Nevertheless, the reduction in verb production did not occur in all semantic categories. Once they activated the semantic space, the NC group and the miAD group clustered verbs and switched between the semantic dimensions in some categories to an equal extent, while in some others, the number of clusters and switches was reduced in the miAD group. Relative to the NC group and the miAD group, the moAD group was significantly impaired in the ability to generate verbs, clusters of verbs, and switches in all semantic categories. Performance of the miAD and the NC group were semantically as coherent and efficient in each semantic verb category, as far as the cluster size and the proportion of all clustered verbs were concerned. Instead, the moAD group showed a significantly less coherent and less effective pattern of verb production than the NC group and the miAD group, but only in some of the verb categories. However, the appearance of perseverations in the fluency performance of the AD groups affects the interpretation concerning the AD groups’ coherent and efficient semantic fluency performance (see conclusions 4 and 6). 4. The responses given for the verb categories in all subject groups not only included specific verbs, but also nominalized verb forms, general verbs, phrasal structures, and noun responses. The NC group and the miAD group did not seem to differ in their responses, whereas the moAD group tended to produce fewer specific verbs and more general all-purpose verbs and nouns than the NC group. The finding was interpreted as an impaired activation of specific semantic features of verbs, a tendency to process the semantic information of verbs at a more general level, and as a sign of the dependency of verbs on other parts of speech. Both AD groups showed signs of an altered and deficient semantic feature activation by producing significantly more intrusions, mainly concrete nouns, than the NC group. Both AD groups also showed a tendency to get stuck on Conclusions 185 previous responses by producing significantly more perseverations than the NC group. However, intrusions and perseverations did not seem to characterize the performance of the AD groups in all semantic categories. Relative to the miAD group, the moAD group produced significantly more intrusions indicating a more severe change or damage to the semantic feature activation of verbs. The number of perseverations remained the same between the AD groups. The variety of subcategories from which verbs were produced tended to be significantly narrower in both AD groups than in the NC group. The severity of dementia appeared to have affected the semantic range with the miAD group being better able to activate a wider scope of semantic subcategories than the moAD group. The clusters formed by the NC group consisted of verbs denoting different kinds of motion, change of state and/or result of an action which could be carried out, for example, by different manners of acting, by using the whole body or different parts of the body, and by using various instruments. Verbs were also produced in a script-like manner in which the temporal-causal order of the actions was brought to the fore. The clusters formed by the miAD group and the moAD group tended to lack some parts of the scripts that were covered by the NC group. The moAD group also lacked specific clusters of instrument verbs denoting specific tools or instruments with which the action can be carried out. The subject groups were able to produce verbs with an equal degree of prototypicality and frequency. 5. The nature of the noun and verb categories, probably owing to their different semantic feature constellations, tended to have an effect on the semantic fluency performance of the AD groups. Some categories were more prone than others to eliciting fewer responses and more errors (intrusions and perseverations) in the AD groups than in the NC group. Therefore, conclusions about the deteriorated semantic fluency performance in AD should not be based on a single semantic category only. 6. Perseverations are likely to have affected the ability of the AD patients to perform the semantic fluency task. The tendency to get stuck on the previous responses may have prevented the AD patients from activating new responses for the tasks. Perseverations may also have inflated factors with which efficiency and semantic coherence of word production were measured (i.e., the cluster size and the proportion of clustered words). Therefore, conclusions about the effectiveness and coherence of the clustering performance of the AD groups should be made with caution until the location of perseverations (i.e., between or in the clusters) is checked and the size of the intact clusters (i.e., clusters without perseverations) is compared between the NC group and the AD groups. 7. Characteristic of the performance of the two AD groups on both noun and verb fluency tasks was the reduction in the number of responses in general and the reduction of very specific responses. The tendency towards using more general responses was notable in the moAD group. This finding may imply deteriorated 186 Conclusions or damaged feature integration at the very specific level of semantic features of nouns and verbs. 8. Overall, the performance of the AD patients on the semantic fluency task reflected a semantically rather coherent but less specific, effective, and flexible functioning of the semantic memory or the semantic layer of the mental lexicon. However, the emergence of intrusions and perseverations in some of the categories may not only be interpreted as an impaired spread of activation and feature integration at the semantic level, but also as an impaired functioning of other levels of the mental lexicon (e.g., lemma level) participating in the process of word production. 9. Based on the significantly worse performance on the semantic fluency tasks and on most of the control tasks used for examining semantic processing, it can be concluded that semantic processing of both nouns and verbs was impaired in each AD group, more severely in the moAD group than in the miAD group. However, it should be borne in mind that the impaired working memory and executive function common to AD patients may also have played a crucial role during the semantic fluency performance, as well as while carrying out other tasks requiring semantic processing. References Abeysinghe, S. C., Bayles, K. A., & Trosset, M. W. (1990). Semantic memory deterioration in Alzheimer’s subjects: Evidence from word association, definition, and associate ranking tasks. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 574582. Abrahams, S., Leigh, P. N., Harvey, A., Vythelingum, G. N. Grisé, D., & Goldstein, L. H. (2000). Verbal fluency and executive dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Neuropsychologia, 38, 734-747. Aitchison, J. (1994). Words in the mind. An introduction to the mental lexicon (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. Alafuzoff, I. (2001). Neuropatologinen tutkimus [Neuropathological examination]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen, & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 424-433). Helsinki: Duodecim. Albert, M., & Milberg, W. (1989). Semantic processing in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 37, 163-171. Albert, M. L., & Sandson, J. (1986). Perseveration in aphasia. Cortex, 22, 103115. Alhainen, K., & Rosenvall, A. (2001). Toimintakyvyn ja vaikeusasteen arvioiminen [Evaluation of functional ability and degree of severity]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen, & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 378-384). Helsinki: Duodecim. Allen, H. A., Liddle, P. F., & Frith, C. D. (1993). Negative features, retrieval processes and verbal fluency in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 769-775. Allport, D. A. (1985). Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia. In S. Newman, & R. Epstein (Eds.), Current Perspectives in Dysphasia (pp. 32-60). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. 188 References Almkvist, O. (1996). Neuropsychological features of early Alzheimer’s disease: Preclinical and clinical stages. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 165, 63-71. Almkvist, O., & Bäckman, L. (1993). Detection and staging of early clinical dementia. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 88, 10-15. Alzheimer, A. (1907). Über eine eigenartige Erkrankung der Hirnrinde [On a peculiar disease of the cerebral cortex]. Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtliche Medizin, 64, 146-148. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Appell, J., Kertesz, A., & Fisman, M. (1982). A study of language functioning in Alzheimer patients. Brain and Language, 17, 73-91. Astell, A. J., & Harley, T. A. (1996). Tip-of-the-tongue states and lexical access in dementia. Brain and Language 54, 196-215. Astell, A. J., & Harley, T. A. (1998). Naming problems in dementia: Semantic or lexical? Aphasiology, 12, 357-374. Astell, A. J., & Harley, T. A. (2002). Accessing semantic knowledge in dementia: evidence from a word definition task. Brain and Language 82, 312-326. Auriacombe, S., Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., & Hurtig, H. I. (1993). Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 7, 182-192. Azuma, T., Bayles, K. A., Cruz, R. F., Tomoeda, C. K., Wood, J. A., McGeagh, A., & Montgomery, E. B., Jr. (1997). Comparing the difficulty of letter, semantic, and name fluency tasks for normal elderly and patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 488-497. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 281-288. Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 114, 2521-2542. Baddeley, A. D., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1986). Dementia and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 603-540. Balota, D. A., Dolan, P. O., & Duchek, J. M. (2000). Memory changes in healthy adults. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory (pp. 395-409). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Barsalou, L. W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. Memory and Cognition, 10, 82-93. References 189 Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition, 11, 211-227. Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101-140). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Barsalou, L. W. (1992). Frames, concepts, and conceptual fields. In A. Lehrer & E.F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts: New essays in semantic and lexical organization (pp. 21-74). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Barsalou, L. W., & Sewell, D. R. (1985). Contrasting the representation of scripts and categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 646-665. Bastiaanse, R. (1991). Retrieval of instrumental verbs in aphasia: An explorative study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 5, 355-368. Bastiaanse, R., & Jonkers, R. (1998). Verb retrieval in action naming and spontaneous speech in agrammatic and anomic aphasia. Aphasiology, 12, 951969. Bauer, J. (1994). Die Alzheimer-Krankheit. Neurobiologie, Psychosomatik, Diagnostik und Therapie [Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology, psychosomatics, diagnostics, and therapy]. Stuttgart: Schattauer. Baumann, M., Tienari, P., & Haltia, M. (1999). Käsitykset Alzheimerin taudin patogeneesista muuttumassa [Conceptions of the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease under change]. Duodecim, 115, 767-776. Bayles, K. A. (1982). Language function in senile dementia. Brain and Language, 16, 265-280. Bayles, K. A. (1991). Age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Relation to language dysfunction. Archives of Neurology, 48, 155-159. Bayles, K. A. (2003). Effects of working memory deficits on the communicative functioning of Alzheimer’s dementia patients. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 209-219. Bayles, K. A., Boone, D. R., Tomoeda, C. K., Slauson, T. J., & Kaszniak, A. W. (1989). Differentiating Alzheimer’s patients from the normal elderly and stroke patients with aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 74-87. Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1983). Confrontation naming impairment in dementia. Brain and Language, 19, 98-114. Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1991). Caregiver report of prevalence and appearance order of linguistic symptoms in Alzheimer’s patients. The Gerontologist, 31, 210-216. 190 References Bayles, K. A., Tomoeda, C. K., Kaszniak, A. W., Stern, L. Z., & Eagans, K. K. (1985). Verbal perseveration of dementia patients. Brain and Language, 25, 102116. Bayles, K. A., Tomoeda, C. K., & Trosset, M. W. (1990). Naming and categorical knowledge in Alzheimer’s disease: The process of semantic memory deterioration. Brain and Language, 39, 498-510. Bayles, K. A., Trosset, M. W., Tomoeda, C. K., Montgomery, E. B. Jr., & Wilson, J. (1993). Generative naming in Parkinson disease patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 547-562. Beatty, W. W., Salmon, D. P., Testa, J. A., Hanisch, C., & Tröster, A. I. (2000). Monitoring the changing status of semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease: An evaluation of several process measures. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7, 94-111. Beatty, W. W., Testa, J. A., English, S., & Winn, P. (1997) Influences of clustering and switching on the verbal fluency performance of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4, 273-279. Behrend, D. A. (1990). The development of verb concepts: Children’s use of verbs to label familiar and novel events. Child Development, 61, 681-696. Bell, E. E., Chenery, H. J., & Ingram, J. C. L. (2000). Strategy-based semantic priming in Alzheimer’s dementia. Aphasiology, 14, 949-965. Bell, E. E., Chenery, H. J., & Ingram, J. C. L. (2001). Semantic priming in Alzheimer’s dementia: Evidence for dissociation of automatic and attentional processes. Brain and Language, 76, 130-144. Berndt, R. S., Haendiges, A. N., Mitchum, C. C., & Sandson, J. (1997). Verb retrieval in aphasia. 2. Relationship to sentence processing. Brain and Language, 56, 107-137. Bhatnagar, S. C., Mandybur, G. T., Buckingham, H. W., & Andy, O. J. (2000). Language representation in the human brain: Evidence from cortical mapping. Brain and Language, 74, 238-259. Binetti, G., Bianchetti, A., Padovani, A., Lenzi, G., De Leo, D., & Trabucchi, M. (1993). Delusions in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 88, 5-9. Binetti, G., Magni, E., Cappa, S. F., Padovani, A., Bianchetti, A., & Trabucchi, M. (1995). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis of category fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 82-89. Binetti G., Magni, E., Padovani, A., Cappa, S. F., Bianchetti, A., & Trabucchi, M. (1996). Executive dysfunction in early Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 60, 91-93. References 191 Bird, H., Howard, D., & Franklin, S. (2000). Why is a verb like an inanimate object? Grammatical category and semantic category deficits. Brain and Language, 72, 246-309. Bird, H., Howard, D., & Franklin, S. (2001). Noun-verb differences? A question of semantics: A response to Shapiro and Caramazza. Brain and Language, 76, 213-222. Bird, H., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). The rise and fall of frequency and imageability: Noun and verb production in semantic dementia. Brain and Language, 73, 17-49. Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., & Turner, T. J. (1979). Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 177-220. Bowles, N. L., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Naming errors in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Cortex, 23, 519-524. Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239-259. Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1996). Evolution of the neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, Suppl. 165, 3-12. Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Schwartz, M. F. (1998). Semantic factors in verb retrieval: An effect of complexity. Brain and Language, 63, 1-31. Buckingham, H. W., Jr., Whitaker, H., & Whitaker, H. A. (1979). On linguistic perseveration. In H. Whitaker & H. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 4, pp. 329-352). New York: Academic Press. Bucks, R. S., Singh, S., Cuerden, J. M., & Wilcock, G. K. (2000). Analysis of spontaneous, conversational speech in dementia of Alzheimer type: Evaluation of an objective technique for analysing lexical performance. Aphasiology, 14, 71-91. Buckwalter, G. J., Rizzo, A. A., McCleary, R., Shankle, R., Dick, M., & Henderson, V. W. (1996). Gender comparisons of cognitive performances among vascular dementia, Alzheimer disease, and older adults without dementia. Archives of Neurology, 53, 436-439. Burns, A., Jacoby, R., & Levy, R. (1990a). Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. I: Disorders of thought content. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 72-76. Burns, A., Jacoby, R., & Levy, R. (1990b). Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. II: Disorders of perception. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 76-81. Burns, A., Jacoby, R., & Levy, R. (1990c). Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. III: Disorders of mood. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 81-86. 192 References Burns, A., Jacoby, R., & Levy, R. (1990d). Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. IV: Disorders of behaviour. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 86-94. Bushell, C. M., & Martin, A. (1997). Automatic semantic priming of nouns and verbs in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1059-1067. Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D. P., Grant, I., & Wolfe, J. (1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 479-497. Cacciari, C., & Levorato, M. C. (2000). The semantic structure of vision verbs: A psycholinguistic investigation of Italian. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 87-106. Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., & Barbarotto, R. (1999). Gender affects word retrieval of certain categories in semantic fluency tasks. Cortex, 35, 273-278. Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., & Basso, A. (1998). Phonetically cued word-fluency, gender differences and aging: A reappraisal. Cortex, 34, 779-783. Cappa, S. F., Binetti, G., Pezzini, A., Padovani, A., Rozzini, L., & Trabucchi, M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 50, 351-355. Caramazza, A. (2000). The organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1037-1046). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Caramazza, A., Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B. C., & Romani, C. (1990). The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161189. Caramazza, A., & Shelton, J. R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate - inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 1-34. Carew, T. G., Lamar, M., Cloud, B. S., Grossman, M., & Libon, D. J. (1997). Impairment in category fluency in ischenic vascular dementia. Neuropsychology, 11, 400-412. Carlomagno, S., Ivanore, A., Nolfe, G., Bourene, G., Martin, C., & Deloche, G. (1999). Dyscalculia in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 99, 166-174. Chan, A. S., Butters, N., Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D. P., Swenson, M. R., & Maloney, L. T. (1993). An assessment of the semantic network in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 254-261. Chang, T. M. (1986). Semantic memory: Facts and models. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 199-220. References 193 Chertkow, H., & Bub, D. (1990). Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. What do various measures measure? Brain, 113, 397-417. Chertkow, H., Bub, D., & Caplan, D. (1992). Constraining theories of semantic memory processing: Evidence from dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 327-365. Chertkow, H., Bub, D., & Seidenberg, M. (1989). Priming and semantic memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 36, 420-446. Cohen, M., & Stanczak, D. E. (2000). On the reliability, validity, and cognitive structure of the Thurstone word fluency test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 267-279. Cohen, M. J., Morgan, A. M., Vaughn, M., Riccio, C. A., & Hall, J. (1999). Verbal fluency in children: Developmental issues and differential validity in distinguishing children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and two subtypes of dyslexia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 433-443. Coleman, L., & Kay, P. (1981). Prototype semantics: The English word lie. Language, 57, 26-44. Collina, S., Marangolo, P., & Tabossi, P. (2001). The role of argument structure in the production of nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1125-1137. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428. Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-247. Coltheart, M. (2001). Assumptions and methods in cognitive neuropsychology. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. What Deficits Reveal about the Human Mind (pp. 3-21). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Age-related changes in human memory. In D.C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 75-92). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Cronin-Golomb, A., Keane, M. M., Kokodis, A., Corkin, S., & Growdon, J. H. (1992). Category knowledge in Alzheimer’s disease: Normal organization and a general retrieval deficit. Psychology and Aging, 7, 359-366. Croot, K., Hodges, J. R., Xuereb, J., & Patterson, K. (2000). Phonological and articulatory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: A case series. Brain and Language, 75, 277-309. 194 References Crossley, M., D’Arcy, C., & Rawson, N. S. B. (1997). Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 52-62. Crowe, S. F. (1996). The performance of schizophrenic and depressed subjects on tests of fluency: Support for a compromise in dorsolateral prefrontal functioning. Australian Psychologist, 31, 204-209. Crowe, S. F. (1998). Decrease in performance on the verbal fluency test as a function of time: Evaluation in a young healthy sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 391-401. Cruse, A. D. (2000). Meaning in language. An introduction to semantics and pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crystal, D. (1987). The Cambridge encyclopedia of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cummings, J. L., Benson, F., Hill, M. A., & Read, S. (1985). Aphasia in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology, 35, 394-397. Cummings, J. L., Houlihan, J. P., & Hill, M. A. (1986). The pattern of reading deterioration in dementia of the Alzheimer type: Observations and implications. Brain and Language, 29, 315-323. Cummings, J. L., Vinters, H. V., Cole, G. M., & Khachaturian, Z. S. (1998). Alzheimer’s disease. Etiologies, pathophysiology, cognitive reserve, and treatment opportunities. Neurology, 51 (Suppl. 1), S2-S17. Daigneault, S., Braun, C. M. J., & Whitaker, H.A. (1992). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99-114. Damasio, A. R., & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 90, 4957-4960. Davidoff, J., & Masterson, J. (1996). The development of picture naming: Differences between verbs and nouns. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 69-83. Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283-321. Dell, G. S., Burger, L. K., & Svec, W. R. (1997). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 104, 123-147. Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghda, P. G. (1991). Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: A comment on Levelt et al. (1991). Psychological Review, 98, 604-614. References 195 Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghda, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition, 42, 287-314. Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic subjects. Psychological Review, 104, 801-838. Della Sala, S., Lorenzi, L., Spinner, H., & Zuffi, M. (1993). Components in the breakdown of verbal communication in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology, 7, 285300. De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). The Token-test. Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token-test. Cortex, 14, 14-19. De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L.A. (1962). The Token-test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85, 665-667. Devlin, J. T., Gonnerman, L. M., Andersen, E. S., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Category-specific semantic deficits in focal and widespread brain damage: A computational account. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 77-94. Devlin, J. T., Russell, R. P., Davis, M. H., Price, C. J., Moss, H. E., Fadili, M. J., & Tyler, L. K. (2002). Is there an anatomical basis for category-specificity? Semantic memory studies in PET and fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 40, 54-75. Diesfeldt, H. F. A. (1985). Verbal fluency in senile dementia: an analysis of search and knowledge. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 4, 231 – 239. Diesfeldt, H. F. A. (1989). Semantic impairment in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Aphasiology, 3, 41-54. Dixon, R. M. W. (1991). A new approach to English grammar, on semantic principles. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Ellis, A. W., & Young, A. W. (1988). Human cognitive neuropsychology. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Emery, O. B. (1988): Language and memory processing in senile dementia Alzheimer’s type. In L.L. Light & D.M. Burke (Eds.), Language, Memory, and Aging (pp. 221-243). New York: Cambridge University Press. Engelkamp, J. (1975). The interaction of semantic features of transitive verbs and their objects. Psychological Research, 37, 299-308. Engelkamp, J. (1986). Nouns and verbs in paired-associate learning: Instructional effects. Psychological Research, 48, 153-159. Engelkamp, J., Zimmer, H. D., & Denis, M. (1989). Paired associate learning of action verbs with visual- or motor-imaginal encoding instructions. Psychological Research, 50, 257-263. 196 References Erkinjuntti, T. (1988). Dementia. Clinical diagnosis and differential diagnosis with special reference to multi-infarct dementia. Doctoral dissertation. Helsinki: Department of Neurology, University of Helsinki. Erkinjuntti, T. (2001). Muistihäiriöiden ja dementioiden mekanismit ja syyt [Mechanisms and causes of memory disorders and dementia]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 73-76). Helsinki: Duodecim. Erkinjuntti, T., Rinne, J., Alhainen K., & Soininen, H. (2001). Muistihäiriöt ja dementia [Memory disorders and dementia]. Helsinki: Duodecim. Erkinjuntti, T., Rinne, J., & Soininen, H. (2001). Muistihäiriöt ja dementia [Memory disorders and dementia]. In S. Soinila, M. Kaste, J. Launes & H. Somer (Eds.), Neurologia (pp. 322-341). Helsinki: Duodecim. Faber-Langendoen, K., Morris, J. C., Knesevich, J. W., LaBarge, E., Miller, J. P., & Berg, L. (1988). Aphasia in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Annals of Neurology, 23, 365-370. Farah, M. J., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality specificity and emergent category specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 339-357. Fellbaum, C. (1998a). A semantic network of English verbs. In C. Fellbaum (Ed.), Wordnet. An electronic lexical database (pp. 69-104). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fellbaum, C. (1998b). Wordnet. An electronic lexical database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fischer, P., Gatterer, G., Marterer, A., & Danielczyk, W. (1988). Nonspecificity of semantic impairment in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Archives of Neurology, 45, 1341-1343. Fivush, R. (1987). Scripts and categories: interrelationships in development. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 234-253). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fivush, R., & Slackman, E. (1986). The acquisition and development of scripts. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 7196). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Flicker, C., Ferris, S. H., Crook, T., & Bartus, R.T. (1987). Implications of memory and language dysfunction in the naming deficit of senile dementia. Brain and Language, 31, 187-200. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. References 197 Foygel, D., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of impaired lexical access in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 182-216. Fratiglioni, L., Launer, L. J., Andersen, K., Breteler, M. M., Copeland, J. R., Dartigues, J. F., Lobo, A., Martinez-Lage, J., Soininen, H., & Hofman, A. (2000). Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology, 54 (Suppl. 5), S10-S15. Frawley, W. (1992). Linguistic semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Fromm, D., & Holland, A. L. (1989). Functional communication in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 535-540. Fung, T. D., Chertkow, H., Murtha, S., Whatmough, C., Péloquin, L., Whitehead, V., & Templeman, F.D. (2001). The spectrum of category effects in object and action knowledge in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neuropsychology, 15, 371379. Funnell, E. (1995). Objects and properties: A study of the breakdown of semantic memory. Memory, 3, 497-518. Funnell, E. (2001). Evidence for scripts in semantic dementia: Implications for theories of semantic memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 323-341. Gainotti, G., Di Betta, A. M., & Silveri, M. C. (1996). The production of specific and generic associates of living and nonliving, high- and low-familiarity stimuli in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language 54, 262-274. Galambos, J. A. (1983). Normative studies of six characteristics of our knowledge of common activities. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 15, 327340. Garnham, A., Shillcock, R. C., Brown, G. D. A., Mill. A. I. D., & Cutler, A. (1981). Slips of the tongue in the London - Lund corpus of spontaneous conversation. Linguistics, 19, 805-817. Garrard, P., & Hodges, J. R. (1999). Semantic dementia: implications for the neural basis and meaning. Aphasiology, 13, 609-623. Garrard, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, A. (2001). Prototypicality, distinctiveness, and intercorrelations: Analysis of the semantic attributes of living and nonliving concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 125174. Garrett, M. F. (1992). Lexical retrieval processes: Semantic field effects. In A. Lehrer & E.F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts. New essays in semantic and lexical organization (pp. 377-395). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 198 References Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263-297. Gewirth, L. R., Shindler, A. G., & Hier, D. B. (1984). Altered patterns of word associations in dementia and aphasia. Brain and Language 21, 307-317. Goldstein, F. C., Green, J., Presley, R., & Green, R. C. (1992). Dysnomia in Alzheimer’s disease: An evaluation of neurobehavioral subtypes. Brain and Language, 43, 308-322. Goldstein, F. C., Levin, H. S., Roberts, V. J., Goldman, W. P., Kalechstein, A. S., Winslow, M., & Goldstein, S. J. (1996). Neuropsychological effects of closed head injury in older adults: A comparison with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 147-154. Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (1998). Reuniting perception and conception. Cognition 65, 231-262. Gonnerman, L. M., Andersen, E. S., Devlin, J. T., Kempler, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). Double dissociation of semantic categories in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 57, 254-279. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. Graesser, A. C. (1978). How to catch a fish: The memory and representation of common procedures. Discourse Processes, 1, 72-89. Grafman, J., Thompson, K., Weingartner, H., Martinez, R., Lawlor, B. A., & Sunderland, T. (1991). Script generation as an indicator of knowledge representation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 40, 344358. Graham, K. S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Determining the impact of autobiographical experience on “meaning”: New insights from investigating sports-related vocabulary and knowledge in two cases with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 801-837. Grober, E., Buschke, H., Kawas, C., & Fuld, P. (1985). Impaired ranking of semantic attributes in dementia. Brain and Language, 26, 276-286. Grossman, M., D’Esposito, M., Hughes, E., Onishi, K., Biassou, N., WhiteDevine, T., & Robinson, K. M. (1996). Language comprehension profiles in Alzheimer’s disease, multi-infarct dementia, and frontotemporal degeneration. Neurology, 47, 183-189. References 199 Grossman, M., Mickanin, J., Onishi, K., & Hughes, E. (1996). Verb comprehension deficits in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 53, 369-389. Gruenewald, P. J., & Lockhead, G. R. (1980). The free recall of category examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 225-240. Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language acquisition. The growth of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hagoort, P. (1998). The shadows of lexical meaning in patients with semantic impairments. In B. Stemmer & H.A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 235-248). San Diego: Academic Press. Hahn, U., & Chater, N. (1997). Concepts and similarity. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 43-92). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Hahn, U., & Chater, N. (1998). Similarity and rules: Distinct? Exhaustive? Empirically distinguishable? Cognition, 65, 197-230. Hakulinen, L. (1979). Suomen kielen rakenne ja kehitys [The structure and development of Finnish]. Helsinki: Otava. Hampton, J. A. (1998). Similarity-based categorization and fuzziness of natural categories. Cognition, 65, 137-165. Harley, T. A. (1998). The semantic deficit in dementia: Connectionist approaches to what goes wrong in picture naming. Aphasiology, 12, 299-318. Harley, T. A. (2001). The psychology of language. From data to theory (2nd ed.). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Hart, S., Smith, C. M., & Swash, M. (1988). Word fluency in patients with early dementia of Alzheimer type. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 115-124. Hebert, L. E., Wilson, R. S., Gilley, D. W., Beckett, L. A., Scherr, P. A., Bennett, D. A., & Evans, D. A. (2000). Decline of language among women and men with Alzheimer’s disease. Journals of Gerontology: Series B - Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 55B, P354-360. Heit, E. (1997). Knowledge and concept learning. In K. Lamberts & E. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 7-41). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Helmick, J. W., & Berg, C. B. (1976). Perseveration in brain-injured adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 9, 143-156. 200 References Helstrup, T. (1989). Memory for performed and imaged noun pairs and verb pairs. Psychological Research, 50, 237-240. Henderson, V. W., & Buckwalter, G. J. (1994). Cognitive deficits of men and women with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 44, 90-96. Herlitz, A., Hill, R. D., Fratiglioni, L., & Backman, L. (1995). Episodic memory and visuospatial ability in detecting and staging dementia in a community-based sample of very old adults. Journals of Gerontology: Series A – Biological Sciences & Medical Sciences, 50, M107-113. Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, K., & Shindler, A. G. (1985). Language disintegration in dementia: Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language, 25, 117-133. Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In D.E. Rumelhart, J.L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations (pp. 77-109). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hinton, G. E., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1986). Learning and relearning in Bolzmann machines. In D.E. Rumelhart, J.L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations (pp. 282-317). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hodges, J. R. (2000). Memory in the dementias. In E. Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 441-459). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic memory consistently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia, 33, 441-459. Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Graham, N., & Dawson, K. (1996). Naming and knowing in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Brain and Language 54, 302-325. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1990). Differential impairment of semantic and episodic memory in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease: a controlled prospective study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 53, 1089 – 1095. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1991). The nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Brain, 14, 1547-1558. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1992). Semantic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 30, 301-314. Holappa, J., & Nauha, R. (2000). Aivoverenkiertohäiriöpotilaiden sanasujuvuus [Word fluency in patient with ischaemic attack]. Unpublished Master’s thesis, References 201 University of Oulu, Department of Finnish, Saami, and Logopedics, Oulu, Finland. Horner, J., Heyman, A., Dawson, D., & Rogers, H. (1988). The relationship of agraphia to the severity of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 45, 760-763. Hotz, G., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1995a). Perseveration. Part I: A review. Brain Injury, 9, 151-159. Hotz, G., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1995b). Perseveration. Part II: A study of perseveration in closed-head injury. Brain Injury, 9, 161-172. Howell, D. C. (1997). Statistical methods for psychology (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press. Hudson, A. J. (1968). Perseveration. Brain, 91, 571-582. Hudson, A. J. (1986). Memories are made of this: General event knowledge and the development of autobiographic memory. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 97-118). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Huff, F. J. (1988). The disorder of naming in Alzheimer’s disease. In L. L. Light & D. M. Burke (Eds.), Language, memory, and aging (pp. 209-220). New York: Cambridge University Press. Huff, F. J., Becker, J. T., Belle, S. H., Nebes, R. D., Holland, A. L., & Boller, F. (1987). Cognitive deficits and clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 37, 1119-1124. Huff, F. J., Corkin, S., & Growdon, J. H. (1986). Semantic impairment and anomia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 28, 235 – 249. Huff, F. J., Mack, L., Mahlmann, J., & Greenberg, S. (1988). A comparison of lexical-semantic impairments in left hemisphere stroke and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 34, 262-278. Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566-572. Huttenlocher, J., & Lui, F. (1979). The semantic organization of some simple nouns and verbs. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 141-162. Huttenlocher, J., Smiley, P., & Charney, R. (1983). Emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meaning. Psychological Review, 90, 72-93. 202 References Hänninen, T., & Pulliainen, V. (2001). Neuropsykologinen seulonta [Neuropsychological screening]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.) Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 348-347). Helsinki: Duodecim. Hänninen, T., Pulliainen, V., Salo, J., Hokkanen, L., Erkinjuntti, T., Koivisto, K., Viramo, V., Soininen, H., & Suomen muistitutkimusyksiköiden asiantuntijaryhmä (1999). Kognitiiviset testit muistihäiriöiden ja alkavan dementian varhais– diagnostiikassa: CERAD-tehtäväsarja [Cognitive tests in diagnosing memory disorders and early dementia: CERAD measures]. Suomen Lääkärilehti, 54, 19671975. Joanette, Y., & Goulét, P. (1986). Criterion-specific reduction of verbal fluency in right brain-damaged right-handers. Neuropsychologia, 24, 875-879. Joanette, Y., Goulét, P., & Le Dorze, G. (1988). Impaired word naming in rightbrain-damaged right-handers: Error types and time-course analyses. Brain and Language, 34, 56-64. Johnson, M. K., Hermann, A. M., & Bonilla, J. L. (1995). Semantic relations and Alzheimer’s disease: Typicality and direction of testing. Neuropsychology, 9, 529536. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). The mental representation of the naming of words. Cognition, 25, 189-211. Jonkers, R., & Bastiaanse, R. (1996). The influence of instrumentality and transitivity on action naming in Broca’s and anomic aphasia. Brain and Language, 55, 37-39. Jonkers, R., & Bastiaanse, R. (1998). How selective are selective word class deficits? Two case studies of action and object naming. Aphasiology, 12, 245-256. Juva, K., Valvanne, J., & Voutilainen, P. (2001). Dementiapotilaan laitoshoito [Institutional Care of Patients with Dementia]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäitiöt ja dementia (pp. 500-515). Helsinki: Duodecim. Kaleva, O., & Vanhala, M. (2001). Sanasujuvuus kuusi-, kahdeksan- ja kymmenvuotiailla lapsilla [Word Fluency in 6-, 8-, and 10-year-old Children]. Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Oulu, Department of Finnish, Saami, and Logopedics, Oulu, Finland. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. Karlsson, F. (1998). Yleinen kielitiede [General linguistics] (Rev. ed.). Helsinki: Helsinki University Press. References 203 Karlsson, F., & Koskenniemi, K. (1985). A process model of morphology and lexicon. Folia Linguistica, 19, 207-231. Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. Methods for clinical and applied settings. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Keil, F. C., Smith, C. W., Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition, 65, 103-135. Kemmerer, D., & Tranel, D. (2000). Verb retrieval in brain-damaged subjects: 1. Analysis of stimulus, lexical and conceptual factors. Brain and Language, 73, 347-392. Kempler, D. (1995). Language changes in dementia of the Alzheimer type. In R. Lubinski (Ed.), Dementia and Communication (pp. 98-114). San Diego: Singular Publishing Group. Kempler, D., Curtiss, S., & Jackson, C. (1987). Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 343-350. Kersten, A. W., & Billman, D. (1997). Event category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 638-658. Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. London, Ont.: University of Western Ontario. Kertesz, A., Appell, J., & Fisman, M. (1986). The dissolution of language in Alzheimer’s disease. Le Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques, 13, 415418. Kim, M., & Thompson, C. K. (2000). Patterns of comprehension and production of nouns and verbs in agrammatism: Implications for lexical organization. Brain and Language, 74, 1-25. Kim, M., & Thompson, C. K. (2001). Verb deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and agrammatism: Semantic versus syntactic impairments. Brain and Language, 79, 125-127. Kirshner, H. S., Webb, W. G., & Kelly, M. P. (1984). The naming disorder of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 22, 23-30. Klimesch, W. (1994). The structure of long-term memory. A connectivity model of semantic processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 204 References Knight, R. G. (1996). Facilitation by semantic priming in Alzheimer’s disease and control groups matched for accuracy of word recognition. Neuropsychology, 10, 96-100. Knowlton, B. (1997). Declarative and nondeclarative knowledge: Insights from cognitive neuroscience. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, Concepts, and Categories (pp. 215-246). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Koenig, T., & Lehmann, D. (1996). Microstates in language-related brain potential maps show noun-verb differences. Brain and Language, 53, 169-182. Koivisto, M., & Laine, M. (1999). Strategies of semantic categorization in the cerebral hemispheres. Brain and Language, 66, 341-357. Komatsu, L. K. (1992). Recent views of conceptual structure. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 500-526. Kontiola, P., Laaksonen, R., Sulkava, R., & Erkinjuntti, T. (1990). Pattern of language impairment is different in Alzheimer’s disease and multi-infarct dementia. Brain and Language, 38, 364-383. Kopelman, M. D. (1994). Working memory in the amnesic syndrome and degenerative dementia. Neuropsychology, 8, 555-562. Laatu, S. (1999). Käsitteiden ymmärtämisen vaikeuksia Alzheimerin taudissa, Parkinsonin taudissa sekä MS-taudissa [Difficulties in understanding concepts in Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and Multiple Sclerosis]. Suomen logopedis-foniatrinen aikakauslehti, 19, 72-85. Laatu, S., Portin, R., Revonsuo, A., Tuisku, S., & Rinne, J. (1997). Knowledge of concept meanings in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 33, 27-45. Labov, W. (1973). The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C.-J.N. Bailey & R.W. Shuy (Eds.), New ways of analyzing variation in English (pp. 340-373). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Lafosse, J. M., Reed, B. R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S. B., Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W. J., (1997). Fluency and memory differences between ischemic vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 514-522. Laine, M. (1989). On the mechanisms of verbal adynamia. A neuropsychological study. Doctoral dissertation. Turun yliopiston julkaisuja. Annales Universitatis Turkuensis. Series B, Pt. 185. Turku: Turun yliopisto. Laine, M., Koivuselkä-Sallinen, P., Hänninen, R., & Niemi, J. (1997). Bostonin nimentätesti [The Finnish version of the Boston Naming Test]. Helsinki: Psykologien Kustannus. Laine, M., & Martin, N. (1996). Lexical retrieval deficit in picture naming: Implications for word production models. Brain and Language, 53, 283-314. References 205 Laine, M., & Virtanen, P. (1999). WordMill lexical search program. Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Turku, Finland. Laine, M., Vuorinen, E., & Rinne, J. O. (1997). Picture naming deficits in vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 126-140. Lakoff, G. (1987a). Cognitive models and prototype theory. In U, Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 63-100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lakoff, G. (1987b). Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Lamar, M., Podell, K., Carew, T. G., Cloud, B. S., Resh, R., Kennedy, C., Goldberg, E., Kaplan, E., & Libon, D. J. (1997). Perseverative behavior in Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Neuropsychology, 11, 523-534. Lamberts, K. (1997). Process models of categorization. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 371-403). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. 2. Descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Lees, A. J., & Smith, E. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 106, 257-270. Lehtovirta, M. (2001). Perinnöllisyys ja geenitestit [Heredity and gene tests]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 418-423). Helsinki: Duodecim. Leino, P. (1999). Polysemia - Kielen Moniselitteisyys. Suomen kielen kognitiivista kielioppia 1. Kieli 7 [Polysemy – ambiguity of the language. Cognitive grammar of the Finnish language 1. Language 7] (2nd ed.). University of Helsinki: Department of Finnish. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Levelt, W. J. M. (1999a). Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 223-232. Levelt, W. J. M. (1999b). Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In C.M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 83122). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Spoken word production: A theory of lexical access. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 13464-13471. 206 References Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-75. Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J. (1991a). Normal and deviant lexical processing: Reply to Dell and O’Seaghdha (1991). Psychological Review, 98, 615-618. Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J. (1991b). The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review, 98, 122-142. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Liepmann, H. (1905). Über Störungen des Handelns bei Gehirnkranken [On the functional disorders of the brain damaged]. Berlin: Verlag von S. Karger. Lobo, A., Launer, L. J., Fratiglioni, L., Andersen, K., Di Carlo, A., Breteler, M. M., Copeland, J. R., Dartigues, J. F., Jagger, C., Martinez-Lage, J., Soininen, H., & Hofman, A. (2000). Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology, 54 (Suppl. 5), S4-S9. Locascio, J. J., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S. (1995). Cognitive test performance in detecting, staging and tracking Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 52, 1087-1099. Lucariello, J., Kyratzis, A., & Nelson, K. (1992). Taxonomic knowledge: What kind and when. Child Development, 63, 978-998. Lucariello, J., & Rifkin, A. (1986). Event representations as the basis for categorical knowledge. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 189-203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Luria, A. R. (1965). Two kinds of motor perseveration in massive injury of the frontal lobes. Brain, 88, 1-12. Luzzatti, C., Laiacona, M., & Agazzi, D. (2003). Multiple patterns of writing disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type and their evolution. Neuropsychologia, 41, 759-772. Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Markman, E. M. (1987). How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 255-287). New York: Cambridge University Press. Marshall, J., Chiat, S., Robson, J., & Pring, T. (1996). Calling a salad a federation: An investigation of semantic jargon. Part 2 - verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 251-260. References 207 Marshall, J., Pring, T., Chiat, S., & Robson, J. (1996). Calling a salad a federation: An investigation of semantic jargon. Part 1 - nouns. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 237-250. Martin, A., Browers, P., Cox, C., & Fedio, P. (1985). On the nature of the verbal memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 25, 323-341. Martin, A. & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease: The breakdown of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19, 124-141. Martin, N., Dell, G. S., Saffran, E. M., & Schwartz, M. F. (1994). Origins of paraphasias in deep dysphasia: Testing the consequences of a decay impairment to an interactive spreading activation model of lexical retrieval. Brain and Language 47, 609-660. Martin, N., Weisberg, R. W., & Saffran, E. M. (1989). Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language 28, 462-485. Masur, D. M., Sliwinski, M., Lipton, R. B., Blau, A. D., & Crystal, H. A. (1994). Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons. Neurology, 44, 1427-1432. Mattis, S. (1976). Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly patient. In L. Bellack & T. Katasu (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry: A handbook of psychiatrists and primary care physicians (pp. 77-121). New York: Grune and Straton. Maurer, K., Ihl, R., & Frölich, L. (1993). Alzheimer. Grundlagen, Diagnostik, Therapie [Alzheimer. Basics, diagnostics, therapy]. Berlin: Springer. Maurer, K., Volk, S., & Gerbaldo, H. (1997). Auguste D and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet, 349, 1546-1549. Mayr, U. (2002). On the dissociation between clustering and switching in verbal fluency: Comment on Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander and Stuss. Neuropsychologia, 40, 562-566. Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Complex semantic processing in old age: Does it stay or does it go? Psychology and Aging, 15, 29-43. McClelland, J. L., & Kawamoto, A. H. (1986). Mechanisms of sentence processing: Assigning roles to constituents of sentences. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models (pp. 272-325). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 208 References McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 1: Foundations (pp. 3-44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. McKhann, G., Drachmann, P., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report on the NINCDSADRDA work group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services task force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939-944. McRae, K., & Cree G. S. (2002). Factors underlying category-specific semantic impairments, In E.M.E. Forde & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 211-249). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. McRae, K., de Sa, V. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 99-130. Medin, D. L., & Smith, E. E. (1984). Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 113-138. Mega, M. S., Cummings, J. L., Fiorello, T., & Gornbein, J. (1996). The spectrum of behavioral changes in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 46, 130-135. Mentis, M., Briggs-Whittaker, J., & Gramigna, G. D. (1995). Discourse topic management in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1054-1066. Mickanin, J., Grossman, M., Onishi, K., Auriacombe, S., & Clark, C. (1994). Verbal and nonverbal fluency in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 8, 385-394. Miller, G. A., & Fellbaum, C. (1991). Semantic networks of English. Cognition, 41, 197-229. Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976). Language and perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Miller Sommers, L., & Pierce, R. S. (1990). Naming and semantic judgements in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Aphasiology, 4, 573-586. Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D. P., Brugger, P., & Swenson, M. R. (1994). A comparison of category and letter fluency in Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 8, 25-30. Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L. J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 49, 1253-1258. References 209 Monsch, A. U., Seifritz, E., Taylor, K. I., Ermini- Funfschilling, D., Stähelin, H. B., & Spiegel, R. (1997). Category fluency is also predominantly affected in Swiss Alzheimer’s disease patients. Acta Neurologia Scandinavica, 95, 81-84. Morris, R. G. (1994). Working memory in Alzheimer-type dementia. Neuropsychology, 8, 544-554. Moss, H. E., Tyler, L. K., & Devlin, J. T. (2002). The emergence of categoryspecific deficits in a distributed semantic system. In E.M.F. Forde & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 115-147). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Murdoch, B. E., Chenery, H. J., Wilks, V., & Boyle, R. S. (1987). Language disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain and Language 31, 122-137. Murphy, G. L., & Lassaline, M. E. (1997). Hierarchical structure in concepts and the basic level of categorization. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 93-131). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Nakamura, G. V., Kleiber, B. A., & Kim, K. (1992). Categories, propositional representations, and schemas: Test of structural hypothesis. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 575-590. Nebes, R. D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377-394. Nebes, R. D. (1992). Semantic memory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease:Disruption of semantic knowledge or information-processing limitation? In L.R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (2nd ed., pp. 233-240). New York: The Guilford Press. Nebes, R. D., Brady, C. B., & Huff, F. J. (1989). Automatic and attentional mechanisms of semantic priming in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 219-230. Nebes, R. D., & Halligan, E. M. (1996). Sentence context influences the interpretation of word meaning by Alzheimer patients. Brain and Language, 54, 233-245. Nebes, R. D., Martin, D. C., & Horn, L. C. (1984). Sparing of semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 321-330. Neisser, U. (1987). From direct perception to conceptual structure. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 11-24). New York: Cambridge University Press. Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge and cognitive development. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 1-19). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 210 References Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1986). Children’s scripts. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 21-46). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Albert, M. L., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1985). Empty speech in Alzheimer’s disease and fluent aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 405-410. Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Au, R., & Albert, M. L. (1996). On the nature of naming errors in aging and dementia: A study of semantic relatedness. Brain and Language, 54, 184-195. Nickels, L. (2001). Spoken word production. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 291320). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Niemi, J., & Laine, M. (1994). Suomen kielen sanojen mentaalisen käsittelyn malli. [Mental processing of Finnish words]. Suomen logopedis-foniatrinen aikakauslehti, 14, 65-71. Ober, B. A., Dronkers, N. F., Koss, E., Delis, D. C., & Friedland, R. P. (1986). Retrieval from semantic memory in Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 75-92. Ober, B. A., & Shenaut, G. K. (1988). Lexical decision and priming in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 26, 273-286. Obler, L. K. (1983). Language and brain dysfunction in dementia. In S. J. Segalowitz (Ed.), Language functions and brain organization (pp. 267-282). New York: Academic Press. Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1981). Language in the elderly aphasic and in the demented patient. In M.T. Sarno (Ed.), Acquired aphasia (pp. 385-398). New York: Academic Press. Obler, L. K., & Gjerlow, K. (1999). Language and the brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Orange, J. B., Lubinski, R. B., & Higginbotham, D. J. (1996). Conversational repair by individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Speech and Hearing research, 39, 881-895. Orange, J. B., & Ryan, E. B. (2000). Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Implications for physician communication. In R.D. Adelman & M.G. Greene (Eds.), Clinics in geriatric medicine. Communication between older patients and their physicians (Vol. 16, pp. 153-167). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders Company. References 211 Pajunen, A. (1998). Partiaalis-refleksiiviset asennon muutosta ilmaisevat verbit suomen kielessä [Partial-reflexive verbs denoting a change of position in Finnish]. Virittäjä, 3, 330-360. Pajunen, A. (1999). Suomen verbirektiosta. Verbin argumenttirakenteen jäsenten valinnasta [On the government of the argument case by the verb in Finnish. On the selection of the members of the argument structure]. Yleisen kielitieteen julkaisuja 1. Turun yliopisto. Turku: Åbo Akademis tryckeri. Pajunen, A. (2001). Argumenttirakenne. Asiaintilojen luokitus ja verbien käyttäytyminen suomen kielessä [Argument structure. Classification of the state of affairs and behavior of verbs in Finnish]. Helsinki: Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura. Papagno, C. (2001). Comprehension of metaphors and idioms in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. A longitudinal study. Brain, 124, 1450-1460. Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In D.C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 3-21). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Partanen, K., Mäntylä, R., Erkinjuntti, T., & Rinne, J. (2001). Aivojen kuvantaminen [Brain imaging]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia, (pp. 385-402). Helsinki: Duodecim. Pasquier, F., Lebert, F., Grymonprez, L., & Petit, H. (1995). Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal lobe type and dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58, 81-84. Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina, S., Rosa, M. M., & Fazio, F. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing. A PET study. Brain, 122, 2337-2344. Persson, I-B. (1995). Connectionism, language production and adult aphasia. Elaboration of a connectionist framework for lexical processing and a hypothesis of agrammatic aphasia. Doctoral dissertation. Commentationes Humanarum Litterarum, 106. Helsinki: The Finnish Society of Sciences and Letters. Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Kokmen, E., & Tangalos, E. G. (1994). Memory function in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 44, 867-872. Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., Koller, W. C., & Tröster, A. I. (1999). Lexical semantic and action verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 435-443. Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., & Tröster, A. I. (1999). Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: Convergent and divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1499-1503. 212 References Pihlajamäki, M., Tanila, H., Hänninen, T., Könönen, M., Laakso, M., Partanen, K., Soininen, H., & Aronen, H. J. (2000). Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Annals of Neurology, 47, 470-476. Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition. The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pirttilä, T., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2001). Alzheimerin taudin kliininen kuva ja diagnoosi [Clinical picture and diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 122-141). Helsinki: Duodecim. Platel, H., Lambert, J., Eustache, F., Cadet, B., Dary, M., Viader, F., & Lechevalier, B. (1993). Characteristics and evolution of writing impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1147-1158. Powell, J., & Davidoff, J. (1995). Selective impairments of object knowledge in a case of acquired cortical blindness. Memory, 3, 435-461. Price, C., Indefrey, P., & Van Turennout, M. (1999). The neural architecture underlying the processing of written and spoken word forms. In C. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The Neurocognition of Language (pp. 211-240). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pulliainen, V., & Kuikka, P. (1998). Ennen dementiaa - varhaisen Alzheimerin taudin piirteitä [Before dementia – characteristics of early Alzheimer’s disease]. Duodecim, 114, 21-30. Pulman, S. G. (1983). Word meaning and belief. London: Croom Helm. Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253-336. Pulvermüller, F., Härle, M., & Hummel, F. (2001). Walking or talking?: Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing. Brain and Language, 78, 143-168. Pulvermüller, F., Mohr, B., & Schleichert, H. (1999). Semantic or lexico-syntactic factors: what determines word-class specific activity in the human brain? Neuroscience Letters, 275, 81-84. Ramage, A., Bayles, K., Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Cruz, R. (1999). Frequency of perseveration in normal subjects. Brain and Language, 66, 329-340. Randolph, C., Braun, A. R., Goldberg, T. E., & Chase, T. N. (1993). Semantic fluency in Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease: Dissociation of storage and retrieval failures. Neuropsychology, 7, 82-88. References 213 Ranta, E., Rita, H., & Kouki, J. (1991). Biometria. Tilastotiedettä ekologeille [Biometrics. Statistics for ecologists] (3rd rev. ed.). Helsinki: Helsinki University Press. Rantakrans, M. (1996). Mini-mental state examination ja sen laajennettu versio dementian seulontamenetelminä: Kriteerivaliditeetin vertailu [Mini-mental state examination and its extended version as a screening method of dementia: Comparison of the validity criteria]. Alzheimer-keskusliitto ry. Raportti 1/1996. Helsinki: Alzheimer-keskusliitto. Rapcsak, S. Z., Arthur, S. A., Bliklen, D. A., & Rubens, A. B. (1989). Lexical agraphia in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 46, 65-68. Rapp, B., & Caramazza, A. (1993). On the distinction between deficits of access and deficits of storage: A question of theory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 113141. Raskin, S. A., Sliwinski, M., & Borod, J. C. (1992). Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 30, 95-99. Ratcliff, G., Ganguli, M., Chandra, V., Sharma, S., Belle, S., Seaberg, E., & Pandav, R. (1998). Effects of literacy and education on measures of word fluency. Brain and Language, 61, 115-122. Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., De Leon, M. J., & Crook, T. (1982). The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1136-1139. Rende, B., Ramsberger, G., & Miyake, A. (2002). Commonalities and differences in the working memory components underlying letter and category fluency tasks: A dual-task investigation. Neuropsychology, 16, 309-321. Reyna, V. F. (1987). Understanding verbs: easy extension, hard comprehension. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (Vol. 3, pp. 301-315). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rich, J. B., Troyer, A. K., Bylsma, F. W., & Brandt, J. (1999). Longitudinal analysis of phonemic clustering and switching during word-list generation in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 13, 525-531. Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2001). Object recognition, In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 45-74). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Coltheart, M., & Funnell, E. (1988). Semantic system or systems? Neuropsychological evidence re-examined. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 3-25. 214 References Ripich, D. N., & Ziol, E. (1998). Dementia: A review for the speech-language pathologist. In A.F. Johnson & B.H. Jacobson (Eds.), Medical speech-language pathology: A practitioner’s guide (pp. 467-494). New York: Thieme Medical. Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., & Smith, E. E. (1973). Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 1-20. Riva, D., Nichelli, F., & Devoti, M. (2000). Developmental aspects of verbal fluency and confrontation naming in children. Brain and Language 71, 267-284. Roberts, P., & Le Dorze, G. (1994). Semantic verbal fluency in aphasia: A quantitative and qualitative study in test-retest conditions. Aphasiology, 8, 569582. Roberts, P. M., & Le Dorze, G. (1997). Semantic organization, strategy use, and productivity in bilingual semantic verbal fluency. Brain and Language, 59, 412449. Robinson, K. M., Grossman, M., White-Devine, T., & D’Esposito, M. (1996). Category-specific difficulty naming with verbs in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 47, 178-182. Rochford, G. (1971). A study of naming errors in dysphasic and in demented patients. Neuropsychologia, 9, 437-443. Rochon, E., Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1994). Sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 46, 329-349. Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107-142. Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192-233. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B.B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27-48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rosch E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605. Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Bream, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382-439. Rosen, W. G. (1980). Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 135-146. Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R.W. (1997). The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 211-227. References 215 Rosser, A., & Hodges, J. R. (1994). Initial letter and semantic category fluency in Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, 1389-1394. Rubin, E. H., Kinscherf, D. A., & Morris, J. C. (1993). Psychopathology in younger versus older persons with very mild and mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 639-642. Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57, 394-405. Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). A general framework for parallel distributed processing. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 1: Foundations, (pp. 4576). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models (pp. 7-57). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Saarela, T., Koponen, H., Erkinjuntti, T., Alhainen, K., & Viramo, P. (1997). Alzheimerin taudin nonkognitiiviset oireet [Non-cognitive symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease]. Suomen Lääkärilehti, 52, 2797-2802. Saeed, J. I. (1997). Semantics. Oxford: Blackwell. Saffran, E. M., Schwartz, M. F., & Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Evidence from aphasia: Isolating the components of a production model. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production. Volume 1. Speech and talk (pp. 221-241). London: Academic Press. Saffran, E. M., & Sholl, A. (1999). Clues to the functional and neural architecture of word meaning. In C. M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 241-272). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of perseveration. Neuropsychologia, 22, 715-732. Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Perseveration in behavioral neurology. Neurology, 37, 1736-1741. Santos, L. R., & Caramazza, A. (2002). The domain-specific hypothesis: A developmental and comparative perspective on category-specific deficits. In E. M. E. Forde & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 1-23). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. 216 References Sartori, G., Coltheart, M., Miozzo, M., & Job, R. (1994). Category specificity and informational specificity in neuropsychological impairment of semantic memory. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV. Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 537-550). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Saukkonen, P., Haipus, M., Niemikorpi, A., & Sulkala, H. (1979). Suomen kielen taajuussanasto [A frequency dictionary of Finnish]. Porvoo: WSOY. Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). What are the memory systems of 1994? In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory Systems 1994 (pp. 1-38). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schacter, D. L., Wagner, A. D., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory systems of 1999. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 627-643). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependency: A theory of natural language understanding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 552-631. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. An inquiry into knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schreuder, R., & Flores D’Arcais, G. B. (1989). Psycholinguistic issues in the lexical representation of meaning. In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representation and process (pp. 409-436). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schwartz, B., & Reisberg, D. (1991). Learning and memory. New York: W. W. Norton. Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Bloch, D. E., & Dell. G. S. (1994). Disordered speech production in aphasic and normal speakers. Brain and Language 47, 5288. Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S. M., & Saffran, E. M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language, 7, 277-306. Schyns, P. G., & Rodet, L. (1997). Categorization creates functional features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 681696. Selnes, O. A. (2001). A historical overview of contributions from the study of deficits. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 23-41). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. References 217 Selnes, O. A., Carson, K., Rovner, B., & Gordon, B. (1988). Language dysfunction in early- and late-onset possible Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 38, 1053-1056. Seltzer, B., & Sherwin, I. (1983). A comparison of clinical features in early- and late-onset primary degenerative dementia. One entity or two? Archives of Neurology, 40, 143-146. Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Shapiro, K., & Caramazza, A. (2001a). Language is more than its parts: A reply to Bird, Howard, and Franklin (2001). Brain and Language, 78, 397-401. Shapiro, K., & Caramazza, A. (2001b). Sometimes a noun is just a noun: Comments on Bird, Howard, and Franklin (2000). Brain and Language, 76, 202212. Shapiro, L. P., Zurif, E., & Grimshaw, J. (1987). Sentence processing and the mental representation of verbs. Cognition, 27, 219-246. Shelton, J. R., & Caramazza, A. (2001). The organization of semantic memory. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. What deficits reveal about the human mind, (pp. 423-443). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Sheridan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1993). A verbal-semantic category-specific semantic impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 143-184. Shindler, A. G., Caplan, L. R., & Hier, D. B. (1984). Intrusions and perseverations. Brain and Language, 23, 148-158. Shuttleworth, E. C., & Huber, S. J. (1988). The naming disorder of dementia of Alzheimer type. Brain and Language, 34, 222-234. Silveri, M. C., & Leggio, M. G. (1996). Influence of disorders of visual perception in word-to-picture matching tasks in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 54, 326-334. Skelton-Robinson, M., & Jones, S. (1984). Nominal dysphasia and the severity of senile dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 168-171. Slackman, E. A., Hudson, J. A., & Fivush, R. (1986). Actions, actors, links, and goals: The structure of children’s event representations. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 47-69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sloman, S. A., & Rips, L. J. (1998). Similarity as an explanatory construct. Cognition, 65, 87-101. 218 References Small, S. L. (1997). Semantic category imprecision: A connectionist study of the boundaries of word meanings. Brain and Language, 57, 181-194. Small, S. L., Hart, J., Nguyen, T., & Gordon, B. (1995). Distributed representations of semantic knowledge in the brain. Brain, 118, 441-453. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of categorization. InM.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 10131022). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Smith, E. E., Patalano, A. L., & Jonides, J. (1998). Alternative strategies of categorization. Cognition, 65, 167-196. Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., & Ribs, L. J. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review, 81, 214-241. Smith, L. B., & Samuelson, L. K. (1997). Perceiving and remembering: Category stability, variability and development. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 161-195). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Smith, S., Faust, M., Beeman, M., Kennedy, L., & Perry, D. (1995). A property level analysis of lexical semantic representation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 49, 263-279. Smith, S. R., Chenery, H. J., & Murdoch, B. E. (1989). Semantic abilities in dementia of the Alzheimer type: II. Grammatical semantics. Brain and Language, 36, 533-542. Smith, S. R., Murdoch, B. E., & Chenery, H. J. (1989). Semantic abilities in dementia of the Alzheimer type. I. Lexical semantics. Brain and Language, 36, 314-324. Smolensky, P. (1986). Information processing in dynamical systems: Foundations of harmony theory. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group, Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 1: Foundations (pp. 194-281). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Soininen, H. (1997). Alzheimerin taudin hoito [Treatment of Alzheimer’s disease]. Duodecim, 113, 1739-1746. Soininen, H. (2001). Muistihäiriöiden ja dementian ennaltaehkäisy [Prevention of memory disorders and dementia]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 435-441). Helsinki: Duodecim. References 219 Soininen, H., & Hänninen, T. (1999). Kuka on vaarassa sairastua Alzheimerin tautiin? [Who is in danger of getting Alzheimer’s disease?]. Suomen Lääkärilehti, 54, 1519-1523. Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. Squire, L. R. (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 203-231). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Squire, L. R., & Knowlton, B. J. (2000). The medial temporal lobe, the hippocampus, and the memory systems of the brain. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences. (2nd ed., pp. 765-779). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Stemberger, J. P. (1985). The lexicon in a model of language production. New York: Garland Publishing. Stern, Y., Liu, X., Albert, M., Brandt, J., Jacobs, D. M., Del Castillo-Castaneda, C., Marder, K., Bell, K., Sano, M., & Bylsma, F. (1996). Modeling the influences of extrapyramidal signs on the progression of Alzheimer Disease. Archives of Neurology, 53, 1121-1126. Storms, G., & De Boeck, P. (1997), Formal models for intra-categorical structure that can be used for data analysis. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 439-459). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Suhr, J. A., & Jones, R. D. (1998). Letter and semantic fluency in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s and Parkinson’s dementias. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 13, 447-454. Sulkava, R. (1982). Alzheimer’s disease and senile dementia of Alzheimer type. A comparative study. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 65, 636-650. Sulkava, R. (1983). Alzheimer’s disease. Neurological, neuropsychological, neuropathological and genetic studies. Doctoral dissertation. Department of Neurology, University of Helsinki and Koskela Hospital, Helsinki, Finland. Sulkava, R., & Amberla, K. (1982). Alzheimer’s disease and senile dementia of Alzheimer type. A neuropsychological study. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 65, 651-660. Sulkava, R., Erkinjuntti, T., & Palo, J. (1989). Dementia. Tutkimus ja hoito [Dementia. Examination and treatment]. SITRA 106. Sosiaali- ja terveysministeriö, Suomen itsenäisyyden juhlavuoden 1967 rahasto. Jyväskylä: Gummerus. 220 References Swihart, A. A., Panisset, M., Becker, J. T., Beyer, J. R., & Boller, F. (1989). The Token Test: Validity and diagnostic power in Alzheimer’s disease. Developmental Neuropsychology, 5, 69-78. Taylor, J. R. (1995). Linguistic categorization. Prototypes in linguistic theory (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Thompson, C. K., Lange, K. L., Schneider, S. L., & Shapiro, L. P. (1997). Agrammatic and non-brain-damaged subjects’ verb and verb argument structure production. Aphasiology, 11, 473-490. Tienari, P., & Haltia, M. (2001). Alzheimerin taudin patogeneesi [Pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 105-121). Helsinki: Duodecim. Tippett, L. J., McAuliffe, S., & Farah, M. J. (1995), Preservation of categorical knowledge in Alzheimer’s disease: A computational account. Memory, 3, 519-533. Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tomer, R., & Levin, B. E. (1993). Differential effects of aging on two verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 465-466. Tomoeda, C. K., Bayles, K. A., Boone, D. R., Kaszniak, A.W., & Slauson, T. J. (1990). Speech rate and syntactic complexity effects on the auditory comprehension of Alzheimer patients. Journal of Communication Disorders, 23, 151-161. Tranel, D., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). A neural basis for the retrieval of words for actions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 655-670. Travniczek-Marterer, A., Danielczyk, W., Simanyi, M., & Fischer, P. (1993). Ideomotor apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 88, 14. Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 370378. Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11, 138-146. Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: the effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36, 499-504. References 221 Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Leach, L., & Freedman, M. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 137-143. Tröster, A. I., Fields, J. A., Testa, J. A., Paul, R. H., Blanco, C. R., Hames, K. A., Salmon, D. P., & Beatty, W. W. (1998). Cortical and subcortical influences on clustering and switching in the performance of verbal fluency task. Neuropsychologia, 36, 295-304. Tröster, A. I., Salmon, D. P., McCullough, D., & Butters, N. (1989). A comparison of the category fluency deficits associated with Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Brain and Language, 37, 500-513. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donalson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York: Academic Press. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Tulving, E. (2000). Introduction. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 727-732). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1983). Categories of environmental scenes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 121-149. Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 169-193. Tyler, L. K., & Moss, H. E. (2001). Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 244-252. Tyler, L. K., Moss, H. E., Durrant-Peatfield, M. R., & Levy, J. P. (2000). Conceptual structure and the structure of concepts: A distributed account of category-specific deficits. Brain and Language, 75, 195-231. Tyler, L. K., Russell, R., Fadili, J., & Moss, H. E. (2001). The neural representation of nouns and verbs: PET studies. Brain, 124, 1619-1634. Ungerer, F., & Schmid, H-J. (1996). An introduction to cognitive linguistics. London: Longman. Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1996). Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature, 383, 254-256. Vataja, R. (2001). Dementiaan liittyvät käytösoireet [Behavioral symptoms related to dementia]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 94-103). Helsinki: Duodecim. Vilkki, J. (1989). Differential perseverations in verbal retrieval related to anterior and posterior left hemisphere lesions. Brain and Language, 36, 543-554. 222 References Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2002). A semantic analysis of grammatical class impairments: Semantic representations of object nouns, action nouns and action verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 317-351. Viramo, P., & Sulkava, R. (2001). Muistihäiriöiden ja dementian epidemiologia [Epidemiology of memory disorders and dementia]. In T. Erkinjuntti, J. Rinne, K. Alhainen & H. Soininen (Eds.), Muistihäiriöt ja dementia (pp. 20-36). Helsinki: Duodecim. Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635-657. Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859-878. Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1987). Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273-1296. Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain 107, 829-854. Watkins, M. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1979). An appreciation of generate-recognize theory of recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 687-704. Watson, C. M., Chenery, H. J., & Carter, M. S. (1999). An analysis of trouble and repair in the natural conversation of people with dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Aphasiology, 13, 195-218. Wayland, S. C., Berndt, R. S., & Sandson, J. R. (1996). Aphasic patients’ sensitivity to structural and meaning violations when monitoring for nouns and verbs in sentences. Neuropsychology, 10, 504-516. Wechsler, D. (1986). Wechslerin muistiasteikko [Wechsler Memory Scale]. Helsinki: Psykologien Kustannus. Weingartner, H., Grafman, J., Boutelle, W., Kaye, W., & Martin, P. R. (1983). Forms of memory failure. Science, 221, 380-382. Weingartner, H. J., Kawas, C., Rawlings, R., & Shapiro, M. (1993). Changes in semantic memory in early stage Alzheimer’s disease patients. The Gerontologist, 33, 637-643. Westbury, C. (1998). Research strategies. Psychological and psycholinguistic methods in neurolinguistics. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 83-93). San Diego: Academic Press. Whatmough, C., & Chertkow, H. (2002). Category-specific recognition impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. In E.M.E. Forde & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), References 223 Category specificity on brain and mind (pp. 181-210). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Whatmough, C., Chertkow, H., Murtha, S., Templeman, D., Babins, L., & Kelner, N. (2003). The semantic category effect increases with worsening anomia in Alzheimer’s type dementia. Brain and Language, 84, 134-147. White-Devine, T., Grossman, M., Robinson, K. M., Onishi, K., Biassou, N., & D’Esposito, M. (1996). Verb confrontation naming and word-picture matching in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 495-503. White-Devine, T., Robinson, K., Onishi, K., Seidl, A., D’Esposito, M., & Grossman, M. (1995). Verb naming and recognition impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 51, 34-36. Williams, S. E., & Canter, G. J. (1987). Action-naming performance in four syndromes of aphasia. Brain and Language, 32, 124-136. Willmes, K. (1998). Methodological and statistical considerations in cognitive neurolinguistics. In B. Stemmer & H.A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 57-70). San Diego: Academic Press. Williamson, D. J. G., Adair, J. C., Raymer, A. M., & Heilman, K. M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 34, 601-610. World Health Organization (1993). ICD-10 Classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: WHO. Ylikoski, R. (2000). The relationship of neuropsychological functioning with demographic characteristics, brain imaging findings, and health in elderly individuals. Doctoral dissertation. Helsinki: Departments of Psychology and Neurology, University of Helsinki. http://ethesis.helsinki.fi. Ylikoski, R., Erkinjuntti, T., Sulkava, R., Juva, K., Tilvis, R., & Valvanne, J. (1992). Correction for age, education, and other demographic variables in the use of the Mini Mental State Examination in Finland. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 85, 391-396. Ylikoski, R., Ylikoski, A., Keskivaara, P., Tilvis, R., Sulkava, R., & Erkinjuntti, T. (1999). Heterogenity of cognitive profiles in aging: successful aging, normal aging, and individuals at risk for cognitive decline. European Journal of Neurology, 6, 645-652. Zec, R. F., Landreth, E. S., Fritz, S., Grames, E., Hasara, A., Fraizer, W., Belman, J., Wainman, S., McCool, M., O’Connell, C., Harris, R., Robbs, R., Elble, R., & Manyam, B. (1999). A comparison of phonemic, semantic and alternating word fluency in Parkinson’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 255264. 224 References Appendix 1 225 Appendix 1A. Cluster division for the noun categories Clothes Accessories: belt, bow tie, handkerchief, suspenders, tie. Coats: bathrobe, blazer, cardigan, cloak, coat, fur coat, jacket, leather coat, leather jacket, parka, rain cape, raincoat, suit coat, trench coat, Windbreaker, winter coat. Footwear: ankle boots, boots, cleats, galoshes, inside shoes, outdoor shoes, riding boots, rubber boots, sandals, shoes, ski boots, slippers, sneakers, tennis shoes, winter boots, winter footwear. Gloves: gloves, mittens, muff, wool gloves. Headwear: cap, fur hat, hat, scarf. Indoor clothes: ankle socks, apron, blazer, blouse, bow tie, cardigan, dress, dress shirt, jeans, knee socks, pantihose, pants, shirt, shoes, skirt, socks, suit, suit coat, suit pants, sweater, sweater vest, tie, t-shirt, top, undershirt, vest, wool skirt, wool socks, wool sweater. Men’s clothing: bow tie, dress coat, long johns, pants, suit, tie, tuxedo. Outdoors clothes: cap, cardigan, cloak, coat, felt hat, footwear, fur coat, fur hat, gloves, hat, leather coat, leather jacket, mittens, muff, outdoor pants, overalls, overcoat, parka, rain cape, raincoat, scarf, shoes, ski pants, ski socks, ski suit, slalom pants, summer coat, sweater vest, sweat pants, swimsuit, trench coat, ulster, Windbreaker, winter coat, wool gloves, wool pants, wool socks, wool sweater. Outfits: ball gown, bikini, cocktail dress, diving suit, dress, evening dress, evening gown, evening wear, jogging suit, kimono, military uniform, national costume, pajamas, ski suit, sportswear, suit, swimsuit, tailcoat, tracksuit, tuxedo, wrestling leotard. Pants: jeans, long johns, pantihose, shorts, ski pants, sports pants, sports shorts, suit pants, swim pants, underpants, wool pants. Scarves: scarf, shawl. Shirts: blouse, dress shirt, shirt, sports shirt, sweater, t-shirt, top, undershirt. Skirts: dress, skirt, slip, wool skirt. Socks: ankle socks, knee socks, wool socks. Underwear: bathrobe, bra, corset, girdle, long johns, negligee, nightgown, nightie, pajamas, pantihose, shirt, slip, underpants, undershirt. Women’s clothing: bra, corset, dress, girdle, skirt. Wool clothes: wool pants, wool scarf, wool socks, wool sweater. 226 Appendix1 Vegetables Cabbages: broccoli, Brussels sprouts, cabbage, cauliflower, Chinese cabbage, Savoy cabbage. Color: green vegetables: cucumber, dill, lettuce, parsley, peas, spinach. Herbs and spices: basil, chervil, chives, cilantro, cumin, dill, ginger, fennel, marjoram, parsley, summer savory, tarragon, thyme. Leguminous plants: beans, peas. Onions: chives, garlic, leek, onion. Root- and tuberous vegetables: beet, carrot, horseradish, Jerusalem artichoke, parsnip, radish, rutabaga, sugar beet, turnip. Sprouts and greens: artichoke, asparagus, broccoli, Brussels sprouts, cabbage, cauliflower, celery, Chinese cabbage, chives, endive, garlic, iceberg lettuce, leek, lettuce, onion, rhubarb, Savoy cabbage, spinach. Vegetables used for salads: avocado, cucumber, lettuce, squash, tomato, zucchini. Vehicles Beasts of burden: dog, horse, horse and rider, horse cab, reindeer and sleigh. Boats and ships: barge, boat, canoe, dinghy, motorboat, rowboat, sailboat, ship, steamboat, submarine. Cars: automobile, bus, police car, racecar, sports car, taxi, toy car, truck, van. Means of mass transportation: airplane, bus, jet plane, passenger ship, ship, subway, taxi, train, tram. Muscle-powered vehicles: bicycle, boat, canoe, carriage, feet, ice skates, kickboard, kick-sled, mountain bike, racing bicycle, roller skates, roller skis, rowboat, skateboard, skis, sled, snowboard, tandem bicycle, tricycle, wheelchair. On-road vehicles: bicycle, bus, car, convertible, jeep, kick sled, moped, motorcycle, motorcycle and sidecar, police car, tandem bicycle, taxi, tractor, truck, van. Vehicles in the air: airplane, glider, helicopter, hot air balloon, jet plane, jumbo jet, parachute, rocket, sailplane, shuttle. Vehicles on the rails: handcar, subway, train, tram. Vehicles on wheels: bike, car, moped, motorcycle, roller skates, skateboard, taxi, truck, van. Vehicles used in wintertime: ice skates, kick-sled, reindeer and sleigh, skis, snowboard, snowmobile. Vehicles used on water: barge, boat, canoe, dinghy, ferry, motorboat, rowboat, sailboard, sailboat, steamboat, submarine, water-skis. Others: elevator, escalator. Appendix 1 227 Animals Beasts of burden: camel, donkey, horse, mule, ox. Birds: bird of paradise, blackbird, black grouse, capercailzie, chaffinch, crane, crow, eagle, emu, falcon, finch, goldeneye, goose, gull, hazelhen, magpie, mallard, ostrich, parrot, peacock, pheasant, pied flycatcher, skylark, sparrow, starling, stork, swallow, swan, tit, vulture, willow grouse, woodpecker. Canine: dog, fox, hyena, wolf. Deer: elk, reindeer. Exotic, foreign animals: alligator, alpaca, bison, camel, caracal, chameleon, cheetah, crocodile, donkey, elephant, emu, giraffe, gorilla, hippopotamus, hyena, impala, jaguar, kangaroo, karakul, leopard, lion, llama, marmot, monkey, mule, ostrich, panda, panther, parrot, polar bear, puma, rattlesnake, rhinoceros, sable, skunk, snake, tiger, turtle, whale, wild boar, zebra. Farm animals: bull, calf, cat, cow, dog, foal, goat, goose, hen, horse, kid, mare, ox, pig, pony, rabbit, rooster, sheep, stallion. Feline: cat, cheetah, cougar, jaguar, leopard, lion, lynx, panther, puma, tiger. Fish: Baltic herring, burbot, gold-fish, herring, lavaret, perch, pike, roach, salmon, vendace, zander. Insects: ant, bedbug, butterfly, cockroach, dragonfly, flea, fly, louse, mosquito, spider, wasp. Invertebrates: worm, slug. Pets: canary, cat, dog, goldfish, guinea pig, mouse, parrot, rabbit. Reptiles: alligator, crocodile, frog, grass snake, lizard, newt, rattlesnake, snake, toad, tortoise. Rodents: beaver, guinea pig, hamster, hare, hedgehog, mink, mole, mouse, muskrat, rat, squirrel. Water animals: alligator, beaver, crocodile, fish, frog, muskrat, newt, otter, seal, toad, turtle, whale. Wild mammals, found in Finland: Arctic fox, badger, bear, beaver, deer, elk, ermine, fox, hare, hedgehog, lynx, mink, mole, mouse, muskrat, otter, pine marten, polecat, raccoon dog, rat, reindeer, seal, weasel, wolf, wolverine. 228 Appendix1 Appendix 1B. Cluster division for the verb categories Preparing food Baking: add breadcrumbs, allow dough to rise, bake, beat, butter, dust with flour, dust with sugar, foam, frost, knead dough, make dough, mix, pipe cream on cake, put dough in mold, roll, season, simmer, strain. Cleaning: clean, rinse, scrub, wash. Cleaning and preparing: chop, clean, cut, cut into pieces, dice, fillet, grate, mash, peel, pluck, reduce, rinse, scale and gut fish, skin, slice, split, wash. Cooking: bake, blanch, boil, broil, brown, burn, cook, flame, frizzle, fry, gratiné, grill, keep watch, roast, simmer, smoke, turn off/on the stove. Diminishing: chop, cut, dice, mince, puree, reduce, slice. Dining: clear the table, drink, eat, pour drinks, serve, set the table, wash the dishes. Handling food: allow to cool, beat, clean, crumble, freeze, grate, grind, mash, melt, mix, puree, scoop, skim, spoon, spread, strain, stir, thicken, thin, turn, warm up, wash. Pre-preparing: bring home, buy, get food, make the fire, put water in pot, shop, turn on stove. Preserving: can, freeze, preserve, salt. Seasoning: marinate, salt, season, spice, taste. Scripts: Listing actions in sequential order (e.g., rinse potatoes, peel potatoes, boil potatoes). Playing sports Boxing and wrestling: box, go to the gym, lift weights, wrestle. Dancing: dance, polka. Flying: fly, glide. Games: kickball, play baseball/golf/basketball/hockey/soccer/squash/tennis/ volleyball, play with a ball. Gymnastics: aerobics, bend, do gymnastics, jump, lift, roll shoulders, swing arms, turn head. Horse racing: ride a horse, show jumping, steeplechase. Jumping: high jump, jump, leap, long jump. Motor sports: car racing, motor sports. Playing sports with some equipment: bicycle, fence, skate, ski, snowboard. Running: jog, run, run long distance, sprint. Skating: ice-skate, figure skate. Appendix 1 229 Sports with the emphasis on using hands/arms: hit, javelin, lift, lift weights, shotput, throw, throw a ball/discus/hammer. Sports with the emphasis on using legs: cross-country running, cross-country walking, high jump, hop on one foot, hurdles, jog, jump, jump rope, kick, kickball, leap, long distance running, long jump, orienteering, relay race, run, run 100/400/ 1000m, sprint, triple jump, walk. Throwing: javelin, shot-put, throw a ball. Water sports: butterfly, crawl, deep-water diving , dive, paddle, row, sail, surf, swim, synchronized swimming. Winter sports: cross-country skiing, downhill skiing, figure skate, skate, ski, ski jump, slalom skiing, snowboard. Construction Basic work: beat, build a frame, build scaffolding, cast, dig, drain, fasten, lay the foundation, lay pipes, level, mix concrete, pour cement, prepare the site, quarry, sand the ground, set corner stones, tamp, till soil. Brickwork: carry bricks, cast, concrete, even out the plaster, grind, joint, lay brick, lay the foundation, masonry, mix concrete/mortar, plaster, pour concrete, rub down, smooth out. Diminishing: cut, saw, split. Fastening : fasten, glue, seam. Handling the surfaces: coat, cover, decorate, glue, grind, paint, panel, plaster, tile, wallpaper. Interior work: decorate, electrical work, furnish, install the fittings, lighting, make the floor, paint, put up the cupboards, tile, wallpaper. Preparing and supervising: apply for building permit, buy building materials, draft, keep watch, order material/walls, pay building costs, plan, supply with materials, survey (the building site). Repairing a building: mend, putty, raise, repair, renovate, replace, take down, tear down, trim, wreck. Woodworking: beat, board up, carpentry, carry boards, carve, clean the wood, cut the logs to length, drill, fit, fit the boards, grind, lath, measure, nail, paint, panel, plane, sandpaper, saw, screw, shorten, stain, varnish. Working on the fabric of a building: beat, board up, build the frame/the wall/the roof/the stairs, carry materials, cover, drill, electrical work, fasten, fasten the windows, glaze, install insulation, lift boards, make the floor, make the stone base, outdoor painting, putty, raise the walls, screw, seal up, set up, set rain gutters, supply with water, water and sewage work. Scripts: listing actions in a temporal-causal, serial order (e.g., draft, apply for building permit, survey (the building site), lay the foundation, build the frame, put up the walls, build the roof, insulate, paint, wallpaper, furnish). 230 Appendix1 Cleaning up Airing: air, air the apartment/bedding/carpets/linen, beat, beat the bedding/carpets, dust, shake, shake a carpet. Arranging: clean out the cupboards, organize the dishes, put the dishes in the cupboard, take out the trash. Cleaning the floor: air the carpets, beat the carpets, bring the carpets in, brush, clean, clean the floor, clean the floor with a machine, dry, mop, polish, polish with a polishing machine, put the carpets down, rinse, rub, scour, scrub, shake the carpets, sweep, sweep the rubbish, use a vacuum-cleaner, vacuum, wash, wash the floor, wax, wipe. Cleaning up the other surfaces: clean, clean the doors, clean off spots, clean the window frames, clean the windows, dry, dust, dust off the walls, polish, rinse, rub, scour, scrub, wash, wash the cupboards, wash the walls, wipe, wipe the furniture, wipe the tables. Defrosting the refrigerator: arrange, defrost the refrigerator, dry the refrigerator, put things in. Preparing to clean: add the detergent, buy a brush/stuff for cleaning up, get the washing water, pour water into a bucket. Taking care of the cleaning equipment: clean the cleaning equipment, put the cleaning equipment in places, take care of the cleaning equipment. Taking care of the linen and carpets: air the bedding/carpets/linen, beat, beat the bedding/carpets/clothes, change sheets, do laundry, dust the clothes, dry, get out the linen, hang, hang on a line to dry, make the beds, shake the carpets, straighten, wash the curtains. Ways of cleaning: clean, disinfect, do the dishes, rinse, scour, scrub, wash, wash the floor, wash with a brush, wipe. Scripts: listing actions in a temporal-causal, serial order (e.g., air the linen, dust, clean the floor, beat the carpets). 6-7 (highly prototypical) blouse (pusero) 7.00, shirt (paita) 7.00, pants (pitkät housut) 6.93, jeans (farkut) 6.79, coat (takki) 6.71, t-shirt (t-paita) 6.57, cardigan (villatakki) 6.57, dress (mekko) 6.4, jacket (pikkutakki) 6.36 tomato (tomaatti) 7.00, pepper (paprika) 6.79, cucumber (kurkku) 6.57, iceberg lettuce (jäävuorisalaatti) 6.43, lettuce (lehtisalaatti) 6.36, cauliflower (kukkakaali) 6.29, Chinese cabbage (kiinankaali) 6.29, cabbage (keräkaali) 6.14, broccoli (parsakaali) 6.07, leek (purjosipuli) 6.00 train (juna) 6.93, bus (bussi) 6.93, car (auto) 6.86, subway (metro) 6.86, bicycle (polkupyörä) 6.79, plane (lentokone) 6.79, tram (raitiovaunu) 6.71, ship (laiva) 6.57, motorcycle (moottoripyörä) 6.43, taxi (taksi) 6.36 Category Clothes Vegetables Vehicles truck (kuorma-auto) 5.93, jumbo jet (jumbojetti) 5.79, steamboat (höyrylaiva) 5.71, motorboat (moottorivene) 5.64, sports car (urheiluauto) 5.50, moped (mopedi) 5.50, trailer truck (rekkaauto) 5.29, rowboat (soutuvene) 5.29, racing bike (kilpapyörä) 5.07, sailboat (purjevene) 5.00 Brussels sprouts (ruusukaali) 5.86, onion (sipuli) 5.71, potato (peruna) 5.64, carrot (porkkana) 5.64, bean (papu) 5.64, rutabega (lanttu) 5.57, spinach (pinaatti) 5.50, turnip (nauris) 5.43, celery (lehtiselleri) 5.29, parsnip (palsternakka) 5.00 winter coat (talvitakki) 5.93, long johns (kalsarit) 5.71, nightgown (yöpaita) 5.64, underpants (alushousut) 5.64, vest (liivi) 5.64, bra (rintaliivit) 5.50, raincoat (sadetakki) 5.29, wool socks (villasukat) 5.14, scarf (kaulahuivi) 5.14 5-6 (prototypical) ferry (lossi) 4.93, sled (kelkka) 4.57, speedboat (pikavene) 4.50, horse-drawn carriage (hevoskärryt) 4.43, canoe (kanootti) 4.36, race car (kilpa-auto) 4.36 pumpkin (kurpitsa) 4.86, Savouy cabbage (savoijinkaali) 4.86, parsley (persilja) 4.71, radish (retikka) 4.71, garlic (valkosipuli) 4.57, sugar beet (sokerijuurikas) 4.29, herbs (yrtit) 4.07 pantihose (sukkahousut) 4.93, wool pants (villahousut) 4.86, socks (sukat) 4.79, mittens (lapaset) 4.64, cap (lakki) 4.50, swim-suit (uimapuku) 4.43, hat (hattu) 4.36, tie (kravatti) 4.29, headwear (päähine) 4.14, sports shoes (urheilukengät) 4.07 4-5 (intermediately prototypical) reindeer and sleigh (poronpulkka) 3.00, feet (jalat) 2.64, skates (luistimet) 2.64, escalator (rullaportaat) 2.50, horse and rider (ratsukko) 2.43, wood sled (halkoreki) 2.36, parachute (laskuvarjo) 2.29, shuttle (sukkula) 2.07 endive (endiivi) 2.71 suspenders (henkselit) 3.07, boots (saappaat) 3.00, track shoes (piikkarit) 3.00, ski boots (hiihtokengät) 2.93, 2.50, diving suit (sukelluspuku) 2.00 2-3 (not so prototypical) Appendix 2. A sample of prototypicality ratings of the words produced for the semantic fluency tasks Appendix 2 231 6-7 (highly prototypical) boil (keittää) 6.64, make meatballs (tehdä lihapullia) 6.57, fry (paistaa) 6.50, season (maustaa) 6.21, grill (grillata) 6.14, simmer (hauduttaa) 6.14, cook (kypsentää) 6.07, smoke (savustaa) 6.07 play soccer (pelata jalkapalloa) 6.93, ski (hiihtää) 6.86, run (juosta) 6.79, swim (uida) 6.71, jog (lenkkeillä) 6.64, play volleyball (pelata lentopalloa) 6.57, throw the javelin (heittää keihästä) 6.50, wrestle (painia) 6.43, play baseball (pelata pesäpalloa) 6.36, high jump (hypätä korkeutta) 6.14 build a roof (rakentaa katto) 6.71, masonry (muurata) 6.36, pour foundation (valaa perusta) 6.29, nail (naulata) 6.21, 6.14, build a frame (rakentaa kehikko) 6.14, panel (paneloida) 6.00 vacuum (imuroida) 7.00, sweep (lakaista) 6.86, beat carpets (piiskata matot) 6.79, mop (mopata) 6.71, dust (pyyhkiä pölyt) 6.64, scrub (jynssätä) 6.36, take carpets out (viedä matot ulos) 6.07 Categories Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning up Appendix 2 (continued) brush (harjata) 5.86, wash (pestä) 5.71, brush (harjata) 5.43, air the linen (tuulettaa liinavaatteet) 5.43, bring in carpets (tuoda matot sisään) 5.36, beat (piiskata) 5.36, arrange (järjestää) 5.29, scrub (hangata) 5.14, air (tuulettaa) 5.00 cast (valaa) 5.93, hammer (vasaroida) 5.86, wallpaper (laittaa tapetit) 5.79, dig the foundation (kaivaa pohja) 5.64, drill (porata) 5.36, 5.29, screw (ruuvata) 5.29, paint (maalata) 5.14, insulate (eristää) 5.07 cross-country running (maastojuoksu) 5.93, do gymnastics (voimistella) 5.86, ride a bike (ajaa pyörällä) 5.79, crosscountry skiing (murtomaahiihto) 5.71, figure skate (kaunoluistella) 5.64, slalom skiing (pujotella) 5.50, throw the discus (heittää kiekkoa) 5.29, shot-put (työntää kuulaa) 5.43, sail (purjehtia) 5.36, throw the hammer (heittää moukaria) 5.29 slice (suikaloida) 5.93, fillet (fileoida) 5.79, bake (leipoa) 5.79, roast (paahtaa) 5.64, stir (hämmentää) 5.50, beat (vatkata) 5.50, mash (muussata) 5.36, heat (kuumentaa) 5.29, mix (sekoittaa) 5.21, peel (kuoria) 5.00 5-6 (prototypical) wax (vahata) 4.93, polish (kiillottaa) 4.79, rinse (huuhdella) 4.57, change bed sheets (vaihtaa lakanat) 4.57, shake (kopistella) 4.36, wash dishes (pestä astioita) 4.14, take care of the cleaning equipment (huoltaa siivousvälineet) 4.00 grind (hioa) 4.93, plane (höylätä) 4.86, mix (sekoittaa) 4.64, mix the concrete (sekoittaa betoni) 4.64, install the fixtures (asentaa kalusteet) 4.50 pile up (kasata) 4.14, repair (korjata) 4.43, level (tasoittaa) 4.36, measure (mitata) 4.00 play ball (palloilla) 4.93, dance (tanssia) 4.71, play golf (pelata golfia) 4.64, walk (kävellä) 4.57, surf (surffata) 4.50, car racing (ajaa autolla kilpaa) 4.43, throw (heittää) 4.29, jump (hypätä) 4.21, stretch (venytellä) 4.21, shoot (ampua) 4.00 season (höystää) 4.86, salt (suolata) 4.79, dice (palotella) 4.71, whisk (vaahdottaa) 4.64, slice (viipaloida) 4.50, turn on the stove (laittaa hella päälle) 4.43, thicken (saostaa) 4.36, cut (leikata) 4.07, warm up (lämmittää) 4.00 4-5 (intermediately prototypical) beat (hakata) 2.86, hang (ripustaa) 2.36, embellish (kaunistaa) 2.29, decorate (koristella) 2.00 take down (purkaa) 2.93, split (halkaista) 2.93, fasten (laittaa kiinni) 2.86, cut (leikata) 2.79, raise (korottaa) 2.71, carry (kantaa) 2.57, shorten (lyhentää) 2.43, reduce (pienentää) 2.43, decorate (koristaa) 2.36, clean (puhdistaa) 2.00 dance the polka (mennä polkkaa) 2.93, hit (lyödä) 2.79, lift (nostaa) 2.71, bend (taivuttaa) 2.71, do the butterfly stroke (perhostella) 2.64, drive (ajaa) 2.36, swing oneʼs arms (heiluttaa käsiä) 2.29, play (leikkiä) 2.21, rotate (pyöriä) 2.21, swing (keinua) 2.14 freeze (pakastaa) 3.00, preserve (säilöä) 3.00, rinse (huuhdella) 2.93, spread (levittää) 2.93, lay the table (kattaa) 2.93, pour (kaataa) 2.57, spoon (lusikoida) 2.50, thin (ohentaa) 2.50, wash (pestä) 2.29, clean (puhdistaa) 2.14 2-3 (not so prototypical) 232 Appendix 2 5-7 (very frequent) coat (takki) 5.93, pants (housut) 5.86, shirt (paita) 5.71, t-shirt (t-paita) 5.57, pantihose (sukkahousut) 5.50, jeans (farkut) 5.43, night gown (yöpaita) 5.29, wool sweater (villapaita) 5.14, underpants (pikkuhousut) 5.07, leather coat (nahkatakki) 5.00 potato (peruna) 6.00, cucumber (kurkku) 5.71, carrot (porkkana), 5.36, salad (salaatti) 5.50, tomato (tomaatti) 5.43, garlic (valkosipuli) 5.00 car (auto) 6.29, bicycle (polkupyörä) 6.00, train (juna) 5.79, bus (linjaauto) 5.64, walking (kävely) 5.57, feet (jalat) 5.64, taxi (taksi) 5.43, truck (kuorma-auto) 5.21, plane (lentokone) 5.21, boat (vene) 5.00 dog (koira) 6.00, cow (lehmä) 5.93, mosquito (hyttynen) 5.64, pig (sika) 5.57, horse (hevonen) 5.50, chicken (kana) 5.43, fly (kärpänen) 5.36, ant (muurahainen) 5.21, gull (lokki) 5.0, moose (hirvi) 5.00 Category Clothes Vegetables Vehicles Animals butterfly (perhonen) 4.86, Baltic herring (silakka) 4.79, tiger (tiikeri) 4.50, eagle (kotka) 4.43, reindeer (poro) 4.36, elephant (elefantti) 4.29, rat (rotta) 4.21, fox (kettu) 4.14, salmon (lohi) 4.07, bull (härkä) 4.00 tram (raitiovaunu) 4.71, motorcycle (moottoripyörä) 4.64, horse (hevonen) 4.57, trailer truck (rekkaauto) 4.57, skis (sukset) 4.43, van (pakettiauto) 4.36, motorboat (moottorivene) 4.29, skates (luistimet) 4.29, sailboat (purjevene) 4.00 pepper (paprika) 4.93, cabbage (kaali) 4.86, pea (herne) 4.79, cauliflower (kukkakaali) 4.64, garlic (valkosipuli) 4.57, lettuce (lehtisalaatti) 4.57, beet (punajuuri) 4.43, Chinese cabbage (kiinankaali) 4.21, bean (papu) 4.07, cabbage (keräkaali) 4.00 underwear (alusvaatteet) 4.93, gloves (hanskat) 4.86, scarf (kaulahuivi) 4.79, skirt (hame) 4.71, cardigan (villatakki) 4.64, suit (puku) 4.50, hat (hattu) 4.36, dress (mekko) 4.29, long johns (kalsarit) 4.14, tie (kravatti) 4.00 4-5 (intermediately frequent) rabbit (kaniini) 3.93, swan (joutsen) 3.86, wolverine (ahma) 3.79, giraffe (kirahvi) 3.71, lynx (ilves) 3.64, flea (kirppu) 3.57, camel (kameli) 3.50, dragonfly (sudenkorento) 3.43, leopard (leopardi) 3.29, alligator (alligaattori) 3.14 sled (pulkka) 3.93, rowboat (soutuvene) 3.86, passenger ship (matkustajalaiva) 3.79, escalator (rullaportaat) 3.64 , snowmobile (moottorikelkka) 3.50, tricycle (kolmipyörä) 3.43, police car (poliisiauto) 3.36, moped (mopedi) 3.29, ferry (lossi) 3.14, roller skates (rullaluistimet) 3.00 broccoli (parsakaali) 3.93, chives (ruohosipuli) 3.86, cabbage (valkokaali) 3.57, rutabaga (lanttu) 3.71, celery (lehtiselleri) 3.21, turnip (nauris) 3.57, zucchini (kesäkurpitsa) 3.43, spinach (pinaatti) 3.36, pumpkin (kurpitsa) 3.21, avocado (avokado) 3.00 sports pants (urheiluhousut) 3.93, fur coat (turkki) 3.79, blazer (bleiseri) 3.64, mittens (kintaat) 3.57, jacket (jakku) 3.50, apron (esiliina) 3.36, vest (liivi) 3.21, pajamas (yöpyjama) 3.14, rain cape (sadeviitta) 3.07, headwear (päähine) 3.00 2-3 (not so frequent) karakul (karakulla) 1.43, raccoon dog (supikoira) 1.86 church trap (kirkkokiesit) 1.93 - ulster (ulsteri) 1.93, kimono (kimono) 1.86, corset (kureliivi) 1.86, evening gown (päivällispuku) 1.79 1-2 (rare) Appendix 3. A sample of frequency ratings of the words produced for the semantic fluency tasks Appendix 3 233 5-7 (very frequent) boil (keittää) 6.00, wash (pestä) 5.71, fry (paistaa) 5.57, warm up (lämmittää) 5.57, fetch food (hakea ruoka) 5.57, heat (kuumentaa) 5.29, clean (siivota) 5.29, do the shopping (käydä kaupassa) 5.14, stir (sekoittaa) 5.14, do the dishes (tiskata) 5.14 run (juosta) 5.93, drive (ajaa) 5.93, walk (kävellä) 5.79, play (pelata) 5.50, lift (nostaa) 5.43, practice (harjoitella) 5.36, swim (uida) 5.29, sweat (hikoilla) 5.21, throw (heittää) 5.07 build a roof (rakentaa katto) 6.71, pour foundation (valaa perusta) 6.29, build the frame (rakentaa kehikko) 6.14, install doors and windows (laittaa ovia ja ikkunoita) 6.00, repair (korjata) 5.50, cut (leikata) 5.29, measure (mitata) 5.21, dig (kaivaa) 5.00 wash (pestä) 6.00 , do the dishes (tiskata) 5.71, clean (puhdistaa) 5.64, arrange (järjestää) 5.50, vacuum (imuroida) 5.36, wipe (pyyhkiä) 5.29, rinse (huuhdella) 5.21, brush (harjata) 5.00 Category Preparing food Playing sports Construction Cleaning up Appendix 3 (continued) dust (pyyhkiä pölyt) 4.79, sweep (lakaista) 4.64, air (tuulettaa) 4.71, clean the windows (pestä ikkunat) 4.43, wipe the floor (pyyhkiä lattia) 4.29, make the bed (pedata) 4.21, bring in carpets (tuoda matot sisään) 4.14, keep tidy (pitää järjestys) 4.07, wash the floor 4.00, scrub (hangata) 4.00 saw (sahata) 4.93, hammer (vasaroida) 4.79, paint (maalata) 4.64, drill (porata) 4.57, furnish (kalustaa) 4.43, seal (tiivistää) 4.36, level (tasoittaa) 4.29, screw (ruuvata) 4.21, plane (höylätä) 4.14, lay brick (muurata) 4.00 jump (hypätä) 4.93, jog (lenkkeillä) 4.86, do gymnastics (voimistella) 4.71, dance (tanssia) 4.50, play ice hockey (pelata jääkiekkoa) 4.29, stretch (venytellä) 4.21, dive (sukeltaa) 4.14, bend (taivuttaa) 4.07, lift weights (nostaa painoja) 4.00 cook (kypsentää) 4.79, freeze (pakastaa) 4.71, defrost (sulattaa) 4.59, slice (viipaloida) 4.50, chop (pilkkoa) 4.43, stir (hämmentää) 4.29, grill (grillata) 4.21, mash (muussata) 4.14, slice (suikaloida) 4.07 4-5 (intermediately frequent) 1-2 (rare) tear down (repiä pois) 2.93, fill the foundation (täyttää perustukset) 2.86, tamp (juntata) 2.79 glide (liitolentää) 2.64, rhythm skate (rytmiluistella) 2.00 beat carpets (tampata mattoja) 3.93, decorate (koristella) 2.93, hang (ripustaa) 3.79, wax (vahata) polish (kirkastaa) 2.50 3.64, beat (hakata) 3.57, disinfect (desinfioida) 3.43, mop (mopata) 3.36, scour (kuurata) 3.21, clean walls (puhdistaa seinät) 3.14, arrange the refrigerator (järjestää jääkaappi) 3.07, take care of cleaning equipment (huoltaa siivousvälineet) 3.00 wallpaper (tapetoida) 3.93, caulk (saumata) 3.86, split (halkaista) 3.79, carve (veistää) 3.64, plaster (rapata) 3.57, cast (valaa) 3.43, mix the concrete (sekoittaa betoni) 3.36, install rain gutters (asentaa sadekouru) 3.14 wrestle (painia) 3.93, play volleyball (pelata lentopalloa) 3.79, box (nyrkkeillä) 3.64, relay race (viestijuoksu) 3.57, paddle (meloa) 3.50, shot-put (työntää kuulaa) 3.43, throw hammer (heittää moukaria) 3.36, do the pole vault (hypätä seivästä) 3.29, bowl (keilailla) 3.21, ski jump (hypätä mäkeä) 3.14 fillet (fileerata) 3.93, allow the dough flame (flambeerata) 2.07 raise (kohottaa taikina) 3.86, grind (jauhaa) 3.79, strain (siivilöidä) 3.71, preserve (säilöä) 3.64, knead (alustaa) 3.57, juice (mehustaa) 3.29, frost (kuorruttaa) 3.21, 3.14, marinate (marinoida) 3.07 2-3 (not so frequent) 234 Appendix 3 Appendix 4 235 Appendix 4A. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group : clothes NC18 miAD43 moAD52 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: sukat/ kalsarit, aluspaita/ (aluspaita), päällyspaita, pusero, villapaita/ (villapaita), takki, ulsteri, turkki, pipo, myssy, kengät, saappaat/ tyyny, lakanat, täkki, peitto/ villapaita/ nenäliina/ rukkaset, hanskat, sormikkaat. puserot, hameet, leningit/ takit, puserot, takit, hatut, käsineet/ puserot, hameet/ takit, hatut, käsineet, huivit, kaulahuivit. kengät, sukat, housut/ hattu, myssy/ ääninauha/ jäljennys. English: English: English: socks long johns, under-shirt/ (under-shirt), shirt, blouse, wool sweater/ (wool sweater), coat, ulster, fur coat, knit hat, cap, shoes, boots/ pillow, sheets, quilt, cover/ wool sweater/ handkerchief/ mittens, gloves, gloves/ blouses, skirts, dresses/ coats, blouses, coats, hats, gloves/ blouses, skirts/ coats, hats, gloves, scarves, neckerchiefs. shoes, socks, pants/ hat, cap/ audio tape/ copy. Words total: 22 Words total: 15 Words total: 7 No of switches (/): 7 No of switches (/): 3 No of switches (/): 3 No of clusters: 5 No of clusters: 4 No of clusters: 2 Mean cluster size: 2 Mean cluster size: 2.3 Mean cluster size: 0.8 Words in clusters: 19/22 Words in clusters: 15/15 Words in clusters: 5/7 Semantic strategy: 4/5 Semantic strategy: 4/4 Semantic strategy: 2/2 Mixed strategy: 1/5 Mixed strategy: - Mixed strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Correct words: 15/22 Correct words: 8/15 Correct words: 5/7 Intrusions: 5/22 Semantically related intr. 5 Semantically unrelated intr. 0 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 2/7 Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: 2/22 Perseverations: 7/15 Perseverations: - Semantic subcategories: 5 Semantic subcategories: 2 Semantic subcategories: 2 Mean prototypic. of words: 4.80 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.72 Mean prototypic. of words: 4.92 Mean frequency of words: 4.57 Mean frequency of words: 4.85 Mean frequency of words: 5.01 Note. Words in parentheses belong to two overlapping clusters. 236 Appendix 4 Appendix 4B. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: vegetables NC5 miAD31 moAD63 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: porkkana, nauris, lanttu, peruna/ keräkaali, kukkakaali, brysselinkaali, kyssäkaali/ punajuuri/ sipuli, valkosipuli/ tomaatti/ herne. porkkana, punajuuri/ sipuli/ peruna, papaija, punajuuri/ viinimarja, vadelma. porkkana, lanttu, peruna, peruna/ kaali/ vehnä/ mansikatki istutettu. English: English: English: carrot, turnip, rutabaga, potato/ cabbage, cauliflower, Brussels sprouts, kohlrabi/ beetroot/ onion, garlic/ tomato/ pea. carrot, beetroot/ onion/ potato, papaya, beetroot/ currant, raspberry. carrot, rutabaga, potato, potato/ cabbage/ wheat/ strawberries also planted. Words total: 13 Words total: 8 Words total: 7 No of switches (/): 5 No of switches (/): 3 No of switches (/): 3 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 1 Mean cluster size: 1.0 Mean cluster size: 1 Mean cluster size: 0.8 Words in clusters: 10/13 Words in clusters: 7 Words in clusters: 4/7 Semantic strategy: 2/3 Semantic strategy: 1/3 Semantic strategy: 1/1 Mixed strategy: 1/3 Mixed strategy: 1/3 Mixed strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: 1/3 Phonemic strategy: - Correct words: 13/13 Correct words: 4/8 Correct words: 4/7 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 3/8 Semantically related intr. 3 Semantically unrelated intr. 0 Intrusions: 2/7 Semantically related intr. 2 Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: - Perseverations: 1/8 Perseverations: 1 Semantic subcategories: 3 Semantic subcategories: 2 Semantic subcategories: 1 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.73 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.61 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.71 Mean frequency of words: 4.42 Mean frequency of words: 5.32 Mean frequency of words: 4.98 Appendix 4 237 Appendix 4C. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: vehicles NC6 miAD37 moAD53 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: juna/ auto, polkupyörä/ lentokone, laiva/ (laiva), moottorivene, soutuvene/ (soutuvene), potkulauta/ juna/ kanootti, sukset, potkukelkka/ moottoripyörä/ raitiovaunu, metro. auto, linja-auto, henkilöauto, rekka-auto, autoja, linja-auto, leikkiauto, lasten leikkiauto, maitoauto. auto, pyörä/ lentokone, lentokone/ pyörä, polkupyörä. English: English: English: train/ car, bicycle/ plane, ship/ (ship), motor-boat, row-boat/ (rowing boat), kick-board/ train/ canoe, skis, kick-board/ motor-cycle/ tram, subway. car, bus, passenger car, trailer truck, cars, bus, toy car, childrenʼs toy car, milk van. car, bike/ plane, plane/ bike, bicycle. Words total: 15 Words total: 9 Words total: 6 No of switches (/): 8 No of switches (/): - No of switches (/): 2 No of clusters: 6 No of clusters: 1 No of clusters: 1 Mean cluster size: 0.9 Mean cluster size: 8.0 Mean cluster size: 0.3 Words in clusters: 12/15 Words in clusters: 9/9 Words in clusters: 2/6 Semantic strategy: 5/6 Semantic strategy: - Semantic strategy: 1/1 Mixed strategy: 1/6 Mixed strategy: 1/1 Mixed strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Correct words: 15/15 Correct words: 5/9 Correct words: 3/6 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 2/9 Semantically related intr. 2 Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: - Perseverations: 2 Perseverations: 3/6 Semantic subcategories: 5 Semantic subcategories: 1 Semantic subcategories: 1 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.94 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.49 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.70 Mean frequency of words: 4.72 Mean frequency of words: 5.61 Mean frequency of words: 5.79 Note. Words in parentheses belong to two overlapping clusters. 238 Appendix 4 Appendix 4D. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: animals NC12 miAD46 moAD68 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: hiiri, rotta/ kissa, koira, kani, kili, hevonen, lehmä, lammas/ jänis, kettu, susi, karhu/ leijona, seepra. karhu, kettu, susi, naali, jänis, orava/ hevonen, lammas, lehmä, vuohi/ sarvikuono, alligaattori, krokotiili. kissa, koira/ varis/ hiiri, rotta. English: English: English: mouse, rat/ cat, dog, rabbit, kid, horse, cow, sheep/ hare, fox, wolf, bear/ lion, zebra. bear, fox, wolf, arctic fox, hare, squirrel/ horse, sheep, cow, goat/ rhinoceros, alligator, crocodile. cat, dog/ crow/ mouse, rat. Words total: 15 Words total: 13 Words total: 5 No of switches (/): 3 No of switches (/): 2 No of switches (/): 2 No of clusters: 4 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 2 Mean cluster size: 2.8 Mean cluster size: 3.3 Mean cluster size: 0.7 Words in clusters: 15/15 Words in clusters: 13/13 Words in clusters: 4/5 Semantic strategy: 3/3 Semantic strategy: 1/3 Semantic strategy: 1/1 Mixed strategy: 1 Mixed strategy: 2/3 Mixed strategy: 1/1 Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Phonemic strategy: - Correct words: 15/15 Correct words: 13/13 Correct words: 5/5 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: - Perseverations: - Perseverations: - Semantic subcategories: 4 Semantic subcategories: 3 Semantic subcategories: 2 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.73 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.68 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.67 Mean frequency of words: 4.86 Mean frequency of words: 4.48 Mean frequency of words: 5.19 Appendix 4 239 Appendix 4E. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: preparing food NC15 miAD48 moAD60 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: paistaa, keittää, käristää, grillata/ maustaa, sekottaa, maistella/ kypsentää, höyryttää, kiehuttaa/ vispata/ saostaa, sulattaa/ keittämiset/ kuoria. keittää, paistaa, keittämistä ja paistamista/ syöminen siinä tarvitaan/ paistaa uunissa ja keittää. keittäminen, paistaminen/ perunoitten kuoriminen/ puuron teko, mämmin teko/ lettujen paistaminen/ makaroonilaatikkoo, makaroonilaatikkoo, lihapullia, kaalikääryleitä, kesäkeittoo, porkkanalaatikkoo, lanttulaatikkoo/ kinkun valmistusta. English: fry, cook, crisp, grill/ season, stir, taste/ cook, steam, boil/ whip up/ thicken, melt/ cookings/ peel. English: cook, fry, cooking and frying/ eating is needed/ bake in the oven and cook. English: cooking, frying/ peeling potatoes/ making porridge, making Easter pudding/ frying pancakes/ macaroni casserole, meat-balls, cabbage rolls, summer soup, carrot casserole, rutabaga casserole/ preparing ham. Words total: 15 Words total: 7 Words total: 14 No of switches (/): 6 No of switches (/): 2 No of switches (/): 5 No of clusters: 4 No of clusters: 2 No of clusters: 3 Mean cluster size: 1.3 Mean cluster size: 1.3 Mean cluster size: 1.2 Words in clusters: 12/15 Words in clusters: 6/7 Words in clusters: 11/14 Correct words: 14/15 Correct words: 4/7 Correct words: 5/14 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 6/14 Semantically related intr. 6 Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: 1/15 Perseverations: 3/7 Perseverations: 3/14 Semantic subcategories: 3 Semantic subcategories: 1 Semantic subcategories: 2 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.71 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.57 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.84 Mean frequency of words: 4.44 Mean frequency of words: 5.81 Mean frequency of words: 4.86 240 Appendix 4 Appendix 4F. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: playing sports NC19 miAD37 moAD55 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: juosta, kävellä, hypätä/ keilailla, pelata pesäpalloa/ uida, purjehtia, soutaa/ nyrkkeillä, painia/ hiihtää, mäkihypätä, syöksylaskea/ autourheilu. juosta/ työntää kuulaa/ hypätä korkeutta/ uida, sukeltaa, sukeltaa/ potkukelkkailla, luistella/ sukeltaa, syvyyssukellus. juosta/ kylpeä/ potkia, hyppiä. English: English: run, walk, jump/ bowl, play baseball/ swim, sail, row/ box, wrestle/ ski, ski jump, downhill skiing/ motorsports. run/ shot put/ high jump/ swim, dive, dive/ kick-sled, skate/ dive, deep-water diving. English: run/ bath/ kick, jump. Words total: 14 Words total: 10 Words total: 4 No of switches (/): 5 No of switches (/): 5 No of switches (/): 2 No of clusters: 5 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 1 Mean cluster size: 2.3 Mean cluster size: 0.7 Mean cluster size: 0.3 Words in clusters: 13/14 Words in clusters: 7/10 Words in clusters: 2/4 Correct words: 14/14 Correct words: 8/10 Correct words: 3/4 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 1/4 Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. 1 Perseverations: - Perseverations: 2/10 Perseverations: - Semantic subcategories: 5 Semantic subcategories: 2 Semantic subcategories: 1 Appendix 4 241 Appendix 4G. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: construction NC3 MiAD41 moAD56 Finnish: Finnish: perustan kaivaminen, valu/ seinän pystytys, kattaminen/ maalaaminen, laatottaminen, tapetoiminen/ kalustaminen. Finnish: höylää, hakkaa, höylää, hakkaa/ nostaa niitä tavaroita, nostaa, nostaa/ nauloja tietysti hakataan sinne, höylääminen. English: English: English: digging the foundation, pour/ putting up the walls, to roof/ painting, tiling, wallpapering/ furnishing. plane, hit, plane, hit/ lift those things, lift, lift/ nails of course are hit there, planing down. boards, nails, a hammer, a saw, an axe are needed/ hi---/ [he or she] paints. Words total: 8 Words total: 9 Words total: 6 No of switches (/): 3 No of switches (/): 2 No of switches (/): 2 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 2 No of clusters: 1 Mean cluster size: 1.0 Mean cluster size: 1.3 Mean cluster size: 2.0 Words in clusters: 7/8 Words in clusters: 6/8 Words in clusters: 5/6 Correct words: 8/8 Correct words: 3/9 Correct words: 1/6 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 5/6 Semantically related intr. 5 Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: - Perseverations: 6/9 Perseverations: - Semantic subcategories: 3 Semantic subcategories: 1 Semantic subcategories: 1 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.10 Mean prototypic. of words: 4.19 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.00 Mean frequency of words: 3.88 Mean frequency of words: 3.98 Mean frequency of words: 4.22 siellä tarvitaan lautoja, nauloja, vasaraa, sahaa, kirvestä/ hak---/ maalaa. 242 Appendix 4 Appendix 4H. Examples of the semantic fluency performance given by a participant in each subject group: cleaning up NC26 MiAD39 moAD65 Finnish: Finnish: Finnish: pestä, imuroida, laasta, luututa/ tomuttaa, tampata/ pyyhkiä. lakaista, luututa/ pölyjä pyyhkiä/ mattoja tampata/ pestä, huuhdella/ soittaminen/ verhoja vois laittaa vaikka ja nuita koristeita tuonne seinälle tai no kuuluuko se nyt sitte, tauluja voi laittaa. ämpäri, pesuvati/ pestä, pestä/ lakaista/ pe- niin pesu tuli jo/ lakaista/ kuurata/ siivota se on puhdistukseen/ ikkunan pesua, pestä ikkunoita. English: English: English: wash, vacuum, sweep, wash the floor/ dust, beat/ wipe. sweep, wash the floor/ dust/ beat the carpets/ wash, rinse/ playing/ curtains could be put up and the decorations on the walls or does it belong now there, pictures could be put. bucket, basin/ wash, wash/ sweep/ wa- yes washing came already/ sweep/ scrub/ to clean up it is to the cleaning/ washing windows, wash windows. Words total: 7 Words total: 10 Words total: 11 No of switches (/): 2 No of switches (/): 5 No of switches (/): 7 No of clusters: 2 No of clusters: 3 No of clusters: 1 Mean cluster size: 0.8 Mean cluster size: 0.7 Mean cluster size: 0.1 Words in clusters: 6/7 Words in clusters: 7/10 Words in clusters: 2/11 Correct words: 7/7 Correct words: 6/10 Correct words: 5/11 Intrusions: Semantically related intr. Semantically unrelated intr. - Intrusions: 4/10 Semantically related intr. 3 Semantically unrelated intr. 1 Intrusions: 2/11 Semantically related intr. 2 Semantically unrelated intr. - Perseverations: - Perseverations: - Perseverations: 4/11 Semantic subcategories: 2 Semantic subcategories: 2 Semantic subcategoires: 1 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.56 Mean prototypic. of words: 6.25 Mean prototypic. of words: 5.97 Mean frequency of words: 4.56 Mean frequency of words: 4.75 Mean frequency of words: 4.60 2b. Differences in the number of correct words between different categories 2a. Number of correct nouns U = 70.0, p < .001 U = 30.0, p < .001 U = 134.5, p < .001 U = 122.5, p < .001 U = 112.0, p < .001 Vehicles Animals All categories U = 48.0, p < .001 U = 8.0, p < .001 U = 78.0, p < .001 U = 75.0, p < .001 Animals All categories |39.6 – 59.4|= 19.8 (*) Appendix continues n = 20, k = 4: n = 20, k = 4: (*) = p ≈ .05 (*) = p ≈ .05 21.3 < w4 < 21.59, p < .05 = * 21.3 < w4 < 21.59, p < .05 = * |31.6 – 66.6| = 35* n = 30, k = 4: w4 ≥ 26.09, p < .05 = * Vegetables – Animals |42.6 – 111.9| = 69.3* |58.0 – 59.4| = 1.4, n.s. |39.6 – 43.0| = 3.4, n.s. |64.4 – 66.6|= 2.2, n.s. |31.6 – 37.6| = 6, n.s. |84.9 – 111.9| = 27.0* Clothes – Animals |58.0 – 39.6| = 18.4 (*) |58.0 – 43.0| = 15.0, n.s. |64.4 – 31.6| = 32.8* Friedman’ s post hoc pair-wise comparisons moAD group U = 60.5, p < .001 U = 68.5, p < .001 U = 90.5, p < .001 U = 100.0, p < .001 U = 68.5, p < .001 U = 85.5, p < .002 U = 93.0, p < .004 U = 114.5, p < .020 U = 127.0, p < .047 U = 79.0, p < .001 Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD |64.4 – 37.6| = 26.8* Vegetables – Vehicles |42.6 – 60.6| = 18.0, n.s. |84.9 – 42.6| = 42.3* |84.9 – 60.6| = 24.3, n.s. Friedman’ s post hoc pair-wise comparisons Friedman’ s post hoc pair-wise comparisons Clothes – Vegetables miAD group U = 8.5, p < .001 NC group Clothes – Vehicles Paired categories U = 12.0, p < .001 U = 88.0, p < .001 Vehicles U = 10.0, p < .001 U = 158.0, p < .01 U = 99.5, p < .001 Clothes Vegetables U = 28.0, p < .001 U = 22.5, p < .001 U = 40.0, p < .001 U = 179.0, p < .016 U = 137.0, p < .001 Clothes 1. Total number of words NC vs. moAD Mann-Whitney U test NC vs. miAD Mann-Whitney U test Vegetables Category Variable Appendix 5. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the noun fluency tasks Appendix 5 243 7. Proportion of correct nouns 6. Proportion of words in clusters 5. Cluster size 4. Number of clusters U = 124.5, p < .001 All categories U = 163.0, p < .004 U = 171.0, p < .007 U = 152.0, p < .003 Vehicles Animals All categories U = 184.0, p < .015 U = 231.0, p = .160, n.s. Clothes Vegetables U = 139.0, p < .001 All categories U = 155.0, p < .004 U = 204.5, p < .05 Clothes All categories U = 104.0, p < .001 All categories U = 209.5, p = .073, n.s U = 145.0, p < .002 Animals Clothes U = 177.0, p < .013 U = 89.5, p < .001 Vegetables Vehicles U = 202.0, p < .049 U = 178.0, p < .014 Animals Clothes U = 182.0, p < .018 U = 123.5, p < .001 Vegetables Vehicles U = 59.0, p < .001 U = 133.0, p<.001 U = 118.5, p<.001 U = 155.0, p < .004 U = 119.5, p < .001 U = 159.0, p < .005 U = 166.0, p < .007 U = 200.5, p < .049 U = 171.5, p < .01 U = 11.5, p < .001 U = 48.0, p < .001 U = 33.5, p < .001 U = 64.5, p < .001 U = 54.0, p < .001 U = 47.0, p < .001 U = 61.0, p < .001 U = 92.5, p < .001 U = 70.0, p < .001 U = 91.5, p < .001 Mann-Whitney U test U = 235.0, p = .192, n.s. Mann-Whitney U test Clothes 3. Number of switches NC vs. moAD NC vs. miAD Category Variable Appendix 5 (continued) U = 105.0, p < .009 U = 139.0, p = .102, n.s. U = 157.0, p = .253, n.s. U = 161.0, p = .301, n.s. U = 137.5, p = .091, n.s. U = 169.5, p = .409, n.s. U = 164.5, p = .333, n.s. U = 183.5, p = .659, n.s. U = 154.5, p = .218, n.s. U = 70.0, p < .001 U = 102.0, p < .007 U = 108.0, p < .010 U = 118.0, p < .021 U = 73.5, p < .001 U = 75.0, p < .001 U = 106.5, p < .010 U = 116.0, p < .022 U = 101.5, p < .006 U = 82.5, p < .001 Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD Appendix continues 244 Appendix 5 Note. n.s. = nonsignificant U = 174.0, p < .013 U = 180.0, p < .017 U = 204.5, p < .059, n.s. Vehicles Animals All categories U = 183.0, p < .020 U = 285.0, p = .766, n.s. Vehicles Vegetables U = 150.0, p < .002 Animals 12. Degree of prototypicality of nouns U = 158.0, p < .004 U = 113.0, p < .001 Vegetables Vehicles U = 83.0, p < .001 U = 197.0, p < .037 All categories Clothes U = 255.5, p = .754, n.s. Animals U = 103.0, p <.001 U = 259.5, p =.821, n.s All categories Clothes 13. Degree of frequency of nouns 11. Number of different semantic subcategories 10. Strategies: semantic strategy U = 180.0, p < .009 U = 181.0, p < .012 Vehicles Animals U = 224.0, p = .097, n.s. Vegetables U = 277.0, p = .543, n.s. U = 133.0, p < .001 Clothes U = 156.5, p < .004 U = 205.0, p = .060, n.s. U = 122.5, p < .001 U = 155.5, p < 004 U = 174.5, p < .013 U = 41.5, p < .001 U = 42.5, p < .001 U = 78.0, p < .001 U = 67.5, p < .001 U = 8.0, p < .001 U = 126.0, p < .016 U = 118.5, p < .008 U = 71.0, p <.001 U = 133.0, p < .001 U = 136.0, p < .001 U = 157.5, p < .002 U = 232.0, p = .072, n.s. U = 192.5, p < .015 Mann-Whitney U test Mann-Whitney U test Clothes NC vs. moAD NC vs. miAD 8. Proportion of intrusions Category 9. Proportion of perseverations Variable Appendix 5 (continued) U = 169.0, p = .402, n.s. U = 189.0, p = .766, n.s. U = 157.0, p = .253, n.s. U = 138.5, p = .096, n.s. U = 168.0, p = .398, n.s. U = 82.0, p < .001 U = 116.0, p < .023 U = 121.0, p < .024 U = 94.5, p < .003 U = 75.5, p < .001 U = 82.0, p < .050 U = 83.5, p < .045 U = 125.5, p <.044 U = 139.0, p = .093, n.s. U = 151.0, p = .175, n.s. U = 144.5, p = .121, n.s. U = 144.5, p = .108, n.s. U = 119.0, p < .028 Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD Appendix 5 245 2b. Differences in the number of correct words between different categories 2a. Number of correct verbs U = 148.5, p < .003 Playing sports U = 128.5, p < .001 | 96.6 – 78.6 | = 18.0, n.s. | 78.6 – 53.1| = 25.5 (*) Playing sports – Construction Construction – Cleaning n = 30, k = 4: (*) = p ≈ .05 w4 = 26.09, p < .05 = * | 72.0 – 78.6 | = 6.6, n.s. | 72.0 – 53.1| = 18.9, n.s. Preparing food – Construction Preparing food – Cleaning Preparing food – Playing sports | 72.0 – 96.6 | = 24.6 (*) Friedman’ s post hoc pair-wise comparisons NC group All categories Paired categories U = 132.5, p < .001 U = 228.0, p = .149, n.s. Construction Cleaning U = 108.5, p < .001 Playing sports U = 163.5, p < .007 U = 172.0, p < .011 All categories Preparing food U = 142.5, p < .002 U = 275.5, p = .625, n.s. Construction Cleaning Mann-Whitney U test U = 17.5, p < .001 U = 62.5, p < .001 U = 51.5, p < .001 U = 36.4, p < .001 U = 18.0, p < .001 U = 43.5, p < .001 U = 62.5, p < .001 U = 68.5, p < .001 U = 50.5, p < .001 U = 98.5, p < .001 Mann-Whitney U test U = 239.5, p = .228, n.s. Preparing food 1. Number of total words NC vs. moAD NC vs. miAD Category Variable Appendix continues U = 84.0, p < .001 U = 78.5, p < .001 U = 115.0, p < .021 U = 86.0, p < .002 U = 78.0, p < .001 U = 96.5, p < .005 U = 83.5, p < .002 U = 120.5, p < .030 U = 89.5, p < .003 U = 109.5, p < .014 Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD Appendix 6. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the verb fluency tasks 246 Appendix 6 U = 288.0, p = .774, n.s. U = 140.0, p < .001 U = 185.5, p < .018 U = 149.0, p < .003 Playing sports Construction All categories U = 151.5, p < .002 Preparing food Concrete nouns 8. Proportion of correct verbs U = 246.5, p = .217, n.s. General verbs U = 298.5, p = .976, n.s. U = 260.0, p = .427, n.s. Preparing food Specific verbs 6. Proportion of words in clusters U = 294.0, p =.905, n.s. U = 282.0, p =.721, n.s Preparing food U =162.0, p < .006 All categories Cleaning U = 192.0, p < .028 U = 249.0, p = .288, n.s. Construction Cleaning U = 215.5; p = .078, n.s. U = 180.5, p < .015 Preparing food Playing sports U = 133.0, p < .001 All categories 7. Word forms 5. Cluster size 4. Number of clusters U = 102.0, p < .001 U = 294.5, p = .912, n.s. Construction Cleaning U = 216.0, p = .091, n.s. U = 128.5, p < .001 Preparing food 3. Number of switches U = 106.5, p < .001 U = 177.0, p < .037 U = 144.5, p < .002 U = 105.5, p < .001 U = 200.0, p < .027 U = 175.0, p < .006 U = 180.0, p < .017 U = 165.5, p < .008 U = 171.0, p <.010 U = 154.0, p <.004 U = 29.0, p < .001 U = 127.0, p < .001 U = 86.5, p < .001 U = 93.0, p < .001 U = 49.5, p < .001 U = 57.5, p < .001 U = 158.5, p < .004 U = 58.5, p < .001 U = 46.0, p < .001 U = 132.5, p < .001 NC vs. moAD Mann-Whitney U test NC vs. miAD Mann-Whitney U test Playing sports Category Variable Appendix 6 (continued) U = 178.5, p < .065, n.s. U = 170.5, p = .784, n.s. U = 160.0, p = .411, n.s. U = 121.5, p = .087, n.s. U = 129.0, p < .033 U = 148.0, p = .137, n.s. U = 148.0, p = .165 U = 126.0, p < .044 U = 143.0, p =.121, n.s. U = 122.0, p <.032 U = 80.5, p < .001 U = 117.5, p < .018 U = 116.5, p < .018 U = 124.5, p < .033 U = 75.0, p < .001 U = 104.5, p < .01 U = 111.5, p < .015 U = 116.5, p < .020 U = 118.0, p < .024 U = 130.0, p < .050 Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD Appendix continues Appendix 6 247 Note. n.s. = nonsignificant 12. Degree of frequency of verbs 11. Number of different semantic subcategories 10. Proportion of perseverations U = 228.0, p = .276, n.s U = 155.5, p < .002 U = 109.0, p < .001 Construction All categories U = 232.0, p = .178, n.s. U = 236.0, p = .180, n.s. U = 151.0, p < .003 Cleaning All categories All categories U = 130.0, p < .001 U = 173.0, p < .009 U = 180.0, p < .014 Playing sports Construction U = 224.5, p = .135, n.s. U = 38.0, p < .001 U = 117.5, p < .001 U = 97., p < .001 U = 207.0, p < .05 Preparing food U = 52.5, p < .001 U = 144.5, p < .002 U = 245.5, p = .356, n.s. U = 211.0, p = .065, n.s. U = 170.0, p < .005 U = 146.0, p < .006 U =160.5, p < .004 Preparing food U = 244.5, p = .236, n.s. All categories U = 165.0, p < .001 U =160.5, p < .001 Playing sports U = 225.0, p < .004 U = 223.0, p < .011 Preparing food 9. Proportion of intrusions NC vs. moAD Mann-Whitney U test NC vs. miAD Mann-Whitney U test Playing sports Category Variable Appendix 6 (continued) U = 116.5, p < .024 U = 92.0, p < .003 U = 117.0, p < .017 U = 125.5, p < .034 U = 142.0, p = .097, n.s. U = 75.5, p < .001 U = 186.5, p = .715, n.s. U = 128.5, p = .114, n.s. U = 144.5, p = .178, n.s. U = 148.5, p = .349, n.s. U = 125.5, p < .043 U = 152.5, p = .426, n.s. U = 139.0, p = .102, n.s. Mann-Whitney U test moAD vs. miAD 248 Appendix 6 Appendix 7 249 Appendix 7. Results of the post-hoc pair-wise analyses of the control tasks Test NC vs. miAD NC vs. moAD miAD vs. moAD Mann-Whitney U test Mann-Whitney U test Mann-Whitney U test Boston Naming Test U = 82.5, p < .001 U = 27.0, p < .001 U = 111.0, p < .015 Naming, nouns U = 240.5, p = .132, n.s. U = 134.5, p < .001 U = 118.0, p < .026 Naming, verbs U = 137.0, p < .001 U = 31.0, p < .001 U = 75.0, p < .001 Serial naming, nouns U = 189.0, p < .001 U = 110.5, p < .001 U = 128.5, p < .052 Serial naming, verbs U = 163.0, p < .001 U = 17.0, p < .001 U = 76.5, p < .001 Digit span forwards U = 164.0, p < .005 U = 58.5, p < .001 U = 118.0, p < .026 Token test U = 106.5, p < .001 U = 25.0, p < .001 U = 68.0, p < .001 Category recognition, nouns U = 300.0, p = 1.0 U = 225.0, p < .014 U = 150.0, p = .183, n.s. Category recognition, verbs U = 270.0, p = .08 U = 195.0, p < .002 U = 147.5, p = .157, n.s. Verbal tests Non-verbal tests In-category recognition, nouns U = 288.0, p = .613, n.s. U = 165.0, p < .001 U = 125.0, p < .043 In-category recognition, verbs U = 253.5, p = .244, n.s. U = 12.5, p < .001 U = 57.0, p < .001 Card sorting, nouns U = 161.5, p < .001 U = 38.5, p < .001 U = 77.5, p < .001 Card sorting, verbs U = 98.0, p < .001 U = 2.0, p < .001 U = 64.0, p < .001 Note. n.s. = nonsignificant