Happiness in Everyday Life - The Doris A. Howell Foundation for
Transcription
Happiness in Everyday Life - The Doris A. Howell Foundation for
MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI and JEREMY HUNTER HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE: THE USES OF EXPERIENCE SAMPLING∗ (Received 29 January 2003; Accepted 17 February 2003) ABSTRACT. This paper uses the Experience Sampling Method data drawn from a national sample of American youth. It examines the proximal environmental factors as well as behaviors and habits that correlate to personal happiness. Momentarylevel scores show that reported happiness varies significantly both by day of week and time of day. Furthermore, particular activities are associated with varying degrees of happiness. School activities rate below average scores in happiness, while social, active and passive leisure activities are above average. Particular companions also correlate to differing level of happiness. Being alone rates the lowest levels of happiness, while being with friend corresponds to the highest. Person-level averages of happiness suggest that both higher social class and age correlate with lower levels of happiness, while gender and race do not. Paradoxically, youth who spend more time in school and social activities are happier than those who spend less. Unexpectedly, students who spend more time pleasure reading report lower levels of happiness. Finally, feeling good about the self, excited, proud, sociable, active as well as being in the conditions for flow experience are the strongest predictors of trait happiness. KEY WORDS: experience sampling, happiness, usual daily activities Current understanding of human happiness points at five major effects on this emotion. These are, moving from those most impervious to change to those that are most under personal control: genetic determinants, macro-social conditions, chance events, proximal environment and personality. It is not unlikely that, as behavioral geneticists insist, a “set level” coded in our chromosomes accounts for perhaps as much as half of the variance in self-reported happiness (Lykken and Tellegen, 1996; Tellegen et al., 1988). These effects are probably mediated by temperamental traits like extraversion, which are partly genetically determined and which are in turn linked to happiness (Myers, 1993). Cross-national comparisons suggest that macro-social conditions such as extreme poverty, war and social injustice are all obstacles to happiness (Inglehart and Klingemann, 2000; Veenhoven, 1995). Chance events like personal tragedies, illness, or sudden strokes of good fortune may drastically affect the level of happiness, but apparently these ∗ This study was made possible by a grant from the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation. Journal of Happiness Studies 4: 185–199, 2003. © 2003 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 186 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER effects do not last long (Brickman et al., 1978; Diener, 2000). One might include under the heading of the proximal environment the social class, community, family and economic situation – in other words, those factors in the immediate surroundings that may have an impact on a person’s well-being. And finally, habits and coping behaviors developed by the individual will have an important effect. Hope, optimism and the ability to experience flow can be learned and thus moderate one’s level of happiness (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997; Seligman, 2002). In this paper, we present a method that allows investigators to study the impact of momentary changes in the environment on people’s happiness levels, as well as its more lasting, trait-like correlates. Research on happiness generally considers this emotion to be a personal trait. The overall happiness level of individuals is measured by a survey or questionnaire, and then “happy” people – those who score higher on a one-time response scale – are contrasted with less happy ones. Whatever distinguishes the two groups is then assumed to be a condition affecting happiness. This perspective is a logical outcome of the methods used, namely, one-time measures. If a person’s happiness level is measured only once, it is by definition impossible to detect intraindividual variations. Yet, we know quite well that emotional states, including happiness, are quite volatile and responsive to environmental conditions. Of course both common sense and psychological research suggests that when positive events happen in a person’s life, happiness increases. For instance Schwartz and Strack (1999) have shown that even such trivial events as one’s home team winning a soccer match, or the information that the weather in one’s hometown is better than the weather in surrounding areas, will raise happiness levels. However, they warn that: “. . . subjective well-being cannot be predicted on the basis of objective circumstances, unless one takes the construal process into account” (p. 61). In other words, the impact of external events on happiness is mediated by the person’s system of values and cognitive interpretive structures. It is to detect variations in emotional states over time that the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) was developed. This method relies on subjects’ responses to an electronic pager that signals at random times during the waking hours of the day, yielding up to fifty measures of happiness at specific moments during an average week. Each time the pager signals, the respondents rate their experiential states, including their levels of happiness (e.g. Csikszentmihalyi et al., HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 187 1977; Kubey et al., 1996; Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2001; a handbook for using the ESM is in preparation, see Hektner, in press). This method not only accounts for momentary states, but can also yield trait-like measure by adding up for each person the separate momentary responses. Daniel Kahneman (1999) has described this approach as measuring “point-instant utility”, and argued for its theoretical importance: “An assessment of a person’s objective happiness over a period of time can be derived from a dense record of the quality of experience at each point” (p. 3). Thus repeated measures taken over a representative segment of a person’s life can be used in two ways: (a) as indicators of momentary happiness, which can help us understand the effect of Immediate environmental circumstances; and (b) as personal traits derived from aggregating the repeated responses over a week’s time, to derive a trait-like measure of personal happiness. The first comparison of state-like and trait-like characteristics of subjective experience using the ESM was a doctoral dissertation by Ronald Graef (1978). In that work Graef found that while all the emotions were more trait-determined than state-determined, this was particularly true of happiness. In other words, a person’s average level of happiness explained more of the variance in his or her responses over the week than was explained by what that person was doing, where he or she was, or whom he or she was with. This “set level” (cf. Tellegen et al., 1988) explained about twice the variance in happiness compared to other mood states. Longitudinal studies suggest a somewhat different conclusion. In a 2-year follow-up of 455 high school students, the average ESM happiness scores correlated 0.55, more or less at the same level as other mood variables. But a 4-year follow-up of a subset of 187 of these students showed only a correlation of 0.22 for happiness, while r’s for all the other variables ranged from 0.34 (being in control) to 0.56 (being relaxed), suggesting that self-reported happiness is less stable than other dimensions of experience (Moneta et al., 2001; Patton, 1998; Hektner, in press). In any case, there is obviously a great deal of variance unexplained by a “set level” of happiness. In this paper we are going to use ESM data on a group of over 800 adolescents to explicate the contributions of some of the momentary conditions on intra-individual reports of happiness, and then look from a trait-like perspective at how demographic variables and patterns of behavior relate to over-all levels of happiness. 188 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER METHOD The Participants The participants of this study are primary school students from the Alfred P. Sloan Study of Youth and Social Development, a national multi-year study involving 6th, 8th, 10th and 12th graders from 33 elementary and secondary schools from 12 communities across the country. These sites were chosen to create a nationally-representative sample based on the variation in labor force composition and participation, ethnicity, urbanicity, geographic location, and student ability (see Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2000 for a fuller description). The 828 students included here are part of a focal group of 1215 youth. The group here represents those who provided the minimum amount of Experience Sampling Data and include 342 males (41.3%) and 486 females (58.7%), 491 Whites (59.3%), 54 Asians (6.5%), 131 Latinos (15.8%), 145 African Americans (17.5%) and a small number (7) of Native Americans (0.8%). Two-hundred and thirty-three, 6th graders represented 28.1% of the sample, while the remainder were 236 Eighth graders (28.5%), 196 Tenth graders (23.7%) and 163 Twelth graders (19.7%). Social Class was measured on the community-level (rather than through household income) and consisted of 118 students (14.4%) from Poor communities, 133 (16.2%) from Working, 271 (33%) from Middle, 212 (25.8%) from Upper Middle and 87 (10.6%) Upper classes. Measures Measures of subjective experience and time use are drawn from the ESM, where each participant was given a programmable wristwatch set to signal at random moments eight times a day from 7:30 am to 10:30 pm for one week. Upon hearing the signal, participants completed a form containing open-ended questions about what they were doing at that moment as well as multiple-choice items regarding whom they were with and close-ended scales addressing a wide range of feelings and conditions associated with that moment. The data included here are from those students who completed at least 15 responses over the course of the week. The open-ended items about the student’s current activity were coded into several dozen specific categories, that can also be converted into much more generalized groupings such as School (eg. studying, listening to lecture), Active Leisure (playing games, sports), Passive Leisure HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 189 (watching tv, listening to music), Maintenance (grooming, eating, transportation) and Work activities (after school jobs). In addition, two variables used for assessing the activity’s conditions for flow experiences are (1) the amount to which they found the current activity Challenging (a 1–9 scale, where 1 is the lowest and 9 the highest value) and (2) the student’s level of Skill in the activity (using the same 1–9 scheme). Mood variables include a 1–7 scale (1 being the most negative and 7, the most positive value) asking the student if they felt Happy (vs. Sad), Strong (vs. Weak), Proud (vs. Ashamed), Sociable (vs. Lonely), Excited (vs. Bored), Active (vs. Passive) and a 1–10 scale (where 1 is the most negative and 10, the most positive) asking “Did you feel good about yourself?”. These variables can be used to refer to specific moments in time, for example what is the level of happiness when watching television versus doing sports? Furthermore, an individual’s total responses can also be combined to form a Person-level variable. Such variables can be used to compare people who rank happier than others overall. A third way these variables can be used is to combine the contextual and the personal. For example, using happiness as referent, a Person-Level contextual variable tells the amount of happiness a particular individual experiences in a specific activity. Momentary Changes in Happiness Days of the Week There is a widely held belief that people are more sad on certain days of the week than on others. “Blue Mondays” in particular are held to be depressing. In this sample variation in happiness (using “z” scores calibrated on individual means) was very slight, although significant. An ANOVA produced an F value of 3.4 (p < 0.002). The lowest happiness was reported on Sundays, and each day afterwards happiness increased slightly, reaching its peak on Saturdays (see Figure 1). Post-hoc Bonferroni tests indicated that respondents were significantly happier on Saturdays than they were on Mondays, Tuesdays and Wednesdays (Sunday responses were fewer and had a greater variance in happiness, thus yielded no significant differences). Clearly, the social structure of time has an impact on happiness: The early part of the weekend, with its freedom from work or school, is experienced as liberating. The effect is probably greater on adults, for whom the working week is presumably even more constraining than it is for teenagers. 190 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER Figure 1. Happiness (beep-level z-score) by day of week. Figure 2. Happiness (beep-level z-score) throughout the weekday. Times of Day During the weekdays, time is structured by work or school requirements according to a circadian pattern. The first part of the day, spent at work or school, tends to be less happy, except for a peak at lunch-time. There is a dip after lunch, followed by higher reports of happiness in the afternoon when one is again free (see Figure 2). If we contrast afternoon reports with those obtained before noon, the difference in happiness is striking (F = 56.5, p < 0.00001). Activities What one happens to be doing at the moment of the signal has an even more specific effect on happiness. There are ten main activities that teenagers do during the week, each taking up 2% or more of their waking time. For seven of these ten, the average level of happiness is HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 191 TABLE I Happiness (aggregated Person–Level z-score) by top ten most frequent activities∗ TV Talking with Friends Eating a Meal Unspecified Homework Individual Work Listening to Lecture Chores Fun Reading/Writing Mathematics Talking with Family Happy (z-score) T-value P< N 0.03 0.35 0.19 −0.30 −0.11 −0.21 −0.21 −0.01 −0.25 −0.03 1.24 9.87 5.78 −8.21 −2.99 −5.36 −4.44 −0.14 −5.27 −0.53 NS 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.003 0.000 0.000 NS 0.000 NS 666 325 524 409 358 381 343 324 327 281 ∗ Activities representing at least 2% of time during the week (1% is roughly equal to 1 h) significantly higher or lower than it is on the average (see Table I). The highest level of happiness is reported when talking with friends (Mean z = 0.35, t = 9.87, p < 0.00001), and the lowest when doing school-related homework (Mean z = −0.30, t = −8.21, p < 0.00001). Another way to observe the effects of activities is by combining them into six major categories, which together account for 21,631 responses, or 93% of the total. Four of the six categories are significantly different from the average (p < 0.00001). Whenever students are involved with School-related activities, their happiness level is below average (Mean z = −0.19); when Socializing with friends, when involved in Active Leisure, or in Passive Leisure it is above average (Mean z = 0.28, 0.19 and 0.11, respectively). Some of the happiest experiences reported in the Active Leisure category are Sports (Mean z = 0.50), Music (z = 0.29) and visual Art (z = 0.27). The other two major categories, which are indistinguishable from the average in terms of happiness, are Working and Maintenance activities such as doing chores, eating, dressing, and so on. Companions Who one happens to be with companions, it also impacts significantly on the level of reported happiness. In terms of companionship, youth experience the lowest levels of happiness when they are Alone (Mean z = −0.12, p < 0.0001), with Teachers (Mean z = −0.09, 192 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER p < 0.0001), and with Classmates (Mean z = −0.07, p < 0.0001) while being with friends corresponds to the highest level (Mean z = 0.21, p < 0.0001). Being with Parents is at the average for happiness, which is lower than being with a Sibling (Mean z = 0.03, p < 0.016). Spending time with a Relative, however, is associated with more happiness (Mean z = 0.09, p < 0.002) than either of these two familial groups. Person-Level Correlates of Happiness The analysis thus far focused on how happiness is experienced at the moment – how situational context relates to shifting levels of happiness within the individual. The ESM data can be also analyzed at the person level, making it possible to answer the question, what differentiates young people who on the average report higher levels of happiness from those who during the week report being less happy? Demographic Characteristics General traits of the person have rather strong relationships to happiness. The largest difference reflects the Social Class of Community (SCC) in which the teenagers live. SCC was computed on five levels of increasing affluence: Poor (mostly single-parent, unemployed), Working Class, Middle Class, Upper-Middle Class and Upper Class. Contrary to expectations, the highest level of happiness was reported by young people living in Working Class communities, then by those in Middle Class, Poor, Upper Class and finally Upper Middle Class environments. An ANOVA in which all the demographic variables (i.e. age, gender, SCC, Ethnic background) were entered showed the strongest effect for SCC (F = 8.09, p < 0.0001). Age was the second most important factor (F = 6.45, p < 0.0001) Happiness decreases through the teenage years; it reaches its lowest point by age 16, and then shows a small recovery by age 18 (see also Moneta, 2001). Gender and Ethnic background did not show significant effects, even though African-American and Hispanic youth had higher levels of happiness than Caucasians and Asians – but these differences appear to be due more to social class than to ethnicity. Boys and girls generally did not differ in terms of happiness. However, the ANOVA showed one significant interaction (F = 2.92, p < 0.02) between gender and SCC. Poor girls (5.5) experience more happiness than Poor boys (5.0) (t = −2.51, p < 0.014). HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 193 Activities In the previous section, we have seen that teenagers are happier when they do certain things (e.g. in leisure) than when they do others (e.g. study). Here we are looking at the issue from a trait-like, rather than a state-like perspective: in other words, are teenagers who spend more time in leisure activities during the week happier than those who spend more time studying? Contrary to what one might expect, the amount of time spent in school-related activities during the week is positively related to happiness (multiple regression (MR), t = 2.25, p < 0.024), indicating that those teenagers who study more are in fact happier, even though studying is lower in happiness than most other activities. This apparently paradoxical finding is one of the important ways in which the ESM can reveal the fact that relationships that are negative at the state level can at time be positive at the trait level. The percent of time students spend socializing is also positively related to happiness (t = 2.61, p < 0.009). In this case, both momentary and Person–Level relationships point in the same direction. Young people feel happier when they interact with peers, and those who do so more often are on the average happier than those who interact less. One unexpected finding was that of the smaller activity categories the one that showed the strongest relation to happiness at the person level was Reading a book for pleasure. The relationship was negative (t = −2.09, p < 0.04), suggesting that teenagers who spend more time during the week are also generally less happy. This result could be due to the fact that young people who read more are less often in the company of their peers. There is a slight negative correlation (−0.09, p < 0.08, n = 825) between the amount of time spent reading and the percent of time spent with friends. Companions The social context affects happiness in complex ways. Those young people who spend more time alone are in general less happy (MR, t = −3.85, p < 0.0001). Those who spend more time with relatives during the week tend to be happier (MR, t = 2.24, p < 0.01). Although being with friends is related to happiness it is not significantly so, because older teenagers spend more time with friends, while being less happy than younger ones. Therefore, the age effect cancels out the beneficial effect of spending time with friends. 194 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER TABLE II Standardized regression coefficients from multiple stepwise regression of mean (Person–Level) happiness on Person–Level mood variables (controlling for demographic variables) Independent variables Mean (Person–Level) happiness T-score P< Strong Feel Good About Self Sociable Excited Proud Active Grade Level in School Constant 0.099 0.093 0.160 0.230 0.230 −0.050 −0.050 1.650 2.5 6.62 4.5 11.74 5.75 3 −5.06 9.2 0.012 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.003 0.000 0.000 Adjusted R 2 F -value 0.540 135.3 0.000 The Relation of Happiness to Other Moods What other dimensions of subjective experience differentiate a happy young person from one who is less so? To answer that question, we did a regression in which the dependent variable was a person’s average happiness score for the week, and the predictors included all the other mood variables. Such a MR explained 55% of the variance in happiness (Table II). The strongest predictor of trait happiness was how Excited (vs. Bored) a person felt, followed by the variables Feeling Good about Self, Proud (vs. Ashamed), Sociable (vs. Lonely), feeling Active and Strong (vs. Weak). The correlation coefficients of these variables with Happiness (and controlling for age), were 0.58, 0.59, 0.47 and 0.53, respectively (with N = 799, all p < 0.0001). Happiness and the Conditions for Flow It was expected that young people who spend more time in situations they perceive as being conducive to flow would be on the whole happier. To measure whether a person was more likely to be in a Flow condition we calculated the percent of time spent in situations that were above the mean level of challenge and the mean level of skill at the same time. When a person was above the mean of skills but below mean challenge, the condition was considered conducive to Relaxation. High challenges and low skills were counted as Anxiety, and low challenges with low skills as Apathy. HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 195 TABLE III Standardized regression coefficients from multiple stepwise regression of mean (Person–Level) happiness on flow conditions (controlling for demographic variables) Independent variables Mean (Person–Level) happiness T-score P< Flow Condition Relaxation Condition Grade Level in School Social Class of Community Constant 0.013 0.008 −0.097 −0.080 5.560 6.05 4.93 −7.01 −3.1 31 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.000 Adjusted R 2 F -value 0.124 29.56 0.000 Table III shows the final regression model, which includes Age and the Gender by SCC interaction as well as the four Flow-related variables. The full model explains 12.4% of the variance in happiness. The frequency of time spent in the Flow condition is a very strong predictor of happiness (t = 6.05, p < 0.0001) even after taking the significant demographic variables into account. The Final Model To see if combining all the correlates of happiness in one model would enhance understanding of the phenomenon, we created a final regression model that included the most promising variables form previous analyses – excluding, however, the mood variables which as we have seen above (Table II), explain 54% of the variance in happiness. The resulting model is the one reported in Table IV. The combined predictive value is not much higher than that of some of the demographic variables taken singly, as it attains only 15% of the variance in happiness. Nevertheless, the pattern is suggestive. The pattern can be summarized as follows: Happier teenagers tend to be younger, from lower socio-economic circumstances. They spend less time alone and less time reading books. They spend more time either in high challenge/high skill Flow producing situations, or low challenge/high skill Relaxing situations. These are also the young people who feel more Excited, Proud, Sociable, Strong, Active and Good about themselves. 196 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER TABLE IV Standardized regression coefficients from multiple stepwise regression of mean (Person–Level) happiness on flow conditions and time usage (controlling for demographic variables) Independent variables Mean (Person–Level) happiness Percent of Time Spent Alone In Flow Condition In Relaxation Condition Spent Reading/Writing for Fun Grade Level in School Social Class of Community Constant −0.010 0.013 0.009 −0.014 −0.080 −0.065 5.590 Adjusted R 2 F -value 0.150 24.77 T-score −4.60 6.2 5.22 −2.08 −5.8 −2.5 31.46 P< 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.037 0.012 0.000 0.000 0.000 DISCUSSION The ESM makes it possible to separate the immediate context of happiness from more long-term conditions. In terms of momentary effects, it is clear that what one does and whom one is with will modify a person’s base-line of happiness. Freely chosen activities and the company of peers raise the level of happiness, while obligatory activities like schoolwork and the condition of solitude lowers it. The social structure of time affects happiness in a similar way: young people are much happier in the afternoons and evenings of weekdays, when they are free of requirements imposed by adults, and on weekends. But by the end of the weekend, on Sunday afternoons, their happiness decreases in anticipation of the school-day to come. The demographic analyses provide rather counterintuitive suggestions. That happiness decreases during the conflicted teenage years is not surprising, and the recovery around age 18 has been documented before (Moneta, 2001). What is surprising is the lack of positive correlation between happiness and financial affluence. That teenagers from working-class, and even impoverished backgrounds should be happier than upper-middle-class teenagers living in exclusive suburban communities is difficult to explain. It is possible that some selection bias is responsible for this result: perhaps relatively more students from lower class backgrounds who were happy volunteered and completed HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 197 the ESM compared with more affluent students. But the rates of volunteering had been high in all schools, including the ones in the inner city neighborhoods, so this explanation could not account entirely for the findings. Perhaps in the affluent suburban sub-culture it is not “cool” to admit to being happy. Or perhaps material well-being is in fact an obstacle to happiness. Recent research on materialism suggests that excessive concern with consumer goods and material possessions is inversely related with positive developmental outcomes (Schmuck and Sheldon, 2001). In any case, this finding clearly deserves further study. Aggregating responses over a week’s time suggests that happiness is strongly related to an extraverted lifestyle. Not being alone, feeling excited, proud, being in high-challenge, high skill situations are all related to how happy a young person feels. It seems that at least at this stage of life an experience of what we may call “vitality,” or eros, is the most distinctive feature of happiness (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; 1999; see also Ryan and Frederick, 1997, for recent studies dealing with vitality). At the same time, it is important to notice that studying, which produces an experience of sadness as it is occurring, helps young people feel happier in the long run. This is an example of how building “ psychological capital” involves the transformation of potentially negative experiences in positive experience over time (Csikszentmihalyi, 2003). For example, in a longitudinal study of talented teenagers we found that only those who learned to enjoy practicing their talent (i.e. mathematics, music, science, art, athletics) were able to continue developing it through the high school years. Those who became bored or stressed when working on their talent sooner or later gave up, while those who experienced flow in their work continued to perfect their talent (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1997). These results suggest that momentary happiness, at least for young people, is a function of the ability to express their potential vitality as fully as it is possible given the socialization demands the adult world places on them. Teenagers ascribe “happiness” to their moods when they are in situations of relative freedom, in the company of age-mates, able to engage in flow activities that stretch their skills and makes them feel alive and proud. The same conditions are implicated in more enduring, trait-like happiness. Here, however, happiness is also affected by preparation for the future: young people who study more are on the whole happier, presumably because they realize that 198 MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI AND JEREMY HUNTER by building psychological capital the range of opportunities and hence their freedom will increase in the future. If this is the case, the results have important implications for education and social policy. Happiness will increase to the extent that individuals are provided with the means to learn skills that can be deployed to meet reasonable challenges; that they are given freedom to express themselves within bounds of responsibility; that they are allowed to experience the joy of interaction with peers of one’s choice and with adults that care for their well-being. These requirements for happiness presumably operate at every level of societal complexity, from the macro-level of the economy and political structure to the meso- and micro-levels of community, school and family. There are clear trends in contemporary life that militate against such conditions. It is difficult for a young person to be happy when living in a sterile suburb that lacks opportunities for action, forced to attend schools where there is little chance to express oneself except in abstract intellectual terms, surrounded by a small nuclear family that is seldom together and relaxed enough to interact freely. Understanding more clearly the conditions that affect happiness is a prerequisite if social scientists are to help improve the quality of life. REFERENCES Brickman, P., D. Coates and R.J. Janoff-Bulman: 1978, ‘Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative?’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (36), pp. 917–927. Csikszentmihalyi, M.: 1990, Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience (HarperCollins, New York). Csikszentmihalyi, M.: 1997, Finding Flow: The Psychology of Engagement with Everyday Life (Basic Books, New York). Csikszentmihalyi, M.: 2003, Good Business: Leadership, Flow, and the Making of Meaning (Viking, New York). Csikszentmihalyi, M and B. Schneider: 2001, ‘Conditions for optimal development in adolescence: An experiential approach’, Applied Developmental Science 5(3), pp. 122–124. Csikszentmihalyi, M., R. Larson and S. Prescott: 1977, ‘The ecology of adolescent activities and experiences’, Journal of Youth and Adolescence 6(3), pp. 281–294. Csikszentmihalyi, M., K. Rathunde and S. Whalen: 1993, Talented Teenagers: The Roots of Success and Failure (Cambridge University Press, New York). Diener, E.: 2000, ‘Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index’, American Psychologist, 55(1), pp. 34–43. HAPPINESS IN EVERYDAY LIFE 199 Graef, R.: 1978, ‘An analysis of the person by situation interaction through repeated measures’, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The University of Chicago. Hektner, J.: ESM Handbook. (in preparation). Inglehart, R. and H.D. Klingemann: 2000, ‘Genes, culture, democracy and happiness’, in E. Diener and E.M. Suh (eds), Culture and Subjective Well-being (The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp. 165–183. Kahneman, D.: 1999, ‘Objective happiness’, in D. Kahneman, E. Diener and N. Schwartz (eds), Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (Russel Sage, New York), pp. 3–25. Kubey, R. Larson and M. Csikszentmihalyi: 1996, ‘Experience sampling method applications to communication research questions’, Journal of Communication 46(2), pp. 99–120. Lykken, D. and A. Tellegen: 1996, ‘Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon’, Psychological Science 7, pp. 186–189. Moneta, G., B. Schneider and M. Csikszentmihalyi: 2001, ‘A longitudinal study of the self-concept and experiential components of self-worth and affect across adolescence’, Applied Developmental Science 5(3), pp. 125–152. Patton, J.D.: 2002, ‘The role of problem pioneers in creative innovation’, Creativity Research Journal, 14(1), pp. 111–126. Ryan, R. and C. Frederick: 1997, ‘On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being’, Journal of Personality, 65(3), pp. 529–565. Schwartz, N. and F. Strack: 1999, ‘Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications’, in D. Kahneman, E. Diener and N. Schwartz (eds), Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (Russel Sage, New York), pp. 61–84. Schmuck, P. and K.M. Sheldon: 2001, Life-goals and Well-being (Hogrefe & Huber, Göttingen). Seligman, E.P.: 2002, Authentic Happiness (Free Press, New York). Tellegen, A., D.T. Lykken, T.J. Bouchard, K.J. Wilcox, N.L. Segal and S. Rich: 1988, ‘Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, pp. 1031–1039. Veenhoven, R.: 1995, ‘The cross-national pattern of happiness: Test of predictions implied in three theories of happiness’, Social Indicators Research 34, pp. 33–68. Address for Correspondence: MIHALY CSIKSZENTMIHALYI Claremont Graduate University The Quality of Life Research Center 171 E, Tenth Street, CA 91711 Claremont, USA Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2008, Vol. 95, No. 5, 1045–1062 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013262 Open Hearts Build Lives: Positive Emotions, Induced Through Loving-Kindness Meditation, Build Consequential Personal Resources Barbara L. Fredrickson Michael A. Cohn University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University of Michigan Kimberly A. Coffey and Jolynn Pek Sandra M. Finkel University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University of Michigan B. L. Fredrickson’s (1998, 2001) broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions asserts that people’s daily experiences of positive emotions compound over time to build a variety of consequential personal resources. The authors tested this build hypothesis in a field experiment with working adults (n ⫽ 139), half of whom were randomly-assigned to begin a practice of loving-kindness meditation. Results showed that this meditation practice produced increases over time in daily experiences of positive emotions, which, in turn, produced increases in a wide range of personal resources (e.g., increased mindfulness, purpose in life, social support, decreased illness symptoms). In turn, these increments in personal resources predicted increased life satisfaction and reduced depressive symptoms. Discussion centers on how positive emotions are the mechanism of change for the type of mind-training practice studied here and how loving-kindness meditation is an intervention strategy that produces positive emotions in a way that outpaces the hedonic treadmill effect. Keywords: emotions, meditation, positive psychology, broaden-and-build, mindfulness marital satisfaction (Harker & Keltner, 2001), higher incomes (Diener, Nickerson, Lucus, & Sandvik, 2002), and better physical health (Doyle, Gentile, & Cohen, 2006; Richman et al., 2005). People who experience frequent positive emotions have even been shown to live longer (Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001; Moskowitz, 2003; Ostir, Markides, Black, & Goodwin, 2000). Indeed, a recent meta-analysis of nearly 300 findings concluded that positive emotions produce success and health as much as they reflect these good outcomes (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005). How do they do this? How do people’s fleeting and subtle pleasant states pave the way to their later success, health, and longevity? Fredrickson’s (1998) broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions outlines a possible path: Because positive emotions arise in response to diffuse opportunities, rather than narrowlyfocused threats, positive emotions momentarily broaden people’s attention and thinking, enabling them to draw on higher-level connections and a wider-than-usual range of percepts or ideas. In turn, these broadened outlooks often help people to discover and build consequential personal resources. These resources can be cognitive, like the ability to mindfully attend to the present moment; psychological, like the ability to maintain a sense of mastery over environmental challenges; social, like the ability to give and receive emotional support; or physical, like the ability to ward off the common cold. People with these resources are more likely to effectively meet life’s challenges and take advantage of its opportunities, becoming successful, healthy, and happy in the months and years to come. Thus, the personal resources accrued, often unintentionally, through frequent experiences of positive emotions are posited to be keys to later increases in well-being. Put simply, the broaden-and-build theory states that positive emotions widen people’s outlooks in ways that, little by little, reshape who they are. A paradox surrounds positive emotions. On one hand, they are fleeting: Like any emotional state, feelings of joy, gratitude, interest, and contentment typically last only a matter of minutes. Moreover, positive emotions are less intense and less attentiongrabbing than negative emotions (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001) and are more diffuse (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988). Yet on the other hand, research indicates that positive emotions contribute to important downstream life outcomes, including friendship development (Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006), Barbara L. Fredrickson, Kimberly A. Coffey, and Jolynn Pek, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Michael A. Cohn, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan; Sandra M. Finkel, Preventive Cardiology Services, University of Michigan. This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH59615 to Barbara L. Fredrickson, with additional financial and procedural support from the Compuware Corporation (Detroit, Michigan). We thank the leaders at Compuware who opened their minds and doors to the study reported here and offer special thanks to the Compuware employees who devoted their time and energy across months to participate in this project. We acknowledge the contributions of Li Cai, Daniel Serrano, and Patrick Curran in guiding us safely through new statistical terrain. Thanks also go to Benjamin Figa for overseeing data collection; Jordana Adler and Sid Tsai for assistance with data management and coding; and Tracey Callison, Lahnna Catalino, and Bethany Kok for their comments. Finally, Sandra M. Finkel wishes to acknowledge Ngawang Gehlek Rinpoche, her personal mentor in loving-kindness. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department of Psychology, Davie Hall, CB 3270, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599. E-mail: blf@unc.edu 1045 1046 FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL The key hypotheses of the broaden-and-build theory have received empirical support from multiple laboratories. First, the broaden hypothesis holds that positive emotions broaden people’s attention and thinking. Experiments have shown that, relative to neutral and negative states, induced positive emotions widen the scope of people’s visual attention (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Rowe, Hirsh, & Anderson, 2007; Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006), broaden their repertoires of desired actions (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), and increase their openness to new experiences (Kahn & Isen, 1993) and critical feedback (Raghunathan & Trope, 2002). At the interpersonal level, induced positive emotions increase people’s sense of “oneness” with close others (Hejmadi, Waugh, Otake, & Fredrickson, 2008), their trust in acquaintances (Dunn & Schweitzer, 2005), and their ability to accurately recognize individuals of another race (Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005). The empirical evidence is mounting, then, that positive emotions broaden people’s attention and thinking in both personal and interpersonal domains. The second part of the theory, the build hypothesis, holds that positive emotions set people on trajectories of growth that, over time, build consequential personal resources. To date, the empirical evidence for the build hypothesis has been largely indirect. Prospective correlational studies have shown that people who, for whatever reasons, experience or express positive emotions more than others show increases over time in optimism and tranquility (Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003), ego-resilience (Cohn, Fredrickson, Brown, Mikels, & Conway, 2008), mental health (Stein, Folkman, Trabasso, & Richards, 1997), and the quality of their close relationships (Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman, 2006; Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006). Here we present the first experimental evidence that directly tests the build hypothesis. Such research has been virtually nonexistent (but see Emmons & McCullough, 2003; King, 2001), largely because resources are expected to accrue only after many experiences of positive emotions over separate occasions, which necessitates a longitudinal design as well as a reliable, repeatable method for evoking positive emotions. The well-documented hedonic treadmill effect (Diener, Lucas, & Scollon, 2006) assures that emotion-elicitation techniques used with success in the laboratory (e.g., film clips, gifts of candy) would likely become inert if repeated daily. As the novelty of an experience subsides, people’s emotions tend to revert to a trait-like baseline. In this study, we sought to overcome this challenge by using an induction based on meditation. We suspected that meditation would outpace the hedonic treadmill for several reasons. First, it incorporates mindful attention, which has been shown to undo hedonic adaptation (Schwarz, Kahneman, & Xu, in press). Second, unlike watching a film or receiving a gift, meditation practice is active and personalized. Participants can lengthen the meditation, alter their focus, or otherwise try to get more out of their practice, keeping it within a range that is feasible but not boring. Most important, participants can use the insights and psychological skills developed during meditation practice in many situations and life domains. Meditation, then, offers opportunities for enhanced emotions throughout the day, not simply during meditations, per se. Meditation and mindfulness, which are perhaps best known as elements of Buddhist spiritual practice, have also proven to be fruitful topics within empirical research on well-being (Baer, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002; Wallace & Shapiro, 2006). For instance, for more than 2 decades, Kabat-Zinn and colleagues have reported evidence that meditation helps people self-regulate stress, anxiety, chronic pain, and various illnesses (for a review, see Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Building on the observation that when formerly depressed individuals see their thoughts and emotions from a wider perspective, they are more resistant to relapse, Teasdale et al. (2000) developed a successful therapy that combines mindfulness meditation with cognitive therapy. More recently, Kabat-Zinn collaborated with Davidson et al. (2003) to examine the affective, brain, and immunological effects of beginning a meditation practice. Volunteers were randomly assigned to either a waitlist control group (n ⫽ 16) or an 8-week mindfulness-based stress-reduction workshop (n ⫽ 25), which required a daily practice of guided meditation lasting about 1 hr. As in past studies, trait anxiety was significantly reduced in the meditation group. Both immediately after the training period and 4 months later, electroencephalogram monitoring revealed that meditators showed increases in left-sided anterior brain activation, which has been repeatedly linked to greater positive, approachrelated emotions (for a review, see Davidson, 2000). Meditators also showed a more robust and effective immune response to an influenza vaccine administered at the end of the training period, and the strength of this response was correlated with the magnitude of left-sided anterior brain activation. The suggestion that meditation practice increases positive affect is also supported by at least one experience sampling study (Easterlin & Cardeña, 1998). Most empirical work on meditation has centered on mindfulness meditation (e.g., Davidson et al., 2003; Teasdale et al., 2000). Because we were particularly interested in evoking positive emotions, we employed a related mind-training practice, loving-kindness meditation (LKM). LKM is a technique used to increase feelings of warmth and caring for self and others (Salzberg, 1995). Like other meditation practices, LKM involves quiet contemplation in a seated posture, often with eyes closed and an initial focus on the breath. Yet whereas mindfulness meditation involves training one’s attention toward the present moment in an open-minded (nonjudgmental) way, LKM involves directing one’s emotions toward warm and tender feelings in an open-hearted way. Individuals are first asked to focus on their heart region and contemplate a person for whom they already feel warm and tender feelings (e.g., their child, a close loved one). They are then asked to extend these warm feelings first to themselves and then to an ever-widening circle of others. Thus, LKM may well cultivate broadened attention in addition to positive emotions. According to the broaden-and-build theory, these two experiential consequences go hand in hand. In LKM, people cultivate the intention to experience positive emotions during the meditation itself, as well as in their life more generally. Moreover, mind-training practices like LKM are thought to not only shift people’s fleeting emotional states but also reshape their enduring personality traits (Davidson et al., 2003), a coupling of momentary with long-term gains fully compatible with the broadenand-build theory. We acknowledge that mind-training practices, including LKM, are not simply vehicles for improving emotion experiences. The primary goal within contemplative traditions is, instead, to learn about the nature of one’s mind and dispel false assumptions about the sources of one’s happiness (Dalai Lama & Cutler, 1998). These insights can, in turn, shift people’s basic outlooks on themselves in relation to others, increasing empathy and compassion. Approaching daily life with the new insights and outlooks developed through mind-training practice is what is thought to enhance people’s POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES emotion experiences. That said, the goal of the present study was to test the build hypothesis, which required a means of reliably eliciting positive emotions over the span of months. We saw LKM as a suitable vehicle to meet this goal. Future empirical work is needed to test whether the cognitive shifts outlined by scholars of contemplative practices are indeed responsible for any success LKM has in enhancing positive emotions. LKM involves a range of thoughts and visualizations, and it directly evokes only select positive emotions (i.e., love, contentment, and compassion) and carries some potential to evoke negative emotions. Moreover, given the possibility of gradual shifts in people’s outlooks and personality traits, we expected the positive emotions generated by LKM to increase over time. Our study involved daily assessments of time spent meditating and of a wide range of discrete positive and negative emotions. This strategy allowed us to determine whether (a) positive emotions, measured directly, are responsible for any changes produced by LKM; (b) different classes of positive emotions (low- vs. high-arousal, e.g., contentment vs. amusement; or self- vs. other-focused, e.g., pride vs. love) are differentially induced by this practice; and (c) the effects of LKM on positive emotions increase (because of practice) or decrease (because of adaptation) over time. We are aware of only one other field experiment that has tested the effects of LKM. Carson et al. (2005) compared a group of chronic pain patients who were taught LKM (n ⫽ 18) with a group receiving standard care (n ⫽ 25). Results from this pilot trial indicated that LKM reduced pain, anger, and psychological distress. The present study tests LKM in a larger sample, with a wider variety of outcome measures. Most critically, it gathers detailed data on positive emotions as a potential mediator of the benefits of this form of meditation. Overview of Empirical Strategy We conducted a randomized, longitudinal field experiment to test whether positive emotions, induced through LKM, build consequential personal resources. In designing our experiment, we grappled with selecting the most appropriate comparison condition. In laboratory research, we have used sham meditation (i.e., sitting with eyes closed) to achieve precise experimental control. For a 7-week intervention that asked participants for a substantial investment of time and effort, both ethical and face-validity concerns led us away from this sort of placebo meditation. Another approach is to choose a comparison condition that best addresses the current state of knowledge in a given area. Our review of the scientific literature had uncovered no published evidence that LKM could produce sustained increases in positive emotions and only limited and indirect evidence that positive emotions could build personal resources. Given this embryonic state of evidence, an appropriate initial comparison group would reflect treatment as usual, which, outside the clinical literature, is perhaps better phrased as life as usual. Thus, we chose a waitlist control design, which can assess treatment efficacy while controlling for selfselection, history, maturation, regression to the mean, and the effects of repeated testing (Chambless & Hollon, 1998; Kazdin, 2003). Although the groups differ in terms of experimenter demand, delivery format, and expectation of improvement, we address these limitations procedurally and analytically to the extent possible (see Discussion). 1047 In the context of a workplace wellness program, we offered a 7-week meditation workshop to employees interested in stress reduction and willing to respond to questionnaires and provide daily, web-based reports of their emotions. All volunteers completed an initial survey that assessed their life satisfaction, depressive symptoms, and status on a range of personal resources. Volunteers were then randomly assigned to either our meditation workshop or a waitlist control group (which received the same workshop after the study ended). Over the next 9 weeks (including 1 week before and after the workshop), participants in both groups completed daily reports of their emotion experiences and meditation practice. About 2 weeks after the workshop ended, participants completed a final survey that reassessed their life satisfaction, depressive symptoms, and status on the same personal resources measured previously. In addition to daily reports of emotion experiences, which may well underestimate the frequency of emotion experiences, at the time of the final survey, participants also completed a detailed account of the emotions they experienced that particular day using the day reconstruction method (DRM; Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2004). The DRM is a survey method that builds on the strengths of two older methods: time-use assessment and momentary data capture (i.e., experience sampling). Like each of these earlier methods, the DRM minimizes recall biases and provides a comprehensive picture of daily experience. Participants first reconstruct a detailed diary of “this morning” by dividing it into sequences of episodes. Next, they complete a series of questions, including emotion reports, for each episode of their morning. We predicted that participation in the 7-week LKM workshop would increase individuals’ daily experiences of positive emotions, over time across the 9 weeks of daily reporting and within the specific morning targeted by the DRM. Drawing from the broaden-and-build theory, we further predicted that increases in positive emotions, produced by LKM, would, in turn, build participants’ personal resources. To test the generality of the build effect of positive emotions, we targeted a wide range of personal resources, including cognitive resources (e.g., mindfulness, the ability to savor positive experiences), psychological resources (e.g., ego-resilience, environmental mastery), social resources (e.g., positive relations with others, social support given and received), and physical resources (e.g., illness symptoms, duration of sleep). Finally, we investigated whether these resources actually made a difference in participants’ lives. To do so, we tested whether any increments in resources, in turn, contributed to changes in overall life satisfaction, a judgment of fulfillment and well-being that differs from positive affectivity in its global focus and cognitive emphasis (Lucas, Diener, & Suh, 1996). As a secondary way to assess whether newly built resources were consequential, we tested whether they led to decreases in depressive symptoms. We distill this series of predictions into the following overarching mediational hypothesis: Hypothesis: Becoming skilled in LKM will, over time, increase people’s daily experiences of positive emotions, which, in turn, build a variety of personal resources that hold positive consequences for the person’s mental health and overall life satisfaction. Figure 1 portrays the conceptual model that underlies the build hypothesis as we tested it here. Note that this study does not directly assess momentary changes in broadened cognition, be- FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL 1048 Figure 1. Conceptual model depicting predicted causal paths between loving-kindness meditation, change in positive emotions, change in resources, and change in life satisfaction. cause of the lack of valid measures that could be used repeatedly and in the field, nor does it directly assess the cognitive shifts produced by LKM that trigger positive emotions. As such, this study evaluates positive emotions as a mechanism for the effects of LKM but does not further decompose the mechanisms by which LKM and positive emotions exert their influence. Method Participants The study was conducted at the Compuware Corporation, a large business software and information technology services company in Detroit, Michigan. All full-time employees working at Compuware’s Detroit headquarters (approximately 1,800 individuals, 38% female, 34% ethnic minorities) received an e-mail message from Compuware executives inviting them to participate in the study.1 The study was described as a scientific investigation of “the benefits of meditation. . . [to] reduce stress.” The e-mail included a link to a website where employees could learn more about the project. The information made clear that the study was being conducted by university researchers, that the results would be confidential, and that the choice of whether to participate would not affect their standing with their employer. Two hundred two Compuware employees attended the study orientation, gave their consent, and completed the initial survey. Of these, 102 were assigned to the LKM group and 100 were assigned to the waitlist control group. Participants were excluded from analyses for the following reasons: (a) They violated random assignment (n ⫽ 7), (b) they failed to complete Time 2 measures (n ⫽ 27), (c) they were assigned to the meditation condition but attended fewer than three of the six weekly classes (n ⫽ 5), or (d) they completed fewer than 30 of the 61 daily reports (n ⫽ 24). In total, 63 participants were excluded, 34 from the LKM group and 29 from the waitlist group. Attrition and disqualification affected the LKM and waitlist groups equally, 2(1, N ⫽ 202) ⫽ 0.4, p ⫽ .51, and was comparable with other studies on meditation (Carson et al., 2005; Davidson et al., 2003; Teasdale et al., 2000). The final sample, then, consisted of 139 participants, 67 of whom were in the LKM group and 72 of whom were in the waitlist control group. Demographic information is presented in Table 1. The compositions of the initial and completer samples were similar: Most participants were female, most had bachelor’s or master’s degrees, and the average age was 41 years (SD ⫽ 9.6). The completer sample was 65.5% female, 73.7% White, 9.5% Black, 8.8% South Asian, 6.6% East Asian, 0.7% Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, and 0.7% Hispanic. Male participants were disproportionately lost to attrition and disqualification, 2(1, N ⫽ 180) ⫽ 10.9, p ⫽ .001. There was also a trend towards loss of married participants, 2(1, N ⫽ 178) ⫽ 3.2, p ⫽ .07. These groups, however, were lost equally between conditions (waitlist ⫽ 64% female, meditators ⫽ 67% female), 2(1, N ⫽ 139) ⫽ .17, p ⫽ .69, (waitlist ⫽ 56% married, meditators ⫽ 60% married), 2(1, N ⫽ 137) ⫽ .22, p ⫽ .67, implying that married and male participant attrition related to the study in general and not to LKM. Otherwise, the initial and completer samples did not differ on demographic characteristics, condition assignment, or depression and life-satisfaction scores ( p ⬎ .24). Four participants in the completer sample had a meditation practice at the start of the study. Although these participants were higher than others on positive emotions throughout the study, removing their data did not alter the pattern of findings reported here. In addition to providing access to the participant pool, Compuware supported this study in multiple ways. All study orientation meetings and meditation workshop sessions were held during business hours at Compuware’s Detroit office. The meditation workshops were offered free of charge to all interested employees. Compuware also provided employee release time so that participants could attend a study orientation meeting, six meditation workshop sessions, and complete all web surveys during work time, without loss of compensation. Participants received monetary compensation for time spent on study measures. They received $10 for completing the initial survey, $20 for completing the final survey, and $1 for each daily report. In addition, participants who completed daily reports for at least 40 of the 61 days received a $10 bonus and a copy of a popular book on meditation by Jon Kabat-Zinn (valued at $24.95). The total possible payment for the study was $101, plus the book. Procedure All study orientation sessions were held during employees’ lunch hour, in a large auditorium on Compuware premises. At orientation, Barbara L. Fredrickson or Michael A. Cohn introduced interested employees to the rationale for investigating the effects of meditation on health and well-being. We sought to enhance prospective participants’ investment in the study by describing benefits of meditation already featured in the popular press and regularly used to draw attendees to comparable workplace wellness courses, specifically, the potential to reduce stress and improve health and well-being. We also described the timeline of the study and the details of compensation and explained the value of gathering data from a waitlist control group. We did not describe LKM, the broaden-and-build theory, our hypotheses regarding mediation by daily positive emotions, or other information that might have created detailed expectancy or demand effects. Those who could not attend an orientation session received information by phone. Within the week following orientation, interested employees logged on to a secure website, gave consent to participate in the 1 The population was limited to those Compuware employees with a Compuware e-mail address. This included executives, developers, and administrators, but not maintenance workers or cleaning staff. POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES 1049 Table 1 Participant Demographics Participant characteristic Intent-to-treat Per-protocol Completersa N % providing demographic informationb % in meditation group % female Agec Education levelc % married Incomec ($) Depressiond (CES–D, full scale) Baseline Posttest Life satisfaction (SWLS) Baseline Posttest % White (non-Hispanic) 195 88.2 49.2 59.8 41 Bachelor’s degree 60.5 ⬎85,000 175 93.9 43.4 60.8 41 Bachelor’s degree 59.8 ⬎85,000 139 100.0 48.2 65.5 41 Bachelor’s degree 57.7 ⬎85,000 16.1 12.7 15.4 12.4 15.9 12.8 4.12 4.42 73.7 4.17 4.46 73.3 4.10 4.50 72.6 Note. CES–D ⫽ Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression Measure (Radloff, 1977). SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). a For exclusion criteria, see Methods section. b Twenty-three participants declined to provide demographic information. Median and percentage calculations use only participants who provided data. Group-assignment data were available for all participants. c Value reported is median. d To facilitate comparison with previously published work, we report values that represent scores based on the full CES–D scale, including both positively and negatively worded items. In subsequent analyses, we omit the positively worded items to minimize conceptual overlap with positive emotions. study, and responded to the initial (T1) survey (described below). Participants learned their group assignment (meditation workshop or waitlist control) only after completing the T1 survey. The daily reporting phase of the study began 1 week following orientation and continued for approximately 9 weeks. Each day, participants visited our secure website to complete a short report on their emotions and time spent in “meditation, prayer, or solo spiritual activity” over the past day. After approximately 1 week of baseline reporting, workshop classes and daily practice began for the meditation group (described below). Daily reporting continued for approximately 1 week after the meditation workshop ended. After the daily reporting phase ended, the final (T2) survey became available online. Participants visited our website a final time and completed the same measures as at T1, followed by a day reconstruction (described below) and a demographics questionnaire. After data collection was completed, participants received debriefing information explaining more about the details of the study.2 Approximately 2 months later, meditation classes began for the waitlist control group. No further data were collected at that time. The websites for the initial questionnaires and the daily reports were available around the clock. The final survey was available only between noon and 2:00 a.m., because of the specifics of the DRM. Although participants were encouraged to complete the surveys at work, they were asked to practice meditation at home. Participants who missed more than three consecutive weekday report forms, or who did not fill out the final survey, received an automated e-mail reminder asking them to visit our website. The study team did not otherwise initiate contact with participants. LKM Workshops The meditation training involved six 60-minute group sessions (held over 7 weeks, because of religious holidays) with 20 –30 participants per group. All sessions were led by a stressmanagement specialist (Sandra M. Finkel) with extensive experience practicing and teaching LKM. The median number of sessions attended was five (M ⫽ 4.3, SD ⫽ 1.8). At the first session, participants were given a CD that included three guided meditations of increasing scope, led by the workshop instructor. During Week 1, participants practiced a meditation directing love and compassion toward themselves. During Week 2, the meditation added loved ones. During subsequent weeks, the meditation built from self, to loved ones, to acquaintances, to strangers, and finally, to all living beings. The first meditation lasted 15 min, and the final one lasted 22 min. Each workshop session included 15–20 min for a group meditation, 20 min to check on participants’ progress and answer questions, and 20 min for a didactic presentation about features of the meditation and how to integrate concepts from the workshop into one’s daily life. Participants were assigned to practice LKM at home, at least 5 days per week, with the guided recordings. The text of the guided meditations and week-by-week content outlines are available by request from Sandra M. Finkel.3 Measures Cognitive Resources: T1 and T2 Mindfulness and Awareness Scale. The Mindfulness and Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003) assesses awareness of 2 We contacted participants who did not complete the T2 survey to request demographic information and to make the debriefing information available. 3 Sandra M. Finkel can be reached by e-mail at smfinkel@med .umich.edu 1050 FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL one’s circumstances, as well as tendencies towards automated, “mindless” behavior or acting on “autopilot.” Participants indicate the frequency of 15 behaviors on a 6-point scale (1 ⫽ almost always, 6 ⫽ almost never). Items include “I snack without being aware of what I am eating” and “I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some time later.” All items are reverse-scored. (␣T1 ⫽ .86, ␣T2 ⫽ .89). Agency thinking and pathways thinking. We used the Trait Hope Scale (Snyder et al., 1991; Snyder, Rand, & Sigmon, 2002) to assess these two cognitive components of Snyder’s hope theory. Participants use a 4-point scale to indicate agreement or disagreement (1 ⫽ definitely false, 4 ⫽ definitely true) with 10 items divided between two subscales: agency thinking (belief that one has been/will be personally able to achieve one’s goals), including “I meet the goals I set for myself” (␣T1 ⫽ .84, ␣T2 ⫽ .81), and pathways thinking (belief that there are multiple ways to achieve one’s goals), including “There are lots of ways around any problem” (␣T1 ⫽ .84, ␣T2 ⫽ .83). Savoring Beliefs Inventory. The Savoring Beliefs Inventory (Bryant, 2003) assesses one’s tendency to enjoy pleasant experiences in the moment (savoring the present), pleasurably anticipate them beforehand (savoring the future), and pleasurably recall them afterward (savoring the past). Participants indicate agreement on a 7-point scale with 24 items, including “It’s easy for me to rekindle the joy from pleasant memories” and “When I think about a pleasant event before it happens, I often start to feel uneasy or uncomfortable” (reverse scored; savoring the past, ␣T1 ⫽ .88, ␣T2 ⫽ .92; savoring the present, ␣T1 ⫽ .88, ␣T2 ⫽ .89; savoring the future, ␣T1 ⫽ .87, ␣T2 ⫽ .91). Psychological Resources: T1 and T2 Life Orientation Test. The Life Orientation Test—Revised (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) is a 6-item scale that assesses generalized optimism as the belief that positive things are possible in the future. Participants indicate agreement or disagreement on a 5-point scale (1 ⫽ I agree a lot, 5 ⫽ I disagree a lot) with 10 statements (4 items are fillers), including “In uncertain times, I usually expect the best” and “If something can go wrong for me, it will” (reverse scored; ␣T1 ⫽ .82, ␣T2 ⫽ .79). Ego-resilience. The ego-resilience measure (Block & Kremen, 1996) assesses the ability to bounce back from adversity and flexibly adapt to shifting demands. Participants indicate agreement or disagreement on a 4-point scale with 14 items, including “I quickly get over and recover from being startled” and “I like to do new and different things” (␣T1 ⫽ .73, ␣T2 ⫽ .74). Psychological well-being. We measured five additional psychological resources using subscales of Ryff’s (1989) broader psychological well-being measure. Participants indicate agreement on a 6-point scale (1 ⫽ strongly disagree, 6 ⫽ strongly agree) with seven to eight items for each of the following five subscales: personal growth, with items like “For me, life has been a continuous process of learning, changing, and growth” (␣T1 ⫽ .76, ␣T2 ⫽ .80); environmental mastery, with items like “I often feel overwhelmed by my responsibilities” (reverse scored; ␣T1 ⫽ .78, ␣T2 ⫽ .80); autonomy, with items like “I am not afraid to voice my opinions, even when they are in opposition to the opinions of most people” (␣T1 ⫽ .72, ␣T2 ⫽ .77); self-acceptance, with items like “I like most parts of my personality” (␣T1 ⫽ .88, ␣T2 ⫽ .86); and purpose in life, with items like “My daily activities often seem trivial and unimportant to me” (reverse scored; ␣T1 ⫽ .80, ␣T2 ⫽ .80). Social Resources: T1 and T2 Dyadic Adjustment Scale. The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976) measures social support as the amount of emotional support the participant provides to and receives from close others. Using a 5-point scale (0 ⫽ not at all, 4 ⫽ an extreme amount), participants respond to questions, including “On the whole, how much do your friends and relatives make you feel loved and cared for?” and “If one of your close friends got sick or were injured in a car accident, how much could they count on you to take care of them?” Items are divided into subscales for social support given (␣T1 ⫽ .81, ␣T2 ⫽ .81) and social support received (␣T1 ⫽ .83, ␣T2 ⫽ .83). Positive relations with others. Our third index of social resources was drawn from Ryff’s (1989) psychological well-being scale (see above). The 7-item subscale includes items like, “I know that I can trust my friends, and they know they can trust me” and “I often feel lonely because I have few close friends with whom to share my concerns” (reverse scored; ␣T1 ⫽ .81, ␣T2 ⫽ .81). Physical Resources: T1 and T2 Illness symptoms. This self-report measure assesses 13 common symptoms of illness or poor health, including headaches, chest pain, congestion, and weakness (Elliot & Sheldon, 1998). Participants use a 7-point scale to rate the frequency of each symptom over the past month (1 ⫽ not at all, 7 ⫽ very frequently; ␣T1 ⫽ .82, ␣T2 ⫽ .84). Sleep duration. This single item, extracted from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989), asks participants to respond to the question “During the past month, how many hours of actual sleep did you get at night?” Outcome Measures: T1 and T2 Satisfaction with life scale. We assessed cognitive evaluations of life satisfaction with this five-item scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). It assesses participants’ global satisfaction with their lives and circumstances. Participants indicate agreement with each item on a 7-point scale, including “So far I have gotten the important things I want in life” (␣T1 ⫽ .88, ␣T2 ⫽ .90). Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression Measure. We assessed depressive symptoms with the Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression Measure (Radloff, 1977). We excluded the four positively worded items to minimize conceptual overlap with positive emotions (see Moskowitz, 2003; Ostir et al., 2000). On a 5-point scale, participants indicated how often they had felt symptoms of depression in the past week (0 ⫽ never, 4 ⫽ most of the time), including “I felt that I could not shake off the blues even with help from my family or friends” (␣T1 ⫽ .86, ␣T2 ⫽ .88). Emotions and Meditation Practice: Daily Assessments During daily reports, participants completed the Modified Differential Emotions Scale (mDES; Fredrickson et al., 2003). The mDES asks participants to recall the past 24 hr and rate POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES their strongest experience of each of 19 specific emotions on a 4-point scale (0 ⫽ not at all, 4 ⫽ extremely). The emotions listed were amusement, anger, awe, compassion, contempt, contentment, disgust, embarrassment, gratitude, hope, joy, interest, love, pride, guilt, sadness, shame, fear, and surprise. Participants also reported whether they had engaged in “meditation, prayer, or solo spiritual activity” since the last time they filled out the survey (not necessarily the same 24-hr time span as mDES responses). Both meditation and waitlist participants responded to these questions. DRM: T2 1051 2. an intent-to-treat sample (n ⫽ 195), comprising all of the participants who were successfully randomly assigned to experimental condition; and 3. a per-protocol sample (n ⫽ 175), comprising (a) all of the participants successfully randomly assigned to the waitlist control condition (n ⫽ 98) and (b) those participants assigned to LKM who received a predetermined “minimum effective dose” of LKM training (at least three of the six weekly loving-kindness sessions; n ⫽ 77). Analyses with the complete data sample are described below. At the end of the section, analyses with the other samples are discussed. We used the DRM (Kahneman et al., 2004) to assess participants’ time-varying emotion experiences during a specific day. Because of time constraints, we limited our assessment to the morning of the targeted day. We asked participants to divide their morning—from the time they awoke until they completed lunch—into a continuous series of episodes and to provide a descriptive label for each episode. We allowed a maximum of 10 episodes. Thereafter, participants revisited each labeled episode to provide ratings from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) for the emotion adjectives from the mDES, as described above (Fredrickson et al., 2003). For each episode, participants were also asked “What were you doing?” followed by a checklist of several activities that included “praying/worshiping/meditating.” They also responded “yes” or “no” to the question, “Were you interacting with anyone (including on the phone, in a teleconference, etc)?” Time spent in “meditation, prayer, or solo spiritual activity” was assessed each day. As expected, during the baseline period, meditators and control participants did not differ in duration of meditative activity, t(135) ⫽ ⫺0.25, p ⫽ .80 (Ms ⫽ 13 and 12 min/week, respectively). Beginning with Week 1 of the study, and for each subsequent week, participants in the LKM group engaged in significantly more meditative activity than did those in the control group, averaging about 80 min/week, although this dropped to about 60 min/week after the workshop ended. Results Effects of LKM on Emotions Overview of Data Analytic Strategy Given the complexity of the data set, we performed a range of analyses, which we forecast here.4 As a manipulation check, we used t tests to confirm that participants in the LKM condition were, in fact, meditating and were meditating more than the control participants. A series of hierarchical linear models, with time nested within individual—also known as growth models— investigated the impact of experimental condition, passage of time, and time spent meditating on self-reported emotions. An additional set of analyses examined participants’ emotions within a single morning, incorporating information about the amount of time that participants had meditated over the course of the study and whether they had meditated on the particular morning in question. We then tested the build hypothesis in a combined latent growth curve and path-analysis structural equation model (SEM). The growth curve for positive emotions from the hierarchical linear model analyses was reparameterized as a SEM-based latent trajectory model. In the path-analysis portion of the model, baseline positive emotions and slope of change in positive emotions predicted change in the targeted resource, which then predicted change in life satisfaction or depression. Each of the 18 resources we measured was tested in a separate model.5 Results were analyzed separately in three samples: 1. individuals who adhered to the study requirements described above (our “complete data” sample, n ⫽ 139); Manipulation Check Did Participants in the Meditation Condition Comply With Instructions to Meditate? Did LKM Impact Positive Emotions Over the Course of the Study? We averaged measurements for nine positive emotions— amusement, awe, contentment, joy, gratitude, hope, interest, love, and pride—within each day, and then we averaged these daily means over the week to create a composite positive emotions variable for each week of the study. Across weeks, this index score had an average alpha coefficient of .94 (range ⫽ .94 –.95). The impact of LKM on positive emotions over time was tested using hierarchical linear modeling, with time nested within individual. Experimental condition, week in the study, and their interaction were included as predictors. The model also included random effects for the intercept, which represented each participant’s 4 Preliminary analyses incorporated sex of participant as a predictor. It did not significantly predict positive emotions, the impact of experimental condition on positive emotions, or the impact of experimental condition on positive emotions over time. In addition, it was not related to the constructs we examine in subsequent models. For this reason, all reported analyses collapse across male and female participants, and we do not consider the impact of participant sex further. 5 We explored whether we might reduce the number of models tested by considering the 18 different resources assessed as indicators of either one latent “resources” factor, or four latent factors distinguished by type of resource (e.g., cognitive, psychological, social, and physical resources). However, confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses suggested that no such reduction was warranted. The correlation matrix is available on request. FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL 1052 level of positive emotions at baseline, and for the impact of week in the study, which represented each participant’s change in positive emotions over time. Both random effects were significant (intercept variance ⫽ 0.34, SE ⫽ 0.05, p ⬍ .0001; week variance ⫽ 0.002, SE ⫽ 0.0006, p ⫽ .0002), indicating that participants varied in their baseline levels of positive emotions and showed differing rates of change over time. The fixed effects for experimental condition and week were not significant, but their interaction was (b ⫽ 0.041, SE ⫽ 0.011, p ⫽ .0004). Thus, neither time nor condition alone predicted positive emotions, but over time, a difference between conditions emerged (see Figure 2). We probed the interaction by treating time as the focal predictor and experimental condition as the moderating variable (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006). These analyses revealed that time did not significantly predict positive emotions for control participants (b ⫽ ⫺0.008, SE ⫽ 0.0079, p ⫽ .31) but did significantly predict positive emotions for participants in the LKM condition (b ⫽ 0.03, SE ⫽ 0.008, p ⫽ .0001). Thus, these results confirm that LKM increased participants’ positive emotions over the course of the study. We then tested similar growth models for each of the nine positive emotions included in the composite. In all cases, neither main effect was significant, but their interaction was significant. (The sole exception to this was that interest also showed both main effects; see Table 2.) These results suggest that the findings for the composite positive emotions variable were not determined by any single positive emotion and that it is appropriate to consider the positive emotions collectively. We tested an additional growth model that examined compassion over the duration of the study. Neither the main effects for experimental condition and week, nor their interaction (b ⫽ 0.021, SE ⫽ 0.016, p ⫽ .21), was significant. Visual inspection revealed the same pattern for compassion as for the positive emotions, but the increase over time for meditators was not statistically significant. amine any changes in the impact of meditation practice on positive emotions over the course of the study, we entered meditation practice for each week of the study as a separate variable. We deliberately left experimental condition in the model to test the unique contribution of time spent meditating each week, above and beyond the impact of participation in the workshop or interaction with the meditation instructor. Unexpectedly, time spent in “meditation, prayer, or solo spiritual activity” significantly predicted positive emotions during the baseline week before the workshops began ( p ⫽ .05), even when we excluded the participants who reported a preexisting meditation practice. After the first week of meditation instruction, time spent in meditative activity predicted positive emotions for all time points except Week 4 ( p ⫽ .08), even after we controlled for the other predictors in the model. These results are presented in Table 3.6 To estimate the impact of LKM instruction and practice on positive emotions, we tested a separate model with the meditators alone. By excluding the control participants, who were not receiving LKM instruction, we avoided diluting the estimate for the impact of “meditation, prayer, or solo spiritual activity” on positive emotions with non-LKM forms of spiritual practice. In this model, 1 hr of meditation practice during Week 2 was associated with a 0.06-unit increase in positive emotions (SE ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ .06) on the 5-point Likert scale described above. This value increased steadily during Weeks 3–7 of the study. By Week 7, each hour of meditation practice was associated with a 0.17-unit increase in positive emotions (SE ⫽ 0.03, p ⬍ .0001). These data suggest that the dose-response relationship between the practice of LKM and the experience of positive emotions tripled over the course of the study. Furthermore, even though meditation practice dropped after the workshop ended in Week 7, 1 hr of meditation practice in Week 8 still exerted approximately the same influence on positive emotions as it had in Week 7 (b ⫽ 0.18, SE ⫽ 0.05, p ⫽ .0004). What Role Did Individual Effort Play in the Impact of the Intervention on Positive Emotions? Did LKM Influence Negative Emotions Over the Course of the Study? The impact of LKM on positive emotions might be expected to be a function not only of experimental condition but also of individual effort put into daily practice. We tested a growth model for positive emotions that included the number of hours of meditation practice each week as a fixed effect, time-varying predictor, along with time and experimental condition. To allow us to ex- We also examined the impact of LKM on negative emotions over the course of the study. Negative emotions were indexed by a composite of daily ratings for anger, shame, contempt, disgust, embarrassment, guilt, sadness, and fear. Across weeks, this index score had an average alpha coefficient of .85 (range ⫽ .81–.90). As described above for positive emotions, the model included experimental condition, week in the study, Time ⫻ Condition interaction, and hours of meditation practice each week. None of the predictors were significant. Thus, neither experimental condition, week in the study, their interaction (b ⫽ ⫺0.011, SE ⫽ 0.011, p ⫽ .28), nor time spent meditating during any weeks of the study (range p ⫽ .11–.74) significantly influenced the negative emotions sampled in this study. 2.9 2.8 Control 2.7 Meditation 2.6 2.5 2.4 Ba se li W ne ee k W 1 ee k W 2 ee k W 3 ee k W 4 ee k W 5 ee k W 6 ee k W 7 ee k 8 Positive Emotions 3 Time Figure 2. Positive emotions by experimental condition. 6 To address concerns about multicollinearity between experimental condition and time spent meditating, we tested a separate model in which number of hours of meditation practice was group meancentered. The pattern of significant findings was identical, except that the impact of time spent meditating became nonsignificant for Week 3, as well as Week 4. Table 3 reports the uncentered meditation time values, for ease of interpretation. POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES 1053 Table 2 Impact of Loving-Kindness Meditation on Specific Positive Emotions Experimental condition Experimental condition ⫻ week Week Emotion Estimate SE p Estimate SE p Estimate SE p Amusement Awe Contentment Gratitude Hope Interest Joy Love Pride ⫺0.112 ⫺0.163 0.036 ⫺0.010 ⫺0.139 ⫺0.421 0.0005 0.060 ⫺0.249 0.125 0.123 0.120 0.141 0.127 0.136 0.124 0.134 0.1369 .37 .19 .76 .94 .28 .002 .997 .66 .07 ⫺0.012 ⫺0.0003 ⫺0.002 0.0006 ⫺0.006 ⫺0.022 ⫺0.013 ⫺0.009 ⫺0.016 0.009 0.010 0.011 0.010 0.010 0.011 0.010 0.010 0.010 .20 .97 .83 .96 .55 .05 .21 .33 .15 0.040 0.046 0.043 0.035 0.044 0.060 0.037 0.036 0.048 0.014 0.014 0.016 0.014 0.015 0.016 0.014 0.014 0.014 .003 .001 .006 .01 .003 .0002 .01 .009 .0008 Did LKM Influence Emotions Within a Targeted Morning? The DRM provided data on participants’ emotional experiences within the episodes of an ordinary morning. This offered a window into the impact of our intervention on emotional experiences in response to specific daily events, rather than emotions summarized over an entire day. Five participants did not provide DRM data, leaving 134 for analysis. There were 918 episodes recorded in total, with each participant reporting a mean of about seven episodes (M ⫽ 6.85, SD ⫽ 2.38). As with the daily reports, composite scores of positive and negative emotions were computed by taking the mean of positive items and negative items, respectively. Consistent with the daily reports, participants reported higher positive emotions (M ⫽ 1.16, SD ⫽ 0.15) than negative emotions (M ⫽ 0.15, SD ⫽ 0.28). Positive and negative emotions were largely uncorrelated (r ⫽ ⫺.06, p ⫽ .09). Multilevel random-coefficient regression modeling has been recommended for analyzing DRM data (Stone et al., 2006). We estimated a series of models predicting positive or negative emotions for a given episode from experimental condition, total number of hours engaged in meditative activity over the course of the study, the time of day of the episode, whether the episode included meditation, whether the episode included social interaction, and the interaction between social interaction and total hours of meditative activity. This Table 3 Impact of Experimental Condition, Week, and Time Spent Meditating on Positive Emotions Predictor Estimate SE p Intercept Experimental condition Week Experimental Condition ⫻ Week Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Baseline Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 1 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 2 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 3 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 4 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 5 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 6 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 7 Time Spent Meditating ⫺ Week 8 2.717 ⫺0.124 ⫺0.010 0.026 0.167 0.006 0.083 0.068 0.045 0.093 0.107 0.144 0.130 0.075 0.110 0.008 0.013 0.086 0.039 0.032 0.031 0.026 0.029 0.028 0.029 0.048 ⬍.0001 .26 .20 .04 .05 .88 .01 .03 .08 .002 .0001 ⬍.0001 .007 interaction term was included to explore whether LKM—which focused on kindness and compassion toward others— had a specific influence on the participant’s response to interactions with others. All quantitative predictors were mean centered. We established that the best fitting unconditional models for positive and negative emotions had significant random intercepts ( ps ⬍ .0001) and autoregressive covariance structures ( ps ⬍ .0001), indicating that participants began the day with significant variability in their levels of positive and negative emotions and that temporally close measures of emotion were more highly correlated than more distant measures. Time of day positively predicted positive emotions (b ⫽ 0.065, SE ⫽ 0.009, p ⬍ .0001), whereas no time trend emerged for negative emotions (b ⫽ 0.002, SE ⫽ 0.003, p ⫽ .54). These findings are consistent with diurnal rhythms of positive emotions, which have been found to peak at noon (Stone et al., 2006). Experimental condition was not significant for either positive or negative emotions (b ⫽ 0.067, SE ⫽ 0.118, ns, and b ⫽ ⫺0.082, SE ⫽ 0.048, ns, respectively). We next tested the total number of hours spent in meditative activity (over the previous 9 weeks) as a predictor of emotional experiences during the episodes of the targeted morning.7 A positive effect of time spent meditating on positive emotions emerged, above and beyond the effect of time (b ⫽ 0.033, SE ⫽ 0.010, p ⫽ .0008). This was not true for negative emotions (b ⫽ ⫺0.005, SE ⫽ 0.004, p ⫽ .2064). Hence, time spent in meditative activity over the previous 9 weeks was associated with more frequent positive emotions and no change in negative emotions across episodes within the targeted morning. We do not consider negative emotions further. A small number of participants (n ⫽ 9) indicated in their DRM responses that they had engaged in meditative activity that morning. To assess whether the target day’s meditative activity alone could account for the significant effects on positive emotions reported above, we reran the models in two ways. First, we added meditation at episode as a time-varying predictor to test the effects of engaging in meditative activity on positive emotions experienced during that same episode. Second, in place of the episodelevel predictor, we added a dummy variable indicating whether or 7 It is not surprising that experimental condition and time spent meditating were highly correlated, r(139) ⫽ 0.71, p ⬍ .0001. Thus, we examined them separately as predictors of emotions within the morning targeted by the DRM. 1054 FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL not participants meditated that day to test the effects of engaging in meditative activity on positive emotions experienced that day. Meditating during an episode predicted higher positive emotions during that episode (b ⫽ 0.39, SE ⫽ 0.17, p ⫽ .0207) but did not change the effect of hours engaged in meditative activity over the previous 9 weeks on positive emotions experienced that morning (b ⫽ 0.033, SE ⫽ 0.010, p ⫽ .0008). Meditating any time that morning also predicted positive emotions experienced that morning (b ⫽ 0.52, SE ⫽ 0.23, p ⫽ .0247) but also did not change the effect of total hours meditated throughout the study (b ⫽ 0.029, SE ⫽ 0.010, p ⫽ .0031). Thus, we can attribute much of the increase in positive emotions on this particular day to the time participants had spent meditating over the last several weeks. Taken together, these DRM findings indicate that (a) meditation produces positive emotions during meditation practice; (b) these positive emotions persist after the meditation session has ended; and (c) over time, repeated LKM practice produces a cumulative increase in positive emotions that appears on subsequent days, whether or not the individual meditates on that day. Previous research has shown that, in general, people experience more intense positive emotions when interacting with others than when alone (McIntyre, Watson, Clark, & Cross, 1991). We explored whether time spent meditating over the previous 9 weeks differentially influenced participants’ experiences of positive emotions, depending on whether they were interacting with others or not. We tested a model with time of day, social interaction, time spent meditating over the previous 9 weeks, and the interaction of social interaction and time spent meditating as predictors. The slope for social interaction was allowed to vary (Varslope ⫽ 0.052, SE ⫽ 0.25, p ⫽ .0187), confirming that interacting with others predicted positive emotions differentially across individuals. Beyond the effects of time and hours spent in meditation, episodelevel social interactions (b ⫽ 0.232, SE ⫽ 0.059, p ⬍ .0001) and the interaction between time spent meditating and social interactions (b ⫽ 0.014, SE ⫽ 0.006, p ⫽ .0363) predicted positive emotions in that morning. That is, more time spent meditating was associated with higher positive emotions, and this effect was stronger during social interactions. Testing the Build Hypothesis We tested the full build hypothesis by combining a growth model for positive emotions with an SEM path analysis. This combined model used the strengths of growth modeling, which considers individual trajectories of change over time, and path analyses, which can examine direct and indirect effects in mediational models. The growth model for positive emotions was reparameterized as a latent trajectory model in an SEM framework (Curran & Hussong, 2003). Experimental condition and time spent meditating during the week predicted positive emotions for each week of the study. Time spent meditating was entered as a timevarying predictor. An intercept and slope for positive emotions over the course of the study were created by allowing the indicators for positive emotions, representing positive emotions during each week of the study, to cross-load on both intercept and slope latent variables. The latent variable that reflected the intercept of positive emotions, at baseline, was created by fixing factor loadings for the indicators to 1.0. The latent variable that reflected change in positive emotions over the course of the study was then created by specifying factor loadings that reflected week in the study (0.0 ⫽ baseline, 1.0 ⫽ Week 1, 2.0 ⫽ Week 2, etc.). In the path-analysis portion of the model, the intercept, representing each participant’s initial level of positive emotions during the baseline week, and slope, representing each participant’s rate of change in positive emotions over time, predicted change in his or her resources between T1 and T2, which then predicted change in life satisfaction between T1 and T2. In other words, each participant’s baseline level of positive emotions and individual rate of change in positive emotions over the course of the study, calculated within the latent trajectory portion of the model, became predictors in the path-analysis portion of the models we tested. Given our experimental design, only change in positive emotions (i.e., slope) was predicted to build participants’ resources. Thus, we predicted that the path from slope of positive emotions to resources would be significant, but the path from baseline positive emotions to change in resources would not. The resource variable was a difference score that represented change between T1 and T2 in the specific resource featured within each model. We tested the model for each resource assessed, and this was the only variable that changed across models. Last, the life-satisfaction variable was also a difference score, representing change in life satisfaction between T1 and T2. Thus, the model examined whether initial positive emotions and changes in positive emotions over the course of the study predicted changes in resources over the course of the study, which, in turn, predicted changes in life satisfaction over the course of the study. Participants with greater increases in positive emotions were hypothesized to exhibit greater increases in resources and, in turn, life satisfaction. A diagram of the model tested is depicted in Figure 3. The model was tested for each of the 18 resources identified in Table 4, using LISREL 8.80 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). A multitude of factors may be associated with individual trajectories of change over time, therefore, it is rare for growth models or combined growth and path-analysis models to fit well when assessed using standard SEM fit indices, such as root-mean-square error of approximation (Widaman & Thompson, 2003). For this reason, it is noteworthy that each of the models we tested produced an estimated root-mean-square error of approximation of less than 0.08 (range ⫽ 0.068 – 0.076), indicating an acceptable fit to the data. Given that all of the models tested were acceptable fits to the data, and that overall model fit was influenced by the fit of the latent trajectory portion of the model (which was the same for each resource), we examined the significance of the individual path coefficients in the models for each resource to test the build hypothesis. As predicted, the path from baseline positive emotions to change in resources (Path A) was not significant for any of the resources, indicating that change in resources over the course of the study was not significantly affected by participants’ initial levels of positive emotions (see Table 4). The paths from change in positive emotions (i.e., slope) to change in resources (Path B) and from change in resources to change in life satisfaction (Path C) are central to the build hypothesis. These paths were significant for 9 of the 18 resources tested: mindfulness, pathways thinking, savoring the future, environmental mastery, self-acceptance, purpose in life, social support received, positive relations with others, and illness symptoms. In other words, increases in positive emotions over the course of the study were associated with significant POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES 1055 Figure 3. Combined latent trajectory and path-analysis model. Avg. daily pos. emo. ⫽ average daily positive emotion; PE ⫽ positive emotion; SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). increases in these resources, which were, in turn, associated with significant increases in life satisfaction. Table 4 presents the parameter estimates for all path coefficients tested. The first two columns of Table 5 present the amount of variance explained in the changes in resource and life satisfaction variables when the predicted build paths were significant. Six of the nine remaining resources showed significant paths influencing life satisfaction (Path C) but were not significantly influenced by change in positive emotions (Path B). These resources were agency thinking, savoring the past, savoring the present, optimism, personal growth, and autonomy. This suggests that these six measures are indeed consequential resources, even though increases in positive emotions did not significantly augment them. Did Changes in Positive Emotions Directly Influence Life Satisfaction, in Addition to Their Indirect Influence Through Built Resources? We examined the possibility that changes in positive emotions could exert a direct effect on increases in life satisfaction (Path D), in addition to the indirect effects via built resources (Paths B and C). To examine this, we tested a series of models that included a direct effect from change in positive emotions to change in life satisfaction. The “Path D” column in Table 4 presents the results for this path coefficient. (In Table 4, the columns for Paths A, B, and C report values for these path coefficients when Path D is not in the model.) The direct effect from change in positive emotions to change in life satisfaction was not significant for any of the models tested, nor did the model fit significantly improve when this path was included. For the nine resources that were found in previous analyses to exhibit the predicted pattern of significant build paths, these path coefficients remained significant when the direct effect of change in positive emotions on change in life satisfaction was included in the model. These results indicate that changes in positive emotions only produced changes in life satisfaction to the extent that they built personal resources. This further underscores the conceptual distinction between transient experiences of positive emotions and global judgments of life quality (Cohn et al., 2008; Diener et al., 2006). Did Experimental Condition and Time Spent Meditating Directly Impact Resources and Life Satisfaction, in Addition to the Impact They Exerted Via Their Influence on Changes in Positive Emotion? We also examined the possibility that experimental condition and amount of time spent meditating directly influenced changes in resources and life satisfaction, in addition to their indirect influence via positive emotions. We tested this possibility in a new series of models. For purposes of clarity, these paths are not represented in Figure 3, but they entail direct effects from experimental condition and from each week’s variable for time spent meditating to both change in resource and change in life satisfaction. These effects were generally nonsignificant, with values that varied depending upon the path and the resource being tested. There was one exception: The direct effect from time spent meditating in Week 2 to change in life satisfaction was significant for each of the 18 resources tested (e.g., mindfulness, b ⫽ 0.49, z ⫽ 2.47). Excluding this direct effect, there were other, isolated significant effects, which represented a total of 4% of the 360 path coefficients estimated, but there was no pattern to these effects, and they did not exceed the percentage of path coefficients that would be expected on the basis of chance alone. These results suggest that experimental condition and time spent meditating FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL 1056 Table 4 Resource Models Tested With Life Satisfaction as Outcome Variable RMSEA (90% CI) 2(137, N ⫽ 139) Mindfulnessa Agency thinking Pathways thinkinga Savoring the past Savoring the present Savoring the futurea 0.068 (0.051–0.083) 0.074 (0.058–0.089) 0.071 (0.056–0.087) 0.070 (0.053–0.085) 0.071 (0.055–0.086) 0.072 (.056–0.087) 224.58 ( p ⫽ .00) 241.15 ( p ⫽ .00) 234.24 ( p ⫽ .00) 228.99 ( p ⫽ .00) 232.88 ( p ⫽ .00) 235.94 ( p ⫽ .00) Optimism Ego-resilience Personal growth Environmental masterya Autonomy Self-acceptancea Purpose in lifea 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.073 (0.057–0.088) 0.070 (0.054–0.086) 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.072 (0.056–0.088) 0.076 (0.060–0.091) Psychological resources 243.70 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.06 (z ⫽ ⫺0.70) 0.04 (z ⫽ 0.38) 243.13 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.07 (z ⫽ ⫺0.83) 0.25 (z ⫽ 2.53) 237.88 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.00 (z ⫽ ⫺0.05) 0.14 (z ⫽ 1.42) 231.02 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.06 (z ⫽ 0.66) 0.33 (z ⫽ 3.37) 243.87 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.94) ⫺0.01 (z ⫽ ⫺0.07) 236.21 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.92) 0.27 (z ⫽ 2.77) 245.55 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.11 (z ⫽ 1.30) 0.29 (z ⫽ 2.95) Resource tested Path A Path B Path C Path D 0.20 (z ⫽ 2.04) 0.17 (z ⫽ 1.74) 0.22 (z ⫽ 2.25) 0.15 (z ⫽ 1.52) 0.18 (z ⫽ 1.87) 0.20 (z ⫽ 2.08) 0.25 (z ⫽ 3.04) 0.36 (z ⫽ 4.46) 0.24 (z ⫽ 2.94) 0.18 (z ⫽ 2.17) 0.30 (z ⫽ 3.72) 0.28 (z ⫽ 3.38) 0.12 (z ⫽ 1.24) 0.11 (z ⫽ 1.15) 0.12 (z ⫽ 1.19) 0.15 (z ⫽ 1.49) 0.12 (z ⫽ 1.19) 0.12 (z ⫽ 1.22) 0.26 (z ⫽ 3.10) 0.14 (z ⫽ 1.65) 0.30 (z ⫽ 3.75) 0.38 (z ⫽ 4.86) 0.18 (z ⫽ 2.15) 0.42 (z ⫽ 5.45) 0.40 (z ⫽ 5.09) 0.16 (z ⫽ 1.64) 0.14 (z ⫽ 1.42) 0.13 (z ⫽ 1.37) 0.06 (z ⫽ 0.64) 0.17 (z ⫽ 1.72) 0.05 (z ⫽ 0.58) 0.07 (z ⫽ 0.71) 0.15 (z ⫽ 1.77) 0.21 (z ⫽ 2.54) 0.36 (z ⫽ 4.54) 0.15 (z ⫽ 1.57) 0.13 (z ⫽ 1.28) 0.08 (z ⫽ 0.79) Cognitive resources ⫺0.10 (z ⫽ ⫺1.17) ⫺0.03 (z ⫽ ⫺0.38) ⫺0.03 (z ⫽ ⫺0.32) 0.05 (z ⫽ 0.51) ⫺0.13 (z ⫽ ⫺1.45) ⫺0.06 (z ⫽ ⫺0.74) Social resources Social support given 0.071 (0.055–0.087) 233.63 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.16 (z ⫽ 1.82) Social support receiveda 0.072 (0.056–0.087) 235.84 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.09 (z ⫽ ⫺0.98) Positive relations with othersa 0.071 (0.055–0.086) 232.06 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.01 (z ⫽ ⫺0.10) Illness symptomsa Duration of sleep 0.15 (z ⫽ 1.49) 0.25 (z ⫽ 2.54) 0.29 (z ⫽ 2.97) Physical resources 0.071 (0.055–0.086) 232.82 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.09 (z ⫽ ⫺1.01) ⫺0.24 (z ⫽ ⫺2.47) ⫺0.20 (z ⫽ ⫺2.37) 0.13 (z ⫽ 1.27) 0.072 (0.057–0.088) 236.80 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.11 (z ⫽ ⫺1.22) ⫺0.14 (z ⫽ ⫺1.35) 0.01 (z ⫽ 0.14) 0.18 (z ⫽ 1.78) Note. RMSEA ⫽ root-mean-square error of approximation; CI ⫽ confidence interval. Parameter estimates are reported in standardized units. Path D was tested in a separate set of models, for which the RMSEA values and parameter estimates for Paths A, B, and C were slightly different than those listed above. For purposes of brevity, we have not presented these slightly different values when Path D was incorporated in the model. a Model was a good fit for the data, and the predicted build-hypothesis paths (Paths B and C) were significant. exerted their influence on resources and life satisfaction because of their impact on positive emotions. Do Positive Emotions Influence Depressive Symptoms Through the Same Mechanism (i.e., Built Resources) by Which They Influence Life Satisfaction? To explore whether positive emotions might beneficially influence measures of negative psychological adjustment through the same mechanisms by which they influence life satisfaction, we tested a series of models for which depressive symptoms was the ultimate variable in the model, replacing life satisfaction. In these models, change in positive emotions predicted change in the resource, which, in turn, predicted change in depressive symptoms. Model fit, as determined by root-mean-square error of approximation, remained acceptable. In addition, the predicted build paths were significant for the same nine resources for which these paths were significant when life satisfaction was the ultimate variable in the models. These findings suggest that increases in positive emotions decrease depressive symptoms through the same mechanisms by which they increase life satisfaction: built resources. We also examined the possibility that positive emotion directly influenced depressive symptoms, in addition to its indirect impact via built resources. In the first set of models, we examined the significance of the direct effect from change in positive emotions to change in depressive symptoms (Path D). Unlike the results for life satisfaction, this path was significant for all models tested. In addition, the overall fit of the models significantly improved for all 18 resources when this path was included in the model ( p ⬍ .0025). Even so, the predicted build paths remained significant for eight of the nine previously significant resources. This pattern of results suggests that increases in positive emotions influenced the decline in depressive symptoms via both built resources and a direct impact on depressive symptoms. The one resource for which this was not the case was social support received, for which the mediated build paths were not significant when Path D was included. Table 6 presents the parameter estimates for these models. The last two columns of Table 5 present the amount of variance explained in the changes in resource and depressive symptoms variables when the predicted build paths were significant. In a second set of models, we examined the direct effects from experimental condition and time spent meditating each week to the changes in resources and depressive symptoms, for each of the resources tested. Although isolated paths were significant, these represented only 2.8% of the paths tested, and there was no discernible pattern to which paths were significant. Intent-to-Treat Analyses To test for possible effects of differential participant completion on our results, we repeated the analyses above using our intentto-treat and per-protocol samples. The impact of experimental condition over time on positive emotions was not significant in either the intent-to-treat, t(1380) ⫽ 1.37, p ⫽ .17, or per-protocol POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES 1057 Table 5 Variance Explained in Change in Resources and Change in Life Satisfaction and Depression for Significant Resources Life-satisfaction models Resource ⌬ resource R2 ⌬ life satisfaction R2 Depression models (negative symptoms only) ⌬ resource R2 ⌬ depression R2 Mindfulness Pathways thinking Savoring the future 0.06 0.05 0.05 Cognitive resources 0.06 0.06 0.08 0.06 0.05 0.05 0.22 0.15 0.16 Environmental mastery Self-acceptance Purpose in life 0.10 0.09 0.08 Psychological resources 0.15 0.18 0.16 0.10 0.09 0.08 0.25 0.24 0.29 Social support received Positive relations with others 0.08 0.08 Social resources 0.04 0.13 0.08 0.09 0.15 0.24 Illness symptoms 0.06 Physical resources 0.04 0.05 0.18 Note. The variance estimates reported for the life-satisfaction models is for models that include Paths A, B, and C, but not D, because this path was not significant for any of the life-satisfaction models tested. The variance reported for the depression models is for models that include Paths A, B, C, and D, because path D was significant for each of the depression models tested. Direct effects from experimental condition and time spent meditating to resource and life satisfaction/depression were not included in any of the models. samples, t(1380) ⫽ 1.58, p ⫽ .11, whereas it was significant in our completer sample (discussed above). The impact of time spent meditating on positive emotions remained significant in both samples, starting with the first week of instruction. The resources for which we found significant build paths (paths from positive emotion change to resource to life satisfaction; shown as Paths B and C in Figure 3) generally showed the same significant paths in the intent-to-treat and per-protocol samples. Positive emotions significantly predicted savoring the future only in the completer sample, and change in ego resilience significantly predicted change in life satisfaction and depression in the intent-to-treat and per-protocol samples, even though it did not do so in the completer sample. Overall, the hypothesis that positive emotions help people build consequential personal resources was supported in the intent-totreat analysis. However, these analyses suggest that conclusions about the efficacy of LKM may need to be restricted to individuals who invest adequate effort in training and practice (approximately 70% in this sample). Discussion The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001) states that, over time, recurrent experiences of positive emotions allow people to build consequential personal resources. The data reported here provide the first experimental test of the build hypothesis. The findings are clear cut: The practice of LKM led to shifts in people’s daily experiences of a wide range of positive emotions, including love, joy, gratitude, contentment, hope, pride, interest, amusement, and awe. These increases in positive emotions were evident both within the trajectories of change in daily emotions over the span of 9 weeks and within a detailed analysis of a given morning 2 weeks after formal training ended. These shifts in positive emotions took time to appear and were not large in magnitude, but over the course of 9 weeks, they were linked to increases in a variety of personal resources, including mindful attention, self-acceptance, positive relations with others, and good physical health. Moreover, these gains in personal resources were consequential: They enabled people to become more satisfied with their lives and to experience fewer symptoms of depression. Simply put, by elevating daily experiences of positive emotions, the practice of LKM led to long-term gains that made genuine differences in people’s lives. The conceptual model— drawn from the broaden-and-build theory and depicted in Figure 1—is unambiguously supported by the evidence reported here. Most important, positive emotions emerge as the clear centerpiece of the model. LKM was beneficial precisely because it helped people experience positive emotions; direct effects of LKM, circumventing the hypothesized build paths, were virtually nonexistent. Positive emotions emerged as the mechanism through which people build the resources that make their lives more fulfilling and help keep their depressive symptoms at bay. These data also echo a message from our recent work that unpacks the relationship between positive emotions and life satisfaction (Cohn et al., 2008). Although both can be considered facets of happiness or subjective well-being (Lucas et al., 1996), we found positive emotions, and not life satisfaction, to predict change in resources. Furthermore, the association between increased positive emotions and increased life satisfaction was fully mediated by resource building. This suggests that people judge their lives to be more satisfying and fulfilling, not because they feel more positive emotions per se, but because their greater positive emotions help them build resources for living successfully. FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL 1058 Table 6 Resource Models Tested With Depressive Symptoms (Negative Symptoms Only) as Outcome Variable Resource tested RMSEA (90% CI) 2(136, N ⫽ 139) Mindfulnessa Agency thinking Pathways thinkinga Savoring the past Savoring the present Savoring the futurea 0.070 (0.053–0.085) 0.074 (0.059–0.090) 0.072 (0.056–0.087) 0.069 (0.053–0.085) 0.073 (0.057–0.088) 0.072 (0.056–0.087) 227.53 ( p ⫽ .00) 240.88 ( p ⫽ .00) 233.37 ( p ⫽ .00) 226.33 ( p ⫽ .00) 237.09 ( p ⫽ .00) 233.93 ( p ⫽ .00) Optimism Ego-resilience Personal growth Environmental masterya Autonomy Self-acceptancea Purpose in lifea Social support given Social support received Positive relations with othersa Illness symptomsa Duration of sleep Path A Path B Path C Path D 0.20 (z ⫽ 2.06) 0.18 (z ⫽ 1.77) 0.22 (z ⫽ 2.23) 0.16 (z ⫽ 1.59) 0.19 (z ⫽ 1.89) 0.21 (z ⫽ 2.15) ⫺0.28 (z ⫽ ⫺3.88) ⫺0.27 (z ⫽ ⫺3.41) ⫺0.17 (z ⫽ ⫺2.02) ⫺0.23 (z ⫽ ⫺2.81) ⫺0.42 (z ⫽ ⫺5.62) ⫺0.19 (z ⫽ ⫺2.37) ⫺0.31 (z ⫽ ⫺3.06) ⫺0.30 (z ⫽ ⫺3.25) ⫺0.31 (z ⫽ ⫺3.22) ⫺0.32 (z ⫽ ⫺3.39) ⫺0.27 (z ⫽ ⫺3.04) ⫺0.31 (z ⫽ ⫺3.24) 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.075 (0.059–0.090) 0.071 (0.056–0.087) 0.072 (0.057–0.087) 0.073 (0.058–0.089) 0.076 (0.061–0.091) Psychological resources 241.11 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.06 (z ⫽ ⫺0.70) 0.04 (z ⫽ 0.40) 242.45 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.07 (z ⫽ ⫺0.83) 0.25 (z ⫽ 2.53) 241.56 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.00 (z ⫽ ⫺0.04) 0.14 (z ⫽ 1.45) 232.54 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.06 (z ⫽ 0.69) 0.33 (z ⫽ 3.39) 243.07 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.94) ⫺0.01 (z ⫽ 0.07) 237.81 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.93) 0.27 (z ⫽ 2.78) 246.05 ( p ⫽ .00) 0.12 (z ⫽ 1.33) 0.29 (z ⫽ 2.97) ⫺0.10 (z ⫽ ⫺1.27) ⫺0.02 (z ⫽ ⫺0.25) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺4.58) ⫺0.37 (z ⫽ ⫺4.61) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.98) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺4.36) ⫺0.43 (z ⫽ ⫺5.65) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺3.63) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺3.45) ⫺0.30 (z ⫽ ⫺3.35) ⫺0.24 (z ⫽ ⫺2.57) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺3.66) ⫺0.25 (z ⫽ ⫺2.71) ⫺0.24 (z ⫽ ⫺2.66) 0.071 (0.055–0.087) 0.072 (0.056–0.088) 231.87 ( p ⫽ .00) 234.70 ( p ⫽ .00) Social resources 0.16 (z ⫽ 1.81) ⫺0.08 (z ⫽ ⫺0.98) 0.16 (z ⫽ 1.59) 0.25 (z ⫽ 2.60) ⫺0.06 (z ⫽ ⫺0.69) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺3.58) ⫺0.15 (z ⫽ ⫺1.80) ⫺0.32 (z ⫽ ⫺3.21) 0.071 (0.055–0.086) 230.43 ( p ⫽ .00) ⫺0.01 (z ⫽ ⫺0.08) 0.30 (z ⫽ 3.02) ⫺0.35 (z ⫽ ⫺4.33) ⫺0.25 (z ⫽ ⫺2.71) 0.072 (0.056–0.088) 0.073 (0.057–0.088) 234.74 ( p ⫽ .00) 235.69 ( p ⫽ .00) Cognitive resources ⫺0.10 (z ⫽ ⫺1.18) ⫺0.03 (z ⫽ ⫺0.39) ⫺0.03 (z ⫽ ⫺0.31) 0.05 (z ⫽ 0.52) ⫺0.13 (z ⫽ ⫺1.46) ⫺0.07 (z ⫽ ⫺0.75) Physical resources ⫺0.09 (z ⫽ ⫺1.02) ⫺0.24 (z ⫽ ⫺2.40) 0.26 (z ⫽ 3.17) ⫺0.29 (z ⫽ ⫺3.09) ⫺0.10 (z ⫽ ⫺1.11) ⫺0.15 (z ⫽ ⫺1.48) ⫺0.13 (z ⫽ ⫺1.50) ⫺0.37 (z ⫽ ⫺3.87) Note. RMSEA ⫽ root-mean-square error of approximation; CI ⫽ confidence interval. Parameter estimates are reported in standardized units. Model was a good fit for the data, and all predicted build-hypothesis paths (Paths B and C) were significant. a Nine of the eighteen resources we tested fit the hypothesized build paths. Of the remaining nine, six showed changes in the expected direction on the build paths (see Table 4). We speculate that these six resources may be affected by positive emotions, albeit less strongly or more slowly than other resources, and not that the build hypothesis is categorically inapplicable to them. The resources that did show significant build effects might be loosely grouped into two categories. The first involves having a loving attitude toward oneself and others and includes selfacceptance, social support received, and positive relations with others. The second involves a feeling of competence about one’s life and includes pathways thinking, environmental mastery, purpose in life, and ego-resilience (which was influenced by positive emotions, although just shy of significantly influencing life satisfaction). We speculate that increases in positive emotions may impact these resources more rapidly and to a greater extent than others. This study confirms yet again that positive emotions are more than momentary good feelings. Laboratory experiments have documented that positive emotions broaden cognition (for a review, see Fredrickson & Cohn, 2008). Now we have evidence from a field experiment to document that positive emotions also place people on trajectories of growth, leaving them better able to ward off depressive symptoms and become ever more satisfied with life. This experiment also carries the inspiring implication that people can take deliberate action to cultivate meaningful experiences of positive emotion and reap these benefits as a result. This field experiment also further documents the benefits of meditation. When people initiated a practice of LKM, they enjoyed payoffs both immediately, in terms of self-generated positive emotions, and over time, in terms of increased resources and overall well-being. Meditators even experienced enhanced positive emotions in ordinary life situations, especially those involving other people. This substantiates the claim that this type of meditation changes the way people approach life. We found that the effects of LKM were specific to positive emotions, without a comparable decrease in negative emotions. This resembles the work of Teasdale et al. (2000), who anecdotally reported that their mindfulness-based protocol does not reduce negative emotions but, instead, alters responses to negative emotions that can lead to depression (Segal et al., 2002). In contrast, Carson et al. (2005) uncovered a marginal decrease in trait anger in their pilot study of LKM. They also observed reductions in anxiety and distress, but these may have been due to the study’s central outcome of pain amelioration. Davidson et al. (2003) also found a decrease in trait anxiety with mindfulness-based stress reduction but only weak support for changes in negative emotion. Future work will need to resolve these inconsistencies. Another curious finding was the null effect of LKM on selfratings of compassion. In hindsight, we speculate that our sole daily item for compassion (“In the past 24 hours, what is the most sympathy, concern or compassion you have felt?”) may have oriented respondents toward compassion felt in response to the suffering of others, rather than kindness or equanimity per se. If the suffering of others was not directly salient to participants on a daily basis, increases on this particular item may have been lim- POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES ited. We suggest that future work tailor measures of compassion to more directly reflect the teachings of contemplative traditions. A final puzzling finding was the initially lower level of positive emotions in the meditation group. We speculate that this difference reflects the difficulties of initiating any self-change effort, even if those changes are self-chosen. Consider the parallel to the perennial New Year’s resolution to lose weight to be healthier. At the peak of a person’s motivation to shed pounds, he or she might join a local gym. Then, days later, the person realizes that he or she must actually go to the gym and exercise. Starting a meditation practice may similarly involve a period of doing something unfamiliar, difficult, and draining without immediate rewards. Contemplative traditions have articulated five obstacles facing novice meditators, including craving, anger, boredom, restlessness, and doubt (Kabat-Zinn, 2005). These obstacles are thought to result from increased awareness of challenging inner states that may be commonly present, although not noticed during one’s typical busy and outward-directed focus. Indeed, nearly all attrition occurred during the initial weeks, when participants may not have been sufficiently “in shape” to feel competent at meditation or derive benefits from it. Yet if people can endure these first difficult weeks, meditation becomes more effective, and positive emotions begin to accumulate and compound, changing people for the better. Because we set out to develop a durable method of inducing positive emotions, the dose-response results we documented are particularly inspiring. We found that the amount of positive emotions participants gained per hour spent meditating increased over the course of the study, tripling from the first week to the last. Rather than becoming bored with or jaded to the effects of meditation, our participants seemed to be building a dependable skill for selfgenerating positive emotions again and again. These findings are especially noteworthy given that most of our participants were novice meditators and our meditation workshop lasted only 7 weeks. Limitations and Future Directions This study breaks new ground in several ways, which leaves ample room for future research to probe or refine its findings. First, the sample was predominantly White, educated, and motivated for selfchange. Mindfulness-based programs have shown widespread emotional and medical benefits in diverse populations and for individuals without prior interest in meditation (Kabat-Zinn, 1990), and it will be important to determine whether the same holds true for LKM. Second, the duration of the experiment was just over 10 weeks. In the future, it will be important to investigate the extent to which the resources endure beyond the end of the intervention or into periods of heightened stress or negative emotions. We found that after the formal workshop ended, time spent meditating and positive emotions decreased in tandem, even though meditation remained effective at evoking positive emotions. Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, and Schkade (2005) have argued that intentional activity is required to sustain gains in happiness. Future research will benefit from assessing the duration of gains or determinants of continued, independent practice. Finally, the current experiment did not include daily measures of broadened cognition, which would have allowed a more precise test of the proposed links between positive emotions, broadened thinking, and resources. Currently, no measures of broadening are valid, repeatable, and administrable outside the lab, but once one has been developed 1059 and validated, it will be an important contribution to this research program. Another necessity in future work will be to move beyond selfreport data to eliminate concerns of shared method variance. Implicit or behavioral measures, observer reports, and physiological markers will be especially useful. Specifically, researchers can track changes in emotions over time with implicit or behavioral measures of affect (Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005) or positivity bias (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005). In a more recent study of LKM, we obtained observer reports from peers identified by study participants. Preliminary analyses suggest that, as expected, observers judge meditators to be more helpful than control participants (Fredrickson, 2008). Romantic partners, supervisors and physicians would also be fruitful informants in future research. Finally, in current and planned work, we are investigating whether LKM produces changes in respiratory sinus arrhythmia, progesterone, and oxytocin, each of which has been linked to positive social relations (e.g., Brown et al., 2008; Eisenberg et al., 1995; Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham, & Light, in press). The comparison condition within this experiment was a waitlist control group. Although typical for initial tests of psychological interventions (e.g., Davidson et al., 2003; Teasdale et al., 2000), this experimental design can inadvertently create experimenter demand, expectation of improvement, or nonspecific effects related to delivery format. We address each possibility in turn: First, the explicit focus on love and kindness may have created demand to elevate self-reports of these emotions. However, our data indicated that (a) LKM was associated with changes in many positive emotions, not just the ones explicitly discussed; (b) guided meditations featured the terms “love” and “compassion” beginning in Week 1, yet the profile of changes in self-reported positive emotions (see Figure 2) shows that positive emotions did not significantly increase until Week 3; and (c) self-reported positive emotions fit into a full set of mediational pathways (see Figure 3), which participants were unlikely to intuit and use to shape their responses.8 Second, simply participating in a meditation workshop might create the expectation of improvement. These expectations might give rise to positive emotions, such as hope and confidence, a legitimate, though nonspecific, effect of the intervention. We underscore that the increase in positive emotions evident in the current study did not appear until Week 3 (see Figure 2), whereas placebo responses typically emerge rapidly (Scott et al., 2007). Third, nonspecific effects of delivery format, including contact with a caring instructor, group interaction, and weekly workrelease time might also have contributed to increases in positive emotions. However, we found that when controlling for group assignment, time spent meditating still predicted increases in positive emotions. Even among participants who received the nonspe8 Another way experimenter demand might have produced the results is if meditation participants gradually began to skip responding on days low in positive emotions. Meditation participants did respond less frequently over time (dropping from 5.2 to 4.6 responses per week), whereas waitlist participants did not, F(7, 132) ⫽ 3.75, p ⫽ .002. However, the week-by-week correlations between positive emotions and response frequency were very low in both groups. The sole significant correlation suggested that, if anything, the highest positive emotions were reported by participants who responded most frequently (i.e., least selectively). Also, recall that emotion measures were analyzed using per-participant means for each week, meaning that frequent responders did not contribute disproportionately to the data. FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL 1060 cific benefits, meditation itself—the proposed core of the intervention—predicted positive change. We also examined whether participants reported a boost in positive emotions on the day of workshop sessions or the day after, comparing waitlist participants, LKM participants who did not attend that week’s workshop, and LKM participants who did. The results did not differ from chance, suggesting that the higher positive emotions reported by LKM participants reflected a continuous upward trend, rather than a temporary response to the one day each week that involved time off of work, social support, and contact with the instructor. Overall, patterns in our data argue against spurious results arising from our use of a waitlist control group. Now that LKM has shown efficacy in increasing positive emotions and building personal resources, future work will be able to directly control for nonspecific effects and expectancies by comparing LKM with other meditative or self-change techniques. Another alternative explanation for our findings is that whatever positive emotions our participants were feeling at T2 cast a rosy glow over all their self-reports and artificially produced the appearance of growth in resources. The reports from the DRM provided an estimate of positive emotions for the day the T2 measures were completed. We regressed that day’s positive emotions on aggregate positive emotions over the 9 weeks of daily reports (R ⫽ .69) and created a residual term, representing positive emotions that were present at T2 and could have cast a rosy glow over responses but that were not present during the time resources were being built. We tested the residual variable in our mediational models, in place of change in positive emotions over time. It did not predict change in any of the resources. This suggests that positive emotions experienced over time exerted a gradual, cumulative effect, rather than simply biasing responding at the moment participants were responding to T2 questionnaires. Conclusion One of the most deflating concepts facing positive psychology is the hedonic treadmill (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman, 1978): Even though positive and negative events (e.g., winning the lottery, becoming paraplegic) temporarily alter levels of happiness, people quickly adapt to them and return to a fixed emotional set-point. The hedonic treadmill, as classically stated, implies that all efforts to improve happiness are doomed to failure. Yet more nuanced research (Diener et al., 2006) indicates that adaptation is not necessarily inevitable and may be strongest for negative affect and weaker for positive affect and life satisfaction. The evidence reported here reveals that one way to outpace the hedonic treadmill is to begin a practice of LKM. Participants who invested an hour or so each week practicing this form of meditation enhanced a wide range of positive emotions in a wide range of situations, especially when interacting with others. We find these data especially promising. LKM appears to be one positive emotion induction that keeps on giving, long after the identifiable “event” of meditation practice. Positive emotions feel good, and feelings like love, joy, and contentment can be valuable in and of themselves. Yet the broaden-and-build theory posits that natural selection sculpted our ancestors’ positive emotions to be useful in more far-reaching ways as well. These desirable states built resources that gave our ancestors’ an edge in circumstances that impinged on their sur- vival. To our knowledge, this is the first experiment to provide clear support for the build hypothesis. By random assignment, one group of individuals began a mind-training practice that increased their positive emotions and, in turn, their personal resources and well-being. Just as the broaden-and-build theory predicts, then, when people open their hearts to positive emotions, they seed their own growth in ways that transform them for the better. References Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125–143. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. Block, J., & Kremen, A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 349 –361. Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822– 848. Brown, S. L., Fredrickson, B. L., Wirth, M., Poulin, M., Meier, E. A., Heaphy, E., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Social closeness increases salivary progesterone in humans. Manuscript submitted for publication. Bryant, F. B. (2003). A scale for measuring beliefs about savoring. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 175–196. Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193–213. Carson, J. W., Keefe, F. J., Lynch, T. R., Carson, K. M., Goli, V., Fras, A. M., & Thorp, S. R. (2005). Loving-kindness meditation for chronic low back pain: Results from a pilot trial. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 23, 287–304. Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 117–121. Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically supported treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 7–18. Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. (2008). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Manuscript submitted for publication. Curran, P. J., & Hussong, A. M. (2003). The use of latent trajectory models in psychopathology research. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 526 –544. Dalai Lama, & Cutler, H. C. (1998). The art of happiness: A handbook for living. New York: Riverhead Books. Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804 – 813. Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55, 1196 –1214. Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564 –570. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61, 305–314. POSITIVE EMOTIONS BUILD RESOURCES Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229 –259. Doyle, W. J., Gentile, D. A., & Cohen, S. (2006). Emotional style, nasal cytokines, and illness expression after experimental rhinovirus exposure. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 20, 175–181. Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 736 –748. Easterlin, B. L., & Cardeña, E. (1998). Cognitive and emotional differences between short- and long-term Vipassana meditators. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 18, 69 – 81. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 1360 –1384. Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K. M. (1998). Avoidance personality goals and the personality-illness relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1282–1299. Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 301–331. Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective wellbeing in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377–389. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300 –319. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218 –226. Fredrickson, B. L. (2008). [Implicit, behavioral, observer and physiological measures of the effects of loving-kindness meditation]. Unpublished raw data. Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 313–332. Fredrickson, B. L., & Cohn, M. A. (2008). Positive emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.). Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 777–796). New York: Guilford Press. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–376. Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for me when things go right? Supportive responses to positive event disclosures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 904 –917. Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotion in women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 112–124. Hejmadi, A., Waugh, C. E., Otake, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2008). Cross-cultural evidence that positive emotions broaden views of self to include close others. Manuscript in preparation. Holt-Lunstad, J., Birmingham, W. A., & Light, K. C. (in press). The influence of a “warm touch” support enhancement intervention among married couples on ambulatory blood pressure, oxytocin, alpha amylase and cortisol. Psychosomatic Medicine. Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the same to me”: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875– 881. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. Chicago: Scientific Software International. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delta. 1061 Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144 –156. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses: Healing ourselves and the world through mindfulness. New York: Hyperion. Kahn, B. E., & Isen, A. M. (1993). The influence of positive affect on variety seeking among safe, enjoyable products. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 257–270. Kahneman, D. Krueger, A., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2004, December 3). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience. Science, 306, 1176 –1780. Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. King, L. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 798 – 807. Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616 – 628. Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803– 855. Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131. McIntyre, C. W., Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Cross, S. A. (1991). The effect of induced social interaction on positive and negative affect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 67–70. Moskowitz, J. T. (2003). Positive affect predicts lower risk of AIDS mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 620 – 626. Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Black, S. A., & Goodwin, J. S. (2000). Emotional well-being predicts subsequent functional independence and survival. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48, 473– 478. Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 427– 448. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES–D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385– 401. Raghunathan, R., & Trope, Y. (2002). Walking the tightrope between feeling good and being accurate: Mood as a resource in processing persuasive messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 510 –525. Richman, L. S., Kubzansky, L., Maselko, J., Kawachi, I., Choo, P., & Bauer, M. (2005). Positive emotion and health: Going beyond the negative. Health Psychology, 24, 422– 429. Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 383–388. Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069 –1081. Salzberg, S. (1995). Loving-kindness: The revolutionary art of happiness. Boston: Shambhala. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and selfesteem): A re-evaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078. Schwarz, N., Kahneman, D., & Xu, J. (in press). Global and episodic reports of hedonic experience. In R. Belli, F. Stafford, & D. Alwin 1062 FREDRICKSON, COHN, COFFEY, PEK, AND FINKEL (Eds.), Calendar and time diary methods in life course research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Scott, D. J., Stohler, C. S., Egnatuk, C. M., Wang, H., Koeppe, R. A., & Zubieta, J.-K. (2007). Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron, 55, 325–336. Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulnessbased cognitive therapy for depression. New York: Guilford Press. Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran, S. A., Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T., et al. (1991). The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 570 –585. Snyder, C. R., Rand, K. L., & Sigmon, D. R. (2002). Hope Theory: A member of the positive psychology family. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 257–276). New York: Oxford University Press. Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28. Stein, N., Folkman, S., Trabasso, T., & Richards, T. A. (1997). Appraisal and goal processes as predictors of psychological well-being in bereaved caregivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 872– 884. Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Schwarz, N., Schkade, D., Krueger, A., & Kahneman, D. (2006). A population approach to the study of emotion: Diurnal rhythms of a working day examined with the day reconstruction method. Emotion, 6, 139 –149. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615– 623. Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 89 –101. Wallace, B. A., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). Mental balance and well-being: Building bridges between Buddhism and Western Psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 690 –701. Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive emotions, self-other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation of new relationships. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 93–106. Widaman, K. F., & Thompson, J. S. (2003). On specifying the null model for incremental fit indices in structural equation modeling. Psychological Methods, 8, 16 –37. Received April 27, 2007 Revision received April 26, 2008 Accepted June 3, 2008 䡲 E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online! Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available! NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. NIH-PA Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Am Psychol. 2001 March ; 56(3): 218–226. The Role of Positive Emotions in Positive Psychology: The Broaden-and-Build Theory of Positive Emotions Barbara L. Fredrickson University of Michigan Abstract NIH-PA Author Manuscript In this article, the author describes a new theoretical perspective on positive emotions and situates this new perspective within the emerging field of positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory posits that experiences of positive emotions broaden people's momentary thought-action repertoires, which in turn serves to build their enduring personal resources, ranging from physical and intellectual resources to social and psychological resources. Preliminary empirical evidence supporting the broaden-and-build theory is reviewed, and open empirical questions that remain to be tested are identified. The theory and findings suggest that the capacity to experience positive emotions may be a fundamental human strength central to the study of human flourishing. NIH-PA Author Manuscript The mission of positive psychology is to understand and foster the factors that allow individuals, communities, and societies to flourish (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). What role do positive emotions play in this mission? On first consideration, the answer seems simple: Positive emotions serve as markers of flourishing, or optimal well-being. Certainly moments in people's lives characterized by experiences of positive emotions— such as joy, interest, contentment, love, and the like—are moments in which they are not plagued by negative emotions—such as anxiety, sadness, anger, and despair. Consistent with this intuition, the overall balance of people's positive and negative emotions has been shown to predict their judgments of subjective well-being (Diener, Sandvik, & Pavot, 1991). Building on this finding, Kahneman (1999) suggested that “objective happiness” can best be measured by tracking (and later aggregating) people's momentary experiences of good and bad feelings (but see Fredrickson, 2000c). According to these perspectives, positive emotions signal flourishing. But this is not the whole story: Positive emotions also produce flourishing. Moreover, they do so not simply within the present, pleasant moment but over the long term as well. The take-home message is that positive emotions are worth cultivating, not just as end states in themselves but also as a means to achieving psychological growth and improved well-being over time. A review of current perspectives on emotions, affect, and their respective functions provides an important backdrop. A selective review follows. Perspectives on Emotions and Affect Working definitions of emotions and affect vary somewhat across researchers. Yet despite ongoing debate (e.g., Diener, 1999; Ekman & Davidson, 1994), consensus is emerging that emotions are but a subset of the broader class of affective phenomena. Emotions, according Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 525 East University Avenue, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109. blf@umich.edu.. Author's note. Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department of Psychology and Research Center for Group Dynamics at the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. Fredrickson Page 2 NIH-PA Author Manuscript to this perspective, are best conceptualized as multicomponent response tendencies that unfold over relatively short time spans. Typically, an emotion begins with an individual's assessment of the personal meaning of some antecedent event. This appraisal process may be either conscious or unconscious, and it triggers a cascade of response tendencies manifest across loosely coupled component systems, such as subjective experience, facial expression, cognitive processing, and physiological changes. NIH-PA Author Manuscript Affect, a more general concept, refers to consciously accessible feelings. Although affect is present within emotions (as the component of subjective experience), it is also present within many other affective phenomena, including physical sensations, attitudes, moods, and even affective traits. Thus, emotions are distinct from affect in multiple ways. First, emotions are typically about some personally meaningful circumstance (i.e., they have an object), whereas affect is often free-floating or objectless (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999; Ryff & Singer, in press). Additionally, emotions are typically brief and implicate the multiple-component systems described above, whereas affect is often more long-lasting and may be salient only at the level of subjective experience (Ekman, 1994; Rosenberg, 1998; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999). Finally, emotions are often conceptualized as fitting into discrete categories of emotion families, like fear, anger, joy, and interest. Affect, by contrast, is often conceptualized as varying along two dimensions, either pleasantness and activation (Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999) or positive and negative emotional activation (Teilegen, Walson, & Clark, 1999). Perspectives on the Functions of Affect and Emotions Positive affect, according to numerous theorists, facilitates approach behavior (Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1999; Davidson, 1993; Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, & Teilegen, 1999) or continued action (Carver & Scheier, 1990; Clore, 1994). From this perspective, experiences of positive affect prompt individuals to engage with their environments and partake in activities, many of which are adaptive for the individual, its species, or both. This link between positive affect and activity engagement provides an explanation for She oftendocumented positivity offset, or the tendency for individuals to experience mild positive affect frequently, even in neutral contexts (Diener & Diener, 1996; Ito & Cacioppo, 1999). Without such an offset, individuals most often would be unmotivated to engage with their environments. Yet with such an offset, individuals exhibit the adaptive bias to approach and explore novel objects, people, or situations. (See Watson et al., 1999, for a related explanation for diurnal patterns of positive emotional activation.) NIH-PA Author Manuscript Because positive emotions include a component of positive affect, they too function as internal signals to approach or continue. Even so, positive emotions share this function with a range of other positive affective states. Sensory pleasure, for instance, motivates people to approach and continue consuming whatever stimulus is biologically useful for them at the moment (Cabanac, 1971). Likewise, free-floating positive moods motivate people to continue along any line of thinking or action that they have initiated (Clore, 1994). As such, functional accounts of positive emotions that emphasize tendencies to approach or continue may only capture the lowest common denominator across all affective states that share a pleasant subjective feel, leaving additional functions unique to specific positive emotions uncharted. Discrete emotion theorists often link the function of specific emotions to the concept of specific action tendencies (Frijda, 1986; Frijda, Kuipers, & Schure, 1989; Lazarus, 1991; Levenson, 1994; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). Fear, for example, is linked with the urge to escape, anger with the urge to attack, disgust with the urge to expel, and so on. It is not that people invariably act out these urges when feeling particular Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 3 NIH-PA Author Manuscript emotions. Rather, people's ideas about possible courses of action narrow in on a specific set of behavioral options. A key idea from this perspective is that a specific action tendency is what makes an emotion evolutionarily adaptive: These are among the actions that presumably worked best in helping human ancestors survive life-or-death situations (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). Another key idea from trie specific emotions perspective is that specific action tendencies and physiological changes go hand in hand. So, for example, when someone experiences an urge to escape when feeling fear, that person's body reacts by mobilizing appropriate autonomic support for the possibility of running (Levenson, 1994). Although specific action tendencies have been invoked to describe the function of specific posiiive emotions as well, the action tendencies identified for positive emotions are notably vague and underspecified (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). For instance, joy has been linked with aimless activation, interest with attending, and contentment with inactivity (Frijda, 1986). These tendencies are far too general to be called specific (Fredrickson, 1998). They resemble generic urges to do anything or do nothing more than urges to do something quite specific, like flee, attack, or spit. This is troublesome: If the action tendencies triggered by positive emotions are vague, their effects on survival may be inconsequential. So, like the view centered on generic approach tendencies, the view centered on specific action tendencies yields an incomplete analysis of the function of positive emotions. NIH-PA Author Manuscript The Broaden-and-Build Theory of Positive Emotions To advance understanding in this area, I formulated a new theoretical model to better capture the unique effects of positive emotions. I call this the broaden-and-buiid theory of posiiive emotions (Fredrickson, 1998). This theory states that certain discrete positive emotions—including joy, interest, contentment, pride, and love—although phenomenologically distinct, all share the ability to broaden people's momentary thoughtaction repertoires and build their enduring personal resources, ranging from physical and intellectual resources to social and psychological resources. NIH-PA Author Manuscript I contrast this new theory (o traditional models based on specific action tendencies. Specific action tendencies work well to describe the function of negative emotions and should be retained for models of this subset of emotions. Without loss of theoretical nuance, a specific action tendency can be redescribed as the outcome of a psychological process that narrows a person's momentary thought–action repertoire by calling to mind an urge to act in a particular way (e.g., escape, attack, expel). In a life-threatening situation, a narrowed thought–action repertoire promotes quick and decisive action that carries direct and immediate benefit. Specific action tendencies called forth by negative emotions represent the sort of actions that likely worked best to save human ancestors' lives and limbs in similar situations. Although positive emotions can occur in adverse circumstances, the typical context of positive emotions is not a life-threatening situation. As such, a psychological process that narrows a person's momentary thought-action repertoire to promote quick and decisive action may not be needed. Instead, the positive emotions of joy, interest, contentment, pride, and love appear to have a complementary effect: They broaden people's momentary thoughtaction repertoires, widening the array of the thoughts and actions that come to mind (Fredrickson, 1998; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2001). Conceptual analyses of a range of positive emotions support this claim. Joy, for instance, broadens by creating the urge to play, push the limits, and be creative. These urges are evident not only in social and physical behavior, but also in intellectual and artistic behavior (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988; Frijda, 1986). Interest, a phenomenologically distinct positive emotion, broadens by creating the urge to explore, take in new information and experiences, and expand the self in the process Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 4 NIH-PA Author Manuscript (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Izard, 1977; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Tomkins, 1962). Contentment, a third distinct positive emotion, broadens by creating the urge to savor current life circumstances and integrate these circumstances into new views of self and of the world (Izard, 1977). Pride, a fourth distinct positive emotion that follows personal achievements, broadens by creating the urge to share news of the achievement with others and to envision even greater achievements in the future (Lewis, 1993). Love, conceptualized as an amalgam of distinct positive emotions (e.g., joy, interest, contentment) experienced within contexts of safe, close relationships (Izard, 1977), broadens by creating recurring cycles of urges to play with, explore, and savor experiences with loved ones. These various thought-action tendencies—to play, to explore, to savor and integrate, or to envision future achievement— each represent ways that positive emotions broaden habitual modes of thinking or acting (Fredrickson, 1998,2000a; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2001). In contrast to negative emotions, which carry direct and immediate adaptive benefits in situations that threaten survival, the broadened thought-action repertoires triggered by positive emotions are beneficial in other ways. Specifically, these broadened mindsets carry indirect and long-term adaptive benefits because broadening builds enduring personal resources, which function as reserves to be drawn on later to manage future threats. NIH-PA Author Manuscript Take play, the urge associated with joy, as an example. Animal research has found that specific forms of chasing play evident in juveniles of a species, like running into a flexible sapling or branch and catapulting oneself in an unexpected direction, are seen in adults of that species exclusively during predator avoidance (Dolhinow, 1987). Such correspondences suggest that juvenile play builds enduring physical resources (Boulton & Smith, 1992; Caro, 1988). Play also builds enduring social resources: Social play, with its shared amusement, excitement, and smiles, builds lasting social bonds and attachments (Aron, Norman, Aron, McKenna, & Heyman, 2000; Lee, 1983; Simons, McCluskey-Fawcett, & Papini, 1986), which can become the locus of subsequent social support. Childhood play also builds enduring intellectual resources by increasing levels of creativity (Sherrod & Singer, 1989), creating theory of mind (Leslie, 1987), and fueling brain development (Panksepp, 1998). Other positive emotions, like interest, contentment, pride, and love, similarly augment individuals' personal resources, ranging from physical and social resources to intellectual and psychological resources. (Fuller descriptions of the broaden-and-build theory are available in Fredrickson, 1998, 2000a, in press; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2001.) NIH-PA Author Manuscript It is important to note that the personal resources accrued during states of positive emotions are conceptualized as durable. They outlast the transient emotional states that led to their acquisition. By consequence, then, the often incidental effect of experiencing a positive emotion is an increase in one's personal resources. These resources function as reserves that can be drawn on in subsequent moments and in different emotional states. In short, the broaden-and-build theory describes the form of positive emotions in terms of broadened thought–action repertoires and describes their function in terms of building enduring personal resources. In doing so, the theory provides a new perspective on the evolved adaptive significance of positive emotions. Human ancestors who succumbed to the urges sparked by positive emotions to play, explore, and so on would have by consequence accrued more personal resources. When these same ancestors later faced inevitable threats to life and limb, their greater personal resources would have translated into greater odds of survival, and, in turn, greater odds of living long enough to reproduce. To the extent, then, that the capacity to experience positive emotions is genetically encoded, this capacity, through the process of natural selection, would have become part of universal human nature. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 5 Evidence for the Broaden-and-Build Theory NIH-PA Author Manuscript Empirical support for several key propositions of the broaden-and-build theory can be drawn from multiple sub-disciplines within psychology, ranging from cognition and intrinsic motivation to attachment styles and animal behavior (for a review, see Fredrickson, 1998). This evidence suggests that positive emotions broaden the scopes of attention, cognition, and action and that they build physical, intellectual, and social resources. Yet much of this evidence, because it predated the broaden-and-build theory, provides only indirect support for the model. My collaborators and I have since initiated direct tests of hypotheses drawn from the broaden-and-build theory. Although much work remains to be done, I briefly describe our preliminary findings here. My hope is that this initial evidence will cultivate interest among readers to conduct further studies on positive emotions that may serve to test and refine the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2000b). Positive Emotions Broaden Thought-Action Repertoires NIH-PA Author Manuscript Foundational evidence for the proposition that positive emotions broaden people's momentary thought-action repertoires comes from two decades of experiments conducted by Isen and colleagues (for a review, see Isen, 2000). They have documented that people experiencing positive affect show patterns of thought that are notably unusual (Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985), flexible (Isen & Daubman, 1984), creative (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987), integrative (Isen, Rosenzweig, & Young, 1991), open to information (Estrada, Isen, & Young, 1997), and efficient (Isen & Means, 1983; Isen et al., 1991). They have also shown that those experiencing positive affect show an increased preference for variety and accept a broader array of behavioral options (Kahn & Isen, 1993). In general terms, Isen has suggested that positive affect produces a “broad, flexible cognitive organization and ability to integrate diverse material” (Isen, 1990, p. 89), effects recently linked to increases in brain dopamine levels (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999). So although Isen's work does not target specific positive emotions or thought-action tendencies per se, it provides the strongest evidence that positive affect broadens cognition. Whereas negative emotions have long been known to narrow people's attention, making them miss the forest for the trees (or the suspect's style of dress for the gun), recent work suggests that positive affect may expand attention (Derryberry & Tucker, 1994). The evidence comes from studies that use global-local visual processing paradigms to assess biases in attentional focus. Negative states—like anxiety, depression, and failure—predict local biases consistent with narrowed attention, whereas positive states—like subjective well-being, optimism, and success—predict global biases consistent with broadened attention (Basso, Schefft, Ris, & Dember, 1996; Derryberry & Tucker, 1994). NIH-PA Author Manuscript These findings provide initial empirical footing for the hypothesis, drawn from the broadenand-build theory, that distinct types of positive emotions serve to broaden people's momentary thought–action repertoires, whereas distinct types of negative emotions serve to narrow these same repertoires. With Christine Branigan, I tested this broadening hypothesis by showing research participants short, emotionally evocative film clips to induce the specific emotions of joy, contentment, fear, and anger. We also used a nonemotional film clip as a neutral control condition. Immediately following each film clip, we measured the breadth of participants' thought–action repertoires. We asked them to step away from the specifics of the film and imagine being in a situation in which similar feelings would arise. We then asked them to list what they would like to do right then, given this feeling. Participants recorded their responses on up to 20 blank lines that began with the phrase “I would like to.” Tallying the things each participant listed, Branigan and I found support for the broadening hypothesis. Participants in the two positive emotions conditions (joy and contentment) Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 6 NIH-PA Author Manuscript identified more things that they would like to do right then relative to those in the two negative emotion conditions (fear and anger) and, more important, relative to those in the neutral control condition. Those in the two negative emotion conditions also named fewer things than did those in the neutral control condition (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2000). These data provide preliminary evidence that two distinct types of positive emotion—a high activation state of joy and a low activation state of contentment—each produce a broader thought-action repertoire than does a neutral state. Likewise, two distinct types of negative emotion—fear and anger—each produce a narrower thought-action repertoire than does a neutral state. This pattern of results supports a core proposition of the broaden-and-build theory: that distinct positive emotions widen the array of thoughts and actions that come to mind. By contrast, distinct negative emotions, as models based on specific action tendencies would suggest, shrink this same array. NIH-PA Author Manuscript Despite this encouraging initial evidence, many questions arise: Do other positive and negative emotions (e.g., interest, pride, love and sadness, disgust) conform to these effects? Do the effects generalize to other measures of broadened cognition? If so, what basic cognitive processes underlie this phenomenon? Do distinct positive emotions broaden (and distinct negative emotions narrow) the scope of attention or the scope of working memory? What are the neurological underpinnings? Are these effects mediated by changing levels of circulating brain dopamine, as Ashby and colleagues (1999) have suggested? What brain structures, circuits, and processes are involved? Finally, how are broadened thought–action repertoires translated into decisions and action? These and other questions provide directions for future work. Positive Emotions Undo Lingering Negative Emotions NIH-PA Author Manuscript Evidence for the broadening hypothesis has clear implications for the strategies that people use to regulate their experiences of negative emotions. If negative emotions narrow the momentary thought–action repertoire and positive emotions broaden this same repertoire, then positive emotions ought to function as efficient antidotes for the lingering effects of negative emotions. In other words, positive emotions might correct or undo the after effects of negative emotions; my colleagues and I call this the undoing hypothesis (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, & Tugade, in press). The basic observation that positive emotions (or key components of them) are somehow incompatible with negative emotions is not new and has been demonstrated in earlier work on anxiety disorders (e.g., systematic desensitization; Wolpe, 1958), motivation (e.g., opponent-process theory; Solomon & Corbit, 1974), and aggression (e.g., principle of incompatible responses; Baron, 1976). Even so, the precise mechanism ultimately responsible for this incompatibility has not been adequately identified. The broadening function of positive emotions may play a role. By broadening a person's momentary thought–action repertoire, a positive emotion may loosen the hold that a negative emotion has gained on that person's mind and body by dismantling or undoing preparation for specific action. One marker of the specific action tendencies associated with negative emotions is increased cardiovascular activity, which redistributes blood flow to relevant skeletal muscles. In the context of negative emotions, then, positive emotions should speed recovery from or undo this cardiovascular reactivity, returning the body to more midrange levels of activation. By accelerating cardiovascular recovery, positive emotions create the bodily context suitable for pursuing the broader array of thoughts and actions called forth. My collaborators and I tested this undoing hypothesis by first inducing a high-activation negative emotion in all participants (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson et al., in press). In one study (Fredrickson et al., in press), we used a time-pressured speech Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 7 NIH-PA Author Manuscript preparation task. In just one minute, participants prepared a speech on the topic “Why you are a good friend,” believing that their speech would be videotaped and evaluated by their peers. This speech task induced the subjective experience of anxiety along with increases in heart rate, peripheral vasoconstriction, and systolic and diastolic blood pressure. Into this context of anxiety-related sympathetic arousal, we randomly assigned participants to view one of four films. Two films elicited mild positive emotions (joy and contentment), and a third served as a neutral control condition. Notably, these three films, when viewed following a resting baseline, elicit virtually no cardiovascular reactivity (Fredrickson et al., in press). So the two positive-emotion films used in this study are indistinguishable from neutrality with respect to cardiovascular changes. Our fourth film elicited sadness. We chose sadness as an additional comparison because, among the negative emotions, it has not been definitively linked to a high-energy action tendency, and thus it could be a contender for speeding cardiovascular recovery. NIH-PA Author Manuscript The undoing hypothesis predicts that those who experience positive emotions on the heels of a high-activation negative emotion will show the fastest cardiovascular recovery. My colleagues and I tested this by measuring the time elapsed from the start of the randomly assigned film until the cardiovascular reactions induced by the negative emotion returned to baseline levels. In three independent samples, participants in the two positive emotion conditions (joy and contentment) exhibited faster cardiovascular recovery than did those in the neutral control condition. Participants in the sadness condition exhibited the most protracted recovery (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson et al., in press). Although the two positive-emotion films and the neutral film did not differ in what they do to the cardiovascular system, these data suggest that they do differ in what they can undo within this system. Two distinct types of positive emotions—mild joy and contentment— share the ability to undo the lingering cardiovascular aftereffects of negative emotions. Although the precise cognitive and physiological mechanisms of the undoing effect remain unknown, the broaden-and-build theory suggests that broadening at the cognitive level mediates undoing at the cardiovascular level. Phenomenologically, positive emotions may help people place the events in their lives in broader context, lessening the resonance of any particular negative event. Perhaps pointing to physiological markers of broadening effects, some have suggested that parasympathetic cardiac control (measured as heart rate variability or respiratory sinus arrhythmia) underlies positive emotions as well as the ability to regulate negative emotions (Fox, 1989; McCraty, Atkinson, Tiller, Rein, & Watkins, 1995; Porges, 1995). Testing these suggestions and extending the work to other emotions and other contexts provide a road map for future research. NIH-PA Author Manuscript Positive Emotions Fuel Psychological Resiliency Evidence for the undoing effect of positive emotions suggests that people might improve their psychological well-being, and perhaps also their physical health, by cultivating experiences of positive emotions at opportune moments to cope with negative emotions (Fredrickson, 2000a). Folkman and colleagues have made similar claims that experiences of positive affect during chronic stress help people cope (Folkman, 1997; Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000; Lazarus, Kanner, & Folkman, 1980). Evidence supporting this claim can be drawn from experiments showing that positive affect facilitates attention to negative, selfrelevant information (Reed & Aspinwall, 1998; Trope & Neter, 1994; Trope & Pomerantz, 1998; for a review, see Aspinwall, 1998). Extrapolating from these findings, Aspinwall (2001) described how positive affect and positive beliefs serve as resources for people coping with adversity (see also Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997; Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower, & Gruenewald, 2000). Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 8 NIH-PA Author Manuscript It seems plausible that some individuals, more than others, might intuitively understand and use the benefits of positive emotions to their advantage. One candidate individual difference is psychological resilience. Resilient individuals are said to bounce back from stressful experiences quickly and efficiently, just as resilient metals bend but do not break (Carver, 1998; Lazarus, 1993). This theoretical definition of resilience suggests that, relative to their less resilient peers, resilient individuals would exhibit faster cardiovascular recovery following a high-activation negative emotion. Additionally, the broaden-and-build theory suggests that this ability to bounce back to cardiovascular baseline may be fueled by experiences of positive emotion. NIH-PA Author Manuscript With Michele Tugade, I tested these two hypotheses about resilient individuals using the same time-pressured speech preparation task (described earlier) to induce a high-activation negative emotion. We measured psychological resilience using Block and Kremen's (1996) self-report scale. It is interesting to note that resilience did not predict the levels of anxiety participants reported experiencing during the speech task or the magnitude of their cardiovascular reactions to the stressful task, both of which were considerable. Resilience did, however, predict participants' reports of positive emotions. Before the speech task was even introduced, more resilient individuals reported higher levels of preexisting positive affect on an initial mood measure. When later asked how they felt during the time-pressured speech preparation phase, more resilient individuals reported that alongside their high anxiety, they also experienced higher levels of happiness and interest. As predicted by the theoretical definition of psychological resilience, more resilient participants exhibited significantly faster returns to baseline levels of cardiovascular activation following the speech task. Moreover, as predicted by the broaden-and-build theory, this difference in time needed to achieve cardiovascular recovery was mediated by differences in positive emotions (Tugade & Fredrickson, 2000). NIH-PA Author Manuscript These data suggest that positive emotions may fuel psychological resilience. In effect, then, resilient individuals may be—wittingly or unwittingly—expert users of the undoing effect of positive emotions. Again, questions arise from this initial study: Do resilient individuals intentionally recruit positive emotions to cope? If so, how do they do it? Folkman and Moskowitz (2000) identified three kinds of coping that can generate positive affect during stressful circumstances: positive reappraisal, problem-focused coping, and the infusion of ordinary events with positive meaning. Do resilient individuals use any or all of these strategies? If so, can these strategies be taught to less resilient individuals? Finally, do resilient individuals think more broadly, as the broaden-and-build theory would suggest? If so, does broadened thinking enable people to find positive meaning within adversity? Again, these remaining questions provide directions for future work. Positive Emotions Build Psychological Resiliency and Trigger Upward Spirals Toward Improved Emotional Well-Being Preliminary evidence suggests that positive emotions may fuel individual differences in resilience. Noting that psychological resilience is an enduring personal resource, the broaden-and-build theory makes the bolder prediction that experiences of positive emotions might also, over time, build psychological resilience, not just reflect it. That is, to the extent that positive emotions broaden the scopes of attention and cognition, enabling flexible and creative thinking, they should also augment people's enduring coping resources (Aspinwall, 1998, 2001; Isen, 1990). In turn, by building this psychological resource, positive emotions should enhance people's subsequent emotional well-being. Consistent with this view, studies have shown that people who experience positive emotions during bereavement are more likely to develop long-term plans and goals. Together with positive emotions, plans and goals predict greater psychological well-being 12 months postbereavement (Stein, Folkman, Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 9 NIH-PA Author Manuscript Trabasso, & Richards, 1997; for related work, see Bonanno & Keltner, 1997; Keltner & Bonanno, 1997). One way people experience positive emotions in the face of adversity is by finding positive meaning in ordinary events and within the adversity itself (Affleck & Tennen, 1996; Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000; Fredrickson, 2000a). It is important to note that the relation between positive meaning and positive emotions is considered reciprocal: Not only does finding positive meaning trigger positive emotion, but also positive emotions, because they broaden thinking, should increase the likelihood of finding positive meaning in subsequent events (Fredrickson, 2000a). These suspected reciprocal relations among positive emotions, broadened thinking, and positive meaning suggest that over time the effects of positive emotions should accumulate and compound. The broadened attention and cognition triggered by earlier experiences of positive emotion should facilitate coping with adversity, and this improved coping should predict future experiences of positive emotion. As this cycle continues, people build their psychological resilience and enhance their emotional well-being. NIH-PA Author Manuscript The cognitive literature on depression has already documented a downward spiral in which depressed mood and the narrowed, pessimistic thinking it engenders influence one another reciprocally, over time leading to ever-worsening moods and even clinical levels of depression (Peterson & Seligman, 1984). The broaden-and-build theory predicts a comparable upward spiral in which positive emotions and the broadened thinking they engender also influence one another reciprocally, leading to appreciable increases in emotional well-being over time. Positive emotions may trigger these upward spirals, in part by building resilience and influencing the ways people cope with adversity. (For a complementary discussion of upward spirals, see Aspinwall, 1998, 2001.) With Thomas Joiner, I conducted an initial prospective test of the hypothesis that through cognitive broadening, positive emotions produce an upward spiral toward enhanced emotional well-being. We assessed positive and negative emotions, as well as a concept we called broad-minded coping, at two time points, five weeks apart. Broad-minded coping was tapped by items such as “think of different ways to deal with the problem” and “try to step back from the situation and be more objective.” Our data revealed clear evidence for an upward spiral. Individuals who experienced more positive emotions than others became more resilient to adversity over time, as indexed by increases in broad-minded coping. In turn, these enhanced coping skills predicted increased positive emotions over time (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2000). NIH-PA Author Manuscript These findings suggest that positive emotions and broad-minded coping mutually build on one another. Not only do positive emotions make people feel good in the present, but also, through their effects on broadened thinking, positive emotions increase the likelihood that people will feel good in the future. Because broad-minded coping is a form of psychological resilience, these data are consistent with the prediction, drawn from the broaden-and-build theory, that momentary experiences of positive emotion can build enduring psychological resources and trigger upward spirals toward enhanced emotional well-being. Again, many questions arise from these data. Does this upward spiral effect hold over longer time intervals and across other measures of well-being and broadening? What are the mechanisms of the effect? Do positive emotions beget future positive emotions because the broadened thinking associated with earlier positive emotions helps people solve their original problems, or because this broadened thinking enables people to find positive meaning in other life circumstances and thus experience additional positive emotions? Future studies, including experimental designs, are needed to answer these questions. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 10 Concluding Remarks NIH-PA Author Manuscript What role do positive emotions play within positive psychology? Traditional views would suggest that experiences of positive emotion signal well-being and perhaps guide behavior in the moment. Without minimizing the importance of these functions, the broaden-andbuild theory casts positive emotions in a much larger role. The theory suggests that positive emotions, although fleeting, also have more long-lasting consequences. From the perspective of the broaden-and-build theory, positive emotions are vehicles for individual growth and social connection: By building people's personal and social resources, positive emotions transform people for the better, giving them better lives in the future. NIH-PA Author Manuscript More specifically, the broaden-and-build theory suggests that multiple, discrete positive emotions are essential elements of optimal functioning. As such, the capacities to experience joy, interest, contentment, and love might be construed as fundamental human strengths that yield multiple, interrelated benefits (Fredrickson, 2000e). My own research outlines a small subset of these benefits. Specifically, I have shown that positive emotions (a) broaden people's thought–action repertoires (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2000), (b) undo lingering negative emotions (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson et al., in press), (c) fuel psychological resilience (Tugade & Fredrickson, 2000), and (d) build psychological resilience and trigger upward spirals toward enhanced emotional well-being (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2000). Complementing this work, two new perspectives highlight the lasting personal and social benefits of the positive emotions of gratitude (McCullough, Kil-patrick, Emmons, & Larson, 2001; see also Fredrickson, 2000d) and elevation (Haidt, 2000). I hope these initial findings inspire the further investigations of positive emotions that are needed to test, refine, uphold, or discard the broaden-and-build theory, which in turn will advance positive psychology. NIH-PA Author Manuscript One topic in particular need of study is the long-held but scantly supported hypothesis that positive emotions foster physical health (for reviews, see Ryff & Singer, 1998; Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler, & Steward, 2000). For instance, negative emotions, with their heightened and often prolonged cardiovascular activation, have been implicated in the etiology of coronary heart disease (Blascovich & Katkin, 1993; Fredrickson et al., 2000; Williams, Barefoot, & Shekelle, 1985). If positive emotions shorten the duration of negative emotional arousal, perhaps they may also slow the incremental progression toward disease (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). Relaxation techniques are known to reduce blood pressure in hypertensive adults (Blumenthal, 1985; Schneider et al., 1995), and they may do so precisely because they capitalize on the broadening and undoing effects of contentment (Fredrickson, 2000a). Additionally, Ryff and Singer (1998) suggested that physical health depends on having quality connections to others and leading a life of purpose. Recent evidence seems to support this assertion. For instance, people who consistently experienced positive emotions with their parents as children and then later with their spouses as adults were less than half as likely as others to exhibit high levels of cumulative wear and tear on the body (Ryff, Singer, Wing, & Love, in press). Similarly, in a longitudinal study of 2,282 older Mexican Americans, those who reported high positive affect, compared with those with less positive affect, were half as likely to have become disabled or to have died during a two-year follow-up (Ostir, Markides, Black, & Goodwin, 2000). These new findings, although somewhat isolated, underscore the message that positive emotions may be essential for optimizing both psychological and physical functioning (Fredrickson, 2000a). Yet the benefits of positive emotions identified thus far are likely just the tip of the proverbial iceberg. As the positive psychology movement inspires additional research on positive emotions, even more reasons to cultivate positive emotions may be discovered. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 11 Acknowledgments NIH-PA Author Manuscript My research on positive emotions is supported by Grants MH53971 and MH59615 from the National Institute of Mental Health, a Rackham Faculty Grant and Fellowship from the University of Michigan, and funds from the John Templeton Foundation. Biography Barbara L. Fredrickson NIH-PA Author Manuscript REFERENCES NIH-PA Author Manuscript Affleck G, Tennen H. Construing benefits from adversity: Adaptational significance and dispositional underpinnings. Journal of Personality. 1996; 64:899–922. [PubMed: 8956517] Aron A, Norman CC, Aron EN, McKenna C, Heyman RE. Couple's shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000; 78:273–284. [PubMed: 10707334] Ashby FG, Isen AM, Turken AU. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review. 1999; 106:529–550. [PubMed: 10467897] Aspinwall LG. Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion. 1998; 22:1–32. Aspinwall, LG. Dealing with adversity: Self-regulation, coping, adaptation, and health. In: Tesser, A.; Schwarz, N., editors. The Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Vol 1. lntrapersonal processes. Blackwell; Maiden, MA: 2001. p. 159-614. Aspinwall LG, Taylor SE. A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological Bulletin. 1997; 121:417–436. [PubMed: 9136643] Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 12 NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Baron RA. The reduction of human aggression: A field study of the influence of incompatible reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1976; 6:260–274. Basso MR, Schefft BK, Ris MD, Dember WN. Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1996; 2:249–255. [PubMed: 9375191] Blascovich, J.; Katkin, E. Cardiovascular reactivity to psycho logical stress and disease. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1993. Block J, Kremen AM. IQ and ego-resilience: Conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996; 70:349–361. [PubMed: 8636887] Blumenthal JA. Relaxation therapy, biofeedback, and behavioral medicine. Psychotherapy: Research, Practice, Training. 1985; 22:516–530. Bonanno GA, Keltner D. Facial expressions of emotion and the course of conjugal bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997; 106:126–137. [PubMed: 9103724] Boulton, MJ.; Smith, PK. The social nature of play fighting and play chasing: Mechanisms and strategies underlying cooperation and compromise. In: Barkow, JH.; Cosmides, L.; Tooby, J., editors. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press; New York: 1992. p. 429-444. Cabanac M. Physiological role of pleasure. Science. September 17.1971 173:1103–1107. [PubMed: 5098954] Cacioppo JT, Gardner WL, Berntson GG. The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999; 76:839– 855. Caro TM. Adaptive significance of play: Are we getting closer? Tree. 1988; 3:50–54. [PubMed: 21227072] Carver CS. Resilience and thriving: Issues, models, and link-ages. Journal of Social Issues. 1998; 54:245–266. Carver CS, Scheier MF. Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review. 1990; 97:19–35. Clore, GL. Why emotions are felt. In: Ekman, P.; Davidson, R., editors. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. p. 103-111. Csikszentmihalyi, M. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. HarperPerennial; New York: 1990. Davidson, RJ. The neuropsychology of emotion and affective style. In: Lewis, M.; Haviland, JM., editors. Handbook of emotion. Guilford Press; New York: 1993. p. 143-154. Derryberry, D.; Tucker, DM. Motivating the focus of attention. In: Neidenthal, PM.; Kitayama, S., editors. The heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1994. p. 167-196. Diener E. Introduction to the special section on the structure of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999; 76:803–804. [PubMed: 10353203] Diener E, Diener C. Most people are happy. Psychological Science. 1996; 7:181–185. Diener, E.; Sandvik, E.; Pavot, W. Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive versus negative affect. In: Strack, F., editor. Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective. Pergamon Press; Oxford, England: 1991. p. 119-139. Dolhinow, PJ. At play in the fields. In: Topoff, H., editor. The natural history reader in animal behavior. Columbia University Press; New York: 1987. p. 229-237. Ekman, P. Moods, emotions, and traits. In: Ekman, P.; Davidson, RJ., editors. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. p. 56-58. Ekman, P.; Davidson, RJ., editors. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. Ellsworth PC, Smith CA. Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 1988; 2:301–331. Estrada CA, Isen AM, Young MJ. Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1997; 72:117–135. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 13 NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Folkman S. Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Social Science Medicine. 1997; 45:1207–1221. [PubMed: 9381234] Folkman S, Moskowitz JT. Positive affect and the other side of coping. American Psychologist. 2000; 55:647–654. [PubMed: 10892207] Fox NA. Psychological correlates of emotional reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology. 1989; 25:364–372. Fredrickson BL. What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology. 1998; 2:300–319. Fredrickson, BL. Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-being. Prevention and Treatment, 3. 2000a. Retrieved January 20, 2001, from the World Wide Web: http://www.journals.apa.org/prevention/volume3/pre0030001a.html Fredrickson, BL. Cultivating research on positive emotions. Prevention and Treatment, 3. 2000b. Retrieved January 20, 2001, from the World Wide Web: http://www.journals.apa.org/prevention/volume3/pre0030007r.html Fredrickson BL. Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 2000c; 14:577–606. Fredrickson, BL. Gratitude and other positive emotions broaden and build. In: Emmons, RA., Chair, editor. Kindling the science of gratitude. Symposium sponsored by the John Templeton Foundation; Dallas, TX: October. 2000d Fredrickson, BL. Positive emotions as strengths: Implications for the VIA taxonomy. In: Vaillant, GA., Chair, editor. Toward the VIA taxonomy of strengths and virtues. Symposium cosponsored by the Values in Action Institute and the Positive Psychology Network; Fogelsville, PA: October. 2000e Fredrickson, BL. Positive emotions. In: Snyder, CR.; Lopez, SJ., editors. Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford University Press; New York: in press Fredrickson, BL.; Branigan, CA. Positive emotions broaden action urges and the scope of attention. 2000. Manuscript in preparation Fredrickson, BL.; Branigan, C. Positive emotions. In: Mayne, TJ.; Bonnano, GA., editors. Emotion: Current issues and future directions. Guilford Press; New York: 2001. p. 123-151. Fredrickson, BL.; Joiner, T. Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. 2000. Manuscript submitted for publication Fredrickson BL, Levenson RW. Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 1998; 12:191–220. Fredrickson, BL.; Mancuso, RA.; Branigan, C.; Tugade, M. Motivation and Emotion. The undoing effect of positive emotions. in press Fredrickson BL, Maynard KE, Helms MJ, Haney TL, Seigler IC, Barefoot JC. Hostility predicts magnitude and duration of blood pressure response to anger. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2000; 23:229–243. [PubMed: 10863676] Frijda, NH. The emotions. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, England: 1986. Frijda NH, Kuipers P, Schure E. Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989; 57:212–228. Haidt, J. The positive emotion of elevation. Prevention and Treatment, 3. 2000. Retrieved January 20, 2001, from the World Wide Web: http://www.journals.apa.org/prevention/volume 3/preOO3OO03c.html Isen, AM. The influence of positive and negative affect on cognitive organization: Some implications for development. In: Stein, N.; Leventhal, B.; Trabasso, T., editors. Psychological and biological approaches to emotion. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1990. p. 75-94. Isen, AM. Positive affect and decision making. In: Lewis, M.; Haviland-Jones, JM., editors. Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed.. Guilford Press; New York: 2000. p. 417-435. Isen AM, Daubman KA. The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1984; 47:1206–1217. Isen AM, Daubman KA, Nowicki GP. Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987; 52:1122–1131. [PubMed: 3598858] Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 14 NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Isen AM, Johnson MMS, Mertz E, Robinson GF. The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985; 48:1413–1426. [PubMed: 4020605] Isen AM, Means B. The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. Social Cognition. 1983; 2:18–31. Isen AM, Rosenzweig AS, Young MJ. The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making. 1991; II:221–227. [PubMed: 1881279] Ito, TA.; Cacioppo, JT. The psychophysiology of utility appraisals. In: Kahneman, D.; Diener, E.; Schwartz, N., editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 1999. p. 470-488. Izard, CE. Human emotions. Plenum; New York: 1977. Kahn BE, Isen AM. The influence of positive affect on variety-seeking among safe, enjoyable products. Journal of Consumer Research. 1993; 20:257–270. Kahneman, D. Objective happiness. In: Kahneman, D.; Diener, E.; Schwarz, N., editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 1999. p. 3-25. Keltner D, Bonanno GA. A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997; 73:687–702. [PubMed: 9325589] Lazarus, RS. Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press; New York: 1991. Lazarus RS. From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks. Annual Review of Psychology. 1993; 44:1–22. Lazarus, RS.; Kanner, AD.; Folkman, S. Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. In: Plutchik, R.; Kellerman, H., editors. Theories of emotion. Academic Press; New York: 1980. p. 189-217. Lee, PC. Play as a means for developing relationships. In: Hinde, RA., editor. Primate social relationships. Blackwell; Oxford, England: 1983. p. 82-89. Leslie AM. Pretense and representation: The origins of 'theory of mind.'. Psychological Review. 1987; 94:412–426. Lewis, M. Self-conscious emotions: Embarassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In: Lewis, M.; Haviland, JM., editors. Handbook of emotions. Guilford Press; New York: 1993. p. 563-573. Levenson, RW. Human emotions: A functional view. In: Ekman, P.; Davidson, R., editors. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. p. 123-126. McCraty R, Atkinson M, Tiller W, Rein G, Watkins AD. The effects of emotions on short-term power spectrum analysis of heart rate variability. The American Journal of Cardiology. 1995; 76:1089– 1093. [PubMed: 7484873] McCullough ME, Kilpatrick SD, Emmons RA, Larson DB. Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin. 2001; 127:249–266. [PubMed: 11316013] Oatley, K.; Jenkins, JM. Understanding emotions. Blackwell; Cambridge, MA: 1996. Ostir GV, Markides KS, Black SA, Goodwin JS. Emotional well-being predicts subsequent functional independence and survival. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2000; 48:473–478. [PubMed: 10811538] Panksepp J. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, psychostimulants, and intolerance of childhood playfulness: A tragedy in the making? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1998; 7:91– 98. Peterson C, Seligman MEP. Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review. 1984; 91:347–314. [PubMed: 6473583] Porges SW. Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage: A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology. 1995; 32:301–318. [PubMed: 7652107] Reed MB, Aspinwall LG. Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Motivation and Emotion. 1998; 22:99–132. Rosenberg EL. Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psychology. 1998; 2:247–270. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Fredrickson Page 15 NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Russell JA, Feldman Barrett L. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999; 76:805– 819. [PubMed: 10353204] Ryan RL, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist. 2000; 55:68–78. [PubMed: 11392867] Ryff CD, Singer BH. Contours of positive human health. Psychological Inquiry. 1998; 9:1–28. Ryff, CD.; Singer, BH. The role of emotion on pathways to positive health. In: Davidson, RJ.; Goldsmith, HH.; Scherer, K., editors. Handbook of affective science. Oxford University Press; New York: in press Ryff, CD.; Singer, BH.; Wing, E.; Love, GD. Elective affinities and uninvited agonies: Mapping emotion with significant others onto health. In: Ryff, CD.; Singer, BH., editors. Emotion, social relationships, and health: Third annual Wisconsin symposium on emotion. Oxford University Press; New York: in press Salovey P, Rothman AJ, Detweiler JB, Steward WT. Emotional states and physical health. American Psychologist. 2000; 55:110–121. [PubMed: 11392855] Schneider RH, Staggers F, Alexander CN, Sheppard W, Rainforth M, Kondwani K, Smith S, King CG. A randomized controlled trial of stress reduction for hypertension in older African Americans. Hypertension. 1995; 26:820–827. [PubMed: 7591024] Seligman MEP, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist. 2000; 55:5–14. [PubMed: 11392865] Sherrod, LR.; Singer, JL. The development of make-believe play. In: Goldstein, J., editor. Sports, games and play. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1989. p. 1-38. Simons, CJR.; McCluskey-Fawcett, KA.; Papini, DR. Theoretical and functional perspective on the development of humor during infancy, childhood, and adolescence. In: Nahemow, L.; McCluskeyFawcett, KA.; McGhee, PE., editors. Humor and aging. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1986. p. 53-77. Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review. 1974; 81:119–145. [PubMed: 4817611] Stein NL, Folkman S, Trabasso T, Richards TA. Appraisal and goal processes as predictors of psychological well-being in bereaved caregivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997; 72:872–884. [PubMed: 9108700] Taylor SE, Kemeny ME, Reed GM, Bower JE, Gruenewald TL. Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist. 2000; 55:99–109. [PubMed: 11392870] Tellegen A, Watson D, Clark LA. On the dimensional and hierarchical structure of affect. Psychological Science. 1999; 10:297–303. Tomkins, SS. Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol 1 The positive affects. Springer; New York: 1962. Tooby J, Cosmides L. The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology. 1990; 11:375–424. Trope Y, Neter E. Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: The role of self-control in feedback seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994; 66:646–657. [PubMed: 8189345] Trope Y, Pomerantz EM. Resolving conflicts among self-evaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resource for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion. 1998; 22:53–72. Tugade, M.; Fredrickson, BL. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional arousal. 2000. Manuscript in preparation Watson D, Wiese D, Vaidya J, Tellegen A. The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999; 76:820–838. Williams, RB.; Barefoot, JC.; Shekelle, RB. The health consequences of hostility. In: Chesney, MA.; Rosenman, RH., editors. Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders. Hemisphere; Washington, DC: 1985. p. 173-185. Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford University Press; Stanford, CA: 1958. Am Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 June 24. Psychological Bulletin 2005, Vol. 131, No. 6, 803– 855 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0033-2909/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803 The Benefits of Frequent Positive Affect: Does Happiness Lead to Success? Sonja Lyubomirsky Laura King University of California, Riverside University of Missouri—Columbia Ed Diener University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign and The Gallup Organization Numerous studies show that happy individuals are successful across multiple life domains, including marriage, friendship, income, work performance, and health. The authors suggest a conceptual model to account for these findings, arguing that the happiness–success link exists not only because success makes people happy, but also because positive affect engenders success. Three classes of evidence— crosssectional, longitudinal, and experimental—are documented to test their model. Relevant studies are described and their effect sizes combined meta-analytically. The results reveal that happiness is associated with and precedes numerous successful outcomes, as well as behaviors paralleling success. Furthermore, the evidence suggests that positive affect—the hallmark of well-being—may be the cause of many of the desirable characteristics, resources, and successes correlated with happiness. Limitations, empirical issues, and important future research questions are discussed. Keywords: happiness, subjective well-being, positive affect, positive emotions, meta-analysis (1999) reviewed the correlations between happiness and a variety of resources, desirable characteristics, and favorable life circumstances. Although the authors recognized that the causality can be bidirectional, they frequently used wording implying that cause flows from the resource to happiness. For example, they suggested that marriage might have “greater benefits for men than for women” (p. 290), apparently overlooking the possibility that sex differences in marital patterns could be due to differential selection into marriage based on well-being. Similarly, after reviewing links between money and well-being, Diener and his colleagues pointed out that “even when extremely wealthy individuals are examined, the effects [italics added] of income are small” (p. 287), again assuming a causal direction from income to happiness. We use quotes from one of us to avoid pointing fingers at others, but such examples could be garnered from the majority of scientific publications in this area. The quotes underscore the pervasiveness of the assumption among well-being investigators that successful outcomes foster happiness. The purpose of our review is not to disconfirm that resources and success lead to well-being—a notion that is likely valid to some degree. Our aim is to show that the alternative causal pathway—that happy people are likely to acquire favorable life circumstances—is at least partly responsible for the associations found in the literature. “A merry heart goes all the day, Your sad tires in a mile-a.” —William Shakespeare “The joyfulness of a man prolongeth his days.” —Sirach 30:22 “The days that make us happy make us wise.” —John Masefield Research on well-being consistently reveals that the characteristics and resources valued by society correlate with happiness. For example, marriage (Mastekaasa, 1994), a comfortable income (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002), superior mental health (Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004), and a long life (Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001) all covary with reports of high happiness levels. Such associations between desirable life outcomes and happiness have led most investigators to assume that success makes people happy. This assumption can be found throughout the literature in this area. For example, Diener, Suh, Lucas, and Smith Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside; Laura King, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri—Columbia; Ed Diener, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign and The Gallup Organization, Omaha, Nebraska. This work was supported in part by grants from the Positive Psychology Network. We are grateful to Fazilet Kasri, Rene Dickerhoof, Colleen Howell, Angela Zamora, Stephen Schueller, Irene Chung, Kathleen Jamir, Tony Angelo, and Christie Scollon for conducting library research and especially to Ryan Howell for statistical consulting. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521. E-mail: sonja@citrus.ucr.edu A PRELIMINARY CONCEPTUAL MODEL In this article, we review evidence suggesting that happy people—those who experience a preponderance of positive emotions—tend to be successful and accomplished across multiple life domains. Why is happiness linked to successful outcomes? We propose that this is not merely because success leads to happiness, but because positive affect (PA) engenders success. Positively 803 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 804 valenced moods and emotions lead people to think, feel, and act in ways that promote both resource building and involvement with approach goals (Elliot & Thrash, 2002; Lyubomirsky, 2001). An individual experiencing a positive mood or emotion is encountering circumstances that he or she interprets as desirable. Positive emotions signify that life is going well, the person’s goals are being met, and resources are adequate (e.g., Cantor et al., 1991; Carver & Scheier, 1998; Clore, Wyer, Dienes, Gasper, & Isbell, 2001). In these circumstances, as Fredrickson (1998, 2001) has so lucidly described, people are ideally situated to “broaden and build.” In other words, because all is going well, individuals can expand their resources and friendships; they can take the opportunity to build their repertoire of skills for future use; or they can rest and relax to rebuild their energy after expending high levels of effort. Fredrickson’s model (Fredrickson, 2001) suggests that a critical adaptive purpose of positive emotions is to help prepare the organism for future challenges. Following Fredrickson, we suggest that people experiencing positive emotions take advantage of their time in this state—free from immediate danger and unmarked by recent loss—to seek new goals that they have not yet attained (see Carver, 2003, for a related review). The characteristics related to positive affect include confidence, optimism, and self-efficacy; likability and positive construals of others; sociability, activity, and energy; prosocial behavior; immunity and physical well-being; effective coping with challenge and stress; and originality and flexibility. What these attributes share is that they all encourage active involvement with goal pursuits and with the environment. When all is going well, a person is not well served by withdrawing into a self-protective stance in which the primary aim is to protect his or her existing resources and to avoid harm—a process marking the experience of negative emotions. Positive emotions produce the tendency to approach rather than to avoid and to prepare the individual to seek out and undertake new goals. Thus, we propose that the success of happy people rests on two main factors. First, because happy people experience frequent positive moods, they have a greater likelihood of working actively toward new goals while experiencing those moods. Second, happy people are in possession of past skills and resources, which they have built over time during previous pleasant moods. This unifying framework builds on several earlier bodies of work—the broaden-and-build model of positive emotions (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001), the notion that positive emotions convey specific information to the person (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988), the idea of positivity offset (Ito & Cacioppo, 1999), work on the approach-related aspects of PA (Watson, 2000), and, finally, Isen’s (e.g., 2000) groundbreaking research on the behaviors that follow positive mood inductions. We extend the earlier work in predicting that chronically happy people are in general more successful, and that their success is in large part a consequence of their happiness and frequent experience of PA. Although the vast majority of research on emotions has been on negative states, a body of literature has now accumulated that highlights the importance of positive emotions in people’s long-term flourishing. Classes of Evidence Figure 1 displays our general conceptual model, which proposes that successful outcomes are caused by happiness and do not merely correlate with it or follow from it. Specifically, below the conceptual model, we display four classes of evidence that can be used to test it. The first type of evidence (Type A) represents positive correlations derived from cross-sectional studies. Although it is a truism that correlation does not imply causation, correlations must generally be positive to be consistent with propositions about causality. Except in the rare case in which strong third-variable suppressor effects exist across studies, an absence of correlation between two variables indicates an absence of causality in either direction. Thus, correlational evidence is germane to our argument because the absence of positive correlations suggests that happiness does not cause success. The second class of evidence (Type B) is based on longitudinal research, and is somewhat more informative about causal direction than cross-sectional correlations. If one variable precedes another in time and other potential causal variables are statistically controlled, the resulting causal model can be used to reject a causal hypothesis. In cases in which changes in variable X are shown to precede changes in variable Y, this form of evidence is even more strongly supportive of a causal connection, although the influence of third variables might still contaminate the conclusions and leave the direction of cause in doubt. Evidence of Type C, the classic laboratory experiment, is commonly believed to represent the strongest evidence for causality, although even in this case it can be difficult to determine exactly what aspect of the experimental manipulation led to changes in the dependent variable. Finally, long-term experimental intervention studies (Type D evidence) would offer the strongest test of our causal model, although again the active ingredients in the causal chain are usually not known with certainty. Empirical Tests of Model and Organizational Strategy Because no single study or type of evidence is definitive, an argument for causality can best be made when various classes of evidence all converge on the same conclusion. Therefore, we document several types of evidence in our article in order to most rigorously test the idea that happiness leads to success. Our review covers the first three classes of evidence (Types A, B, and C) and is organized around five focal questions arising from these three categories: 1. Cross-sectional studies (Type A) Question 1: Are happy people successful people? Question 2: Are long-term happiness and short-term PA associated with behaviors paralleling success— that is, with adaptive characteristics and skills? 2. Longitudinal studies (Type B) Question 3: Does happiness precede success? Question 4: Do happiness and positive affect precede behaviors paralleling success? 3. Experimental studies (Type C) Question 5: Does positive affect lead to behaviors paralleling success? First, we document the extensive cross-sectional correlational evidence (Type A), as shown in Figure 1. The first question addressed by this evidence is the one that forms the basis of our causal hypothesis—that is, are happy people more likely to suc- BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Figure 1. 805 Empirically testing the conceptual model. PA ⫽ positive affect; Grp. ⫽ group. ceed at culturally valued goals (e.g., concerning work, love, and health) than their less happy peers? However, the large number of available correlational studies in this category also includes relevant research examining behavior and cognition that parallel successful life outcomes—that is, the characteristics, resources, and skills that help people succeed (e.g., attributes such as selfefficacy, creativity, sociability, altruism, immunity, and coping). Accordingly, the second question addressed by this evidence explores the relations of behavior paralleling success to long-term happiness and short-term PA. Because we define happiness as the LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 806 frequent experience of positive emotions over time (see below), our model assumes that the correlations involving long-term happiness are parallel to those of short-term positive moods. In conclusion, only if the correlations generated by Questions 1 and 2 are generally positive will we consider our causal hypothesis further. Second, we consider longitudinal studies, which address two further questions. Is happiness at Time 1 associated with successful outcomes at Time 2 (Question 3)? Is happiness and PA at Time 1 correlated with behaviors paralleling success at Time 2 (Question 4)? In summary, prior levels of happiness and positive affect must correlate with later levels of successful outcomes and behavior for our causal hypothesis not to be rejected. In laboratory experimentation, the third type of evidence, causality is put to a stronger test. In this case, however, because of the limits of the laboratory, only short-term changes in behavior and cognitions that parallel successful life outcomes are assessed. Thus, the fifth and final question we address is whether PA causes the cognitive and behavioral characteristics paralleling success. Again, because positive affect is defined here as the basic constituent of happiness, our model requires that the outcomes of shortterm positive moods are parallel to the successful outcomes in our conceptual model. Furthermore, this question is critical, as it speaks to whether PA may be a mediator underlying the relationship between happiness and flourishing—that is, whether PA causes the adaptive characteristics that help happy people succeed. Although the fourth type of evidence shown in Figure 1 (Type D) would provide the strongest type of data for our model, unfortunately, to our knowledge no studies of this type exist. Nevertheless, support for our conceptual model from all three of the previously described types of evidence, while not definitive, will suggest a likelihood that our causal model is correct. Furthermore, combining the three types of evidence represents an advance beyond laboratory experimentation alone, because the relatively greater rigor and control provided by experimentation are supplemented by the relatively greater ecological validity provided by the other types of studies. Thus, the first two classes of evidence (Types A and B) speak to the plausibility of generalizing the causal laboratory findings to the context of success and thriving in everyday life. Meanwhile, by revealing the processes uncovered in the laboratory, the experimental evidence (Type C) illuminates the possible causal sequence suspected in the correlational data. Taken together, consistent findings from all three types of data offer a stronger test than any single type of data taken alone. After describing our methodology and defining our terms, we address each of the five focal questions in order, documenting the three classes (A, B, and C) of relevant empirical evidence. Then, we turn to a discussion of several intriguing issues and questions arising out of this review, caveats and limitations, and important further research questions. Methodological Approach To identify the widest range of published papers and dissertations, we used several search strategies (Cooper, 1998). First, we searched the PsycINFO online database, using a variety of key words (e.g., happiness, satisfaction, affect, emotion, and mood). Next, using the ancestry method, the reference list of every empirical, theoretical, and review paper and chapter was further combed for additional relevant articles. To obtain any papers that might have been overlooked by our search criteria, as well as to locate work that is unpublished or in press, we contacted two large electronic listserves, many of whose members conduct research in the area of well-being and emotion—the Society of Personality and Social Psychology listserv and the Quality of Life Studies listserv. Twenty-four additional relevant articles were identified with this method. The final body of literature was composed of 225 papers, of which 11 are unpublished or dissertations. From these 225 papers, we examined 293 samples, comprising over 275,000 participants, and computed 313 independent effect sizes. A study was included in our tables if it satisfied the following criteria. First, measures of happiness, PA, or a closely related construct had to be included, in addition to assessment of at least one outcome, characteristic, resource, skill, or behavior. Second, the data had to include either a zero-order correlation coefficient or information that could be converted to an r effect size (e.g., t tests, F tests, means and standard deviations, and chi-squares). If a study did not report an r effect size, we computed one from descriptive statistics, t statistics, F ratios, and tables of counts (see Rosenthal, 1991). If no relevant convertible statistics were presented, other than a p value, we calculated the t statistic from the p value and an r-sub(equivalent) (Rosenthal & Rubin, 2003). When a paper reported p ⬍ .05, p ⬍ .01, or ns, we computed rsub(equivalent) with p values of .0245, .005, and .50 (one-tailed), respectively, which likely yielded a highly conservative estimate of the effect size. Finally, the sample size had to be available. When possible, we also contacted authors for further information. Descriptions of the critical elements of each study (i.e., authors, year, sample size, happiness/PA measure or induction, related construct, and effect size [r]) are included in Tables 1, 2, and 3, which present cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental work, respectively. Table 2 additionally presents the length of time between assessments, and Table 3 includes the comparison groups used in the studies. Studies with subscripts after their name are those that appear in more than a single section or table, usually because multiple outcome variables are included. Furthermore, mirroring our documentation of the literature presented in this paper, Tables 1–3 are subdivided into substantive categories (or panels). For example, Table 1 is subdivided into nine categories—work life, social relationships, health, perceptions of self and others, sociability and activity, likability and cooperation, prosocial behavior, physical well-being and coping, and, finally, problem solving and creativity. The mean and median effect size (r), weighted and unweighted by sample size, as well as a test of heterogeneity, is provided for each category for the three classes of data (cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental) in Table 4. Tables 1, 2, and 3 report all effect sizes of interest to readers— including instances of two or more effect sizes generated from the same sample or dataset. For example, the relation of happiness with income and marital status derived from a single study may appear in two different panels of a table (i.e., work life and social relationships). Alternatively, the correlation between happiness and coping derived from a single longitudinal study may appear in two different tables (e.g., the cross-sectional table and the longitudinal table). However, in order to meta-analytically combine the 464 effect sizes listed in Tables 1–3, we had to ensure a degree of (text continues on page 816) BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 807 Table 1 Study Information and Effect Sizes for Nine Categories of Cross-Sectional Research Study n Happiness/PA measure Effect size (r) Correlated construct Work life Credé et al., 2005 Credé et al., 2005 Credé et al., 2005 Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a (first assessment) Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a (second assessment) DeLuga & Mason, 2000 Donovan, 2000 Donovan, 2000 Donovan, 2000 Donovan, 2000 Donovan, 2000 Foster et al., 2004 Foster et al., 2004 Frisch et al., 2004 George, 1989 George, 1995 George, 1995 Graham et al., in pressa (1995 assessment) Graham et al., in pressa (2000 assessment) Howell et al., in press Jundt & Hinsz, 2001 Krueger et al., 2001a Lucas et al., 2004 Magen & Aharoni, 1991a Magen & Aharoni, 1991a Miles et al., 2002 Seligman & Schulman, 1986a (Study 1) Staw & Barsade, 1993a 959 959 959 60 PANAS PANAS PANAS Index of Psychological Well-Being Organizational citizenship behavior Counterproductive work behavior Job withdrawal Supervisory evaluations 60 Index of Psychological Well-Being Supervisory evaluations Affectometer 2 Current Mood Report Current Mood Report Current Mood Report Current Mood Report Current Mood Report Job Affect Scale Job Affect Scale Quality of Life Inventory Job Affect Scale PANAS (leader) PANAS (aggregated group) One-item happiness One-item happiness SWLS Seven-point semantic differentials MPQ positive emotionality One-item happiness Four-item positive affect Four-item positive affect Job-Related Affective Well-Being Scale Attributional Style Questionnaire Three-measure composite of positive affectivity Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job One-item happiness One-item happiness (12 times over 4 days) PANAS PANAS Fordyce HM Scale PANAS Index of Psychological Well-Being Job performance Organizational citizenship behavior Turnover intentions Work withdrawal Organizational retaliatory behavior Satisfaction with work Organizational climate for performance Employee health and well-being Academic retention absenteeism 92 188 188 188 188 188 41 41 3,638 254 53 53 4,524 5,134 307 164 397 24,000 260 260 203 94 83 Staw et al., 1994a 272 Staw et al., 1994a 272 Staw et al., 1994a 272 Thoits & Hewitt, 2001a Totterdell, 2000* Van Katwyk et al., 2000a (Study 3) Van Katwyk et al., 2000a (Study 3) Weiss et al., 1999a Wright & Cropanzano, 1998 Wright & Cropanzano, 2000 (Study 1) Wright & Cropanzano, 2000 (Study 2) Wright & Staw, 1999a (Study 1, second assessment) Wright & Staw, 1999a (Study 2, first assessment) Wright & Staw, 1999a (Study 2, second assessment) 3,617 17 111 111 24 52 47 Judged customer service Judged customer service Income Income Material wealth Task performance Self-reported altruism Income Transpersonal commitment Involvement in community service Organizational citizenship behavior .37 ⫺.25 ⫺.25 .29 .34 .22 .20 ⫺.38 ⫺.20 ⫺.22 .50 .32 .29 .18 ⫺.28 .41 .35 .20b .16b .23 .19 .44 .20 .21 .36 .23 Quarterly insurance commissions Judged managerial performance .18 .20 Job autonomy, meaning, and variety .22 Gross annual salary .12 Supervisory evaluations (creativity) .30 Time spent volunteering Cricket batting average .09 .36 Interpersonal conflict Intention to quit Job satisfaction Emotional exhaustion Job performance ⫺.12 ⫺.33 .29 ⫺.39 .32 37 45 Index of Psychological Well-Being Index of Psychological Well-Being Supervisory evaluations Supervisory evaluations .34 .33 62 Index of Psychological Well-Being Supervisory evaluations .25 64 Index of Psychological Well-Being Supervisory evaluations .43 Social relationships Baldassare et al., 1984 Baldassare et al., 1984 Baldassare et al., 1984 Berry & Willingham, 1997 Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 1 & Study 2) Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 2) 202 202 202 127 118 118 Four-item happiness Four-item happiness Four-item happiness PANAS SWLS SWLS Instrumental support Emotional support Companionship Commitment to current relationship Satisfaction with friends Satisfaction with social activities .17 .15 .30 .27 .31 .37 (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 808 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Effect size (r) Social relationships (continued) Diener & Seligman, 2002a Diener et al., 2000 Gladow & Ray, 1986a Gladow & Ray, 1986a Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black female sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black male sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White female sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White male sample) Graham et al., in pressa (1995 assessment) Graham et al., in pressa (2000 assessment) Headey et al., 1991a (1981 assessment) Headey et al., 1991a (1983 assessment) Headey et al., 1991a (1985 assessment) Headey et al., 1991a (1987 assessment) Kozma & Stones, 1983 Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987 (male sample) Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987 (male sample) Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987 (female sample) Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987 (female sample) Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Mastekaasa, 1994 Mishra, 1992a Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989a Phillips, 1967* (healthy sample) Requena, 1995 (Spanish sample) Requena, 1995 (U.S. sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (husbands sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (husbands sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (wives sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (wives sample) Stack & Eshleman, 1998 (male sample) Stack & Eshleman, 1998 (female sample) Staw et al., 1994a Strayer, 1980a 106 SWLS, affect balance, memory recall One-item life satisfaction One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness Relationshipswithclosefriends .48 Marital status Support from friends Support from neighbors Marital happiness .07b .35 .31 .18 167 One-item happiness Marital happiness .22 820 One-item happiness Marital happiness .53 1,872 One-item happiness Marital happiness .37 One-item happiness One-item happiness Life-as-a-Whole Index Life-as-a-Whole Index Life-as-a-Whole Index Life-as-a-Whole Index MUNSH Seven-item morale Seven-item morale Seven-item morale Seven-item morale SHS SHS Bradburn’s Scales, one-item life satisfaction, one-item happiness Index of Life Satisfaction Marital status Marital status Satisfaction with marriage Satisfaction with marriage Satisfaction with marriage Satisfaction with marriage Marital status No. of close friends Loneliness No. of close friends Loneliness Satisfaction with friends Satisfaction with recreation Marital status 59,169 63 63 89 4,524 5,134 649 649 649 649 600 1,321 1,321 1,551 1,551 621 621 25,810 720 1,927 59 430 1,084 1,534 317 317 317 317 9,237 10,127 272 14 Life Satisfaction Inventory MUNSH One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Observational count of happy affect Willi, 1997 383 Relationship-relevant happiness Achat et al., 2000a Bogner et al., 2001 Chang & Farrehi, 2001 Chang & Farrehi, 2001 Collins et al., 1992 Diener & Seligman, 2002a 659 168 402 402 73 106 Diener & Seligman, 2002a 106 Diener & Seligman, 2002a 106 LOT SWLS LOT-Revised SWLS MAACL-Revised SWLS, affect balance, memory recall SWLS, affect balance, memory recall SWLS, affect balance, memory recall Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Social interactions with nonfamily members Marital status Jealousy in specific relationship Social participation No. of friends No. of friends Marital well-being Spouse’s marital well-being Marital well-being Spouse’s marital well-being Marital status Marital status Emotional and tangible support from supervisors Observational count of empathic responses to others Extent in love with partner .03b .02b .47 .55 .49 .47 .20 .23 ⫺.50 .19 ⫺.51 .50 .51 .29 .41 .23 ⫺.03 .17 .13 .08 .12 .16 .41 .34 .15b .16b .33 .59 .19 Health Gil Gil Gil Gil Gil Gil et et et et et et al., al., al., al., al., al., 2004a 2004a 2004a 2004a 2004a 2004a 41 41 41 41 41 41 Vitality History of substance abuse Depressive symptoms Depressive symptoms Quality of life Depression .14b ⫺.27 ⫺.36 ⫺.57 .32 ⫺.61 Hypochondriasis ⫺.24 Schizophrenia ⫺.53 Pain ER visits Hospital visits Doctor calls Medication use Work absences ⫺.42 ⫺.06b ⫺.06b ⫺.08b ⫺.08b ⫺.09b BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 809 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Effect size (r) Health (continued) Graham et al., in pressa (1995 assessment) Graham et al., in pressa (2000 assessment) Kashdan & Roberts, 2004a Kehn, 1995a Laidlaw et al., 1996 Lobel et al., 2000 Lu & Shih, 1997 Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1978-1980 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1981-1983 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1984-1986 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1987-1989 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1990-1992 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1993-1995 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1996-1998 sample) Mroczek & Spiro, 2005a (1999-2000 sample) Phillips, 1967a Røysamb et al., 2003a Røysamb et al., 2003a Windle, 2000a ⫺.03b ⫺.05b ⫺.34 .43 ⫺.33 ⫺.20 ⫺.36 .43 ⫺.29 ⫺.49 .23 4,524 5,134 104 98 38 129 191 621 621 621 1,254 One-item happiness One-item happiness PANAS Life Satisfaction Index One-item peacefulness LOT Chinese Happiness Inventory SHS SHS SHS Life Satisfaction Inventory Health problems Health problems Social phobia/anxiety Global health Size of allergic reaction Delivery of low-birth-weight infants Poor mental health Satisfaction with health Physical symptoms Depressed affect Global health 1,267 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .31 1,283 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .31 1,641 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .24 965 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .26 974 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .29 919 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .29 389 Life Satisfaction Inventory Global health .34 One-item happiness SWB Index SWB Index Revised Dimension of Temperament Survey Overall mental health Global health Musculoskeletal pain Delinquent activity .22 .50 ⫺.25 ⫺.22 PANAS SWLS PANAS SWLS PANAS Inventory of Personal Happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness One-item happiness Quality of conversation Satisfaction with relatives Satisfaction with relatives Satisfaction with friends Satisfaction with friends Hostility toward other women Support received from friends Support received from relatives Satisfaction with friendships .27 .22 .12 .31 .23 ⫺.21 .35 .14 .22 167 One-item happiness Satisfaction with friendships .23 820 One-item happiness Satisfaction with friendships .29 89 One-item happiness Satisfaction with friendships .13 1,872 One-item happiness Satisfaction with family life .25 167 One-item happiness Satisfaction with family life .15 820 One-item happiness Satisfaction with family life .39 89 One-item happiness Satisfaction with family life .17 Neutral Objects Satisfaction Questionnaire Neutral Objects Satisfaction Questionnaire SWLS SWLS Judged favorability of reference letter (hypothetical) Judged favorability of reference letter (actual) Self-esteem Optimism .29 593 6,576 6,576 1,016 Positive perceptions of self and others Berry & Hansen, 1996a (Study 1) Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 1 & Study 2) Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 1 & Study 2) Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 1 & Study 2) Cooper et al., 1992a (Study 1 & Study 2) Cowan et al, 1998 Gladow & Ray, 1986a Gladow & Ray, 1986a Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White male sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black male sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White female sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black female sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White male sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black male sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (White female sample) Glenn & Weaver, 1981a (Black female sample) Judge & Higgins, 1998 (Study 1) Judge & Higgins, 1998 (Study 2) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 1) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 1) 112 118 118 118 118 90 63 63 1,872 110 95 212 212 .17 .59 .60 (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 810 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Effect size (r) Positive perceptions of self and others (continued) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 2) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 2) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 3) Lucas et al., 1996 (Study 3) Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998a (Study 1) Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998a (Study 3) Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998a (Study 3) Mayer et al., 1988 (preliminary study) Mayer et al., 1988 (Study 2) Mongrain & Zuroff, 1995 Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989a Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989a Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989a Ryff, 1989 Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 1) Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 2) Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 1) Tarlow & Haaga, 1996 Totterdell, 2000a Weiss et al., 1999a 109 109 172 172 621 621 621 621 621 621 621 621 621 621 621 105 47 38 206 193 152 123 123 123 321 136 124 136 124 18 24 SWLS SWLS SWLS SWLS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS Mood-State Introspection Scale Mood-State Introspection Scale Four positive adjectives MUNSH MUNSH MUNSH Life Satisfaction Index SWLS SWLS SWLS PANAS One-item happiness (12 times over 4 days) Fordyce HM Scale Self-esteem Optimism Self-esteem Optimism Self-esteem Optimism Sense of mastery Perceived control Satisfaction with family relations Satisfaction with friends Satisfaction with health Satisfaction with education Satisfaction with recreation Satisfaction with housing Satisfaction with transportation Evaluations of past life events Liking of videotaped target Evaluations of real-life target Inferences about people Inferences about people Self-criticism Cognitive jealousy Emotional jealousy Behavioral jealousy Personal growth Self-rated assertiveness Self-rated assertiveness Self-rated warmth Self-esteem Self-rated performance .65 .59 .54 .57 .62 .60 .55 .47 .41 .50 .43 .27 .51 .43 .34 .41 .29 .36 .29 .29 ⫺.39 ⫺.08 ⫺.24 ⫺.17 .38 .21 .36 .27 .57 .50 Satisfaction with job .29 Attendance at club meetings Quality of conversation Degree of disclosure in conversation Degree of engagement in conversation Intimacy of conversation No. of daily interactions Extraversion Extraversion .31 .27 .06 .10 .09 .34 .31 .43 Extraversion Extraversion Social activities Extraversion Extraversion Informant-rated energy Extraversion .21 .54 .40 .16 .16 .39 .49 Peer ratings of target’s relationships .65 Performance-approach goals Personal conversations Intrinsically motivating experiences (%) Extraversion Self-rated affiliation Observer-rated affiliation Extraversion Extraversion Flow Satisfaction with activities .15 .35 .28 Sociability and activity Bahr & Harvey, 1980 Berry & Hansen, 1996a Berry & Hansen, 1996a Berry & Hansen, 1996a Berry & Hansen, 1996a Berry & Hansen, 1996a Brebner et al., 1995 Brebner et al., 1995 (Study (Study (Study (Study (Study 1) 1) 1) 1) 2) 44 112 112 112 112 105 95 95 Brebner et al., 1995 Burger & Caldwell, 2000a Burger & Caldwell, 2000a Costa & McCrae, 1980a Costa & McCrae, 1980a Diener & Fujita, 1995a Diener & Seligman, 2002a 95 134 134 753 554 186 106 Diener & Seligman, 2002a 106 Elliot & Thrash, 2002 Gladow & Ray, 1986a Graef et al., 1983 176 63 107 Griffin et al., in press Harker & Keltner, 2001a Harker & Keltner, 2001a Headey & Wearing, 1989 Headey & Wearing, 1989 Hektner, 1997a Kahana et al., 1995 1,051 49 114 649 649 281 257 One-item happiness PANAS PANAS PANAS PANAS PANAS Oxford Happiness Inventory Personal State Questionnaire, Version 5 LOT PANAS PANAS Bradburn’s Scales Bradburn’s Scales SWLS SWLS, affect balance, memory recall SWLS, affect balance, memory recall General Temperament Survey One-item happiness One-item happiness PANAS FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile Life Satisfaction Index Bradburn’s Scales One-item happy mood Fifteen items from the 22-item screening score .32 .33 .69 .20 .18 .27 .38 BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 811 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Effect size (r) Correlated construct Sociability and activity (continued) Kashdan & Roberts, 2004a Kashdan & Roberts, 2004a Lu & Argyle, 1991 Lu & Argyle, 1991 Lucas et al., 2000 Lucas et al., 2000 Lucas et al., 2000 Lucas, 2001a (daily study) Lucas, 2001a (daily study) Lucas, 2001a (daily study) Lucas, 2001a (moment study) Lucas, 2001a (moment study) Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Matikka & Ojanen, in press Matikka & Ojanen, in press Mishra, 1992a 104 104 114 114 5,842 5,842 5,842 144 144 144 124 124 621 621 376 376 720 Mishra, 1992a Mishra, 1992a Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study Stones & Kozma, 1986a Watson, 1988a Watson et al., 1992a (Study 1) Watson et al., 1992a (Study 2) Watson et al., 1992a (Study 1) 1) 1) 1) 2) 2) Watson et al., 1992a (Study 2) Watson et al., 1992a (Study 2) PANAS PANAS Oxford Happiness Inventory Oxford Happiness Inventory PANAS PANAS PANAS PANAS PANAS PANAS Time felt happy and pleasant (%) Time felt happy and pleasant (%) SHS SHS Three-item happiness Three-item happiness Index of Life Satisfaction 720 Index of Life Satisfaction 720 136 136 136 124 124 408 71 85 127 79 Index of Life Satisfaction SWLS SWLS SWLS SWLS SWLS MUNSH Positive Emotionality Scale PANAS (weekly, over 13 weeks) PANAS (daily, over 6–7 weeks) PANAS, extraversion, positive temperament PANAS, joviality PANAS, extraversion, positive temperament 96 120 Attraction to partner Closeness to partner Attitude toward joint activities Attitude toward group activities Extraversion Ascendance Affiliation Experience of Affiliation/warmth Time spent with friends Time spent leading Time spent leading Time spent with friends and family Extraversion Satisfaction with recreation Social participation Social inclusion Engaging in hobbies and special interests Interaction with members of voluntary organizations Engaging in occupational activities Extraversion Gregariousness Informant ratings of how active Friendliness Gregariousness Activity level Social activity Weekly social activity Weekly social activity Weekly social activity .50 .30 .25 .22 .62 .30 .27 .48 .22 .20 .24 .19 .36 .51 .22 .21 .63 Weekly social activity Weekly social activity .31 .28 .50 .64 .33 .26 .24 .43 .21 .13b .34 .36 .39 .35 Likeability and cooperation Barsade et al., 2000 Barsade et al., 2000 Bell, 1978 Berry & Hansen, 1996a (Study 1) Berry & Hansen, 1996a (Study 1) Diener & Fujita, 1995a 62 20 120 112 112 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Diener & Fujita, 1995a 186 Harker Harker Harker Harker 114 114 114 114 & & & & Keltner, Keltner, Keltner, Keltner, 2001a 2001a 2001a 2001a MPQ well-being MPQ well-being Personal Feelings Scale PANAS PANAS Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness Delighted-Terrible Scale, one-item happiness FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile ⫺.30 .38 .43 .09 .06 .33 Fordyce Task conflict Group cooperativeness Likeability as work partner Intimacy of conversation Degree of disclosure in conversation Judged physical attractiveness Fordyce Judged intelligence/competence .30 Fordyce Judged social skills .41 Fordyce Judged public speaking ability .28 Fordyce Judged self-confidence .36 Fordyce Judged assertiveness .25 Fordyce Judged number of close friends .35 Fordyce Judged likelihood of having a strong romantic relationship Judged likelihood of having family support Observer-rated affiliation Observer-rated negative emotionality Judged positive emotionality Judged competence .33 Fordyce .34 .69 ⫺.57 .71 .21 (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 812 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Effect size (r) Likeability and cooperation (continued) Kashdan & Roberts, 2004a Kashdan & Roberts, 2004a King & Napa, 1998 (Study 1) King & Napa, 1998 (Study 1) King & Napa, 1998 (Study 2) King & Napa, 1998 (Study 2) Mathes & Kahn, 1975 (female sample) Mathes & Kahn, 1975 (male sample) Perry et al., 1986 (eighth grade sample) Rimland, 1982 Scheufele & Shah, 2000 Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 1) Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 2) Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 1) Schimmack et al., 2004a (Study 2) Staw & Barsade, 1993a Taylor et al., 2003 Van Katwyk et al., 2000a (Study 3) 104 104 104 104 264 264 101 110 32 1,991 3,462 136 124 136 124 111 55 111 PANAS PANAS Three -item happiness Three-item happiness Three-item happiness Three-item happiness Happiness Happiness Dichotomous “Who is happier?‘ Dichotomous “Happy or not?‘ Four-item Index of Life Satisfaction SWLS SWLS SWLS SWLS Three-measure composite Ten-measure composite PANAS Partner-rated attraction Partner-rated closeness Judged moral goodness Judged likelihood of going to heaven Judged moral goodness Judged likelihood of going to heaven Judged physical attractiveness Judged physical attractiveness Helpfulness Selfishness Personality strength Informant-rated warmth Informant-rated friendliness Informant-rated assertiveness Informant-rated assertiveness Judged managerial potential Judged positive personal qualities Interpersonal conflict .34 .30 .29 .25 .26 .26 .37 .09 .44 ⫺.60 .21 .28 .33 .20 .25 .20 .28 ⫺.12 Prosocial behavior Feingold, 1983 (male sample) Feingold, 1983 (female sample) George, 1991 George, 1991 Krueger et al., 2001a Lucas, 2001a (daily study) Lucas, 2001a (moment study) Magen & Aharoni, 1991a 87 88 221 221 397 144 124 260 Magen & Aharoni, 1991a 260 Rigby & Slee, 1993 869 Strayer, 1980a Williams & Shiaw, 1999 One-item happiness One-item happiness Job Affect Scale Job Affect Scale MPQ positive emotionality PANAS Time felt happy and pleasant (%) Four-item intensity of positive experience Four-item intensity of positive experience Life-as-a-Whole Index 14 Observational count of happy affect 139 Watson 10-item positive affectivity scale Unselfishness Unselfishness Extrarole prosocial behavior Customer service Self-reported altruistic acts Time spent helping Time spent helping Transpersonal commitment .27 .09 .24 .26 .44 .36 .27 .21 Involvement in community service .36 Tendency to act in a prosocial or cooperative manner Observational count of empathetic responses Anticipated organizational citizenship behavior .36 .59 .42 Physical well-being and coping Achat et al., 2000a Achat et al., 2000a Audrain et al., 2001 Bardwell et al., 1999 (healthy sample) Bardwell et al., 1999 (healthy sample) Benyamini et al., 2000a Carver et al., 1993a (presurgery assessment) Carver et al., 1993a (presurgery assessment) Carver et al., 1993a (presurgery assessment) Carver et al., 1993a (presurgery assessment) C. C. Chen et al., 1996 Dillon & Totten, 1989 Goldman et al., 1996 659 659 227 40 40 851 59 59 59 59 121 16 134 Irving et al., 1998 Kehn, 1995a Keltner & Bonanno, 1997 Lox et al., 1999 Lutgendorf et al., 1999 (movers sample) Lyons & Chamberlain, 1994 Lyons & Chamberlain, 1994 Lyubomirsky et al., in pressa Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998a (Study 1) 115 98 39 121 26 158 158 621 105 LOT LOT PANAS One-item vigor One-item vigor 12-item positive affect LOT LOT LOT LOT General Health Questionnaire Coping Humor Scale Repair Subscale of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale Hope Scale Life Satisfaction Index FACS Duchenne laughter Affective Reactions Measure Sense of Coherence Scale Uplifts Scale LOT SHS SHS General health Pain Physical activity Sleep quantity Sleep quality Self-reported health Active coping Coping by positive reframing Coping by humor Coping by denial Engagement coping Presence of upper respiratory infection Reported illnesses .23b ⫺.09b .19 .32 .36 .49 .33 .41 .40 ⫺.39 .31 ⫺.58 ⫺.21 Hope-related coping responses Global health Perceived adjustment Amount of physical exercise NK cell activity Upper respiratory infection symptoms Upper respiratory infection symptoms Satisfaction with health Perception of life events .35 .43 .31 .19 .49 ⫺.03 ⫺.23 .43 .41 BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 813 Table 1 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Effect size (r) Physical well-being and coping (continued) McCrae & Costa, 1986 (Study 1) McCrae & Costa, 1986 (Study 1) Mishra, 1992a Pettit et al., 2001a Pettit et al., 2001a Pettit et al., 2001a Riddick, 1985 (male sample) Riddick, 1985 (female sample) Røysamb et al., 2003a Røysamb et al., 2003a Stone et al., 1987 Stone et al., 1994 Stones & Kozma, 1986a Sullivan et al., 2001 Valdimarsdottir & Bovbjerg, 1997 (with daily NA) Valdimarsdottir & Bovbjerg, 1997 (no daily NA) Vitaliano et al., 1998a Watson, 1988a Watson, 1988a Watson, 1988a Watson, 2000 Watson, 2000 Watson et al., 1992a (Study 1) Watson et al., 1992a (Study 2) Weinglert & Rosen, 1995 Zinser et al., 1992 254 254 720 140 140 140 806 753 6,576 6,576 30 96 408 105 26 22 42 80 80 80 354 354 85 127 71 22 Bradburn’s Scales Bradburn’s Scales Index of Life Satisfaction PANAS PANAS PANAS Life Satisfaction Index Life Satisfaction Index SWB Index SWB Index Nowlis Mood Adjective Checklist PANAS MUNSH PANAS Profile of Mood States Profile of Mood States Uplifts-Hassles 10-item PA Scale (daily, over 6–8 weeks) 10-item PA Scale (daily, over 6–8 weeks) Positive Emotionality Scale (daily) Positive temperament Positive temperament PANAS (weekly, over 13 weeks) PANAS (daily, over 6–7 weeks) Positive mood checklist Mood Adjective Check List Coping effectiveness Mature coping Overall activity level Presence and severity of medical conditions Cigarette use Alcohol intake Leisure activities Leisure activities Global health Musculoskeletal pain Secretory IgA antibody activity Antibody activity Global health Self-reported physical health NK cell activity NK cell activity NK cell activity .27 .26 .61 ⫺.26 ⫺.24 ⫺.22 .37 .44 .50 ⫺.25 .44 .05 .19b .23 0.64 .05 .26 Daily physical complaints ⫺.18 Daily physical exercise Physical exercise Injury visits to health center Illness visits to health center Weekly social activity Weekly social activity Somatic symptoms Urges to smoke .12 .12 .12 .15 .36 .39 ⫺.10 ⫺.38 Exploration strivings Absorption in activities Creative episodes Creativity Maximizing tendencies Maximizing tendencies Maximizing tendencies Maximizing tendencies Maximizing tendencies Maximizing tendencies .44 .33 .41 .25 ⫺.21 ⫺.34 ⫺.17 ⫺.10 ⫺.28 ⫺.17 Creativity and problem solving Kashdan et al., 2004 (Study 2) Kashdan et al., 2004 (Study 2) Richards & Kinney, 1990 Schuldberg, 1990 Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 1) Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 2) Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 3) Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 4) Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 5) Schwartz et al., 2002 (Sample 6) 214 214 48 334 82 72 100 401 752 220 Shapiro & Weisberg, 1999 52 Staw & Barsade, 1993a 83 Staw et al., 1994a 272 PANAS activated PANAS activated Diagnosis of manic periods Hypomanic traits SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS SHS General Behavior Inventory (hypomanic plus biphasic) Three-measure composite of positive affectivity Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Trait creativity .33 Judged managerial performance .20 Judged creativity .30 Note. PA ⫽ positive; PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; MPQ ⫽ Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction With Life Scale; HM ⫽ Happiness Measure; MUNSH ⫽ Memorial University of Newfoundland Scale of Happiness; SHS ⫽ Subjective Happiness Scale; LOT ⫽ Life Orientation Test; MAACL ⫽ Multiple Adjective Affect Checklist; SWB ⫽ Subjective Well-Being; FACS ⫽ Facial Action Coding System; NEO ⫽ Neuroticism/Extraversion/Openness Scale; ER ⫽ emergency room. Subscript a indicates that the study appears in more than one section or table. Subscript b indicates that the effect size was calculated controlling for one or more other variables. LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 814 Table 2 Study Information and Effect Sizes for Seven Categories of Longitudinal Research Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Time period Effect size (r) .35 .36 .27 .17 .18 .03b .04b ⫺.02b .03 ⫺.36 .13 Work life Burger & Caldwell, 2000a Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a Cropanzano & Wright, 1999a Diener et al., 2002 Graham et al., in pressa Graham et al., in pressa Marks & Fleming, 1999 Pelled & Xin, 1999 Roberts et al., 2003 99 60 60 60 60 7,882 4,455 4,489 1,322 99 859 PANAS Index of Psychological Well-Being Index of Psychological Well-Being Index of Psychological Well-Being Index of Psychological Well-Being On-item cheerfulness One-item residual happiness One-item residual happiness Nine-item SWB index PANAS MPQ communal positive emotionality MPQ agency positive emotionality MPQ communal positive emotionality MPQ agency positive emotionality MPQ communal positive emotionality MPQ agency positive emotionality Attributional Style Questionnaire Proportion of second interviews Supervisory evaluations Supervisory evaluations Supervisory evaluations Supervisory evaluations Income Income Unemployment Income Absenteeism Financial security 3 months 1 year 4 years 4.5 years 5 years 19 years 5 years 5 years 1–15 years 5 months 8 years Financial security Occupational attainment 8 years 8 years .06 .19 Occupational attainment Work autonomy 8 years 8 years .16 .06 Work autonomy Quarterly insurance commissions .13 .27 Job autonomy, meaning, and variety Gross annual salary 1.5 years .24 Judged creativity 1.5 years .16 44 Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Index of Psychological Well-Being 8 years 6 months to 1 year 1.5 years Supervisory evaluations 3.5 years .47 63 Index of Psychological Well-Being Supervisory evaluations 1 year .46 Roberts et al., 2003 Roberts et al., 2003 859 859 Roberts et al., 2003 Roberts et al., 2003 859 859 Roberts et al., 2003 Seligman & Schulman, 1986a (Study 2) Staw et al., 1994a 859 68 129 Staw et al., 1994a 191 Staw et al., 1994a 191 Wright & Staw, 1999a (Study 1) Wright & Staw, 1999a (Study 2) .23 Social relationships Harker & Keltner, 2001a Harker & Keltner, 2001a Harker & Keltner, 2001a Headey et al., 1991a Lucas et al., 2003 Marks & Fleming, 1999a Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001 Ruvolo, 1998a (wives sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (wives sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (husbands sample) Ruvolo, 1998a (husbands sample) Spanier & Furstenberg, 1982 Staw et al., 1994a 71 111 112 649 1,761 1,322 489 317 FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile Life-as-a-Whole Index One-item happiness Nine-item SWB index General Self-Esteem One-item happiness Marital satisfaction Marital status Single status Satisfaction with marriage Marital status Marital status Closeness with all relationships Marital well-being 31 years 6 years 22 years 6 years 4⫹ years 1–15 years 4 years 1 year .20 .19 ⫺.20 .30 .20 .09 .19b .30 317 One-item happiness Spouse’s marital well-being 1 year .15 317 One-item happiness Marital well-being 1 year .28 317 One-item happiness Spouse’s marital well-being 1 year .40 180 251 Cantril’s Ladder Scale Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job Remarriage after divorce Emotional and tangible support form supervisors 2.5 years 1.5 years .16 .25b Mortality rate Probability of dying relative to peers Survival CHD risk reduction Age at death Pain Hospital visits Emergency room visits Health problems last 30 days Lifetime 26–28 years 4 years 9 months lifetime 2 days 1 day 1 day 5 years Health Danner et al., 2001 Deeg & van Zonneveld, 1989 Devins et al., 1990 Fitzgerald et al., 2000 Friedman et al., 1993 Gil et al., 2004a Gil et al., 2004a Gil et al., 2004a Graham et al., in pressa 180 2,645 97 42 1,178 3,565 3,546 3,546 4,455 No. of positive emotional words One-item life satisfaction Life Happiness Rating Scale LOT Cheerfulness-Humor Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Daily Mood Scale Two-item residual happiness ⫺.31 ⫺.11 .15 .30b ⫺.09 ⫺.06b ⫺.04b ⫺.06b ⫺.06b BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 815 Table 2 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Time period Effect size (r) Health (continued) Kirkcaldy & Furnham, 2000 Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2001 Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2002 (male sample) Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2002 (female sample) Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004 (male sample) Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004 (female sample) Krause et al., 1997 Four databases 29,137 SWB Automobile fatalities 8 years ⫺.56 Four-item life satisfaction Suicides ⫺.03 14,348 Four-item life satisfaction 14,789 Four-item life satisfaction 11,037 Four-item life satisfaction 11,099 Four-item life satisfaction 330 Eight-item life satisfaction Fatal intentional and unintentional injuries Fatal intentional and unintentional injuries Work disability pension for psychiatric and nonpsychiatric causes Work disability pension for psychiatric and nonpsychiatric causes Survival fatal and nonfatal coronary heart disease Up to 20 years Up to 20 years Up to 20 years Up to 11 years Kubzansky et al., 2001 1,306 Kubzansky et al., 2001 1,306 Kubzansky et al., 2001 1,306 ⫺.06 ⫺.02 ⫺.11 Up to 11 years ⫺.12 11 years .18 12 years ⫺.14 Fatal coronary heart disease 12 years ⫺.07 Nonfatal angina and heart attacks 12 years ⫺.12 Survival Days survival 7 years 22.6 years .36 .25 Mortality rate Survival Stroke incidence 3–6 years 2 years 6 years ⫺.06 .08 ⫺.13b Maier & Smith, 1999 Ostir et al., 2000 Ostir et al., 2001 (male sample) Ostir et al., 2001 (female sample) Palmore, 1969 513 2,276 772 Revised Optimism-Pessimism Scale Revised Optimism-Pessimism Scale Revised Optimism-Pessimism Scale Affect Balance Scale-Joy Attitudes Toward Own Aging Subscale PANAS CESD Positive Affect Scale CESD Positive Affect Scale 1,706 CESD Positive Affect Scale Stroke incidence 6 years ⫺.05b Mortality rate 15 years ⫺.26 Peterson et al., 1998 1,097 One-item interviewer-rated happiness Optimistic (global) attributional style Optimistic (global) attributional style Optimistic (global) attributional style Optimistic (global) attributional style S Life satisfaction scale LOT LOT LOT LOT LOT One-item vigor Revised Dimension of Temperament Survey Revised Dimension of Temperament Survey Revised Dimension of Temperament Survey One-item interviewer-rated happiness One-item interviewer-rated happiness Mortality rate Lifetime ⫺.11 Mental health problems 10 years ⫺.14 Poor adjustment 10 years ⫺.11 High levels of drinking 10 years ⫺.07 Survival Survival No. of days to begin walking Physical recovery Postsurgical quality of life Resume vigorous exercise Return to normal activities Hockey injury Delinquent activity 10 years 1 year 1 week 1 week 6 months 6 months 6 months 6 months 6 months .13 .13 ⫺.36 .35 .67 .33 .38 ⫺.32 ⫺.25 Delinquent activity 12 months ⫺.15 Delinquent activity 18 months ⫺.12 Mortality rate 2 years ⫺.07 Mortality rate 2 years ⫺.14 Levy et al., 1988 Levy et al., 2002 (Study 2) 36 660 265 Peterson et al., 1998 (male sample) Peterson et al., 1998 (male sample) Peterson et al., 1998 (male sample) Pitkala et al., 2004 Reynolds & Nelson, 1981 Scheier et al., 1989a Scheier et al., 1989a Scheier et al., 1989a Scheier et al., 1989a Scheier et al., 1989a Smith et al., 1997 Windle, 2000a 491 154 46 46 45 45 44 86 1,016 Windle, 2000a 1,016 Windle, 2000a 1,016 Zuckerman et al., 1984 (healthy sample) Zuckerman et al., 1984 (unhealthy sample) 622 622 622 182 168 Positive perceptions of self and others Harker & Keltner, 2000a Harker & Keltner, 2001a 104 100 FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile Self-rated competence Self-rated competence 22 years 31 years .20 .29 (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 816 Table 2 (continued ) Study n Happiness/PA measure Correlated construct Time period Effect size (r) Sociability and activity Costa et al., 1981 Costa et al., 1981 Costa & McCrae, 1980a Harker & Keltner, 2001a Harker & Keltner, 2001a Stones & Kozma, 1986a 396 114 234 104 100 408 Chicago Attitude Inventory Chicago Attitude Inventory Bradburn’s Scales FACS Duchenne smile FACS Duchenne smile MUNSH Extraversion Extraversion Extraversion Self-rated affiliation Self-rated affiliation Activity level 2–10 years 10–17 years 10 years 22 years 31 years 1.5 years .20 .24 .23 .23 .16 .29b Judged creativity 1.5 years .31b Time spent engaged in volunteer work 3 years .04b Creativity and prosocial behavior Staw et al., 1994a Thoits & Hewitt, 2001a 191 2,681 Experience and expression of positive emotion on the job One-item happiness Physical well-being and coping Benyamini et al., 2000a Benyamini et al., 2000a Benyamini et al., 2000a Carver et al., 1993a Carver et al., 1993a Carver et al., 1993a Carver et al., 1993a Cohen et al., 2003 Epping-Jordan et al., 1999 Epping-Jordan et al., 1999 Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002 Graham et al., in pressa Graham et al., in pressa Pettit et al., 2001a Scheier et al., 1989a Scheier et al., 1989a Stones & Kozma, 1986a Vitaliano et al., 1998a 791 678 525 59 59 59 59 334 80 80 138 1,683 1,252 1,33 47 47 408 23 12-item positive affect 12-item positive affect 12-item positive affect LOT LOT LOT LOT Positive emotional style LOT LOT PANAS One-item residual happiness One-item residual happiness PANAS LOT LOT MUNSH Uplifts-Hassles Global health Global health Global health Active coping Coping by positive reframing Coping by humor Coping by denial Presence of clinical infection Symptoms of anxiety/depression Symptoms of anxiety/depression Broad-minded coping Alcohol intake Smoking Presence and severity of medical conditions Coping by information seeking Coping by suppression Global health NK cell activity 1 3 5 3 3 3 3 1 3 6 5 5 5 5 year years years months months months months month months months weeks years years weeks .13b .11b .17b .16 .26 .38 ⫺.37 ⫺.14 ⫺.40 ⫺.55 .19 ⫺.04b ⫺.06b ⫺.31 1 week 1 week 1.5 years 1.5 years .43 ⫺.30 .28b .48 Note. PA ⫽ positive affect; PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; MPQ ⫽ Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; FACS ⫽ Facial Action Coding System; CES-D ⫽ Center for Epidemiologic Depression Scale; SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction With Life Scale; LOT ⫽ Life Orientation Test; MUNSH ⫽ Memorial University of Newfoundland Scale of Happiness; HM ⫽ Happiness Measure; SWB ⫽ Subjective Well-Being; NK ⫽ natural killer. Subcript a indicates that the study appears in more than one panel or table. Subscript b indicates that the effect size was calculated controlling for one or more other variables independence among them. To this end, several guidelines were followed. First, when more than one effect size was generated from the same sample, and these effect sizes involved moderately to highly correlated constructs (e.g., judgments of friendliness, gregariousness, and assertiveness), they were not treated independently. As a result, the unweighted average of each set of correlated effect sizes was used in our analyses. This procedure was used for effect sizes generated from the same sample and reported within a particular panel of a table (e.g., those involving work life in Table 1). Second, as mentioned previously, we recognized that sometimes the same sample was used to generate effect sizes in more than one panel of a table (e.g., sociability/activity and health) or even across tables (e.g., supervisory evaluations assessed at a single point in time [in Table 1] as well as two points in time [in Table 2]). In such instances, we selected only one specific effect size for our analyses. If the rs came from the same table, the effect size tapping the best fitting construct was chosen. If the rs spanned Tables 1 and 2, the effect size reflecting a longitudinal correlation was selected, as longitudinal data speak relatively more strongly to causation. Third, even when generated from the same sample or dataset, effect sizes were considered independent if they involved variables that are not highly correlated with one another (e.g., income and alcohol consumption; see Meng, Rosenthal, & Rubin, 1992). Defining Our Terms What Is the Hallmark of Happiness? Our focus in this article is on happy individuals—that is, those who experience frequent positive emotions, such as joy, interest, and pride, and infrequent (though not absent) negative emotions, such as sadness, anxiety, and anger. Although many definitions of happiness have been used in the literature, ranging from life satisfaction and an appreciation of life to momentary feelings of pleasure, we define happiness here as a shorthand way of referring BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 817 Table 3 Study Information and Effect Sizes for Six Categories of Experimental Research Study n Affect induction Comparison groups Effect size (r) Dependent variable Positive perceptions of self and others Baron, 1987 71 False feedback Positive vs. negative and control Positive vs. negative and control Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. negative Likeability of accomplice .44 Baron, 1987 71 False feedback Judgment of whether employee matches job requirements Self-efficacy Self-set goals Overall evaluation of accomplice for job Overall rating of employee .43 Baron, 1990a Baron, 1990a Baron, 1993 80 80 92 Fragrance Fragrance Recall event(s) Baron et al., 1992 (Study 1) 91 Lighting Baron et al., 1992 (Study 1) 91 Lighting Baron et al., 1992 (Study 3)a 80 Gift High illuminance vs. low illuminance High illuminance vs. low illuminance Positive vs. control Hiring decision .34 Judgment of whether hypothetical employee deserves merit raise Judgment of whether hypothetical employee deserves promotion Perceived task performance Certainty of future success Attribution of successes to stable causes Recall of positive aspects of past experiences Interpersonal attraction .23 Baron et al., 1992 (Study 3)a 80 Gift Positive vs. control Barsade, 2002a Brown, 1984 Brown, 1984 93 61 61 Confederate role playing Velten Velten Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative .21 .73 .26 Clark & Waddell, 1983 45 False feedback Positive vs. control Griffitt, 1970 40 Temperature Hom & Arbuckle, 1988 Samson & Rachman, 1989 Sarason et al., 1986 (Study 2) 31 84 60 Recall event(s) Music and thought Self-description task Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in pressa Wright & Mischel, 1982 Wright & Mischel, 1982 50 72 72 Velten Recall event(s) Recall event(s) Extreme heat vs. normal temperature Happy vs. sad Happy vs. sad Positive vs. negative self-feelings Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Goal setting Self-efficacy Positive self-references .48 .28 .39 Self-efficacy regarding health Satisfaction with performance Recall of past successes .43 .64 .75 Elated vs. neutral and depressed Elated vs. neutral and depressed Elated vs. neutral and depressed Positive vs. negative Interest in being with friends .20 Interest in social activities .31 Interest in leisure activities .33 High self-disclosure .44 Positive vs. negative Total communication .53 Positive vs. negative and control Positive vs. negative Positive vs. neutral Amused vs. neutral Interest in category-sorting task .56 Attentive Intrinsic interest Perceived relationship closeness .71 .74 .27 More monetary concessions Resolving interpersonal conflict through avoidance Resolving interpersonal conflict through competition Preference to resolve conflict through avoidance Preference to resolve conflict through avoidance Preference to resolve conflict through collaboration Resolving interpersonal conflict through collaboration .23 ⫺.23 .16 .22 .17 .53 .21 .36 .33 Sociability and activity Cunningham, 1988a (Study 1)a 102 Velten Cunningham, 1988a (Study 1)a 102 Velten Cunningham, 1988a (Study 1)a 102 Velten Cunningham, 1988b 77 Cunningham, 1988b 77 Hirt et al., 1996 McMillen et al., 1977 (Study 1) Murray et al., 1990 (Study 3) Waugh & Fredrickson, 2003 194 27 85 94 False feedback and videotape False feedback and videotape Velten False feedback Velten and film clip(s) Film clip(s) Negotiation and conflict resolution Baron, 1990a Baron, 1990a 80 80 Fragrance Fragrance Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Baron, 1990a 80 Fragrance Positive vs. control Baron et al., 1990 (Study 2)a 16 Humor Humor vs. control Baron et al., 1990 (Study 2)a 16 Flattery Flattery vs. control Baron et al., 1990 (Study 2)a 16 Flattery Flattery vs. control Baron et al., 1992 (Study 2)* 72 Lighting Warm vs. cool lighting ⫺.23 ⫺.50 ⫺.50 .50 .29 (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 818 Table 3 (continued ) Study n Affect induction Comparison groups Dependent variable Effect size (r) Negotiation and conflict resolution (continued) Baron et al., 1992 (Study 2)a 72 Lighting Warm vs. cool lighting Barsade, 2002a Barsade, 2002a Carnevale & Isen, 1986a Forgas, 1998 (Study 1) 26 26 80 72 Confederate role playing Confederate role playing Cartoon(s) and gift False feedback Positive Positive Positive Positive Forgas, 1998 (Study 1) 72 False feedback Positive vs. control Forgas, 1998 (Study 2) 132 False feedback Positive vs. negative Forgas, 1998 (Study 2) 132 False feedback Positive vs. negative Forgas, 1998 (Study 3) 96 False feedback Positive vs. negative Forgas, 1998 (Study 3) 96 False feedback Positive vs. negative vs. vs. vs. vs. negative negative control control Resolving interpersonal conflict through avoidance Judged group cooperativeness Judged group conflict Persist at negotiation Planned use of cooperation as bargaining strategy Planned use of competition as bargaining strategy Planned use of cooperation as bargaining strategy Planned use of competition as bargaining strategy Planned use of cooperation as bargaining strategy Planned use of competition as bargaining strategy ⫺.30 .44 ⫺.42 .41 .32 ⫺.32 .21 ⫺.21 .30 ⫺.24 Prosocial behavior Aderman, 1972 Baron & Bronfen, 1994 (Study 2) Baron et al., 1992 (Study 3)a 120 72 80 Velten Fragrance Lighting and gift Berkowitz, 1987 (Study 1) Berkowitz, 1987 (Study 2) Carnevale & Isen, 1986a Cunningham, 1988a (Study 1)a 108 60 80 102 Velten Velten Cartoon(s) and gift Velten 90 160 80 78 14 12 10 20 20 52 Find dime Find dime Velten Velten False feedback False feedback False feedback False feedback False feedback Gift 90 36 20 Recall event(s) Recall event(s) Stories Cunningham et al., 1980 (Study Cunningham et al., 1980 (Study Cunningham et al., 1990 (Study Cunningham et al., 1990 (Study Isen, 1970 (Study 1) Isen, 1970 (Study 2) Isen, 1970 (Study 2) Isen, 1970 (Study 3) Isen, 1970 (Study 3) Isen & Levin, 1972 (Study 1) 1) 2) 1) 2) O’Malley & Andrews, 1983 Rosenhan et al., 1974 Rosenhan et al., 1981 Elated vs. depressed Positive vs. control Positive/high illuminance vs. control/low illuminance Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Positive vs. control Elated vs. neutral and depressed Positive vs. neutral Positive vs. neutral Positive vs. neutral Positive vs. neutral Positive vs. negative Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. negative Positive/help vs. control/ distraction Happy vs. neutral Positive vs. control Positive vs. neutral Volunteering Time spent helping Time willing to help .45 .28 .25 Helping Helping Helping Interest in prosocial activities .34 .68 .62 .27 Helping Donating to charity Helping Helping Amount of donation Helping Attentiveness Helping Initiation of conversation Willingness to help .28 .21 .33 .34 .58 .57 .58 .58 .61 .36 Donating blood Contributing to needy children Helping experimenter .26 .52 .59 Physical well-being and coping Alden et al., 2001 Cogan et al., 1987 (Study 1) Dillon et al., 1985 Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998 (Study 2) Fredrickson et al., 2000 (Study 1, Sample 1) Fredrickson et al., 2000 (Study 1, Sample 2) Futterman et al., 1994 38 40 10 72 Mental imagery Audiotape Humorous videotape Video clip(s) Positive vs. negative Positive vs. control Happy vs. control Smiling vs. not smiling 95 Video clip(s) Content vs. sad 75 Video clip(s) Content vs. neutral 25 Reflect on scenario(s) Lefcourt et al., 1990 (Study 1) Lefcourt et al., 1990 (Study 2) 45 34 Humorous audio clip(s) Humorous video clip(s) Positive and negative vs. control Pre- vs. postaudio clip Pre- vs. postvideo clip Lefcourt et al., 1990 (Study 3) McClelland & Cheriff, 1997 (Study 1) McClelland & Cheriff, 1997 (Study 3) Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in pressa Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in pressa 41 57 85 50 50 Humorous Audio clip(s) Video clip(s) Video clip(s) Velten Velten Pre- vs. postaudio clip Positive vs. negative Positive vs. control Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Pain ratings Pain threshold Immune function Duration of cardiovascular reactivity Duration of cardiovascular reactivity Duration of cardiovascular reactivity Immune function .51 .45 .38 ⫺.26 ⫺.27 ⫺.28 .76 Immune function Immune function .47 .46 Immune function Immune function Immune function Self-efficacy regarding health Health perceptions .50 .33 .05 .43 .53 BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 819 Table 3 (continued ) Study n Affect induction Comparison groups Dependent variable Effect size (r) Physical well-being and coping (continued) Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in pressa 50 Velten Positive vs. negative Smith et al., 2004 82 Recall event(s) Recall of close vs. casual relationship Treatment-related behavioral intentions Blood pressure reactivity to stressful task .31 ⫺.23 Creativity and problem solving Adaman & Blaney, 1995 Ambady & Gray, 2002 (Study 1) Ambady & Gray, 2002 (Study 2) Baron, 1990a 71 35 47 80 Music Video clip(s) Video clip(s) Fragrance Sad vs. neutral Happy and control vs. sad Happy and control vs. sad Positive vs. control Bless et al., 1996 (Study 1) 82 Write about event(s) Happy vs. sad Bless et al., 1996 (Study 2) 61 Video clip(s) Happy vs. sad Bless et al., 1996 (Study 3) 80 Video clip(s) Happy vs. sad Happy/accountable/no stereotype activated vs. neutral/not accountable/stereotype activated Neutral vs. sad Neutral vs. sad Positive vs. neutral Bodenhausen et al., 1994 (Study 4) 131 Recall event(s) Bodenhausen et al., 2000 (Study 1) Bodenhausen et al., 2000 (Study 2) Dovidio et al., 1995 70 51 43 Recall event(s) Recall event(s) Candy Elsbach & Barr, 1999 120 Erez & Isen, 2002 (Study 1) Erez & Isen, 2002 (Study 1) Erez & Isen, 2002 (Study 1) Estrada et al., 1994 Estrada et al., 1997 Estrada et al., 1997 Forgas, 1989 97 97 97 44 29 29 72 Candy vs. difficult anagram problems Candy Candy Candy Candy Candy Candy False feedback Forgas, 1989 72 False feedback Isen et al., 1985 (Study 1) 29 Positive vs. negative Positive Positive Positive Positive Positive Positive Positive vs. vs. vs. vs. vs. vs. vs. neutral neutral neutral control control control negative Positive/personal relevance vs. negative/impersonal relevance Positive vs. neutral Isen & Daubman, 1984 (Study 1) 162 Word associations to affectively valenced words Gift Isen & Daubman, 1984 (Study 2) 162 Video clip(s) Positive vs. control Isen & Daubman, 1984 (Study 3) 74 Gift Positive vs. control Isen & Means, 1983 22 False feedback Positive vs. control Isen & Means, 1983 22 False feedback Positive vs. control Isen & Means, 1983 Kahn & Isen, 1993 (Study 1) Kahn & Isen, 1993 (Study 2) Kahn & Isen, 1993 (Study 3) Kavanagh, 1987 Kavanagh, 1987 Mackie & Worth, 1989 (Study 1) 22 69 54 45 85 85 215 False feedback Gift Gift Gift Recall event(s) Recall event(s) False feedback Mackie & Worth, 1989 (Study 2) 260 Video clip(s) Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. control Positive vs. negative Positive vs. negative Positive/limited viewing of Argument vs. remaining conditions Positive vs. neutral Positive vs. control Originality Judgment accuracy Judgment accuracy Use of efficient strategy on coding task Reliance on preexisting general knowledge structures Reliance on preexisting general knowledge structures Reliance on preexisting general knowledge structures Avoiding stereotypic judgments ⫺.31 .83 .63 .22 Anchoring bias Anchoring bias Making more inclusive group representations Careful execution of steps in highly structured task Performance on anagram task Motivation Time spent on anagram task Creativity Time before diagnosis considered Anchoring bias Speed and efficiency in reaching decision Decision-making efficiency ⫺.24 ⫺.27 .39 .26 .31 .31 .21 ⫺.33 .21 .30 .33 .33 ⫺.48 ⫺.40 .32 .43 No. of unusual word associations .48 Tendency to group items as belonging together Tendency to group items as belonging together Tendency to group items as belonging together Propensity to return to alreadyreviewed information Pieces of information considered during decision making Time to complete mental task Variety seeking Variety seeking Variety seeking Performance on anagram task Time spent on anagram task Differentiation of strong vs. weak arguments .23 .29 .40 ⫺.51 ⫺.26 ⫺.43 .31 .31 .23 .32 .34 ⫺.18 Differentiation of strong vs. weak ⫺.15 arguments (table continues) LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 820 Table 3 (continued ) Study n Affect induction Comparison groups Dependent variable Effect size (r) Creativitity and problem solving (continued) Melton, 1995 Reed & Aspinwall, 1998 57 66 Cartoon(s) Survey Humor vs. control Positive vs. control Reed & Aspinwall, 1998 66 Survey Positive vs. control Sinclair & Mark, 1995 (Study 1) 39 Velten Sinclair & Mark, 1995 (Study 2) 83 Velten Positive vs. negative and neutral Positive vs. negative Trope & Pomerantz, 1998 (Study 3) 68 False feedback Positive vs. negative Urada & Miller, 2000 (Study 1) 43 Recall event(s) Positive vs. neutral Urada & Miller, 2000 (Study 4) 61 Recall event(s), music, candy Positive vs. neutral Performance on syllogisms No. of facts ignored about caffeine and fibrocystic breast disease Recall of disconfirming information about caffeine and fibrocystic breast disease Accuracy of correlational judgments Accuracy of correlational judgments Interest in receiving feedback regarding liabilities vs. assets Using broad definition of group membership Using broad definition of group membership ⫺.43 ⫺.22 .28 .43 .25 .35 .30 .25 Note. Subscript a indicates that the study appears in more than one section or table. to the frequent experience of positive emotions. In our theoretical framework, it is the experience of positive emotions that leads to the behavioral outcomes we review, and “happiness” describes people who experience such emotions a large percentage of the time (Diener, Sandvik, & Pavot, 1991). Although more inclusive definitions of happiness have been offered by others (e.g., Veenhoven, 1984), we restrict our definition to the experience of frequent positive affect because that definition encompasses the findings we review. The notion that frequent positive affect is the hallmark of happiness has strong empirical support. Diener and his colleagues (1991) found that the relative proportion of time that people felt positive relative to negative emotions was a good predictor of self-reports of happiness, whereas the intensity of emotions was a weaker predictor. That is, happy people feel mild or moderate positive affect the majority of the time; they do not appear to experience frequent intense positive states. In several studies and using a variety of happiness measures, Diener and his colleagues found that happy people experienced positive moods and emotions most of the time (see also Diener, Larsen, Levine, & Emmons, 1985). Indeed, people who report high levels of happiness appear to have predominantly positive affect—that is, stronger positive feelings than negative ones— 80% or more of the time. For example, in a large international sample of more than 7,000 college students in 41 diverse nations collected by Diener’s laboratory, individuals who reported that they were pleased with their lives expressed feelings of joy over half of the time. In the World Value Survey I, which comprises probability samples of almost 60,000 adults in 41 nations, 64% of the respondents reported more positive than negative affect, with only 18% reporting more negative than positive affect (World Value Survey Group, 1994). Notably, of those reporting above-neutral happiness on this survey, nearly everyone reported more positive than negative affect, prompting Diener et al. (1991) to conclude that happiness is best regarded as a state in which people feel a preponderance of positive emotions most of the time. One reason for the tendency of happy people to feel positive emotions more frequently may be that the chronically happy are relatively more sensitive to rewards in their environment—that is, they have a more reactive behavioral approach system (Gray, 1994)—and are more likely to approach, rather than avoid, rewarding situations (Watson, 1988). In addition, Larsen and colleagues demonstrated that dispositional positive affectivity involves a susceptibility to experience positive moods (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991; Rusting & Larsen, 1997). Characterizing and Measuring High Average Positive Affect In short, the research evidence supports the notion that it is the amount of time that people experience positive affect that defines happiness, not necessarily the intensity of that affect. Furthermore, happy people have been found to experience positive emotions the majority of the time. Thus, in this article, we identify happy individuals as those who experience high average levels of positive affect. These high average levels of positive affect, which we variously refer to as chronic happiness, trait PA, or subjective well-being, may be rooted in personality predispositions (e.g., a genetically determined “set point”; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005), the person’s current life circumstances, the person’s intentional activities, or all of these. Because the existing literature does not discriminate which effects of long-term PA come from which of these different sources, we refer in our article to individuals who show high average level of PA, without reference to the source of this state. However, the findings from the experimental studies suggest that positive emotions can produce desirable outcomes even in the absence of a very happy disposition, although a happy disposition is likely to be a cause of positive emotions. The research we cite uses a variety of measures of long-term PA, happiness, and well-being. The vast majority of assessment of chronic PA is by self-report measures, which have been validated BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT 821 Table 4 Measures of Central Tendency and Dispersion for Effect Sizes by Category Mean of effect sizes Category n Sampling units Unweighted Weighted Median of effect sizes Unweighted Weighted 2 test of heterogeneity Cross-sectional data Work life Social relationships Health Positive perceptions of self and others Sociability and activity Likeability and cooperation Prosocial behavior Physical well-being and coping Creativity and problem solving 19 22 19 16 34,794 120,256 17,693 2,821 .27 .27 .32 .39 .20 .15 .32 .42 .29 .23 .31 .37 .20 .07 .31 .39 188.82**** 3,079.60**** 67.98**** 59.36**** 26 11,773 .33 .37 .32 .40 116.20**** 15 6,930 .32 .34 .29 .21 217.64**** 7 25 2,097 5,093 .32 .29 .35 .31 .32 .31 .36 .35 11.45 197.32**** 10 2,275 .26 .24 .27 .28 18.25* Longitudinal data Work life Social relationships Health Positive perceptions of self and others Sociability and activity Creativity and prosocial behavior Physical well-being and coping 11 8 26 1 15,080 5,106 37,421 100 .24 .21 .18 .25 .05 .19 .09 N/A .25 .20 .14 .25 .03 .20 .09 N/A 621.63**** 30.43**** 418.90**** N/A 4 1,117 .25 .29 .24 .33 2 2,872 .18 .06 .18 .04 55.67**** 10 2,999 .27 .15 .29 .14 80.98**** 4.66 Experimental data Positive perceptions of self and others Sociability and activity Negotiation and conflict resolution Prosocial behavior Physical well-being and coping Creativity and problem solving * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. 13 900 .36 .34 .36 .33 19.66 6 579 .51 .49 .52 .56 16.30** 8 574 .33 .29 .31 .27 4.15 17 14 1,170 749 .43 .38 .37 .34 .36 .40 .34 .28 26.53* 18.79 34 2,707 .25 .16 .30 .25 *** p ⬍ .001. 193.63**** **** p ⬍ .0001. in a number of studies (e.g., Sandvik, Diener, & Seidlitz, 1993; Watson, 2000). In experimental research on induced moods, positive moods are operationalized by the manipulations that induce them; in longitudinal research, the measures depend on moods at the moment or over a recent period such as the past week or month; and, in individual difference research on chronic positive emotions, the measures usually ask about the person’s moods in general. Although these methods are not without shortcomings, they appear to be at least moderately valid. A more detailed discussion of measurement issues is available elsewhere (see Diener, 1994; Diener et al., 1999; Sandvik et al., 1993). Discriminating Happiness and Positive Affect From Related Constructs Notably, a large number of studies have assessed life satisfaction as an indicator of well-being, and such investigations are occasionally included here. Although the construct of satisfaction 822 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER is not identical to positive affect—some researchers argue that it has an affective dimension (Veenhoven, 1997), whereas others define it as a purely cognitive judgment of life or its facets (Diener et al., 1999)—we review studies of satisfaction because they frequently represent the only available evidence in an area. Furthermore, life satisfaction and positive affect have been found to correlate at around .40 to .50 in undergraduates (Lucas, Diener, & Suh, 1996) and .52 in business students (Staw & Barsade, 1993). In addition, of people who say they are above neutral in satisfaction with their lives, 85% have been found to report that they feel happy at least half of the time (Lucas et al., 1996). Thus, life satisfaction is a defensible proxy for chronic happiness, in cases in which no studies exist using more direct measures of happiness; Lucas and his colleagues demonstrated that it is separable but not independent from chronic PA. Similarly, Lucas et al. found that optimism is also related to positive affectivity, but separable from it. Again, we sometimes refer to findings based on measures of optimism because the findings can be striking, but we eagerly await the day when a full set of findings based on measures of positive affect, as well as related concepts, is available. The sections of this article that address research on positive affect similarly include studies using a variety of affect measures and mood inductions. Most researchers focus on global pleasant affect, without discriminating among specific positive emotions or between emotions and moods, and our review reflects this characteristic of the field. Finally, when no research on positive affect is available, we infrequently cite the literature on negative affect or depression. Although positive affect and negative affect often exhibit a degree of independence in the long run (e.g., Diener, Smith, & Fujita, 1995), these two types of affect regularly show moderate inverse relations across individuals, justifying the use of such negative states as the inverse of PA or subjective well-being, to address our questions when more direct measures are absent. Furthermore, depression has been defined not only by high levels of negative affect, but also low levels of PA (Watson & Clark, 1995). CROSS-SECTIONAL EVIDENCE Question 1: Are Happy People Successful People? Being successful means accomplishing those things that are valued by one’s culture, flourishing in terms of the goals set forth by one’s society. Hence, our focal question is whether happy people on average are better able to achieve the values and goals they have been socialized to believe are worthwhile. As Sigmund Freud reportedly once said, lieben und arbeiten—to love and to work—are what a “normal” person should be able to perform well. Few people would oppose, in any culture, the addition of health to love and work as a critical ingredient to a successful life. Accordingly, the following section is divided into three parts: work life, social relationships, and health. Specifically, in this section, we review the cross-sectional evidence addressing the question of whether happy people are relatively more successful in various life domains, ranging from marriage to work, from altruistic community involvement to making money, and from mental health to physical health and longevity. Work Life In modern Western society, work fills a large number of people’s waking hours. Furthermore, it is important in terms of producing income, influencing self-esteem, creating opportunities for meaningful activities, and producing the goods and services needed by society. Thus, work is highly valued. Are happy people more successful than their less happy peers on job-related and performance variables? We will first review the correlational evidence bearing on this question (for study information and effect sizes, see Panel 1 of Table 1). Employment and Quality of Work The cross-sectional evidence reveals that happy workers enjoy multiple advantages over their less happy peers. Individuals high in subjective well-being are more likely to secure job interviews, to be evaluated more positively by supervisors once they obtain a job, to show superior performance and productivity, and to handle managerial jobs better. They are also less likely to show counterproductive workplace behavior and job burnout. Even before entering the workforce, people with high subjective well-being are more likely to graduate from college (Frisch et al., 2004). Furthermore, happy individuals appear to secure “better” jobs. In one study, employees high in dispositional positive affect had jobs, as rated by trained observers, that had more autonomy, meaning, and variety (Staw, Sutton, & Pelled, 1994). Finally, evidence from a variety of sources shows that happy people are more satisfied with their jobs (e.g., Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000; Tait, Padgett, & Baldwin, 1989; Weiss, Nicholas, & Daus, 1999). In a meta-analysis of 27 studies of affect and job satisfaction, Connolly and Viswesvaran concluded that 10%–25% of the variance in job satisfaction was accounted for by measures of dispositional affect. In their analyses, the mean corrected correlation between positive affect and job satisfaction was .49. Once a happy person obtains a job, he or she is more likely to succeed. Employees high in dispositional positive affect receive relatively more favorable evaluations from supervisors and others (Staw et al., 1994). For example, in Staw and colleagues’ study, managers of high positive affect employees of three Midwestern organizations gave them higher evaluations for work quality, productivity, dependability, and creativity. Wright and his colleagues have replicated this effect, showing that happy people receive higher ratings from supervisors (Cropanzano & Wright, 1999; Wright & Staw, 1999). Finally, work performance may be more strongly predicted by well-being than by job satisfaction. In two studies, Wright and Cropanzano (2000) found that job performance, as judged by supervisors, was significantly correlated with well-being (rs of .32 and .34, respectively), but uncorrelated with measures of job satisfaction (rs of ⫺.08 and .08, respectively). Staw and Barsade (1993) found that, as rated by objective observers, those high in dispositional positive affect performed objectively better on a manager assessment task (including leadership and mastery of information). Other evidence for happy people’s relative success on the job includes findings that individuals high in dispositional positive affect are more likely to be in the supervisory in-group (Graen, 1976). Dormitory resident advisors were rated by residents as being more effective if they were high on trait positive affect (DeLuga & Mason, 2000), and happier BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT cricket players had higher batting averages (Totterdell, 2000). George (1995) found that service departments with happy leaders were more likely to receive high ratings from customers, and that the positive affective tone of the sales force was an independent predictor of customer satisfaction. Corroborating these results, a recent study showed that CEOs of manufacturing companies with high positive affect were relatively more likely to have employees who rated themselves as happy and healthy, and who reported a positive, warm climate for performance. In turn, organizational climate was correlated with productivity (r ⫽ .31) and profitability (r ⫽ .36; Foster, Hebl, West, & Dawson, 2004). Of interest, these patterns were not found for negative affect. Finally, optimistic life insurance agents appear to sell more insurance (Seligman & Schulman, 1986), and optimistic CEOs receive higher performance ratings from the chairpersons of their boards and head companies with greater returns on investment (Pritzker, 2002). Coté (1999) reviewed the effects of well-being on job performance, and concluded that the causal relation between pleasant affect and strong performance is bidirectional. Undoubtedly, one of the reasons that happy, satisfied workers are more likely to be high performers on the job is that they are less likely to show “job withdrawal”—namely, absenteeism, turnover, job burnout, and retaliatory behaviors (Donovan, 2000; Locke, 1975; Porter & Steers, 1973; Thoresen, Kaplan, Barsky, Warren, & de Chermont, 2003). For example, positive moods at work predicted lower withdrawal and organizational retaliation and higher organizational citizenship behavior (Donovan, 2000; see also Credé, Chernyshenko, Stark, & Dalal, 2005; Miles, Borman, Spector, & Fox, 2002; Thoresen et al., 2003), as well as lower job burnout (Wright & Cropanzano, 1998). Positive affect at work has also been found to be directly associated with reduced absenteeism (George, 1989). Finally, those who experience low arousal positive affect on the job are less likely to want to quit and to be in conflict with other workers (Van Katwyk, Fox, Spector, & Kelloway, 2000). 823 comes from studies of individuals who are satisfied with their jobs. Notably, both positive affect on the job and chronic happiness have been found to predict job satisfaction (Weiss et al., 1999). In turn, job satisfaction predicts organizational citizenship behavior—that is, acts that go beyond the requirements of the job, such as spreading goodwill and aiding coworkers (Donovan, 2000; George & Brief, 1992; Organ, 1988). However, studies of recurring positive affect corroborate these results. Borman, Penner, Allen, and Motowildo (2001) reviewed evidence showing that positive affect predicts organizational citizenship, and that negative affect inversely correlates with it, even when peer ratings rather than self-ratings of citizenship are used. In addition, George and Brief argued that habitual positive affect at work is pivotal in understanding so-called “organizational spontaneity,” which includes helping coworkers, protecting the organization, making constructive suggestions, and developing one’s own abilities within the organization (see also Donovan, 2000). Community Involvement Despite a scarcity of studies in this area, some evidence underscores the contributions of happy people to their communities. Happy people appear to volunteer at higher levels than their unhappy peers for charity and community service groups, including religious, political, educational, and health-related organizations (Krueger, Hicks, & McGue, 2001; Thoits & Hewitt, 2001) and to invest more hours in volunteer service (Thoits & Hewitt, 2001). Furthermore, in a study of Israeli high school students, those with high positive affect were more likely to be involved in community service and to express a desire to contribute to society and be of assistance to others (Magen & Aharoni, 1991). In summary, as we describe in the section on prosocial behavior, happy people seem to be relatively more inclined to help others (Feingold, 1983). Social Relationships Income An important indicator of success in modern societies is income. Do happier people earn higher incomes? Several studies suggest the answer to be yes. For example, a study of 24,000 German residents revealed a correlation between income and life satisfaction of .20 (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2004), and a study of Russians revealed correlations between real household income and happiness of .48 in 1995 and .39 in 2000 (Graham, Eggers & Sukhtankar, in press). Among indigenous Malaysian farmers, the correlation between life satisfaction and material wealth (their only available indicator of income) was found to be .23 (Howell, Howell, & Schwabe, in press). According to Diener and BiswasDiener (2002), most surveys report correlations between income and happiness in the range of .13–.24. In a meta-analysis of 286 empirical investigations of older adults, income was significantly correlated with happiness and life satisfaction, and, surprisingly, more so than with education (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000). Organizational Citizenship Are happy workers relatively better organizational “citizens”? Much of the cross-sectional evidence pertaining to this question Berscheid (2003) highlighted the centrality of social relationships to successful human functioning when she wrote that “relationships constitute the single most important factor responsible for the survival of homo sapiens” (p. 39). Do happy people have better social relationships than their less happy peers? Our review reveals this to be one of the most robust findings in the literature on well-being. Next, we begin by presenting cross-sectional evidence regarding the question whether individuals high in trait positive affect, happiness, and life satisfaction have more friends and more social support, as well as experience happier interpersonal relationships. Relevant study information is contained in Panel 2 of Table 1. Friendship and Social Support Do happy people have more friends and stronger social support networks than less happy people? Cross-sectional studies have documented an association between chronic happiness and the actual number of friends or companions people report they can rely on (Baldassare, Rosenfield, & Rook, 1984; Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987; Mishra, 1992; Phillips, 1967; Requena, 1995), as well as overall social support and perceived companionship (Baldassare et LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 824 al., 1984; see Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000, for a large metaanalysis). In the workplace, employees with high dispositional positive affect have been found to receive more emotional and tangible assistance from both coworkers and supervisors (Staw et al., 1994). Friendship has been found to have one of the highest positive correlations with self-rated happiness (Campbell, Converse, & Rogers, 1976). For example, the happiest college students (the top 10%) have been shown to have high-quality social relationships (Diener & Seligman, 2002). In a meta-analysis of 286 studies, the quantity and quality of contacts with friends was a strong predictor of well-being, even stronger than that of contacts with family members (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000). Happy people also report being more satisfied with their friends and their social activities (Cooper, Okamura, & Gurka, 1992; Gladow & Ray, 1986; Lyubomirsky, Tkach, & DiMatteo, in press) and less jealous of others (Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). Not surprisingly, loneliness is negatively correlated with happiness, especially in older adults (Lee & IshiiKuntz, 1987), and positively correlated with depression (Peplau & Perlman, 1982; Seligman, 1991). Marriage and Romance More than 9 in 10 people worldwide eventually get married (Myers, 2000), and an even greater percentage are in committed intimate relationships. Surveys show that married people are happier than those who are single, divorced, or widowed (Diener et al., 1999). For example, in a study of 19 countries, Mastekaasa (1994) found that married people were happier than all of the other groups. Numerous studies with respondents from diverse cultures support this finding (e.g., Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000; Glenn & Weaver, 1979; Graham et al., in press; Kozma & Stones, 1983; Lee, Seccombe, & Shehan, 1991; Marks & Fleming, 1999; Stack & Eshleman, 1998). Happy individuals tend to have fulfilling marriages and to be more satisfied with their marriages. Indeed, several writers have suggested that satisfaction with marriage and family life is the strongest correlate of happiness (Headey, Veenhoven, & Wearing, 1991; Myers, 1992, 2000). For example, data from six U.S. national surveys indicate that marital happiness is more strongly related to global, personal happiness than any other kind of domain satisfaction (Glenn & Weaver, 1981). Individual happiness is even associated with high marital satisfaction in one’s spouse (Ruvolo, 1998). The findings on marriage generalize to other romantic relationships. Undergraduates high in trait positive affect are more likely than those low in trait positive affect to describe their current romantic relationship as being of higher quality (Berry & Willingham, 1997), and happy people who are either married or in committed relationships are more likely to describe their partner as being their “great love” than their less happy peers (Willi, 1997). Health “A merry heart doeth good like a medicine: but a broken spirit drieth the bones.” —Proverbs 17:22 Are happier people healthier in general? In this section, we review the cross-sectional evidence addressing the question whether happiness is associated with superior mental and physical health (see Table 1, Panel 3). Because only longitudinal studies can address whether happy individuals have higher odds of survival and longevity, these studies are reviewed in the section discussing the longitudinal literature. For a more detailed review, focusing entirely on the relations between positive affect and physical health, see Pressman and Cohen (2005). Mental Health Because positive affective experience has been described as an important component of mental health (e.g., Jahoda, 1958; Taylor & Brown, 1988), it would not be surprising to find that happy individuals are more mentally healthy than their less happy peers. Diener and Seligman (2002) reported that their happiest group of people had few symptoms of psychopathology, such as depression, hypochondriasis, or schizophrenia (see also Chang & Farrehi, 2001; Lu & Shih, 1997; Phillips, 1967). As the absence of positive affect has been argued to be a distinguishing characteristic of depression (L. A. Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994; Watson & Clark, 1995), individuals high in trait positive affect are, of course, less likely to suffer from this debilitating condition (e.g., Lyubomirsky et al., 2005), as well as from social phobia or anxiety (Kashdan & Roberts, 2004). Parallel findings are observed when a construct closely related to happiness— optimism—is examined. Dispositional optimism has been shown, for example, to relate to higher levels of self-reported vitality and mental health (Achat, Kawachi, Spiro, DeMolles, & Sparrow, 2000) and lower levels of depression (e.g., Chang & Farrehi, 2001). Substance abuse is another indicator of poor mental health. Although positive affect is clearly an initial outcome of engaging in some potentially detrimental behaviors (i.e., individuals may smoke or consume drugs to feel good), evidence suggests that positive mood is related to a lower probability of drug use. Thus, not surprisingly, happy individuals are less likely to report a history of substance abuse (Bogner, Corrigan, Mysiw, Clinchot, & Fugate, 2001). Furthermore, lowered positive affect is associated with delinquent activity in adolescents (Windle, 2000). Physical Health According to the cross-sectional data, do happy people show superior physical health? Unfortunately, direct, non–self-report evidence is limited. Not surprisingly, happy people self-report better health and fewer unpleasant physical symptoms (Kehn, 1995; Lyubomirsky et al., in press; Mroczek & Spiro, 2005; Røysamb, Tambs, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Neale, & Harris, 2003). Positive affect has been shown to relate to quality of life in cancer patients over the course of their illnesses (Collins, Hanson, Mulhern, & Padberg, 1992) and to smaller allergic reactions among healthy students (Laidlaw, Booth, & Large, 1996). In a study of individuals with sickle cell disease, positive mood was associated with fewer emergency room and hospital visits, fewer calls to the doctor, less medication use, and fewer work absences (Gil et al., 2004). In addition, those patients with positive moods were relatively less likely to report pain on the same day and 2 days later (Gil et al., 2004). The number of days of work missed because of health problems was also related to happiness in a large Russian study (Graham et al., in press). BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Finally, studies using variables correlated with subjective wellbeing show similar results. Optimism predicted less pain in a sample of aging veterans (Achat et al., 2000), even after controlling for such variables as age, body mass index, and chronic health conditions; optimistic women were less likely to deliver low-birth weight infants (Lobel, DeVincent, Kaminer, & Meyer, 2000). 2. sociability and activity, 3. likability and cooperation, 4. prosocial behavior, 5. physical well-being and coping, and Summary 6. problem solving and creativity. In summary, our review of the cross-sectional empirical literature suggests that happiness is positively correlated with indicators of superior mental and physical health. Happiness, as well as the concomitant experience of frequent positive affect, likely plays a role in health through its effects on social relationships, healthy behavior, stress, accident and suicide rates, and coping, as well as possible effects on immune function. These variables are discussed in the next section. Conclusion Are happy people better off? Although the research in some areas is limited, our examination of the cross-sectional correlational literature thus far suggests that high subjective well-being is related to positive outcomes in many areas of life. Specifically, as displayed in the first three panels of Table 1 and on the top of Table 4, happy people appear to be more successful than their less happy peers in the three primary life domains: work (mean r ⫽ .27),1 relationships (mean r ⫽ .27), and health (mean r ⫽ .32). Question 2: Are Long-Term Happiness and Short-Term Positive Affect Associated With Behaviors Paralleling Success? The effect sizes presented in Tables 1 and 4 indicate robust associations between happiness and desirable life outcomes. Is this because successes bolster happiness, or the reverse? In this section, we pull together evidence consistent with the argument that it is happiness that promotes success, in part because happy individuals show numerous adaptive characteristics. That is, we turn to examining whether happy people are relatively more likely to exhibit behaviors and thoughts that parallel culturally valued success and thriving—that is, the attributes, resources, and skills that help people thrive and succeed. Earlier, we reviewed evidence showing that the key indicator of happiness is the experience of frequent positive emotions. Hence, it is also important to investigate whether positive emotions and moods are associated with desirable characteristics. We anticipate that the correlations involving long-term happiness will be parallel to those of short-term positive moods. In summary, when the relevant data are available, we document the characteristics that cooccur with happiness and PA. In conducting this review of the literature, we attempted to find as many empirical investigations as possible that included measures of happiness and positive affect and at least one other valenced construct. The resulting collection of articles yielded six categories of studies reporting significant correlates of long-term happiness and short-term PA: Each category is discussed in detail in the following sections, and relevant study information is presented in the last six panels (Panels 4 –9) of Table 1. Effect sizes appear in Tables 1 and 4. Positive Perceptions of Self and Others Self-Perceptions Are self-nominated chronically happy people inclined to evaluate themselves and their futures in positive ways? It appears that happy people are characterized by high personal competence and self-esteem (Campbell et al., 1976; Kozma & Stones, 1978; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005; Scheufele & Shah, 2000; Schimmack, Oishi, Furr, & Funder, 2004; Tarlow & Haaga, 1996), optimism (Campbell, 1981; Lyubomirsky et al., in press), and a sense of personal mastery and control (Csikszentmihalyi & Wong, 1991; Grob, Stetsenko, Sabatier, Botcheva, & Macek, 1999; Lyubomirsky et al., in press; Ryff, 1989). For example, Lucas and colleagues (1996) found that life satisfaction was consistently related to self-esteem and optimism in multimethod assessments in which one characteristic was measured by self-report and the other was reported by informants. Happiness is also related to positive perceptions of all life domains. Happy and contented individuals have been found to be satisfied with their family life, their romantic relationships and their friends, their health, their education and their jobs, their leisure activities, and even their housing and transportation (e.g., Lyubomirsky et al., in press; Weiss et al., 1999). Are the correlational findings regarding short-term positive affect parallel to those for long-term happiness? The few correlational studies in this area examining happy moods suggest that PA is also associated with relatively more positive self-perceptions. For example, in a correlational study, higher levels of positive affect were related to being less self-critical (Mongrain & Zuroff, 1995). Furthermore, participants in a naturally occurring good mood set higher goals for themselves and reported more selfefficacy on a laboratory clerical task (Jundt & Hinsz, 2001), and cricket players judged their performances more favorably (Totterdell, 2000). Perceptions, Memories, and Judgments of Others Chronically happy individuals have also been shown to have a more positive attitude toward others. For example, in one study, participants interacted with a female confederate in the laboratory, then viewed a series of videotapes depicting an unfamiliar student in various situations (Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998). Relative to their unhappy peers, happy participants recalled the person they 1 1. positive perceptions of self and others, 825 Mean rs cited within the text are all unweighted by sample size, as our goal was to generalize across studies, not across individuals. LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 826 met in more favorable terms—for example, as kind, self-assured, open, tolerant, and warm—and reported wanting to be friends with her and to be her partner in a class project. Happy participants also liked the person they saw on videotape more than did unhappy ones. Berry and Hansen (1996) found that when participants were “accidentally” left alone with a fellow student, those high in trait PA liked their partner more than those low in trait PA. In another study, happy faculty were found to write relatively more favorable letters of recommendation, and happy undergraduates wrote relatively more positive recommendations for hypothetical employees (Judge & Higgins, 1998). Finally, college women who were generally happy were less likely to be hostile toward other women than women who were less happy (Cowan, Neighbors, DeLaMoreaux, & Behnke, 1998). Happy people also feel more positive than their unhappy peers toward the people that they know. They judge their friends, spouses, and families more favorably (Cooper et al., 1992; Gladow & Ray, 1986; Glenn & Weaver, 1981; Lyubomirsky et al., in press), and are less jealous of other people competing for their partner’s affections (Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). However, we found only one correlational study examining whether short-term PA is also associated with greater liking and fondness for others. In this investigation, happy moods were found to be associated with positively toned inferences and attributions (Mayer, Mamberg, & Volanth, 1988). For example, after reading a fictitious biography of “Jim,” students who had relatively higher positive affect were more likely to agree with positive inferences from the narrative (e.g., “Jim is interesting”) and less likely to agree with negative inferences (e.g., “Jim will get divorced”; Mayer et al., 1988). Sociability and Activity Do happy people tend to be social and active people? In this section, we review the cross-sectional literature examining whether chronically happy people are relatively more likely to participate in a variety of social and physical activities, to enjoy their leisure time, and to experience more energy and “flow” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). Sociability and Extraversion The literature on the relations of happiness with sociability and extraversion suggests that happy individuals have outgoing, extraverted personalities. Extraverts are warm, gregarious, sociable, assertive, interested in new things, affiliative, lively, active, and energetic (see Lucas, 2001, for a review). Extraversion has been found to be related to happiness, chronic PA, and life satisfaction in many investigations—among both undergraduates and old people, in questionnaire studies and in diary studies, and across many nations (e.g., Brebner, Donaldson, Kirby, & Ward, 1995; Diener & Seligman, 2002; Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh, & Shao, 2000; Lyubomirsky et al., in press). Indeed, the findings of a meta-analysis revealed that, of the most widely researched personality traits, the highest average correlation with happiness was affiliation, the inclination to relate to other people (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). It is noteworthy that Lucas and colleagues (2000) showed that positive affectivity is the “glue” holding together various aspects of extraversion such as ascendance, sociability, and affiliation. Cross-sectional studies have shown that levels of positive affect are also positively correlated with measures of extraversion and sociability (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Griffin, Mroczek, & Spiro, in press; Headey & Wearing, 1989). For example, in one experience sampling study, high school students from the United States and Italy were beeped several times a day over the course of a week (Csikszentmihalyi & Wong, 1991). Positive affect reported during any particular time of day was related to feeling sociable. Harker and Keltner (2001) found that women who expressed positive affect in their college photos were more likely to describe themselves as high in affiliation—that is, as warm, cheerful, pleasant, sociable, understanding, contented, and affectionate—and were more likely to be described as such by observers. Social Interaction, Activity, and Energy The empirical evidence suggests that individuals high in trait positive affect, happiness, or satisfaction tend to be more social, active, and energetic. Cross-sectional investigations reveal that happy and satisfied people report engaging in a greater frequency of activities, in general (Burger & Caldwell, 2000; Matikka & Ojanen, in press; Mishra, 1992; Veenhoven, 1994; Watson, Clark, McIntyre, & Hamaker, 1992), and in social interactions (Gladow & Ray, 1986; Lucas, 2001; Watson, 1988) and group leisure activities, in particular (Mishra, 1992). Relative to their less happy peers, happy individuals also report having stronger social support (Matikka & Ojanen, in press), attending club meetings more frequently (Bahr & Harvey, 1980; Lebo, 1953), and holding more organizational affiliations (Bahr & Harvey, 1980; Mishra, 1992). This flurry of activity does not appear to go unnoticed. Happier individuals are more likely to be rated as energetic and active by their families and friends (Diener & Fujita, 1995; Schimmack et al., 2004). Okun and colleagues meta-analyzed 556 sources to determine the relation between happiness and the frequency of social activities (both formal and informal) in older adults (Okun, Stock, Haring, & Witter, 1984). A positive significant association was found, regardless of type of activity (e.g., having to do with voluntary organizations vs. friends) or activity partner (e.g., friends vs. neighbors). Furthermore, in a study of men and women over 60 years old, happiness was related to a desire to learn a new skill or take a class, an expressed need for a larger number of cultural and educational activities in their community, and being informed about politics (Lebo, 1953). Thus, happier people appear to be not only more social and more active, but also more interested and more informed. In summary, individuals high in happiness or trait PA appear to be more likely to approach rewarding activities, especially social ones, and may even be more sensitive to pleasurable stimuli in general (such as social interactions or hobbies; Watson, 1988). Indeed, positive emotionality has been found to relate to approach goals (Elliot & Thrash, 2002). What about research relevant to transient mood? The evidence indicates that positive mood is also related to relatively greater involvement in activities, more frequent social interactions, and increased feelings of energy. For example, the results of 2- to 13-week long diary studies reveal that high levels of positive affect, as reported in daily or weekly logs, are associated with reports of spending relatively more time socializing with friends, BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT family, or romantic partners (Lucas, 2001; Watson et al., 1992) and a greater frequency of being engaged in a variety of activities (e.g., going to a party, a museum, or out for a meal; going shopping or on a week-end trip; Cameron, 1975; Watson et al., 1992). In an experience sampling study, positive affect reported during any particular time of day was related to feeling alert and active, to being with friends, and to engaging in a variety of leisure activities (e.g., sports and games, socializing, and arts and hobbies; Csikszentmihalyi & Wong, 1991; see also Lucas, 2001). Enjoyment of Activities and Social Interactions The data suggest that happy people participate in more activities than their less happy peers. However, do they derive more satisfaction from them? Extensive correlational evidence indicates that, indeed, happy individuals are more likely to enjoy their leisure activities and social interactions, to experience more “flow” states, and to be more satisfied with their activities in general. Lu and Argyle (1991) found that happy community-dwelling men and women were more likely to report enjoying leisure activities and group activities. Happiness has been found to be significantly related to satisfaction with one’s leisure and recreational activities among both students and retirees (Kahana et al., 1995; Lyubomirsky et al., in press; Veenhoven, 1994). Laboratory and diary studies corroborate these findings—that is, happy individuals appear to have more pleasurable and more successful social interactions with others. When female students were left alone in a room with a peer, those high in trait PA were more satisfied with the conversation that resulted; liked their partner more; and reported that their conversation was more enjoyable, pleasant, smooth, relaxed, and less awkward and forced than those low in trait PA (Berry & Hansen, 1996). Kashdan and Roberts (2004) described very similar findings. Furthermore, in a daily diary study, students high in trait PA reported engaging in more enjoyable social interactions than their low positive affect peers (Berry & Hansen, 1996). Finally, in a study of working adults, global happiness was found to be associated with intrinsically rewarding experiences— that is, activities that the individual wants to be doing for their own sake (Graef, Csikszentmihalyi, & Gianinno, 1983). The authors speculated that chronically happy people may be able to perceive any activity— even routine, commonplace situations—as intrinsically motivating, and therefore discover rewards even in ordinary, mundane events. Correlational studies of transient positive moods have also found such moods to be associated with satisfying social interactions. For example, when engaged in an intimacy-building task, those experiencing positive affect are relatively more likely to feel closer and more attracted to their partner (Kashdan & Roberts, 2004). Furthermore, positive moods have been linked with the intrinsically rewarding state called flow. The concept of flow was introduced by Csikszentmihalyi (1975, 1997), who described it as an experience so engrossing and enjoyable, it is worth doing for its own sake. When in flow, people report feeling enraptured, as though in a different reality, lacking self-consciousness, and lacking a sense of the passage of time. Indeed, transient positive affect is often accompanied by flow (Hektner, 1997). For example, the results of experience sampling studies suggest that hour-by-hour experiences of positive affect are related to reports of being in flow (Csikszentmihalyi & Wong, 1991; Hektner, 1997). 827 Likability and Cooperation Likability Is there truth in the sentiment that happy people are disliked because they are shallow and annoyingly cheerful? The existing cross-sectional studies actually show the reverse pattern of results. That is, most respondents like happy people much more than they like their less-than-happy peers. Happy and satisfied individuals are judged as more physically attractive (Diener, Wolsic, & Fujita, 1995; Mathes & Kahn, 1975); more intelligent and competent (Diener & Fujita, 1995); more friendly, warm, and assertive (Schimmack et al., 2004); less selfish (Rimland, 1982); more moral; and even more likely to go to heaven (King & Napa, 1998). Diener and Fujita (1995) found that friends and family members of happy students, relative to those of less happy ones, rated them as more socially skilled (e.g., more articulate and well mannered), better public speakers, self-confident, and assertive, and as having more close friends, a strong romantic relationship, and more family support. These findings were replicated in a recent study, which found that friends of students relatively high on chronic happiness evaluated them more favorably relative to their peers on a variety of attributes and skills, such as academic ability, self-respect, selfishness, and pretentiousness (Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, Sage, & McDowell, 2003). Furthermore, happiness in children, as rated by their teachers, was found to relate to the children’s popularity (S.-M. Chen, 1980). In other studies exploring the happiness–likability relation, secondary school and college students read stories about hypothetical people. Second through eighth graders judged the happiest targets as the ones most likely to help (Perry, Perry, & Weiss, 1986), and undergraduates, no matter what mood they themselves were experiencing, judged the happiest targets as being more likable (Bell, 1978). Finally, in laboratory studies, when female students were left alone with a peer, the partners of the high PA women found the social interaction to be more enjoyable and of higher quality, and so did neutral observers of the interaction (Berry & Hansen, 1996); objective observers rated participants high in dispositional positive affect as evidencing stronger leadership while performing a management task (Staw & Barsade, 1993). Happy moods also tend to lead people to appear more appealing and inviting to possible interaction partners (Veenhoven, 1988). Support for this assertion comes from an intriguing study by Harker and Keltner (2001), who rated the positive affect expressed in yearbook photographs of graduating seniors from a women’s college. The personalities of the women in the photos were later judged both by observers (who interacted with them in person when the women were in their 20s) and by coders (who only viewed the photos). The observers judged women who had shown the highest levels of sincere PA (i.e., Duchenne smiles) as relatively higher on the personality dimension of affiliation (e.g., generous, considerate, protective of close ones, and capacity for close relationships) and lower on the dimension of negative emotionality (e.g., not irritable, fearful, or hostile toward others). The judges, who only had the opportunity to see the photos, inferred that women with high positive affect were also relatively higher in affiliation and lower in negative emotionality, as well as being higher in positive emotionality (e.g., cheerful, sociable, and appreciative of and responsive to humor) and competence (e.g., productive, dependable, and high intellectual capacity). In summary, LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 828 women who expressed genuine happiness were liked more than women who looked less happy. Indeed, the judges of the photos reported that they expected future hypothetical interactions with the high positive affect women to be relatively more rewarding. A study by Kashdan and Roberts (2004) corroborated these findings. Participants were asked to answer a series of questions with two peers (actually confederates), such that ever-greater levels of self-disclosure and intimacy were required. The higher the participants’ levels of PA, the more likely that the confederates felt themselves interpersonally closer and more attracted to them. It is worth noting that, although the correlational studies described in this section cannot definitively establish causality, the causal direction is unlikely to flow solely from likability to happiness. That is, studies in which informants rate hypothetical targets or fresh acquaintances are unlikely to suggest that the informants’ high ratings are the cause of the target’s happiness. Negotiation and Conflict Resolution Are happy people or those experiencing pleasant moods superior at resolving conflict? The majority of research in this area involves laboratory experiments, which will be described later. To our knowledge, the only study to examine conflict resolution in chronically happy people was an investigation of the CEOs of 62 U.S. companies and their top managers. The results of this study revealed that work groups whose members were high in average trait PA were less likely to experience conflict and more likely to cooperate (Barsade, Ward, Turner, & Sonnenfeld, 2000). Furthermore, a correlational study of PA—also conducted in a work setting—found that the experience of particular positive emotions at the office is related to reduced conflict with colleagues (Van Katwyk et al., 2000). affect also related to generosity? The few studies in this area support an affirmative conclusion. For example, according to the results of experience sampling and diary studies, the percentage of time spent in a good mood was associated with self-reported altruism among school-age youths (Csikszentmihalyi, Patton, & Lucas, 1997) and with amount of time spent helping others among college undergraduates (Lucas, 2001). An investigation of high school students found that those who reported having the most intense positive experiences were relatively more likely to be involved in community service activities (e.g., volunteering) and reported more desires to contribute to society and to be of assistance to others (Magen & Aharoni, 1991, also cited previously). Finally, in a study of preschoolers, those who displayed happy moods most frequently were also most likely to show empathy toward others—for example, by providing positive reinforcement or comfort (Strayer, 1980). Research with working adults has shown similar effects. For example, positive affect experienced at work has been related to intentions to perform behaviors that are beyond the call of duty (Williams & Shiaw, 1999), even after controlling for such variables as demographics and trait positive affect. Positive moods experienced at work have also been related to actual prosocial organizational behavior. Even after controlling for dispositional affect, positive affect in salespeople predicted more helping of customers and more customer service, as well as more extrarole prosocial behavior on the job (George, 1991). In summary, cross-sectional investigations suggest that happy people are inclined to be kind and charitable people. Furthermore, these findings dovetail with the work on links between happiness and interpersonal relationships. If happy people are more altruistic, they will be liked more, will profit more from future social interactions (i.e., through the norm of reciprocity), and will have stronger and more supportive social networks. Prosocial Behavior “The good life, as I conceive it, is a happy life. I do not mean that if you are good you will be happy; I mean that if you are happy you will be good.” —Bertrand Russell In reviewing the cross-sectional research on prosocial behavior, we address the question whether happy people, as well as those experiencing pleasant moods, are inclined to be more altruistic, generous, and charitable people. Individuals who score high on happiness or trait PA report in correlational questionnaire studies a relatively greater interest in helping people (Feingold, 1983), a tendency to act in a prosocial or cooperative manner (e.g., as enjoying sharing or helping others; Rigby & Slee, 1993), and intentions to perform specific altruistic, courteous, or conscientious behaviors at work (e.g., helping a colleague with work problems despite one’s own heavy workload; Williams & Shiaw, 1999). Happy people also report having performed more altruistic acts in the recent past (e.g., shopping for a sick friend or stopping to help a stranger; Krueger et al., 2001). Furthermore, in two studies that tracked subjects’ behaviors from once to seven times a day, students high in trait PA reported spending a relatively greater percentage of their time helping others (Lucas, 2001). Have similar effects been found in cross-sectional investigations of transient mood—that is, is the day-to-day experience of positive Physical Well-Being and Coping The literature on physical well-being, healthy behavior, and coping has not generally focused on positive affect or positive experience (for some notable exceptions, see Antonovsky, 1988, 1993; Seligman, 1991; Snyder, 2000). Instead, the overwhelming majority of research in this area has examined the effects of hostility, stress, depression, and anxiety (Kubzansky & Kawachi, 2000). Thus, few studies on health have measured well-being, although some have included assessments of such related constructs as optimism, hope, and sense of humor. Next, we review the small number of relevant correlational studies available in this area. Health Perceptions Happy people consistently report themselves as healthier. Relative to their less happy peers, happy respondents rate themselves higher in global health (e.g., Achat et al., 2000; Kehn, 1995; Lyubomirsky et al., in press; Mroczek & Spiro, 2005; Røysamb et al., 2003; Stones & Kozma, 1986, as cited previously) and report higher levels of social and physical functioning (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000) and lower levels of pain (Achat et al., 2000; Røysamb et al., 2003, as cited earlier). Additionally, well-being is related to higher rates of patient compliance, a predictor of good BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT health (DiMatteo, Lepper, & Croghan, 2000). However, because positive self-report biases could account for these findings, they must be considered together with other data on the health and coping abilities of happy people (see next). Furthermore, it is notable that Watson (2000) reported small but positive correlations between measures of positive emotionality (extraversion and positive temperament) and injury visits to a university health center (rs ⫽ .12 and .15)—suggesting, perhaps, one of the downsides of the active lifestyle of happy folk. In addition, extraversion and positive temperament were related to more health visits for illness (rs range from .15 to .17) in two samples. These modest correlations may suggest that extraverts show a “readiness to use services” (Rosenstock & Kirscht, 1979). Not surprisingly, high positive affect and low negative affect have also been associated with subjective reports of better health (Benyamini, Idler, Leventhal, & Leventhal, 2000; Pettit, Kline, Gencoz, Gencoz, & Joiner, 2001; Sullivan, LaCroix, Russo, & Walker, 2001) and fewer physical symptoms (Watson, 1988; Weinglert & Rosen, 1995; though Watson & Pennebaker, 1989, found no relation between PA and symptom reports [rs between –.19 and .04]). In one investigation, individuals who worked hard to maintain their happy moods reported fewer illnesses (Goldman, Kraemer, & Salovey, 1996). Healthy Behavior As delineated previously, research evidence reveals that chronically happy people are relatively more energetic and more involved in a variety of social, recreational, occupational, and physical activities (e.g., Mishra, 1992; Riddick, 1985; Veenhoven, 1994; Watson, 1988; Watson et al., 1992). Happy individuals are also less likely to engage in a variety of harmful and unhealthy behaviors, including smoking, unhealthy eating, and abuse of drugs and alcohol (e.g., Graham et al., in press; Piko, Gibbons, Luszcynska, & Teközel, 2002). Within the Big Three approach to personality (L. A. Clark & Watson, 1999), psychoticism and neuroticism tend to be viewed as most relevant to addictions (e.g., Eysenck, 1997), although it is notable that drug addicts tend to have relatively lower scores on extraversion (see Eysenck, 1997, for a review). L. A. Clark and Watson (1999) likewise reported that disinhibition (rather than negative or positive temperament) tends to be related to drug use, smoking, drinking alcohol, and positive attitudes about promiscuous sex. Thus, research has tended to support the notion that, to the extent that positive emotionality is relevant to the question of addictions and risky behavior, it is related to lowered levels of these unhealthy states. Although the data are quite limited, short-term positive emotions also appear to be associated with illness preventative and health promotive behaviors and behavioral tendencies. For example, recent positive happy moods were associated with less cigarette use and alcohol intake (Pettit et al., 2001) and with higher sleep quality and quantity (Bardwell, Berry, Ancoli-Israel, & Dimsdale, 1999). Furthermore, in addition to its links with higher levels of activity and energy, positive affect is positively correlated with higher levels of physical exercise (Lox, Burns, Treasure, & Wasley, 1999; Watson, 1988). In a study of women at moderate risk for breast cancer, positive affect predicted engagement in physical activity, particularly during leisure time (Audrain, Schwartz, Herrera, Golman, & Bush, 2001). Physical activity, in 829 turn, is associated with many positive health outcomes (Fraser & Shavlik, 2001; Shephard, 1993). Thus, positive affect might benefit health by indirect relations to health promoting activities. Immunity The evidence described previously suggests that happiness and positive affect may be associated with enhanced physical wellbeing because of their relation to such variables as physical exercise and social support, which, in turn, are linked to improved health. However, might long-term happiness and short-term positive affect also have direct effects on health? One mechanism through which psychological states impinge on physical health directly is through their effects on the immune system. Immune system disruption has been implicated in the etiology and progression of a wide array of illnesses (cf. Baum & Poluszny, 1999; Kiecolt-Glaser, McGuire, Robles, & Glaser, 2002). Before highlighting the research on transient mood, we first address whether happy people—that is, those who experience habitual positive moods—might show enhanced immune function. Unfortunately, direct evidence on this question is practically nonexistent. Certainly, research supports the notion that the chronic absence of positive affect is related to immune deficiency. Longterm deficits in positive mood—that is, sadness or depressive symptoms—are associated with decreased lymphocyte production (McGuire, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser, 2002). Individuals with attributes closely related to happiness have also been found to show heightened immunocompetence. For example, in two separate investigations, humor was associated with enhanced immune function in participants who were predisposed to use humor as a routine coping device (Dillon, Minchoff, & Baker, 1985; Dillon & Totten, 1989). Furthermore, sense of coherence (Antonovsky, 1993) was associated with enhanced natural killer (NK) cell activity among older adults facing the stress of relocation (Lutgendorf, Vitaliano, Tripp-Reimer, Harvey, & Lubaroff, 1999). Finally, in another relevant study, optimism was negatively associated with incidence of upper respiratory infection (Lyons & Chamberlain, 1994). Research on naturally occurring moods corroborates these findings. It should be noted, however, that results linking positive mood with immunity are rarely straightforward and depend on the levels of negative affect experienced, as well as on the particular immune measure used in a given study (Booth & Pennebaker, 2000). In one investigation, immunity was elevated on days when positive mood predominated and was reduced on days when negative mood predominated (Stone, Cox, Valdimarsdottir, Jandorf, & Neale, 1987). Controlling for negative mood, however, the effect for positive mood fell to marginal significance. In a similar study, Stone and colleagues (1994) found enhanced antibody activity on days with more positive than negative moods. The experience of uplifts during daily hassles has been related to heightened NK cell activity in individuals with cancer histories (Vitaliano et al., 1998; see also Lyons & Chamberlain, 1994). Other research has also shown a relation between positive affect and NK cell activity. For example, naturally occurring positive mood was found to be associated with higher levels of NK cells, but only among women who reported some negative mood (Valdimarsdottir & Bovbjerg, 1997). These results may indicate a possible buffering of the effects of negative mood by positive moods (Booth & Pennebaker, 830 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 2000), or they may distinguish the healthy happy from the repressive happy. Coping Do long-term and short-term PA also play a positive role in effective coping with stress and challenge? If yes, this finding may elucidate another pathway through which happiness and positive emotions might have salutary effects on health. We explore this issue in this section. Cross-sectional research pertaining to the question of whether happy people are better copers is primarily indirect, although a few studies have assessed global happiness. For example, McCrae and Costa (1986) examined personality variables as predictors of effective coping. They concluded that positive affectivity, or trait PA, was associated with more effective and more “mature” coping efforts. A strong correlation has also been found between positive emotionality and coping by active engagement (Miller & Schnoll, 2000). A number of constructs have been introduced in the coping literature to explain the capacity of some individuals to maintain a positive outlook during negative life circumstances. Optimism (defined as attributional style, Seligman, 1991, or as general positive expectancy, Carver & Scheier, 2001), sense of coherence (Antonovsky, 1988, 1993), hope (Snyder, 2000), and hardiness (Maddi & Kobasa, 1991) all refer to general traits that are correlated with happiness and promote positive feelings during difficult circumstances, and all have been linked with important health outcomes (e.g., Maruta, Colligan, Malinchoc, & Offord, 2000; Peterson, Seligman, Yurko, Martin, & Friedman, 1998; Snyder, 2000). For example, research has demonstrated that optimistic individuals attend to and remember potentially threatening healthrelevant information more than pessimistic ones (Aspinwall, 1998; Aspinwall & Brunhart, 1996) and use humor and positive reframing, instead of denial, when coping with highly stressful events (Carver et al., 1993). A similar construct— hope—was also found to relate to adaptive coping with cancer (Irving, Snyder, & Crowson, 1998). One possibility is that the effects of these constructs on positive mood mediate their relations to physical health outcomes (as has been shown by Segerstrom, 2000, in the case of optimism). Although these concepts differ in a variety of ways, their correlations with positive affect are well established (cf. L. A. Clark & Watson, 1991). To the extent that happiness predisposes people to look on the bright side, it should relate to superior coping during difficult times. For example, in one study, happy students reported experiencing similar types of both positive and negative life events as did their less happy peers, but the happy students tended to think about both types of events more favorably and positively—for example, by seeing humor and didactic value in adversity and by emphasizing recent improvement in their lives (Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998). In summary, research supports the argument that the extent to which a person can maintain sensitivity to pleasurable opportunities, even in difficult times, may be highly adaptive. Research in a variety of areas demonstrates that positive experience and positive emotion, even in the midst of stress or challenge, may be associated not with maladjustment and denial, but, rather, with particularly good outcomes. Indeed, Aspinwall (1998) has argued cogently for the recognition of positive affect as a resource in coping and self-regulation. Thus, although previous models viewed positive affect as primarily sending a “maintain” message to the goal seeker (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981, 1990), Aspinwall pointed out the role of positive affect in harnessing attention even to negatively tinged information. A number of studies are consistent with this argument. Keltner and Bonanno (1997) showed that the expression of genuine positive emotion (particularly Duchenne laughter) during bereavement relates to heightened adjustment on a variety of levels. Fredrickson and colleagues found that positive emotions undo the effects of negative emotion on cardiovascular function (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, & Tugade, 2000). A study of coping with stress following the September 11, 2001, attacks in the United States found that resilient individuals were less likely to experience depression and more likely to report increases in psychological growth after the attacks (Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003). Of importance, positive emotions experienced after the attacks completely mediated the relation between resilience and coping variables. Finally, Pennebaker (1993) found that those who used relatively more positive than negative emotion words while writing expressively during difficult or distressing times were most likely to benefit from disclosive writing (cf. Pennebaker & Francis, 1996; Pennebaker, Mayne, & Francis, 1997). Thus, the experience of the positive in the context of traumatic or negative events has implications for psychological and physical well-being. These moments of positive emotion may be viewed as opportunities to replenish one’s system, which has been depleted by grief (cf. Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000, for a similar view). Creativity and Problem Solving “The happiest people are those who think the most interesting thoughts.” —William Lyon Phelps Flexibility and Originality The few existing correlational studies in this area suggest that chronically happy people score higher on measures of creativity. For example, relatively higher scores on tests of creativity have been documented in happy, relaxed, and bold children (Cacha, 1976) and in individuals with hypomanic personality traits (Schuldberg, 1990). Certain personality characteristics such as flexibility and openness are related to creativity, and these traits are most common among those who have periods of hypomania without depression, but are not as common in those who have hypomania with intermittent depression (Shapiro & Weisberg, 1999). Notably, the most creative group in the Shapiro and Weisberg study was composed of individuals with elevated moods and without symptoms of depression. Furthermore, eminently creative people have been shown to be characterized by dominance and self-confidence (Feist, 1998)—two facets of extraversion to be consistently related to long-term well-being (e.g., Lucas et al., 2000). However, Openness (the fifth major factor in the Big Five Factor Model) is usually not related to chronic happiness (McCrae & Costa, 1991; rs ⫽ .01 and ⫺.05), suggesting that the connection between happiness and flexibility is at the momentary level, not necessarily at the longterm level of personality. BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Obviously, research on whether happy individuals are relatively more flexible is scarce, and much more research is needed to draw firm conclusions in this area. Fortunately, studies of naturally occurring moods—although also rare— corroborate these findings. For example, Richards (1994) described “everyday creativity” in which people find new ways to approach activities and problems in their daily lives. She found that everyday creativity occurred when people were in a normal or elevated mood, and rarely when they were depressed. Positive moods—particularly those involving high-arousal emotions such as excitement or joy—are also related to curiosity and desire for exploration (Kashdan, Rose, & Fincham, 2004). In a field study, positive affect expressed by employees on the job was correlated .30 with supervisors’ evaluations of the employees’ creativity (Staw et al., 1994). Finally, mildly manic states have been found to be associated with creative episodes in such fields as poetry (Richards, 1994; Richards & Kinney, 1990; cf. Jamison, 1990). 831 positive construals of self and other (mean r ⫽ .39), sociability and activity (mean r ⫽ .33), prosocial behavior (mean r ⫽ .32), popularity (mean r ⫽ .31), healthy behavior (mean r ⫽ .33), high immune functioning (mean r ⫽ .33), and good coping with distress (mean r ⫽ .34). The evidence, although less conclusive, also suggests that chronically happy people and those in pleasant moods might be more creative as well as more efficient problem solvers (mean r ⫽ .26) and might show superior conflict resolution skills (mean r ⫽ .23). Most, if not all, of these attributes appear to promote active goal involvement, which is adaptive in many circumstances and likely facilitates success in a broad range of life domains. However, additional research is needed in many of the areas we reviewed, because only a handful of studies are available that are relevant to certain domains. LONGITUDINAL EVIDENCE Question 3: Does Happiness Precede Success? Performance on Complex Mental Tasks Surprisingly, few cross-sectional studies have examined the performance of chronically happy people on complex mental tasks. One exception is an investigation by Staw and Barsade (1993), who had participants perform a manager assessment task that was based on a complex 3-hr “in-basket task”—that is, a procedure assessing a person’s ability to effectively complete a series of diverse assignments. As rated by objective observers, those high in dispositional positive affect performed better on the manager assessment task—for example, they received relatively higher scores on mastery of information. In another set of studies, after being led to believe that they “failed” on an earlier task, chronically happy individuals showed superior performance, relative to less happy individuals, on the reading comprehension portion of the Graduate Record Examination (Lyubomirsky, Kasri, Zehm, & Dickerhoof, 2005). Research on choice and decision making, albeit indirect, further suggests that happy people make better and more efficient decision makers. For example, people high in well-being appear to be less susceptible to negative influences from advertising (Geier, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2003). Happy individuals are also more likely than their unhappy peers to optimize or satisfice in their decision making, rather than maximizing to achieve the best outcome regardless of the cost in time and effort (B. Schwartz et al., 2002). That is, unhappy people carefully search for the very best answer, regardless of the importance of the context or the novelty of the problem. This effortful strategy is likely to be inefficient in situations that have been frequently encountered in the past and for which a reasonable solution has been found, as well as in much of life in which time is limited and tasks are complex. Is positive affect also associated with enhanced performance on complex mental tasks? The overwhelming majority of the literature in this area consists of experimental studies; hence, these investigations are discussed in a later section. Conclusion In this section, we reviewed cross-sectional evidence suggesting that both long-term well-being and momentary positive affect are associated with a number of desirable characteristics, including Our review of the cross-sectional literature revealed how extensive and robust are the correlations found between chronic happiness, as well as short-term positive affect, and numerous indicators of culturally valued success, including successful outcomes in work, relationships, and health, as well as characteristics and behaviors paralleling these outcomes. However robust and wideranging the correlations we observed, they offer only preliminary evidence that a causal relationship might exist between happiness and success. In the next two sections, we consider the much smaller number of longitudinal studies, which increase our confidence—though not definitively—in the notion that long-term happiness and short-term positive moods might actually cause the outcomes with which they correlate. The relevant study characteristics are presented in the first half of Table 2, and the effect sizes are displayed in Table 2 and in the middle panel of Table 4. Work Life Employment and Quality of Work Longitudinal studies corroborate the correlational literature linking happiness and desirable work outcomes. For example, people with high subjective well-being who are interviewing for a job are relatively more likely to receive a callback second interview 3 months later (Burger & Caldwell, 2000). A study by Roberts, Caspi, and Moffitt (2003) is instructive regarding the causal direction between happiness and work outcomes because measures of positive affect were collected at age 18 and the work outcomes were assessed at age 26. Positive affectivity at the end of adolescence predicted outcomes such as financial independence, occupational attainment, and work autonomy in young adulthood. However, positive job characteristics also led to increases in positive affect, suggesting a bidirectional influence. Paralleling these findings, in a prospective longitudinal study, employees high in dispositional positive affect had jobs 1.5 years later that allowed them more autonomy, meaning, and variety (Staw et al., 1994). Furthermore, Verkley and Stolk (1989) found that people who were happy were less likely to lose their jobs in the ensuing period. Additionally, unemployed individuals with high subjective wellbeing were more likely to be reemployed at follow-up than unhappy people. Although the effects of subjective well-being were LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 832 small in both directions in this study, they persisted when factors such as length of unemployment and health were statistically controlled. Once employed, those who show high positive affect on the job receive relatively more favorable evaluations from supervisors— for example, for quality of work, productivity, and dependability—a year and a half later (Staw et al., 1994). Happy people have also been shown to receive higher supervisory ratings over time in other studies (Cropanzano & Wright, 1999; Wright & Staw, 1999). For example, happier employees were rated by their administrative officers as superior up to 3.5 years later in the four dimensions of support, work facilitation, goal emphasis, and team building (Wright & Staw, 1999, Study 1). Positive affect on the job has also been found to predict reduced absenteeism 5 months later (Pelled & Xin, 1999). Finally, a construct related to positive affect, dispositional optimism, predicted the success of life insurance agents (Seligman & Schulman, 1986). Income Longitudinal and prospective studies of the link between happiness and income are more persuasive regarding causal direction than cross-sectional investigations. For example, in an Australian panel study, young adults who described themselves as happy during a particular period of time were more likely to increase in income during the following period (Marks & Fleming, 1999). Similar results were obtained in a Russian panel study, in which individuals’ happiness levels in 1995 were positively correlated with higher income and lower unemployment in 2000, even after controlling for other demographic variables (Graham et al., in press). Diener and his colleagues related measures of cheerfulness as students entered college to the respondents’ income when they were in their 30s (Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik, 2002). Students with greater cheerfulness in the first year of college earned more money 16 years later, and this effect persisted even after controlling for their parents’ income. The effect of cheerfulness was strongest for the respondents whose parents were well off. When the parents’ income was high (i.e., above $50,000 U.S.), the most cheerful college students later made $25,000 more per year than did the least cheerful college students. Furthermore, the cheerful college students were less likely to experience long-term unemployment after college. Corroborating these results, in a study of working adults, employees who were high in dispositional positive affect received greater pay increases over time than those low in positive affect (Staw et al., 1994). These data suggest that high subjective well-being leads to later financial prosperity. Social Relationships Friendship, Social Support, and Marriage While a number of longitudinal studies have examined marriage, we are aware of only one longitudinal investigation relevant to social support. In the workplace, employees who showed high dispositional positive affect received more emotional and tangible assistance more than a year later (Staw et al., 1994). This study suggests that happy people may draw others to help and befriend them. As described earlier, numerous cross-sectional investigations have documented a link between happiness and the state of being married. However, the most powerful evidence for the argument that happiness leads to marriage, as opposed to the reverse, comes from several longitudinal investigations (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2003; Marks & Fleming, 1999; Spanier & Furstenberg, 1982; see also Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001). Marks and Fleming (1999) conducted a longitudinal study with four cohorts of nationally representative young Australians, who participated every 1 to 2 years over the course of a total of up to 15 years. Unmarried respondents who were one standard deviation above the mean on happiness were 1.5 times more likely to be married at a later time period than those with mean levels of happiness. Unmarried respondents two standard deviations above the mean on happiness were twice as likely to be married later. A 16-year panel study carried out in Germany corroborated these results (Lucas et al., 2003). German men and women who were highly satisfied with their lives were more likely to get married 4 or more years later than those who were initially less satisfied. Finally, U.S. women who expressed sincere positive affect in their college yearbook photos at age 21 were relatively more likely to be married by age 27 and less likely to remain single through middle adulthood (Harker & Keltner, 2001). Longitudinal investigations also bolster the cross-sectional findings regarding relationship satisfaction, suggesting that individual happiness may bolster marital well-being (Headey et al., 1991; Headey & Veenhoven, 1989; Ruvolo, 1998; Spanier & Furstenberg, 1982). For example, in a 6-year, four-panel study of Australians, participants’ individual happiness at earlier time periods increased the likelihood that they would have a happy marriage at later time periods (Headey & Veenhoven, 1989). Furthermore, in a 2.5-year prospective study, Spanier and Furstenberg (1982) found that happier people were more likely to remarry after a divorce. An even longer term result was reported in the previously mentioned study of women’s college yearbook photos (Harker & Keltner, 2001). The amount of positive affect expressed in these photos at age 21 predicted marital satisfaction 31 years later.2 Finally, an intriguing finding is that people’s global happiness at one point in time can potentially influence the marital well-being of their spouses a year later (Ruvolo, 1998). Self-esteem has also been related to relationship quality in longitudinal research. For example, German adults, ages 18 to 30 years, reported on several characteristics of their social relationships at two time periods (Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001). Respondents’ levels of self-esteem at Time 1 predicted, over the course of four years, increases in the closeness and importance of their social relationships (especially those with friends and colleagues) and decreases in the feelings of security and amount of conflict participants had in their relationships. 2 These conclusions must be tempered in light of a meta-analysis by Karney and Bradbury (1995). In their review of 115 longitudinal studies of marital stability and satisfaction, extraversion was found to have a weak negative relationship to marital stability (average r ⫽ ⫺.04 for wives, ⫺.08 for husbands, and ⫺.08 for couples). However, these results were based on 1 or 2 studies and the measures of extraversion used were not specified. BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Why Might Happier People Reap Larger Social Rewards? Both the cross-sectional evidence and the longitudinal evidence we have reviewed thus far strongly suggest that happy people are better able to develop social relationships and build a rich network of support. To quote Wilson’s (1967) oft-cited review of the literature on well-being, “Perhaps the most impressive single finding lies in the relation between happiness and successful involvement with people” (p. 304). Furthermore, the social rewards of happiness cannot be overstated, as strong social bonds and social support have been shown to further elevate positive emotions and enhance social acceptance, health, and emotional adjustment (Argyle & Martin, 1991; Cohen, 1988; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Myers, 1992) and, as some writers have suggested, even to aid human survival (Berscheid, 2003; Myers, 1999). We submit that the primary mechanism underlying the relation between long-term happiness and the quality and quantity of social relationships is the experience of frequent positive emotions. A growing body of research suggests that happy feelings have a marked positive influence on interpersonal behavior (Isen, 1999). People are more likely to want to become friends with and to provide emotional and tangible assistance to individuals with a positive outlook (Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler, & Steward, 2000). For example, an observational study of children found that displays of happy affect were most frequently responded to by other kids with empathic behaviors—for example, positive reinforcement and physical or verbal comfort (Strayer, 1980). Befriending and supporting a happy person may evoke positive feelings in the friend or helper, increasing the likelihood that they will seek to maintain the relationship (Staw et al., 1994). In Gottman’s work on happy marriages, he has found that the longevity of marriages is best predicted by the ratio of positive versus negative interactions (Gottman & Levenson, 1999). Furthermore, genuine Duchenne smiles and laughter, which characterize chronically happy people, signal to other people that one is friendly and open to social interaction, inviting them to become engaged (Frijda & Mesquita, 1994; Keltner & Kring, 1998; Ruch, 1993). This argument is supported by theory and research on the role of positive emotions in infant– caregiver interactions. Smiles in infants appear to build infant– caregiver attachment, ensuring that the caregiver will continue to care for the child and secure his or her well-being (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001; Tomkins, 1962). Mothers who express positive emotions have infants who begin to express positive emotions as well (e.g., Haviland & Lelwica, 1987). Indeed, such observations led Eric Fromm (1962) to go so far as to argue that “a mother must not only be a ‘good mother,’ but also a happy person” (p. 49). Mental health. Although few longitudinal studies address issues of mental health, the existing evidence parallels the crosssectional data. For example, a longitudinal investigation showed that people who were high in subjective well-being were less likely to suffer depressive symptoms if they became unemployed in the ensuing period (Verkley & Stolk, 1989). Furthermore, in a longitudinal study of the Finnish Twin cohort, life satisfaction was associated with lower risk of work disability that was due to psychiatric causes (as well as nonpsychiatric ones) from 1 to 12 years later (Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004). Within the same sample, subjective well-being was also shown to be negatively associated with suicide. That is, life satisfaction was found to 833 relate to a lower risk of suicide 20 years later, even after controlling for other risk factors such as substance use, smoking, physical activity, age, and sex (Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2001). An additional relevant study concerns substance use, another indicator of poor mental health. In a longitudinal study of more than 1,700 adolescents, positive affect buffered the relation between negative affect and substance use (Wills, Sandy, Shinar, & Yaeger, 1999). Finally, a construct related to well-being— optimistic attribution style—was found to predict mental health problems, poor adjustment, and high levels of drinking 10 years later (Peterson et al., 1998). Physical health. Longitudinal studies are also useful in giving researchers a process picture of the relation between happiness and physical health. For example, in a study of close to 5,000 individuals, happiness was related to relatively better health (as measured by self-reported health problems, days missed that were due to illness, and hospitalization) 5 years later (Graham et al., in press; see also Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004). The experience of positive mood predicted a lower incidence of stroke 6 years later, especially in men (Ostir, Markides, Peek, & Goodwin, 2001), as well as sports-related injuries during the course of hockey season (A. M. Smith, Stuart, Wiese-Bjornstal, & Gunnon, 1997). A related construct— optimism—was also associated with lowered incidence of cardiovascular disease (e.g., heart attacks and angina) 10 years later (Kubzansky, Sparrow, Vokonas, & Kawachi, 2001), with higher quality of life, heightened physical recovery, and quicker return to normal behaviors 6 months following cardiac surgery (Scheier et al., 1989), and with better risk reduction for cardiovascular heart disease 8 months after surgery (Fitzgerald, Prochaska, & Pransky, 2000). Shorter term longitudinal studies mirror these results. For example, low positive affect in adolescents predicted greater delinquent activity 6 months later (Windle, 2000). Furthermore, in two investigations, increasing numbers of positive events were negatively associated with incidence of upper respiratory infection over a 2-week period (Lyons & Chamberlain, 1994). An even shorter term study showed that positive mood assessed on a particular day predicted fewer emergency room and hospital visits on the next day (Gil et al., 2004). Longevity and survival. Is happiness associated with superior longevity and survival? A number of studies—all longitudinal, by definition— have shown that happy people are less likely to die of certain causes. In a study of more than 37 nations, subjective well-being was negatively related to automobile fatalities (Kirkcaldy & Furnham, 2000). A recent study of Scandinavians found that over a 19-year period, dissatisfaction with life predicted fatal unintentional injuries as well as intentional injuries (KoivumaaHonkanen, Honkanen, Koskenvuo, Viinamaeki, & Kaprio, 2002). Research on the influence of emotions on longevity has primarily stressed the role of negative emotions in decreasing survival times (e.g., Denollet & Brutsaert, 1998; Naughton et al., 2002). However, studies have also demonstrated longer survival times, after an illness, for people with positive emotional traits. Individuals experiencing end-stage renal disease who scored highly on overall happiness were more likely to survive 4 years later (Devins, Mann, Mandin, & Leonard, 1990), women experiencing a recurrence of breast cancer who reported joy were more likely to survive 7 years later (S. M. Levy, Lee, Bagley, & Lippman, 1988), and those individuals with spinal cord injuries reporting greater satisfaction with their lives were more likely to survive 11 years later (Krause, 834 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER Sternberg, Lottes, & Maides, 1997). Additionally, a longitudinal study using a sample of 513 Berlin residents revealed a significant link between well-being and all-cause mortality (Maier & Smith, 1999). Because happiness is associated with a variety of life outcomes related to survival (e.g., stable relationships, lower accident and suicide rates, superior coping, and less stress; Baum & Poluszny, 1999; House et al., 1988), it would not be surprising if sustained levels of positive affect should relate to overall longevity. A few large-scale prospective studies have examined the link between positive traits and longevity. Levy and colleagues examined attitudes about aging in older adults (B. R. Levy, Slade, Kunkel, & Kasl, 2002). Individuals with positive self-perceptions of aging lived on average 7.5 years longer than those with less positive perceptions, even after controlling for age, sex, socioeconomic status, loneliness, and functional health. Notably, the effect for positive aging attitudes surpassed the effects for body mass, smoking, and exercise. In a study of older individuals, 12% died over the course of 2 years, but those rated as happy were significantly less likely to die than those rated as unhappy (Zuckerman, Kasl, & Ostfeld, 1984; see also Pitkala, Laatkonen, Strandberg, & Tilvis, 2004). Palmore (1969) investigated predictors of longevity in a sample of 268 older adults. To control for age effects, the author examined the number of years a person had survived compared with the number of years he or she would be expected to live. This “longevity quotient” was correlated .26 with interviewer-assessed happiness. Of importance, the objective happiness rating was the second strongest predictor of longevity— weaker than work satisfaction, but stronger than physical functioning and tobacco use. Corroborating these findings for the link between happiness and longevity, Deeg and van Zonneveld (1989) showed that a 70-year-old man of average health is expected to live 20 months longer if he reports being satisfied with his life one standard deviation higher than his peers. Danner and his colleagues (2001) examined predictors of longevity in a large longitudinal sample of Roman Catholic nuns. Higher levels of positive emotion expressed in autobiographies written at an average age of 22 were associated with a 2.5-fold difference in risk of mortality when the nuns were in their 80s and 90s. These results are impressive given that the environments of these nuns are expected to be quite similar throughout their lives, and that other health relevant variables, such as diet and activity levels, might be assumed to be relatively homogeneous in this sample. In a study of 2,000 older Mexican Americans (Ostir, Markides, Black, & Goodwin, 2000), positive emotionality significantly predicted survival 2 years later, even after controlling for such variables as marital status, diet, smoking, and negative affect. Finally, it is notable that studies examining optimism parallel these results. For example, optimism was associated with lower risk of death for 800 patients followed for 30 years (Maruta et al., 2000; see also Peterson et al., 1998), and men with an optimistic explanatory style were less likely to die of coronary heart disease 10 years later (Kubzansky et al., 2001). In contrast to these recent studies, the well-known Terman Study of gifted individuals found that a childhood measure of cheerfulness (rated by parents and teachers) was associated with earlier death (Friedman et al., 1993). However, the sample used in this study showed little variability in happiness—most were extremely happy. Thus, the results might reflect the fact that above some high level, happiness might not be adaptive. However, this conjecture is obviously speculative. As it stands, we are uncertain why the results of the Terman Study diverge from other findings in this area. Conclusion In summary, although the longitudinal literature is much less extensive than the correlational work, it is still impressive in the robustness and consistency of its results. Study after study shows that happiness precedes important outcomes and indicators of thriving, including fulfilling and productive work (mean r ⫽ .24), satisfying relationships (mean r ⫽ .21), and superior mental and physical health and longevity (mean r ⫽ .18). However, relatively few longitudinal studies were identified altogether, and none were found in the areas of citizenship and friendship, indicating a clear need for future research. In the next section, we continue our review of the longitudinal literature, examining studies that relate short- and long-term happiness at Time 1 with resources and characteristics paralleling successful outcomes at Time 2. Question 4: Do Happiness and Positive Affect Precede Behaviors Paralleling Success? Positive Self-Perceptions To our knowledge, the only relevant longitudinal investigations in this area concern the link between life satisfaction and positive affect, respectively, to self-perceptions. The first study indicates that high life satisfaction can lead to feelings of self-confidence. Using a panel design, Headey and Veenhoven (1989) investigated the direction of influence between life satisfaction and feelings of superiority, and found evidence for causality in both directions. That is, feeling above average on a number of characteristics preceded higher life satisfaction, but high life satisfaction was also followed by greater feelings of superiority. The second relevant investigation revealed that women who expressed positive affect at age 21 were relatively more likely to rate themselves high in competence two to three decades later (Harker & Keltner, 2001; mean r ⫽ .25). Sociability and Activity Sociability and Extraversion Longitudinal studies reinforce the cross-sectional findings linking happiness and extraversion, demonstrating that this link holds even when the two variables are measured many months or years apart (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Costa, McCrae, & Norris, 1981; Headey & Wearing, 1989). Longitudinal studies have also shown that levels of positive affect are positively correlated with measures of extraversion and sociability, assessed from 3 months to 10 years apart (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Headey & Wearing, 1989). For example, returning once again to the yearbook study, women who expressed positive affect at age 21 were more likely to describe themselves as high in affiliation many years later—at ages 43 and 52 (Harker & Keltner, 2001). Social Interaction and Activity Corroborating the fairly extensive correlational results, longitudinal studies of older people have shown a significant association BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT between chronic, global happiness, and participation in a variety of activities 18 months later (Kozma & Stones, 1983; Stones & Kozma, 1986). As another example, in a recent short-term prospective study, happy students were more likely than their less happy peers to adopt over the course of a semester a variety of new goals and activities that “gave them a boost” (Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, in press). Another study focused on temporary pleasant moods, rather than long-term happiness, as a possible stimulus for engagement in social and recreational activities (Lucas, 2001). In this investigation, positive affect at Time 1 predicted the amount of time participants spent on recreation and on activities with friends and family members at Time 2, even after controlling for Time 1 activity levels. Summary As can be seen in Panel 5 of Table 2, reasonably strong evidence exists for the hypothesis that happiness precedes desirable resources and behaviors relevant to sociability and activity, such as extraversion and engagement in activities. The mean effect size (r) for these studies is .25. Prosocial Behavior In the only relevant longitudinal work we identified, research suggests that altruism may follow from happiness, as well as the reverse. Thoits and Hewitt (2001) showed that the causal connection between volunteer work and subjective well-being is bidirectional. Following a large sample over two waves, they found those with high happiness and life satisfaction increased in the hours they spent in volunteer activities over the course of the study. Controlling for other variables, a conservative estimate for the effect size (r) is .04. At the same time, those who volunteered more hours increased in happiness. Physical Well-Being and Coping Health Perceptions and Healthy Behavior We identified only a few relevant longitudinal studies in this area. A daily diary study of sickle cell disease patients found that positive mood during Day 1 was related with lower reported pain during Day 3 (Gil et al., 2004, cited previously). Furthermore, happy people were less likely to drink and smoke 5 years later in a Russian study (Graham et al., in press; see also Peterson et al., 1998) and to describe themselves as healthy in a U.S. study (Stones & Kozma, 1986). Immunity Intriguing short-term longitudinal results were obtained from a recent study that examined immunity indirectly, by assessing susceptibility to illness (Cohen, Doyle, Turner, Alper, & Skoner, 2003). Healthy volunteers were exposed to a rhinovirus and monitored for host resistance to the common cold. Those with a positive emotional style—that is, who typically reported experiencing positive emotions—were relatively less likely to develop a cold, and, important to note, this association was independent of a negative emotional style. Furthermore, typical negative emotional 835 experience was not associated with colds. Finally, in a study assessing immune function directly, cancer survivors with more uplifts than hassles showed enhanced NK cell activity 18 months later (Vitaliano et al., 1998). Coping Research using prospective longitudinal designs has examined coping as a function of traits linked with positive functioning, such as optimism, as well as a function of preexisting positive mood. For example, optimism has been associated with better psychological adjustment after breast cancer diagnosis (Epping-Jordan et al., 1999), with the tendency to cope with breast cancer surgery through active engagement (Carver et al., 1993) and with more problem-focused coping and less denial following open-heart surgery (Scheier et al., 1989). Furthermore, two studies focused on the link between positive mood and coping with potential and actual breast cancer. C. C. Chen and colleagues (1996) found that positive affective responses predicted engaged coping among women who were undergoing biopsy for suspected breast cancer. A study predicting coping and mood following surgery for breast cancer showed that preexisting levels of positive mood predicted the tendency to cope through active engagement (Carver et al., 1993). The effect of positive affect on coping has also been demonstrated in research by Fredrickson and Joiner (2002), who found that experiences of positive emotion at one time period were associated with more effective coping and even greater positive experiences later. Conclusion In summary, accumulating research shows that happiness, pleasant moods, and closely related constructs precede indicators of physical well-being and adaptive coping (mean r ⫽ .27; see Panel 7 of Table 2). Creativity and Problem Solving The vast majority of investigations in this area are experimental and thus are described in the next section. The only longitudinal study we identified was conducted by Staw and his colleagues (2004), who found that positive affect expressed on the job by employees predicted their supervisor’s evaluation of the employees’ creativity a year and a half later (r ⫽ .31). Conclusion The longitudinal literature is undoubtedly sparser than the crosssectional work. We were able to identify few relevant longitudinal studies in several areas and none at all examining the topics of perceptions of other people, enjoyment of social activities and interactions, likability, negotiation and conflict resolution, and performance of complex mental tasks. Despite the scarcity of relevant studies, every single investigation we found corroborated the correlational findings in the direction predicted by our model. That is, both long-term happiness and short-term pleasant moods tend to precede the desirable characteristics, resources, and behaviors with which they are correlated. Clearly, longitudinal research should be a high priority for the future. Fortunately, many relevant experimental studies exist, which offer an even stronger test of our LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 836 causal hypothesis regarding happiness and success. In the next section, we document the fairly large and growing literature examining the effects of induced pleasant affect—the hallmark of long-term happiness— on behaviors, resources, and skills paralleling culturally defined success. In short, this evidence addresses the critical question of whether positive affect causes the adaptive characteristics that help happy people thrive. EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE Question 5: Does Positive Affect Lead to Behaviors Paralleling Success? Positive Perceptions of Self and Others Self-Perceptions Experimental work on induced positive moods suggests that positive emotions have a causal influence on positive self-feelings. For example, students induced into experiencing a positive mood thereafter describe themselves in more positive terms (Sarason, Potter, & Sarason, 1986), assess their task performance as superior (Barsade, 2002), report more favorable global self-evaluations, and recall more positive experiences and successes from their past (M. S. Clark & Waddell, 1983; Wright & Mischel, 1982). Happy moods also appear to increase feelings of self-efficacy. Those put in a good mood report higher self-efficacy (Baron, 1990; Samson & Rachman, 1989; Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in press) and set higher goals for themselves (Baron, 1990; Hom & Arbuckle, 1988). Induced positive moods also lead people to expect more success on laboratory tasks (Brown, 1984; Wright & Mischel, 1982). Finally, positive mood leads individuals to attribute their successes to stable factors within themselves (Brown, 1984). Thus, the evidence indicates that positive affect makes people feel good about themselves. Perceptions, Memories, and Judgments of Others A review of the experimental literature suggests that short-term positive affect triggers not only positive feelings about oneself, but also promotes greater liking and fondness for others. Experimental studies show that individuals induced to feel happy are more likely than those induced to feel sad to express liking for a stranger (Baron, 1987, 1993; Griffitt, 1970). For example, in one set of studies, students were asked to conduct a simulated job interview. After the interview, those who had been previously induced into a positive mood rated the applicant higher on a number of jobrelated and personal dimensions and were more likely to “hire” him or her than those induced into a negative mood (Baron, 1987, 1993; see also Baron, Rea, & Daniels, 1992). Happy moods have also been found to be associated with recollections of positive information about a particular person (Baron, 1987). Sociability and Activity In this section, we consider the literature on sociability, activity, and energy, to determine whether happy moods prompt more frequent, more enjoyable, and higher quality social activities and interpersonal interactions. Sociability and Extraversion Laboratory studies in which positive moods have been induced support the causal direction from positive affect to sociability (Isen, 1999). In seminal research in this area, Isen (1970) found that participants induced into a positive mood were more sociable with a confederate—for example, more likely to initiate conversation with her and be more attentive (see also McMillen, Sanders, & Solomon, 1977). Almost 20 years later, a very similar study found analogous results (Cunningham, 1988b). When left alone with a female peer, male participants induced to feel happy were more likely to engage in social interaction and self-disclosed more to her than those induced to feel sad. Cunningham (1988b) suggested that the positive affect may have increased the men’s feelings of energy for engaging in a social interaction and raised their expectations for rewards from the communication. In another study, participants who had been induced into a pleasant mood reported that they would choose to engage in social activities if they feel happy (Cunningham, 1988a). In summary, these studies support the notion that positive moods have beneficial social consequences. When feeling happy, people tend to seek out social interactions, in part because they are likely to view such interactions as rich and rewarding (Schaller & Cialdini, 1990). Social Interaction, Activity, and Energy Evidence supporting the argument that positive affect promotes activity, as opposed to the reverse, comes from a well-cited experimental study (Cunningham, 1988a). Students who were induced to feel happy, relative to those induced to feel neutral or sad, showed greater interest in leisure activities (e.g., eating good meals, planning a trip or vacation, or going to a party or a sporting event, rock climbing, and shopping; Cunningham, 1988a). Thus, one of the benefits of positive affect is that it appears to boost activity, energy, and involvement in active leisure. Enjoyment of Activities and Social Interactions When experiencing a pleasant mood, people are more likely, rather than less, to enjoy their activities and social interactions and to derive more satisfaction from them. Laboratory studies have shown that induced happy moods lead participants to derive greater enjoyment from whatever task they are instructed to perform. For example, individuals in an induced positive mood are more likely to enjoy category-sorting tasks (Hirt, Melton, McDonald, & Harackiewicz, 1996; Murray, Sujan, Hirt, & Sujan, 1990), and groups put in a positive mood are more likely to take pleasure in a group negotiation task (Carnevale & Isen, 1986). Summary In summary, transient happy moods appear to lead people to seek out others and to engage with the environment at large, to be more venturesome, more open, and more sensitive to other individuals (Veenhoven, 1988). Supporting this thesis, temporary elation has been associated with greater perceived relationship closeness and self– other overlap (Waugh & Fredrickson, 2003; see also Kashdan & Roberts, 2004); increased activity and physical arousal (Schaller & Cialdini, 1990); as well as excited, affectionate, and affiliative feelings (Lucas, 2001; Watson, 1988). Additionally, BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT positive mood is related to higher levels of energy (Lehr, 1982). Watson (2000) reviewed evidence showing that both positive affect and feeling lively and energetic are aspects of extraversion. Indeed, feeling active is so highly correlated with positive affect that Watson and colleagues used it as one marker for positive emotions on their affect measure, the widely used Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Negotiation and Conflict Resolution The experimental evidence supports the argument that pleasant moods boost people’s abilities at resolving conflict. In one study, those with induced positive affect showed a decreased preference for resolving conflict through avoidance and an increased inclination for reducing conflict through collaboration (Baron, Fortin, Frei, Hauver, & Shack, 1990). Similarly, participants with induced positive mood were found to make more concessions during faceto-face negotiations, and to have a weaker preference for handling future conflicts with avoidance and competition (Baron, 1990; see also Baron et al., 1992). In the same study, those put in a positive mood were more likely to solve conflicts through collaboration, and were more likely to offer help to others. Finally, Forgas (1998) found that an induced positive mood had a beneficial influence on bargaining across a variety of negotiation tasks, with individuals in pleasant moods revealing a marked penchant to be more cooperative and less competitive (see also Barsade, 2002). Similar results have been obtained when investigating groups. Carnevale and Isen (1986) found that a group put in a positive mood was more likely to reach an optimal agreement and less likely to break off negotiation and to use aggressive tactics. Prosocial Behavior In reviewing the experimental research on mood and prosocial behavior, we address the question of whether the experience of pleasant moods stimulates people to be more altruistic, generous, and charitable people. Numerous experimental studies have found that happy moods increase the likelihood and amount of helping. Indeed, this effect is one of the most robust findings in the literature on positive mood and social behavior, having been variously called the “feel good, do good” phenomenon, the “glow of goodwill,” and the “warm glow of success.” Both the inductions of positive mood and the assessments of helping have taken numerous forms in these studies. For example, happy moods, in comparison with sad or neutral moods, have promoted such behaviors as contributing money to charity (Cunningham, Steinberg, & Grev, 1980; Isen, 1970) or to needy children (Rosenhan, Underwood, & Moore, 1974), donating blood (O’Malley & Andrews, 1983), and volunteering for an extra experiment (Aderman, 1972; Baron & Bronfen, 1994; Baron et al., 1992; Berkowitz, 1987; Isen & Levin, 1972; Rosenhan, Salovey, & Hargis, 1981). In summary, the extensive experimental evidence indicates that positive affect fosters helping behavior. However, what about negative affect? Of interest, negative moods such as sadness or guilt have also been shown to promote helping—for example, when the helping promises to improve mood (e.g., Manucia, Baumann, & Cialdini, 1984), when the person in need calls atten- 837 tion to his or her plight (McMillen et al., 1977), when the negative mood does not lead to self-preoccupation (Kidd & Marshall, 1982), or when individuals feel they have harmed someone (Salovey, Mayer, & Rosenhan, 1991). Thus, researchers have argued that positive moods lead to helping under the majority of circumstances, whereas negative moods lead to helping only under certain conditions—namely, when the rewards of helping are high and the costs are low (e.g., Cunningham, Shaffer, Barbee, Wolff, & Kelley, 1990). In contrast, there appear to be multiple reasons that positive affect fosters helping (Carlson, Charlin, & Miller, 1988). The research evidence consistently shows that happy moods lead to increased helping. What are the mechanisms underlying this effect? Considerable theoretical discussion has focused on this question (e.g., Batson, 1990; M. S. Clark & Isen, 1982; Salovey & Rosenhan, 1989; Schroeder, Penner, Dovidio, & Piliavin, 1995), and a variety of hypotheses have been advanced. The most persuasive evidence supports the view that happy moods lead to helping through increases in positive thoughts and more favorable judgments of others—for example, by increasing liking for other people (Baron, 1987, 1993; Griffitt, 1970) and enhancing one’s sense of advantageous resources and good fortune that should be shared equitably with others (Aderman, 1972). Furthermore, individuals in pleasant moods may expect that helpfulness will evoke gratitude and appreciation—that is, they anticipate positive outcomes and rewards of helping, as opposed to the potential costs (M. S. Clark & Waddell, 1983; Cunningham, 1988a). In addition, people in a happy mood may be more likely to recall the positive aspects of their past helping experiences (M. S. Clark & Isen, 1982, 1983) and to view themselves as more generous people, as well as to feel more confident, efficacious, resource laden, in control, and optimistic about their ability to help (M. S. Clark & Isen, 1982; Cunningham, 1988a; Taylor & Brown, 1988). In summary, the sizable experimental literature on helping offers persuasive evidence suggesting that positive affect heightens generosity and helpfulness. Moreover, because this research primarily comprises studies involving experimental inductions of mood, the causal direction is generally known. That is, although helping undoubtedly elevates mood, we can be confident that pleasant moods also foster helping. Physical Well-Being and Coping Health Perceptions Although the experimental literature in the area of health perception is scarce, the results are intriguing. For example, individuals induced into a happy mood have shown relatively higher pain thresholds (Alden, Dale, & DeGood, 2001; Cogan, Cogan, Waltz, & McCue, 1987) and lower blood pressure reactivity to a stressful task (T. W. Smith, Ruiz, & Uchino, 2004). Happy moods may also promote health by boosting self-efficacy, optimism, and efforts to battle illness. Participants who imagined being diagnosed with kidney cancer and then induced into a pleasant mood reported greater ability in managing the illness, more optimism about their prognosis, and stronger intentions to follow the treatment regimen and exert effort to overcome the illness than those induced into a negative mood (Schuettler & Kiviniemi, in press, also cited earlier). LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 838 Healthy Behavior An intriguing line of research suggests that positive moods might help people exert willpower and self-control over unhealthy or harmful urges and addictions. For example, Tice and Wallace (2000) showed that inducing positive mood replenishes the depleted ego, allowing individuals to demonstrate will power once again after it has been worn down by temptation. This finding is consistent with a correlational study of smoking cessation, which found positive affect to be negatively associated with urges to smoke in those withdrawing from smoking (Zinser, Baker, Sherman, & Cannon, 1992). Immunity Research on induced mood and immune activity provides some support for a causal relation between positive moods and immune function. For example, in several investigations, participants who had watched a humorous videotape showed increased levels of immunity (Dillon et al., 1985; Lefcourt, Davidson-Katz, & Kuenemen, 1990; McClelland & Cheriff, 1997; however, see Martin, 2002, for a critique of these data). Furthermore, in a small sample of trained actors, induced positive and negative mood were found to be related inversely to changes in immune function (Futterman, Kemeny, Shapiro, & Fahey, 1994). That is, level of proliferative response to phytohaemagglutininantigen, a marker of immune function, increased in response to positive mood and decreased in response to negative mood (see also Solomon, Segerstrom, Grohr, Kemeny, & Fahey, 1997). Creativity and Problem Solving Flexibility and Originality Does a happy mood prompt a person to be more original and more flexible in his or her thinking? Many investigators, led by Isen and her colleagues, have shown that laboratory inductions of positive affect lead to elevated scores of originality and flexibility, often labeled creativity in these studies. The manipulations used to induce positive moods have been diverse, and several different types of assessments of flexible and original thinking have been used (e.g., Estrada, Isen, & Young, 1994; Hirt et al., 1996; Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985; Sinclair & Mark, 1995). It should be noted that, occasionally, people in a sad mood are also more original than those in a neutral mood (Adaman & Blaney, 1995). However, in most studies, it is the positive group that shows the most original responses. Isen (1993) reviewed the extensive evidence linking induced positive affect to creativity on laboratory measures and concluded that there is little doubt that induced positive affect, compared with neutral affect, heightens performance on the laboratory tasks. In conclusion, the laboratory findings on induced positive moods suggest that pleasant emotions enhance performance on simple measures of flexible thinking and originality. What are the mechanisms by which positive affect leads to creativity? The association might be due to the fact that positive moods make the person feel safe and secure, and, therefore, lead him or her to think in more divergent ways without feeling threatened—in other words, to be more playful (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001). Kahn and Isen (1993) found that individuals put in a good mood sought more variety in their choices, suggesting the role of feeling secure and wanting to try new things as a causal mechanism for the effects of positive affect on creativity. Performance on Complex Mental Tasks The data reviewed previously persuasively suggests that positive affect enhances creativity. Does positive affect also boost performance on complex mental tasks? The experimental evidence regarding this question is rather mixed. One body of evidence indicates that positive affect might be detrimental to accurate judgments and logical, rational decision making. Early research on affect and judgment suggested that people in a positive mood might perform more poorly, compared with those in neutral or sad moods, at decision and judgment tasks. Those induced to feel positive affect have been shown, for example, to do worse at logic problems (Melton, 1995) and to be less likely to differentiate strong from weak arguments in a counterattitudinal essay paradigm (Mackie & Worth, 1989). In contrast, individuals induced to be in a sad mood are more likely to value the quality of arguments over the source (Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, & Strack, 1990), less likely to rely on stereotypes (Edwards & Weary, 1993), and more likely to carefully execute all steps in a highly structured task (Elsbach & Barr, 1999). Furthermore, the literature on depression suggests that people in normal moods, as compared with dysphoric people, are often more likely to overestimate their degree of control (Alloy & Abramson, 1979). However, it should be noted that tests of the depressive realism hypothesis have yielded mixed results, with about as many studies inconsistent with the idea as there are supporting it, depending on the outcome measured (e.g., Dobson & Pusch, 1995; see Ackermann & DeRubeis, 1991, for a review). Other studies, however, have shown that those put in a pleasant mood outperform others. For example, people in a positive mood made faster and more efficient decisions regarding a personally relevant task (Forgas, 1989), performed better on a clerical errorchecking task (Jundt & Hinsz, 2001), solved more anagrams correctly (Erez & Isen, 2002), and considered the correct diagnosis of a disease earlier (Estrada, Isen, & Young, 1997). The results of other studies also suggested more efficient processing—that is, participants placed in a positive mood were less likely to review information they had already seen, were more likely to ignore information judged as unimportant (Isen & Means, 1983) and to adopt an efficient strategy for performing a clerical coding task (Baron, 1990), and showed less anchoring when making a diagnosis (Estrada et al., 1997). In addition, the respondents high in positive affect tended to eliminate alternatives that did not meet a minimum cutoff on important dimensions, a more efficient strategy. People in induced happy moods also appear to persist longer at tasks in which perseverance is required (Erez & Isen, 2002; Kavanagh, 1987). Finally, people in a good mood have been shown to make more inclusive group representations, defining others as part of their in-group (Dovidio, Gaertner, Isen, & Lowrance, 1995; Isen & Daubman, 1984) and using a broader definition of group membership (Urada & Miller, 2000). Thus, individuals in a happy mood are more likely to use stereotypes, but they are also more likely to include more diverse people within their groups. BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Complicating the picture, another body of evidence suggests that, although individuals in a sad mood often perform well at simple analytical tasks, they are not immune to errors. For example, sadness can open the decision maker to greater anchoring effects (Bodenhausen, Gabriel, & Lineberger, 2000), because greater analytical processing can make the anchor more salient. Indeed, Staw and Barsade (1993) concluded that the evidence on decision making and mood supports the happier-and-smarter effect rather than the sadder-but-wiser hypothesis. Heuristic versus analytical processing. Based on the research reviewed previously, it appears that sometimes the performance of people in positive moods is superior, sometimes equal to, and at other times inferior to mildly depressed people and those in a negative mood. How can we make sense of this seemingly contradictory evidence? One conclusion is that the experience of positive affect is not beneficial—and perhaps harmful—when one is engaged in mental tasks. Another response to the mixed evidence is to try to determine in which situations people in a happy mood do better at judgment and decision problems and in which situations they do worse. In recent years, a perspective has emerged that people in positive moods interpret their affect as signifying that events are going well. Thus, they are quicker to make decisions and are likely to use general heuristic answers learned in the past. After all, if all is well, then past successful answers are likely to work. Thus, the person in a positive mood is likely to rely on preexisting general knowledge structures (Bless et al., 1996) that have previously succeeded, because the situation is seen as predictable and safe. In contrast, people in negative moods are likely to process problems analytically and vigilantly. Heuristic shortcuts can be likened to mental habits, which allow for less effortful processing. Because heuristic answers are efficient when they are appropriate to the task, people in happy moods can solve complex tasks better and faster, thus freeing cognitive capacity for other challenges. At the same time, when researchers present respondents with analytical tasks for which past heuristics are not suitable, the performance of sad participants is likely to be superior, especially when no task performance feedback is involved. Because sad individuals are more likely to use effortful and detailed processing for every task, they will do better at problems for which past learning is not particularly helpful. However, the effortful processing of sad people will not be desirable for complex tasks on which efficient strategies learned in the past can be used. For instance, Isen and Means (1983) found that people induced to feel happy were better at performing a complex mental task—namely, eliminating unimportant information and discovering useful heuristics to help solve a difficult problem. Sad individuals, in contrast, are more likely to ignore heuristic shortcuts and to use effortful, vigilant processing even when it is not required, and therefore perform poorly in complex and timelimited situations (Gleicher & Weary, 1991). For example, in a recent study, sad participants displayed reduced accuracy of social judgments based on thin slices of nonverbal behavior (Ambady & Gray, 2002). The authors showed that sadness impaired accuracy by promoting a deliberative style of processing information, which can interfere with a person’s ability to understand others. Notably, happy moods can produce good performance even on complex and novel tasks, but only when cues are present to indicate that the situation is important and that care is required. That is, because positive affect signals that all is well in the 839 situation, when novel problems are encountered, individuals in a pleasant mood require information indicating that they need to exert additional effort, to consider new and careful strategies, and not to rely on preexisting mental structures. As Schwarz (1990) noted, people in good moods see little need to expend effort unless it is necessitated by currently active goals. However, cues about motivation, an explicit goal to be accurate, or the negative affect that is engendered by initial failures at the task can all motivate the happy person to engage in more effortful processing when it is needed. Thus, when the situation calls for it, a person in a positive mood is capable of slowing down and analytically thinking through a problem in a more careful and deliberate way. For example, people placed in a positive mood overcame their stereotypes if they learned they would be held accountable for their decisions (Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Süsser, 1994). A number of studies support this argument. For example, research participants who are put into a good mood seem to make riskier judgments if nothing is at stake, but make more conservative bets when real losses are possible (see Isen, 2000, for a review). Aspinwall (1998) reviewed evidence suggesting that people in a positive mood do not ignore negative information if it is important and self-relevant. She hypothesized that happy people will use heuristics in many laboratory tasks that appear to be unimportant, but will use more systematic processing when the task is important and self-relevant (e.g., Forgas, 1989). For example, those put in a positive mood were less likely than those in a control condition to ignore self-relevant medical information about their risky health behaviors, and were better able to recall the risk information at follow-up (Reed & Aspinwall, 1998). In another study involving an important, self-relevant situation, students who had undergone a positive experience were more open to receiving feedback regarding their liabilities in reaching their life goals and their careers, whereas those undergoing a negative experience showed greater interest in hearing about their assets (Trope & Pomerantz, 1998). These results indicate that, in a situation in which people’s weaknesses are related to important life goals, having a positive experience can lower ego defensiveness and make people more open to learning about their weaknesses. Further support of the notion that happy moods can instigate careful processing, but only when appropriate cues are present, comes from research showing that people in a good mood will do better if the task is more ecologically valid. For example, in one study, depressed respondents made relatively less realistic predictions about future events in their own lives in the upcoming semester (Dunning & Story, 1991). That is, depressed participants were less accurate in their predictions, and more overconfident than nondepressed individuals, when predictions were of future events in their own lives rather than predictions of laboratory stimuli (for mixed evidence, however, see Shrauger, Mariano, & Walter, 1998). Supporting the argument that positive affect is likely to be helpful in natural settings, several researchers have concluded that depressive realism is found in trivial, artificial laboratory tasks (Dobson & Franche, 1989; Pacini, Muir, & Epstein, 1998). The authors argued that dysphoric individuals use excessive rational control even in trivial situations, leading to their superior performance in certain inconsequential laboratory tasks, but that nondysphoric individuals perform more optimally in consequential contexts. Corroborating this conclusion, people in an induced positive mood were found to take more risks when the LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 840 stakes were low, but to be more risk averse when the stakes were high (Isen & Geva, 1987; for similar findings, see Isen & Patrick, 1983; Leith & Baumeister, 1996). Summary. In conclusion, happy moods are not a panacea when it comes to solving problems or tackling laboratory tasks. Happy people’s reliance on simple heuristics is a handicap if they are attempting a novel task for which previous knowledge is not useful. On the other hand, their more frequent use of mental shortcuts allows them to allocate additional resources to secondary tasks, thus, permitting them to use their time and resources more efficiently and to perform well at complex problems (Isen, 2000). Furthermore, people in a positive mood are more likely to have richer associations within existing knowledge structures, and thus are likely to be more flexible and original. Those in a good mood will excel either when the task is complex and past learning can be used in a heuristic way to more efficiently solve the task or when creativity and flexibility are required. However, many laboratory activities provide little or no feedback to participants on how they are performing on the task, and, thus, the happy person has no clue in those studies that things are going poorly. In contrast, research suggests that, in everyday, naturalistic situations, a person in a happy mood will quit relying on heuristics when he or she sees that they are not working. In summary, the evidence shows that people experiencing happy moods have potential deficits when it comes to problem solving, but they can overcome these deficits if they are motivated to perform well at the task. CONCLUSION To sum up then, we return to our initial question: Does positive affect promote positive, favorable characteristics? Our review of the relevant experimental literature reveals compelling evidence that positive affect fosters the following resources, skills, and behaviors: sociability and activity (mean r ⫽ .51), altruism (mean r ⫽ .43), liking of self and others (mean r ⫽ .36), strong bodies and immune systems (mean r ⫽ .38), and effective conflict resolution skills (mean r ⫽ .33). The evidence is weaker, but still consistent, that pleasant moods promote original thinking (mean r ⫽ .25). It is fair to say that the evidence is almost nonexistent regarding whether individuals induced to experience happy moods also have superior coping abilities, greater popularity, and healthier behavior. Finally, positive affect sometimes leads to poor problem solving and sometimes to more efficient solving of complex tasks, depending on the situation. It is intriguing, however, that despite the presence of some contradictory results, the mean effect size for performance on complex mental tasks is .25. Summarizing the Evidence In this article, we set out to document three classes of evidence to test our conceptual model, positing that happiness, or the longterm propensity to experience frequent positive emotions, promotes culturally valued success and thriving (see Figure 1). To this end, we documented the cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental literature examining happiness and positive affect and their associations with successful outcomes, as well as with behaviors paralleling success. Although our review revealed gaps in the existing research, it also highlighted the robustness and wide range of the relationships that were observed. First, as indicated by the consistently strong average effect sizes, we discovered a vast number of correlational studies showing positive associations between happiness and successful outcomes within all of the major life domains (i.e., work, love, health). Second, cross-sectional work indicated copious positive relations of happiness and positive affect with an array of desirable attributes, propensities, and behaviors (e.g., positive perceptions of self and other, sociability, prosocial behavior, likability, creativity, and coping, among others). Third, although the longitudinal literature was found to be quite limited— especially when compared with the cross-sectional body of evidence—it was persuasive in showing that many of the correlations we had documented were replicated within the temporal sequence predicted by our model. That is, a number of studies demonstrated that (a) long-term happiness precedes the successful outcomes with which it correlates and (b) both longterm happiness and short-term positive affect precede the desirable resources and characteristics with which they are related. Finally, and perhaps most important, a sizable experimental literature offered strong evidence that short-term positive affect— the hallmark of a happy person— causes a range of behaviors paralleling success. These data suggest that positive affect may very well be the critical mediator underlying the relationship between happiness and culturally valued success. In summary, although many researchers presuppose that happiness follows from successes and accomplishments in life, our review provides strong, albeit not conclusive, evidence that happiness may, in many cases, lead to successful outcomes, rather than merely following from them. Questions, Caveats, and Future Research The evidence presented here highlights the functional benefits of positive affect and chronic happiness. It would be absurd, however, to suggest that chronic happiness is necessary for all forms of success and thriving. Plenty of exceptions are in evidence. The conclusion we draw is much more modest—that positive affect is one strength among several that can help achieve approach-oriented success. Certainly other resources, such as intelligence, family connections, expert skill, and physical fitness, can also figure prominently in success. In this section, we discuss questions arising from our preliminary conceptual framework, bring up several potential limitations and empirical issues, and outline the important empirical research that is needed to address the remaining issues. Methodological and Generalizability Issues Experimental and Longitudinal Designs More experimental and longitudinal research is needed in a number of areas to fill the gaps in our review and to provide a stronger test of our conceptual model. For example, although a substantial amount of experimental research has been conducted in the areas of creativity and altruism, less experimentation exists on the effects of positive affect on likability, coping, and health. Furthermore, few or no longitudinal studies exist in many areas, including friendship, judgments of others, organizational citizenship, negotiation, and performance of mental tasks. Longitudinal BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT research is also essential to confirm that positive affect predicts success even after controlling for earlier levels of resources and success. Cross-sectional studies, in which outcomes are simply correlated with happiness, might produce spurious findings that are due to the causality running from the variable to happinessor the presence of third variables. For this reason, it is important to consider longitudinal studies that examine the effects of happiness on outcomes while controlling for happiness at Time 1 or for potential third variables at Time 1. For example, baseline health might be controlled in a study of the effects of happiness on longevity. Although this procedure has not always been followed in the studies we review, and represents a serious limitation of some investigations, it should be a high priority for future research. Finally, the existing evidence for a causal link between positive affect and behaviors that lead to success currently comes from short-term laboratory studies (Type C evidence in Figure 1). If these same behaviors are also increased by long-term interventions to enhance global happiness and happy moods (e.g., through self-help, therapy, or drug treatment)—that is, collect evidence of Type D—the case for happiness being causally related to success will be strengthened even more. Reporting Biases A good portion of the evidence presented in this article involves self-reports, which are prone to the biasing effects of mood. That is, an individual in a happy mood is likely to rate everything as positive, including herself, her health, her job, her coping abilities, and her marriage. Such potential reporting biases may render some of the reviewed findings to some extent ambiguous. This is undoubtedly a limitation of the evidence. Fortunately, however, many of the outcomes we reviewed are not dependent on global self-reports. Among others, these include mortality data, immune marker levels, income, job supervisor ratings, peer judgments, marital status, and laboratory measures of creativity, task performance, and helping. Furthermore, positive mood biases may sometimes not be artifactual, but, instead, may represent the phenomenon in question. Clearly, more non–self-report measures of key variables are needed in future studies. Assessing Happiness and Positive Affect Can we be certain of the validity of the happiness measures used in the literature reviewed here? Recall that our conceptual model rests on a definition of happiness as the frequent experience of positive emotions. In the studies we review, some of the long-term measures assess happiness, others more directly assess the longterm experience of positive affect, and yet others assess what Veenhoven (1984) labeled the “hedonic level of affect” and what Bradburn (1969) called “affect balance”—the experience of positive emotions minus negative emotions. Regarding measures of happiness, Diener and his colleagues (1991) reported that individuals scoring high on such scales do indeed experience frequent positive emotions. Studies using hedonic-level-of-affect measures also provide a test of our model—albeit a less direct one— because positive affect represents half of such scores. Although negative affect is also included in affect balance measures, the outcomes we review (such as sociability) are likely to result from positive affect. 841 What about measures of positive affect such as the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988), whose items (e.g., excited, active) might be the ingredients leading to success rather than pleasant emotions per se? Although an important topic for future research is dissecting the various types and components of positive emotions, there is reason to believe that positive emotions, not merely its components like energy or arousal, play a causal role. First, experimental manipulations of positive affect produce similar outcomes, and many of these manipulations (e.g., receiving a small gift or listening to soothing music) are unlikely to produce high levels of arousal or activation. Second, measures of less activated forms of positive emotion such as happy or content produce effects that are consistent with those found with the whole PANAS. Thus, positive emotions do appear to be an active ingredient leading to successful outcomes, although researchers need to explore further the augmenting role of arousal/energy and the possibly diverging role of various specific types of positive emotions. For example, high- and low-arousal positive emotions may activate different types of goals and behaviors, such as the goals of influencing versus adjusting to one’s environment, respectively (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2004). Alternative measures of positive affect that have been included in studies reviewed here include facial expressions of positive emotion and positive emotional language usage. Research using such measures is not susceptible to self-report biases and is relatively free from the conceptual ambiguities that characterize our current understanding of the structure of positive emotion. That studies using these more subtle measures have supported the relations of positive affect to valued life outcomes (and have typically done so longitudinally) strengthens the case for the contribution of positive emotional experience in securing the goods of life. Generalizability A critical question is the degree to which the findings presented in this article generalize to other cultures. The majority of the evidence we reviewed comes from Westernized, industrialized nations, where positive emotions are highly valued (e.g., Eid & Diener, 2001). Other cultures, by contrast, give less emphasis to happiness and its pursuit (Lyubomirsky, 1997; Suh, 2000) and hold different definitions of success. It is possible then that the characteristics that follow from positive emotions, such as selfconfidence, activity, sociability, and original thought, are more likely to lead to success in some societies than in others. For example, cultures centered around the idea of avoiding bad outcomes rather than approaching good ones may be less likely to reward those who are high in positive affect. Happiness might lead to outcomes that are considered beneficial only in particular types of societies that happen to be where most research to date has been conducted. Alternatively, the relation between positive affect and success behaviors may also be universal across cultures or may be nearly universal in resource-rich societies. Furthermore, perhaps all cultures value the three domains of success we posit—work life, social relationships, and health— but they prioritize them differently. Thus, an important objective for future research is to collect cross-cultural data so that the limiting conditions of our framework can be understood. 842 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER Situational Specificity Although we found consistent effects for happiness across all three classes of evidence, there was significant heterogeneity across studies in a number of areas, suggesting the need for future research to explore moderators of the effects of happiness and positive affect. For example, although Diener and colleagues (2002) found that happier college students earned more income many years after leaving college, this effect was moderated by parental wealth, with richer students benefiting more from being happy. Furthermore, there are times when being flexible, sociable, and optimistic might not be appropriate, and might distract one from the task at hand. For example, some clerical or accounting jobs might require a person who is asocial and quiet as well as very careful about errors and who strictly follows rules. Lucas and Diener (2003) discussed the types of jobs that might most profit from happy workers and those vocations in which happiness might be less of an advantage. In summary, perhaps the most important limitation to our conclusions is that being happy is more adaptive in certain situations than in others. For example, happiness may be most functional when it occurs in benign life circumstances and may be less helpful during dangerous times. Nonetheless, the advantages of happiness reviewed in this article are rather striking, and a major task for future research will be to determine whether these benefits always accrue or whether circumstances exist in which a dysphoric personality leads to greater success. Causality and Possible Third Variables We found in our review that experimental studies generate the same basic conclusions as the cross-sectional and longitudinal research. Yet, a question nags: Could happy people be successful simply because they possess more resources in the first place, which is the reason they are happy? Certainly, some of the effects we review may be due to some degree to the effects of beneficial circumstances on happiness (e.g., Headey & Veenhoven, 1989). The longitudinal research, however, suggests that this is not the complete explanation because happiness often long precedes the successful outcomes. For example, happiness in college (long before the person enters marriage or the workforce) precedes higher income and a more satisfying marriage many years later, even when Time 1 factors are controlled. Furthermore, in an 18-month longitudinal study that used causal modeling to test two competing models—that is, happiness as influencing five of its correlates versus the reverse—the results supported the happinessas-cause model for 17 out of 18 predictions that differentiated the models (Stones & Kozma, 1986). Nevertheless, the possibility remains that individuals with certain personal resources such as good social skills, high activity levels, self-efficacy, and creativity are likely to be more successful at an early age, and, hence, to maintain and reinforce their success and happiness at a later age because they continue to have more personal resources and, therefore, more successes. Again, however, the laboratory experimental studies suggest that this is not the entire explanation for happy people’s success. Furthermore, it is important to establish that positive affect is the key variable associated with desirable characteristics and, ultimately, with successful outcomes—not the absence of negative affect or depression. Some studies are able to address this question directly, as they include both positive affect and negative affect as variables or manipulations. For example, in the helping literature, experimental inductions of positive affect produce increased prosocial behavior, whereas inductions of negative affect do not necessarily decrease helping (e.g., Aderman, 1972; Berkowitz, 1987; Isen & Levin, 1972; Rosenhan et al., 1974). Regarding social interactions (e.g., Cunningham, 1988a, 1988b), creativity (e.g., Hirt et al., 1996; Richards, 1994), and evaluations of strangers (e.g., Baron, 1987, 1993), it appears that positive mood inductions do not produce symmetrical effects. Experimental laboratory research—for example, on helping, creativity, and task performance—also often includes neutral mood conditions, which do not parallel the effects of happy mood (e.g., Berkowitz, 1987; Cunningham, 1988a; Estrada et al., 1994; Isen, 1970, 1993; Isen et al., 1985; Rosenhan et al., 1974). As another example, some crosssectional and longitudinal studies show significant effects of positive affect, but not negative affect—for example, on mortality risk in the nun study (Danner et al., 2001), on likelihood of developing a cold (Cohen et al., 2003), or on organizational citizenship (Credé et al., 2005). In summary, many of the effects we describe in this article cannot be attributed simply to the absence of negative affect in happy people, although it is possible that some of the effects are due to lack of negative affect. Disentangling the effects of positive affect from lack of negative affect or depression should be a goal of future research, as many studies, such as in the areas of immunity and health, still consistently fail to include measures of positive emotion. Further evidence that the effects reviewed here are not simply due to unspecified third variables comes from within-person studies, which show that positive moods correlate over time with desirable outcome variables. On the days or moments when people are in positive mood states, they are more likely to feel and behave in certain ways than when they are in negative mood states. Although studies of within-person patterns are not conclusive proof of causality, they add yet another type of evidence from which the effects of positive emotions can be inferred. In summary, taken together, a variety of different sources of evidence suggest that positive affect leads to certain outcomes rather than simply being caused by them. Nonetheless, longitudinal and longterm experimental studies, which assess a variety of personality characteristics, as well as positive emotions and outcomes, would advance understanding of the intricate relations between personal attributes and chronic predispositions to positive affect. Process Issues Mediation One conclusion from our review of the empirical evidence is that most of the effects of being happy are due to the fact that happy people experience positive moods most of the time and, by definition, experience them more than unhappy people. However, happy people might possess certain characteristics and behavioral propensities that are helpful even in the absence of a positive mood. Although little is known at present about the genetic predispositions characterizing chronically happy individuals, we cannot discount the possibility that long-term happiness may be directly linked—perhaps through inborn correlates—with particular BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT desirable outcomes or characteristics (e.g., extraversion, optimism, sociability). As a result, these qualities may be evident and may produce beneficial results even when chronically happy people are in neutral or even unpleasant moods. An alternative and persuasive perspective of the role of positive affect as mediator of the happiness–success relation comes from Fredrickson’s (1998, 2001) broaden-and-build model. She suggested that positive emotions broaden our cognitive and behavioral repertoire and allow for an accrual of resources, the learning of new skills, and so forth. In our framework, the experience of positive affect is critical—that is, individual differences in longterm happiness may influence the ease with which a person enters a “good mood” (cf. Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991), but the affect itself is the key to the positive outcomes associated with positive moods. Thus, while happy people overall may be found to enjoy a variety of positive outcomes, the frequent experience of positive mood (in the absence of a temperamental disposition toward such moods) should still convey benefits. Notably, because the key to success is happy affect and not necessarily a happy genetic predisposition, we submit that chronically unhappy people are therefore not fated to failure. Another implication is that future happiness-increasing interventions should focus on how people can adopt new practices and habits, and restructure their lives, in ways that allow for a stream of positive experiences and positive emotions (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005). We know from the experimental studies that momentary positive emotions produce many of the outcomes we reviewed. We also know from cross-sectional and longitudinal studies that happy people exhibit behaviors that are parallel to the outcomes found in the experimental studies. What we do not know, because there is little research on mediation in this area, is the degree to which people’s current moods mediate the effects of chronic happiness on behavior. As mentioned previously, Fredrickson’s (1998, 2001) model suggests that even in the absence of a positive mood— indeed, even in a negative mood— happy people will generally perform better on many tasks because of the skills they have learned and resources they have accumulated because of their frequent experiences of positive moods in the past (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Fredrickson et al., 2003). We suspect that this is a case of partial mediation—namely, that happy people perform many of the desirable behaviors we review because they are more often in a pleasant mood, but that being happy in the past might lead to the accumulation of skills, social support, other resources, and adaptive habits that the happy person can use even when in a negative mood. Thus, an individual’s current mood is likely to produce certain benefits, but current mood might not entirely explain the successful performance of happy people. It will be important in future research to separate the effects of happy temperament, happiness engendered by current life circumstances, and induced happy moods on the characteristics we reviewed earlier. Tests of the mediation hypothesis promise to be an exciting direction for future scientific work. The search for mediational variables might begin with an examination of the various outcomes we have examined here. In this review, we have treated a variety of desirable life outcomes as if they hold equal status in their overall importance in people’s lives. It may be, of course, that these outcomes are themselves intercorrelated and perhaps even differentially important. For instance, the social benefits of positive affect may be the central mediator of the 843 effect of positive affect on other life outcomes. Research that combines a variety of life outcomes will be needed in order to answer the important question of how these various “goods of life” relate to and potentially promote each other. The Varieties of Positive Affect An important topic of investigation for the future involves distinguishing the effects of positive affect at a general level versus the effects of discrete positive emotions such as contentment, affection, curiosity, elevation, pride, and joy. Specific emotions may be linked with specific beneficial outcomes—for example, contentment with originality, affection with sociability, pride with helping, and curiosity with learning and problem solving—and these possibilities remain an intriguing direction for future research. One important question concerns whether the effects of positive affect that we review apply to all positive emotions or only to those high in arousal. In the case of positive emotions, we can ask whether the outcomes reviewed in this article apply to contentmentor only to elation and joy. The work of Watson (2000) suggested that feelings of energy and activity are much more likely to accompany elation than they are to accompany contentment. However, little more is known about the effects of the two types of affect, and whether individuals who have chronic tendencies to contentment will be as successful as those who are prone to joy. An interesting finding in this regard comes from a study that predicted work outcomes at age 26 from reports of emotions at age 18 (Roberts et al., 2003). The authors found that occupational attainment was predicted by both positive affect– communion and by positive affect–agency, whereas financial independence was significantly predicted only by positive affect– communion. In the affect literature, the distinction between moods and emotions is often seen as pivotal. However, in the literatures that we reviewed, the two are rarely, if ever, separately measured. Thus, we are unable to draw conclusions about whether a propensity to positive moods versus emotions is more conducive to the outcomes we describe. For example, the possibility exists that the types of emotions induced in the experimental studies stand out as figure against ground and are more likely to produce the effects outlined in this article. On the other hand, moods are relatively longer lasting and are less likely to be in focal awareness; therefore, relative to emotions, moods may influence behavior in more subtle ways. Once again, assessing moods versus emotions and examining their discrete effects on the behaviors we review, and on various types of success, is an important avenue for future research. Future Research Questions Additional questions for research are needed to extend the pattern of findings we describe here and to support the unifying framework we present. Because positive affect has often been treated as an outcome rather than a predictor of the goods of life, the potential benefits of positive affect, itself, have remained largely untested. Hence, research should begin to address the potential causal role of positive affect in securing positive life outcomes. In addition, examining positive affect in this way opens a variety of new questions for research on the positive benefits of 844 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER positive affect. For example, what types of success are most enhanced by positive affect? Are there long-term beneficial outcomes in some areas for unhappy people? Are different outcomes likely for individuals who are prone to experience different forms of positive affect, such as joy, affection, or contentment? What are the control processes that prevent positive affect from amplifying in a cycle with success and moving ever upward to dysfunctional levels? At the broadest level, what is the optimal level of positive affect in different tasks, contexts, and cultures? Our hope is that our review stimulates research that examines each of these questions in detail. Is Positive Affect a Magic Elixir? Readers of our review might conclude that happiness and positive affect are the royal road to the perfect life. Leaving this impression is not our intent. There are a number of ways that positive emotions can produce adverse effects. For one thing, in some situations, positive affect is not the most functional response. For another thing, happy people might use their creativity, selfconfidence, negotiation ability, and sociability to achieve aims that are not beneficial to society, such as being the “king” of the local bar or even to achieve aims that are harmful to society, such as being an effective confidence man. It follows from our review, for instance, that a happy member of the Mafia might be more effective than an unhappy one, and a happy scam artist might be more effective at committing fraud without being caught. Thus, success must be defined in terms of a cultural and behavioral context, and positive emotions might not lead every individual to be successful at goals that the broader culture or the world believes are desirable. We are aware that many mildly dysphoric individuals function very well in society. They write newspaper columns, run psychology departments, act in popular movies, argue before the U.S. Supreme Court, and perform many other jobs with distinction. Qualities such as intelligence, perseverance, conscientiousness, and social criticism of the status quo are desirable characteristics in many situations, and are to some degree separable from positive affectivity. A happy person with these characteristics might be very effective in many vocations and roles, but some situations might exist, as yet undefined, in which mild dysphoria leads to superior functioning. It is important to keep in mind that human emotional life is rich, and that the relations of positive affect and negative affect to functioning are complex ones. At times, happiness will be most adaptive and at other times may require a level of misery or at least discontent. Our framework suggests that positive bias in a flexible system may well be adaptive, but an exclusively happy life is not only unrealistic—it is not necessarily the most desirable life. A commonly expressed claim is that happy people are satisfied with the status quo and are not motivated to accomplish new goals or to “change the world.” The data reviewed in this article indicate that this blanket condemnation of happiness is inaccurate— happy people perform well in many areas of life, including domains such as work and income, that require motivation and persistence. Happy people can have lofty goals and experience positive affect that is due to the progress they make toward those goals. At the same time, specific dissatisfactions can also motivate happy people to work for change and to pursue new directions. Andrews and Withey (1976) found that people who are happy with their lives are not inevitably satisfied when they judge the conditions in society; they found that satisfaction with one’s life and with society and government were distinct. Furthermore, many of the characteristics observed in happy people— optimism, energy, social engagement, originality, altruism, likability, productivity, good health— are the very characteristics that could help them improve the conditions of theirs and others’ lives. In summary, although happy people are generally more satisfied people, this does not appear to prevent them from being achievement oriented. Indeed, happy people appear to be relatively more likely to seek approach goals. The interaction of cognitive judgments about the desire for change with the propensity for positive affect is a ripe area of inquiry for the future. Do Happy People Experience Negative Emotions? If happy people were unable to experience negative emotions when things go wrong, their responses would likely be dysfunctional because they might not react appropriately to threats, losses, and other significant negative events. If, on the other hand, happy people can occasionally experience negative emotions, as we suggest they do, they might be particularly successful because they can be approach focused most of the time, but not when conditions become adverse. Thus, happy people can remain in a positive mood as long as things are positive or neutral, but experience a negative mood when things are very bad, thus motivating them to withdraw, conserve resources, or otherwise avoid harm. Furthermore, there may be a resetting point around a person’s current circumstances so that people are most likely to react with negative affect when conditions worsen from their current state (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). The negative affect produced by bad events causes a change in thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. When adverse events occur, people quit using heuristic processing and switch to vigilant and careful analytical processing, which does not involve an overreliance on previous solutions to challenges. The person needs to safeguard resources and focus on solving the immediate problem. At the same time, it is functional for the person to return to a positive state when conditions improve. Extending the ideas of Fleeson (2001), happiness could be considered a “density function” in which people vary in terms of how much they are in the state of positive experience. Although frequent experience of this state appears to be functional, an ability to react to significant negative events is also likely to be beneficial. Thus, even happy people show substantial variations in their moods over time. Research confirms that the happiest individuals do feel occasionally unhappy. For example, Diener and Seligman’s (2002) happiest college students all displayed ups and downs in their moods, but stayed in the positive zone the majority of the time. However, the findings showed that they also occasionally descended into the negative range and experienced unpleasant emotions, indicating that the happiest individuals are not trapped in a positive mood regardless of circumstance. We found the same pattern in the World Value Survey (1994)—most of those responding with a 10 on a 10-point life satisfaction scale (the top 14% of respondents) reported having experienced a negative mood or emotion during the past few weeks. In a laboratory study, chronically happy people reported negative feelings (such as being sad, anxious, and discouraged) in BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT response to negative feedback about their own performance (Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997). The fact that happy people can and do respond emotionally to events may provide part of the answer to why they are likely to succeed in life. Unlike in short-term mood manipulation studies, in which an induced positive mood often elicits an inappropriate response to the situation, in everyday life, happy people can react negatively when it is appropriate to the context, but later return to a positive state. Costs and Trade-Offs of Happiness Despite the many benefits of long-term positive affect described in this article, happiness can have costs or downsides in some situations and, in other situations, have trade-offs with other desired values. Happy people are characterized by certain behaviors, as reviewed previously, and, in some situations, these characteristics may prove detrimental. For example, if a happy individual uses a heuristic to solve a frequently encountered problem, but the heuristic now provides the wrong answer and there is no failure feedback available in the situation, he or she will perform more poorly than an unhappy person. As discussed earlier, this possibility likely accounts for findings of “depressive realism” in some studies (Alloy & Abramson, 1979), as well as for the findings that, under some conditions, happy people show more stereotypical thinking because they rely on heuristic shortcuts (e.g., Edwards & Weary, 1993). To the extent that relying on shortcuts leads to nonoptimal performance, individuals in a positive mood may suffer. Clearly, the social costs of relying on stereotypes to make decisions may be great. In addition, because positive moods signal to the individual that things are going well, less happy individuals may be superior at critical thinking and error checking (e.g., Mackie & Worth, 1989; Melton, 1995). Thus, it is unlikely that one particular mood state leads to superior problem solving or task performance in general— rather, positive affect and negative affect probably have differing effects on cognitive processing that may or may not be well suited to a particular task. Lucas and Diener (2003) suggested that mildly dysphoric individuals are likely to underperform in leadership and social tasks, but might excel in jobs such as monitoring a nuclear power plant where constant vigilance for possible problems is absolutely essential. Clearly, the relation of moods to decision making and problem solving is complex and the match between individual differences and situational requirements may determine the adaptive significance of any emotional style. Earlier we suggested that happy people may feel positive emotions more frequently because they are relatively more sensitive to rewards in their environment. Tying positive emotionality to Gray’s (1994) “behavioral approach system” makes a good deal of sense and it also highlights potential pitfalls of happiness. Clearly, the complexity of human life requires that one avoid some circumstances, and always moving toward evermore tantalizing rewards might lead an individual’s existence to devolve into hedonism or inappropriate risk taking. In addition, the tendency to recognize and move toward rewards in the environment may make a happy person susceptible to approach–approach conflicts, caught between two potentially positive life paths. In addition to the possibility that happy individuals might be outperformed by their less happy peers in some situations, it is worth noting that people make trade-offs in their decisions regard- 845 ing activities and happiness. For example, compared with European Americans, Asians and Asian Americans are more likely to persist at a task at which they are not performing well in order to master it and may thus suffer in terms of mood because they continue working on something at which they are not proficient (Heine et al., 2001; Oishi & Diener, 2003). Other researchers have found that placing high importance on goals can heighten happiness when goals are achieved, but can also increase worry about the goals and amplify greater negative affect when the goals are not achieved (Diener, Colvin, Pavot, & Allman, 1991; Pomerantz, Saxon, & Oishi, 2000). Happiness is one of life’s goods, but it exists in the context of a variety of other goods (Ryff, 1989; Ryff & Singer, 1998). Thus, individuals might well sacrifice happiness in the pursuit of other valued ends. Notably, however, it may be the happy who are particularly willing to sacrifice positive affect for the sake of other goals, primarily because of the other resources and capacities that have been afforded to them by the experience of happiness. Furthermore, particular circumstances or degrees of positive affect may lead to decrements in performance. Recent work has shown that extremely happy individuals perform slightly lower in some achievement situations compared with very happy persons, suggesting that the optimum level of happiness might vary depending on the task at hand (Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2005). Extremely happy individuals might be inclined to be too sociable to perform at the maximum level in some achievement situations that occur over long periods of time. Very happy individuals may also be judged harshly for their apparent satisfaction—for example, as shallow or complacent. Indeed, research has shown that happiness in the context of a potentially lazy or meaningless life is judged quite negatively (Scollon & King, 2004). It may be that the positive social perception of happy individuals depends on the particular situations in which individuals encounter them. An apparently happy person may be judged quite favorably in an acquaintanceship situation, but an incongruously happy person may well be judged more negatively. The capacity to downregulate positive affect may be a facet of social skill and effective self-regulation (e.g., M. W. Erber & Erber, 2001; R. Erber & Erber, 2000). Although existing evidence indicates that happy individuals indeed tend to be successful across a number of life domains, we must be mindful that negative emotions can be functional under some circumstances. Individuals who are temperamentally prone to greater levels of negative emotions may help their groups under some conditions. In addition, happiness in some circumstances may be sacrificed in order to reach long-term goals. We do not yet fully understand the limits of the success of happy people, and we do not know the extent to which the effects of positive affect are dependent on culture and cultural norms for emotion. The current findings indicate that happy people are in many ways successful people. This does not mean, of course, that happy people show superior performance in every activity and situation. Final Remarks We have reviewed extensive evidence demonstrating that happy people are successful and flourishing people. Part of the explanation for this phenomenon undoubtedly comes from the fact that success leads to happiness. Our review, however, focuses on the LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER 846 reverse causal direction—that happiness, in turn, leads to success. Happy people show more frequent positive affect and specific adaptive characteristics. Positive affect has been shown, in experimental, longitudinal, and correlational studies, to lead to these specific adaptive characteristics. Thus, the evidence seems to support our conceptual model that happiness causes many of the successful outcomes with which it correlates. Furthermore, the data suggest that the success of happy people may be mediated by the effects of positive affect and the characteristics that it promotes. It appears that happiness, rooted in personality and in past successes, leads to approach behaviors that often lead to further success. At the same time, happy people are able to react with negative emotions when it is appropriate to do so. The desire to be happy is prevalent in Western culture (e.g., Diener, Suh, Smith, & Shao, 1995; King & Broyles, 1997), and a happy life is very much the preferred life (King & Napa, 1998). If subjective well-being feels good but otherwise leaves people impaired, for example, in terms of decision making, social relationships, physical health, or success in life, we might question its net value for society and for the individual. In this article, we reviewed cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental data showing that happy individuals are more likely than their less happy peers to have fulfilling marriages and relationships, high incomes, superior work performance, community involvement, robust health, and a long life. The three classes of evidence also indicated that positive emotions, as well as chronic happiness, are often associated with resources and characteristics that parallel success and thriving— that is, desirable behaviors and cognitions such as sociability, optimism, energy, originality, and altruism. Although our conclusions run counter to the belief that successful outcomes and desirable characteristics are primarily the causes, rather than the consequences, of happiness, a surprisingly large amount of evidence now appears to challenge this belief. References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. *Achat, H., Kawachi, I., Spiro, A., III, DeMolles, D. A., & Sparrow, D. (2000). Optimism and depression as predictors of physical and mental health functioning: The Normative Aging Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 127–130. Ackermann, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real? Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584. *Adaman, J. E., & Blaney, P. H. (1995). The effects of musical mood induction on creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 29, 95–108. *Aderman, D. (1972). Elation, depression, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 91–101. *Alden, A. L., Dale, J. A., & DeGood, D. E. (2001). Interactive effects of the affect quality and directional focus of mental imagery on pain analgesia. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 26, 117–126. Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441– 485. *Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 947–961. Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York: Plenum Press. Antonovsky, A. (1988). Unraveling the mystery of health. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Social Science and Medicine, 36, 725–733. Argyle, M., & Martin, M. (1991). The psychological causes of happiness. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 77–100). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in selfregulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 1–32. Aspinwall, L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1996). Distinguishing optimism from denial: Optimistic beliefs predict attention to health threats. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 993–1003. *Audrain, J., Schwartz, M., Herrera, J., Golman, P., & Bush, A. (2001). Physical activity in first degree relatives of breast cancer patients. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 24, 587– 603. *Bahr, H. M., & Harvey, C. D. (1980). Correlates of morale among the newly widowed. Journal of Social Psychology, 110, 219 –233. *Baldassare, M., Rosenfield, S., & Rook, K. S. (1984). The types of social relations predicting elderly well-being. Research on Aging, 6, 549 –559. *Bardwell, W. A., Berry, C. C., Ancoli-Israel, S., & Dimsdale, J. E. (1999). Psychological correlates of sleep apnea. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 583–596. *Baron, R. A. (1987). Interviewer’s moods and reactions to job applicants: The influence of affective states on applied social judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 911–926. *Baron, R. A. (1990). Environmentally induced positive affect: Its impact on self-efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 368 –384. *Baron, R. A. (1993). Interviewers’ moods and evaluations of job applicants: The role of applicant qualifications. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 253–271. *Baron, R. A., & Bronfen, M. I. (1994). A whiff of reality: Empirical evidence concerning the effects of pleasant fragrances on work-related behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 1179 –1203. *Baron, R. A., Fortin, S. P., Frei, R. L., Hauver, L. A., & Shack, M. L. (1990). Reducing organizational conflict: The role of socially-induced positive affect. International Journal of Conflict Management, 1, 133– 152. *Baron, R. A., Rea, M. S., & Daniels, S. G. (1992). Effects of indoor lighting (illuminance and spectral distribution) on the performance of cognitive tasks and interpersonal behaviors: The potential mediating role of positive affect. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 1–33. *Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 644 – 675. *Barsade, S. G., Ward, A. J., Turner, J. D. F., & Sonnenfeld, J. A. (2000). To your heart’s content: A model of affective diversity in top management teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 802– 836. Batson, C. D. (1990). Affect and altruism. In B. S. Moore & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Affect and social behavior (pp. 89 –125). New York: Cambridge University Press. Baum, A., & Poluszny, D. M. (1999). Health psychology: Mapping biobehavioral contributions to health and illness. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 137–163. *Bell, P. A. (1978). Affective state, attraction, and affiliation: Misery loves happy company. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 616 – 619. *Benyamini, Y., Idler, E. L., Leventhal, H., & Leventhal, E. A. (2000). Positive affect and function as influences on self-assessments of health: Expanding our view beyond illness and disability. Journals of Gerontology:Psychological Sciences, 55B, P107–P116. *Berkowitz, L. (1987). Mood, self-awareness, and willingness to help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 721–729. *Berry, D. S., & Hansen, J. S. (1996). Positive affect, negative affect, and BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 796 – 809. *Berry, D. S., & Willingham, J. K. (1997). Affective traits, responses to conflict, and satisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 564 –576. Berscheid, E. (2003). The human’s greatest strength: Other humans. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology (pp. 37– 47). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 331–345. *Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wolk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665– 679. *Bodenhausen, G., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias: The case of anchoring. Psychological Science, 11, 320 –323. *Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 621– 632. *Bogner, J. A., Corrigan, J. D., Mysiw, W. J., Clinchot, D., & Fugate, L. (2001). A comparison of substance abuse and violence in the prediction of long-term rehabilitation outcomes after traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 82, 571–577. Booth, R. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Emotions and immunity. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 558 –572). New York: Guilford Press. Borman, W. C., Penner, L. A., Allen, T. D., & Motowildo, S. J. (2001). Personality predictors of citizenship performance. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 52– 69. Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Alpine. *Brebner, J., Donaldson, J., Kirby, N., & Ward, L. (1995). Relationships between happiness and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 251–258. *Brown, J. (1984). Effects of induced mood on causal attributions for success and failure. Motivation and Emotion, 8, 343–353. *Burger, J. M., & Caldwell, D. F. (2000). Personality, social activities, job-search behavior and interview success: Distinguishing between PANAS trait positive affect and NEO extraversion. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 51– 62. Cacha, F. B. (1976). Figural creativity, personality, and peer nominations of pre-adolescents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 20, 187–195. Cameron, P. (1975). Mood as an indicant of happiness: Age, sex, social class, and situational differences. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 216 –224. Campbell, A. (1981). The sense of well-being in America. New York: McGraw-Hill. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rogers. W. L. (1976). The quality of American life. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Cantor, N., Norem, J., Langston, C., Zirkel, S., Fleeson, W., & CookFlannagan, C. (1991). Life tasks and daily life experience. Journal of Personality, 59, 425– 451. Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping behavior: A test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 211–229. *Carnevale, P., & Isen, A. M. (1986). The influence of positive affect and visual access on the discovery of integrative solutions in bilateral negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 1–13. Carver, C. S. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: 847 Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 241–261. *Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M., Robinson, D., et al. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 375–390. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: SpringerVerlag. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19 –35. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and selfregulation. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 31–51). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. *Chang, E. C., & Farrehi, A. S. (2001). Optimism/pessimism and information-processing styles: Can their influences be distinguished in predicting psychological adjustment. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 555–562. *Chen, C. C., David, A., Thompson, K., Smith, C., Lea, S., & Fahy, T. (1996). Coping strategies and psychiatric morbidity in women attending breast assessment clinics. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 265– 270. Chen, S.-M. (1980). A study of sociometric status of Chinese school children (Chinese). Bulletin of Educational Psychology, 13, 119 –132. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. P. (1991). General affective dispositions in physical and psychological health. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective (pp. 221–245). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. P. (1999). Temperament: A new paradigm for trait psychology. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 399 – 423). New York: Guilford Press. Clark, L. A., Watson, D. P., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 103–116. Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In A. H. Hastorf & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 71–108). New York: Elsevier/North-Holland. *Clark, M. S., & Waddell, B. A. (1983). Effects of moods on thoughts about helping, attraction and information acquisition. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 31–35. Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Jr., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., & Isbell, L. M. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 27– 62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. *Cogan, R., Cogan, D., Waltz, W., & McCue, M. (1987). Effects of laughter and relaxation on discomfort thresholds. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 139 –144. Cohen, S. (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology, 7, 269 –297. *Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R. B., Alper, C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003). Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 652– 657. *Collins, J. E., Hanson, K., Mulhern, M., & Padberg, R. M. (1992). Sense of coherence over time in cancer patients: A preliminary report. Medical Psychotherapy, 5, 73– 82. Connolly, J. J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2000). The role of affectivity in job satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 265–281. 848 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER Cooper, H. (1998). Synthesizing research: A guide for literature reviews (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. *Cooper, H., Okamura, L., & Gurka, V. (1992). Social activity and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 573– 583. *Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668 – 678. *Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Norris, A. H. (1981). Personal adjustment to aging: Longitudinal prediction from neuroticism and extraversion. Journals of Gerontology, 36, 78 – 85. Coté, S. (1999). Affect and performance in organizational settings. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 65– 68. *Cowan, G., Neighbors, C., DeLaMoreaux, J., & Behnke, C. (1998). Women’s hostility toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 267–284. *Credé, M., Chernyshenko, O. S., Stark, S., & Dalal, R. S. (2005). The relationship of job satisfaction to personological and environmental antecedents and volitional workplace behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication. *Cropanzano, R., & Wright, T. A. (1999). A 5-year study of change in the relationship between well-being and job performance. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 51, 252–265. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow. New York: Basic Books. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy? American Psychologist, 54, 821– 827. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Patton, J. D., & Lucas, M. (1997). Le bonheur, l’experience optimale et les valeurs spirituelles: Une etude empirique aupres d’adolescents [Happiness, the optimal experience, and spiritual values: An empirical study of adolescents]. Revue Quebecoise de Psychologie, 18, 167–190. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Wong, M. M. (1991). The situational and personal correlates of happiness: A cross-national comparison. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 193–212). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. *Cunningham, M. R. (1988a). Does happiness mean friendliness? Induced mood and heterosexual self-disclosure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 283–297. *Cunningham, M. R. (1988b). What do you do when you’re happy or blue? Mood, expectancies, and behavioral interest. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 309 –331. *Cunningham, M. R., Shaffer, D. R., Barbee, A. P., Wolff, P. L., & Kelley, D. J. (1990). Separate processes in the relation of elation and depression to helping: Social versus personal concerns. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 13–33. *Cunningham, M. R., Steinberg, J., & Grev, R. (1980). Wanting to and having to help: Separate motivations for positive mood and guiltinduced helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 181–192. *Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804 – 813. *Deeg, D. J. H., & Van Zonneveld, R. J. (1989). Does happiness lengthen life? The prediction of longevity in the elderly. In R. Veenhoven (Ed.), How harmful is happiness? Consequences of enjoying life or not (pp. 29 –34). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Universitaire Pers Rotterdam. *DeLuga, R. J., & Mason, S. (2000). Relationship of resident assistant conscientiousness, extraversion, and positive affect with rated performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 225–235. DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A metaanalysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197–229. Denollet, J., & Brutsaert, D. L. (1998). Personality, disease severity, and the risk of long-term cardiac events in patients with a decreased ejection fraction after myocardial infarction. Circulation, 97, 167–173. *Devins, G. M., Mann, J., Mandin, H. P., & Leonard, C. (1990). Psychosocial predictors of survival in end-stage renal disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 127–133. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research, 57, 119 –169. Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., Pavot, W. G., & Allman, A. (1991). The psychic costs of intense positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 492–503. *Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926 –935. *Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 419 – 436. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1253–1265. Diener, E., Oishi, R., & Lucas, R. (2005). Optimum levels of happiness. Manuscript in preparation. *Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R. E., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229 – 259. Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Pavot, W. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive versus negative affect. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 119 –139). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press. *Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13, 81– 84. Diener, E., Smith, H. L., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130 –141. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276 –302. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Smith, H. L., & Shao, L. (1995). National differences in reported well-being: Why do they occur? Social Indicators Research, 34, 7–32. Diener, E., Wolsic, B., & Fujita, F. (1995). Physical attractiveness and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 120 –129. *Dillon, K. M., Minchoff, B., & Baker, K. H. (1985). Positive emotional states and enhancement of the immune system. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 15, 13–18. *Dillon, K. M., & Totten, M. C. (1989). Psychological factors, immunocompetence, and health of breast-feeding mothers and their infants. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 155–162. DiMatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., & Croghan, T. W. (2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2101–2107. Dobson, K., & Franche, R. L. (1989). A conceptual and empirical review of the depressive realism hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 419 – 433. Dobson, K. S., & Pusch, D. (1995). A test of the depressive realism hypothesis in clinically depressed subjects. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 179 –194. *Donovan, M. A. (2000). Cognitive, affective, and satisfaction variables as predictors of organizational behaviors: A structural equation modeling examination of alternative models. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60(9-B), 4943. (UMI No. AAI9944835) *Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Isen, A. M., & Lowrance, R. (1995). Group BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT representations and intergroup bias: Positive affect, similarity, and group size. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 856 – 865. Dunning, D., & Story, A. L. (1991). Depression, realism, and the overconfidence effect: Are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions and events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 521–532. Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. (1993). Depression and the impression formation continuum: Piecemeal processing despite the availability of category information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 636 – 645. Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter- and intranational differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 869 – 885. *Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach–avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804 – 818. *Elsbach, K., & Barr, P. (1999). The effects of mood on individuals’ use of structured decision protocols. Organization Science, 10, 181–198. *Epping-Jordan, J. E., Compas, B. E., Osowiecki, D. M., Oppedisano, G., Gerhardt, C., Primo, K., et al. (1999). Psychological adjustment in breast cancer: Processes of emotional distress. Health Psychology, 18, 315– 326. Erber, M. W., & Erber, R. (2001). The role of motivated social cognition in the regulation of affective states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 275–290). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Erber, R., & Erber, M. W. (2000). The self-regulation of moods: Second thoughts on the importance of happiness in everyday life. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 142–148. *Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 1055–1067. *Estrada, C., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect influences creative problem solving and reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299. *Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117–135. Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Addiction, personality and motivation. Human Psychopharmacology, 12, 79 – 87. *Feingold, A. (1983). Happiness, unselfishness, and popularity. Journal of Psychology, 115, 3–5. Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290 –309. *Fitzgerald, T. E., Prochaska, J. O., & Pransky, G. S. (2000). Health risk reduction and functional restoration following coronary revascularization: A prospective investigation using dynamic stage typology clustering. International Journal of Rehabilitation and Health, 5, 99 –116. Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. American Psychologist, 55, 647– 654. *Forgas, J. P. (1989). Mood effects on decision making strategies. Australian Journal of Psychology, 41, 197–214. *Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects on negotiator cognition and bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 565–577. *Foster, J. B., Hebl, M. R., West, M., & Dawson, J. (2004, April). Setting the tone for organizational success: The impact of CEO affect on organizational climate and firm-level outcomes. Paper presented at the 17th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Fraser, G. E., & Shavlik, D. J. (2001). 10 years of life: Is it a matter of choice? Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 1645–1652. 849 Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300 –319. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218 –226. *Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172– 175. *Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220. *Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C., & Tugade, M. M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 237–258. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises?: A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–376. *Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Schwartz, J. E., Wingard, & Criqui, M. H. (1993). Does childhood personality predict longevity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 176 –185. Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51– 87). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. *Frisch, M. B., Clark, M. P., Rouse, S. V., Rudd, M. D., Paweleck, J. K. Greenstone, A., et al. (2004). Predictive and treatment validity of life satisfaction and the Quality of Life Inventory. Assessment, 10, 1–13. *Fromm, E. (1962). The art of loving. New York: Harper & Row. *Futterman, A. D., Kemeny, M. E., Shapiro, D., & Fahey, J. L. (1994). Immunological and physiological changes associated with induced positive and negative mood. Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 499 –511. *Geier, A. B., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2003). “Before and after” diet advertisements escalate weight stigma. Eating and Weight Disorders, 8, 282–288. *George, J. M. (1989). Mood and absence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 317–324. *George, J. M. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299 –307. *George, J. M. (1995). Leader positive mood and group performance: The case of customer service. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 778 –795. George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1992). Feeling good—Doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work—Organizational spontaneity. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 310 –329. *Gil, K. M., Carson, J. W., Porter, L. S., Scipio, C., Bediako, S. M., & Orringer, E. (2004). Daily mood and stress predict pain, health care use, and work activity in African American adults with sickle-cell disease. Health Psychology, 23, 267–274. *Gladow, N. W., & Ray, M. P. (1986). The impact of informal support systems on the well-being of low income single parents. Family Relations: Journal of Applied Family and Child Studies, 35, 113–123. Gleicher, F., & Weary, G. (1991). Effect of depression on quantity and quality of social inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 105–114. Glenn, N. D., & Weaver, C. N. (1979). A note on family situation and global happiness. Social Forces, 57, 960 –967. *Glenn, N. D., & Weaver, C. N. (1981). The contributions of marital happiness to global happiness. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 161–168. *Goldman, S. L., Kraemer, D. T., & Salovey, P. (1996). Beliefs about mood moderate the relationship of stress to illness and symptom reporting. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41, 115–128. 850 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1999). What predicts change in marital interaction over time? A study of alternative medicine. Family Process, 38, 143–158. *Graef, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Gianinno, S. M. (1983). Measuring intrinsic motivation in everyday life. Leisure Studies, 2, 155–168. Graen, T. (1976). Role-making processes within complex organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of organizational and industrial psychology (pp. 1201–1245). Chicago: Rand McNally. *Graham, C., Eggers, A., & Sukhtankar, S. (in press). Does happiness pay? An exploration based on panel data from Russia. Journal of Economic Behaviour and Organization. Gray, J. A. (1994). Personality dimensions and emotion systems. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 329 –331). New York: Oxford University Press. *Griffin, P. W., Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (in press). Variability in affective change among aging men: Findings from the VA normative aging study. Journal of Research in Personality. *Griffitt, W. B. (1970). Environmental effects of interpersonal affective behavior: Ambient effective temperature and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 240 –244. Grob, A., Stetsenko, A., Sabatier, C., Botcheva, L., & Macek, P. (1999). A cross-national model of subjective well-being in adolescence. In F. D. Alsaker, A. Flammer, & N. Bodmer (Eds.), The adolescent experience: European and American adolescents in the 1990s (pp. 115–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. *Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotions in women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 112–124. Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: Ten-week-old infants’ responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104. Headey, B., & Veenhoven, R. (1989). Does happiness induce a rosy outlook? In R. Veenhoven (Ed.), How harmful is happiness? Consequences of enjoying life or not (pp. 106 –127). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Universitaire Pers Rotterdam. *Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24, 81–100. *Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739. Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., et al. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599 – 615. *Hektner, J. M. (1997). Exploring optimal personality development: A longitudinal study of adolescents. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57(11B), 7249. (UMI No. AAM9711187) *Hirt, E. R., Melton, R. J., McDonald, H. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Processing goals, task interest, and the mood–performance relationship: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 245–261. *Hom, H., & Arbuckle, B. (1988). Mood induction effects upon goal setting and performance in young children. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 113–122. House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241, 540 –545. *Howell, C. J., Howell, R. T., & Schwabe, K. A. (in press). Does wealth enhance life satisfaction for people who are materially deprived? Exploring the association among the Orang Asli of Peninsular Malaysia. Social Indicators Research. *Irving, L. M., Snyder, C. R., & Crowson, J. J. (1998). Hope and coping with cancer by college women. Journal of Personality, 66, 195–214. *Isen, A. M. (1970). Success, failure, attention and reaction to others: The warm glow of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 294 –301. Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (1st ed., pp. 261– 277). New York: Guilford Press. Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 521–539). Chichester, England: Wiley. Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 417– 435). New York: Guilford Press. *Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206 – 1217. Isen, A. M., & Geva, N. (1987). The influence of positive affect on acceptable level of risk: The person with a large canoe has a large worry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 145–154. *Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426. *Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 384 –388. *Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. Social Cognition, 2, 18 –31. Isen, A. M., & Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 194 –202. Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999). The psychophysiology of utility appraisals. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Wellbeing: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 470 – 488). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New York: Basic Books. Jamison, K. R. (1990). Manic-depressive illness and accomplishment: Creativity, leadership, and social class. In F. K. Goodwin & K. R. Jamison (Eds.), Manic-depressive illness (pp. 332–367). New York: Oxford University Press. *Judge, T. A., & Higgins, C. A. (1998). Affective disposition and the letter of reference. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75, 207–221. *Jundt, D., & Hinsz, V. B. (2001, May). Are happier workers more productive workers? The impact of mood on self-set goals, self-efficacy, and task performance. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. *Kahana, E., Redmond, C., Hill, G. J., Kahana, B., Johnson, J. R., & Young, R. F. (1995). The effects of stress, vulnerability, and appraisals on the psychological well-being of the elderly. Research on Aging, 17, 459 – 489. *Kahn, B. E., & Isen, A. M. (1993). The influence of positive affect on variety-seeking among safe, enjoyable products. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 257–270. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263–291. Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, methods, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34. *Kashdan, T. B., & Roberts, J. E. (2004). Trait and state curiosity in the genesis of intimacy: Differentiation from related constructs. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 792– 816. *Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P., & Fincham, F. D. (2004). Curiosity and BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT exploration: Facilitating positive subjective experiences and personal growth opportunities. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82, 291–305. *Kavanagh, D. J. (1987). Mood, persistence, and success. Australian Journal of Psychology, 39, 307–318. *Kehn, D. J. (1995). Predictors of elderly happiness. Activities, Adaptation, and Aging, 19, 11–30. *Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687–702. Keltner, D., & Kring, A. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2, 320 –342. Kidd, R. F., & Marshall, L. (1982). Self-reflection, mood, and helpful behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 16, 319 –334. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., & Glaser, R. (2002). Emotions, morbidity, and mortality: New perspectives from psychoneuroimmunology. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 83–107. King, L. A., & Broyles, S. J. (1997). Wishes, gender, personality, and well-being. Journal of Personality, 65, 49 –76. *King, L. A., & Napa, C. N. (1998). What makes a life good? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 156 –165. *Kirkcaldy, B., & Furnham, A. (2000). Positive affectivity, psychological well-being, accident- and traffic-deaths and suicide: An international comparison. Studia Psychologica, 42, 97–104. *Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Honkanen, R., Koskenvuo, M., Viinamaeki, H., & Kaprio, J. (2002). Life satisfaction as a predictor of fatal injury in a 20-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 444 – 450. *Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Honkanen, R., Viinamaeki, H., Heikkilae, K., Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (2001). Life satisfaction and suicide: A 20-year follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 433– 439. *Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Koskenvuo, M., Honkanen, R. J., Viinamaki, H., Heikkilae, K., & Kaprio, J. (2004). Life dissatisfaction and subsequent work disability in an 11-year follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 34, 221–228. Kozma, A., & Stones, M. J. (1978). Some research issues and findings in the assessment of well-being in the elderly. Canadian Psychological Review, 19, 241–249. *Kozma, A., & Stones, M. J. (1983). Predictors of happiness. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 626 – 628. *Krause, J. S., Sternberg, M., Lottes, S., & Maides, J. (1997). Mortality after spinal cord injury: An 11-year prospective study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 78, 815– 821. *Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and antisocial behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality correlates, distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 397– 402. Kubzansky, L. D., & Kawachi, I. (2000). Going to the heart of the matter: Do negative emotions cause coronary heart disease? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48, 323–337. *Kubzansky, L. D., Sparrow, D., Vokonas, P., & Kawachi, I. (2001). Is the glass half empty or half full? A prospective study of optimism and coronary heart disease in the normative aging study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 910 –916. *Laidlaw, T. M., Booth, R. J., & Large, R. G. (1996). Reduction in skin reactions to histamine after a hypnotic procedure. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 242–248. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140. Lebo, D. (1953). Some factors said to make for happiness in old age. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 9, 385–387. *Lee, G. R., & Ishii-Kuntz, M. (1987). Social interaction, loneliness, and emotional well-being among the elderly. Research on Aging, 9, 459 – 482. Lee, G. R., Seccombe, K., & Shehan, C. L. (1991). Marital status and 851 personal happiness: An analysis of trend data. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 839 – 844. *Lefcourt, H. M., Davidson-Katz, K., & Kuenemen, K. (1990). Humor and immune system functioning. Humor, 3, 305–321. Lehr, U. M. (1982). Depression und Lebensqualität im Alter–Korrelate negativer und positiver Gestimmtheit [Depression or “quality of life” in old age: Correlates of negative and positive morale-findings from the Bonn Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA)]. Zeitschrift für Gerontologie, 15, 241–249. Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk-taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1250 –1267. *Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Kunkel, S. R., & Kasl, S. V. (2002). Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 261–270. *Levy, S. M., Lee, J., Bagley, C., & Lippman, M. (1988). Survival hazard analysis in first recurrent breast cancer patients: Seven-year follow-up. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50, 520 –528. *Lobel, M., DeVincent, C. J., Kaminer, A., & Meyer, B. A. (2000). The impact of prenatal maternal stress and optimistic disposition on birth outcomes in medically high-risk women. Health Psychology, 19, 544 – 553. Locke, E. A. (1975). Personnel attitudes and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 26, 457– 480. *Lox, C. L., Burns, S. P., Treasure, D. C., & Wasley, D. A. (1999). Physical and psychological predictors of exercise dosage in healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 31, 1060 –1064. *Lu, L., & Argyle, M. (1991). Happiness and cooperation. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1019 –1030. *Lu, L., & Shih, J. B. (1997). Personality and happiness: Is mental health a mediator? Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 249 –256. *Lucas, R. E. (2001). Pleasant affect and sociability: Towards a comprehensive model of extraverted feelings and behaviors. Dissertation Abstracts International, 61 (10-B), 5610. (UMI No. AAI9990068) *Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539. *Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 8 –13. Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2003). The happy worker: Hypotheses about the role of positive affect in worker productivity. In M. Burrick & A. M. Ryan (Eds.), Personality and work (pp. 30 –59). San Francisco: JosseyBass. *Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 452– 468. *Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616 – 628. *Lutgendorf, S. K., Vitaliano, P. P., Tripp-Reimer, T., Harvey, J. H., & Lubaroff, D. M. (1999). Sense of coherence moderates the relationship between life stress and natural killer cell activity in health older adults. Psychology and Aging, 14, 552–563. *Lyons, A., & Chamberlain, K. (1994). The effects of minor events, optimism and self-esteem on health. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 559 –570. Lyubomirsky, S. (1997, May). The meaning and expression of happiness: Comparing the United States and Russia. Paper presented at the ninth conference of the American Psychological Society, Washington, DC. Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others?: The role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American Psychologist, 56, 239 –249. 852 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K., & Dickerhoof, R. (2005). The cognitive and hedonic costs of excessive self-reflection. Manuscript submitted for publication. Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1997). Hedonic consequences of social comparison: A contrast of happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1141–1157. Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131. *Lyubomirsky, S., Tkach, C., DiMatteo, M. R., & Lepper, H. S. (in press). What are the differences between happiness and self-esteem? Social Indicators Research. *Lyubomirsky, S., & Tucker, K. L. (1998). Implications of individual differences in subjective happiness for perceiving, interpreting, and thinking about life events. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 155–186. *Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. (1989). Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27– 40. Maddi, S. R., & Kobasa, S. C. (1991). The development of hardiness. In A. Monat & R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (3rd ed., pp. 245–257). New York: Columbia University Press. *Magen, Z., & Aharoni, R. (1991). Adolescents’ contributing toward others: Relationship to positive experiences and transpersonal commitment. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 31, 126 –143. *Maier, H., & Smith, J. (1999). Psychological predictors of mortality in old age. Journal of Gerontology, 54B, 44 –54. Manucia, G. K., Baumann, D. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (1984). Mood influences on helping: Direct effects or side effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 357–364. *Marks, G. N., & Fleming, N. (1999). Influences and consequences of well-being among Australian young people: 1980 –1995. Social Indicators Research, 46, 301–323. Martin, R. A. (2002). Is laughter the best medicine? Humor, laughter, and physical health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 216 – 220. Maruta, T., Colligan, R. C., Malinchoc, M., & Offord, K. P. (2000). Optimists vs. pessimists: Survival rate among medical patients over a 30-year period. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 75, 140 –143. *Mastekaasa, A. (1994). Marital status, distress, and well-being: An international comparison. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 183– 205. *Mathes, E. W., & Kahn, A. (1975). Physical attractiveness, happiness, neuroticism, and self-esteem. Journal of Psychology, 90, 27–30. *Matikka, L. M., & Ojanen, M. (in press). Happiness in persons with intellectual disabilities. European Journal of Mental Disability. *Mayer, J. D., Mamberg, M. H., & Volanth, A. J. (1988). Cognitive domains of the mood system. Journal of Personality, 56, 453– 486. *McClelland, D. C., & Cheriff, A. D. (1997). The immunoenhancing effects of humor on secretory IgA and resistance to respiratory infection. Psychology and Health, 12, 329 –344. *McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of Personality, 54, 385– 405. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1991). Adding Liebe un Arabeit: The fully five-factor model and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227–232. McGuire, L., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (2002). Depressive symptoms and lymphocyte proliferation in older adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 192–197. *McMillen, D. L., Sanders, D. Y., & Solomon, G. S. (1977). Self-esteem, attentiveness, and helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 257–261. *Melton, R. J. (1995). The role of positive affect in syllogism performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 788 –794. Meng, X., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 172–175. *Miles, D. E., Borman, W. E., Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2002). Building an integrative model of extra role work behaviors: A comparison of counterproductive work behavior with organizational citizenship behavior. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 10, 51–57. Miller, S. M., & Schnoll, R. A. (2000). When seeing is feeling: A cognitive– emotional approach to coping with health stress. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 538 –557). New York: Guilford Press. *Mishra, S. (1992). Leisure activities and life satisfaction in old age: A case study of retired government employees living in urban areas. Activities, Adaptation and Aging, 16, 7–26. *Mongrain, M., & Zuroff, D. C. (1995). Motivational and affective correlates of dependency and self-criticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 347–354. *Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2005). Change in life satisfaction during adulthood: Findings from the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 189 –202. *Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. (1990). The influence of mood on categorization: A cognitive flexibility interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 411– 425. Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness: Who is happy and why. New York: William Morrow. Myers, D. G. (1999). Close relationships and quality of life. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 374 –391). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55, 56 – 67. Naughton, M. J., Herndon, J. E., II, Shumaker, S. A., Miller, A. A., Kornblith, A. B., Chao, D., et al. (2002). The health-related quality of life and survival of small-cell lung cancer patients: Results of a companion study to CALGB 9033. Quality of Life Research, 11, 235–248. *Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality–relationship transaction in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1190 –1204. *O’Malley, M. N., & Andrews, L. (1983). The effect of mood and incentives on helping: Are there some things money can’t buy? Motivation and Emotion, 7, 179 –189. Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2003). Culture and well-being: The cycle of action, evaluation, and decision. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 939 –949. Okun, M. A., Stock, W. A., Haring, M. J., & Witter, R. A. (1984). The social activity/subjective well-being relation: A quantitative synthesis. Research on Aging, 6, 45– 65. Organ, D. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. *Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Black, S. A., & Goodwin, J. S. (2000). Emotional well-being predicts subsequent functional independence and survival. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48, 473– 478. *Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Peek, M. K., & Goodwin, J. S. (2001). The association between emotional well-being and the incidence of stroke in older adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 210 –215. Pacini, R., Muir, F., & Epstein, S. (1998). Depressive realism from the perspective of cognitive– experiential self-theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1056 –1068. *Palmore, E. B. (1969). Predicting longevity: A follow-up controlling for age. Gerontologist, 9, 247–250. *Pelled, L. H., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Down and out: An investigation of the relationship between mood and employee withdrawal behavior. Journal of Management, 25, 875– 895. BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539 – 548. Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (1996). Cognitive, emotional, and language processes in disclosure. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 601– 626. Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 863– 871. Peplau, L. A., & Perlman, D. (Eds.). (1982). Loneliness. New York: Wiley. *Perry, L. C., Perry, D. G., & Weiss, R. J. (1986). Age differences in children’s beliefs about whether altruism makes the actor feel good. Social Cognition, 4, 263–269. *Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko, K. H., Martin, L. R., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Catastrophizing and untimely death. Psychological Science, 9, 127–130. *Pettit, J. W., Kline, J. P., Gencoz, T., Gencoz, F., & Joiner, T. E. (2001). Are happy people healthier: The specific role of positive affect in predicting self-reported health symptoms. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 521–536. *Pfeiffer, S. M., & Wong, P. T. (1989). Multidimensional jealousy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 181–196. *Phillips, D. L. (1967). Mental health status, social participation, and happiness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 8, 285–291. Piko, B. F., Gibbons, F. X., Luszcynska, A., & Teközel, M. (2002). Does culture matter? Cross-cultural comparison of smoking patterns among adolescents. In I. Leal, T. Botelho, & J. P. Ribeiro (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th Conference of the Health Psychology Society (pp. 9 –12). Lisbon, Portugal: Institute for the Application of Psychology. Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 15, 187–224. *Pitkala, K. H., Laatkonen, M. L., Strandberg, T. E., & Tilvis, R. S. (2004). Positive life orientation as a predictor of 10-year outcome in an aged population. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 57, 409 – 414. Pomerantz, E. M., Saxon, J. L., & Oishi, S. (2000). The psychological trade-offs of goal investment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 617– 630. Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1973). Organizational, work, and personal factors in employee turnover and absenteeism. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 151–176. Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971. Pritzker, M. A. (2002). The relationship among CEO dispositional attributes, transformational leadership behaviors and performance effectiveness. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62(12-B), 6008. (UMI No. AAI3035464) *Reed, M. B., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 99 –132. *Requena, F. (1995). Friendship and subjective well-being in Spain: A cross-national comparison with the United States. Social Indicators Research, 35, 271–288. *Reynolds, D. K., & Nelson, F. L. (1981). Personality, life situation and life expectancy. Suicide and Life Threatening Behavior, 11, 99 –110. Richards, R. (1994). Creativity and bipolar mood swings: Why the association? In M. P. Shaw & M. A. Runco (Eds.), Creativity and affect (pp. 44 –72). Stamford, CT: Ablex. *Richards, R., & Kinney, D. K. (1990). Mood swings and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 202–217. *Riddick, C. C. (1985). Life satisfaction determinants of older males and females. Leisure Sciences, 7, 47– 63. *Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relation among Australian children and implications for psychological wellbeing. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 33– 42. 853 *Rimland, B. (1982). The altruism paradox. Psychological Reports, 51, 521–522. *Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 582–593. *Rosenhan, D. L., Salovey, P., & Hargis, K. (1981). The joys of helping: Focus of attention mediates the impact of positive affect on altruism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 899 –905. *Rosenhan, D. L., Underwood, B., & Moore, B. (1974). Affect moderates self-gratification and altruism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 546 –552. Rosenstock, I. M., & Kirscht, J. P. (1979). Why people seek healthcare. In G. C. Stone, N. G. Cohen, & N. E. Adler (Eds.), Health psychology—A handbook (pp. 161–188). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (2003). r-sub(equivalent): A simple effect size indicator. Psychological Methods, 8, 492– 496. *Røysamb, E., Tambs, K., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Neale, M. C., & Harris, J. R. (2003). Happiness and health: Environmental and genetic contributions to the relationship between subjective well-being, perceived health, and somatic illness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1136 –1146. Ruch, W. (1993). Exhilaration and humor. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 605– 616). New York: Guilford Press. Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1997). Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: A test of two theoretical models. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 607– 612. *Ruvolo, A. P. (1998). Marital well-being and general happiness of newlywed couples: Relationships across time. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 470 – 489. *Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069 –1081. Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28. Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1991). Mood and helping: Mood as a motivator of helping and helping as a regulator of mood. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Prosocial behavior (pp. 215–237). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Salovey, P., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1989). Mood states and prosocial behavior. In H. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social psychophysiology (pp. 371–391). Chichester, England: Wiley. Salovey, P., Rothman, A. J., Detweiler, J. B., & Steward, W. T. (2000). Emotional states and physical health. American Psychologist, 55, 110 –121. *Samson, D., & Rachman, S. (1989). The effect of induced mood on fear reduction. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 227–238. Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Subjective well-being: The convergence and stability of self-report and non-self-report measures. Journal of Personality, 61, 317–342. *Sarason, I. G., Potter, E. H., & Sarason, B. R. (1986). Recording and recall of personal events: Effects on cognitions and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 347–356. Schaller, M., & Cialdini, R. B. (1990). Happiness, sadness, and helping: A motivational integration. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 265–296). New York: Guilford Press. *Scheier, M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens, J. F., Magovern, G. J., Lefebvre, R. C., Abbott, R., A., et al. (1989). Dispositional optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1024 –1040. *Scheufele, D. A., & Shah, D. V. (2000). Personality strength and social 854 LYUBOMIRSKY, KING, AND DIENER capital: The role of dispositional and informational variables in the production of civic participation. Communication Research, 27, 107–131. *Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Personality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1062–1075. Schroeder, D. A., Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., & Piliavin, J. A. (1995). The psychology of helping and altruism: Problems and puzzles. New York: McGraw-Hill. *Schuettler, D., & Kiviniemi, M. T. (in press). Does how I feel about it matter?: The role of affect in cognitive and behavioral reactions to a chronic illness diagnosis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. *Schuldberg, D. (1990). Schizotypal and hypomanic traits, creativity, and psychological health. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 218 –230. *Schwartz, B., Ward, A. H., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1178 –1197. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Cognitive foundations of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press. Scollon, C. N., & King, L. A. (2004). Is the good life the easy life? Social Indicators Research, 68, 127–162. Segerstrom, S. C. (2000). Personality and the immune system: Models, methods, & mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 180 –190. Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: A. A. Knopf. *Seligman, M. E. P., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 832– 838. *Shapiro, P. J., & Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and bipolar diathesis: Common behavioral and cognitive components. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 741–762. *Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (in press). Achieving sustainable increases in happiness: Change your actions, not your circumstances. Journal of Happiness Studies. Shephard, R. J. (1993). Exercise in the prevention and treatment of cancer: An update. Sports Medicine, 15, 258 –280. Shrauger, J. S., Mariano, E., & Walter, T. J. (1998). Depressive symptoms and accuracy in the prediction of future events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 880 – 892. *Sinclair, R. C., & Mark, M. M. (1995). The effects of mood state on judgmental accuracy: Processing strategy as a mechanism. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 417– 438. *Smith, A. M., Stuart, M. J., Wiese-Bjornstal, D. M., & Gunnon, C. (1997). Predictors of injury in ice hockey players: A multivariate, multidisciplinary approach. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 500 –507. *Smith, T. W., Ruiz, J. M., & Uchino, B. N. (2004). Mental activation of supportive ties, hostility, and cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory stress in young men and women. Health Psychology, 23, 476 – 485. Snyder, C. R. (2000). The past and possible futures of hope. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 11–28. Solomon, G. F., Segerstrom, S. C., Grohr, P., Kemeny, M., & Fahey, J. (1997). Shaking up immunity: Psychological and immunologic changes after a natural disaster. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 114 –127. *Spanier, G. B., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1982). Remarriage after divorce: A longitudinal analysis of well-being. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 44, 709 –720. *Stack, S., & Eshleman, J. R. (1998). Marital status and happiness: A 17-nation study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 527–536. *Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypothesis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 304 –331. *Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., & Pelled, L. H. (1994). Employee positive emotion and favorable outcomes at the workplace. Organization Science, 5, 51–71. *Stone, A. A., Cox, D. S., Valdimarsdottir, H., Jandorf, L., & Neale, J. M. (1987). Evidence that secretory IgA antibody is associated with daily mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 988 –993. *Stone, A. A., Neale, J. M., Cox, D. S., Napoli, A., Valdimarsdottir, V., & Kennedy-Moore, E. (1994). Daily events are associated with a secretory immune response to an oral antigen in men. Health Psychology, 13, 440 – 446. *Stones, M. J., & Kozma, A. (1986). “Happy are they who are happy . . .” A test between two causal models of relationships between happiness and its correlates. Experimental Aging Research, 12, 23–29. *Strayer, J. (1980). A naturalistic study of empathic behaviors and their relation to affective states and perspective-taking skills in preschool children. Child Development, 51, 815– 822. Suh, E. M. (2000). Self, the hyphen between culture and subjective well-being. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 63– 86). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. *Sullivan, D., LaCroix, A. Z., Russo, J. E., & Walker, E. A. (2001). Depression and self-reported physical health in patients with coronary disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 248 –256. Tait, M., Padgett, M. Y., & Baldwin, T. (1989). Job and life satisfaction: A reevaluation of the strength of the relationship and gender effects as a function of the date of the study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 502–507. *Tarlow, E. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (1996). Negative self-concept: Specificity to depressive symptoms and relation to positive and negative affectivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 120 –127. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210. *Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Portrait of the self-enhancer: Well-adjusted and well-liked or maladjusted and friendless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 165–176. *Thoits, P. A., & Hewitt, L. N. (2001). Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 115–131. Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A. P., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 914 –945. Tice, D. M., & Wallace, H. (2000). Mood and emotion control: Some thoughts on the state of the field. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 214 –217. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, and consciousness: Vol. I. The positive affects. New York: Springer. *Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective performance in professional sports teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 848 – 859. *Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among selfevaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resources for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 53–72. Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2004). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Manuscript submitted for publication. *Urada, D., & Miller, N. (2000). The impact of positive mood and category importance on crossed categorization effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 417– 433. *Valdimarsdottir, H. B., & Bovbjerg, D. H. (1997). Positive and negative mood: Association with natural killer cell activity. Psychology and Health, 12, 319 –327. *Van Katwyk, P. T., Fox, S., Spector, P. E., & Kelloway, E. K. (2000). Using the Job-Related Affective Well-being Scale (JAWS) to investigate affective responses to work stressors. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 52, 219 –230. Veenhoven, R. (1984). Conditions of happiness. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel. BENEFITS OF FREQUENT POSITIVE AFFECT Veenhoven, R. (1988). The utility of happiness. Social Indicators Research, 20, 333–354. Veenhoven, R. (1994). World database of happiness: Correlates of happiness: 7837 findings from 603 studies in 69 nations 1911–1994 (Vols. 1–3). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Erasmus University. Veenhoven, R. (1997). Progrès dans la compréhension du bonheur [Progress in understanding happiness]. Revue Quebecoise de Psychologie, 18, 29 –74. Verkley, H., & Stolk, J. (1989). Does happiness lead into idleness? In R. Veenhoven (Ed.), How harmful is happiness? (pp. 79 –93). Rotterdam, Amsterdam: University of Rotterdam. *Vitaliano, P. P., Scanlan, J. M., Ochs, H. D., Syrjala, K., Siegler, I. C., & Snyder, E. A. (1998). Psychosocial stress moderates the relationship of cancer history with natural killer cell activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 199 –208. *Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1020 –1030. *Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1995). Depression and the melancholic temperament. European Journal of Personality, 9, 351–366. *Watson, D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre, C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and social activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 1011–1025. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234 –251. *Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). Feeling good and feeling close: The effect of positive emotion on self– other overlap. Manuscript submitted for publication. *Weinglert, L., & Rosen, A.-S. (1995). Optimism, self-esteem, mood, and subjective health. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 653– 661. *Weiss, H. M., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. S. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in affective experiences over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 1–24. 855 *Willi, J. (1997). The significance of romantic love for marriage. Family Process, 36, 171–182. *Williams, S., & Shiaw, W. T. (1999). Mood and organizational citizenship behavior: The effects of positive affect on employee organizational citizenship behavior intentions. Journal of Psychology, 133, 656 – 668. Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., Shinar, O., & Yaeger, A. (1999). Contributions of positive and negative affect to adolescent substance use: Test of a bidimensional model in a longitudinal study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 327–338. Wilson, W. (1967). Correlates of avowed happiness. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 294 –306. *Windle, M. (2000). A latent growth curve model of delinquent activity among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 4, 193–207. World Value Survey Group. (1994). World Values Survey, 1981–1984 and 1990 –1993. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. *Wright, J., & Mischel, W. (1982). Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 901–914. *Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Emotional exhaustion as a predictor of job performance and voluntary turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 486 – 493. *Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as predictors of job performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 84 –94. *Wright, T. A., & Staw, B. M. (1999). Affect and favorable work outcomes: Two longitudinal tests of the happy—Productive worker thesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 1–23. *Zinser, M. C., Baker, T. B., Sherman, J. E., & Cannon, D. S. (1992). Relation between self-reported affect and drug urges and cravings in continuing and withdrawing smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 617– 629. *Zuckerman, D. M., Kasl, S. V., & Ostfeld, A. M. (1984). Psychosocial predictors of mortality among the elderly poor. The role of religion, well-being, and social contacts. American Journal of Epidemiology, 119, 410 – 423. Received March 5, 2004 Revision received March 11, 2005 Accepted April 12, 2005 䡲 New Editor Appointed, 2007–2012 The Publications and Communications (P&C) Board of the American Psychological Association announces the appointment of a new editor for a 6-year term beginning in 2007. As of January 1, 2006, manuscripts should be directed as follows: • Emotion (www.apa.org/journals/emo.html), Elizabeth A. Phelps, PhD, Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, Room 863, New York, NY 10003. Electronic manuscript submission. As of January 1, 2006, manuscripts should be submitted electronically via the journal’s Manuscript Submission Portal (see the Web site listed above). Authors who are unable to do so should correspond with the editor’s office about alternatives. Manuscript submission patterns make the precise date of completion of the 2006 volumes uncertain. The current editors, Richard J. Davidson, PhD, and Klaus R. Scherer, PhD, will receive and consider manuscripts through December 31, 2005. Should 2006 volumes be completed before that date, manuscripts will be redirected to the new editor for consideration in 2007 volume. Review of General Psychology 2005, Vol. 9, No. 2, 111–131 Copyright 2005 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 1089-2680/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111 Pursuing Happiness: The Architecture of Sustainable Change Sonja Lyubomirsky Kennon M. Sheldon University of California, Riverside University of Missouri—Columbia David Schkade University of California, San Diego The pursuit of happiness is an important goal for many people. However, surprisingly little scientific research has focused on the question of how happiness can be increased and then sustained, probably because of pessimism engendered by the concepts of genetic determinism and hedonic adaptation. Nevertheless, emerging sources of optimism exist regarding the possibility of permanent increases in happiness. Drawing on the past well-being literature, the authors propose that a person’s chronic happiness level is governed by 3 major factors: a genetically determined set point for happiness, happiness-relevant circumstantial factors, and happiness-relevant activities and practices. The authors then consider adaptation and dynamic processes to show why the activity category offers the best opportunities for sustainably increasing happiness. Finally, existing research is discussed in support of the model, including 2 preliminary happiness-increasing interventions. The pursuit of happiness holds an honored position in American society, beginning with the Declaration of Independence, where it is promised as a cherished right for all citizens. Today, the enduring U.S. obsession with how to be happy can be observed in the row upon row of popular psychology and self-help books in any major bookstore and in the millions of copies of these books that are sold. Indeed, many social contexts in the United States have the production of happiness and positive feelings as their primary purpose, and questions Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside; Kennon M. Sheldon, Department of Psychology, University of Missouri—Columbia; David Schkade, Rady School of Management, University of California, San Diego. This work was supported in part by grants from the Positive Psychology Network. We are grateful to Linda Houser-Marko, Kathleen Jamir, and Chris Tkach for conducting library research and to Shelley Taylor, David Sherman, and the other members of Psychology 421 for valuable comments on a draft. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521, or Kennon M. Sheldon, Department of Psychological Sciences, 112 McAlester Hall, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211. E-mail: sonja@citrus.ucr.edu or sheldonk@missouri .edu such as “Are you happy?” and “Are you having fun?” fit nearly every occasion (Markus & Kitayama, 1994). Not surprisingly, the majority of U.S. residents rate personal happiness as very important (Diener, Suh, Smith, & Shao, 1995; Triandis, Bontempo, Leung, & Hui, 1990) and report thinking about happiness at least once every day (Freedman, 1978). Furthermore, the pursuit of happiness is no longer just a North American obsession, but instead it is becoming ever more global as people seek to fulfill the promises of capitalism and political freedom (Diener et al., 1995; Freedman, 1978; Triandis et al., 1990). It seems that nearly all people believe, or would like to believe, that they can move in an “upward spiral” (Sheldon & HouserMarko, 2001) toward ever greater personal well-being. Is the pursuit of happiness merely a bourgeois concern, a symptom of Western comfort and self-centeredness, a factor that has no real impact on psychological adjustment and adaptation? The empirical evidence suggests that this is not the case. Indeed, a number of researchers and thinkers have argued that the ability to be happy and contented with life is a central criterion of adaptation and positive mental health (e.g., Diener, 1984; Jahoda, 1958; Taylor & Brown, 1988). Bolstering this notion, Lyubomirsky and her colleagues recently com- 111 112 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE piled evidence showing that happiness has numerous positive byproducts that appear to benefit individuals, families, and communities (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2004; see also Fredrickson, 2001). Furthermore, Lyubomirsky et al.’s analysis revealed that happy people gain tangible benefits in many different life domains from their positive state of mind, including larger social rewards (higher odds of marriage and lower odds of divorce, more friends, stronger social support, and richer social interactions; e.g., Harker & Keltner, 2001; Marks & Fleming, 1999; Okun, Stock, Haring, & Witter, 1984), superior work outcomes (greater creativity, increased productivity, higher quality of work, and higher income; e.g., Estrada, Isen, & Young, 1994; Staw, Sutton, & Pelled, 1995), and more activity, energy, and flow (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi & Wong, 1991). Further supporting the argument that subjective happiness may be integral to mental and physical health, happy people are more likely to evidence greater self-control and self-regulatory and coping abilities (e.g., Aspinwall, 1998; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Keltner & Bonanno, 1997), to have a bolstered immune system (e.g., Dillon, Minchoff, & Baker, 1985; Stone et al., 1994), and even to live a longer life (e.g., Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001; Ostir, Markides, Black, & Goodwin, 2000). Also, happy people are not just self-centered or selfish; the literature suggests that happy individuals instead tend to be relatively more cooperative, prosocial, charitable, and “other-centered” (e.g., Isen, 1970; Kasser & Ryan, 1996; Williams & Shiaw, 1999). In summary, happy individuals appear more likely to be flourishing people, both inwardly and outwardly. Thus, we argue that enhancing people’s happiness levels may indeed be a worthy scientific goal, especially after their basic physical and security needs are met. Unfortunately, however, relatively little scientific support exists for the idea that people’s happiness levels can change for the better. For example, the happiness-boosting techniques proposed in the self-help literature generally have limited grounding in scientific theory and even less empirical confirmation of their effectiveness (Norcross et al., 2000). Consider a representative best seller, You Can Be Happy No Matter What: Five Principles for Keeping Life in Perspective, by Carlson (1997). Do the five princi- ples work? Do some work better than others? Do the principles work better for some people than for others? Are any positive effects of the principles due, ultimately, to placebo effects? If the book actually helps people “get happier,” does the happiness boost last? Although it is possible that some of the advice given in this and other similar books could well be appropriate and effective, the authors provide almost no empirical research in support of their claims. One receives little more guidance from contemporary academic psychology. Of course, research psychologists have identified many predictors of people’s happiness or subjective wellbeing. For example, well-being has been shown to be associated with a wide variety of factors, including demographic status (e.g., Argyle, 1999; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Myers, 2000), personality traits and attitudes (e.g., Diener & Lucas, 1999), and goal characteristics (e.g., McGregor & Little, 1998). However, a limitation of previous research is that the vast majority of studies have been cross sectional and have reported between-subjects effects rather than investigating well-being longitudinally and examining within-subject effects. In addition, very few happiness intervention studies have been conducted. Thus, researchers still know surprisingly little about how to change well-being, that is, about the possibility of “becoming happier.” Doubtless, part of the reason for this neglect is the difficulty of conducting longitudinal and intervention studies. The problem is further compounded by the tendency of applied mental health researchers to focus on pathology rather than on positive mental health (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and by the thorny issues raised when theorists speculate on how people “should” live their lives to maximize their potential for happiness (Schwartz, 2000). However, we believe the principal reason for the neglect of this question is the considerable scientific pessimism over whether it is even possible to effect sustainable increases in happiness. Historical Sources of Pessimism Three considerations serve to illustrate the depth of this pessimism. First is the idea of a genetically determined set point (or set range) for happiness. Lykken and Tellegen (1996) have provided evidence, based on twin studies SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS and adoption studies, that the heritability of well-being may be as high as 80% (although a more widely accepted figure is 50%; Braungart, Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1992; Tellegen et al., 1988; cf. Diener et al., 1999). Whatever the exact coefficient, its large magnitude suggests that for each person there is indeed a chronic or characteristic level of happiness. Consistent with this idea, Headey and Wearing (1989) found, in a four-wave panel study, that participants tended to keep returning to their own baselines over time (see also Suh, Diener, & Fujita, 1996). Thus, although there may be substantial variation around this baseline level in the short term, in the long term people perhaps cannot help but return to their set point, or to the middle of their set range: “What goes up must come down” (a more detailed description of the happiness set point is provided later). A second and closely related source of pessimism comes from the literature on personality traits. Traits are cognitive, affective, and behavioral complexes that are, by definition, consistent across situations and across the life span and therefore may account for part of the stability of the set point. In support of the latter assumption, McCrae and Costa (1990) have shown impressive long-term stability for the “Big Five” traits, including the two traits most closely related to well-being: neuroticism and extraversion. Specifically, people tend to maintain the same rank ordering in their levels of worry, rumination, and guilt, as well as in their levels of social engagement, enthusiasm, and self-confidence. Because of the close relation between psychological well-being and these personality characteristics, McCrae and Costa argued that people also tend to maintain the same relative level of happiness over time (see also Costa, McCrae, & Zonderman, 1987; Diener & Lucas, 1999). A third source of pessimism arises from the concept of the hedonic treadmill (Brickman & Campbell, 1971), which suggests that any gains in happiness are only temporary, because humans so quickly adapt to change (see also Kahneman, 1999; Tversky & Griffin, 1991). Thus, although new circumstances may temporarily cause people to become happier or sadder, they rapidly adjust, and the effect of these new circumstances on happiness then diminishes quickly or even disappears entirely. For example, Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman 113 (1978) showed that, after 1 year, lottery winners were no happier than controls, and furthermore recent paralysis victims were not as unhappy as one would expect. Further evidence of hedonic adaptation comes from findings of remarkably small correlations between happiness and wealth (Diener & Lucas, 1999) and Myers’s (2000) observation that while U.S. citizens’ personal income has more than doubled in the past 50 years, their happiness levels have remained the same. The notion of an individual fighting against the effects of adaptation brings to mind an image of a pedestrian walking up a descending escalator. Although the improving circumstances of her life may propel her upward toward ever greater happiness, the process of adaptation forces her back to her initial state. Together, these concepts and findings suggest that trying to become happier may be as futile as trying to become taller (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996). Indeed, some have argued that pursuing happiness may backfire altogether, if the pursuit becomes a conscious “extrinsic” goal that distracts people from enjoying the moment (Schooler, Ariely, & Loewenstein, in press; see also Sheldon, 2004). Moreover, striving for happiness may inevitably result in deep disappointment for many people. From this perspective, rather than seeking an upward spiral, maybe people would be better off simply accepting their current personality and happiness levels (McCrae & Costa, 1994). In Zen terms, perhaps one should try to transcend the pursuit of happiness rather than trying to maximize it (Gaskins, 1999). Indeed, a number of philosophical traditions embrace the notion that happiness should not be increased beyond an ideal level, one akin to a “Golden Mean” (Aristotle, 1974) between agony and ecstasy. To be sure, most people would undoubtedly reject an unrestrained, ceaseless pursuit of well-being. Present Sources of Optimism Is the pursuit of happiness futile? We believe not. Despite the seemingly compelling reasons we have listed for pessimism regarding attempts to elevate levels of well-being, there are also compelling reasons for optimism. In the following, we briefly describe four sources of optimism, returning to consider some of them in greater detail later. First, some researchers have had success, albeit limited and short term, in 114 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE using interventions to increase happiness (e.g., Fava, 1999; Fordyce, 1977, 1983, Lichter, Haye, & Kammann, 1980; Sheldon, Kasser, Smith, & Share, 2002). The potential of happiness-enhancing interventions is further reflected in emerging research in the positive psychology tradition demonstrating that practicing certain virtues, such as gratitude (Emmons & McCullough, 2003), forgiveness (McCullough, Pargament, & Thoresen, 2000), and thoughtful selfreflection (King, 2001; Lyubomirsky, Sousa, & Dickerhoof, 2004), can bring about enhanced well-being. Furthermore, research documenting the long-term effectiveness of cognitive and behavioral strategies to combat negative affect and depression has encouraging implications for the possibility of elevating long-term happiness (e.g., Gloaguen, Cottraux, Cucherat, & Blackburn, 1998; Jacobson et al., 1996). Second, many different motivational and attitudinal factors have been linked to well-being, factors that are presumably amenable to some volitional control. Examples of possible motivational factors include the successful pursuit of life goals that are intrinsic in content (e.g., Kasser & Ryan, 1996); concordant with a person’s interests, motives, and values (Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Grassman, 1998; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; Sheldon & Kasser, 1995); and internally consistent (e.g., Emmons & King, 1988; Sheldon & Kasser, 1995). Examples of potentially controllable attitudinal factors include the tendency to take an optimistic perspective on one’s life situations (e.g., DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; McCrae & Costa, 1986), the inclination to avoid social comparisons and contingent selfevaluations (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997), and the tendency to feel a sense of optimism or efficacy regarding one’s life (Bandura, 1997; Scheier & Carver, 1993; Seligman, 1991; Taylor & Brown, 1988). A third reason for optimism is provided by recent findings that older people tend to be somewhat happier than younger people (Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Diener & Suh, 1998; Roberts & Chapman, 2000; Sheldon & Kasser, 2001). Specifically, both cross-sectional and longitudinal work has shown that older persons report higher life satisfaction and lower negative affect. Although these main effects do not always emerge, they are observed frequently enough to suggest that greater happiness can indeed be achieved over time, not just by a few people but perhaps by the majority of people. Indeed, Carstensen’s (1995) socioemotional selectivity theory suggests that older people learn to structure their lives and pursue particular goals that maximize positive emotions, consistent with the proposal that people can learn to sustainably increase their wellbeing. Further supporting this notion are Sheldon and Kasser’s (2001) results, which showed that age-related increases in well-being are in part mediated by volitional changes, including older people’s ability to select more enjoyable and self-appropriate goals. Yet another reason why genes are not necessarily destiny is that they appear to influence happiness indirectly, that is, by influencing the kinds of experiences and environments one has or seeks to have. Thus, unwanted effects of genes could be minimized by active efforts to steer oneself away from situations that detract from well-being or by avoiding being enticed toward maladaptive behaviors (Lykken, 2000; Lyubomirsky, 2001). In addition, it is worth noting that heritability coefficients describe covariations, not mean levels. Furthermore, even a high heritability coefficient for a particular trait (such as happiness) does not rule out the possibility that the mean level of that trait for a specific population can be raised. Under the right conditions, perhaps anyone can become happier, even if her or his rank ordering relative to others remains stable. To summarize, it appears there is a paradox: Some theoretical perspectives and empirical data suggest that happiness can be increased, whereas other theories and data imply that it cannot. How can these conflicting perspectives on the possibility of happiness enhancement be resolved? Also, if enhanced happiness is indeed possible, what kinds of circumstances, activities, or habits of mind are most likely to bring gains, especially gains that can be maintained? Model of Happiness Accordingly, the primary question addressed in this article is the following: Through what mechanisms, if any, can a chronic happiness level higher than the set point be achieved and sustained? To this end, we describe the architecture of sustainable happiness. The integrative model of happiness we present accommodates the role of both personality/genetic and SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS circumstantial/demographic factors in happiness. However, it also goes beyond these crosssectional or concurrent factors to incorporate dynamic, time-sensitive factors. This extension allows the question of within-subject change in well-being, and maintained change, to be addressed. Most important, the model incorporates the role of motivational and attitudinal factors, consistent with the assumption that happiness can be actively pursued. We attempt to show that certain types of intentional activities indeed offer ways to achieve sustainable changes in well-being, despite the counteracting effects of adaptation. In the sections to follow, we first provide a working definition of chronic happiness. Then we define the three factors that affect it (genetic set point, circumstances, and activities) and argue that intentional activities offer the best potential route to higher and sustainable levels of happiness. Subsequently, we consider some more complex issues pertaining to the achievement of sustainable well-being via intentional activity, such as the role of person–activity fit, optimal timing and variety of activity, and the supportive role of sustained effort and positive habits. Then, in the final section of the article, we describe several preliminary efforts to increase happiness, based on our model, and discuss the nature of effective happiness interventions. Defining Happiness Here we define happiness as it is most often defined in the literature, that is, in terms of frequent positive affect, high life satisfaction, and infrequent negative affect. These three constructs are the three primary components of subjective well-being, according to Diener and colleagues (for reviews, see Diener, 1984, 1994; Diener et al., 1999). Supporting the legitimacy of considering them as indicators of the same underlying construct, we find that the measures are highly correlated and typically yield a single factor after negative affect has been recoded (Sheldon & Kasser, 1998, 2001; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2004). To refer to this group of measures, we use the term happiness or subjective well-being, although we also discuss mood and life satisfaction at times according to the specific ideas and data being presented. 115 It is important to note as well that we use a subjectivist definition of happiness, one that commonly relies on people’s self-reports. We believe this is appropriate and even necessary given our view that happiness must be defined from the perspective of the person. In other words, happiness is primarily a subjective phenomenon for which the final judge should be “whoever lives inside a person’s skin” (Myers & Diener, 1995, p. 11; see also Diener, 1994). However, the fact that the judgment of happiness is necessarily subjective does not mean that influences on that judgment cannot be studied empirically; for example, researchers might investigate the effects of factors such as a person’s recent experiences of positive emotion (Frijda, 1999), the frame in which the question is presented (Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999), the meaning that the person ascribes to the question (Schwarz & Strack, 1999), and the person’s sense of making satisfactory progress toward life goals at the time of the judgment (Carver & Scheier, 1990). We consider some of these factors in greater detail in a later section. Finally, the fact that self-reported happiness is subjective does not mean that it is unrelated to relatively more “objective” variables. For example, research has shown significant convergence of self-reported well-being with peer and spouse reports of well-being (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999; Sandvik, Diener, & Seidlitz, 1993), with recall of particular types of events (e.g., Seidlitz, Wyer, & Diener, 1997), with smiling behavior (e.g., Harker & Keltner, 2001), and with physiological responses (e.g., Lerner, Taylor, Gonzalez, & Stayn, 2002). Chronic Happiness Level Our primary focus in this article is on a person’s characteristic level of happiness during a particular period in his or her life, which we term the chronic happiness level. We define happiness this way because we wish to identify a quantity that is more enduring than momentary or daily happiness but that is also somewhat malleable over time and, thus, amenable to meaningful pursuit. According to this definition, although it is possible to alter one’s chronic happiness level, it is much more difficult to do so than to alter one’s happiness level at a particular moment or on a particular day. Operationally, one might define a person’s 116 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE chronic happiness level in terms of his or her retrospective summary judgments regarding his or her mood and satisfaction during some recent period (such as the past 2, 6, or 12 months) or as the average of momentary judgments of mood and satisfaction made at several times during the selected period. It is worth adding, however, that people may vary in their “hedonic profiles,” such that two individuals with similar chronic happiness levels might differ in their relative levels of contentment with life versus their relative frequency of experiencing positive and negative mood states. Determinants of the Chronic Happiness Level We focus on three primary types of factors that we believe causally affect the chronic happiness level, namely, the set point, life circumstances, and intentional activity. We focus on these three factors because they have historically received the majority of attention in the well-being literature, providing a substantial research base. We also focus on this three-factor distinction because it allows us to address several important issues and paradoxes, such as the question of whether it is even possible to “become happier” given strong genetic influences on happiness, the question of why past wellbeing research has revealed such weak associations between demographic/circumstantial variables and happiness, and the question of how a person might appropriately take action to “pursue” happiness. Figure 1 provides an illustration of the ap- Figure 1. Three primary factors influencing the chronic happiness level. proximate percentage of the variance that each of the three factors accounts for in cross-sectional well-being, as suggested by past research. As can be seen in the pie chart, existing evidence suggests that genetics account for approximately 50% of the population variation (Braungart et al., 1992; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Tellegen et al., 1988), and circumstances account for approximately 10% (Argyle, 1999; Diener et al., 1999). This leaves as much as 40% of the variance for intentional activity, supporting our proposal that volitional efforts offer a promising possible route to longitudinal increases in happiness. In other words, changing one’s intentional activities may provide a happiness-boosting potential that is at least as large as, and probably much larger than, changing one’s circumstances. In the following, we provide a definition of each factor, briefly consider whether and how changing that factor can lead to changes in people’s chronic well-being, and discuss whether such changes may be sustainable over the long term, that is, whether the forces of hedonic adaptation can be counteracted by that factor. Happiness Set Point We assume that an individual’s chronic happiness level is in part determined by her or his set point, which is defined as the central or expected value within the person’s set range. The happiness set point is genetically determined and is assumed to be fixed, stable over time, and immune to influence or control. Consistent with this assumption, twin studies (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Tellegen et al., 1988), long-term panel studies (Headey & Wearing, 1989), and studies of the effects of life events on well-being (Brickman et al., 1978) all indicate substantial long-term stability in happiness. For example, Lykken and Tellegen (1996) assessed well-being in twins at 20 years of age and then again at 30 years of age. The test–retest correlation was a considerable .50. Even more important, the cross-twin, cross-time correlation for the happiness of monozygotic twins was .40 (or 80% of the test–retest correlation), suggesting that the heritability of the “stable” component of happiness is approximately .80. In contrast, the cross-twin, crosstime correlation for dizygotic twins was close to zero (.07). Other studies, although differing in SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS their estimates of heritability, have consistently shown that monozygotic twins exhibit considerably more similar patterns of happiness change than do dizygotic twins, providing converging support that the variance in adult happiness is in large part determined genetically. The set point probably reflects relatively immutable intrapersonal, temperamental, and affective personality traits, such as extraversion, arousability, and negative affectivity, that are rooted in neurobiology (e.g., Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999; Davidson, 1999; Depue & Collins, 1999; Gray, 1990; Kagan, 2003; Robinson, Emde, & Corley, 2001), are highly heritable (Tellegen et al., 1988), and change little over the life span (McCrae & Costa, 1990). For example, Kagan has followed children from 4 months to 11 years of age and shown that sociability in 11-year olds can be traced to a particular type of infant temperament (called “low reactive”) that appears to involve a distinct neurochemical profile. Other writers, including Gray and Depue, have also compiled persuasive evidence for the neurobiological underpinnings of personality. This rapidly growing body of research supports the set point theory of personality and affect. Implications of the Set Point for Sustainable Increases in Chronic Happiness The preceding analysis implies that one’s chronic happiness during a particular life period can be increased, but not by changing one’s set point, because by definition it is constant. In other words, although it is possible that future scientists will learn how to alter people’s basic temperaments and dispositions, at present it appears that focusing on the set point is not a fruitful avenue for happiness increase. Again, however, one can posit that nongenetic factors also influence a person’s chronic happiness level, helping to determine whether the person falls in the lower or upper portion of his or her potential range at a particular time. The remaining variables in the model are designed to represent these other factors. Circumstances This category consists of happiness-relevant circumstantial factors, that is, the incidental but 117 relatively stable facts of an individual’s life. Happiness-relevant circumstances may include the national, geographical, and cultural region in which a person resides, as well as demographic factors such as age, gender, and ethnicity (see Diener et al., 1999, for a review). Circumstantial factors also include the individual’s personal history, that is, life events that can affect his or her happiness, such as having experienced a childhood trauma, being involved in an automobile accident, or winning a prestigious award. Finally, circumstantial factors include life status variables such as marital status, occupational status, job security, income, health, and religious affiliation. Again, previous cross-sectional research has linked all of the circumstantial factors just described to subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1999). For example, empirical evidence shows that people who are paid more are relatively happier (e.g., Diener, Sandvik, Seidlitz, & Diener, 1993) and that middle-class individuals are somewhat happier than working-class individuals (e.g., Warr & Payne, 1982). Married people are happier than those who are single, divorced, or widowed (e.g., Mastekaasa, 1994), even in cultures as diverse as those of Belarus and Spain (Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000). Findings also reveal that religiously committed people are relatively more likely to rate themselves as “very happy” (Gallup, 1984) and that, not surprisingly, healthy people, especially older ones, declare themselves to be slightly happier than sick people (e.g., Okun et al., 1984). However, as suggested earlier, all circumstances combined account for only 8% to 15% of the variance in happiness levels (Argyle, 1999; Diener et al., 1999). These relatively weak associations have been deemed surprising and paradoxical, given well-being researchers’ initial expectations that circumstantial factors such as income and physical health would be strongly related to happiness (Diener et al., 1999). We believe that these counterintuitively small effects can be largely accounted for by hedonic adaptation and the fact that people adapt rapidly to new circumstances and life events. This appears to be the case because adaptation—whether it is sensory (e.g., to a foul odor or a heavy weight; Brown, 1953), physiological (e.g., to very hot or cold temperatures; Dar, Ariely, & Frank, 1995), or hedonic (e.g., to a salary raise; Brickman et al., 1978; Parducci, 118 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE 1995)— occurs in response to stimuli that are constant or repeated. By definition, constancy is a feature of most circumstantial changes. Implications of Circumstances for Sustainable Increases in Chronic Happiness Of the different types of circumstances, life status variables in particular seem to offer some potential for increasing chronic happiness, in that individuals often have considerable control over them. For example, a college football player may sign a lucrative NFL contract, a middle-aged divorcee may remarry, or a retired couple may move to Florida to a condominium with a view, all becoming happier as a result. Will such new happiness last, however? Perhaps not, because, as mentioned earlier, hedonic adaptation tends to shuttle people back to their starting point following any positive circumstantial change. For example, Headey and Wearing (1989) found in their four-wave panel study that positive and negative events (e.g., “made lots of new friends,” “got married,” “experienced serious problems with children,” or “became unemployed”) influenced life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect as would be expected but that people kept returning to their original baselines. And Schkade and Kahneman (1998) revealed that although “living in California” is a seductive notion for many, it does not actually make people any happier in the long run. Furthermore, Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, and Diener (2003) showed that, for most people, the life satisfaction benefits derived from getting married tended to fade over the years. Thus, although one may gain a temporary “boost” by moving to a new region, increasing one’s income level, or changing one’s appearance, such boosts will probably not last, because people tend to adapt to constant circumstances. Other reasons why circumstantial changes may prove ineffectual for permanently increasing happiness include the fact that circumstantial changes can be costly (e.g., in terms of money, resources, and time) and, in many cases, impractical or even impossible. Also, once a realistic “ceiling” of positive circumstances is reached, it may be difficult to improve matters further. In short, the data suggest that changes in circumstances have limited potential for pro- ducing sustainable changes in chronic happiness. Although this strategy may work in the short term, it probably will not work in the long term. Of course, if people have not achieved basic subsistence and security, then it is logical for them to attend to these circumstances and basic needs first, before focusing on maximizing their happiness. However, we assume that, at best, satisfying basic needs can move people only up to their set point, not beyond. Intentional Activity Now we turn to the third and arguably most promising means of altering one’s happiness level: intentional activity. This is a very broad category that includes the wide variety of things that people do and think in their daily lives. Obviously, humans are very active creatures, with innumerable behaviors, projects, and concerns to which they devote energy. By “intentional,” we mean discrete actions or practices in which people can choose to engage (although the choice to initiate the activity may have become habitual, as discussed in a later section). We also assume that intentional activities require some degree of effort to enact. That is, the person has to try to do the activity; it does not happen by itself. Indeed, this point touches on one of the critical distinctions between the category of activity and the category of life circumstances; that is, circumstances happen to people, and activities are ways that people act on their circumstances. There is good reason to believe that intentional activity can influence well-being. For example, some types of behavioral activity, such as exercising regularly or trying to be kind to others, are associated with well-being (e.g., Keltner & Bonanno, 1997; Magen & Aharoni, 1991), as are some types of cognitive activity, such as reframing situations in a more positive light or pausing to count one’s blessings (Emmons & McCullough, 2003; King, 2001; Seligman, 1991), and some kinds of volitional activity, such as striving for important personal goals (Sheldon & Houser-Marko, 2001) or devoting effort to meaningful causes (M. Snyder & Omoto, 2001). Notably, it is impossible to fully separate behavioral, cognitive, and volitional activity; still, we believe the distinction is useful, and we continue to use it throughout the article. SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS Implications of Intentional Activity for Sustainable Increases in Chronic Happiness Again, it appears that increasing one’s set point and changing one’s life circumstances are not fruitful avenues for sustainable increases in chronic happiness. What, if anything, can provide such an avenue? In the following, we argue that intentional behavioral, cognitive, or volitional activity offers the best potential route. Some work has already investigated the impact of adopting new behaviors on longitudinal wellbeing, showing, for example, that faithfully engaging in a new exercise program positively boosts people’s mood and vitality and can even maintain the boosts for as long as 6 months (e.g., Ransford & Palisi, 1996; Stewart et al., 1997). Although little work has directly investigated the longitudinal effects of changing one’s cognitive attitudes and practices on enhanced well-being, the general success of cognitive– behavioral therapy in reducing suffering (Gloaguen et al., 1998) and recent work indicating positive effects of prompting people to practice positive psychological “virtues” such as gratitude (Emmons & McCullough, 2003), hope (C. R. Snyder, Ilardi, Michael, & Cheavens, 2000), and forgiveness (McCullough et al., 2000) suggest that cognitive activity offers many excellent possibilities for happiness interventions (Fordyce, 1983). Turning to the third type of intentional activity, recent longitudinal studies have focused specifically on volitional activity as a producer of enhanced well-being (see Sheldon, 2002, for a review). In such studies, students are typically asked to pursue self-generated personal goals over the course of a semester. High levels of goal progress or attainment consistently predict increased well-being (i.e., higher positive affect and life satisfaction and lower negative mood) from the beginning to the end of the semester, whereas low levels of progress predict reduced well-being (Brunstein, 1993; Sheldon, 2002). Specifically, Sheldon’s longitudinal research in this area (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998, 1999; Sheldon & Kasser, 1995, 1998) has shown that well-being increases are most likely when a person chooses and attains self-concordant goals, that is, goals that “fit” the person (as described subsequently). This work has also 119 highlighted one potential mediator from successful volitional activity to enhanced well-being, namely, accumulations of positive daily experiences along the way. The question of what other proximal factors may mediate changes in chronic happiness is addressed in more detail in a later section. Notably, these studies do not extend beyond a single span of time. Thus, they do not directly address the crucial question raised by the current article: whether gains in well-being last. Although Headey and Wearing’s important (1989) work suggests that gains in happiness do not last, notably, their study focused only on life events (“circumstances,” in our model) and did not take intentional activity into direct account. Recently, Sheldon and Houser-Marko (2001) addressed the question of sustainability by examining the effects of goal attainment on emotional well-being over two consecutive semesters. Consistent with earlier studies, they found that students who attained their personal goals during the first semester of their freshman year experienced enhanced adjustment and emotional well-being at the end of that semester. More important, they found that students could maintain their enhanced level of well-being, but only if they continued to do well at their goals during the second semester. In contrast, students who did well in the first semester but not in the second semester tended to regress back to their original well-being levels. This study offers direct support for our assumption that happiness can be enhanced and then maintained at the new level, especially when volitional activity is effectively pursued over long periods of time. Further supporting this conclusion, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2004) recently resurveyed these participants 3 years after the original study and found that initially high-performing students had maintained their earlier gains in emotional well-being throughout their college career. But what about adaptation? Is it not the case that even the most successful striver adapts to his or her happy situation eventually? More generally, is it not the case that people ultimately adapt to the positive effects of any activity in which they engage, whether it be behavioral, cognitive, or volitional, so that the activity loses its potency over time? Although hedonic adaptation undoubtedly constrains the happiness-inducing effects of in- 120 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE tentional activities, just as it does for circumstances, this adaptation effect appears to be weaker in the case of activity, as shown by recent data. For example, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2004) recently conducted several short-term longitudinal studies in which participants’ well-being (positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction) was measured at Time 1, and positive circumstantial and activity-based life changes were measured at Time 2. Well-being was then measured twice more, at Times 3 and 4. These investigators found consistent support for a path model, displayed in Figure 2, in which both positive circumstantial change and positive activity change predicted enhanced life satisfaction and positive affect at Time 3, but only positive activity change predicted maintained happiness gains at Time 4, with positive circumstantial change dropping out of the model. In other words, consistent with the present model, only activity-based well-being change lasted; circumstance-based happiness change did not. In a separate study, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2004) randomly assigned participants to report on either activity-based positive changes or circumstantially based positive changes in their lives. Relative to those in the circumstantial-change group, those in the activity-change group reported a weaker sense of “having gotten used to the change, such that it does not give the same boost as before,” and more strongly endorsed the statement “the change is something that varies over time, that is, something that adds variety to my life.” These findings further support the claim that activity changes are characterized by less hedonic adaptation than circumstantial changes. Parenthetically, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky’s (2004) findings, taken as a whole, support the validity of our distinction Figure 2. Longitudinal path model predicting maintained changes in well-being from positive circumstantial changes and positive activity changes. Asterisks indicate p ⬍ .01. between circumstantial changes and activity changes. Although the boundaries between these categories can be fuzzy, apparently they are clear enough to produce the predicted effects. Specific Advantages of Intentional Activity What are the sources of the sustainable happiness gains afforded by intentional activity? We posit that activity-based change, unlike circumstance-based change, has several desirable features that may help to combat adaptation. Intentional activity is episodic. One feature of activities is that they are, by definition, episodic and transient; after all, people cannot spend all of their time doing one thing. This in itself suggests that individuals may adapt less readily to new activities than to new circumstances. The episodic nature of activity also suggests that an additional way to maximize the impact of an activity is to attend to the timing of that activity. For example, a person might choose to “count her blessings” only after braving a difficult period, or only when she is especially needful of a boost. Suppose instead that she counts the same blessings every day, in a nonvarying routine. This person may become bored with the routine and cease to extract meaning from it. The length of time before one reengages in a happiness-boosting activity is an important part of its potency in the next application. By being mindful of the “refractory period” (Kalat, 2001) after which a recently performed activity regains its full happiness-inducing potential, individuals may maximize the benefits of the activity over time and avoid reducing or eliminating the activity’s effectiveness through overuse. Thus, people should strive to discover the optimal timing for each activity, that is, a frequency of engagement that allows that activity to remain fresh, meaningful, and positive for a particular person. Intentional activity can be varied. Another important parameter of behavioral, cognitive, and volitional activities is that people can continually vary them, both in their foci and in the ways they engage in them. This may help to reduce adaptation to the activity, allowing it to retain its potency (McAlister, 1982). Indeed, by definition, adaptation does not occur to stimuli that are variable or changeable but only to those that are constant or repeated (cf. Frederick & SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS Loewenstein, 1999). For example, a scientist may regularly ask new questions and become involved in new projects. In the process, she often feels the joy of making fascinating new discoveries and thus may remain particularly happy (i.e., at the upper end of her potential range) over a long period of time. If the person counting her blessings varies the domains of life in which she counts them (i.e., in relationships, in work, in her health, or in her most recently successful domain), then the strategy may remain “fresh” and meaningful and work indefinitely. Supporting this notion, past research suggests that people tend to seek variety in their behavior (McAlister, 1982; Ratner, Kahn, & Kahneman, 1999), perhaps because change—in both thoughts and actions—is innately pleasurable and stimulating (Berlyne, 1970; Rolls et al., 1981). Intentional activity can directly counteract adaptation. Yet another advantage of intentional activity is that it can directly tackle the problem presented by adaptation. For example, the cognitive practice of pausing to savor the good things in one’s life can directly counteract the effects of hedonic adaptation to one’s constant circumstances by drawing attention to the features that produced the initial happiness boost and helping to keep them from being taken for granted. As another example, practiced meditators frequently report renewed appreciation of the ordinary as a result of their intentional reencounters with the world. The fact that intentional activity can directly counteract adaptation and the hedonic treadmill helps shed further light on the distinction between life circumstances and intentional activities. Obviously, many personal characteristics are both. For example, “being married” and “being a student” both denote demographic status, yet they also reflect particular sorts of activities. From our perspective, the crucial distinction with respect to well-being is whether one exerts intentional effort with respect to the circumstantial category, that is, whether one acts upon the circumstance (e.g., using intentional practices to keep the circumstance “fresh”). For example, an individual can engage in a number of intentional activities with respect to the circumstantial category “marriage”: A husband can have the goal of making his marriage work (a volitional activity), he can make the effort to appreciate his wife’s positive qual- 121 ities (an attitudinal activity), and he can try to remember to bring her flowers (a behavioral activity). A person who performs these activities would probably best counteract adaptation to this particular circumstance and derive the most benefit from it. In contrast, consider a husband who is not intentionally engaged in his marriage; for him, this demographic circumstance would essentially become a background factor, to which adaptation is very likely. For all of these reasons, intentional activity appears to offer the best prospects for increasing and sustaining happiness. Of course, following through on new intentions, such as the ubiquitous “New Year’s resolutions,” is not necessarily easy (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998). Indeed, we assume that happiness-increasing strategies can be initiated and effectively pursued only with concerted, consistent commitment and effort. Still, activity-based factors are, by definition, under greater potential control by the individual than are genetic, demographic, and most life status factors. In other words, if anything can do it, intentional activity can. Implementing Happiness-Increasing Strategies In this section, we briefly consider several important issues pertaining to how intentional activity might be used for increasing happiness. In other words, having established that activity can potentially sustainably elevate happiness, how might one put this potential to work? We discuss these strategic issues in roughly chronological order, proceeding from the question of how to choose a particular happiness-boosting activity to the question of how such activity may be initiated and the question of how the activity can be maintained over time to produce a sustained increase in the chronic level of happiness. In the process, we discuss the issue of person–strategy fit, the meaning and nature of effort, the definition and role of habits, and the impact of short-term versus long-term considerations. Choosing an Activity: The Role of Person–Activity Fit Any one particular activity will not help every person become happier. People have endur- 122 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE ing strengths, interests, values, and inclinations that undoubtedly predispose them to benefit more from some strategies than others. For example, extraverts may benefit most from activities that bring them into regular contact with other people, and people high in nurturance motivation may benefit most from activities that afford them opportunities to take care of others. This general “matching” hypothesis (Harackiewicz & Sansone, 1991) is supported by much recent work showing that the positive effects of goal attainment on well-being are moderated by goal–person fit (Brunstein et al., 1998; Diener & Fujita, 1995; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; Sheldon & Kasser, 1998). It is also supported by past well-being intervention research. For example, in several studies that instructed participants to apply 14 different techniques to increase their personal happiness, the particular techniques considered most effective for raising happiness varied greatly from one individual to another and appeared to be determined by each participant’s needs and areas of specific weakness (Fordyce, 1977, 1983). The fit of an activity with a person might be conceptualized in a variety of ways, for example, with respect to individuals’ motive dispositions, basic needs, core values, signature strengths, personal resources, hedonic profiles, or other individual-difference characteristics. There are also a variety of ways that fit might be operationalized, such as in terms of self-reported fit, in terms of consistency between implicit and explicit measures of activity-relevant motives, or in terms of informant-rated person– activity fit. Another approach is to assume that certain kinds of experiences are likely to be beneficial to anyone, because these experiences reflect universal psychological needs. From this point of view, any activity that provides certain experiences, such as those involving belongingness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997), or autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 2000), might be assumed to “fit” the person, a priori. Role of Effort Initiating an activity. We assume that engaging in an activity requires at least two different kinds of effort: first, the effort required to initiate the activity and, second, the effort required to actually carry out and maintain the activity. This distinction is necessary because it is clear that many activities have definite positive effects if the person can only get started doing them. For example, exercising in the morning, making time to work on at least one important project during the day, or pausing to count one’s blessings at the end of the day can have significant benefits, but only if the person can “get over the hurdle” of remembering to do them and overcoming any obstacles to initiating them. Obviously, those who do not implement their activity intentions stand a worse chance of benefiting from them than those who do! We assume that this kind of self-regulatory effort requires considerable self-discipline and willpower. Furthermore, such effort may constitute a limited resource, one that must be marshaled carefully; in Muraven and Baumeister’s (2000) terms, self-regulatory will is like a “muscle” that has limited capacity in a given unit of time and must be used strategically to avoid fatigue. If this analogy is accurate, then it seems logical that some people develop the muscle to a greater extent than others, thus attaining a greater ability to “get started” on their intentions and gaining greater happiness potential. Of course, some activities will appear intrinsically more appealing and will be easier to jumpstart; this is undoubtedly one advantage of selecting an activity that fits one’s personality. For example, rather than running on a track, a fitnessseeking wilderness lover might instead choose to run on a trail through the woods, thereby feeling much less initial resistance to beginning the activity. As another example, rather than learning classical pieces, a jazz-loving piano student might instead choose to work on jazz standards, enhancing the intrinsic appeal of sitting down to practice. Maintaining an activity. This brings us to the second type of effort. Obviously, if a particular activity is to yield sustained happiness change, the person must keep performing the activity over the long term. For many effective happiness-enhancing activities, this will not be difficult, because the task will probably be inherently interesting or rewarding and thus will be “autotelic” in nature (Deci & Ryan, 2000), that is, self-reinforcing and self-sustaining. This is especially true to the extent that the person continually varies what he or she does. If, for example, a person shifts attention among several projects at work, explores new trails in the SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS state park, or seeks out interesting new piano pieces, his or her activities should remain intrinsically enjoyable and conducive to many rewarding “flow” experiences (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). What if the activity is not enjoyable and thus difficult to maintain? In this case, stopping the activity may not be problematic, because it probably is not working anyway. By emphasizing the importance of enjoying one’s intentional activity, however, we do not mean to imply that people should seek out only “fun” activities. Sometimes choosing to endure boring or even aversive experiences in the short term can have considerable positive effects on chronic happiness in the long term; for example, studying for an important exam in a tedious but required class may well represent an excellent investment in one’s future chronic happiness, even though it may detract from one’s momentary happiness. As another example, a naval officer candidate is paying a short-term cost (boot camp) to receive a longer term benefit (a career as an officer). Of note, self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Joiner, & Williams, 2003) posits that the crucial factor in such cases is whether the person has internalized the nonenjoyable activity, that is, whether he or she is able to find meaning and value expression in it, even if it is not pleasant to perform. From this perspective, the naval officer candidate would pay a smaller short-term cost if he could undergo boot camp thinking that “this is important and valuable” rather than thinking that “this is unnecessary and stupid.” The question of when and how to sacrifice short-term happiness in exchange for longer term happiness is an important one, as is the question of how to promote internalization of important happiness-relevant activities that are not intrinsically enjoyable. These questions represent promising directions for future research. Role of Habitual Activity If activities such as “looking on the bright side,” “making time for the things that matter,” and “working on an important life goal” make a difference for happiness, then it seems it would be a good idea to make a habit of doing them. However, on the surface, habits appear to present a conundrum for our model. Is it not the case that acquiring a habit means that one has 123 turned a formerly conscious activity into an unconscious routine, practiced automatically and without variation? If so, is it not the case that one is especially likely to experience hedonic adaptation to that activity, such that it loses its happiness-boosting potential? We think not. However, to illustrate, we must first distinguish between the habit of regularly initiating a potentially beneficial activity and the habit of implementing it the same way every time (the two types of effort mentioned earlier). We assume that hedonic adaptation occurs only with respect to particular experiences, and not with respect to the decisions that give rise to those experiences. Thus, making a habit out of deciding to initiate an activity is not problematic but may instead help people to keep getting “over the hump.” For example, a woman might make running an automatic part of her daily routine, such that she does not even have to make the decision of whether or not to run each day, thus deriving considerable benefit. What is potentially problematic is when people make a habit out of how they implement the activity. When this happens, the flow of experiences produced by such a habit is likely to remain relatively constant, and thus, adaptation is likely to have the most pernicious effects. To overcome this, as suggested earlier, people should mindfully attend to optimal timing and variety in the ways they practice an activity. For example, the woman might want to vary the route, time of day, and speed of her running. This will help forestall the effects of adaptation. Extensions and Further Questions Now that we have presented our basic conceptual model of sustainable changes in happiness, we briefly consider a variety of additional issues that extend beyond this basic model. What are the key ingredients of particular activities that lead a person to a higher level of well-being? Although this question is somewhat peripheral to our model, it merits brief discussion. We assume that happiness increases come from at least two sources that are described, respectively, by bottom-up and top-down theories of well-being (Diener, 1994). Bottom-up theories postulate that people make global wellbeing judgments in part with reference to emotions associated with their recent experiences (Kahneman, 1999). If they can recall a large 124 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE number of recent affectively positive experiences, then they report being very happy (see Sheldon & Elliot, 1999, for supporting data). Studies have produced support for this bottom-up perspective by showing that accumulations of need-satisfying daily experiences over time (such as competence, relatedness, and autonomy; Deci & Ryan, 2000) lead to enhanced global well-being at the end of that time (Reis, Sheldon, Ryan, Gable, & Roscoe, 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996). Furthermore, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2004) found, in their comparison of the sustained effects of circumstantial changes and activity changes on changes in well-being among students, that the more enduring activity-based effects on happiness were mediated by the greater feelings of competence and relatedness associated with activity changes during the semester. But what about when people say they are happy despite having had recent negative emotional experiences? Although bottom-up theories cannot account for this, top-down theories can. According to such models, well-being judgments are in part determined by global attitudinal or meaning-based factors. Thus, a person who “suffers for a cause” might still feel very happy because her suffering demonstrates her commitment to, and also perhaps moves her closer to obtaining, an important life goal. As another example, a man who has a bad day at work might still report being very happy that night, because of a short but meaningful visit from his grandchildren that evening that helped him to reframe the day. Again, we believe that intentional activity can lead to new well-being by both top-down and bottom-up routes, that is, both via accumulations of small positive experiences and via a sense of global meaning and purpose. Preliminary Data: Happiness Interventions The model of sustainable happiness that we have proposed has clear implications for how to design interventions for increasing happiness. Before describing these, we first discuss some of the few happiness intervention studies that have been conducted, to show their general consistency with our approach. As noted earlier, Fordyce (1977, 1983) conducted several happiness intervention studies in which he taught 14 happiness-relevant strategies to students as part of their coursework. All of the strategies fit into one or more of the three categories of activity outlined earlier: behavioral (e.g., “spend more time socializing”), cognitive (e.g., “become present oriented”), and volitional (e.g., “get better organized and plan things out”). Consistent with our conceptual model, Fordyce found that the strategies worked; that is, a significant main effect of participation was found for the experimental conditions. Again, intentional activity can successfully increase happiness. Also consistent with our model, he found that some strategies worked better than others and, in addition, that person–strategy fit had a moderating effect on strategy effectiveness. More recently, Sheldon and colleagues (2002) conducted an intervention study based on participants’ personal goals. Early in the semester, they taught experimental participants a set of four strategies for enhancing their experience and attainment of their personal goals: “own the goal,” “make it fun,” “keep a balance,” and “remember the big picture.” Consistent with the studies cited earlier, goal attainment predicted increases in well-being at the end of the semester. Interestingly, there was no main effect of experimental condition on increased well-being or goal attainment. Instead, a significant interaction was observed, such that only participants whose goals “fit” their interests and values benefited from the intervention. In other words, those with self-concordant goals who received the intervention evidenced the greatest goal attainment and, thus, the greatest increase in well-being. In addition to demonstrating that happiness-boosting interventions can work for at least some people, this finding provides further support for our proposition that the fit of the activity to the person makes a difference. Obviously, much future work remains to be done regarding happiness-increasing interventions, particularly research that explicitly manipulates the various factors in our model. As a preliminary test, Lyubomirsky, Tkach, and Sheldon (2004) recently conducted two 6-week happiness-enhancing interventions based in behavioral and cognitive–attitudinal change. Drawing on promising interventions grounded in the positive psychology tradition—that is, focused on building positive affect and personal strengths rather than on reducing or coping with negative affect, pathology, or weakness—they SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS used two diverse strategies (one cognitive and one social behavioral) to serve as initial existence proofs of our conceptual model. To this end, experimental participants were prompted to perform kind acts or to pause and “count their blessings.” The strategy of committing acts of kindness was expected, on the basis of previous theory and research, to boost temporary moods and long-lasting well-being. For example, individuals who report a greater interest in helping others, an inclination to act in a prosocial manner, or intentions to perform altruistic or courteous behaviors are more likely to rate themselves as dispositionally happy (see Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2004, for a review). We assume that acts of kindness and generosity can boost happiness in a variety of ways. Such acts may foster a charitable perception of others and one’s community, an increased sense of cooperation and interdependence, and an awareness of one’s good fortune. In addition, people who commit acts of kindness may begin to view themselves as altruistic people, as well as to feel more confident, efficacious, in control, and optimistic about their ability to help. Furthermore, acts of generosity can inspire greater liking by others, along with appreciation, gratitude, and prosocial reciprocity (Trivers, 1971), all of which are valuable in times of stress and need. Finally, kind behaviors may help satisfy a basic human need for relatedness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), thereby contributing to increased happiness. Thus, in their first intervention, Lyubomirsky, Tkach, and Sheldon (2004) asked students to perform five acts of kindness per week over the course of 6 weeks, either all five acts in 1 day or five acts spread over the week. Such acts were described as behaviors that benefit other people or make others happy, usually at some cost to oneself (e.g., donating blood, helping a friend with a paper, visiting an elderly relative, or writing a thank-you note to a former professor). A no-treatment control group simply completed measures of well-being immediately before the intervention and immediately after. The results, displayed in the top panel of Figure 3, provided preliminary evidence that a short-term happiness-enhancing activity can increase wellbeing. Furthermore, supporting our model’s predictions, Lyubomirsky et al. found that optimal timing was critical. Whereas control par- 125 Figure 3. Changes in well-being over the course of two 6-week interventions: performing acts of kindness (top) and counting one’s blessings (bottom). ticipants experienced a reduction in happiness over the course of the 6-week period, participants who committed acts of kindness experienced a significant increase in well-being, but this increase was evident only among those who showed their weekly generosity all in a single day. Because many of the kind acts that students performed were small ones, spreading them over the course of a week might have diminished their salience and power or made them less distinguishable from participants’ habitual kind behavior. The second intervention tested a cognitive happiness-increasing activity. Recently, Emmons and McCullough (2003) found that practicing grateful thinking on a regular basis can enhance concurrent well-being. Gratitude promotes the savoring of positive life experiences and situations so that maximum satisfaction and enjoyment are distilled from one’s circumstances. As noted earlier, this practice may directly counteract the effects of hedonic adaptation by helping people extract as much appreciation from the good things in their lives as possible. In addition, the ability to appreciate their life circumstances may also be an adaptive 126 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE coping strategy by which people positively reinterpret stressful or negative life experiences, bolster coping resources, and strengthen social relationships. Finally, the practice of gratitude appears to be incompatible with negative emotions and thus may reduce feelings of envy, anger, or greed. Thus, in the second 6-week intervention, students were instructed to contemplate “the things for which they are grateful” either once a week or three times a week. Examples of “blessings” listed by students included “a healthy body,” “my mom,” and “AOL instant messenger.” Control participants completed only the happiness assessments. The results again suggested that short-term increases in happiness are possible and, furthermore, that optimal timing is important. In summary, students who regularly expressed gratitude showed increases in wellbeing over the course of the study relative to controls, but these increases were observed only among students who performed the activity just once a week (see Figure 3, bottom panel). Perhaps counting their blessings several times a week led people to become bored with the practice, finding it less fresh and meaningful over time. Although the results of these two interventions are encouraging, they notably did not test the sustainability of the well-being increases for the experimental groups (i.e., “kindness” and “blessings”) and did not examine the impact of key moderators of activity effects. In the future, in addition to assessing the efficacy of different activities for producing sustainable increases in well-being, we will investigate the effects of such potential moderators as fit, effectiveness, timing, variety, cultural membership, social support, and the habits associated with the activity. What are the most general recommendations for increasing happiness suggested by our model? Simply, happiness seekers might be advised to find new activities to become engaged in, preferably activities that fit their values and interests. They should make a habit out of initiating the activity while at the same time varying their focus and timing in terms of the way they implement the activity. People might be advised to avoid basing their happiness on the acquisition of particular circumstances or objects (e.g., buying a luxury car or moving to California), because they will tend to habituate to such stable factors. Again, however, one can deter, or at least delay, such adaptation to positive circumstantial changes by engaging in intentional effort and activity with respect to them. That is, if one can remember to appreciate or actively engage with the object or circumstance (i.e., pause to savor the new Mercedes or take advantage of the California weather), then stable objects and circumstances may not be stable after all, from a phenomenological perspective. Thus, it remains the case that only life changes involving intentional activity can be expected to lead to sustainable changes in well-being. Conclusion If it is meaningful and important to pursue happiness, then it is crucial to find out how this can be accomplished. To what extent, and how, can people succeed in making themselves happier? In this article, we have attempted to integrate what is known about happiness change, especially longitudinal variations in well-being, into a single summary model. The model encompasses a wide variety of findings and suggests a number of new directions for research. More than two centuries have passed since the “pursuit of happiness” was proclaimed as a divinely ordained human right. We believe it is finally time for the issue of sustainable wellbeing to be given the scientific attention that it deserves. References Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 353–373). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Aristotle. (1974). The Nichomachean ethics (J. A. K. Thomson, Trans.). New York: Penguin. Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529 –550. Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 1–32. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Berlyne, D. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception & Psychophysics, 8, 279 – 286. Braungart, J. M., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Genetic influence on testerrated infant temperament as assessed by Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record: Nonadoptive and adoptive siblings and twins. Developmental Psychology, 28, 40 – 47. Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory (pp. 287– 302). New York: Academic Press. Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927. Brown, D. R. (1953). Stimulus similarity and the anchoring of subjective scales. American Journal of Psychology, 66, 199 –214. Brunstein, J. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grassman, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494 –508. Carlson, R. (1997). You can be happy no matter what: Five principles for keeping life in perspective. Novato, CA: New World Library. Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19 – 35. Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 136 –151. Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Zonderman, A. B. (1987). Environmental and dispositional influences on well-being: Longitudinal follow-up of an American national sample. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 299 –306. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Wong, M. M. (1991). The situational and personal correlates of happiness: A cross-national comparison. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 193–212). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. 127 Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804 – 813. Dar, R., Ariely, D., & Frank, H. (1995). The effect of past injury on pain threshold and tolerance. Pain, 60, 189 –193. Davidson, R. J. (1999). Neuropsychological perspectives on affective styles and their cognitive consequences. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 103– 123). Chichester, England: Wiley. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 227–268. DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197–229. Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926 –935. Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 419 – 436. Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M. (1993). The relationship between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social Indicators Research, 28, 195–223. Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1998). Subjective wellbeing and age: An international analysis. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics: Focus on emotion and adult development (Vol. 17, pp. 304 –324). New York: Springer. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276 –302. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Smith, H., & Shao, L. (1995). National differences in reported well-being: Why 128 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE do they occur? Social Indicators Research, 34, 7–32. Dillon, K. M., Minchoff, B., & Baker, K. H. (1985). Positive emotional states and enhancement of the immune system. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 15, 13–18. Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict among personal strivings: Immediate and longterm implications for psychological and physical well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1040 –1048. Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective wellbeing in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377–389. Estrada, C., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect influences creative problem solving and reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299. Fava, G. (1999). Well-being therapy: Conceptual and technical issues. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68, 171–179. Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program to increase happiness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 24, 511–521. Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 483– 498. Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-andbuild theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218 –226. Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172–175. Freedman, J. (1978). Happy people: What happiness is, who has it, and why. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Frijda, N. H. (1999). Emotions and hedonic experience. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 190 –210). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1984, March). Commentary on the state of religion in the U. S. today. Religion in America: The Gallup Report, No. 222. Gaskins, R. W. (1999). “Adding legs to a snake”: A reanalysis of motivation and the pursuit of happiness from a Zen Buddhist perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 204 –215. Gloaguen, V., Cottraux, J., Cucherat, M., & Blackburn, I. (1998). A meta-analysis of the effects of cognitive therapy in depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49, 59 –72. Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269 –288. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from here. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 21– 49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotions in women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 112–124. Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739. Isen, A. M. (1970). Success, failure, attention and reaction to others: The warm glow of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 294 –301. Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., et al. (1996). A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatment for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 295–304. Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New York: Basic Books. Kagan, J. (2003). Biology, context and developmental inquiry. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 1–23. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Wellbeing: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Kalat, J. W. (2001). Biological psychology (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280 –287. Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687– 702. King, L. A. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 798 – 807. Larsen, R. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Measurement issues in emotion research. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 40 – 60). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Lerner, J. S., Taylor, S. E., Gonzalez, R. M., & Stayn, H. B. (2002). Emotion, physiological reactivity, SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS and visceral self perception. Manuscript submitted for publication. Lichter, S., Haye, K., & Kammann, R. (1980). Increasing happiness through cognitive retraining. New Zealand Psychologist, 9, 57– 64. Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539. Lykken, D. (2000). Happiness: The nature and nurture of joy and contentment. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186 –189. Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others?: The role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American Psychologist, 56, 239 –249. Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A., & Diener, E. (2004). Is happiness a strength?: An examination of the benefits and costs of frequent positive affect. Manuscript submitted for publication. Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137–155. Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1997). Hedonic consequences of social comparison: A contrast of happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1141–1157. Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Dickerhoof, R. (2004). The medium is the message: The costs and benefits of thinking, writing, and talking about life’s triumphs and defeats. Manuscript submitted for publication. Lyubomirsky, S., Tkach, C., & Sheldon, K. M. (2004). [Pursuing sustained happiness through random acts of kindness and counting one’s blessings: Tests of two six-week interventions]. Unpublished raw data. Magen, Z., & Aharoni, R. (1991). Adolescents’ contributing toward others: Relationship to positive experiences and transpersonal commitment. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 31, 126 –143. Marks, G. N., & Fleming, N. (1999). Influences and consequences of well-being among Australian young people: 1980 –1995. Social Indicators Research, 46, 301–323. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). The cultural shaping of emotion: A conceptual framework. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influences (pp. 339 –351). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 129 Mastekaasa, A. (1994). Marital status, distress, and well-being: An international comparison. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 183–205. McAlister, L. (1982). A dynamic attribute satiation model of variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 141–150. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of Personality, 54, 385– 405. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford Press. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 173–175. McCullough, M. E., Pargament, K. I., & Thoresen, C. E. (Eds.). (2000). Forgiveness: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Guilford Press. McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 494 –512. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Selfregulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55, 56 – 67. Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science, 6, 10 –19. Norcross, J. C., Santrock, J. W., Campbell, L. F., Smith, T. P., Sommer, R., & Zuckerman, E. L. (2000). Authoritative guide to self-help resources in mental health. New York: Guilford Press. Okun, M. A., Stock, W. A., Haring, M. J., & Witter, R. A. (1984). The social activity/subjective wellbeing relation: A quantitative synthesis. Research on Aging, 6, 45– 65. Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Black, S. A., & Goodwin, J. S. (2000). Emotional well-being predicts subsequent functional independence and survival. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 48, 473– 478. Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and judgment: The contextual theory and its applications. Hove, England: Erlbaum. Ransford, H. E., & Palisi, B. J. (1996). Aerobic exercise, subjective health and psychological wellbeing within age and gender subgroups. Social Science and Medicine, 42, 1555–1559. Ratner, R. K., Kahn, B. E., & Kahneman, D. (1999). Choosing less-preferred experiences for the sake of variety. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 1–15. Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Gable, S. L., & Roscoe, J. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419 – 443. 130 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE Roberts, B. W., & Chapman, C. N. (2000). Change in dispositional well-being and its relation to role quality: A 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 26 – 41. Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Corley, R. P. (2001). Dispositional cheerfulness: Early genetic and environmental influences. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on developmental change (pp. 163–177). London: Oxford University Press. Rolls, B., Rowe, E., Rolls, E., Kingston, B., Megson, A., & Gunary, R. (1981). Variety in a meal enhances food intake in man. Physiology and Behavior, 26, 215–221. Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Subjective well-being: The convergence and stability of self-report and non-self-report measures. Journal of Personality, 61, 317–342. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power of positive thinking: The benefits of being optimistic. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 26 –30. Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy?: A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 9, 340 –346. Schooler, J. W., Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (in press). The explicit pursuit and assessment of happiness can be self-defeating. In J. Carrillo & I. Brocas (Eds.), Psychology and economics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Schwartz, B. (2000). Pitfalls on the road to a positive psychology of hope. In J. E. Gillham & J. Templeton (Eds.), The science of optimism and hope: Research essays in honor of Martin E. P. Seligman (pp. 399 – 412). Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press. Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61– 84). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Seidlitz, L., Wyer, R. S., & Diener, E. (1997). Cognitive correlates of subjective well-being: The processing of valenced life events by happy and unhappy persons. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 240 –256. Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of healthy goal-striving: When personal goals correctly represent the person. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination re- search (pp. 65– 86). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Sheldon, K. M. (2004). The benefits of a “sidelong” approach to self-esteem need-satisfaction: Comment on Crocker and Park (2004). Psychological Bulletin, 130, 421– 424. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are personal: Comparing autonomous and controlled reasons for goals as predictors of effort and attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546 –557. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482– 497. Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Selfconcordance, goal-attainment, and the pursuit of happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 152– 165. Sheldon, K. M., Joiner, T., & Williams, G. (2003). Self-determination theory in the clinic: Motivating physical and mental health. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543. Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319 –1331. Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Getting older, getting better?: Personal strivings and psychological maturity across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 37, 491–501. Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., Smith, K., & Share, T. (2002). Personal goals and psychological growth: Testing an intervention to enhance goal-attainment and personality integration. Journal of Personality, 70, 5–31. Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2004). Achieving sustainable gains in happiness: Change your actions, not your circumstances. Manuscript submitted for publication. Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes for a good day?: Competence and autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270 –1279. Snyder, C. R., Ilardi, S., Michael, S. T., & Cheavens, J. (2000). Hope theory: Updating a common process for psychological change. In C. R. Snyder & R. E. Ingram (Eds.), Handbook of psychological change: Psychotherapy processes and practices for the 21st century (pp. 128 –153). New York: Wiley. Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (2001). Basic research and practical problems: Volunteerism and the psy- SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS chology of individual and collective action. In W. Wosinska, R. B. Cialdini, D. W. Barrett, & J. Reykowski (Eds.), The practice of social influence in multiple cultures (pp. 287–307). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., & Pelled, L. H. (1995). Employee positive emotion and favorable outcomes at the workplace. Organization Science, 5, 51–71. Stewart, A. L., Mills, K. M., Sepsis, P. G., King, A. C., McLellan, B. Y., Roitz, K., & Ritter, P. L. (1997). Evaluation of CHAMPS, a physical activity promotion program for older adults. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 353–361. Stone, A. A., Neale, J. M., Cox, D. S., Napoli, A., Vadlimarsdottir, V., & Kennedy-Moore, E. (1994). Daily events are associated with a secretory immune response to an oral antigen in men. Health Psychology, 13, 440 – 446. Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1091–1102. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193– 210. 131 Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1031–1039. Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Leung, K., & Hui, C. H. (1990). A method for determining cultural, demographic, and personal constructs. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 302–318. Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57. Tversky, A., & Griffin, D. (1991). Endowment and contrast in judgments of well-being. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective wellbeing: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 101– 118). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press. Warr, P., & Payne, R. (1982). Experience of strain and pleasure among British adults. Social Science and Medicine, 16, 1691–1697. Williams, S., & Shiaw, W. T. (1999). Mood and organizational citizenship behavior: The effects of positive affect on employee organizational citizenship behavior intentions. Journal of Psychology, 133, 656 – 668. Received September 25, 2004 Accepted September 28, 2004 䡲 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2002, Vol. 83, No. 5, 1178 –1197 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1178 Maximizing Versus Satisficing: Happiness Is a Matter of Choice Barry Schwartz and Andrew Ward John Monterosso Swarthmore College University of Pennsylvania Sonja Lyubomirsky Katherine White and Darrin R. Lehman University of California, Riverside University of British Columbia Can people feel worse off as the options they face increase? The present studies suggest that some people—maximizers— can. Study 1 reported a Maximization Scale, which measures individual differences in desire to maximize. Seven samples revealed negative correlations between maximization and happiness, optimism, self-esteem, and life satisfaction, and positive correlations between maximization and depression, perfectionism, and regret. Study 2 found maximizers less satisfied than nonmaximizers (satisficers) with consumer decisions, and more likely to engage in social comparison. Study 3 found maximizers more adversely affected by upward social comparison. Study 4 found maximizers more sensitive to regret and less satisfied in an ultimatum bargaining game. The interaction between maximizing and choice is discussed in terms of regret, adaptation, and self-blame. principles of rational choice (e.g., J. Baron, 2000; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979, 1984; Tversky, 1969; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981; for a discussion, see Schwartz, 1986, 1994). In particular, modern behavioral economics has acknowledged that the assumption of complete information that characterizes rational choice theory is implausible. Rather than assuming that people possess all the relevant information for making choices, choice theorists treat information itself as a “commodity,” something that has a price (in time or money), and is thus a candidate for consumption along with more traditional goods (e.g., Payne, 1982; Payne, Bettman, & Johnson, 1993). Almost a half century ago, Simon (1955, 1956, 1957) suggested an approach to explaining choice that was more cognizant of human cognitive limitations than rational choice theory. Simon argued that the presumed goal of maximization (or optimization) is virtually always unrealizable in real life, owing both to the complexity of the human environment and the limitations of human information processing. He suggested that in choice situations, people actually have the goal of “satisficing” rather than maximizing. To satisfice, people need only to be able to place goods on some scale in terms of the degree of satisfaction they will afford, and to have a threshold of acceptability. A satisficer simply encounters and evaluates goods until one is encountered that exceeds the acceptability threshold. That good is chosen. In subsequent, accidental encounters with other goods in the relevant domain, the scale of acceptability enables one to reject a formerly chosen good for a higher ranked one should that one turn up. A satisficer thus often moves in the direction of maximization without ever having it as a deliberate goal. Simon’s alternative to rational choice theory questions not only the processes by which options are assessed and choices made, but also the motives that underlie choice. To satisfice is to pursue not the best option, but a good enough option. Rational choice theory has tried to explain preference and choice by assuming that people are rational choosers (von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1944). According to the rational choice framework, human beings have well-ordered preferences—preferences that are essentially impervious to variations in the way the alternatives they face are described or the way in which they are packaged or bundled. The idea is that people go through life with all their options arrayed before them, as if on a buffet table. They have complete information about the costs and benefits associated with each option. They compare the options with one another on a single scale of preference, or value, or utility. And after making the comparisons, people choose so as to maximize their preferences, or values, or utilities. Although the science of economics has historically depended on the tenets of rational choice theory, it is now well established that many of the psychological assumptions underlying rational choice theory are unrealistic and that human beings routinely violate the Barry Schwartz and Andrew Ward, Department of Psychology, Swarthmore College; John Monterosso, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania; Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside; Katherine White and Darrin R. Lehman, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. This research was facilitated by support from the Positive Psychology Network (M. Seligman, Director), an intramural grant from Swarthmore College to Barry Schwartz, a sabbatical grant from the Solomon Asch Center for Study of Ethnopolitical Conflict to Andrew Ward, an intramural grant from the University of California to Sonja Lyubomirsky, a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) doctoral fellowship to Katherine White, and grants from SSHRC and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council to Darrin R. Lehman. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Barry Schwartz, Department of Psychology, Swarthmore College, 500 College Avenue, Swarthmore, Pennsylvania 19081. E-mail: bschwar1@ swarthmore.edu Can There Be Too Much Choice? There is no question that greater choice can provide benefits for the chooser. Indeed it is axiomatic in rational choice theory that 1178 MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING people cannot have too many options. If, for example, one is trying to decide between two models of a CD player, and then discovers that a third model also is available, the third model may be just the thing one is after. If not, one can simply go back to deliberating between the first two. And one can always ignore the new, third option altogether. So it seems irrational to perceive oneself as worse off as a result of added possibilities for choice. Nonetheless, there is now a small body of evidence suggesting that added options are a mixed blessing (e.g., Simenson & Tversky, 1992; Tversky & Shafir, 1992). Results have begun to appear in the decision-making literature indicating that adding options can make a choice situation less rather than more attractive for people—that indeed, sometimes people prefer it if others make the choices for them (Beattie, Baron, Hershey, & Spranca, 1994). In one series of studies (Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; see also Iyengar & Lepper, 1999), participants were more likely to purchase exotic jams or gourmet chocolates when they had 6 options from which to choose than when they had 24 or 30, respectively. And perhaps more importantly, those with fewer options expressed greater satisfaction with the choices they made. Similarly, university students were more likely to write an extracredit essay, and wrote better essays, when they had 6 topics to choose from than when they had 30. Iyengar and Lepper suggested several possible factors that may underlie this effect. One is the avoidance of potential regret. The more options there are, the more likely one will make a nonoptimal choice, and this prospect may undermine whatever pleasure one gets from one’s actual choice. There is ample evidence that regret aversion is a potent force in decision making—perhaps even more potent than the loss aversion that has been a significant feature of Kahneman and Tversky’s (1979) prospect theory of decision making (Beattie et al., 1994; Bell, 1982, 1985; Larrick & Boles, 1995; Loomes & Sugden, 1982; Ritov, 1996; Simenson, 1992; Zeelenberg, 1999; Zeelenberg & Beattie, 1997; Zeelenberg, Beattie, van der Pligt, & de Vries, 1996; Zeelenberg et al., 1998). A second factor that may make increased choice unattractive is that it creates a seemingly intractable information problem. It is hard enough to gather information and go through the deliberations needed to make the best choice among 6 options. To choose the best among 30 options is truly daunting. So rather than even try, people may disengage, choosing almost arbitrarily to complete the process. As a result of this disengagement, many of the psychological processes that normally are recruited to enhance the attractiveness of the choices one makes may not be operative (see Gilovich & Medvec, 1995, for an account of some of these processes, in the context of their theory of regret). Maximizing, Satisficing, and Choice Schwartz (2000) recently argued that the proliferation of options can have a variety of negative effects on well-being. He suggested that as options are added within a domain of choice, three problems materialize. First, there is the problem of gaining adequate information about the options to make a choice. Second, there is the problem that as options expand, people’s standards for what is an acceptable outcome rise. And third, there is the problem that as options expand, people may come to believe that any unacceptable result is their fault, because with so many options, they should be able to find a satisfactory one. Similar problems arise as choice 1179 becomes available in domains in which previously there was no choice. No matter how dissatisfied one is with one’s telephone service, if phone service is provided by a regulated monopoly, one cannot do better, and inadequate service is not one’s fault. However, when choice of phone service becomes available, there is no longer any reason to tolerate inadequate service, and failure to obtain adequate service is one’s responsibility. Schwartz (2000) suggested that people might in general be better off with constrained and limited choice than with unconstrained choice. However, expanded opportunities for choice need not have these negative psychological effects. Consider the different effects that an expanding array of options might have on two people, one of whom aims to maximize his or her outcomes in that domain and one of whom aims to satisfice. For the maximizer, added options pose problems. One cannot be sure that one is making the maximizing choice without examining all the alternatives. And if it is impossible or impractical to examine all the alternatives, then when the maximizer gives up the search and chooses, there will be a lingering doubt that he or she could have done better by searching a bit more. Thus, as options proliferate, the likelihood of achieving the goal of maximization goes down. Further, the potential for regret is ever present, because the question the maximizer is asking him- or herself is not “is this a good outcome?” but “is this the best outcome?”. Expanded opportunities for choice may have different effects on the satisficer. The satisficer is looking for something that crosses the threshold of acceptability—something that is good enough. Adding options in a domain in which the satisficer has already encountered something good enough need have no effect; the new options may simply be ignored. With “good enough” rather than the “best” as a criterion, the satisficer will be less inclined to experience regret if it turns out that an option better than the chosen one was available. And if no satisfactory option has been encountered in a domain, added options will provide new possibilities for finding something that crosses the “good enough” threshold. Thus, the risk of being made worse off by added options may be minimal for satisficers. Are some people maximizers and others satisficers? Do people differ in the nature of the goals they pursue in choice situations? And if so, do people also differ in their sensitivity to potential regret? Is it concern about potential regret that influences some people to be maximizers? We addressed these questions in the present series of studies by creating survey instruments designed to distinguish maximizers from satisficers and to measure sensitivity to regret. And if people do differ in these respects, does it make a difference? We addressed this question in the present studies in several ways. Study 1 examined the relations between one’s scores on a Maximization Scale and a Regret Scale and scores on measures of happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, depression, neuroticism, and perfectionism. In subsequent studies we attempted to validate some of these putative relations and to identify possible mediators. Study 2, guided by the notion that maximizers might seek more information than satisficers when making decisions, used a questionnaire to examine relations between maximization and the amount of social comparison that goes into making purchasing decisions, as well as the satisfaction people derive from those decisions. Study 3, inspired by findings reported by Lyubomirsky and Ross (1997) that unhappy people are more affected by upward social comparison than happy people, further explored SCHWARTZ ET AL. 1180 the relation between maximizing and social comparison processes by examining whether maximizers and satisficers respond differentially to social comparison manipulations. Finally, Study 4 examined the possible causal role of regret in mediating between maximizing and dissatisfaction by exposing participants to a computer game designed to manipulate the potential for regret. We anticipated that maximizers would be more sensitive to regret than satisficers, and would derive less satisfaction from their results in games in which the opportunity for regret was salient. Study 1. Maximizing, Satisficing, and Regret: Scale Development This study involved the creation and evaluation of two new measuring instruments— one designed to assess the tendency to satisfice or maximize and one designed to assess the tendency to experience regret. Materials were administered to seven samples of participants, four of them university students and three of them community adults. In total, 1,747 participants completed the maximizing and regret questionnaires. Method Overview Packets of questionnaires were administered to seven samples (total N ⫽ 1,747). Participants completed the questionnaires either in small groups of 3 to 7 (Samples 1 and 3), during one large, group session (Samples 2, 4, 5, and 7), or individually (Sample 6). Completing the questionnaires required less than 45 min (in the case of Sample 6, less than 15 min). For each administration, the content of the questionnaires varied, as described below. Participants Each of the first four samples comprised students in introductory psychology courses, who received course credit for their participation. The first two samples (n ⫽ 82 and n ⫽ 72) were recruited at Swarthmore College, the third sample (n ⫽ 100) at the University of California, Riverside, and the fourth sample (n ⫽ 401) at the University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada. The fifth sample (n ⫽ 752) consisted of participants at a 1-day seminar for allied health care professionals (mean age ⫽ 47 years), the majority of whom were nurses. The sixth sample (n ⫽ 220) was composed of individuals recruited at a large train station in an urban setting (mean age ⫽ 41 years), and the seventh (n ⫽ 120) comprised individuals in an urban courthouse approached while waiting to be informed if they would serve on a jury (mean age ⫽ 40 years). There were approximately equal numbers of males and females in the first three samples, along with the sixth sample (i.e., individuals at the train station), but the fourth sample (i.e., students at the University of British Columbia) included 258 females and 141 males (2 participants failed to identify their gender), the fifth sample (i.e., health care providers) included 684 females and 60 males (8 participants failed to identify their gender), and the final sample (members of a jury pool) included 87 females, 28 males, and 5 individuals who failed to identify their gender. The third, fourth, and seventh samples also were quite diverse ethnically. The third sample was 39% Asian, 24% Caucasian, 10% Chicano(a)/Latino(a), 8% African American, and 10% other, and the fourth sample (classified using slightly different categories) was 51% of East Asian descent (e.g., Chinese, Taiwanese), 25% of Western European descent (e.g., British, French), 9% of East Indian descent (e.g., Indian, Pakistani), and 15% other. The seventh sample was 48% Caucasian, 45% African American, and 7% other. Materials Sample 1. Our aim with the first sample was to create maximization and regret scales and to investigate correlations between responses to those scales and well-established measures of well-being. Participants completed a preliminary 42-item questionnaire designed to measure maximization (33 items) and regret (9 items). Participants responded to each item using a 7-point, Likert-type scale (1 ⫽ completely disagree, 7 ⫽ completely agree). In addition, they completed a four-item Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) designed to assess dispositional happiness (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999), a depression survey (the 13-item short form of the Beck Depression Inventory [BDI-SF; Beck & Beck, 1972]), and a measure of dispositional optimism (the Life Orientation Test [LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985]). On the basis of item reliability and face validity, the measure was reduced to 22 items, 17 assessing maximization and 5 assessing regret. These 22 items were then presented to 11 judges (advanced undergraduate students majoring either in psychology or economics) who were unaware of either the purpose of our studies or the specific hypotheses under investigation. The judges were asked to examine each item and indicate whether, on the one hand, it probed an individual’s inclination to “get the best out of any situation” or “settle for good enough,” or, on the other hand, it probed a person’s sensitivity to “the possibility that he or she might regret a decision once made.” Of the 5 “regret” items, 4 were judged by 10 of our 11 informants to be about regret and the 5th was judged by 9 informants to be about regret. Of the 17 “maximization” items, 10 were judged by 10 of 11 informants to be about maximization, 3 were so judged by 9 informants, and 4 were so judged by 7 informants. Thus, we were reasonably confident that our measures had face validity—that our understanding of what the questions were asking would be matched by that of the participants. We then submitted the 5 regret items and 17 maximization items to a principal-components factor analysis (PCA), which is reported below. Sample 2. Participants in the second sample received these 22 items, unidentified and intermixed. In an independent test of the putative associations investigated in Sample 1, they were also asked to complete the SHS and the BDI-SF. Sample 3. Participants in the third sample completed the same 22-item questionnaire assessing maximization and regret, along with the SHS, BDI-SF, and LOT. In addition, they completed a questionnaire probing life satisfaction (the Satisfaction With Life Scale [Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985]) and a scale assessing dispositional Neuroticism (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991), a Big Five trait that we thought might be correlated with maximization. Sample 4. The fourth sample of participants completed the 22-item questionnaire along with the SHS. In addition, these participants were asked a series of questions regarding tendencies to engage in social comparison and patterns of purchasing behavior. These materials comprise the substance of Study 2 and thus discussion of them appears later in the article. Sample 5. The fifth sample completed the same 22-item questionnaire in addition to the aforementioned measures of happiness and depression (i.e., the SHS and BDI). Sample 6. The sixth sample also completed the 22-item questionnaire and the SHS, along with a 15-item perfectionism subscale composed of the Self-Oriented Perfectionism items of the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Hewitt & Flett, 1990, 1991). The addition of this scale was intended to investigate participants’ tendencies to hold exceedingly high standards for themselves in a variety of domains. The scale included items such as, “One of my goals is to be perfect in everything I do,” and “I demand nothing less than perfection of myself” (1 ⫽ disagree, 7 ⫽ agree). In addition, a subset of participants (n ⫽ 146) completed the 10-item measure of self-esteem developed by Rosenberg (1965). Sample 7. The only measures relevant to this article that were completed by members of the prospective jury pool were the 22-item maximi- MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING zation/regret survey and the same 10-item measure of self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). Results Factor Analysis We conducted a PCA on the combined samples (n ⫽ 1,747) to determine the factor structure of the regret and maximizing items. We sought the solution that best approximated a simple structure—that is, the one in which most of the items loaded on at least one factor, and each item loaded on only one factor. What emerged, on the basis of a varimax rotation, was a six-factor solution. However, two of the factors contained only two items each, and one item failed to load on any of the factors. In addition, the item-total correlations for all but one of these five items were quite low. We thus eliminated the four items with low item-total correlations, resulting in a 13-item Maximization Scale and a five-item Regret Scale. We conducted another PCA on these 18 items. The resulting four-factor solution is presented in Table 1. The first factor, on which all five regret items loaded, references “regret,” and makes up the Regret Scale. The other factors are subcategories of maximizing/satisficing and make up the Maximization Scale. The second and third factors are largely behavioral examples of maximizing. The second factor includes being open to better jobs, songs on the radio, television shows, and relationships, liking lists that rank things, and fantasizing about alternatives to reality (which also loaded on the “regret” factor, though its loading was lower than that of the other “regret” items; in addition, this item was judged by 9 of our 11 informants to be more about maximizing and satisficing than about regret). The third factor, which concerns primarily shopping behaviors, includes shopping for a friend, renting videos, and shopping for clothing. Writing several drafts of letters so as to word things just right also loads on this factor. The fourth factor represents having high standards, both for oneself and for things in general. One of the items that loaded on this factor also loaded on the “regret” factor. Its loading on the “regret” factor was substantially lower than all the other regret items, and also lower than its loading on this “maximizing” factor. Moreover, this item was judged by 10 of our 11 informants to be about maximizing. All further analyses, in this and subsequent studies, used responses to the modified, 13-item Maximization Scale rather than the 17-item scale participants actually saw. The correlation (across all participants) between scores on the 13and the 17-item scales was .99 ( p ⬍ .001). Cronbach’s alpha was .71 for the Maximization Scale and .67 for the Regret Scale. Correlations With Standard Personality Measures Sample 1. Table 2 presents the Pearson’s zero-order correlations between the variables investigated in Study 1. As can be seen in the table, a tendency for participants to be maximizers rather than satisficers (␣ ⫽ .70) was significantly correlated with a tendency to experience more regret and depression, as well as to be less optimistic, and less happy ( p ⬍ .06). By way of further illustration, of the 18 people who scored 8 or above on the BDI-SF, qualifying for a diagnosis of at least mild depressive symptoms (Beck & Beck, 1972), 8 (44%; Mean BDI score ⫽ 13.25) also scored in the top quartile for maximization, whereas only 1 (6%; BDI score ⫽ 8) scored in the bottom quartile. By contrast, of 1181 the 19 people scoring in the top quartile for happiness, 8 (42%; Mean SHS score ⫽ 16.88) were in the bottom quartile for maximization, whereas only 3 (16%; Mean SHS score ⫽ 16.33) were in the top quartile. Sample 2. Table 2 also presents the correlations between the 13-item maximization composite (␣ ⫽ .60) and the five-item regret composite (␣ ⫽ .78) for Sample 2, along with the BDI-SF and the SHS—the only other measures administered to this sample. Once again, we observed strong associations between maximization and a tendency to experience regret and depression, and lower levels of happiness. Sample 3. Our third sample provided a further opportunity to investigate relations between maximization and various personality constructs. Table 2 presents the intercorrelations between the maximization composite (␣ ⫽ .70), the regret composite (␣ ⫽ .70), and measures of optimism, happiness, depression, neuroticism, and satisfaction with life. Correlations between maximizing and the constructs of regret, depression, and satisfaction with life were significant beyond the p ⬍ .01 range. In addition, maximizing was negatively correlated with optimism ( p ⬍ .05) and happiness ( p ⬍ .10). However, the relation between maximizing and neuroticism was not significant ( p ⬎ .10). In sum, in addition to replicating the results found with Sample 1, this sample provided evidence for a strong relation between maximization and diminished life satisfaction, as well as a nonsignificant relation with neuroticism. Sample 4. Table 2 presents data from our fourth sample on the relations between the maximization composite (␣ ⫽ .63), the regret composite (␣ ⫽ .73), and the SHS. Once again, the correlation between maximizing and regret was significant, although the relation between maximizing and happiness was modest (r ⫽ .10, p ⬍ .05). Sample 5. The fifth section of Table 2 displays the correlations between maximizing (␣ ⫽ .70), regret (␣ ⫽ .74), happiness, and depression for the sample of health care providers who completed the relevant measures. As seen with the previous samples, a tendency to score highly on the Maximization Scale was predictive of greater regret and depression, as well as lower levels of selfreported happiness. Sample 6. The sixth section of Table 2 presents correlations for participants approached at the urban train station. Once again, maximizing (␣ ⫽ .72) was positively correlated with regret (␣ ⫽ .67), and negatively correlated with happiness. In addition, maximizing was significantly correlated with perfectionism (r ⫽ .25, p ⬍ .001). And for the subsample who completed the relevant measure, maximizing and self-esteem were negatively correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.30, p ⬍ .001). Interestingly, however, whereas maximizing and perfectionism were significantly correlated, neither happiness (r ⫽ .12, p ⬍ .08) nor self-esteem (r ⫽ .02, ns) correlated significantly with perfectionism. Indeed, if anything, the relation between perfectionism and happiness was positive rather than negative. Sample 7. The final sample, taken from prospective jury members, replicated the significant association between maximizing (␣ ⫽ .73) and self-esteem exhibited by the subsample in Sample 6, (r ⫽ ⫺.26, p ⬍ .01). Across the seven samples, maximization scores ranged from 1.15 to 6.62, with a mean of 3.88 and a median of 3.85. Also, across all samples, the correlation between maximizing and regret SCHWARTZ ET AL. 1182 Table 1 Factor Analysis of the Regret and Maximization Scales Using PCA With Varimax Rotation Factor F1 F2 F3 F4 Item-total r Regret Scale Whenever I make a choice, I’m curious about what would have happened if I had chosen differently. .78 .62 Whenever I make a choice, I try to get information about how the other alternatives turned out. .74 .57 If I make a choice and it turns out well, I still feel like something of a failure if I find out that another choice would have turned out better. .62 .51 When I think about how I’m doing in life, I often assess opportunities I have passed up. .61 .51 Once I make a decision, I don’t look back. (R) .56 .40 Maximization Scale When I watch TV, I channel surf, often scanning through the available options even while attempting to watch one program. .81 .45 When I am in the car listening to the radio, I often check other stations to see if something better is playing, even if I’m relatively satisfied with what I’m listening to. .77 .46 I treat relationships like clothing: I expect to try a lot on before I get the perfect fit. .51 .33 No matter how satisfied I am with my job, it’s only right for me to be on the lookout for better opportunities. .44 .41 .40 .44 .38 .33 I often fantasize about living in ways that are quite different from my actual life. .43 I’m a big fan of lists that attempt to rank things (the best movies, the best singers, the best athletes, the best novels, etc.). I often find it difficult to shop for a gift for a friend. .73 .39 When shopping, I have a hard time finding clothing that I really love. .71 .31 Renting videos is really difficult. I’m always struggling to pick the best one. .68 .46 I find that writing is very difficult, even if it’s just writing a letter to a friend, because it’s so hard to word things just right. I often do several drafts of even simple things. .57 .33 No matter what I do, I have the highest standards for myself. .80 .20 I never settle for second best. .78 .25 .51 .28 Whenever I’m faced with a choice, I try to imagine what all the other possibilities are, even ones that aren’t present at the moment. .36 Note. Item marked by “R” was reverse scored in the analysis. The factor analysis was a principal-components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation, using eigenvalues greater than 1 as the extraction method. The last column displays the corrected item-total correlations for each item with its respective scale (i.e., regret [first five items] or maximization). MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING 1183 Table 2 Pearson’s Zero-Order Correlations Among Variables in Six Samples Variable Max Regret SHS Sample 1 (n ⫽ 82) Regret SHS BDI LOT .61*** ⫺.21 .24* ⫺.28* ⫺.15 .03 ⫺.07 Sample 2 (n ⫽ 72) Regret SHS BDI .45*** ⫺.34** .44*** ⫺.40** .46*** ⫺.55*** Sample 3 (n ⫽ 100) Regret SHS BDI LOT NR SWLS .36*** ⫺.17 .27** ⫺.25* .16 ⫺.27** ⫺.51*** .47*** ⫺.35*** .35*** ⫺.54*** ⫺.66*** .74*** ⫺.58*** .71*** Sample 4 (n ⫽ 401) Regret SHS .39*** ⫺.10* ⫺.27*** Sample 5 (n ⫽ 752) Regret SHS BDI .46*** ⫺.28*** .31*** ⫺.40*** .39*** ⫺.66*** Sample 6 (n ⫽ 220) Regret SHS .50*** ⫺.17* ⫺.22** — — ⫺.46*** .54*** BDI LOT NR — — — — ⫺.51*** — — — — — — ⫺.54*** .49*** ⫺.68*** ⫺.50*** .59*** ⫺.48*** — — — — — — — — — — — — — — — — — — Note. Dashes indicate that data were not collected for this measure. Max ⫽ Maximization Scale; Regret ⫽ Regret Scale; SHS ⫽ Subjective Happiness Scale; BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory; LOT ⫽ Life Orientation Test; NR ⫽ Neuroticism; SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction With Life Scale. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001. was .52 ( p ⬍ .001), and in the samples in which it was assessed, happiness and maximizing were significantly correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.25, p ⬍ .001)—as were maximizing and depression (r ⫽ .34, p ⬍ .001). Gender Differences No gender differences were found in Samples 1–3 or 5 in participants’ scores on the Maximization Scale, the Regret Scale, or in the association between maximizing and measures of wellbeing and regret. In Samples 4, 6, and 7, a significant gender difference emerged—that is, males were more likely than females to be maximizers in all three of these samples: Sample 4 (Ms ⫽ 4.46 vs. 4.27), t(395) ⫽ 2.41, p ⬍ .02; Sample 6 (Ms ⫽ 4.08 vs. 3.79), t(209) ⫽ 2.26, p ⬍ .05; and Sample 7 (Ms ⫽ 4.33 vs. 3.91), t(107) ⫽ 2.03, p ⬍ .05. Partial Mediation by Regret Because questionnaires from Samples 1–3 and 5 included a common measure of depression and Samples 1– 6 included a common measure of happiness as well as our maximization and regret scales, we were able to investigate a putative mechanism underlying the observed effects, namely, that the relations observed between maximization and both depression and happiness were mediated by a tendency to experience regret. According to R. M. Baron and Kenny (1986; see also Martin, Tesser, & McIntosh, 1993), four criteria must be met to establish mediation: (1) the predictor variable (i.e., maximization) must be related to the criterion variable (e.g., depression); (2) the mediator (i.e., regret) must be related to the predictor; (3) the mediator must be related to the criterion (controlling for the influence of the predictor); and (4) the relation between the predictor and the criterion must be eliminated or significantly reduced when the criterion is regressed simultaneously on the predictor and the mediator. Turning first to depression, across the four samples, we observed a significant relation between maximization and scores on the BDI (r ⫽ .34, p ⬍ .001), meeting the first of the aforementioned criteria. In addition, regret and maximization were strongly correlated (r ⫽ .52, p ⬍ .001), meeting Criterion 2, and the relation between regret and depression (r ⫽ .39) remained significant in a regression SCHWARTZ ET AL. 1184 equation that controlled for the influence of maximization, F(2, 983) ⫽ 105.45, p ⬍ .001, regret  ⫽ .29; maximizing  ⫽ .19 (meeting Criterion 3). Finally, as this last multiple regression equation (which regressed depression simultaneously on regret and maximization) makes clear, although the relation between maximization and depression remained significant after controlling for regret, consistent with the dictates of Criterion 4, the relation was significantly weaker than it had been in the absence of regret (i.e., a change in beta from .34 to .19), an effect confirmed by a test based on Sobel’s (1982) method for determining the existence of a mediational relation (z ⫽ 7.87, p ⬍ .001; see also MacKinnon & Dwyer, 1993; Preacher & Leonardelli, 2001). Similar analyses confirmed a mediational role played by regret in the relation between maximization and happiness, which were significantly negatively correlated across the six samples (r ⫽ ⫺.25, p ⬍ .001). In brief, when regret, which was also negatively correlated with happiness (r ⫽ ⫺.37), was entered into a regression equation along with maximization as predictors of happiness, the aforementioned relation between maximization and happiness was significantly reduced (i.e., a change in beta from ⫺.25 to ⫺.08), as confirmed by a Sobel test of mediation (z ⫽ 10.91, p ⬍ .001). In sum, regret appeared to play a partial mediational role between maximization and depression and between maximization and happiness. However, because of high correlations between regret and other constructs investigated in one or more samples— constructs such as depression, happiness, and subjective wellbeing—any mediational role ascribed to regret should be viewed with caution. And, of course, regret was not manipulated in any of these samples, permitting no causal conclusions to be drawn. Discussion Study 1 provided evidence for individual differences in what people aspire to when they make decisions in various domains of their lives. Maximizers desire the best possible result; satisficers desire a result that is good enough to meet some criterion. When we correlated scores on our Maximization Scale with wellestablished measures of well-being, we found that maximizers reported significantly less life satisfaction, happiness, optimism, and self-esteem, and significantly more regret and depression, than did satisficers. Though Study 1 tells us nothing about the direction of causality, it is possible that whereas a maximizing decision strategy might, as a matter of logic, yield better objective outcomes than a satisficing strategy, it is likely to yield worse subjective outcomes. Study 1 also revealed that although maximizing was significantly correlated with perfectionism (Sample 6), the correlations of each of these measures with happiness and self-esteem in the study were quite different (happiness was negatively correlated with maximizing and positively correlated with perfectionism; self-esteem was negatively correlated with maximizing and uncorrelated with perfectionism), suggesting that maximizing and perfectionism are distinct. Study 1 also tells us nothing about the stability over time of scores on the Maximization Scale. If a maximizing orientation is something like a trait, we would expect response patterns to be stable over time. Although a good deal more research is needed, Gillham, Ward, and Schwartz (2001) have collected repeated measures from 102 undergraduates, who were given the Maximization Scale four times over a period of 9 months. Scores at Time 1 correlated with scores at Time 2 (r ⫽ .81), with scores at Time 3 (r ⫽ .82), and with scores at Time 4 (r ⫽ .73). Though larger samples and longer interevaluation intervals are essential before any firm conclusions can be drawn, these results suggest that a maximizing orientation enjoys some degree of stability. How is a maximizer to judge whether a given outcome is the best possible outcome? In many cases, there is not a finite and transparent set of possibilities to allow for complete and unambiguous judgment. For example, what does it mean to have the best possible salary, meal at a restaurant, wardrobe, or even the best possible spouse? Although imagination could provide a standard, a more probable basis for the maximizer’s assessment in these domains is social comparison (perhaps only with those seen as belonging to an appropriate comparison group). What does it mean to have ordered the best possible meal at a restaurant other than that it is better than anyone else’s meal? Thus, whereas “good enough” usually can be judged in absolute terms, “the best possible” may often require social comparison. Being a maximizer may require one to be concerned with one’s relative position. Festinger (1954) and Frank (1985, 1999; see also Hirsch, 1976) have argued persuasively that people do seem to be guided largely by how they are doing relative to relevant others, and several studies that compared the effects of absolute and relative position on satisfaction have observed that good relative position produces greater satisfaction than good absolute position (Bazerman, Loewenstein, & White, 1992; Bazerman, Moore, Tenbrunsel, WadeBenzoni, & Blount, 1999; Blount & Bazerman, 1996; Hsee, Blount, Loewenstein, & Bazerman, 1999; Solnick & Hemenway, 1998). Poor relative position, however, appears to affect some people more than others. For example, Lyubomirsky and Ross (1997) reported that unhappy people are more affected by upward social comparison than happy people. More specifically, in their first study, Lyubomirsky and Ross found that whereas both happy and unhappy people derived satisfaction from information that their performance was better than that of a peer, only unhappy people seemed to suffer from information that their performance was worse than that of a peer. Especially relevant are the findings from Study 2 of the Lyubomirsky and Ross (1997) article. In that study, happy and unhappy students received positive or negative feedback from the experimenter on a novel teaching task, and then witnessed a same-sex peer receive even more positive or even more negative feedback than themselves. The most striking finding from this study was that unhappy students reported feeling happier and more self-confident when they had received a poor evaluation on their performance (2 out of 7), but heard their peer receive an even worse one (1 out of 7), than when they had received an excellent evaluation (6 out of 7), but heard their peer receive an even better one (7 out of 7). Happy students, by contrast, did not show this pattern of “sensitive” responding to comparisons with peers. These findings lend some credibility to our hypothesis that maximizers may be more concerned with relative position, and thus with social comparison, than satisficers, especially in light of the findings from Study 1 that maximizers are generally less happy than satisficers. Studies 2 and 3 were designed to explore directly the relative importance of social comparison to maximizers and satisficers. Study 2 inquired about social comparison in the context of purchasing decisions. Study 3 replicated Lyubomirsky and Ross’s MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING (1997) first study with groups of participants identified as maximizers or satisficers. Study 2. Maximizing, Satisficing, Social Comparison, and Consumer Behavior Because many of the choices that people make in their daily lives concern the purchase and consumption of goods, Study 2 explored maximizing and satisficing with respect to consumer purchasing decisions. As indicated in the introduction, a proliferation of options can pose significant problems for a maximizer. One cannot be sure that one is making the best choice without examining all the alternatives. And if examination of all the alternatives is not feasible, then when the maximizer finally chooses, there may be a lingering doubt that he or she could have done better with more searching. Thus, as options increase, the likelihood of successful maximization goes down. Further, the potential for regret is ever present because the maximizer is asking “is this the best outcome?” and “could I have done better?” And in attempting to answer these questions, given the time and information-processing constraints that everyone faces, maximizers may be inclined to rely on information about how others are doing as a way of assessing whether their chosen outcomes were indeed the best. Thus, in Study 2, we were particularly interested in the relation between maximizing tendencies and social comparison, regret, and happiness with consumer purchasing decisions. We anticipated that maximizing would predict reports of engaging in more social comparison and experiencing greater regret in general. In addition, we expected that maximizing would predict reports of more product comparison, social comparison, and counterfactual thinking regarding purchases, and that these consumer comparisons would lead to heightened consumer regret and decreased happiness regarding purchases. Method Participants, Materials, and Procedure Participants were the 401 undergraduates described earlier as Sample 4 in Study 1. The materials comprised a questionnaire that included the Maximization Scale, the Regret Scale, and the SHS from Study 1. In addition, we created a scale to measure frequency of social comparison in general, beliefs about the appropriateness of upward social comparison, and beliefs about the appropriateness of downward social comparison.1 In a pretest of this scale (n ⫽ 76), the subscales measuring frequency of social comparison (␣ ⫽ .69), upward social comparison (␣ ⫽ .74), and downward social comparison (␣ ⫽ .70) demonstrated adequate reliability. In addition, this pretest demonstrated that the frequency of social comparison subscale correlated with a validated measure of social comparison tendencies (r ⫽ .50, p ⬍ .001; Gibbons & Buunk, 1999). Consumer behavior items were created regarding people’s general tendencies toward consumer-related social comparison, product comparison, counterfactual thinking, and consumer regret. After completing these items, participants were asked to recall either an expensive or an inexpensive recent purchase. Participants in the inexpensive condition were asked to “recall the most recent item you have purchased that was relatively inexpensive, say around $5.00. For example, the item might be a movie rental, a book, or a magazine.” Those in the expensive condition were asked to “recall the most recent item you have purchased that was relatively expensive, say around $500.00. For example, the item might be sporting equipment, electronic equipment, etc.” 1185 All participants then answered specific consumer behavior questions regarding their purchase, such as product price, product comparison, time to decide on the product, prepurchase and postpurchase social comparison, counterfactual thinking, happiness with the product, and regret regarding the recalled purchase. The order of presentation of the Maximizing and Regret Scales and the consumer behavior items was counterbalanced. Because counterbalancing did not predict significant variance in any of the dependent variables, the results are collapsed across this variable. Participants completed the questionnaire packet in class, and were debriefed at the end of the study. Results Construction of Indexes Composites of social comparison frequency (␣ ⫽ .68), downward social comparison (␣ ⫽ .68), and upward social comparison (␣ ⫽ .71) were created. Composites also were constructed for general product comparison (␣ ⫽ .72), general social comparison (␣ ⫽ .72), and general consumer regret (␣ ⫽ .82). Finally, an index of consumer maximizing tendencies for a specific purchase was created by combining the measures of product comparison, time to decide, prepurchase social comparison, postpurchase social comparison, and counterfactual thinking (␣ ⫽ .81). Happiness, Regret, and Social Comparison Tendencies As reported in Study 1, maximizing was associated with being less happy (though this relation was modest) and experiencing more regret. We anticipated that maximizing also would predict reports of engaging in social comparison. A linear regression analysis with maximizing as the predictor on the overall index of social comparison frequency supported this prediction, F(1, 393) ⫽ 39.07, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .30. Regression analyses on the upward and downward social comparison indexes indicated that maximizing was also predictive of interest in upward, F(1, 397) ⫽ 8.99, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ .15, and downward, F(1, 397) ⫽ 21.14, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .23, social comparisons. In addition, maximizing predicted reports of engaging in upward, F(1, 394) ⫽ 33.63, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .28, and downward, F(1, 395) ⫽ 15.09, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .19, social comparisons more frequently. Regression analyses indicated that both frequency of downward social comparison (when statistically controlling for frequency of upward social comparison), F(2, 392) ⫽ 15.23, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .18, and frequency of upward social comparison (when statistically controlling for frequency of downward social comparison), F(2, 392) ⫽ 42.19, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .31, were predictive of reports of 1 In the interest of brevity, the items measuring social comparison frequency (e.g., “How frequently do you compare yourself to other people in general?”), appropriateness of upward social comparison (e.g., “Comparing oneself to those who are better off can be useful”), appropriateness of downward social comparison (e.g., “It is inappropriate to compare one’s own standing to those who are not doing as well” [reverse scored]), general consumer behavior (e.g., for product comparison: “When I am planning to purchase an item of clothing, I like to look at all the stores first to make certain I get the perfect item”), and specific consumer behaviors (e.g., for product comparison: “How many products did you consider before choosing this particular one?”) can be obtained from authors Katherine White (kjwhitek@interchange.ubc.ca) or Darrin R. Lehman (dlehman@cortex .psych.ubc.ca). 1186 SCHWARTZ ET AL. regret. In addition, although frequency of downward social comparison (when statistically controlling for frequency of upward social comparison) was not predictive of subjective happiness, F(2, 392) ⫽ 0.47, ns,  ⫽ .04, upward social comparison frequency (controlling for downward social comparison) was, F(2, 392) ⫽ 10.09, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ ⫺.16. Of interest, maximizers seemed to be oriented toward both upward and downward social comparisons. Past research and theorizing suggest that upward comparisons may trigger negative affective states, lead to low ratings of subjective well-being, and result in negative consequences for the self (e.g., Diener, 1984; Morse & Gergen, 1970; Salovey & Rodin, 1984), whereas downward comparisons often have the opposite effect, allowing the individual to feel better in comparison to a worse off other (e.g., Morse & Gergen, 1970; Wills, 1981). The puzzle here is that although maximizing was predictive of engaging in more downward social comparison, it was also predictive of regret. Is it the case that maximizers are susceptible to the negative consequences of upward social comparison, but unable to reap the benefits of downward social comparison? This is not implausible in light of suggestive evidence that social comparison in general is not compatible with happiness (Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997; Lyubomirsky, Tucker, & Kasri, 2001). To address this possibility, we examined whether upward social comparison and downward social comparison were related to regret among those high in maximization. We performed a median split on the maximizing scale, and examined the relation between regret and social comparison among those scoring high on the maximizing scale. The results revealed that, among those high on maximizing, frequency of upward social comparison (controlling for downward social comparison) was predictive of regret, F(2, 197) ⫽ 7.08, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ .19, whereas frequency of downward social comparison (controlling for upward social comparison) was not, F(2, 197) ⫽ 2.50, p ⬍ .12,  ⫽ .11. We also found that, among those high on maximizing, frequency of upward social comparison (controlling for downward social comparison) was predictive of decreased happiness, F(2, 198) ⫽ 6.57, p ⬍ .02,  ⫽ ⫺.18, whereas frequency of downward social comparison (controlling for upward social comparison) was not positively related to happiness, F(2, 198) ⫽ .25, ns,  ⫽ .04. This provides some support for the notion that whereas maximizers tend to experience the negative consequences of upward social comparisons, they are unable to benefit from downward social comparisons. General Consumer Behaviors Linear regression analyses on the general consumer behavior items revealed that maximizing predicted the tendency to engage in product comparison, F(1, 397) ⫽ 42.49, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .31, social comparison, F(1, 396) ⫽ 12.27, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ .17, and counterfactual thinking, F(1, 397) ⫽ 29.40, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .26, regarding purchases. Further, maximizing was predictive of reports of consumer regret, F(1, 397) ⫽ 19.16, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .22. Consumer Behaviors for Recalled Purchases Participants were asked to report on either an inexpensive or an expensive purchase. The average amount spent on inexpensive purchases was $6.57, and maximizing was not predictive of the amount spent on inexpensive purchases (F ⬍ 1, ns). The majority of inexpensive purchases were magazines (22.4%), movie rentals (22.0%), food (15.1%), and books (10.7%). Other inexpensive purchases included such things as cosmetics, school supplies, and CDs. The average amount spent on expensive purchases was $538.00, and, once again, maximizing was not predictive of the amount spent (F ⬍ 1, ns). The majority of expensive purchases were stereo equipment (16.8%), computers (15.8%), and clothing (15.3%). Other expensive items included sporting equipment and other electronic items (e.g., TVs, cell phones). Regression analyses indicated that, when recalling a specific purchase, maximizing predicted the consideration of more products, F(1, 389) ⫽ 5.23, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ .12, and taking longer to decide, F(1, 390) ⫽ 13.13, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .18. Maximizing predicted reports of engaging in social comparison both before, F(1, 390) ⫽ 4.51, p ⬍ .04,  ⫽ .11, and after, F(1, 390) ⫽ 5.52, p ⬍ .02,  ⫽ .12, making purchases. Furthermore, maximizing was associated with engaging in more counterfactual thinking regarding purchases, F(1, 390) ⫽ 34.12, p ⬍ .001,  ⫽ .28. Finally, maximizing was predictive of reports of diminished positive feelings toward purchases (i.e., an index of happiness and regret, with regret reverse scored), F(1, 389) ⫽ 9.68, p ⬍ .01,  ⫽ ⫺.16. Thus, it appears that maximizers not only report engaging in more comparisons (i.e., product comparisons, social comparisons, and counterfactual comparisons) regarding their consumer decisions, they also report experiencing heightened regret and decreased happiness. Given the relation between maximizing and happiness observed in these studies, it is possible that findings that we have attributed to individual differences in maximizing may really be due to differences in dispositional happiness, a plausible hypothesis given Lyubomirsky and Ross’s (1997) finding that unhappy people are more affected by upward social comparison information than happy people. To examine this possibility, we conducted partial correlation analyses between maximizing and regret, frequency of social comparison, maximizing tendencies (i.e., an index of time to decide on the purchase, product comparison, prepurchase social comparison, postpurchase social comparison, and counterfactual thinking), and consumer feelings (i.e., an index of consumer happiness and regret), controlling for dispositional happiness. The partial correlations between maximizing and regret (r ⫽ .39, p ⬍ .001), frequency of social comparison (r ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .001), maximizing tendencies (r ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001), and consumer feelings (r ⫽ ⫺.13, p ⬍ .02) all remained significant when levels of happiness were statistically controlled. Thus, it appears that maximizing makes a contribution to regret, to social comparison, to consumer behaviors, and to consumer satisfaction over and above that of dispositional happiness. Discussion As anticipated, maximizing was predictive of reports of engaging in social comparison, being concerned with what others were doing, and finding upward and downward social comparison more appropriate. Maximizing also predicted product comparison, social comparison, and counterfactual thinking with regard to purchases. Moreover, maximizing predicted consumer feelings, such that those high on maximizing ultimately experienced more regret and less happiness regarding their purchases. These patterns held after MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING controlling for dispositional happiness. Furthermore, our findings regarding consumer behavior suggest that social comparisons and product comparisons stimulated counterfactual thoughts, which then engendered regret (see, e.g., Roese, 1997). Although a measure of general counterfactual thinking was not included in this study, recent research indicates that maximizers ruminate more than satisficers (White, Lehman, & Schwartz, 2002). It may be the case that counterfactual thinking and ruminative thoughts are related to the general regret reported by maximizers, as well as to consumption-related regret. Thus, it appears that striving for the best things in life may have paradoxical consequences. Intuition, along with previous research (e.g., Morse & Gergen, 1970), suggests that whereas upward social comparison might yield regret and unhappiness, downward social comparison might yield elation. Study 2 found no such mood enhancing effects of downward social comparison. However, a close look at the recent literature on social comparison suggests that consistent positive effects of downward social comparison are reliably reported only for individuals who have low self-esteem or experience physical or psychological threat (e.g., Affleck & Tennen, 1991; Aspinwall & Taylor, 1993; Gibbons & Gerrard, 1989; Taylor, 1983; see Wills, 1991, for a review). In the general population, the mood effects of social comparison are much less predictable. Recent findings suggest that the affective consequences of social comparison are not intrinsic to its direction (e.g., Buunk, Collins, van Yperen, Taylor, & Dakof, 1990). That is, both upward and downward comparisons can have positive or negative implications for the self (e.g., Brewer & Weber, 1994; Brown, Novick, Lord, & Richards, 1992; Buunk et al., 1990; Hemphill & Lehman, 1991; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997; Taylor, Buunk, & Aspinwall, 1990; Tesser, 1988; Wood & VanderZee, 1997). A limitation of Study 2 is that although it relied on reports of real-life experiences, these were merely recalled by participants. Because participants’ recollections of the purchasing situation could be biased or incomplete, it is important to assess social comparison, happiness, and regret among maximizers and satisficers in other settings as well. Thus, Study 3 attempted to examine reactions to social comparison information in a controlled laboratory setting. Study 3. Maximizing, Satisficing, and Social Comparison Because maximizers are continually chasing the best possible option when making a decision, they try to gather and analyze all of the information available to them. Information about one’s relative standing with one’s peers—that is, social comparison information—is likely to be an important source of information in their decision-making process. Thus, maximizers are expected to be more interested in social comparison feedback and more sensitive to such feedback than satisficers. Accordingly, the primary hypothesis tested in Study 3 was that the moods and self-evaluations of maximizers would be more vulnerable or sensitive to unsolicited social comparison information than would those of satisficers. This study asked participants to solve anagrams at whatever rate they were capable, but manipulated the ostensible performance of an undergraduate peer so that participants experienced relative “success” (i.e., their peer performed worse than themselves) or relative “failure” (i.e., their peer performed better than themselves). This paradigm was developed 1187 by Lyubomirsky and Ross (1997, Study 1), who found support for a parallel prediction regarding chronically unhappy and happy individuals. That is, in their study, self-rated unhappy students who solved puzzles in the presence of a faster peer showed smaller increases in mood and self-confidence and expressed greater doubts about their own ability than those exposed to a slower peer. Happy individuals, by contrast, did not exhibit this pattern of sensitive responding to social comparison feedback. Study 3 was characterized by several notable features. First, to minimize possible experimental demand characteristics and suspicion on the part of participants, and to simulate typical “realworld” peer comparison contexts, social comparison information was provided indirectly. That is, the experimenter never explicitly offered any comparison of performances, although such information was made highly salient to the participants. Second, the relevant task and dimension of evaluation (i.e., anagram-solving ability) was one about which participants were unlikely to have objective standards for evaluating their performance. Finally, participants enjoyed wide latitude in managing the social comparison information they faced. That is, they were free to minimize or maximize the relevance, importance, and controllability of the evaluation dimension; they were free to compete with, identify with, or simply ignore their more or less successful peer; and they were free to attribute their own performance and/or that of their peer to whatever factors they wished. To summarize, whereas in Study 2, participants only responded to questions regarding their social comparison tendencies in connection with consumer choices, Study 3 used a more powerful and more direct manipulation of social comparison information, one involving a real-life peer performing alongside the participant in the laboratory. And in Study 3, rather than measuring participants’ interest in and seeking of such comparison information, we examined the actual effects of social comparison information provided by the context. Method Overview In the context of a purported study of cognitive performance, maximizers and satisficers (as categorized by their earlier responses to the Maximization Scale) solved anagram puzzles while a supposed peer (who was actually an experimental confederate) ostensibly completed the same set of anagrams much faster or much slower than themselves. Participants rated themselves with respect to their current mood and anagram-solving ability both before and after completion of the anagram-solving task. Participants Fifty-four students enrolled in an introductory psychology course at the University of California, Riverside received course credit for their participation in this study. Participants were selected on the basis of their responses to the 13-item Maximization Scale, which was presented in the context of a mass-administered questionnaire (n ⫽ 82). Responses to the 13 items, which displayed good internal consistency (␣ ⫽ .79), were combined and averaged to provide a single composite score, ranging from 2.6 to 6.7, with a median of 4.2 on the 7-point scale. A sample of 26 maximizers and 28 satisficers, that is, those whose composite scores were respectively either in the top or bottom third of the distribution, were recruited for the study by telephone. The maximizers’ group mean on the Maximization Scale was 5.26 (SD ⫽ 0.50), whereas the 1188 SCHWARTZ ET AL. satisficers’ group mean was 3.49 (SD ⫽ 0.43). We should note that the omnibus questionnaire used in selecting these participants also included the SHS and BDI (Beck, 1967). The correlations between participants’ scores on the Maximization Scale and their scores on the SHS and BDI were moderate to high (r ⫽ ⫺.27 and r ⫽ .46, respectively). The inclusion of these scales, although not specifically intended for this purpose, allowed us to pursue issues of discriminant validity. Procedure and Materials In each experimental session, two individuals—a participant and a same-gender confederate pretending to be another participant— completed the relevant questionnaires and experimental tasks together. The experimenter, who was unaware of participants’ maximization status, explained that participants were being paired simply to “save time.” The experiment was introduced as a study of “cognitive performance”— that is, one in which “we hoped to learn how personality and various situational variables affect performance on a problem-solving task.” Accordingly, participants were told they would be asked to solve a series of anagram puzzles during the experimental session. To bolster this cover story, a number of filler items, including questions about how often participants solved puzzles and how much they enjoyed them, as well as their quantitative and verbal Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT) scores, were embedded in the various questionnaires administered throughout the study. Before undertaking the primary experimental task, participants completed a preliminary questionnaire assessing their premanipulation or “baseline” mood. Mood was assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), with 10 items measuring positive affect (PA; ␣ ⫽ .87) and 10 items measuring negative affect (NA; ␣ ⫽ .79) on 5-point Likert-type scales. Participants also provided a baseline measure assessing how good they initially thought they were at solving anagrams (1 ⫽ very poor, 7 ⫽ excellent). Anagram-solving task and social comparison manipulation. After the participants had completed the preliminary questionnaire, a female experimenter gave instructions for the 15-min anagram-solving task, which closely followed a procedure developed by Lyubomirsky and Ross (1997, Study 1). She began by handing each of them a “sample” puzzle card containing three anagrams—that is, Y-O-W-N-S (SNOWY), N-O-T-I-X (TOXIN), and A-S-S-I-B (BASIS)—and indicating that such cards would be used throughout the anagram task. She further explained that upon unscrambling any two of the three anagrams on a given card, they were to write their solutions and the card number on their answer sheet, then hand the card back to the experimenter and receive a new card containing new anagrams. Participants were also given a notebook to use as scratch paper (one page per card). At that point, the experimenter instructed them to begin solving anagrams and handing in their cards. What the participants did not know was that their coparticipant was an experimental confederate who had been instructed to monitor their partner’s pace and solve either approximately twice as many or half as many anagrams as he or she did—a task that they accomplished successfully. The back-and-forth handing of the anagram cards as the participant and confederate worked side-by-side throughout the 15-min period, along with the consecutive numbering of the cards and the turning of notebook pages, served to make it highly salient to participants that their peer was performing at a much faster or much slower pace. Postperformance questionnaires. Immediately following the 15-min anagram task, the participant and confederate were led to separate rooms, and the participant was asked by the experimenter to complete a second set of self-assessments. To assess participants’ changes in mood as a function of their own performance in the two social comparison conditions, participants were asked to complete the PANAS for a second time (␣ ⫽ .92 and ␣ ⫽ .83 for the PA and NA scales, respectively). To assess changes in participants’ perceptions of their own ability in light of their performance and the apparently superior or inferior performance of their peer, participants were asked to rate again how good they thought they were at solving anagrams (1 ⫽ very poor, 7 ⫽ excellent). As a manipulation check, participants were next asked to rate their own just-completed task performance and that of the confederate using 7-point scales (1 ⫽ very poor, 7 ⫽ excellent). Finally, when all dependent variables had been collected, participants completed a debriefing questionnaire and engaged in an oral debriefing, in which they were given an opportunity to share their hunches about the hypothesis of the study and to report any other suspicions. No guesses or notable suspicions were reported. The entire session, including a process debriefing (Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975), lasted approximately 1 hr. Results Premanipulation Measures Measures completed prior to the anagram-solving task suggested no between-group differences in ability or experience. That is, maximizers and satisficers did not differ significantly in their quantitative and verbal SAT scores, in their initial self-ratings of ability at solving anagrams, in their reports of how often they solved anagrams, or in how much they enjoyed solving them (all ts ⬍ 1). Notably, the two groups also did not differ significantly in their baseline moods (ts for both PA and NA ⬍ 1). Manipulation Check Overall, participants solved a mean of 12.7 anagrams (SD ⫽ 7.84) during the allotted 15-min test period. Analyses revealed no significant performance differences between satisficers and maximizers overall, t(53) ⫽ 1, or in either the “faster peer” (t ⬍ 2) or “slower peer” (t ⬍ 1) conditions. Nor was any main effect found for peer performance on the participants’ own performance, t(43) ⫽ 0.74, ns. As instructed, the confederate, depending on experimental condition, “solved” either considerably more anagrams (M ⫽ 27.29; SD ⫽ 8.53) or considerably fewer anagrams (M ⫽ 8.01; SD ⫽ 5.90) than the participant. The participants, moreover, showed themselves to be well aware of these performance differences. Those in the “faster peer” condition rated their peer as significantly better at solving anagrams (M ⫽ 6.07, SD ⫽ 0.94) than did those in the “slower peer” condition (M ⫽ 3.08, SD ⫽ 0.93), t(51) ⫽ 11.73, p ⬍ .001. Finally, participants who witnessed a faster peer rated themselves as significantly worse at solving anagrams (M ⫽ 2.29, SD ⫽ 1.15) than did those who witnessed a slower peer (M ⫽ 3.81, SD ⫽ 1.52), t(46) ⫽ 4.12, p ⬍ .001. Strategies for Statistical Analyses We hypothesized that maximizers’ moods and self-assessments of ability would depend heavily on the quality of their peer’s performance—that is, whether it was inferior or superior to their own. Satisficers’ moods and self-assessments, by contrast, were expected to be less influenced by the performance of their peer. We conducted planned pairwise comparisons of the faster peer and slower peer conditions within the two groups (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1985; see also Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989, 1995). Additionally, we compared maximizers who had witnessed a faster peer with those in the three remaining conditions. Simple change scores reflecting differences between premanipulation and postmanipula- MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING tion ratings provided the primary dependent variables for both types of analyses. Other types of statistical analyses, such as analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) and repeated measures analyses, were also performed and yielded results very similar to those obtained in the analyses reported in this article. For brevity, these results are not discussed. 1189 assessments of the four groups, F(1, 50) ⫽ 11.33, r ⫽ .43, p ⬍ .002, but their mean decline was the only one of the four that was significantly different from zero (M ⫽ ⫺1.71), t(14) ⫽ 5.06, p ⬍ .001. However, differences between the responses of maximizers and satisficers in this condition—at least in terms of changes in their self-assessments of ability— did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance, F(1, 50) ⫽ 2.73, p ⬍ .10. Changes in Self-Assessments of Ability We expected maximizers to offer ratings of their own ability that gave considerable weight to social comparison information. Supporting this prediction, a planned contrast revealed that maximizers gave lower assessments of ability on the anagram task after working alongside a faster peer (M ⫽ ⫺1.71, SD ⫽ 1.27) than after working alongside a slower peer (M ⫽ 0.17, SD ⫽ 1.11), F(1, 50) ⫽ 10.34, p ⬍ .003. Self-assessments of satisficers, by contrast, did not differ significantly between the two social comparison conditions (Ms ⫽ ⫺0.79 vs. 0.14), F(1, 50) ⫽ 2.63. The top panel of Figure 1, as well as the top of Table 3, shows the mean changes in self-assessments of ability, based on 7-point rating scales, for all four groups. As expected, maximizing participants in the “faster peer” condition not only exhibited the largest relative decline in self- Changes in Self-Reported Affect Examination of changes in participants’ self-reported negative mood (NA) produced a similar pattern of results (see the bottom panel of Figure 1 and the top of Table 4). Once again, supporting our predictions, maximizers displayed significantly more elevated NA after witnessing a faster peer (M ⫽ 0.54, SD ⫽ 0.82) than after witnessing a slower peer (M ⫽ ⫺0.03, SD ⫽ 0.50), F(1, 50) ⫽ 6.31, p ⬍ .02. Satisficers, by contrast, showed more similar affect in response to their superior versus inferior peer’s performance (Ms ⫽ 0.12 vs. ⫺0.06), F ⬍ 1. Following the pattern of results for self-assessments, maximizers in the “faster peer” condition not only exhibited the biggest increase in NA of the four groups, F(1, 50) ⫽ 8.70, p ⬍ .005, but, once again, their mean increase was the only one of the four groups Figure 1. Changes in assessments of ability (top) and negative affect (bottom) after working alongside a slower versus faster peer (Study 3). SCHWARTZ ET AL. 1190 Table 3 Maximization Versus Happiness and Dysphoria in Determining Changes in Participants’ Self-Assessments of Ability in Response to a Faster Versus Slower Peer Group Condition Faster peer Slower peer Difference Maximizers Raw means ⫺1.71 ⫺0.17 1.88 Satisficers ⫺0.79 0.14 0.93 Adjusted means (with SHS score as covariate) Faster peer Slower peer Difference ⫺1.98 ⫺0.16 1.82 ⫺0.57 ⫺0.47 1.04 Adjusted means (with BDI score as covariate) Faster peer Slower peer Difference ⫺1.88 0.00 1.88 ⫺0.65 0.32 0.97 Note. SHS ⫽ Subjective Happiness Scale; BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory. that was significantly different from zero (M ⫽ 0.54), t(14) ⫽ 2.43, p ⬍ .03. Furthermore, differences between the responses of maximizers and satisficers in this condition were marginally statistically significant, F(1, 50) ⫽ 3.62, p ⬍ .07. None of the analyses examining group differences in changes in participants’ positive mood (PA) reached conventional levels of statistical significance. Subjective Happiness and Dysphoria as Possible Moderator Variables Readers might question whether the effects reported thus far really reflect the role of a maximization orientation rather than that of chronic happiness or dysphoria. Indeed, similar effects have been reported in an analysis of dispositionally happy and unhappy individuals (Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997, Study 1). Given the moderately high correlations in this study between participants’ scores on the Maximization Scale and their scores on the SHS and the BDI, it was deemed prudent to address this question through covariance analyses. With respect to both happiness (as measured by the SHS) and dysphoria (as measured by the BDI), the results of our analyses were simple and conclusive. When either happiness or dysphoria was introduced as a covariate, neither variable accounted for our between-group differences. That is, both for changes in selfassessment of ability and for changes in NA, ANCOVAs left our “adjusted” means for both ability change (see Table 3) and NA change (see Table 4), as well as the relevant contrasts, virtually unaltered in magnitude. Discussion The results of Study 3 supported our hypothesis that maximizers would be more affected by social comparison information than would satisficers. Maximizers who saw their peer solve anagrams faster than themselves expressed greater doubts about their own ability at the task and displayed a greater increase in negative mood than maximizers who saw their peer solve fewer anagrams. Satisficers, by contrast, showed little or no such response to the social comparison information provided by their peer. Subsequent analyses, moreover, suggested that it was differences in maximization per se, rather than the association of maximization with happiness or dysphoria, that predicted the relevant differences in response. Although the focus of this study was on the ways that students use self-relevant social comparison feedback when evaluating themselves, the relevant social comparison processes are likely to mirror those recruited in decision-making contexts of the sort investigated in Study 2. We suspect that many decisions faced by students—for example, which major to choose, to which graduate school to apply, which job to select—involve self-evaluations, and comparisons with peers can provide feedback about whether one can “cut it” in a particular major, school, or job. We speculate that because satisficers are satisfied with a major, school, or job that is simply “good enough,” they may not require as much information in general—and social comparison information in particular—as do maximizers in order to make decisions. Several issues raised by Study 3 deserve comment. First, given that changes in participants’ PA did not show the expected pattern of results, further research could test the possibility that it is on NA that social comparison has its major influence. Second, because the current study examined differences in maximizers’ and satisficers’ responses to social comparison information and not their interest in or seeking of such information, the latter topics remain worthy of investigation (though the questionnaire responses in Study 2 suggest that maximizers seek more social comparison information than satisficers). And, finally, because Study 3 was conducted in Table 4 Maximization Versus Happiness and Dysphoria in Determining Changes in Participants’ Self-Reported Negative Affect in Response to a Faster Versus Slower Peer Group Condition Maximizers Satisficers Raw means Faster peer Slower peer Difference 0.54 ⫺0.03 0.57 0.12 ⫺0.06 0.18 Adjusted means (with SHS score as covariate) Faster peer Slower peer Difference 0.62 0.06 0.56 0.06 ⫺0.16 0.22 Adjusted means (with BDI score as covariate) Faster peer Slower peer Difference 0.63 0.07 0.56 0.04 ⫺0.17 0.21 Note. SHS ⫽ Subjective Happiness Scale; BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory. MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING the laboratory, the question remains how maximizers and satisficers respond to social comparison feedback during the course of decisions in their everyday life. Taken together, our findings in Studies 2 and 3 provide support for the notion that maximizers are more likely than satisficers to seek out and respond to social comparison information each time they try to make the “perfect choice.” Study 4. Maximizing, Satisficing, and Regret The first three studies have provided evidence of individual differences in the disposition to maximize that correlate with other important variables and are reflected in self-reports about purchasing decisions. Further, there is evidence, both from self-report and experimental data, that maximizers are more inclined to engage in social comparisons and to be more sensitive to their contents than are satisficers. The final study reported here tested whether a disposition to maximize relates to actual decision-making behavior. We created a game that required participants to make decisions, and investigated whether maximizers made different decisions, and experienced different degrees of satisfaction from those decisions, than did satisficers. The second aim of Study 4 was to explore experimentally the relation between maximizing and regret. We reported in Study 1 consistent and substantial correlations between scores on our Maximization Scale and scores on our Regret Scale. We also reported evidence that partially supported the hypothesis that regret mediates the relations between maximization and various measures of well-being. On the basis of this evidence, we suggested that one of the factors that may lead maximizers to experience less happiness and satisfaction with life than satisficers is maximizers’ increased sensitivity to regret— both experienced and anticipated. If that is true, then it should be the case that experimental manipulations designed to enhance the possibility of experiencing regret should have a larger impact on maximizers. The game used in Study 4 was designed to manipulate the potential to experience regret. We used a variant of the ultimatum game (Camerer & Thaler, 1995; Guth, Schmittberger, & Schwarze, 1982). In the ultimatum game, one player has control of a resource (typically a sum of money) and offers some part of that resource to another player. That player may either accept the offer, in which case the resource is divided in keeping with the offer, or reject it, in which case neither player gets anything. This game has been of interest to experimental economists because an analysis of optimal strategy by a rational maximizer of gain would seem to dictate that the proposer make the smallest legal offer, secure in the knowledge that the recipient of that offer will accept it (a little of something is better than nothing). This pattern is virtually never observed among actual participants. First, recipients of offers routinely reject them if they are too low (e.g., less than 30% of the resource). Second, proposers rarely make such low offers. With respect to regret, there is an interesting asymmetry to the ultimatum game. The proposer will always know if he or she has made an offer that is too low, because the recipient of that offer will have rejected it. However, the proposer will not know if the offer was too high because there is no information about the minimum acceptable offer—the reservation price. When the recipient accepts the offer, it could be that the offer was at exactly the price necessary for acceptance or that it was higher than necessary. 1191 Thus, one would expect proposers who are worried about regretting their decisions to make unnecessarily high offers. That way, they will avoid the only source of regret that the situation permits—an offer that is rejected. Suppose, however, that the game were altered so that proposers would be told what the minimum acceptable offer was on trials of the game in which their offers were accepted. Thus, they might offer $5 of a $10 stake, have their offer accepted, and then find out that an offer of $3 also would have been accepted. Under these conditions, it is possible to regret offers that are too high just as it is possible to regret offers that are too low. Zeelenberg and Beattie (1997) found that offers in this modified ultimatum game tended to be lower than offers under the standard procedure. Our question, based on the hypothesis that maximizers are more sensitive to regret than satisficers, was whether the effect observed by Zeelenberg and Beattie would be larger for maximizers than for satisficers. Method Participants The participants were 84 students (48 female and 36 male) enrolled in an introductory psychology course at Swarthmore College who received course credit. Procedure All participants had previously completed a packet of questionnaire materials including the Maximization Scale and the Regret Scale. Approximately 7 weeks later, participants were directed to a Web site for participation in another study. They were given 2 weeks in which to do the tasks on the Web site at a time and place that was convenient to them. About 75% of the participants completed the tasks within the allotted time. The others were sent follow-up reminders by e-mail until all but 7 had complied. No mention was made of the connection between this study and the questionnaire materials they had completed earlier. Each participant played two versions of the ultimatum game, in counterbalanced order: a “standard” version and a modified version (they differed in only one respect, described below). Each version included 10 rounds. In the standard version, participants first encountered a screen that told them that they were “Player 1,” that the computer would be “Player 2,” and that the computer would be making decisions based on the performance of real people playing the identical game. Participants were also told that on each round, they would be given a sum of money (between $8 and $15). They were to make a whole dollar offer to Player 2 (the computer), who would know what amount of money was being divided on each round, and could accept or reject the offer. Participants were further informed that the computer would simulate Player 2’s responses on the basis of past behavior of people who have played this game. Moreover, it was explained, on each round, a different past player would be used for the simulation, so participants were to treat each round as playing with a different Player 2. For each round, if the participant’s offer was accepted, Player 2 (the computer) would “get” the amount offered, while Player 1 (the participant) would get the difference between the total amount and the amount offered. Thus, for example, if a round started with $12 available, and Player 1 made an offer of $5 that was accepted, Player 2 would get the $5, and Player 1 would get $7. If the offer was rejected, neither player would get anything. Participants were also told that there was a chance that they would actually get to keep whatever amount resulted from a given round of the game. At the end of each round, participants were asked to click the mouse along an unmarked line that was anchored on the left with very unsatisfied and on the right with very satisfied to indicate their satisfaction with that round of the game. SCHWARTZ ET AL. 1192 In the modified version, to which all participants also were exposed, at the end of a round, in addition to being told whether their offer was accepted or rejected, they were also told what the smallest offer was that Player 2 would have accepted—Player 2’s “reservation price.” The reservation price of Player 2 was programmed to vary pseudorandomly, with a low of 13% of the initial sum and a high of 57% of the initial sum. The idea behind this manipulation was that in the standard game, players never know that they have offered more than was necessary, and thus will not experience regret over offers that are too high. In this variant of the game, participants would know when they had made offers that were more generous than necessary. Results and Discussion Participants offered their counterpart half of the initial sum of money (rounded to the nearest dollar) on 53.4% of trials, less than half on 37.3% of trials, and more than half on 9.3% of trials. Given the low rates of offers above half, the data were collapsed into offers of less than half (37.3%) and offers of at least half (62.7%). No significant difference in rates of offering less than half was found between males and females (42.2% vs. 33.4% for males vs. females, respectively; t ⬍ 1.5). Maximization score was not correlated with the percentage of offers made below half (r ⫽ .15, n ⫽ 82, ns). However, an ANCOVA (with maximization score as a continuous variable) revealed that there was a significant interaction between gender and maximization score on the number of offers made of less than half, F(1, 82) ⫽ 6.80, p ⬍ .01. The relation between maximization score and offers made was thus analyzed separately for males and females. Among males, participants higher in maximization exhibited a significantly higher percentage of offers below half (r ⫽ ⫺.40, n ⫽ 35, p ⬍ .02). Among female participants, no significant association was found between maximization score and the percentage of offers below half (r ⫽ .14, n ⫽ 47, ns). Within-participant t tests comparing offers made on trials in which the reservation price of opponents was shown versus those in which it was not shown did not indicate any difference in the percentage of offers of less than half (38.4% vs. 36.0%; t ⬍ 1). Thus, we failed to replicate the findings of Zeelenberg and Beattie (1997) for the participants as a group. However, experimental condition did interact with maximization scores in predicting the number of offers made of less than half by each participant. Using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with experimental condition (i.e., whether or not reservation prices were shown) as the repeated factor, there was a significant Condition ⫻ Maximization interaction in the percentage of offers made below half, F(1, 80) ⫽ 8.90, p ⬍ .004. That is to say, satisficers and maximizers tended to adjust their offers differently on the basis of whether reservation prices were shown. To determine the basis of the interaction between experimental condition and maximization score on offers made, participants were divided into satisficers and maximizers on the basis of a median split. Among satisficers, participants exhibited lower rates of offers of less than half on rounds in which reservation prices were seen as compared with those in which reservation prices were not seen (32.2% vs. 39.4%, respectively), t(40) ⫽ 2.42, p ⬍ .02—a surprising result given what Zeelenberg and Beattie (1997) found. Maximizers, however, did the opposite, confirming our expectations. They revealed higher rates of offering less than half on trials in which reservation prices were seen as compared with those in which reservation prices were not seen (48.1% vs. 35.9%, respectively), t(40) ⫽ 3.42, p ⬍ .001. As would be expected, mean ratings of satisfaction were considerably higher on rounds in which the participants’ offer was accepted (M ⫽ 7.02) than on those in which it was rejected (M ⫽ 3.18), t(80) ⫽ 13.40, p ⬍ .001. Controlling for whether offers were accepted, higher maximization scores predicted lower judgments of satisfaction, F(1, 81) ⫽ 7.60, p ⬍ .01. Thus, being a maximizer seemed to mean being less satisfied with the results of an episode, independent of what those results were. Judgments of satisfaction did not differ by condition (5.88 vs. 5.97, for standard vs. modified version, respectively; t ⬍ 1). Further, no interaction was present between condition and maximization scores in predicting judgments of satisfaction (F ⬍ 1). On the basis of a repeated measure ANOVA, there was a trend suggesting an interaction between maximization score and the acceptance of offers as predictors of judgments of satisfaction, F(1, 74) ⫽ 2.60, p ⬍ .11. To explore this trend, the associations between maximization score and ratings of satisfaction were assessed separately for those rounds in which offers were rejected and those in which offers were accepted. No significant correlation was found between maximization score and judgments on those trials in which offers were rejected (r ⫽ ⫺.07, n ⫽ 77, ns), but participants higher in maximization were relatively less satisfied during rounds in which offers were accepted (r ⫽ ⫺.31, n ⫽ 82, p ⬍ .005). This negative correlation was present both on rounds in which reservation prices were shown (r ⫽ ⫺.30, n ⫽ 82, p ⬍ .006), and those in which reservation prices were not shown (r ⫽ ⫺.28, n ⫽ 81, p ⬍ .02). To summarize, the results of Study 4 were consistent with many, but not all, of our predictions. In the ultimatum game, male (but not female) maximizers made smaller offers than male satisficers. Maximizers of both genders offered less when the recipient’s reservation price was going to be revealed (as we expected), but unexpectedly, satisficers offered more when the recipient’s reservation price was going to be revealed. Finally, as hypothesized, maximizers were less satisfied than satisficers with outcomes generally. However, they were not especially dissatisfied in the condition in which reservation price was revealed, as had been expected. The observed interaction on offers made between Maximization scores and gender may reflect the presence of implicit social payoffs present in the task. The presumed incentive to make lower offers when reservation prices are revealed rests on the expectation that finding out that larger gains could have been made will invite regret. To the extent that participants experience the game as a social interaction, however, this may not be the case. Most obviously, motivations of cooperation and fairness may result in the experience of maximal utility with an even split as opposed to one in which the participant gets more than half the money. However, the social motive of competitiveness might result in higher utility for the more financially favorable split. So it is possible that the fact that males, but not females, tended to make lower offers when reservation prices were revealed may reflect a greater display of social motivation toward cooperation and fairness among females and/or greater social motivation toward competitiveness among males. Indeed, it is further possible that, particularly in a situation with little truly at stake, the presence of feedback indicating that the possibility to exploit was present but not taken (the condition MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING with known reservation prices) could make the choice of an equitable split even more rewarding to participants with social motives favoring equity. If so, this might explain the tendency of satisficers to offer more 50 –50 splits in the condition in which reservation prices were revealed than when they were hidden. Finally, the fact that maximizers were not particularly dissatisfied with the condition in which reservation prices were revealed (thus inviting more regret) might have been due to the above reported interaction between condition and maximizing score on offers made. Maximizers tended to make lower offers in the condition in which reservation prices were shown, which may have effectively offset the hypothesized increased tendency of this condition to invite regret. Consistent with this interpretation, maximizers’ lower offers led to obtaining more than half the available money on 20.0% of trials in the condition in which the reservation prices were shown as compared with 12.0% of trials when reservation prices were hidden. Satisficers’ rates for such gains were 10.1% and 13.2%, respectively. General Discussion The present studies provide evidence for individual differences in the orientation to seek to maximize one’s outcomes in choice situations. Study 1 reported data with two new scales, a Maximization Scale and a Regret Scale, designed to measure individual differences in maximization as a goal and in sensitivity to regret. With seven independent samples, we found significant positive correlations between maximization and regret, perfectionism, and depression, and significant negative correlations between maximization and happiness, optimism, satisfaction with life, and selfesteem. We suggested that maximizers may be more concerned with relative position, and thus more inclined to engage in social comparison, than satisficers. We explored this possible relation between maximizing and social comparison in Studies 2 and 3. In Study 2, we found that maximizers were more likely than satisficers to report engaging in social comparison, both in general and in connection with consumer decisions. We also found that maximizers were more regretful and less happy with their consumer decisions than satisficers. In Study 3, we found a tendency for maximizers to be affected by social comparison, this time in an experimental setting in which the opportunities to compare oneself with others had effects on assessments of task ability and on mood for maximizers but not for satisficers. Finally, in Study 4, we found that maximizers were less satisfied than satisficers with their results in an ultimatum bargaining game, and we obtained partial support for the hypothesis that maximizers are more sensitive to regret than satisficers. Wieczorkowska and Burnstein (1999) recently reported evidence for an individual difference variable related to our distinction between maximizing and satisficing. They distinguished between individuals who have “point” search strategies and those who have “interval” strategies in making decisions. For the former group, the set of acceptable options is narrow, whereas for the latter it is broad. The broad “interval” strategy is adaptive when search costs are high or environmental opportunities are scarce. The “point” strategy is adaptive when search costs are low or opportunities are plentiful. Adaptive choosers are those who can adjust their search strategies in keeping with what the environment 1193 makes available. This distinction between point and interval strategies is somewhat similar to the distinction between maximizing and satisficing, but there is at least one important difference. The point strategist differs from the interval strategist in having more stringent standards of acceptability. However, those standards are clear and explicit. The maximizer, in contrast, aspires to the (more amorphous) “best.” Even in an abundant environment (indeed, perhaps, especially in an abundant environment), finding the “best” will always be difficult. Taken together, our studies suggest that although maximizers may in general achieve better objective outcomes than satisficers (as a result of their high standards and exhaustive search and decision procedures), they are likely to experience these outcomes as worse subjectively. In what follows, we explore some of the reasons why this may be so. First, to be a maximizer is to want the best option. That, in turn, requires an exhaustive search of the possibilities. Such a search is hardly possible in any particular domain, and certainly impossible in all domains. What this may mean to a maximizer is that when practical constraints make exhaustive search impossible, there will be anticipated regret about options foregone that might have been better than the chosen option. There may also be experienced regret at the chosen option because the chosen option, though the best of all considered, was not necessarily the best in all respects. That is, other options that may have been inferior overall may have been better than the chosen option on one or more dimensions. Such regret, whether caused by experienced or imagined alternatives, is sure to reduce the satisfaction derived from one’s choice. In this connection, we wonder whether maximizers would be less likely than satisficers to engage in dissonance reduction, for example, in a forced-choice paradigm. Second, the process of adaptation will make virtually every consumption experience less satisfying than one expects it to be (e.g., Brickman & Campbell, 1971; Frederick & Loewenstein, 1999; Kahneman, 1999). What makes the adaptation process even worse is that people tend not to anticipate it and thus mispredict their future feelings about all sorts of experiences (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998; Loewenstein & Schkade, 1999). When the experiences are positive, failure to make allowances for adaptation will make these experiences disappointing, especially to maximizers, because their expectations will be higher than those of satisficers (see below). A particularly relevant example of people’s misprediction of future subjective states was recently reported by Gilbert and Ebert (2002), who conducted a series of studies in which participants made a choice that was either reversible or not. Though they never actually did reverse their choices, participants greatly preferred being able to do so to having their decisions be final. Tellingly, participants who had this decision-reversal option were actually less satisfied with the outcomes of their decisions than those whose decisions were irreversible. Gilbert and Ebert argued that when a decision is final, various psychological processes get recruited (e.g., dissonance reduction, rationalization) that subjectively improve the chosen alternative and denigrate the rejected one. As a result of these processes, people experience enhanced satisfaction with their decision. When people keep the option of decision reversal, however, these psychological processes are not recruited. Though the research has yet to be done, we anticipate that maxi- 1194 SCHWARTZ ET AL. mizers would be much more inclined to desire to keep options open than would satisficers. The effects of adaptation may be worse for maximizers than satisficers for two reasons. In all likelihood, maximizers have higher standards of acceptability than satisficers, so that adaptation is more likely to be disappointing. Also, it seems likely that the decisions of maximizers entail far greater search costs than the decisions of satisficers. If we imagine that these search costs get “amortized” over the period of time in which the consequences of the decision are positive, maximizers have a bigger “debt” to amortize than satisficers. Third, a maximizer is more likely to depend on social comparison than a satisficer. The truth of this claim seems inherent in the logic of the matter. How does a maximizer decide that he or she has attained the best possible outcome? Surely, in part, this decision is shaped by observing the outcomes of others. This logic is buttressed by the evidence from Studies 2 and 3 that indicates that maximizers do in fact engage more in social comparison, and are more affected by it, than satisficers. Fourth, it is plausible that maximizers have higher expectations than satisficers. Given the practical constraints on search and the adaptation processes already mentioned, excessively high expectations are more likely to be met with disappointment. To the extent that subjective well-being is in significant part a function of the relation between expectations and reality, as seems likely, maximizers will often find that relation unsatisfying and unsatisfactory. The foregoing discussion helps explain why being a maximizer may make one less happy, but what about the relation between maximizing and depression? Schwartz (2000), in discussing the choice problem, offered a speculative account of the increase in clinical depression over the course of the last century. Such an increase is surprising, because evidence suggests that having control over what happens is a key to avoiding depression (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Peterson, Maier, & Seligman, 1993; Seligman, 1975), and it appears self-evident that adding options enhances one’s potential control. The data reported in Study 1 seem to support Schwartz’s speculation, at least for maximizers. But why? We believe that if there is a causal link between being a maximizer and depression, there is a key mediating factor—the presence of an overwhelming array of options. Our argument is as follows: the theory of depression based upon the phenomenon of learned helplessness suggests that depression results from a lack of control over significant events, coupled with a particular attributional style for explanations of this lack of control (Abramson et al., 1978). Consider the kinds of attributions people might make when decisions lead to disappointing results. Who is to blame? Is it the decision maker or the world? In a world in which the options are few, it is reasonable to think that people will blame the world for disappointing results. But in a world in which the options are many, people will blame themselves. Thus, we imagine that maximizing (in triggering disappointment) and a proliferation of options (in triggering self-blame for disappointment) will interact to produce internal causal attributions for failure on the part of maximizers. The proliferation of options has two consequences related to this theory of depression. First, it raises people’s standards for determining what counts as a success. From breakfast cereals to automobiles to colleges to careers, it makes sense for people to expect more when the options are plentiful than when they are scarce. Second, failure to meet those standards in a domain containing multiple options encourages one to treat failures as the result of personal shortcomings rather than situational limitations, thus encouraging a causal attribution for failure that we might call “depressogenic.” So, in a world of limited options, a maximizer might be more disappointed than a satisficer with the results of his or her decisions without taking personal responsibility for the disappointing results. But in a world of limitless options, there is simply no excuse for failure. What the above argument suggests is that it is a mistake to equate choice with a sense of control, so that the more one has of the former, the more one has of the latter. The relation between choice and perceived control may, for various reasons already discussed, be nonmonotonic (see Iyengar & Lepper, 2000). It may be that what is often critical about control in preventing or alleviating depression is having a choice, not having many choices. For example, a woman may be depressed because she feels she cannot get out of a bad relationship. Or she may be depressed because she cannot control her own depression from coming and going. The “cure” in cases like these is not an array of choices but a choice. Is maximizing always bad for people’s well-being? This seems highly unlikely. Although relying on a maximizing strategy might produce adverse consequences in some contexts, it is conceivable that in others, maximizing will be an adaptive strategy. For example, an individual who responds to a health threat by searching for information, asking questions, and striving to attain the best treatment available may get better results than someone who simply selects a treatment that is sufficient. Maximizers may engage in more active coping strategies such as planful problem solving and seeking social support, whereas satisficers may cope by accepting the situation and engaging in positive reappraisal. Some of these coping responses may be more adaptive, others may be less adaptive. Thus, it seems that there are real advantages to adopting a maximizing strategy. Presumably, not being satisfied with “good enough” spurs one on to achievements that less ambitious people will not attain, though there is as yet no evidence on this point. Perhaps in the domain of action, greater achievements by maximizers compensate for lower satisfaction with those achievements, whatever they are. But in the domain of consumption, the point of which, after all, is subjective satisfaction, this compensatory feature of maximization is much less clear. Caveats and Questions Throughout this discussion, we have been treating maximizers and satisficers as if there is a clear and distinct line that separates them, measurable by some instrument such as our Maximization Scale. But it is surely more accurate to say that people differ in the extent to which they are maximizers, rather than falling on one or the other side of a maximization line. That said, interesting research questions abound. There must be some variation from one domain of choice to another in the extent to which one maximizes. No one maximizes in all domains. For example, we doubt that anyone searches for the prettiest postage stamp to affix to a federal tax return. We have presented no data on the possible domain specificity of a maximization orientation. Indeed, several of the items on the Maximization Scale were written quite deliberately to MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING be vague as to domain. It is possible that where on the maximizing/ satisficing continuum one falls will be a reflection not of how high one’s standards of acceptability are in general, but of how many different domains of choice are dominated by a maximizing orientation. Research into the possible domain specificity of the maximization orientation, and into whether maximizers and satisficers differ in the standards they apply to decisions in general or in the number of domains in which they apply maximizing standards is in order. And beyond the matter of standards, it is important to note whether maximizers and satisficers differ in their scaling of the objective results of their decisions. It would be interesting to know whether maximizers and satisficers respond differently to measures of “objective happiness” recently pioneered by Kahneman (1999). A second issue to be investigated is whether social comparison is not the only kind of comparison to which maximizers are more sensitive than satisficers. Michalos (1980, 1986), in his “multiple discrepancies theory,” suggests that in assessing well-being, people evaluate not only what they have in relation to others, but also what they have in relation to what they expected to have, what they have had in the past, what they expect to have in the future, what they need, and what they deserve. Each of these assessments is a possible source of systematic differences between satisficers and maximizers, with maximizers consistently experiencing larger “gaps” between hopes and expectations on the one hand, and reality on the other, than satisficers do. Another possibility worth exploring is whether maximizers and satisficers differ in what they perceive to be at stake when they make decisions. For the satisficer, all that may be at stake is the actual result of the decision—the quality of the good or the experience that is chosen. For the maximizer, the results of choices may, in addition, convey information about the self. That is, maximizers may take the outcomes of decisions as evidence about how smart, shrewd, or discerning they are as choosers. Each choice a maximizer makes may say something important about the maximizer as a person. If it is true that maximizers have so much riding on the outcomes of decisions, and if it is true, on the basis of arguments made above, that the outcomes of decisions will usually be disappointing to maximizers, it becomes unsurprising that maximizers are more depressed, more unhappy, and less optimistic than satisficers. Related to this possibility is another. If maximizers care more than satisficers about choices and their outcomes in general, they may be vulnerable not only to choices presented by the world, but to choices that they conjure up themselves. For example, the maximizing university student might imagine that there must be some way to combine a double major in finance and biology (to keep both Master of Business Administration and medical school futures open) with a minor in art, while spending a semester in Thailand and a semester in Mexico, even though all university rules indicate that this is impossible. The satisficer is less likely to be plagued by opportunities that exist only in one’s imagination. Further, future research should examine process differences between maximizers and satisficers when it comes to actual choice behavior. That is, when actually making a choice, do maximizers examine more options before selecting? Do they seek more information about alternatives? Do they desire more opportunities to reverse decisions? Do they engage in more postdecision counterfactual thinking and experience more postdecision regret? Are they 1195 more adversely affected by multiple as opposed to few choice options? The present article presents what we think are powerful data on the relation between maximization and subjective experience. It remains to be determined whether maximizers also consistently act differently than satisficers. Finally, we should note that in discussing the relation between maximizing and dispositional happiness, we have been assuming throughout the article that the causal arrow runs from maximizing to unhappiness. Although this direction seems plausible to us, we must acknowledge that alternative conceptualizations are possible. For example, people who are dispositionally unhappy are likely to be disappointed with the outcomes of many of their choices and decisions. This disappointment may be (mis)attributed to the decisions themselves, rather than their own fundamental unhappiness, leading such individuals continually to strive to make “better” choices and judgments, in an ultimately fruitless effort to enhance their happiness. Of course, such a process could also produce a cyclical relationship, whereby unhappy individuals attempt to maximize (in a misguided effort to raise their affect), leading to more unhappiness. It is critical for future research to clarify whether being maximizers makes people unhappy or being unhappy makes people maximizers. At this point, however, we simply acknowledge that just as happiness may be a matter of choice (i.e., how—and even whether—we make choices influences whether we are happy or not), choice may also be a matter of happiness. References Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49 –74. Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1991). Social comparison and coping with serious medical problems. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 369 –393). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1993). Effects of social comparison direction, threat, and self-esteem on affect, self-evaluation, and expected success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 708 –722. Baron, J. (2000). Thinking and deciding (3rd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Bazerman, M. H., Loewenstein, G. F., & White, S. B. (1992). Reversals of preference in allocation decisions: Judging an alternative versus choosing among alternatives. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 220 –240. Bazerman, M. H., Moore, D. A., Tenbrunsel, A. E., Wade-Benzoni, K. A., & Blount, S. (1999). Explaining how preferences change across joint versus separate evaluation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 39, 41–58. Beattie, J., Baron, J., Hershey, J. C., & Spranca, M. D. (1994). Psychological determinants of decision attitude. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 129 –144. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row. Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in a family practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81– 85. Bell, D. E. (1982). Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 30, 961–981. 1196 SCHWARTZ ET AL. Bell, D. E. (1985). Putting a premium on regret. Management Science, 31, 117–120. Blount, S., & Bazerman, M. H. (1996). The inconsistent evaluation of absolute versus comparative payoffs in labor supply and bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 30, 227–240. Brewer, M. B., & Weber, J. G. (1994). Self-evaluation effects of personal versus intergroup social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 268 –275. Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New York: Academic Press. Brown, J. D., Novick, N. J., Lord, K. A., & Richards, J. M. (1992). When Gulliver travels: Social context, psychological closeness, and selfappraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 717–727. Buunk, B. P., Collins, R. L., van Yperen, N. W., Taylor, W. E., & Dakof, G. A. (1990). The affective consequences of social comparison: Either direction has its ups and downs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1238 –1249. Camerer, C., & Thaler, R. H. (1995). Ultimatums, dictators, and manners. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 2, 209 –219. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542– 575. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 114 –140. Frank, R. H. (1985). Choosing the right pond. New York: Oxford University Press. Frank, R. H. (1999). Luxury fever. New York: Free Press. Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale to measure social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 129 –142. Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward social comparison on mood states. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 8, 14 –31. Gilbert, D. T., & Ebert, E. J. (2002). Decisions and revisions: The affective forecasting of changeable outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 503–514. Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617– 638. Gillham, J., Ward, A., & Schwartz, B. (2001). [Maximizing and depressed mood in college students and young adolescents.] Unpublished raw data. Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379 –395. Guth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3, 367–388. Hemphill, K. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1991). Social comparisons and their affective consequences: The importance of comparison dimension and individual difference variables. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 372–394. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Perfectionism and depression: A multidimensional analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 423– 438. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456 – 470. Hirsch, F. (1976). Social limits to growth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hsee, C. K., Blount, S., Loewenstein, G. F., & Bazerman, M. H. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 576 – 590. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349 –366. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 995–1006. John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The “Big Five” Inventory—Versions 4a and 54. Technical report (July), Institute of Personality Assessment and Research, Berkeley, CA. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263–291. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350. Larrick, R. P., & Boles, T. L. (1995). Avoiding regret in decisions with feedback: A negotiation example. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, 87–97. Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 91–103. Loewenstein, G., & Schkade, D. (1999). Wouldn’t it be nice? Predicting future feelings. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 85–108). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. Economic Journal, 92, 805– 824. Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137–155. Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1997). Hedonic consequences of social comparison: A contrast of happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1141–1157. Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., & Kasri, F. (2001). Responses to hedonically-conflicting social comparisons: Comparing happy and unhappy people. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 1–25. MacKinnon, D. P., & Dwyer, J. H. (1993). Estimating mediated effects in prevention studies. Evaluation Review, 17, 144 –158. Martin, L. L., Tesser, A., & McIntosh, W. D. (1993). Wanting but not having: The effects of unattained goals on thoughts and feelings. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 552–572). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Michalos, A. C. (1980). Satisfaction and happiness. Social Indicators Research, 8, 385– 422. Michalos, A. C. (1986). Job satisfaction, marital satisfaction, and the quality of life: A review and a preview. In F. M. Andrews (Ed.), Research on the quality of life (pp. 57– 83). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency, and the concept of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 148 –156. Payne, J. W. (1982). Contingent decision behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 382– 402. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press. Peterson, C., Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). Learned helplessness: A theory for the age of personal control. New York: Oxford University Press. MAXIMIZING VS. SATISFICING AND WELL-BEING Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374. Preacher, K. J., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2001). Calculation for the Sobel test. Retrieved April 23, 2002, from http://quantrm2.psy.ohio-state.edu/kris/ sobel/sobel.htm Ritov, I. (1996). Probability of regret: Anticipation of uncertainty resolution in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66, 228 –236. Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133–148. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1985). Contrast analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Rosnow, R., & Rosenthal, R. (1989). Definition and interpretation of interaction effects. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 143–146. Rosnow, R., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). “Some things you learn aren’t so”: Cohen’s paradox, Asch’s paradigm, and the interpretation of interaction. Psychological Science, 6, 3–9. Ross, L, Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in selfperception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880 – 892. Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of social-comparison jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 780 –792. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectations. Health Psychology, 4, 219 –247. Schwartz, B. (1986). The battle for human nature: Science, morality, and modern life. New York: Norton. Schwartz, B. (1994). The costs of living: How market freedom erodes the best things in life. New York: Norton. Schwartz, B. (2000). Self determination: The tyranny of freedom. American Psychologist, 55, 79 – 88. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman. Simenson, I. (1992). The influence of anticipating regret and responsibility on purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 105–118. Simenson, I., & Tversky, A. (1992). Choice in context: Tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 29, 281–295. Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 59, 99 –118. Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63, 129 –138. Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man, social and rational: Mathematical essays on rational human behavior. New York: Wiley. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982 (pp. 290 –312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Solnick, S. J., & Hemenway, D. (1998). Is more always better?: A survey on positional concerns. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 37, 373–383. 1197 Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events. American Psychologist, 38, 1161–1173. Taylor, S. E., Buunk, B. P., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1990). Social comparison, stress, and coping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 74 – 89. Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–222). New York: Academic Press. Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 76, 31– 48. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453– 458. Tversky, A., & Shafir, E. (1992). Choice under conflict: The dynamics of deferred decision. Psychological Science, 3, 358 –361. von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. White, K., Lehman, D. R., & Schwartz, B. (2002). [Rumination tendencies among maximizers and satisficers]. Unpublished raw data. Wieczorkowska, G., & Burnstein, E. (1999). Adapting to the transition from socialism to capitalism in Poland: The role of screening strategies in social change. Psychological Science, 10, 98 –105. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245–271. Wills, T. A. (1991). Similarity and self-esteem in downward comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 51–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wood, J. V., & VanderZee, K. (1997). Social comparisons among cancer patients: Under what conditions are comparisons upward and downward? In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping, and well-being (pp. 299 –328). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 93–106. Zeelenberg, M., & Beattie, J. (1997). Consequences of regret aversion 2: Additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 63–78. Zeelenberg, M., Beattie, J., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. K. (1996). Consequences of regret aversion: Effects of expected feedback on risky decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 148 –158. Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., van der Pligt, J., Manstead, A. S. R., van Empelen, P., & Reinderman, D. (1998). Emotional reactions to the outcomes of decisions: The role of counterfactual thought in the experience of regret. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75, 117–141. Received May 23, 2001 Revision received May 1, 2002 Accepted May 2, 2002 䡲 Journal of Happiness Studies (2005) 6:25–41 DOI 10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z Springer 2005 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON, NANSOOK PARK and MARTIN E.P. SELIGMAN ORIENTATIONS TO HAPPINESS AND LIFE SATISFACTION: THE FULL LIFE VERSUS THE EMPTY LIFE ABSTRACT. Different orientations to happiness and their association with life satisfaction were investigated with 845 adults responding to Internet surveys. We measured life satisfaction and the endorsement of three different ways to be happy: through pleasure, through engagement, and through meaning. Each of these three orientations individually predicted life satisfaction. People simultaneously low on all three orientations reported especially low life satisfaction. These findings point the way toward a distinction between the full life and the empty life. KEY WORDS: empty life, eudemonia, flow, full life, hedonism, life satisfaction, meaning INTRODUCTION Philosophers and psychologists have long been concerned with the good life and how it can be achieved (Guignon, 1999; Russell, 1930, 1945). Often they propose a sovereign principle to be followed in order to be happy. So, the doctrine of hedonism – maximizing pleasure and minimizing pain – was articulated thousands of years ago by Aristuppus (435–366 BCE) who championed immediate sensory gratification (Watson, 1895). Hedonism was elaborated by Epicurus (342–270 BCE) into the edict of ethical hedonism, which holds that our fundamental moral obligation is to maximize our experience of pleasure. Early Christian philosophers denounced hedonism as inconsistent with the goal of avoiding sin, but Renaissance philosophers such as Erasmus (1466–1536) and Thomas Moore (1478–1535) argued that it was God’s wish that people be happy, so long as they did not become preoccupied with ‘‘artificial’’ ways of achieving pleasure. Later British philosophers like David Hume 26 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. (1711–1776) and Jeremy Bentham (1748–1832) used the doctrine of hedonism to lay the foundation for utilitarianism, which was ushered into psychology as the underpinning of psychoanalysis and all but the most radical of the behaviorisms. Hedonism is alive and well today in the name of a new field – hedonic psychology (Kahneman et al., 1999). At least in the modern Western world, the pursuit of pleasure is widely endorsed as a way to achieve satisfaction: ‘‘Don’t worry – be happy.’’ Standing in contrast to hedonism is another venerable tradition that can be traced to Aristotle’s (384–322 BCE) notion of eudemonia – being true to one’s inner self (demon). According to this view, true happiness entails identifying one’s virtues, cultivating them, and living in accordance with them (Aristotle, 2000). Aristotle considered sensual pleasure as touted by the hedonists to be vulgar. Similar positions were advanced by John Stuart Mill (1806–1873) and Bertrand Russell (1872–1970) and undergird more modern psychological notions such as Rogers’ (1951) ideal of the fully-functioning person, Maslow’s (1970) concept of selfactualization, Ryff and Singer’s (1996) vision of psychological well-being, and Deci and Ryan’s (2000) self-determination theory. Uniting eudemonic emphases is the premise that people should develop what is best within themselves and then use these skills and talents in the service of greater goods – including in particular the welfare of other people or humankind writ large. Again, in the modern world, the pursuit of a meaningful life is widely endorsed as a way to achieve satisfaction: ‘‘Be all that you can be,’’ and ‘‘Make a difference.’’ As implied, different psychological traditions have respectively addressed these two principles of achieving satisfaction. Often these traditions have proceeded independently from one another, with confusion introduced by the tendency of those working within each tradition to claim ‘‘happiness’’ as a label for their subject matter and to deny – if only implicitly – its use by those in the other camp1. Sometimes the debate becomes explicit, and we see investigators playing off the merits of pleasure and meaning as routes to the psychological good life (e.g., Compton et al., 1996; Waterman, 1993). Research suggests that both points of view can be supported by data (Ryan and Deci, 2000). FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE 27 We extend this line of work by simultaneously examining the pursuit of pleasure and the pursuit of meaning as different routes to happiness. The unique contribution of our research is to consider a third orientation to happiness: the pursuit of engagement (Seligman, 2002). Here we have been influenced by Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) writings on flow: the psychological state that accompanies highly engaging activities. Time passes quickly. Attention is focused on the activity. The sense of self is lost. The aftermath of the flow experience is invigorating. In his studies of eudemonia, Waterman (1993) initially equated the flow state with eudemonia (which he termed personal expressiveness) but then concluded from his data that flow represented an ‘‘amalgam’’ of hedonic and eudemonic features. We suggest instead that flow is distinct. Flow is not the same as sensual pleasure. Indeed, flow is nonemotional and arguably nonconscious. People may describe flow as enjoyable, but this is an after-the fact summary judgment; ‘‘joy’’ is not immediately present during the activity itself. So, flow differs from hedonism, in which positive emotional experience is front-and-center (Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). At least at any given point in time, flow and pleasure may even be incompatible. Although the pursuit of a meaningful life can at times produce flow for some individuals – e.g., those volunteering in a hospice or a soup kitchen – not all flow-producing activities are meaningful in the sense of connecting an individual to a greater good (consider playing bridge or Scrabble), and not all meaningful activities entail the total absorption that defines flow. For example, each of us does committee work at our university. At its best, this work is meaningful, but we have yet to lose ourselves in its performance. We report here an empirical investigation of these three ways of being happy. The following questions guided our inquiry: • Are these three orientations to happiness empirically distinguishable individual differences; • Is an orientation to pleasure incompatible with an orientation to engagement, as implied by the characterization of the flow state as noncognitive and nonemotional, or is it possible for the same person to pursue these different ways of being happy; 28 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. • Do these three orientations each contribute to life satisfaction, or are some more important than others; and • Are there interactions between or among these orientations with respect to life satisfaction; that is, does their joint presence predict more life satisfaction than expected from the individual components, and, conversely, does their joint absence predict less than expected life satisfaction? METHOD Participants Research participants were two groups of adult volunteers who completed measures on-line. During initial instrument development, 180 respondents participated, and for the main study, 845 respondents participated. Demographic characteristics of these two samples are summarized in Table I. Measures Orientations to Happiness To develop the Orientations to Happiness measure used in this study, 12 face-valid items reflecting each of the three orientations were initially drafted by the authors and refined in a focus group of college students (n ¼ 15) enrolled in a positive psychology class at the University of Pennsylvania. Each item required a respondent to answer on a 5-point scale the degree to which the item applied (‘‘1 ¼ very much unlike me’’ through ‘‘5 ¼ very much like me’’). Items tapping pleasure and meaning resemble those used in previous research contrasting hedonic versus eudemonic orientations (cf. King and Napa, 1998; McGregor and Little, 1998). Items measuring engagement were based on Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) characterization of the flow state as self-less absorption in ongoing activity. Along with demographic questions, the initial 36-item Orientations to Happiness measure was placed on the Internet and completed by participants in the instrument development sample (n ¼ 180). Internal consistencies of the three subscales formed by averaging the respective items, were satisfactory (pleasure a ¼ 0.84, flow a ¼ 0.77, and meaning a ¼ 0.88) and exceeded the 29 FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE TABLE I Demographic characteristics of samples Instrument development sample (n = 180) (%) Main sample (n = 845) (%) Age (years) 18–20 21–24 25–34 35–44 45–54 55–64 65+ 27 15 18 17 16 6 1 15 10 19 21 24 8 3 Gender Male Female 38 62 28 72 Education <High school High school graduate Some college Associates degree Baccalaureate >Baccalaureate 1 5 44 5 17 28 1 9 31 6 24 29 Marital status Married/living as Single Widowed Divorced 31 58 1 10 45 39 1 15 Ethnicity African American Asian American Latino/a White Other 4 3 3 82 8 3 3 2 86 6 U.S. citizen 76 85 Town of residence Farm Country Suburban Small city (<50 K) Medium city (<100 K) 1 6 19 16 15 1 12 26 17 15 30 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. Table I. (Continued) Large city (<500 K) Very large city (>500 K) Political leaning (1 = liberal, 7 = conservative) Instrument development sample (n = 180) (%) Main sample (n = 845) (%) 16 27 16 13 M = 3.55 (SD = 1.85) M = 3.37 (SD = 1.84) subscale intercorrelations, which nonetheless were of moderate magnitude (mean r ¼ 0.51). These results suggested that the three orientations to happiness are distinguishable but related. To sharpen the distinctions among the subscales, the six items in each subscale with the highest item-total correlations were chosen, and a revised 18-item measure was created and placed online along with demographic questions to be completed by participants in the main study (n ¼ 845). The revised version of this scale is presented in Table II. Subscale means were calculated by averaging the relevant items. Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) (Diener et al., 1985) The SWLS consists of five items which measure the individual’s evaluation of satisfaction with life in general (e.g., ‘‘I am satisfied with my life,’’ and ‘‘If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing’’). Respondents select one of seven options (ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to ‘‘strongly agree’’) for each question. Responses were averaged to provide a total life satisfaction score. Research has established acceptable psychometric properties for the SWLS (Diener, 1994). In the current study, the SWLS was skewed toward the right, meaning that most respondents were relatively happy (cf. Diener and Diener, 1996; Myers and Diener, 1995). Procedure All measures were placed online at www.positivepsychology.org/ strengths along with demographic questions (age; gender; 31 FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE TABLE II Orientations to happiness subscale items and factor loadings (n = 845) Eigenvalue % of variance Life of meaning 02. My life serves a higher purpose. 05. In choosing what to do, I always take into account whether it will benefit other people. 11. I have a responsibility to make the world a better place. 12. My life has a lasting meaning. 14. What I do matters to society. 17. I have spent a lot of time thinking about what life means and how I fit into its big picture. Life of pleasure 03. Life is too short to postpone the pleasures it can provide. 08. I go out of my way to feel euphoric. 13. In choosing what to do, I always take into account whether it will be pleasurable. 15. I agree with this statement: ‘‘Life is short – eat dessert first.’’ 16. I love to do things that excite my senses. 18. For me, the good life is the pleasurable life. Life of engagement 01. Regardless of what I am doing, time passes very quickly. 04. I seek out situations that challenge my skills and abilities. 06. Whether at work or play, I am usually ‘‘in a zone’’ and not conscious of myself. Factor 1 4.96 28 Factor 2 2.80 16 Factor 3 1.60 9 0.75 0.01 0.08 0.54 0.01 0.32 0.79 )0.01 0.05 0.82 0.73 0.57 0.01 0.01 0.11 0.17 0.11 0.01 0.22 0.68 0.12 0.12 0.60 0.36 0.00 0.75 0.01 )0.01 0.71 )0.01 0.16 0.74 0.01 )0.01 0.79 0.00 0.23 0.00 0.48 0.40 0.12 0.51 0.00 0.01 0.75 32 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. Table II. (Continued) Eigenvalue % of variance 07. I am always very absorbed in what I do. 09. In choosing what to do, I always take into account whether I can lose myself in it. 10. I am rarely distracted by what is going on around me. Factor 1 4.96 28 Factor 2 2.80 16 Factor 3 1.60 9 0.13 0.01 0.78 0.16 0.41 0.49 0.12 0.00 0.61 Note: Numbers in front of items correspond to order in the final Orientations to Happiness measure. Entries in bold represent the factor on which item loaded most highly. education; marital status; ethnicity; U.S. citizenship; size of one’s town of residence, from 1 ¼ farm through 7 ¼ very large city; and liberal-versus-conservative political leaning assessed by a single 7point scale, from 1 ¼ liberal to 7 ¼ conservative). On the first page of the web site, a short description of the study including its purpose and its approximate time commitment was provided. This web site also includes other psychological measures and provides individualized feedback about an individual’s top scores on these other measures upon completion of all measures (feedback on the Orientations to Happiness measure was not provided). The feedback feature is apparently attractive to potential respondents and may explain why we did not need to advertise the survey. To preserve respondent anonymity, we did not track how individuals came across our survey on the Internet. They may have learned about it by following a link on the Positive Psychology Webpage, by following links on other webpages, or by hearing about it from previous respondents or from our media interviews. According to a recent study of Internet users by the UCLA Center for Communication Policy (Lebo, 2003), more than 70% of Americans use the Internet. Although certain limitations exist, Internet surveys provide researchers with the opportunities to recruit efficiently large and diverse samples at relatively little cost (Birnbaum, 2000, 2004; Kraut et al., 2004; Schonlau et al., 2002). 33 FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE RESULTS For the main sample (n ¼ 845), a principal components analysis of the 18 items in the revised Orientations to Happiness measure, using varimax rotation, provided strong support for the a priori assumption that it assessed three different orientations to the good life. Three factors were identified with eigen-values greater than 1.00, and each of the 18 items loaded most strongly on a factor along with the other items intended to assess the same orientation (Table II). Consistent with this analysis, internal consistencies of the three subscales were satisfactory and exceeded the scale intercorrelations (Table III). Subscale means, created by averaging the appropriate items, could range from 1 to 5. Each had a slight skew. Nevertheless, each subscale yielded a range of scores. There were no significant demographic variations in life satisfaction other than being married (Table III). However, several TABLE III Intercorrelations among measures (n = 845) Pleasure Engagement Meaning Life satisfaction Pleasure Engagement Meaning Life satisfaction Age Gender (1 = male, 2 = female) Politics (1 = conservative, 7 = liberal) Education Married (0 = no, 1 = yes) Home town size White (0 = no, 1 = yes) U.S. citizen (0 = no, 1 = yes) 0.31* 0.18* 0.17* )0.22* 0.09 )0.02 0.46* 0.30* 0.07 )0.06 )0.08 0.26* 0.00 )0.03 )0.10 – )0.09 0.06 )0.05 )0.19* )0.17* 0.07 )0.07 )0.03 0.05 0.01 0.05 )0.11 )0.10 0.08 )0.03 0.05 )0.18* )0.08 )0.01 0.17* )0.05 )0.06 0.01 M SD a Skew 3.20 0.84 0.82 )0.09 3.42 0.88 0.82 )0.32 4.93 1.37 0.90 )0.73 * p < 0.001. – – 3.05 0.72 0.72 0.08 – 34 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. modest demographic correlates of the Orientations to Happiness subscales were found. Those who were younger, less educated, or unmarried were somewhat higher in their endorsement of an orientation to pleasure – as Aristotle might have predicted. Ethnic minorities, chiefly African Americans and Asian Americans, scored somewhat higher than Whites on orientation to meaning. Considered individually, each of the orientations to happiness predicted life satisfaction, from small (pleasure) to moderate (engagement, meaning) degrees (Table III). A hierarchical multiple regression predicting life satisfaction was then computed TABLE IV Hierarchical multiple regression predicting life satisfaction (n = 845) b Step 1 Age Education Gender (1 = male, 2 = female) Politics (1 = conservative, 7 = liberal) Home town size U.S. citizen (0 = no, 1 = yes) Married (0 = no, 1 = yes) White (0 = no, 1 = yes) Step 2 Pleasure Engagement Meaning Step 3 Pleasure · engagement Pleasure · meaning Meaning · engagement Step 4 Pleasure · engagement · meaning )0.17* 0.01 0.08** )0.05 )0.06 0.02 0.24* )0.01 R2 = 0.066** )0.11** )0.24* )0.17* DR2 = 0.116* )0.04 )0.03 0.00 DR2 = 0.004 )2.50** DR2 = 0.006** Note: Predictors in steps 1 and 2 were normalized (see text). b weights are for the final model. * p < 0.001; ** p < 0.05. FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE 35 (Table IV), entering in the first step the demographic variables (age, education, gender [1 ¼ male, 2 ¼ female], marital status [married ¼ 1 or not ¼ 0], politics [1 ¼ conservative, 7 ¼ liberal], US citizenship [yes ¼ 1 or no ¼ 1], size of home town [1 ¼ farm, 7 ¼ very large city], and ethnicity [White ¼ 1 versus not ¼ 0]), in the second step by the subscales measuring the three orientations to happiness, in the third step the two-way product terms between the orientations (i.e., all possible pairs of three orientations), and in the last step the three-way product (pleasure · engagement · meaning). Following the guidelines of Aiken and West (1991), we centered the predictors in the first two blocks by normalizing them and computed interactions by multiplying the relevant normalized scores, but we did not center these product terms or the criterion. Multicollinearity diagnostics were well within acceptable limits. The overall regression was significant (R2 ¼ 0.19, F [15,829] ¼13.15, p < 0.001). Above and beyond the influence of demographic variables, the ‘‘main’’ effects of the three orientations (all of which were significant predictors in the final model), and the two-way interactions (none of which was statistically significant), the three-way interaction – entered in the last step – was statistically significant, although small in its absolute effect. We therefore limited our attention to the three-way interaction. To interpret it, we grouped the respondents in various ways on each of the three subscales (e.g., low versus high, low versus medium versus high, quartiles, quintiles, deciles, and so on) and graphed life satisfaction scores as a joint function of these groupings. No matter how we grouped the respondents, the same patterns emerged: (a) somewhat higher life satisfaction scores for respondents simultaneously near the top of all three Orientations to Happiness subscales; and (b) notably lower life satisfaction scores for respondents simultaneously near the bottom of all three subscales. Figure 1 is a representative composite, where respondents simultaneously low on all three orientations (who have what might be called the ‘‘Empty Life’’) reported the least life satisfaction, whereas those simultaneously high on all three orientations (who have what can be identified as the ‘‘Full Life’’) reported the greatest life satisfaction. 36 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. Figure 1. Mean life satisfaction scores along the continuum of the empty life versus the full life. Groups were created by identifying respondents simultaneously in the top 90% of each of the three subscales, simultaneously in the top 80% of each, and so on. DISCUSSION Drawing on past theory and research, we distinguished three possible orientations to happiness (Seligman, 2002). The present study found that these orientations are distinguishable, that they are not incompatible and thus able to be pursued simultaneously, and that each is individually associated with life satisfaction. As previous research has shown, either hedonism or eudemonia can accompany a satisfying life, and so too can engagement. Our interest was in people’s endorsement of these orientations to happiness, and we did not ascertain whether those who said they believe in pursuing pleasure actually have more sensually gratifying experiences than those who do not, whether those who supported engagement as an orientation to happiness more frequently lose themselves in highly absorbing activities, or whether those who agreed with items reflecting a life of meaning are more likely to perform service to others. We intend to pursue such questions in further research, using a version of Waterman’s FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE 37 (1993) procedure that asks respondents to report on ‘‘activities of importance’’ and their features. We also intend to study these orientations and their relationship to life satisfaction over time. We assume that given orientations shape conduct and thereby produce more or less happiness, but the cross-sectional design of the present study does not allow this notion to be tested. In particular, we need to investigate the alternative interpretation of our data that a satisfying life leads to a diverse behavioral repertoire that includes pleasurable, engaging, and meaningful activities (cf. Fredrickson, 2001). Although only trends in the present sample, we found it interesting that the respondents with the fullest life were more likely than those in other groups to be older, to be married, and to be more highly educated – all factors that arguably open doors to diverse and satisfying experiences. The present research nonetheless extends theory in several ways, suggesting in particular that an orientation to engagement differs from orientations to pleasure or to meaning. Also, an orientation to pleasure is not as strong an individual predictor of life satisfaction as orientations to engagement or to meaning. But neither is pleasure irrelevant to life satisfaction, because it represents value added to a life rich in engagement and meaning and value subtracted from a life deficient in these respects. The Full Life as we have defined it predicts life satisfaction somewhat beyond the sum of its parts, and the Empty Life predicts notably less. Many psychologists who study pleasure seem unconvinced that it can be increased, positing a genetically influenced set point of affectivity to which most of us return following hedonically laden experiences, either good or bad (Brickman and Campbell, 1971; Kahneman, 1999). Perhaps the immutability of our ability to experience pleasure explains why its pursuit can be futile (Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). In contrast, a life of engagement seems more under deliberate control (Massimini and Delle Fave, 2000), as does a life of meaning (Frankl, 1963). As positive psychology turns its attention to interventions that cultivate the good life, perhaps interventions that target engagement and meaning will prove most fruitful (Linley and Joseph, in press). However, we would not want to rule out all attempts to increase pleasure, and perhaps the way to boost pleasure is to follow one implication 38 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. of the present results that pleasure-enhancing techniques like savoring be incorporated into those that increase engagement and meaning (Linville and Fischer, 1991). Perhaps increased pleasure can occur as a happy by-product of such interventions. Besides our cross-sectional design, let us note an additional limitation of our research, specifically our strategy of obtaining research participants. Although increasingly common in psychological research, samples obtained from the World Wide Web are often criticized because of the special characteristics of respondents. Individuals need to have access to a computer and the ability to use it. They need to be interested enough to spend time answering questions. But contrast our web sample with those obtained from typical psychology subject pools and ask which provides a sounder basis for generalization. Our respondents ranged across the adult years and different levels of formal education. Men and women were represented. Many were married. They lived in all parts of the United States or came from many different countries. Our main sample included more than 100 (14%) non-White respondents. Respondents fell at all points along the conservative-liberal political spectrum. Without belaboring the point, we observe that Internet samples are at least as diverse as those recruited from psychology subject pool samples at colleges or universities (Birnbaum, 2000, 2004; Kraut et al., 2004; Lebo, 2003; Lenhart, 2000; Schonlau et al., 2002). Many of the individuals who simultaneously scored low on all three orientations were likely depressed, anxious, or otherwise distressed. These are the people that clinical psychology has studied for 50 years, grouping everyone else together as ‘‘normal’’ (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). We suggest that a distinction can be made along the continuum from the mere normal to the supernormal. Investigating those people who have particularly high life satisfaction may well reveal ways of improving the well-being of all of us (Diener and Seligman, 2002). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We acknowledge the support of the Manuel D. and Rhoda Mayerson Foundation in creating the Values in Action Institute, FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE 39 a nonprofit organization dedicated to the development of a scientific knowledge base of human strengths. Address correspondence to Christopher Peterson, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 525 East University, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109; chrispet@umich.edu. NOTES 1 In the present paper, we use ‘‘happiness’’ in its broadest sense to include hedonic features but also fulfillment and contentment (cf. Myers, 1992; Seligman, 2002). We follow Diener’s (1984) lead in defining ‘‘life satisfaction’’ as a summary appraisal of the quality of one’s life regardless of how it is achieved (cf. Pavot and Diener, 1993). REFERENCES Aiken, L.S. and S.G. West: 1991, Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA). Aristotle: 2000, Nicomachean Ethics (R. Crisp, Trans.) (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England). Birnbaum, M.H. (ed.): 2000, Psychological Experiments on the Internet (Academic Press, San Diego). Birnbaum, M.H.: 2004, ‘Human research and data collection via the Internet’, Annual Review of Psychology 55, pp. 803–832. Brickman, P. and D.T. Campbell: 1971, ‘Hedonic relativism and planning the good society’, in M.H. Appley (ed.), Adaptation-Level Theory (Academic Press, New York), pp. 287–305. Compton, W.C., M.L. Smith, K.A. Cornish and D.L. Qualls: 1996, ‘Factor structure of mental health measures’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71, pp. 406–413. Csikszentmihalyi, M.: 1990, Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience (HarperCollins, New York). Csikszentmihalyi, M.: 1999, ‘If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy?’, American Psychologist 54, pp. 821–827. Deci, E.L. and R.M. Ryan: 2000, ‘The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior’, Psychological Inquiry 11, pp. 227–268. Diener, E.: 1984, ‘Subjective well-being’, Psychological Bulletin 95, pp. 542– 575. Diener, E.: 1994, ‘Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities’, Social Indicators Research 31, pp. 103–157. Diener, E. and C. Diener: 1996, ‘Most people are happy’, Psychological Science 7, pp. 181–185. 40 CHRISTOPHER PETERSON ET AL. Diener, E. and M.E.P. Seligman: 2002, ‘Very happy people’, Psychological Science 13, pp. 80–83. Diener, E., R.A. Emmons, R.J. Larsen and S. Griffin: 1985, ‘The satisfaction with life scale’, Journal of Personality Assessment 49, pp. 71–75. Frankl, V.E.: 1963, Man’s Search for Meaning: An Introduction to Logotherapy (Washington Square Press, New York). Fredrickson, B.L.: 2001, ‘The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions’, American Psychologist 56, pp. 218–226. Guignon, C. (ed.): 1999, The Good Life (Hackett, Indianapolis, IN). Kahneman, D.: 1999, ‘Objective happiness’, in D. Kahneman, E. Diener and N. Schwarz (eds), Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (Russell Sage, New York), pp. 3–25. Kahneman, D., E. Diener and N. Schwarz (eds): 1999, Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (Russell Sage, New York). King, L.A. and C.K. Napa: 1998, ‘What makes life good?’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75, pp. 156–165. Kraut, R., J. Olson, M. Banaji, A. Bruckman, J. Cohen and M. Couper: 2004, ‘Psychological research online: Report of Board of Scientific Affairs’ Advisory Group on the Conduct of Research on the Internet’, American Psychologist 59, pp. 105–117. Lebo, H.: 2003, The UCLA Internet Report: Surveying the Digital Future-YearThree (University of California Regents, Los Angeles), Retrieved July 31, 2003, from http://www.ccp.ucla.edu/UCLA-Internet-Report-Year-Three. pdf. Lenhart, A.: 2000, Who’s Not Online: 57% of Those Without Internet Access Say They Do Not Plan to Log on (Pew Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC), Retrieved July 31, 2003, from http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/toc.asp?Report_21. Linley, P.A. and S. Joseph (eds): in press, Positive Psychology in Practice (John Wiley and Sons, New York). Linville, P.W. and G.W. Fischer: 1991, ‘Preferences for separating or combining events’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60, pp. 5– 23. Maslow, A.H.: 1970, Motivation and Personality (2nd ed) (Harper & Row, New York). Massimini, F. and A. Delle Fave: 2000, ‘Individual development in a biocultural perspective’, American Psychologist 55, pp. 24–33. McGregor, I. and B.R. Little: 1998, ‘Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and being yourself ’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, pp. 494–512. Myers, D.G.: 1992, The Pursuit of Happiness: Who is Happy – and Why (William Morrow, New York). Myers, D.G. and E. Diener: 1995, ‘Who is happy?’, Psychological Science 6, pp. 10–19. FULL LIFE VERSUS EMPTY LIFE 41 Pavot, W. and E. Diener: 1993, ‘Review of the satisfaction with life scale’, Psychological Assessment 5, pp. 164–172. Rogers, C.R.: 1951, Client-Centered Therapy: Its Current Practice, Implications, and Theory (Houghton Mifflin, Boston). Russell, B.: 1930, The Conquest of Happiness (Liveright, New York). Russell, B.: 1945, A History of Western Philosophy, and Its Connection With Political and Social Circumstances from the Earliest Times to the Present Day (Simon & Schuster, New York). Ryan, R.M. and E.L. Deci: 2000, ‘On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being’, Annual Review of Psychology 52, pp. 141–166. Ryff, C.D. and B. Singer: 1996, ‘Psychological well-being: Meaning, measurement, and implications for psychotherapy research’, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 65, pp. 14–23. Schonlau, M., R.D. Fricker and M.N. Elliott: 2002, Conducting Research Surveys via E-mail and the Web (RAND, Santa Monica, CA). Seligman, M.E.P.: 2002, Authentic Happiness (Free Press, New York). Seligman, M.E.P. and M. Csikszentmihalyi: 2000, ‘Positive psychology: An introduction’, American Psychologist 55, pp. 5–14. Waterman, A.S.: 1993, ‘Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64, pp. 678–691. Watson, J.: 1895, Hedonistic Theories from Aristippus to Spencer (Macmillan, New York). Address for Correspondence: CHRISTOPHER PETERSON Department of Psychology University of Michigan 525 East University Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109 U.S.A E-mail: chrispet@umich.edu Enhancing Well-Being and Alleviating Depressive Symptoms With Positive Psychology Interventions: A Practice-Friendly Meta-Analysis m Nancy L. Sin University of California, Riverside m Sonja Lyubomirsky University of California, Riverside Do positive psychology interventions—that is, treatment methods or intentional activities aimed at cultivating positive feelings, positive behaviors, or positive cognitions—enhance well-being and ameliorate depressive symptoms? A meta-analysis of 51 such interventions with 4,266 individuals was conducted to address this question and to provide practical guidance to clinicians. The results revealed that positive psychology interventions do indeed significantly enhance well-being (mean r 5 .29) and decrease depressive symptoms (mean r 5 .31). In addition, several factors were found to impact the effectiveness of positive psychology interventions, including the depression status, self-selection, and age of participants, as well as the format and duration of the interventions. Accordingly, clinicians should be encouraged to incorporate positive psychology techniques into their clinical work, particularly for treating clients who are depressed, relatively older, or highly motivated to improve. Our findings also suggest that clinicians would do well to deliver positive psychology interventions as individual (versus group) therapy and for relatively longer periods of time. & 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Clin Psychol: In Session 65: 467–487, 2009. Keywords: depression; meta-analysis; positive psychology; psychotherapy; treatment; well-being Achieving greater happiness is an important goal for many people around the world. Although most individuals are, in fact, happy (Diener & Diener, 1996), fewer than We are grateful to Robert Rosenthal and Robin DiMatteo for their statistical advice. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Sonja Lyubomirsky or Nancy Sin, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521; e-mail: sonja.lyubomirsky@ ucr.edu, nancysin@gmail.com JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: IN SESSION, Vol. 65(5), 467--487 (2009) & 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20593 468 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 20% of adults in the United States report that they are flourishing (Keyes, 2002). Indeed, many appear to be languishing—that is, feeling as if they are ‘‘stuck in a rut’’ or ‘‘wanting more’’—yet are not diagnosable with a mental disorder (Fredrickson, 2008). Because happiness has been shown to be both the cause and the consequence of many desirable life outcomes (including career success, marriage, and health; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Pressman & Cohen, 2005), it is vital to understand how languishing individuals can be lifted to a more optimal state of functioning. This spurs the millennia-old question: How does one enhance well-being and relieve suffering? Over the past decade, research in the field of positive psychology has emerged to provide evidence-based answers and to guide practitioners. Increasingly, psychological well-being (WB) is viewed as not only the absence of mental disorder but also the presence of positive psychological resources, including components of hedonic or subjective well-being (e.g., positive affect, life satisfaction, happiness; Diener, 1984), as well as components of eudaimonic well-being (e.g., self-acceptance, positive relations, autonomy, purpose in life; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff, 1989). A promising approach to increase well-being is through positive psychology interventions (PPIs)—that is, treatment methods or intentional activities that aim to cultivate positive feelings, behaviors, or cognitions. For instance, PPI strategies as diverse as writing gratitude letters, practicing optimistic thinking, replaying positive experiences, and socializing have been shown to increase WB in nonclinical samples (e.g., Fordyce, 1977; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm, & Sheldon, 2008; Ruini, Belaise, Brombin, Caffo, & Fava, 2006). It is worth noting, however, that programs, interventions, or treatments aimed at fixing, remedying, or healing something that is pathological or deficient—as opposed to building strengths—do not fit the definition of a PPI. In the past several years, research on PPIs for treating depression has proliferated. Although PPIs may be an option for treating a variety of mental disorders (e.g., anxiety disorders; Fava et al., 2005), they can be particularly useful for addressing a paucity of positive affect, engagement, and life meaning that characterize depression (Forbes & Dahl, 2005; Seligman, Rashid, & Parks, 2006). For example, depressed individuals are likely to benefit from increases in positive emotions, which have been shown to speed recovery from the physiological effects of negative emotions (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004), to improve broadminded coping skills (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002), and to prevent relapses (Fava & Ruini, 2003). However, PPI studies have provided mixed results. Some studies have shown that positive psychotherapy, delivered to depressed individuals either in a group setting or individually, significantly boosts WB and decreases depression (Fava et al., 2005; Seligman et al., 2006). Other studies, however, have not found PPIs to be beneficial overall compared with no-treatment control or placebo groups (e.g., Della Porta, Sin, & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Froh, Sefick, & Emmons, 2008). The general effectiveness of PPIs remains unclear, as no systematic quantitative review of the research has yet been published. Solid empirical evidence is needed to advance beyond anecdotal or clinical observations. Thus, the purpose of our metaanalysis was to provide guidance to clinical practitioners by answering the following vital questions: * Do PPIs effectively enhance WB and ameliorate depression relative to control groups and, if so, with what magnitude? Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions * 469 Which variables—with respect to both the characteristics of the participants and the methodologies used—moderate the effectiveness of PPIs? Because PPIs span multiple literatures, this meta-analysis is not comprehensive. Instead, we primarily focus on PPIs conducted in positive psychology and include samplings of PPIs from large, related literatures (e.g., mindfulness, life review therapy, forgiveness therapy) to increase the generalizability of the findings. Meta-Analysis Method Literature Search English-language PPI studies, conducted between 1977 and 2008, were located using several search strategies. First, we searched our own libraries by hand for published and unpublished studies. Next, we searched the PsycINFO online database using combinations of the following keywords: depression, intervention, positive affect, positive psychology, positive psychotherapy, well-being therapy, and well-being. Furthermore, references for studies outside the positive psychology field were gathered from colleagues. Finally, the reference sections of obtained papers, as well as PsycINFO-generated listings of works that cite the papers, were examined for additional PPI studies. Studies were included in the meta-analysis if they met the following criteria: 1. The study must empirically test an intervention, therapy, or activity primarily aimed at increasing positive feelings, positive behaviors, or positive cognitions, as opposed to ameliorating pathology or fixing negative thoughts or maladaptive behavior patterns. 2. Pre-intervention and post-intervention measures of WB or depression (as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) must be included. 3. The study must have a comparison group, such as a no-treatment control, neutral control, placebo, or treatment as usual. 4. The study must provide an effect size (or information to calculate an effect size) for the strength and direction of the difference between the PPI group’s and comparison group’s outcomes. Interventions designed to improve physical well-being (health) or that involve physical activity were excluded, as they are beyond the scope of this meta-analysis. Furthermore, mood induction studies were excluded, as their aim is to boost temporary mood, not psychological well-being. Data Recorded The specific measures of WB or depression, sample size, effect sizes, and one-tailed Z’s associated with the significance levels were extracted from each study. In nearly all cases, the r effect size for WB or depression was computed from Cohen’s d, F, t, p, or descriptive statistics. If a result was reported as significant but did not provide an exact probability, the one-tailed p value was assumed to be .025. If a result was simply reported as nonsignificant and no data were provided to calculate an exact probability, it was conservatively assigned p (one-tailed) 5 .5 and r 5 0. For studies in which multiple measures of WB or depression were used, the r for each measure was transformed to its corresponding Fisher Zr and then averaged to form a single Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 470 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 score (a conservative approach; Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). Results in the unpredicted direction (i.e., the comparison group experienced greater increases in WB and greater decreases in depression compared with the PPI group) were recorded as negative values of r. In addition, the following information from each study was coded for the analysis of moderators: depression status of participants (depressed or nondepressed); participant age (child/adolescent up to 17 years old, young adult from 18 to 35 years old, middle adult from 36 to 59 years old, or older adult of at least 60 years old); whether participants self-selected to participate in the positive intervention (selfselected or non-self-selected); intervention format (individual therapy, groupadministered, or self-administered); intervention duration (up to 4 weeks, 5–7 weeks, 8–12 weeks, or more than 12 weeks); and comparison group type (notreatment control, neutral control, placebo, or treatment as usual). See Tables 1 and 2 for a complete list of studies and their relevant characteristics and results. Statistical Analyses Meta-analytic tests were conducted using both fixed effects and random effects models. The fixed effects model, although limited in generalizability to the sample of studies contained in the meta-analysis, is statistically powerful and appropriate for small-sample meta-analyses (Rosenthal, 1995). The random effects model is less powerful; however, it permits generalization to studies outside this sample (Rosenthal, 1995). Significance tests. Significance testing was conducted to determine the probability that the sets of effect sizes for WB and depression are not significantly different from zero. For the fixed effects model, one-tailed p values from each study were converted to Z scores and then combined using the Stouffer method (e.g., Mosteller & Bush, 1954; Rosenthal & Rosnow, 2008, p. 673). For the random effects model, one-sample t-tests were conducted on the mean Fisher Zr effect sizes (Rosenthal, 1995). Fail-safe N. To address the ‘‘file drawer problem’’—that is, the bias for significant results to be more likely published and retrievable for a meta-analysis relative to nonsignificant results—the fail-safe N was computed. This N represents the number of studies averaging null results that must exist to render the overall findings nonsignificant (Rosenthal, 1979, 1991a). The tolerance level was also computed to estimate the number of irretrievable studies that possibly exist, based on the assumption that the number of unpublished studies is not five times greater than the number of published ones (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 2008, p. 689). Heterogeneity test. A chi-square (w2) test was performed to determine the probability that the obtained effect sizes are not heterogeneous (Rosenthal, 1991b, p. 73). A highly significant chi-square result would suggest that moderator variables may account for the heterogeneity of the effect sizes (Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). Moderator analyses. Predictions regarding moderators were translated into linear contrast weights (l), and contrast tests were conducted on the effect sizes based on the fixed effects model (Rosenthal, 1991b, p. 80; Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). For the random effects model, the relationships between moderators and effect sizes were analyzed by correlating Fisher Zr effect sizes with their corresponding contrast weights (Rosenthal, 1991a). Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Journal of Clinical Psychology SWLS; SHS; PA Positive reminiscence Life review therapy Gratitude Gratitude Gratitude n 0.75 0.38 0.37 0.55 0.58 r DOI: 10.1002/jclp nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed 0.20 0.44 20 16 202 0.27 68 0.40 — 0.32 65 depressed nondepressed 0.31 depressed nondepressed — nondepressed depressed nondepressed depressed Depression status 132 0.11 32 130 0.04 14 36 32 Mental health component of 13 the Short Form Health Survey PA 90 WB measure State Hope Scale; Purpose in Life Test Life Satisfaction Index Form A Life Satisfaction Index Form Z SWLS; SHS; PA Hope therapy Positive writing Mindfulness Intervention b global life appraisals; PA Gratitude global life appraisals; PA; connectedness; observer reports Fava et al. (1998) WBT Ryff’s PWB; 4 scales of WB in SQ Fava et al. (2005) CBT/WBT Ryff’s PWB; 4 scales of WB in SQ Fordyce (1977, Fordyce’s happiness Happiness measures (selfStudy 1) program report scores for quality and quantity of happiness) Fordyce (1977, Fordyce’s happiness Happiness measures Study 2) program Della Porta et al. (2008) Della Porta et al. (2008) Emmons et al. (2003, Study 1) Emmons et al. (2003, Study 3) Davis (2004) Burton et al. (2004) Cheavens et al. (2006) Cook (1998) Bédard et al. (2003) Study a Studies Examining the Effect of PPI on Well-Being Table 1 group-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. individual therapy non-self-selected self-admin. self-selected self-selected self-selected YA YA non-self-selected self-admin. 5–7 weeks r4 weeks individual therapy 412 weeks placebo placebo TAU TAU no-treat ctrl placebo r4 weeks r4 weeks placebo r4 weeks neutral ctrl — r4 weeks 8–12 weeks no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl neutral ctrl — 412 weeks 8–12 weeks r4 weeks 8–12 weeks Intervention Comparison duration group typed individual therapy 412 weeks non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected YA MA non-self-selected self-admin. YA YA YA OA OA group-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected YA group-admin. Intervention delivery Moderators Self-selection MA self-selected Age c Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 471 Journal of Clinical Psychology Cultivating sacred moments Life coaching/goals Gratitude Fordyce’s happiness program Fordyce’s happiness program Fordyce’s happiness program Forgiveness Interventiona 12 Hope scale PA; SWLS; Ryff’s PWB; Hope Scale PA global life appraisals; PA; BMSLSS SWLS; Ryff’s PWB 0.31 57 DOI: 10.1002/jclp nondepressed 0.56 0.63 0.09 33 48 35 — nondepressed nondepressed 119 0.27 depressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed 0.32 0.33 0.00 nondepressed depressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed Depression status 0.19 0.13 55 35 52 50 73 141 0.02 0.84 0.32 0.45 r 98 68 n Happiness measures; SelfDescription Inventory Happiness measures Happiness measures WB measureb PA PA; SWLS; Life Orientation Test Kremers et al. Self-Management Social Production Function (2006) Index Level Scale Lichter et al. Discussing beliefs pro-happy beliefs; (1980, Study 1) Affectometer 1; domain satisfaction scores Lichter et al. Rehearsal of positive Affectometer 1; domain (1980, Study 2) statements satisfaction scores Low et al. (2006) Positive writing PA (Vigor subscale of POMS) Grossman et al. Mindfulness (2007) King et al. (2000) Positive writing King (2001) Positive writing Green (2006) Goldstein (2007) Fordyce (1977, Study 3) Fordyce (1983, Study 4) Fordyce (1983, Study 6) Freedman et al. (1996) Froh et al. (2008) Study Table 1 Continued self-admin. Intervention delivery — YA — OA YA YA group-admin. group-admin. self-admin. group-admin. self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected group-admin. self-selected non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected MA self-selected self-selected C/A non-self-selected group-admin. YA 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 5–7 weeks no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl — r4 weeks r4 weeks r4 weeks neutral ctrl neutral ctrl r4 weeks r4 weeks 5–7 weeks TAU 8–12 weeks no-treat ctrl TAU r4 weeks 8–12 weeks no-treat ctrl r4 weeks no-treat ctrl placebo placebo no-treat ctrl Intervention Comparison duration group typed individual therapy 412 weeks non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. self-selected Self-selection MA self-selected YA YA YA Agec Moderators 472 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 Journal of Clinical Psychology Goals Goals Optimism or gratitude Gratitude Kindness Interventiona PA; SWLS SHS; global happiness item; delighted-terrible scale; SWLS; PA SHS; global happiness item; delighted-terrible scale; SWLS; PA SHS; SWB (pleasant affect; unpleasant affect reversed; SWLS) PA; SWLS WB measureb Counting kindnesses SHS PPT Students LS Scale; PPTI Child Version Forgiveness Ryff’s PWB (Environmental Mastery) Ruini et al. (2006) WBT Ryff’s PWB; 4 scales of WB in SQ Seligman (2004) PPT SWLS Seligman et al. Gratitude/personal Steen Happiness Index (2005) strength activities PPT SWLS Seligman et al. (2006, Study 1) Seligman et al. PPT PPTI; SWLS (2006, Study 2) MacLeod et al. (2008, Study 1) MacLeod et al. (2008, Study 2) Otake et al. (2006) Rashid et al. (2006) Reed et al. (2006) Lyubomirsky et al. (2004, Study 1) Lyubomirsky et al. (2004, Study 2) Lyubomirsky et al. (2008) Study Continued Table 1 0.00 r depressed DOI: 10.1002/jclp depressed depressed depressed 185 0.16 411 0.06 0.20 0.40 34 32 depressed nondepressed 111 0.06 0.52 20 nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed 0.52 0.24 nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed Depression status 119 0.20 22 0.32 20 64 315 0.16 147 0.00 84 n Intervention delivery self-selected self-selected self-selected self-selected YA self-selected MA self-selected YA — r4 weeks 8–12 weeks 5–7 weeks 412 weeks r4 weeks individual therapy 8–12 weeks group-admin. self-admin. self-admin. 8–12 weeks TAU no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl placebo TAU TAU no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl r4 weeks 5–7 weeks — no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl 8–12 weeks 5–7 weeks 5–7 weeks Intervention Comparison duration group typed individual therapy 412 weeks C/A non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected YA non-self-selected self-admin. C/A non-self-selected self-admin. self-admin. group-admin. self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. Self-selection MA self-selected YA YA YA YA Agec Moderators Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 473 Goal training Interventiona Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp SWLS PA PA Positive writing Mindfulness Mindfulness Gratitude PA; SWLS; SHS; Ryff’s PWB PA Happiness Inventory; Psychap Measure PA; SWB; Ryff’s PWB PA; Ryff’s PWB; selfactualization questionnaire PA WB measureb 0.01 57 nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed Depression status 20 0.22 175 0.00 71 0.15 156 0.16 depressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed 285 0.04 nondepressed 0.80 0.28 0.00 r 36 67 78 n group-admin. self-admin. group-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected 5–7 weeks r4 weeks r4 weeks neutral ctrl placebo r4 weeks 8–12 weeks placebo neutral ctrl r4 weeks 8–12 weeks neutral ctrl 8–12 weeks no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl placebo neutral ctrl Intervention Comparison duration group typed individual therapy 8–12 weeks non-self-selected group-admin. non-self-selected self-admin. MA self-selected MA self-selected YA YA Intervention delivery non-self-selected group-admin. Self-selection MA self-selected YA YA YA Agec Moderators Note. Dash (—) indicates information could not be found, and the study was excluded from the respective moderator analysis. Positive effect sizes indicate superiority of PPI over comparison group for increasing WB. Negative effect sizes indicate superiority of comparison group over PPI for increasing WB. a CBT 5 cognitive behavioral therapy; PPT 5 positive psychotherapy; WBT 5 well-being therapy. b BMSLSS 5 Brief Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale; LS 5 life satisfaction; PA 5 positive affect; PPTI 5 Positive Psychotherapy Inventory; PWB 5 Psychological Well-Being Scale; SHS 5 Subjective Happiness Scale; SWB 5 Subjective Well-Being; SWLS 5 Satisfaction With Life Scale; WB 5 well-being. c C/A 5 child/adolescent (up to 17 years old); YA 5 young adult (18 to 35 years old); MA 5 middle adult (35 to 59 years old); OA 5 older adult (60 years old and up). d Ctrl 5 control; TAU 5 treatment as usual. Watkins et al. (2003, Study 4) Wing et al. (2006) Zautra et al. (2008, Study 1) Zautra et al. (2008, Study 1) Sheldon et al. Gratitude (2006) Smith et al. (1995) Fordyce’s happiness program/meditation Spence et al. Life coaching/goals (2007) Tkach (2006) Kindness Sheldon et al. (2002) Study Table 1 Continued 474 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp CES-D Children’s Depression Inventory Forgiveness Optimism or gratitude PPT Lin et al. (2004) Lyubomirsky et al. (2008) Rashid et al. (2006) BDI-II BDI Rehearsal of positive statements Forgiveness HADS Fordyce’s happiness program Fordyce (1983, Study 6) Mindfulness Fordyce’s happiness program Fordyce (1983, Study 4) Freedman et al. (1996) Grossman et al. (2007) Lichter et al. (1980, Study 2) CBT/WBT Fava et al. (2005) CID; Kellner’s SQ CID; Kellner’s SQ Depression Adjective Check List Depression Adjective Check List BDI BDI-II Gratitude WBT BDI-II Zung BDI-II CES-D Depression measureb Gratitude Life review therapy Mindfulness Hope therapy Intervention Fava et al. (1998) Della Porta et al. (2008) Della Porta et al. (2008) Bédard et al. (2003) Cheavens et al. (2006) Davis (2004) Study a Studies Examining the Effect of PPI on Depression Table 2 0.03 0.04 22 0.47 0.50 0.30 0.75 315 14 48 52 12 0.26 0.21 98 57 0.40 0.32 0.14 0.11 0.81 0.56 0.35 r 16 20 32 130 14 13 32 n nondepressed nondepressed depressed nondepressed depressed depressed nondepressed nondepressed nondepressed depressed depressed nondepressed — depressed depressed Depression status C/A YA MA YA MA MA YA YA MA YA YA YA OA MA MA Age c non-self-selected self-selected self-selected non-self-selected self-selected self-selected non-self-selected non-self-selected self-selected self-selected non-self-selected non-self-selected self-selected self-selected self-selected Self-selection self-admin. individual therapy self-admin. self-admin. individual therapy group-admin. self-admin. individual therapy individual therapy self-admin. self-admin. individual therapy self-admin. group-admin. group-admin. Intervention delivery Moderators placebo r4 weeks 8–12 weeks 8–12 weeks no-treat ctrl — TAU no-treat ctrl r4 weeks 5–7 weeks TAU no-treat ctrl placebo placebo TAU 8–12 weeks 412 weeks 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 412 weeks TAU placebo r4 weeks 412 weeks — — no-treat ctrl Comparison group typed r4 weeks 8–12 weeks 8–12 weeks Intervention duration Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 475 Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Mindfulness Mindfulness BDI Fordyce’s happiness program/meditation Mindfulness depressive symptoms questionnaire depressive symptoms questionnaire HADS HRSD; Zung PPT 0.23 0.01 71 20 0.28 0.73 17 36 0.51 0.25 0.15 0.15 0.10 0.53 r depressed nondepressed depressed nondepressed depressed depressed nondepressed depressed depressed depressed Depression status MA MA — YA YA MA C/A YA — MA Agec self-selected self-selected self-selected non-self-selected self-selected self-selected non-self-selected self-selected self-selected self-selected Self-selection group-admin. group-admin. group-admin. individual therapy group-admin. group-admin. individual therapy self-admin. self-admin. self-admin. Intervention delivery Moderators 8–12 weeks 8–12 weeks 8–12 weeks 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 412 weeks r4 weeks 412 weeks Intervention duration placebo placebo no-treat ctrl no-treat ctrl TAU no-treat ctrl TAU no-treat ctrl placebo TAU Comparison group typed Note. Dash (—) indicates information could not be found, and the study was excluded from the respective moderator analysis. Positive effect sizes indicate superiority of PPI over comparison group for decreasing depressive symptoms. Negative effect sizes indicate superiority of comparison group over PPI for decreasing depressive symptoms. a CBT 5 cognitive behavioral therapy; PPT 5 positive psychotherapy; WBT 5 well-being therapy. b BDI 5 Beck Depression Inventory; CES-D 5 Center for Epidemiology–Depression Scale; CID 5 Clinical Interview for Depression; HADS 5 Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HRSD 5 Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; SQ 5 Symptom Questionnaire. c C/A 5 child/adolescent (up to 17 years old); YA 5 young adult (18 to 35 years old); MA 5 middle adult (35 to 59 years old); OA 5 older adult (60 years old and up). d Ctrl 5 control, TAU 5 treatment as usual. Zautra et al. (2008, Study 1) Surawy et al. (2005, Study 1) Zautra et al. (2008, Study 1) 34 BDI-II 32 111 185 411 20 n Kellner’s SQ CES-D CES-D Ruini et al. (2006) Seligman (2004) Seligman et al. (2005) Seligman et al. (2006, Study 1) Seligman et al. (2006, Study 2) Smith et al. (1995) BDI-II Depression measureb WBT PPT Gratitude/personal strength activities PPT Forgiveness Interventiona Reed et al. (2006) Study Table 2 Continued 476 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 477 Meta-Analysis Results Overall, PPIs were indeed significantly more effective than comparison groups for boosting WB and for ameliorating depression. Table 3 shows a stem-and-leaf display of all effect sizes, and Table 4 summarizes the meta-analytic findings. The relevant statistics for all the moderator analyses are presented in Tables 5 and 6. Well-Being Forty-nine independent studies were meta-analyzed for WB, totaling 4,235 participants (median n 5 64 per study). The r effect sizes ranged from .31 to .84, with 96% of effect sizes in the predicted, positive direction. The unweighted mean r (.29) was close in magnitude to the median r (.24) and was highly significant based on both the fixed effects (one-tailed p 5 0) and the random effects (one-tailed p 5 4 109) models. Although the file drawer problem (indicated by the asymmetric funnel plot of the stem-and-leaf display; Light & Pillemer, 1984), was likely present, it was not large enough to render the overall results nonsignificant. Indeed, the failsafe N indicated that 2,519 studies averaging null results must exist to render this finding nonsignificant; this number greatly exceeds the tolerance level of 255 unpublished null studies that possibly exist. Furthermore, the set of effect sizes was heterogeneous (w2ð48Þ ¼ 230:92, one-tailed p 5 9 1026), suggesting the presence of moderator variables. Depression The meta-analysis for depression encompassed 25 separate studies, with a median of 32 participants per study and a grand total of 1,812 participants. The r effect sizes spanned from .28 to .81, with 80% of effect sizes in favor of PPI. The unweighted mean r of .31 was close to the median r (.26) and was highly significant based on both the fixed effects (one-tailed p 5 2 1012) and the random effects (one-tailed p 5 .0001) models. The file drawer problem was unlikely to threaten the significant results, given that the fail-safe N was 420, which exceeded the tolerance level of 135. Moreover, as in the case of well-being, the set of effect sizes was heterogeneous Table 3 Back-to-Back Stem-and-Leaf Display of all Effect Sizes WELL-BEING (k 5 49) 8, 7, 3, 8, 7, 7, 9, 6, 9, 6, 6, 4, 2, 1, 0, 8, 6, 5, 2, 2, 4, 2, 6, 6, 0, 0, DEPRESSION (k 5 25) Leaf 4, 0 5 3 5, 2, 2 4, 0, 0 2, 2, 1 0, 0, 0 5, 3, 1 0, 0, 0 1 Stem 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 Leaf 1 3, 5 0, 0, 0, 1, 0, 0, 1, 8 1, 7 2, 3, 5 3, 4, 3, 6 5 5, 6 4 5 Note. ‘‘k’’ is the number of studies included in the meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Journal of Clinical Psychology 49 25 4,235 1,812 64 32 .29 [.21, .37] .31 [.17, .43] 0.24 0.26 Fixed p 2,519 (255) 420 (135) Random Fail-safe N (tolerance) .31, .84 .24 0 4 109 .28, .81 .29 2 1012 .0001 No. of studies Total n Median n per study Mean [95% CI]b Median Min, max SD Unweighted r effect size Note. All p values are one-tailed. a Degrees of freedom is the number of studies minus 1. b Confidence intervals are computed based on the stringent random effects model. Well-Being Depression Variable Sample size Summary of Overall Meta-Analysis Findings Table 4 230.92 146.32 w2 9 1026 2 1019 P Test of heterogeneitya 478 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 DOI: 10.1002/jclp Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 479 (w2ð24Þ ¼ 146:32, one-tailed p 5 2 1019), indicating that moderators may account for the variation in effect sizes. Participant Moderators Depression status. Depressed individuals benefited more from PPIs than nondepressed individuals, according to the fixed effects moderator analysis. However, the random effects moderator analysis did not find a significant moderating effect for depression status. The mean effect sizes, and other relevant statistics for this and all other moderators, are displayed in Tables 5 and 6. Self-selection. Individuals who elected to participate in a PPI—perhaps expecting that the intervention would make them happier or less depressed—were more likely to experience greater gains in WB and depression compared with their non-self-selected counterparts, based on the fixed effects model. However, the moderating effect of self-selection was not significant for WB or depression according to the random effects model. Age. Both the fixed effects and random effects models revealed that the benefits of PPIs increase with age. Although there were not enough studies to include the ‘‘older adult’’ group in the moderator analysis for depression, the other age groups nevertheless showed larger decreases in depression as a linear function of age. Methodological Moderators Intervention format. Intervention format had a significant moderating effect on the effectiveness of PPIs, based on both the fixed effects and the random effects models. Specifically, the mean r effect size was greatest for individual therapy, followed by group-administered PPIs, then self-administered PPIs. Intervention duration. Longer interventions produced greater gains in WB based on the fixed effects model, and this effect approached significance according to the random effects model (one-tailed p 5 .09). For depression, however, the moderating effect of intervention duration was not significant based on both the fixed effects and random effects models. Comparison group type. The fixed effects analysis revealed that the type of comparison group used in a PPI moderated the gains in WB but not when computed using the random effects analysis. In particular, studies that compared a PPI to a notreatment control group showed the greatest boost in WB. Positive interventions that were compared with ‘‘treatment as usual’’ showed the second greatest benefit, suggesting that PPIs may be more effective than standard treatments. Finally, PPIs also produced greater boosts in WB compared with neutral control and placebo activities. The type of comparison group did not have a moderating effect on shifts in depression, based on both the fixed effects and the random effects models. This indicates that participation in PPIs may be more effective for relieving depression than participation in a no-treatment control group, placebo activity, or treatment as usual. Clinical Practices Do positive psychology interventions effectively boost well-being and ameliorate depression? The overwhelming evidence from our meta-analysis suggests that the Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 2023 633 1290 289 1372 1006 1156 324 20 9 14 6 20 8 10 7 274 2710 603 169 3 27 13 3 171 779 3285 1944 2291 25 24 7 14 28 863 3258 Total n 12 34 No. of studies po.05 (one-tailed).po.01 (one-tailed). po .001 (one-tailed). Methodological moderators Intervention format Individual therapy Group-administered Self-administered Intervention duration r 4 weeks 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 4 12 weeks Comparison group type No-treatment control Neutral control Placebo Treatment as usual (TAU) Participant moderators Depression status Depressed Nondepressed Self-selection Self-selected Non-self-selected Age Child/adolescent (r 17 years old) Young adult (18–35 years old) Middle adult (36–59 years old) Older adult ( 60 years old) Moderator Summary of Moderator Findings for Well-Being Table 5 0.37 0.14 0.18 0.29 0.23 0.36 0.24 0.47 0.50 0.34 0.20 0.14 0.23 0.39 0.50 0.33 0.24 0.33 0.26 Unweighted mean r 0.32 0.12 0.25 0.32 0.18 0.31 0.27 0.41 0.44 0.30 0.16 0.06 0.20 0.33 0.38 0.27 0.20 0.27 0.22 Unweighted median r 13 11 1 3 3 1 11 13 11 0 1 3 1 11 13 11 1 11 1 l weight z 5 5.63 z 5 1.73 z 5 1.88 z 5 1.47 z 5 2.28 z 5 4.39 Fixed r 5 .20 r 5 .20 r 5 .42 r 5 .35 r 5 .17 r 5 .12 Random Moderator analysis 480 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 635 141 783 253 386 819 265 5 4 11 5 8 7 7 128 275 1409 133 987 250 2 11 9 7 8 10 1272 534 894 904 Total n 17 8 14 10 No. of studies 0.35 0.08 0.35 0.29 0.46 0.18 0.46 0.57 0.30 0.09 0.06 0.27 0.42 0.35 0.20 0.32 0.21 Unweighted mean r 0.30 0.10 0.40 0.10 0.37 0.21 0.40 0.56 0.28 0.07 0.06 0.25 0.40 0.32 0.13 0.31 0.13 Unweighted median r b ‘‘Older adult’’ was excluded in this analysis because there was only 1 study (n 5 14) in that category. ‘‘Neutral control’’ was excluded from this analysis because no studies had a ‘‘neutral control’’ comparison group. po.05 (one-tailed).po .01 (one-tailed).po.001 (one-tailed). a Methodological moderators Intervention format Individual therapy Group-administered Self-administered Intervention duration r4 weeks 5–7 weeks 8–12 weeks 4 12 weeks Comparison group typeb No-treatment control Placebo Treatment as usual (TAU) Participant Moderators Depression Status Depressed Nondepressed Self-Selection Self-selected Non-self-selected Agea Child/adolescent (r 17 years old) Young adult (18–35 years old) Middle adult (36–59 years old) Moderator Summary of Moderator Findings for Depression Table 6 11 0 1 3 1 11 13 11 0 1 1 0 11 11 1 11 1 l weight z 5 .44 z 5 1.23 z 5 4.07 z 5 4.95 z 5 4.42 z 5 2.36 Fixed r 5 .02 r 5 .04 r 5 .63 r 5 .45 r 5 .23 r 5 .17 Random Moderator analysis Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 481 482 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 Table 7 Binomial Effect Size Displays of the Benefits of PPI Lower well-being Higher well-being Total Effect of PPI on well-being (r 5 .29) PPI Control Total 35 65 100 65 35 100 100 100 200 Effect of PPI on depression (r 5 .31) PPI Control Total 34 66 100 66 34 100 100 100 200 answer is ‘‘yes.’’ The combined results of 49 studies revealed that PPIs do, in fact, significantly enhance WB, and the combined results of 25 studies showed that PPIs are also effective for treating depressive symptoms. The magnitude of these effects is medium-sized (mean r 5 .29 for WB, mean r 5 .31 for depression), indicating that not only do PPIs work, they work well. The practical importance of these effect sizes is illustrated using a binomial effect size display, as shown in Table 7. In a hypothetical group of 200 individuals—half the group treated with a PPI, and half the group untreated—65 individuals treated with a PPI would experience gains in WB, compared with only 35 of the individuals in the control group. Similarly, 66 PPI-treated individuals would experience decreased depression, relative to only 34 individuals in the control group. It is likely that—in addition to learning how to cope with negatives—clients will greatly benefit from attending to, appreciating, and attaining life’s positives. Thus, practitioners are encouraged to incorporate elements of positive psychology into their clinical work. A variety of PPIs have now been found effective, including positive behaviors like engaging in enjoyable activities (Fordyce, 1977) and using one’s signature strengths in new ways (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Cognitive strategies, such as replaying positive experiences and self-monitoring instances of well-being, have also been shown to boost happiness and alleviate depression (Fava, Rafanelli, Cazzaro, Conti, & Grandi, 1998). Finally, the practice of emotional skills—including mindfulness and acceptance—can have a positive impact on a client’s psychological well-being (Bédard et al., 2003; Grossman, Tiefenthaler-Gilmer, Raysz, & Kesper, 2007; Zautra et al., 2008). Our moderator analyses also point to specific clinical practices. First, depression status moderated the effectiveness of PPIs, such that depressed individuals experienced enhanced WB and reduced depressive symptoms relative to nondepressed ones. This finding could be accounted for by a floor effect—that is, depressed individuals may simply have more room to improve—or it may have a substantive explanation. In any case, our result challenges the notion that depressed people might benefit less from PPIs, because their characteristic cognitive, affective, and behavioral deficits prevent them from taking full advantage of the relevant positive activities. Accordingly, practitioners are advised to implement PPIs in the treatment of both clinically depressed and nondepressed clients, as both are likely to garner the benefits. PPIs can be especially effective for treating residual symptoms (Fava et al., 1998) and preventing future relapse for formerly depressed clients (Seligman et al., 2006). Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 483 Second, self-selected individuals—those who possibly were more motivated or who expected the intervention to make them happier—benefited more from PPIs than did their non–self-selected peers. We did not find this surprising, as those with higher levels of motivation or more positive, optimistic expectancies would be expected to work harder and longer at following the instructions of a PPI, to show greater commitment, and to take their assigned task more seriously. Accordingly, practitioners will do well to bolster motivation and provide ample encouragement to clients, particularly to those who are initially reluctant to engage in treatment. Third, the benefits of PPIs increased linearly with age, perhaps due to the greater wisdom and more effective emotional regulation and self-regulation associated with older age (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999; Linley et al., 2007). Older participants may also have treated the PPI with greater seriousness and maturity and applied more effort in carrying out its recommendations. Thus, practitioners may see more improvement when offering PPIs to older, rather than younger, clients. Younger individuals, as well as those who may be less emotionally invested in their treatment, should be encouraged to devote more effort and commitment. Fourth, the format of the treatment also moderated the effectiveness of PPIs: Individual therapy was most effective, followed by group-administered PPIs, then self-administered PPIs. Hence, practitioners offering PPIs as part of individual therapy, and secondarily group therapy, can expect the highest benefits for their clients. The duration of PPIs also moderated their benefits, such that longer interventions were relatively more likely to produce greater gains in WB. Longer durations give participants an opportunity to convert the positive activities they are learning into habits. Please bear in mind that these findings should be interpreted cautiously, as this meta-analysis is not comprehensive (i.e., does not include all PPI studies ever conducted). In addition, causal conclusions cannot be drawn regarding the moderators because the studies were not randomly assigned to particular moderator levels (Rosenthal, 1991b, p. 81). Finally, we offer three suggestions for clinical practice based on the extant research on PPIs, although these are not directly derived from our meta-analysis. * * * Several studies have found that exerting high levels of effort to practice a happiness-boosting strategy, and continuing to practice it even after the intervention is over, results in greater improvements in WB and depression (e.g., Lyubomirsky et al., 2008; Seligman et al., 2005). Thus, clinicians should encourage their clients to regularly practice and keep a record of positive strategies, to incorporate these strategies into their everyday lives, and to turn these strategies into habits. It also appears that a ‘‘shotgun’’ approach, in which individuals practice multiple PPI activities, may be more effective than engaging in only one activity (e.g., Fordyce 1977, 1983; Seligman et al., 2005). Accordingly, practitioners may see the most benefit in their clients when assigning multiple and different positive activities. Members of individualist cultures, whose values and cultural prescriptives are highly supportive of the pursuit of individual happiness, have been found to benefit more from PPIs than members of collectivist cultures (Lyubomirsky et al., 2008). As a result, clinicians are advised to consider a client’s cultural background, as well as his or her unique inclinations, when implementing PPIs. For instance, a client from a collectivist culture may experience greater boosts in Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 484 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 well-being when practicing prosocial and other-focused activities (e.g., performing acts of kindness, writing a letter of gratitude), compared with individual-focused activities (e.g., reflecting on personal strengths). The field of positive psychology is young, yet much has already been accomplished that practitioners can effectively integrate into their daily practices. As our metaanalysis confirms, positive psychology interventions can materially improve the wellbeing of many. Selected References and Recommended Readings Studies preceded by an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: Author. *Bédard, M., Felteau, M., Mazmanian, D., Fedyk, K., Klein, R., Richardson, J. et al. (2003). Pilot evaluation of a mindfulness-based intervention to improve quality of life among individuals who sustained traumatic brain injuries. Disability and Rehabilitation, 25, 722–731. *Burton, C.M., & King, L.A. (2004). The health benefits of writing about intensely positive experiences. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 150–163. Carstensen, L.L., Isaacowitz, D.M., & Charles, S.T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. *Cheavens, J.S., Feldman, D.B., Gum, A., Michael, S.T., & Snyder, C.R. (2006). Hope therapy in a community sample: A pilot investigation. Social Indicators Research, 77, 61–78. *Cook, E.A. (1998). Effects of reminiscence on life satisfaction of elderly female nursing home residents. Health Care for Women International, 19, 109–118. *Davis, M.C. (2004). Life review therapy as an intervention to manage depression and life satisfaction in individuals with right hemisphere cerebral vascular accidents. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25, 503–515. *Della Porta, M.D., Sin, N.L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Searching for the placebo effect in happiness-enhancing interventions: An experimental longitudinal study with depressed and nondepressed participants. Poster to be presented at the 2009 Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Tampa, FL. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181–185. *Emmons, R.A., & McCullough, M.E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377–389. *Fava, G.A., Rafanelli, C., Cazzaro, M., Conti, S., & Grandi, S. (1998). Well-being therapy: A novel psychotherapeutic model for residual symptoms of affective disorders. Psychological Medicine, 28, 475–480. Fava, G.A., & Ruini, C. (2003). Development and characteristics of a well-being enhancing psychotherapeutic strategy: Well-being therapy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34, 45–63. *Fava, G.A., Ruini, C., Rafanelli, C., Finos, L., Salmaso, L., Mangelli, L., et al. (2005). Wellbeing therapy of generalized anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 74, 26–30. Forbes, E.E., & Dahl, R.E. (2005). Neural systems of positive affect: Relevance to understanding child and adolescent depression? Development and Psychopathology, 17, 827–850. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 485 *Fordyce, M.W. (1977). Development of a program to increase personal happiness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 24, 511–521. *Fordyce, M.W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 483–498. Fredrickson, B.L. (2008). Promoting positive affect. In M. Eid & R.J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 449–468). New York: Guilford Press. Fredrickson, B.L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172–175. Fredrickson, B.L., & Levenson, R.W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220. *Freedman, S.R., & Enright, R.D. (1996). Forgiveness as an intervention goal with incest survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 983–992. *Froh, J.J., Sefick, W.J., & Emmons, R.A. (2008). Counting blessings in early adolescents: An experimental study of gratitude and subjective well-being. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 213–233. *Goldstein, E.D. (2007). Sacred moments: Implications on well-being and stress. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 1001–1019. *Green, L.S., Oades, L.G., & Grant, A.M. (2006). Cognitive-behavioral, solution-focused life coaching: Enhancing goal striving, well-being, and hope. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 142–149. *Grossman, P., Tiefenthaler-Gilmer, U., Raysz, A., & Kesper, U. (2007). Mindfulness training as an intervention for fibromyalgia: Evidence of postintervention and 3-year follow-up benefits in well-being. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 226–233. Keyes, C.L.M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 43, 207–222. *King, L.A. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 798–807. *King, L.A., & Miner, K.N. (2000). Writing about the perceiving benefits of traumatic events: Implications for physical health. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 220–230. *Kremers, I.P., Steverink, N., Albersnagel, F.A., & Slaets, J.P.J. (2006). Improvement selfmanagement ability and well-being in older women after a short group intervention. Aging & Mental Health, 10, 476–484. *Lichter, S., Haye, K., & Kammann, R. (1980). Increasing happiness through cognitive retraining. New Zealand Psychologist, 9, 57–64. Light, R.J., & Pillemer, D.B. (1984). Summing up: The science of reviewing research. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. *Lin, W.F., Mack, D., Enright, R.D., Krahn, D., & Baskin, T.W. (2004). Effects of forgiveness therapy on anger, mood, and vulnerability to substance use among inpatient substancedependent clients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 1114–1121. Linley, P.A., Maltby, J., Wood, A.M., Joseph, S., Harrington, S., Peterson, C. et al. (2007). Character strengths in the United Kingdom: The VIA Inventory of Strengths. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 341–351. *Low, C.A., Stanton, A.L., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2006). Expressive disclosure and benefit finding among breast cancer patients: Mechanisms for positive health effects. Health Psychology, 25, 181–189. *Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J.K., & Sheldon, K.M. (2008). Becoming happier takes both a will and a proper way: Two experimental longitudinal interventions to boost well-being. Manuscript under review. Lyubomirsky, S., King, L.A., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803–855. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 486 Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, May 2009 *Lyubomirsky, S., Tkach, C., & Sheldon, K.M. (2004). [Pursuing sustained happiness through random acts of kindness and counting one’s blessings: Tests of two six-week interventions]. Unpublished raw data. Results presented in: Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131. *MacLeod, A.K., Coates, E., & Hetherton, J. (2008). Increasing well-being through teaching goal-setting and planning skills: Results of a brief intervention. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 185–196. Mosteller, F., & Bush, R. (1954). Selected quantitative techniques. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 289–334). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. *Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2006). Happy people become happier through kindness: A counting kindness intervention. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 361–375. Pressman, S.D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971. *Rashid, T., Anjum, A., & Lennox, C. (2006). Positive psychotherapy for middle school children. Unpublished manuscript, Toronto District School Board. Results presented in: Rashid, T., & Anjum, A. (2008). Positive psychotherapy for young adults and children. In J.R.Z. Abela & B.L. Hankin (Eds), Handbook of depression in children and adolescents (pp. 250–287). New York: Guilford Press. *Reed, G.L., & Enright, R.D. (2006). The effects of forgiveness therapy on depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress for women after spousal emotional abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 920–929. Rosenthal, R. (1979). The ‘‘file drawer problem’’ and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. Rosenthal, R. (1991a). Meta-analysis: A review. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 247–271. Rosenthal, R. (1991b). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Rev. ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183–192. Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M.R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59–82. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R.L. (2008). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. *Ruini, C., Belaise, C., Brombin, C., Caffo, E., & Fava, G.A. (2006). Well-being therapy in school settings: A pilot study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 331–336. Ryan, R.M., & Deci, E.L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. Ryff, C.D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. *Seligman, M.E.P. (2004, September). Positive interventions: More evidence of effectiveness. Authentic Happiness Newsletter. Retrieved July 17, 2008, from http://www.authentichappiness.sas.upenn.edu/newsletter.aspx?id 5 45. Results presented in: Rashid, T., & Anjum, A. (2008). Positive psychotherapy for young adults and children. In J.R.Z. Abela & B.L. Hankin (Eds), Handbook of depression in children and adolescents (pp. 250–287). New York, NY: Guilford Press. *Seligman, M.E.P., Rashid, T., & Parks, A.C. (2006). Positive psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 61, 774–788. *Seligman, M.E.P., Steen, T.A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60, 410–421. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Meta-Analysis of Positive Psychology Interventions 487 *Sheldon, K.M., Kasser, T., Smith, K., & Share, T. (2002). Personal goals and psychological growth: Testing an intervention to enhance goal attainment and personality integration. Journal of Personality, 70, 5–31. *Sheldon, K.M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion: The effects of expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 73–82. *Smith, W.P., Compton, W.C., West, W.B. (1995). Meditation as an adjunct to a happiness enhancement program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 269–273. *Spence, G.B., & Grant, A.M. (2007). Professional and peer life coaching and the enhancement of goal striving and well-being: An exploratory study. Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 185–194. *Surawy, C., Roberts, J., & Silver, A. (2005). The effect of mindfulness training on mood and measures of fatigue, activity, and quality of life in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome on a hospital waiting list: A series of exploratory studies. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 103–109. *Tkach, C.T. (2006). Unlocking the treasury of human kindness: Enduring improvements in mood, happiness, and self-evaluations. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67, 603. Tugade, M.M., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333. Watkins, P.C., Woodward, K., Stone, T., & Kolts, R.L. (2003). Gratitude and happiness: Development of a measure of gratitude and relationships with subjective well-being. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 431–452. *Wing, J.F., Schutte, N.S., & Bryne, B. (2006). The effects of positive writing on emotional intelligence and life satisfaction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1291–1302. *Zautra, A.J., Davis, M.C., Reich, J.W., Nicassario, P., Tennen, H., Finan, P. et al. (2008). Comparison of cognitive behavioral and mindfulness meditation interventions on adaptation to rheumatoid arthritis for patients with and without history of recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 408–421. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp