A Tale of Two Motorsports: A Graphical
Transcription
A Tale of Two Motorsports: A Graphical
PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 1 A Tale of Two Motorsports: A Graphical-‐Statistical Analysis of How Practice, Qualifying, and Past Success Relate to Finish Position in NASCAR and Formula One Racing* Kathleen M. Silva and Francisco J. Silva Department of Psychology, University of Redlands, Redlands, California 92373-0999 USA ABSTRACT We analyzed data from the 2009 National Association for Stock Car Auto Racing (NASCAR) and Formula One (F1) racing seasons to examine the relationships between drivers’ finish positions and their performances during the final practice, qualifying, and past races. NASCAR drivers’ performances in practice, qualifying, and past races were usually positively correlated with their finish positions. In F1 racing, only drivers’ performances during qualifying and, to a lesser extent, in past races were positively correlated with their finish positions. In contrast, there was rarely a significant correlation between the finish positions of the best NASCAR drivers and their performances in practice, qualifying, or previous races. We discuss the results in the context of the demands and characteristics of NASCAR and F1 racing. Keywords: correlation, Formula One, motorsports, NASCAR, practice, qualifying, regression, Sprint Cup In terms of their environments or physical features (e.g., cars, racetracks), National Association for Stock Car Auto Racing (NASCAR) and Formula One (F1) racing are very different forms of automotive racing (Blount, 2009; Collantine, 2006; Hook, 2010; “NASCAR’s Jeff Gordon on Formula One racing,” 2004). For example, the premier NASCAR series, currently known as the Sprint Cup Series, consisted of 36 races in 2009.1 Of these races, all were run at tracks in the United States and all but two of the races were on run on speedways shaped like ovals or distorted ovals (e.g., rounded triangles or D’s). In contrast, the F1 Grand Prix series consisted of 17 races in 2009, each of which was run in a different country and all of which consisted of irregularly shaped circuits and road courses. Table 1 presents a summary of some key physical differences between NASCAR and F1 racing. * © 2010 Kathleen M. Silva and Francisco J. Silva. All right reserved. Cite as: Silva, K. M., & Silva, F. J. (2010). A tale of two motorsports: A graphical-statistical analysis of how practice, qualifying, and past success relate to finish position in NASCAR and Formula One racing. Retrieved from http://newton.uor.edu/FacultyFolder/Silva/NASCARvF1.pdf Address correspondence to K. Silva or F. Silva, Department of Psychology, University of Redlands, Redlands, California 92373-0999 USA (email: kathleen_silva@redlands.edu or francisco_silva@redlands.edu). 1 Throughout the manuscript, any reference to NASCAR will mean, more specifically, a reference to NASCAR’s Sprint Cup Series and never to its other series (e.g., Nationwide Series, Camping World Truck Series, K&N Pro Series). Although Sprint Cup Series is more accurate, the name of NASCAR’s premier racing series changes as a function of corporate sponsorship. The Sprint Cup Series was previously known as the Nextel Cup Series and, before that, the Winston Cup Series. Thus, although less precise than Sprint Cup Series, it is commonplace to refer to this racing series simply as NASCAR (e.g., NASCAR on Fox and NASCAR TNT Summer Series, both of which refer to television programs that cover of Sprint Cup races). PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 2 Table 1 Physical Variables: Similarities and Differences Between NASCAR and F1 Racing in 2009 Characteristic Number of Drivers NASCAR 43 Motorsport F1 20 Number of Crew Members During Pit Stops 7 20 Number of Races 36 17 Design of Cars front-engine, “stock" car, heavy (1500 kg) mid-engine, open-wheel, light (600 kg) Technological Sophistication of Cars basic; relatively simple engineering sophisticated; advanced engineering Racing Tracks and Circuits oval-shaped speedways circuits and road courses Width of Tracks and Circuits relatively wide, side-by-side racing is common relatively narrow, side-by-side racing is rare Length of Tracks and Circuits relatively short (0.85 km to 4.18 km) relatively long (3.34 km to 7.00 km) Location of Races 23 locations in USA 17 countries in Asia, Australia, Europe, South America Turning all left turns 34 of 36 races left and right turning Overtaking and Lead Changes relatively common relatively rare Qualifying 1 session consisting of 1 or 2 laps 3 sessions consisting of several (e.g., 7, 12) laps in each Final Practice occurs after qualifying occurs before qualifying Ability to race in wet weather cannot race in rain under any circumstances can race in rain with tires designed for this purpose Time Limit none 2 hours Despite these differences, there are noteworthy similarities between the two motorsports. For example, both NASCAR and F1 award points commensurate with when a driver finishes a race to determine who is the best driver, overall, at the end of the racing season. Also, both NASCAR and F1 set a driver’s starting position for a race based on his2 time during qualifying. The drivers’ qualifying times are ranked such that there is normally a perfect and positive 2 All drivers during all of the races analyzed in the present article were men. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 3 correlation between a driver’s rank during qualifying and his starting position (e.g., fastest in qualifying = 1st or pole position to start a race; slowest in qualifying = last position on the starting grid).3 Finally, both NASCAR and F1 racing allow drivers to practice their driving, and teams to set up their cars for the conditions of the speedway or circuit. But, even here, the two motorsports differ: NASCAR’s final practice session normally occurs the day after qualifying; F1 racing’s final practice occurs before qualifying. In this article, we examine whether there are similarities and differences between NASCAR and F1 racing in terms of performance features – i.e., those factors tied to the drivers’ and their teams’ performance rather than the physical features of the two motorsports (e.g. see Table 1). We examined these performance features by answering six questions. (1) What is the relationship between a driver’s performance during the final practice before a race and his finish position? (2) What is the relationship between a driver’s performance during qualifying (and thus his position at the start of a race) and his finish position? (3) What is the relationship between a driver’s points-standing before a race (i.e., a measure of his overall success) and his finish position? (4) How do the relationships in (1) through (3) change across races? (5) What is a more reliable predictor of a driver’s finish position: His performance during practice, his performance during qualifying, his overall success prior to a race, or a combination of these variables? (6) What factors are related to the degree that a driver’s finish position is predictable from his performance in practice, qualifying, and his success up to that point in the season? To answer these questions, we analyzed the results of the 2009 NASCAR and F1 racing seasons using a combination of visual inspection of graphed data (see, e.g., Kazdin, 1982) and inferential statistics (primarily correlation and regression). Published research that investigated NASCAR drivers’ performance is rare. Most of the studies that do exist take the form of working papers available through the authors’ personal archives. However, two exploratory studies of whether reliable predictors exist for the outcomes of NASCAR races hint at answers to some of the preceding six questions. One project examined the results from 14 races from the 2003 NASCAR season to see whether the outcome of a race was predicted from a variety of performance variables, such as how drivers performed in the practice session closest to the start of the race, the drivers’ performance during qualifying, the drivers’ overall success leading up to a race, the number of laps a driver completed in all races during the previous season, how often a driver did not finish a race, the drivers’ finish positions at the same race the year before, and the drivers’ finish positions in the preceding race. The results showed that drivers’ performance during practice and qualifying, as well as their overall success leading up to a particular race and the number of laps they completed during the previous season, were correlated with their finish position (Pfitzner & Rishel, 2005). A similar project that used the results of 38 races in the 2002 NASCAR season found that the drivers’ 3 Although there is normally a perfect and positive correlation between the rank of a driver’s qualifying time and his starting position, there are occasions when this is not the case. For example, a driver may produce the fastest time during qualifying (and thus be ranked first following qualifying) and yet start a race at the rear of the field for any of several reasons (e.g., changing an engine after qualifying; having his qualifying time disallowed because of a rules violation). These sorts of events, though, are uncommon. Also, in NASCAR, qualifying is sometimes cancelled because of rain. When this happens, the starting positions for a race are determined by so-called “car owner points,” which are usually but not always the same as a driver’s points-standing. Nevertheless, for most intents and purposes (including those of the present article), there is a perfect and positive correlation between a driver’s rank following qualifying and his starting position. During the 2009 NASCAR season, qualifying was cancelled at the following races: Martinsville, Pocono, New Hampshire, *Daytona, *Pocono. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 4 performances during qualifying (and thus their starting positions) and their number of years of NASCAR driving experience were important predictors of their finish positions (Allender, 2008). Published studies that investigated the performance of F1 drivers are non-existent. The present study attempts to fill a void by providing the first comprehensive, quantitative, and comparative analysis of the performance of NASCAR and F1 drivers across an entire racing season. Our ultimate goal is to create a table (comparable to Table 1), based on the answers to the six questions posed above, which highlights the similarities and differences between NASCAR and F1 drivers in relation to key performance variables of these motorsports. Method The data that we analyzed were downloaded from the official websites of NASCAR and F1 racing: nascar.com and formulaone.com. For each points-awarding race in 2009,4 we obtained each driver’s rank during the final practice session before a race, each driver’s rank in qualifying (and, thus, his starting position), each driver’s points-standings (i.e., his ranking based on the number of points he accrued before the start of a race, which is a measure of his overall success up to that race), and each driver’s finish position.5 For short, we refer to these variables as Practice, Qualifying, Points, and Finish. In NASCAR, the final practice session normally occurs after qualifying. In F1 racing, the final practice session normally occurs before qualifying. Thus, for NASCAR, the final practice is the last time a driver is on the speedway before the start of the race; for F1 racing, qualifying is the last time a driver spends any substantial amount of time on the racing circuit before the start of a grand prix.6 The primary results were obtained by calculating Spearman rank correlation coefficients (known as Spearman’s rs) between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish for each NASCAR and F1 race. We chose to calculate Spearman’s rs instead of Pearson’s product moment correlation coefficients or r for three main reasons. One, we calculated a total of 159 correlation coefficients (36 for NASCAR + 17 for F1 x 3 relationships, explained below). Rather than worry about whether the characteristics of any set if data violated one or more assumptions necessary to calculate Pearson’s r, we opted for the assumption-free Spearman’s rs. 4 All of the 36 NASCAR and 17 F1 races analyzed in the present article award drivers points commensurate with their performance in a race. These points are awarded exclusively on the basis of a driver’s finish position in F1 racing and primarily on the basis of a driver’s finish position in NASCAR. Other factors that contribute to a NASCAR driver’s points include leading the most laps in a race and leading a lap during a race. Races that did not contribute to the drivers’ points total (e.g., all-star race) were excluded from the analyses. 5 The one exception was the first race for each of the NASCAR and F1 seasons. Because all drivers start the first race of the season with 0 points, we used the drivers’ final points-standing from the previous season. For 2009 rookie drivers who did not race in 2008, we assigned a value of 0 points to their points-total before the first race of the 2009 season and ranked them accordingly. 6 In NASCAR, there are two notable exceptions to this schedule. Because NASCAR cars cannot race in wet weather (drizzle, showers, rain), final practice is sometimes cancelled because of precipitation. In these circumstances, the final practice occurs before qualifying. Also, 3 of the 36 NASCAR races held in 2009 were socalled impound races. At both Talladega races and the second Daytona race, NASCAR officials impound cars following qualifying. This means that drivers cannot drive their cars and teams cannot work on their cars between the end of qualifying and the start of a race. At impound races, the final practice also occurs before qualifying. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 5 Two, the number of data points used in some analyses was relatively small (n = 20). Pearson correlation coefficients can be misleading when sample sizes are small (Aliaga & Gunderson, 2006). Third, the ranked Practice, Qualifying, and Finish data seemed ideally suited to a Spearman analysis, which converts data to ranks before rs is calculated. We used α < .05 to determine the statistical significance of all two-tailed analyses. To determine how combinations of Practice, Qualifying, and Points correlated with Finish, we tested the following four models: (1) Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying (2) Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Points (3) Finish = α + β1Qualifying + β2Points (4) Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying + β3Points For each analysis, we were interested only in (a) the multiple correlation coefficient or R, which expresses the correlation between Finish and a combination of the predictor variables (Practice, Qualifying, Points) in the equation, and (b) the accompanying F statistic and p value, which provide information about the degree that the combination of variables in the model is significantly correlated with Finish. Given the number of races for which we tested the models listed above,7 “by the book” evaluations of each model for each race – which normally consists of specifying the model, identifying multicollinearity, disentangling partial correlations, removing nonsignificant variables, specifying a new model, evaluating it, and so on (Keith, 2006; Younger, 1985) – were not reasonable. Instead, we relied on visual inspection of graphed data to form generalizations about the four models and their relation to NASCAR and F1 racing. We will explain more completely in the Results section how we evaluated the models by graphical inspection, and how we used chi-square tests of independence to provide corroborating support for our evaluations. Results Correlations: NASCAR Figure 1 shows how Finish was correlated with Practice, Qualifying and Points across the 2009 NASCAR season. Values in the grey area are not statistically significant; values in the white area are statistically significant. An asterisk before the location of the race means it was the second time during the season that a race was held at that location. For all but one race, the data from 43 drivers were used to calculate each rs. Appendix A shows Spearman’s rs values and their associated two-tailed p values used to construct Figure 1. An analysis of NASCAR drivers’ ranking in final practice and their finish positions throughout the 2009 season revealed statistically meaningful correlations between their practice 7 Data from 36 NASCAR and 17 F1 races were analyzed. However, as explained in the Results section, we also analyzed the data of the top 20 points-leaders leading up to each NASCAR race. Thus, two sets of data were analyzed for each NASCAR race: one set that included every driver in the race (n = 43) and one set that included only the top 20 points-leaders before the race (n = 20). In total, 89 sets of data were analyzed: 36 NASCAR races, 17 F1 grands prix, and 36 NASCAR races for the top 20 points-leaders. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 6 performance and their finish positions in 29 of the 36 (81%) races. The better someone performed in practice, the better his finish position. For 27 of the 36 (75%) races, the drivers’ performances in the final practice and in qualifying were significantly correlated with their finish positions in a race. For most races, the correlations between Practice and Finish and between Qualifying and Finish were similar. However, there were some notable exceptions. At Bristol and Kansas, the correlations between Practice and Finish were statistically significant, but the correlations between Qualifying and Finish were not. The reverse was true in the second races held at Daytona and Atlanta. During these races, the correlations between Practice and Finish were not statistically significant, but those between Qualifying and Finish were significant. Finally, Talladega is notable for the fact that it was the only speedway that hosted two races for which the correlations between Practice and Finish and between Qualifying and Finish were not statistically significant in either race. Figure 1. A graph of the correlations (rs) between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish for each race in the 2009 NASCAR season. An asterisk before the name of a race means it was the second time during the season that a race was held at that location. Forty-three drivers started all races except for the first race at Pocono, where 42 drivers started the race. Thus, df = 41 for most races and df = 40 for one race. In general, the values in the grey area are not statistically significant and the values in the white area are statistically significant. However, because of limitations in the graphing software, the plot symbols of some marginally significant/insignificant values are not entirely within the grey or white areas. To see precisely which Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were statistically significant, see Appendix A. Figure 1 also shows that there were statistically significant correlations between the drivers’ finish positions and their points-standings before a race in 31 of the 36 races (86%). Drivers ranked higher in the standings tended to finish a race before drivers ranked lower in the standings. Although this relationship was not evident during the first three races of the season, it occurred reliably thereafter except at both races held at Talladega. Moreover, if we compare the correlations between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points, we see that the PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 7 correlations between Points and Finish are generally greater than those between Practice and Finish and those between Qualifying and Finish. To get a visualization of the raw data used to calculate the correlations shown in Figure 1, Figure 2 shows scatter plots of the strongest and weakest associations in Figure 1. The top graph illustrates the strong linear relationship between Points and Finish, which occurred in the second race held at Richmond. The bottom graph illustrates the almost-zero correlation between Practice and Finish from the first race held at Talladega. Figure 2. Scatter plots showing the strongest (top graph) and weakest (bottom graph) associations among the correlations in Figure 1. The top graph illustrates the relationship between Points and Finish for the second race held at Richmond [rs(41) = .809]. The bottom graph illustrates the relationship between Practice and Finish for the first race held at Talladega [rs(41) = .013]. Note: Although 45 drivers participated in the final practice at Talladega (which occurs before qualifying at this race), only 43 drivers qualified for the race. Correlations: F1 Figure 3 shows how Finish was correlated with Practice, Qualifying, and Points across the 2009 F1 racing season. Values in the grey area are not statistically significant; values in the white area are statistically significant. For most grands prix, the data from 20 drivers were used to calculate PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 8 each rs. Appendix B shows Spearman’s rs values and their associated two-tailed p values used to construct Figure 3. Figure 3. A graph of the correlations (rs) between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish for each grand prix in the 2009 F1 season. Twenty drivers started each grand prix except Hungary (18 drivers) and Japan (19 drivers), thus df = 18 for all grands prix except Hungary (df = 16) and Japan (df = 17). Details are the same as for Figure 1. To see precisely which Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were statistically significant, see Appendix B. In general, and in contrast to the NASCAR drivers, only the correlations between F1 drivers’ qualifying positions and their finish positions and between the F1 drivers’ pointsstanding and their finish positions were statistically significant. Statistically significant correlations between Practice and Finish were rare, occurring in only 7 of the 17 races (41%). Statistically significant correlations between Qualifying and Finish were observed in 14 of the 17 (82%) races, and between Points and Finish in 10 of the 17 races (59%). Also, unlike the NASCAR drivers, the correlations between Practice and Finish and between Qualifying and Finish were often dissimilar. Finally, with the exception of an upward trend in the correlations between Practice and Finish that started with the grand prix held in China and ended with the grand prix held in Great Britain, there were no strong trends in the correlations between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points across the racing season. To get a visualization of the raw data used to calculate the correlations shown in Figure 3, Figure 4 shows scatter plots of the strongest and weakest associations in Figure 3. The top graph illustrates the strong relationship between Qualifying and Finish from the Japanese Grand Prix. The bottom graph illustrates the near-zero correlation between Qualifying and Finish from the Brazilian Grand Prix. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 9 Figure 4. Scatter plots showing the strongest (top graph) and weakest (bottom graph) associations among the correlations in Figure 3. The top graph illustrates the relationship between Qualifying and Finish for the grand prix held in Japan [rs(17) = .849]. The bottom graph illustrates the relationship between Qualifying and Finish for the grand prix held in Brazil [rs(18) = .007]. Correlations: NASCAR’s Top 20 Points-Leaders Because correlation coefficients are affected by sample size, the differences in the results shown in Figures 1 and 3 may have occurred because Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were calculated across different numbers of drivers (43 NASCAR drivers vs. 20 F1 drivers). If we calculated correlation coefficients for only 20 NASCAR drivers, the results of the analyses of the NASCAR and F1 drivers may have been more alike. To evaluate this possibility, we calculated PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 10 Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish for the top 20 points-leading drivers before each NASCAR race.8 Figure 5 shows how Finish was correlated with Practice, Qualifying, and Points across the 2009 NASCAR season for the top 20 points-leaders before each race. Most striking is that there were few statistically significant correlations between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points. This contrasts with the F1 drivers’ data for which there were statistically Figure 5. A graph of the correlations (rs) between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish for the top 20 points-leaders before each race in the 2009 NASCAR season. For all races, df = 18. Details are the same as for Figure 1. To see precisely which Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were statistically significant, see Appendix C. significant correlations between Practice and Finish, between Qualifying and Finish, and between Points and Finish in 41%, 82%, 59% of the grands prix, respectively. For the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR, statistically significant correlations between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish occurred in only 8 of 36 (22%), 10 of 36 (28%), 7 8 It could be argued that taking a random sample of NASCAR drivers, rather than taking a sample of the best NASCAR drivers, is the best way to assess whether the differences between the results of the NASCAR and F1 drivers were because of the difference in the sample sizes. Although this may be true, we chose to use the Top 20 NASCAR drivers instead of a random sample of them because using the best NASCAR drivers may actually be a better way to assess the role of sample sizes in the observed differences between the 43 NASCAR drivers and the 20 F1 drivers. Why? Because the 20 F1 drivers are not a random sample of F1 drivers; they are the best F1 drivers. A comparable sample of NASCAR drivers consists of the 20 best NASCAR drivers, not a random sample of them. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 11 of 36 (19%) races, respectively. Appendix C shows Spearman’s rs values and their associated two-tailed p values used to construct Figure 5. To get a visualization of the raw data used to calculate the correlations shown in Figure 5, Figure 6 shows scatter plots of the strongest and weakest associations in Figure 5. The top graph illustrates the strong relationship between Qualifying and Finish, which occurred in the second race held at Richmond. The bottom graph illustrates the almost-zero correlation between Points and Finish from the first race held at Charlotte. Figure 6. Scatter plots showing the strongest (top graph) and weakest (bottom graph) associations among the correlations in Figure 5. The top graph illustrates the relationship between Qualifying and Finish for the second race held at Richmond [rs(18) = .699]. The bottom graph illustrates the relationship between Points and Finish for the first race held at Charlotte [rs(18) = .001]. Regression: NASCAR Figure 7 shows the values of the multiple correlation coefficient for each of the four models listed in the Method section plotted across the races of the 2009 NASCAR season. For the models with the combinations Practice and Qualifying, Practice and Points, and Qualifying and Points, the R values plotted in the white area are statistically significant and those in the grey area are not. For the model with the combination Practice, Qualifying, and Points, only values PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 12 above the dashed horizontal line are statistically significant. Appendix D shows the R values and the associated F statistics and p values used to construct Figure 7. Figure 7. A graph of the multiple correlation coefficient (R) for each race in the 2009 NASCAR season. The R values plotted in the white area are statistically significant and those in the grey area are not, except for the model with the combination Practice, Qualifying, and Points. For this model, only values above the dashed horizontal line are statistically significant. However, because of limitations in the graphing software, the plot symbols of some marginally significant/insignificant values are not entirely within the grey or white areas. To see precisely which R values were statistically significant, see Appendix D. The degrees of freedom for the models with two predictor variables were (2, 40) for all races except for the second Pocono race (df = 2, 39). For the model with three predictor variables, the degrees of freedom were (3, 39) for all races except for the second Pocono race (df = 3, 38). There are five noteworthy features in the graph. One, most R values were statistically significant. Two, the R values were similar across races. Three, the two races at Talladega produced the lowest R values. Four, the R values for the model with only Practice and Qualifying were often smaller than the R values of the other three models. Five, there was considerable overlap among the R values for the models with Practice and Points, Qualifying and Points, and Practice, Qualifying, and Points. Given that there is a lot of overlap among these three models and that the common variable to all three models is Points, we can conclude that a model with Points as the only predictor variable is as good as any model with it plus Practice and/or Qualifying. In other words, the best predictor of a driver’s finish is his success up to that point in the racing season, and additional information about how he performed in practice and qualifying does little to help predict his finish position in most races. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 13 Regression: F1 Figure 8 shows the R values for each of the four models listed in the Method section plotted across the grands prix of the 2009 F1 season. Appendix E shows the R values and the associated F statistics and p values used to construct Figure 8. Similar to the NASCAR drivers, there was relatively little change in the R values across grands prix. In contrast to the NASCAR drivers’ data, the model with Practice and Points generated lower R values than the other models. Also, there was considerable overlap between the model with Practice, Qualifying, and Points and the models with Practice and Qualifying and with Qualifying and Points. Given that there was a lot of overlap among these three models and that the common variable to all three models is Qualifying, we can conclude that a model with Qualifying as the only predictor variable is as good as any model with it plus Practice and/or Points. In other words, the best predictor of a driver’s finish is his performance during qualifying, and additional information about his success in the season and how he performed in practice does little to help predict his finish position in most grands prix. Figure 8. A graph of the multiple correlation coefficient (R) for each grand prix in the 2009 F1 racing season. The R values plotted in the white area are statistically significant and those in the grey area are not, except for the model with the combination Practice, Qualifying, and Points. For this model, only values above the dashed horizontal line are statistically significant. However, because of limitations in the graphing software, the plot symbols of some marginally significant/insignificant values are not entirely within the grey or white areas. To see precisely which R values were statistically significant, see Appendix E. The degrees of freedom for the models with two predictor variables were (2, 17) for all grands prix except Hungary (df = 2, 15) and Japan (df = 2, 16). For the model with three predictor variables, the degrees of freedom were (3, 16) for all grands prix except Hungary (df = 3, 14) and Japan (df = 3, 15). PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 14 Regression: NASCAR’s Top 20 Points-Leaders Figure 9 shows the R values for each of the four models listed in the Method section plotted across the 2009 NASCAR season for the top 20 points-leaders before each race. Appendix F shows the R values and the associated F statistics and p values used to construct Figure 9. Except for two clusters late in the season (*Michigan, *Bristol, *Atlanta in one cluster and *Talladega, *Texas, *Phoenix in the other cluster) where the R values were near or below .20, there was no trend in the R values across races. Also, in contrast to the data of the F1 drivers and the 43 drivers who comprise the broader NASCAR field, few of the R values from any of the four models reached statistical significance for the top 20 NASCAR points-leaders. Also in contrast to the data of the F1 drivers and the broader NASCAR field, there was no consistent overlap between any of the models across races. Given that none of the four models resulted in R values that were consistently statistically significant and that there was no consistent overlap between any of the models, we can conclude that no combinations of Practice, Qualifying, and/or Points can predict Finish for the top 20 points-leaders in most races. Figure 9. A graph of the multiple correlation coefficient (R) for the top 20 points-leaders before each race in the 2009 NASCAR season. The R values plotted in the white area are statistically significant and those in the grey area are not, except for the model with the combination Practice, Qualifying, and Points. For this model, only values above the dashed horizontal line are statistically significant. However, because of limitations in the graphing software, the plot symbols of some marginally significant/insignificant values are not entirely within the grey or white areas. To see precisely which R values were statistically significant, see Appendix F. The degrees of freedom for the models with two predictor variables were (2, 17) for all races. For the model with three predictor variables, the degrees of freedom were (3, 16) for all races. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 15 Contingency Table Analyses For many reasons, regression can be a complicated procedure (see Keith, 2006; Younger, 1985). Our brute-force approach that relied on visual analysis of the graphed R values is one way to simplify the testing of regression models. Another approach to evaluating the relative importance of Practice, Qualifying, and Points to predicting Finish is to use a chi-square test of independence. This approach involves ranking the magnitude of the correlations in each of the pairs of variables “Practice and Finish,” “Qualifying and Finish,” and “Points and Finish” for each race, and then tallying how often the largest correlation was associated with each pair of variables, how often the median correlation was associated with each pair of variables, and how often the smallest correlation was associated with each pair of variables. This produces a 3 x 3 contingency table with three rows for the pairs of variables (“Practice and Finish,” “Qualifying and Finish,” and “Points and Finish”) and three columns for the rank of a correlation (largest, median, smallest). The results of the contingency table analyses are shown in Table 2. For the 43 drivers who normally start a NASCAR race (top panel), the largest correlations occurred most often between “Points and Finish” (28 of the 36 races). The correlations between “Practice and Finish” and between “Qualifying and Finish” were the median and smallest correlations in 15 to 17 of the 36 races. A chi-square test of independence confirmed that the pairs of variables and the correlation-ranks were not independent [χ2(4) = 48.50, p < .001] and that the largest correlations occurred most often in “Points and Finish” [χ2(2) = 32.17, p < .001]. Table 2 Three Panels Showing 3 x 3 Contingency Tables Drivers Variable Correlated With Finish Rank of rs NASCAR Practice Qualifying Points 1st 5 3 28 2nd 16 17 3 3rd 15 16 5 F1 Practice Qualifying Points 4 11 2 3 4 10 10 2 5 NASCAR (Top 20) Practice Qualifying Points 12 13 11 7 15 14 17 7 12 For the F1 drivers (middle panel), the largest correlations occurred most often between “Qualifying and Finish” (11 of the 17 grands prix). The correlations between “Practice and Finish” and between “Points and Finish” were the median and smallest correlations in 10 of the 17 races. A chi-square test of independence confirmed that the pairs of variables and the correlation-ranks were not independent [χ2(4) = 18.71, p < .001] and that the largest correlations occurred most often in “Qualifying and Finish” [χ2(2) = 7.88, p = .01]. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 16 For the top 20 points-leaders before each NASCAR race (bottom panel), the results showed that, in contrast to the broader NASCAR field and the F1 drivers, there was no relationship between categories and the correlation-ranks [χ2(4) = 7.33, p = .12]. Thus, the 3 x 3 contingency table analyses corroborated our visual analyses of the graphed R values from the four models listed in the Method section. For the 43 NASCAR drivers who started a race, the best predictor of a driver’s finish was his overall success up to that point in the racing season (i.e., his points-standing). For the F1 drivers, the best predictor of a driver’s finish was his performance during qualifying. For the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR, none of the variables (Practice, Qualifying, Points) were useful predictors of a driver’s finish position. How Lead Changes and Cautions Relate to Predictability of Finish The analyses above quantified the correlation between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points. Because correlation and prediction are closely related (the latter dependent on the former), we could say that the values of Spearman’s rs measured the degree that a driver’s finish position was predictable from his performance during practice, his performance during qualifying, and his overall success leading up a race. What we attempt to address next is, what factors are related to the predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, and Points? We focused on two factors that occur during a race: lead changes and cautions. To answer the question, we correlated the values of Spearman’s rs from the analyses above with how often lead changes and cautions occurred in a race. In this context, we refer to the values of Spearman’s rs as Predictability of Finish. Because many races had the same number of lead changes or cautions, we calculated the relationship between Predictability of Finish and each of Lead Changes and Cautions using Pearson’s product moment correlation coefficient (r). This measure of the association between variables works better than Spearman’s rs when many values in the data are the same. Spearman’s rs, but not Pearson’s r, requires that all raw data be converted to ranks, something that can be problematic if there are many values in the data that are the same. NASCAR. The mean number of lead changes in a 2009 NASCAR race was 20 (SD = 11.46) and the mean number of cautions was 8.47 (SD = 3.40). Table 3 shows the correlations between Lead Changes and Predictability of Finish from each of the variables Practice, Qualifying, and Points, as well as between Cautions and Predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, and Points. The results showed that there were statistically significant inverse (negative) correlations between Lead Changes and Predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, and Points. More intuitively, these results show that the more lead changes that occurred during a race, the more difficult it was to predict a driver’s finish position from his performance during practice, his performance during qualifying, or his points-standing. In short, an increase in lead changes meant a decrease in the Predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, and Points. There were no statistically significant relationships between Cautions and Predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, or Points.9 9 The correlation between the number of cautions and the number of lead changes was not statistically significant (r = .03, p < .88). PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 17 Table 3 Correlations (r) Between Lead Changes and Predictability of Finish and Cautions and Predictability of Finish Drivers Variable Predictability of Finish from Practice Qualifying Points NASCAR Lead Changes Cautions -0.636* -0.218 -0.356* -0.110 -0.553* -0.061 F1 Lead Changes Cautions -0.129 n/a 0.277 n/a -0.261 n/a NASCAR (Top 20) Lead Changes Cautions -0.433* 0.091 -0.313 0.119 -0.412* 0.108 Note. For both groups of NASCAR drivers, n = 36; for F1 drivers, n = 17. * p < .05. F1. The mean number of lead changes in an F1 grand prix was 4.53 (SD = 2.45), significantly fewer than the 20 that typically occurred in a NASCAR race [t(51) = 5.48, p < .001]. In contrast to NASCAR races, there were no statistically significant correlations between Lead Changes and Predictability of Finish from Practice, Qualifying, or Points in F1 racing. (Information about the number of cautions in F1 grands prix was not available from formulaone.com, and thus no analyses involving Cautions were possible.)10 NASCAR Top 20 Points-Leaders. The results for the top 20 points-leaders before each NASCAR race were similar to the results of the broader 43-driver field. However, there was one exception: The correlation between Lead Changes and Predictability of Finish from Qualifying approached but did not reach statistical significance (p = .06). Thus, for the top 20 pointsleaders, an increase in lead changes meant a decrease in the Predictability of Finish from Practice and Points. Discussion We used the results of the 2009 NASCAR and F1 seasons to answer six questions related to drivers’ performances in these two motorsports, which differ considerably in terms of their physical features. In what follows, we restate the questions and discuss the answers to each. What is the relationship between a driver’s performance during the final practice before a race and his finish position? 10 Also, yellow- and red-flag cautions in F1 racing are different from those in NASCAR. In NASCAR, all yellowand red-flag cautions apply to the entire speedway. In F1 racing, a red-flag caution also applies to the entire race circuit, but a yellow-flag caution may apply to only a particular section of the circuit (termed a “local caution”) or it may apply to the whole course (termed a “full-course caution”). PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 18 For the 43 drivers who comprise the starting field of a NASCAR race there was a statistically significant relationship between their performances during the final practice and their finish positions in most races (see also Pfitzner & Rishel, 2005). For these statistically significant correlations, the rs values were generally between .4 and .6. This was not the case for the 20 drivers who comprise the starting field of an F1 grand prix. For these drivers, there was no reliable relationship between their performances during final practice and their finish positions; the rs values for most grands prix were between .2 and .4. If we limit our analysis to the top 20 drivers in the NASCAR points-standings, there was no reliable relationship between a driver’s performance in final practice and his finish position. Moreover, in contrast to the F1 drivers, there was more variability in the rs values from race to race. In sum, a NASCAR driver’s performance during final practice is related to his finish position if the entire 43-driver field is entered into the analysis; the better a driver performed in practice, the better he performed in the race. This relationship was not evident for F1 drivers or for the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR. One possible reason for the difference between the standard 43-driver NASCAR field and the F1 drivers relates to when the final practice session occurs. In NASCAR, the final practice session normally occurs after qualifying. In F1 racing, the final practice session occurs before qualifying. These two motorsports thus differ in terms of how contiguous the final practice session is with the start of the race. In NASCAR, the purpose of the final practice is to set up the car for the conditions expected during a race. In F1 racing, the purpose is to set up a car for optimal performance during qualifying conditions, which may not necessarily be the conditions during the race. These differences between the two motorsports reflect what is known about contingencies of reinforcement and the allocation of behavior: People and other animals generally respond to satisfy the requirements of the more immediate consequence (Pear, 2001). Alternatively, the similarity of the results for the top 20 NASCAR points-leaders and the F1 drivers suggests that performance in final practice, regardless of when it occurs, may not relate much to the race-day performance of elite drivers. Perhaps it is that the best NASCAR drivers and their teams are excellent at adapting during a race, that their improvisational skills are better than those of the 23 drivers who comprise the rest of the starting field. In relation to their finish position, how a top 20 points-leading NASCAR driver and his team adapt during a race may be more important than what they do during practice. Or, perhaps what they do during practice is not so much about producing the fastest laps during practice (a short-term payoff) but about testing configurations and strategies that may help them produce consistently fast laps across a variety of race conditions (a long-term payoff). As a result, there were few statistically significant correlations between these drivers’ performance during final practice and their finish positions. Whether this explanation or the difference in the timing of the final practice sessions (or both) best explains the differences among the broader NASCAR field, the best NASCAR driver, and the F1 drivers, we cannot say. What is the relationship between a driver’s performance during qualifying (and thus his position at the start of a race) and his finish position? A driver’s starting position is normally determined by his performance during qualifying. The better a driver performs in qualifying, the closer to the front of the field he will start a race. For the 43 drivers who comprise the starting field of a NASCAR race, there was a statistically significant relationship between their performances in qualifying and their finish positions in PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 19 most races (see also Allender, 2008; Pfitzner & Rishel, 2005). For these statistically significant correlations, the rs values were generally between .4 and .6. Similar results were noted for the F1 drivers, except that the typical rs values were greater (between .6 and .8). This was not the case for the top 20 drivers in the NASCAR points-standings. For these drivers, there was no reliable relationship between a driver’s performance in qualifying and his finish position. Why this relationship was absent for these drivers is unclear. Perhaps here, too, how a top 20 pointsleading driver and his team adapt during a race may be more important to their finish positions than where these drivers start a race. Alternatively, for the best drivers in NASCAR, perhaps it is that individual drivers and their teams are better suited to particular speedways or race conditions (Grace, Reeves, & Fitzgerald, 2003), and it is this fit between driver and environment that is more related to a driver’s finish position than his performance in qualifying. If the difference in the importance of qualifying between NASCAR and F1 racing reflects differences in the demands of these two motorsports, then we might expect the importance of qualifying to be more similar in the two types of racing when the demands of each motorsport are more similar. Such is the case at NASCAR’s Sonoma and Watkins Glen races, both of which are run at road courses similar to the circuits used in F1 racing. The results, though, were inconsistent with this expectation. For the top 20 points-leading NASCAR drivers, the correlations between their qualifying performances and their finish positions were not statistically significant at either Sonoma or Watkins Glen. What is the relationship between the number of points a driver has accrued in a season (i.e., his overall success) and his finish position? For the 43-driver NASCAR field, the more points a driver had accrued before a race, the better his finish position (see also Pfitzner & Rishel, 2005). This relationship got stronger as the season progressed with the correlation between Points and Finish approaching rs = .8 for several races in the last quarter of the season. In contrast, the relationship between Points and Finish was not as reliable for F1 drivers, and there was rarely a statistically significant correlation between these variables for the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR. In relation to NASCAR racing, the different results for the 43-driver field versus the top 20 points-leaders suggests again some sort of fundamental differences between the best drivers and the rest of the field. We speculated above that one of these differences might be the drivers’ and their teams’ abilities to make adjustments throughout the race, perhaps similar to a team who makes adjustments at halftime or alters their behavior as a function of variations that occur during a game (e.g., the other team’s strategy, injuries, weather conditions). That the results of the top 20 points-leading drivers in NASCAR were similar to those of the F1 drivers – all of whom could be considered elite in the sense that the starting field consists of only 20 drivers – adds some support to this speculation. How do the three relationships above (i.e., between Practice and Finish, Qualifying and Finish, and Points and Finish) change across races? In general, there were no trends across the racing seasons in the correlations between drivers’ finish positions and their performances in final practice and qualifying, as well as how successful they were in previous races. This was true for NASCAR and F1 racing. The one exception was PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 20 that the correlations between NASCAR drivers’ finish positions and their points-standing before a race became somewhat larger as the NASCAR season progressed. It was interesting to note that the weakest correlations in NASCAR occurred during the two races at Talladega. During both of these races, NASCAR requires that all cars use restrictor plates that limit the amount of horsepower the engines can produce. The net effect of using a restrictor plate is a reduction in the maximum speed that a car can attain, which then results in a clustering of the cars as they travel around the speedway. Drivers have lamented and columnists have written about the unpredictability of Talladega races (Caraviello, 2009; McGee, 2009; Snider, 2009), which may be partly the result of a situation where a driver’s particular skills are less important. That is, somewhat independent of what a driver does, his car will not go much faster or slower than any other car. Although races at Daytona also require the use of restrictor plates, only the first Daytona race was unpredictable. This result may have as much to do with the use of restrictor plates as it does with Daytona being the first race of the season and its atypical qualifying sessions (known in 2009 as the Gatorade Duels). In F1 racing, the presence of wet weather allows us to evaluate the influence of an uncommon environmental condition on the drivers’ finish positions. If rain is disruptive, we should observe noticeably lower correlations between Practice and Finish and between Qualifying and Finish, and perhaps even Points and Finish. The opportunity to evaluate the influence of rain occurred during the grands prix held in Malaysia and China. At the Malaysian Grand Prix, the practice and qualifying sessions were conducted in dry weather, but the grand prix took place in heavy rain. Despite the different conditions in practice and qualifying versus the grand prix, the correlations between the drivers’ finish positions and their performances in practice, qualifying, and the previous grand prix were all statistically significant. The results were different for the grand prix in China, where it also rained during the grand prix but not during practice or qualifying. At this grand prix, only the correlation between Qualifying and Finish was statistically significant. Although different from the result in Malaysia, the results from the Chinese Grand Prix are consistent with the other grands prix in the 2009 season. It seems that, overall, the relationship between F1 drivers’ performance in practice, qualifying, and previous races and their finish positions is about the same regardless of whether there were very different conditions in the practice and qualifying sessions versus the grand prix. For any particular race, a driver’s performance in practice, qualifying, or previous races, may be better predictors of his finish position. For example, some racing analysts argue that qualifying is key at NASCAR’s Dover races because qualifying well gives a driver both a good starting position and a good pit selection (McReynolds, 2007). This analysis was not supported by the results from the two Dover races in 2009. For the first race at Dover, the correlations between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points were rs = .58, .49, and .49, respectively. Practice was a better predictor of Finish than either Qualifying or Points. For the second race at Dover, the correlations between Finish and each of Practice, Qualifying, and Points were rs = .49, .59, and .74, respectively. For this race, Points was a better predictor than either Practice or Qualifying. If we restrict our analysis to the top 20 points-leaders, the only statistically significant correlation occurred between Points and Finish (rs = .56) in the second race at Dover. Similarly, some NASCAR drivers contend that the final practice before a race is critically important to success (“NASCAR Sprint Cup Series: News and Notes – Lowe’s Motor Speedway,” 2009). But here, too, the results from 2009 only partially support this contention. Although Practice was significantly correlated with Finish in many races, the regression PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 21 analyses and contingency table analyses showed that Practice was not as important as Points for predicting a NASCAR driver’s success. Among the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR, the prevailing result was the unpredictability of the drivers’ finishing positions. What is a more reliable predictor of a driver’s finish position: His performance during practice, his performance during qualifying, his overall success prior to a race, or a combination of these variables? Overall, two different analyses – the visual inspection of the R values from the four regression models evaluated and the contingency table analyses – showed that the best predictor of a NASCAR driver’s finish position was his points-standing. For F1 drivers, the best predictor of their finish position was their performance during qualifying and thus their position at the start of a grand prix. For the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR, there was no reliable predictor across races held in 2009. One reason for this difference between NASCAR and F1 racing is probably related to differences in overtaking (i.e., passing another driver) in these two motorsports. Overtaking in NASCAR is a common occurrence; overtaking in F1 racing is rare. For this reason, lead changes in NASCAR are more prevalent than lead changes in F1 racing. The relative difficulty of overtaking in F1 means that a driver’s starting position is likely to have an important influence on a driver’s finish position (“Hungarian Grand Prix,” 2009). Given the importance of starting position in F1 racing, it should not surprise us that the most reliable correlation was between a driver’s performance in qualifying and his finish position. The lack of reliable predictors for the finish positions of NASCAR’s top 20 points-leaders going into a race is puzzling in light of the reliable predictors noted for the broader 43-driver field that starts most NASCAR races and the 20-driver field that starts most F1 grands prix. We speculated above that predicting the outcome of a NASCAR race based on the top 20 points-leaders’ performance in practice, qualifying, or past races is difficult because what these elite drivers and their teams do during a race may be what distinguishes the best drivers and teams at any given race. What might tip the scale towards a better finish position for a top 20 points-leading NASCAR driver and his team during a race? Any number of variables may be involved: faster pit stops, better mechanical adjustments to the car, more effective pit strategies (e.g., changing two vs. four tires), more effective communication between a driver and his crew chief, the superiority of a driver’s physical fitness in a demanding race, the superiority of a driver’s ability to concentrate in different circumstances, a better fit between the demands of race and a driver’s skills, a greater willingness on the part of a driver to incur risks to produce faster laps times or overtake other cars, and the like. What factors are related to the predictability of a driver’s finish position from his performance in practice, qualifying, and his success up to that point in the season? For the 43 drivers who normally start a NASCAR race, the results showed that an increase in lead changes, but not cautions, was correlated with an increase in the difficulty of predicting a driver’s finish position from his performance during practice, qualifying, and previous races. This result was similar for the top 20 points-leaders in NASCAR. However, for F1 drivers, there was no relationship between lead changes and the predictability of the finish positions from PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 22 drivers’ performances during practice, qualifying, or how they had performed up to that point in the racing season. Intuitively, this result makes sense. By definition, an increase in lead changes means that drivers in the lead are having a difficult time maintaining their position. As variability (of position) increases, predictability (of finish) decreases. This effect was absent in F1 racing because there are many fewer lead changes than in NASCAR. That cautions were unrelated to the predictability of finish position from practice, qualifying, or past success is puzzling. Cautions lead to restarts in which all of the cars are lined up close together. One might think that restarts would cause lead changes because the gaps (distances) between cars are minimized during a restart. Given that there are as many restarts as there are cautions, one might expect a correlation between the number of cautions and the number of lead changes, and yet no such relationship occurred in the 2009 NASCAR season. Conclusions We began this paper with a summary of some key physical differences between NASCAR and F1 racing (see Table 1). We can now create a new table of performance differences based on the results of the analyses of the 2009 races. As shown in Table 4, the most significant difference between these two forms of racing is that the finish positions in NASCAR are related to more variables than the finish positions in F1 racing. NASCAR drivers’ performances in practice, qualifying, and past races were usually positively correlated with their finish positions. In F1 racing, only drivers’ performances during qualifying and, to a lesser extent, in past races were positively correlated with their finish positions. Surprising (at least to us), there was rarely a significant correlation between the finish positions of the best NASCAR drivers and their performances in practice, qualifying, or previous races. Predicting these drivers’ finish positions from any combination of their performances in practice, qualifying, or previous races was usually unsuccessful. It seems that in NASCAR, the best drivers predictably win a race, but who among these drivers will win is not predictable from how they performed in practice, qualifying, or in previous races. It remains for future research to identify and study variables associated with these elite drivers’ finish positions and to identify what makes their performances different from those of drivers in F1 racing. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 23 Table 4 Performance Variables: Similarities and Differences Between NASCAR and F1 in 2009 Characteristic Drivers NASCAR F1 NASCAR (Top 20) Finish position generally correlated with practice performance Yes, 81% of races No, 41% of grands prix No, 22% of races Finish position generally correlated with qualifying performance/starting position Yes, 75% of races Yes, 82% of grands prix No, 28% of races Finish position generally correlated with overall success in season Yes, 86% of races Somewhat, 59% of grands prix No, 19% of races Best overall predictor(s) of finish position Points-standing before a race Qualifying performance (starting position) None of the performance variables studied Relationship between lead changes and predictability of finish position ↑ lead changes, ↓ predictability None based on performance variables studied ↑ lead changes, ↓ predictability based on practice performance and overall success in season Relationship between cautions and predictability of finish position None based on performance variables studied (Data not available) None based on performance variables studied Changes in correlations across season Slight increase in correlation between Finish and Points Transitory increase in correlation between Finish and Practice None based on performance variables studied References Aliaga, M., & Gunderson, B. (2006). Interactive statistics (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Allender, M. (2008). Predicting the outcome of NASCAR races: The role of driver experience. Journal of Business & Economic Research, 6, 79-84. Blount, T. (2009. March 31). F1, Cup made for wildly different day. ESPN.com. Retrieved from cvchttp://sports.espn.go.com/rpm/nascar/cup/columns/story?columnist=blount_terry&id=4029638 Caraviello, D. (2009, November 4). Like an unwelcome journey into NASCAR’s difficult past. NASCAR.com. Retrieved from http://www.nascar.com/2009/news/opinion/11/04/inside.line.dcaraviello.talladega.past/index.html Collantine, K. (2006, July 22). F1 vs NASCAR [Web log post]. Retrieved from http://www.f1fanatic.co.uk/2006/07/22/f1-vs-nascar Graves, T., Reese, C. S., & Fitzgerald, M. (2003). Hierarchical models for permutations: Analysis of auto racing results. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 98, 282-291. Hook, N. (2010, June 12). NASCAR vs. Formula One. Examiner.com. Retrieved from http://www.examiner.com/x-4026-Minneapolis-Autos-Examiner~y2009m6d12-NASCAR-vs-Formula-One Hungarian Grand Prix – Team and Driver Preview Quotes (2009, July 21). Formula1.com. Retrieved from http://www.formula1.com/news/headlines/2009/7/9631.html Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press. Keith, T. Z. (2006). Multiple regression and beyond. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 24 McGee, R. (2009, October 30). A Talladega twist of fate. ESPN.com. Retrieved from http://insider.espn.go.com/espn/blog/index?entryID=4608684&name=mcgee_ryan&action=login&appRedi rect=http%3a%2f%2finsider.espn.go.com%2fespn%2fblog%2findex%3fentryID%3d4608684%26name%3 dmcgee_ryan McReynolds, L. (2007, September 21). Mac Track: Past can’t predict future success. msn.foxsports.com. Retrieved from http://foxsports.foxnews.com/nascar/story/7243656/Mac-Track:-Past-can't-predict-future-success NASCAR’s Jeff Gordon on Formula One Racing (2004, June 17). Formula1.com. Retrieved from http://www.formula1.com/news/features/2004/6/1745.html NASCAR Sprint Cup Series: News and Notes – Lowe’s Motor Speedway (2009, October 14). Retrieved from http://www.autoracingdaily.com/news/nascar-sprint-cup-series/nascar-sprint-cup-series-news-notes-lowesmotor-speedway1 Pear, J. J. (2001). The science of learning. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Pfitzner, B., & Rishel, T. (2005). Do reliable predictors exist for the outcomes of NASCAR races? The Sport Journal, 8(2). Retrieved from http://www.thesportjournal.org/article/do-reliable-predictors-existoutcomes-nascar-races Snider, M. (2009, October 30). Chasers have a lot on their plate as Talladega calls. NASCAR.com. Retrieved from http://www.nascar.com/2009/news/features/10/30/chase.tiering.talladega/index.html Younger, M. S. (1985). A first course in linear regression (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Duxbury Press. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 25 Appendix A Spearman rs (and their p values) used to construct Figure 1 Race Variables Finish and Practice Spearman's rs Daytona Fontana Las Vegas Atlanta Bristol Martinsville Texas Phoenix Talladega Richmond Darlington Charlotte Dover Pocono Michigan Sonoma New Hampshire *Daytona Chicago Indianapolis *Pocono Watkins Glen *Michigan *Bristol *Atlanta *Richmond *New Hampshire *Dover Kansas *Fontana *Charlotte *Martinsville *Talladega *Texas *Phoenix Homestead 0.119 0.557 0.328 0.499 0.469 0.560 0.402 0.627 -0.013 0.402 0.164 0.613 0.580 0.498 0.640 0.426 0.450 0.236 0.614 0.588 0.711 0.516 0.349 0.431 0.105 0.460 0.596 0.485 0.471 0.443 0.510 0.443 0.127 0.168 0.614 0.589 p 0.447 0.000 0.031 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.007 0.000 0.931 0.007 0.292 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.004 0.002 0.126 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.021 0.003 0.504 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.002 0.000 0.020 0.414 0.280 0.000 0.000 Finish and Qualifying Spearman's rs -0.085 0.489 0.270 0.392 0.187 0.645 0.549 0.529 0.084 0.320 0.280 0.460 0.493 0.688 0.450 0.476 0.607 0.557 0.456 0.549 0.783 0.514 0.322 0.300 0.398 0.591 0.460 0.592 0.191 0.397 0.568 0.467 0.215 0.259 0.594 0.353 p 0.587 0.000 0.080 0.009 0.231 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.590 0.036 0.069 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.034 0.050 0.008 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.220 0.008 0.000 0.001 0.165 0.092 0.000 0.020 Note. The degrees of freedom for all races was 41, except for Pocono (df= 40) The * denotes that drivers had raced at that track earlier in the season. Finish and Points Spearman's rs 0.263 0.299 0.131 0.585 0.488 0.645 0.595 0.622 -0.137 0.597 0.407 0.423 0.488 0.692 0.693 0.475 0.586 0.577 0.722 0.633 0.783 0.558 0.477 0.602 0.503 0.809 0.756 0.794 0.665 0.630 0.452 0.801 0.221 0.528 0.754 0.651 p 0.087 0.051 0.401 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.381 0.000 0.006 0.004 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.152 0.000 0.000 0.000 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 26 Appendix B Spearman rs (and their p values) used to construct Figure 3 Grand Prix Variables Finish and Practice Spearman's rs Australia Malaysia China Bahrain Spain Monaco Turkey Great Britain Germany Hungary Europe Belgium Italy Singapore Japan Brazil Abu Dhabi 0.300 0.427 -0.260 0.130 0.251 0.515 0.523 0.654 0.320 0.496 0.281 0.067 0.529 0.505 0.349 -0.515 0.425 p 0.197 0.060 0.267 0.582 0.285 0.019 0.017 0.001 0.168 0.030 0.229 0.776 0.016 0.023 0.142 0.019 0.061 Finish and Qualifying Spearman's rs 0.087 0.630 0.514 0.739 0.804 0.476 0.730 0.830 0.452 0.370 0.720 0.621 0.524 0.715 0.849 -0.007 0.628 p 0.714 0.002 0.020 0.000 0.000 0.030 0.000 0.000 0.045 0.118 0.000 0.003 0.017 0.000 0.000 0.974 0.002 Finish and Points Spearman's rs -0.440 0.558 0.288 0.665 0.558 0.424 0.577 0.695 0.595 0.334 0.474 0.162 0.366 0.459 0.658 0.390 0.566 Note. The degrees of freedom for all races was 18 except for Hungary (df= 16) and Japan (df = 17). p 0.051 0.010 0.216 0.001 0.010 0.062 0.007 0.000 0.005 0.175 0.034 0.494 0.111 0.041 0.002 0.088 0.009 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 27 Appendix C Spearman rs (and their p values) used to construct Figure 5 Race Variables Finish and Practice Spearman's rs Daytona Fontana Las Vegas Atlanta Bristol Martinsville Texas Phoenix Talladega Richmond Darlington Charlotte Dover Pocono Michigan Sonoma New Hampshire *Daytona Chicago Indianapolis *Pocono Watkins Glen *Michigan *Bristol *Atlanta *Richmond *New Hampshire *Dover Kansas *Fontana *Charlotte *Martinsville *Talladega *Texas *Phoenix Homestead 0.067 0.514 0.544 0.369 0.386 0.630 0.333 0.460 -0.048 -0.144 0.186 0.517 0.285 -0.031 0.323 0.209 0.530 -0.239 0.219 0.192 0.428 0.615 0.033 0.254 -0.052 0.261 0.694 0.377 -0.040 0.287 0.183 0.261 0.021 0.046 0.204 0.156 p 0.776 0.020 0.013 0.108 0.092 0.002 0.150 0.041 0.840 0.543 0.431 0.019 0.222 0.894 0.164 0.376 0.016 0.309 0.352 0.416 0.059 0.003 0.889 0.279 0.825 0.265 0.000 0.100 0.865 0.219 0.438 0.265 0.929 0.845 0.387 0.510 Finish and Qualifying Spearman's rs -0.255 0.392 0.478 0.187 0.073 0.350 0.569 0.578 0.069 0.272 0.172 0.467 0.269 0.209 -0.070 0.362 0.547 0.535 0.431 0.541 0.436 0.326 -0.115 0.230 0.081 0.699 0.449 0.204 0.395 0.317 0.550 0.335 -0.240 0.021 0.293 -0.141 Note. The degrees of freedom for all races was 18. The * denotes that drivers had raced at that track earlier in the season. p 0.276 0.086 0.032 0.427 0.757 0.129 0.008 0.007 0.771 0.245 0.465 0.037 0.251 0.376 0.767 0.116 0.012 0.014 0.057 0.013 0.054 0.160 0.626 0.329 0.733 0.006 0.046 0.387 0.084 0.172 0.011 0.148 0.306 0.929 0.209 0.552 Finish and Points Spearman's rs 0.192 -0.073 0.018 0.230 0.242 0.350 0.470 0.285 -0.279 0.169 0.374 0.001 -0.010 0.209 0.269 0.031 0.547 0.535 0.294 0.204 0.436 0.279 -0.022 -0.103 0.067 0.436 0.434 0.555 0.559 0.500 -0.126 0.493 -0.240 0.043 0.390 0.299 p 0.416 0.757 0.939 0.328 0.302 0.129 0.036 0.222 0.232 0.475 0.103 0.994 0.964 0.376 0.251 0.894 0.012 0.014 0.207 0.387 0.054 0.232 0.924 0.663 0.776 0.054 0.055 0.010 0.010 0.024 0.595 0.027 0.306 0.855 0.088 0.199 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 28 Appendix D R values (and their related F statistics and p values) used to construct Figure 9 Race Model Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying Daytona Fontana Las Vegas Atlanta Bristol Martinsville Texas Phoenix Talladega Richmond Darlington Charlotte Dover Pocono Michigan Sonoma New Hampshire *Daytona Chicago Indianapolis *Pocono Watkins Glen *Michigan *Bristol *Atlanta *Richmond *New Hampshire *Dover Kansas *Fontana *Charlotte *Martinsville *Talladega *Texas *Phoenix Homestead Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Points R F p R F 0.169 0.609 0.359 0.537 0.478 0.720 0.591 0.642 0.084 0.404 0.289 0.652 0.626 0.691 0.706 0.576 0.623 0.569 0.624 0.638 0.823 0.586 0.383 0.440 0.398 0.595 0.602 0.599 0.477 0.494 0.585 0.508 0.219 0.273 0.674 0.590 0.589 11.817 2.963 8.120 5.929 21.572 10.735 14.055 0.144 3.921 1.825 14.854 12.932 17.829 19.934 9.941 12.689 9.580 12.762 13.763 42.177 10.463 3.453 4.818 3.779 10.968 11.404 11.235 5.893 6.490 10.448 6.989 1.016 1.612 16.701 10.725 0.559 0.000 0.063 0.001 0.005 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.866 0.027 0.174 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.041 0.013 0.031 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.005 0.003 0.000 0.000 0.371 0.212 0.000 0.000 0.293 0.594 0.332 0.658 0.591 0.741 0.575 0.719 0.135 0.654 0.432 0.650 0.609 0.699 0.743 0.557 0.594 0.579 0.738 0.667 0.812 0.621 0.528 0.629 0.567 0.798 0.740 0.797 0.652 0.617 0.526 0.772 0.219 0.472 0.725 0.697 1.883 10.943 2.485 15.275 10.779 24.496 9.882 21.429 0.374 14.954 4.614 14.654 11.848 18.649 24.775 9.014 10.908 10.117 24.030 16.089 38.863 12.614 7.734 13.130 9.492 35.141 24.339 34.917 14.792 12.323 7.689 29.671 1.016 5.734 22.241 18.917 p 0.165 0.000 0.096 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.690 0.000 0.015 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.371 0.006 0.000 0.000 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 29 Appendix D (continued) Race Model Finish = α + β1Qualifying + β2Points Daytona Fontana Las Vegas Atlanta Bristol Martinsville Texas Phoenix Talladega Richmond Darlington Charlotte Dover Pocono Michigan Sonoma New Hampshire *Daytona Chicago Indianapolis *Pocono Watkins Glen *Michigan *Bristol *Atlanta *Richmond *New Hampshire *Dover Kansas *Fontana *Charlotte *Martinsville *Talladega *Texas *Phoenix Homestead R F p 0.337 0.532 0.305 0.650 0.578 0.687 0.630 0.676 0.162 0.655 0.430 0.580 0.552 0.708 0.681 0.527 0.607 0.565 0.762 0.711 0.796 0.642 0.518 0.631 0.566 0.814 0.736 0.796 0.652 0.625 0.575 0.776 0.217 0.478 0.717 0.628 2.573 7.902 2.058 14.656 10.044 17.944 13.211 16.876 0.542 15.044 4.540 10.179 8.785 19.703 17.347 7.704 11.672 9.427 27.735 20.450 34.635 14.052 7.344 13.255 9.432 39.286 23.777 34.720 14.812 12.857 9.913 30.424 0.997 5.943 21.214 13.051 0.008 0.001 0.141 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.586 0.000 0.016 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.377 0.005 0.000 0.000 Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying + β3Points R F 0.342 0.631 0.364 0.671 0.608 0.742 0.635 0.719 0.166 0.655 0.514 0.681 0.638 0.716 0.754 0.616 0.623 0.586 0.762 0.712 0.826 0.667 0.532 0.643 0.567 0.818 0.741 0.798 0.652 0.631 0.589 0.779 0.222 0.478 0.736 0.697 p 1.726 8.606 1.994 10.676 7.627 15.926 8.818 13.974 0.372 7.153 3.427 11.296 8.962 9.741 17.131 7.952 8.249 6.821 18.047 13.371 28.086 10.443 5.137 9.187 6.170 26.336 15.852 22.948 9.629 8.605 6.923 20.101 0.678 3.864 15.377 12.308 0.177 0.000 0.130 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.733 0.000 0.017 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.004 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.570 0.016 0.000 0.000 Note. The degrees of freedom for the two- and three-variable models were (2, 40) and (3, 39), respectively, for all races except Pocono (df = 2, 39 and 3, 38). The * denotes that drivers had raced at that track earlier in the season. PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 30 Appendix E R values (and their related F statistics and p values) used to construct Figure 8 Grand Prix Model Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying Australia Malaysia China Bahrain Spain Monaco Turkey Great Britain Germany Hungary Europe Belgium Italy Singapore Japan Brazil Abu Dhabi Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Points R F p R F 0.300 0.630 0.635 0.789 0.810 0.536 0.749 0.837 0.478 0.501 0.741 0.660 0.587 0.718 0.866 0.542 0.633 0.845 5.597 5.773 14.108 16.231 3.432 10.881 20.019 2.524 2.523 10.441 6.574 4.490 9.053 24.111 3.551 5.702 0.446 0.013 0.012 0.000 0.000 0.055 0.000 0.000 0.109 0.113 0.001 0.007 0.027 0.002 0.000 0.051 0.012 0.579 0.681 0.418 0.730 0.589 0.572 0.612 0.809 0.608 0.523 0.467 0.103 0.612 0.575 0.576 0.707 0.623 4.308 7.353 1.804 9.705 4.538 4.147 5.113 16.159 5.003 2.826 2.380 0.091 5.102 4.210 3.986 8.522 5.407 p 0.030 0.005 0.194 0.001 0.026 0.034 0.018 0.000 0.019 0.090 0.122 0.913 0.018 0.032 0.039 0.002 0.015 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 31 Appendix E (continued) Grand Prix Model Finish = α + β1Qualifying + β2Points Australia Malaysia China Bahrain Spain Monaco Turkey Great Britain Germany Hungary Europe Belgium Italy Singapore Japan Brazil Abu Dhabi R F p 0.523 0.694 0.523 0.801 0.805 0.538 0.740 0.839 0.603 0.320 0.730 0.626 0.547 0.721 0.879 0.447 0.662 3.217 7.925 3.203 15.234 15.690 3.467 10.331 20.265 4.863 0.859 9.700 5.478 3.643 9.254 27.418 2.127 6.664 0.065 0.003 0.066 0.000 0.000 0.054 0.001 0.000 0.021 0.443 0.001 0.014 0.048 0.001 0.000 0.149 0.007 Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying + β3Points R F 0.583 0.702 0.651 0.805 0.811 0.579 0.755 0.855 0.613 0.529 0.745 0.685 0.626 0.724 0.896 0.713 0.664 p 2.755 5.183 3.937 9.882 10.289 2.701 7.087 14.518 3.222 1.822 6.653 4.724 3.452 5.889 20.498 5.516 4.226 0.076 0.010 0.027 0.000 0.000 0.080 0.003 0.000 0.051 0.189 0.003 0.015 0.041 0.006 0.000 0.008 0.022 Note. The degrees of freedom for the two- and three-variable models were (2, 17) and (3, 16), respectively, for all grands prix except Hungary (df = 2, 15 and 3 ,14) and Japan (df = 2, 16 and 3, 15). PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 32 Appendix F R values (and their related F statistics and p values) used to construct Figure 9 Race Model Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying Daytona Fontana Las Vegas Atlanta Bristol Martinsville Texas Phoenix Talladega Richmond Darlington Charlotte Dover Pocono Michigan Sonoma New Hampshire *Daytona Chicago Indianapolis *Pocono Watkins Glen *Michigan *Bristol *Atlanta *Richmond *New Hampshire *Dover Kansas *Fontana *Charlotte *Martinsville *Talladega *Texas *Phoenix Homestead Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Points R F p R F 0.273 0.460 0.547 0.421 0.306 0.639 0.597 0.587 0.085 0.513 0.317 0.596 0.427 0.241 0.396 0.466 0.672 0.504 0.462 0.465 0.552 0.544 0.145 0.259 0.105 0.736 0.541 0.156 0.543 0.330 0.418 0.308 0.204 0.113 0.191 0.273 0.689 2.283 3.641 1.840 0.883 5.879 4.726 4.479 0.063 3.036 0.952 4.706 1.902 0.528 1.589 2.362 7.024 2.909 2.319 2.356 3.740 3.582 0.184 0.611 0.095 10.058 3.532 0.213 3.562 1.040 1.800 0.894 0.371 0.111 0.322 0.689 0.515 0.132 0.048 0.189 0.432 0.011 0.023 0.027 0.939 0.074 0.405 0.023 0.179 0.599 0.232 0.124 0.005 0.081 0.128 0.125 0.045 0.050 0.833 0.554 0.909 0.001 0.052 0.810 0.057 0.375 0.195 0.427 0.695 0.895 0.729 0.515 0.196 0.435 0.544 0.336 0.351 0.640 0.532 0.463 0.288 0.242 0.555 0.433 0.415 0.292 0.539 0.352 0.680 0.443 0.334 0.244 0.541 0.524 0.066 0.230 0.160 0.445 0.544 0.551 0.564 0.271 0.400 0.342 0.134 0.206 0.108 0.205 0.340 1.990 3.573 1.085 1.195 5.903 3.359 2.320 0.769 0.532 3.788 1.965 1.777 0.795 3.491 1.205 7.314 2.082 1.074 0.540 3.527 3.219 0.038 0.477 0.225 2.103 3.583 3.721 3.967 0.679 1.620 1.131 0.158 0.378 0.101 0.375 p 0.716 0.167 0.050 0.360 0.326 0.011 0.058 0.128 0.478 0.597 0.043 0.170 0.199 0.467 0.053 0.324 0.005 0.155 0.363 0.592 0.052 0.065 0.962 0.628 0.800 0.152 0.050 0.045 0.038 0.520 0.227 0.345 0.855 0.690 0.904 0.692 PRACTICE, QUALIFYING, AND PAST SUCCESS IN NASCAR AND F1 33 Appendix F (continued) R values (and their related F statistics and p values) used to construct Figure 9 Race Model Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Qualifying R F p Finish = α + β1Practice + β2Points R F p Daytona 0.338 1.102 0.354 0.338 0.692 0.570 Fontana 0.345 1.153 0.339 0.460 1.433 0.270 Las Vegas 0.434 1.981 0.168 0.578 2.681 0.081 Atlanta 0.390 1.532 0.244 0.447 1.339 0.296 Bristol 0.275 0.697 0.511 0.351 0.754 0.536 Martinsville 0.343 1.134 0.344 0.640 3.704 0.033 Texas 0.633 5.703 0.012 0.655 4.021 0.026 Phoenix 0.577 4.246 0.031 0.589 2.846 0.070 Talladega 0.287 0.767 0.479 0.288 0.483 0.699 Richmond 0.302 0.857 0.442 0.530 2.087 0.142 Darlington 0.475 2.479 0.113 0.535 2.378 0.108 Charlotte 0.494 2.749 0.092 0.597 2.968 0.063 Dover 0.359 1.261 0.308 0.447 1.332 0.298 Pocono 0.360 1.271 0.305 0.384 0.927 0.450 Michigan 0.350 1.189 0.328 0.552 2.341 0.111 Sonoma 0.430 1.939 0.174 0.476 1.564 0.236 New Hampshire 0.549 3.680 0.047 0.680 4.591 0.016 *Daytona 0.519 3.134 0.069 0.543 2.238 0.123 Chicago 0.536 3.431 0.056 0.543 2.232 0.123 Indianapolis 0.527 3.285 0.062 0.528 2.067 0.144 *Pocono 0.493 2.737 0.093 0.552 2.347 0.111 Watkins Glen 0.425 1.879 0.183 0.544 2.253 0.121 *Michigan 0.159 0.221 0.803 0.159 0.139 0.935 *Bristol 0.242 0.532 0.597 0.278 0.448 0.722 *Atlanta 0.146 0.187 0.830 0.163 0.147 0.930 *Richmond 0.749 10.875 0.000 0.753 7.001 0.003 *New Hampshire 0.382 1.457 0.260 0.548 2.299 0.116 *Dover 0.573 4.166 0.033 0.583 2.750 0.076 Kansas 0.461 2.301 0.130 0.678 4.545 0.017 *Fontana 0.325 1.005 0.386 0.355 0.772 0.526 *Charlotte 0.626 5.489 0.014 0.643 3.771 0.031 *Martinsville 0.401 1.633 0.224 0.404 1.043 0.400 *Talladega 0.248 0.559 0.581 0.253 0.366 0.778 *Texas 0.203 0.366 0.699 0.206 0.237 0.868 *Phoenix 0.177 0.277 0.761 0.191 0.203 0.892 Homestead 0.266 0.651 0.534 0.363 0.812 0.505 Note. The degrees of freedom for the two- and three-variable models were (2, 17) and (3, 16), respectively, for all races. The * denotes that drivers had raced at that track earlier in the season.