RecentResearch-Risk_Factors_for_Dog_bites
Transcription
RecentResearch-Risk_Factors_for_Dog_bites
Recent Research: Risk Factors for Dog Bites Locksley L. McV. Messam DVM., PhD Assistant Clinical Professor (Voluntary Series) University of California Davis: School of Medicine Department of Public Health Sciences llmessam@ucdavis.edu Available online at www.sciencedirect.com The Veterinary Journal The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 www.elsevier.com/locate/tvjl The human–canine environment: A risk factor for non-play bites? Locksley L. McV. Messam *, Philip H. Kass, Bruno B. Chomel, Lynette A. Hart School of Veterinary Medicine, Department of Population Health and Reproduction, 1114 Tupper Hall, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA Accepted 18 August 2007 Abstract Few dog bite risk factor studies have been conducted. This veterinary clinic-based retrospective cohort study was aimed at identifying human–canine environmental risk factors for non-play bites in Kingston, Jamaica (660) and San Francisco (SF), USA (452). Data were analysed using modified Poisson regression with confounders selected using directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) and the change-in-estimate procedure. Dogs acquired for companionship were more likely (RR = 1.66; 95% CI 1.02–2.70) to bite than those acquired for protection. Routinely allowing a dog into the presence of visitors was also positively associated with it biting. A dog sleeping in a family member’s bedroom was a risk factor for biting in Kingston (RR = 2.54; 95% CI 1.43–4.54) but not in SF, while being able to leave the yard unaccompanied was a risk factor for biting in SF (RR = 3.40; 95% CI 1.98–5.85) but not in Kingston. Overall, dogs which were less restricted in their interactions with humans were at elevated risk for biting. An observed association with dog bites in one cultural setting might not exist in another. Ó 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Kingston; Jamaica; San Francisco; USA; Non-play; Dog; Bite; Risk factor Introduction Dog bites to humans are a worldwide problem (Chomel and Trotignon, 1992; Bhanganada et al., 1993; Thompson, 1997; Kumar, 1999; Chen et al., 2000; Ozanne-Smith et al., 2001; Frangakis and Petridou, 2003; Horisberger et al., 2004; Van Eeckhout and Wylock, 2005; Morgan and Palmer, 2007). In the United States there are 300–1000 bites per 100,000 persons per year (Beaver, 1997; Cornwell, 1997), and reports from Switzerland and Belgium have indicated national bite rates of 180 (Horisberger et al., 2004) and 900 (Gisle et al., 2002) per 100,000 per year, respectively. These figures are striking given that some studies suggest that far less than 50% of dog bites are reported (Beck and Jones, 1985; Chomel and Trotignon, 1992; Kahn et al., 2003; De Keuster et al., 2006). * Corresponding author. Present address: School of Veterinary Medicine, Department of Medicine and Epidemiology, 2108 Tupper Hall, University of California Davis, CA 95616, USA. Tel.: +1 530 752 3134; fax: +1 530 752 0414. E-mail address: llmessam@ucdavis.edu (L.L.McV. Messam). 1090-0233/$ - see front matter Ó 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.08.020 Research has largely focused on (1) the circumstances of incidents (Beck et al., 1975; Beck and Jones, 1985; Szpakowski et al., 1989; Mathews and Lattal, 1994; Thompson, 1997; Guy et al., 2001a; Frangakis and Petridou, 2003; Horisberger et al., 2004), (2) the characteristics of both biting dogs (Beck and Jones, 1985; Szpakowski et al., 1989; Gershman et al., 1994; Mathews and Lattal, 1994; Cornwell, 1997; Thompson, 1997; Guy et al., 2001b; Horisberger et al., 2004) and persons bitten (Beck and Jones, 1985; Bhanganada et al., 1993; Mathews and Lattal, 1994; Cornwell, 1997; Thompson, 1997; Savino et al., 2002; Horisberger et al., 2004), (3) the estimation of public health costs (Bhanganada et al., 1993; Weiss et al., 1998), (4) the pathological sequelae to attacks (Fishbein and Robinson, 1993; Mendez Gallart et al., 2002; Peters et al., 2004; Van Eeckhout and Wylock, 2005), and (5) wound care for the victims (Van Eeckhout and Wylock, 2005; Morgan and Palmer, 2007). Unfortunately, few investigators have employed a formal reference series in their studies (Gershman et al., 1994; Chen et al., 2000; Guy et al., 2001c; Drobatz and 206 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Smith, 2003; Reisner et al., 2005), and thus research to date has been of limited value in accurately identifying risk factors. In addition, because hospital based data formed the basis for inferences for all except a few studies, it is questionable whether these results are applicable to the general dog population. An analysis of a case series of 227 biting dogs obtained from a veterinary clientele has reported that 73%, 17.9% and 21.5% animals had bitten an adult (>18 years), a teenager (13–18 years), and children (612 years), respectively, at least once (Guy et al., 2001a). This stands in contrast to hospital data which suggest that children are over represented among dog bite victims (Ozanne-Smith et al., 2001). A consequence of the limited scope of dog bite research is the paucity of epidemiological evidence supporting the belief that a dog’s tendency to bite depends on an interaction of genetics (including sex), early experiences, later socialization and training, reproductive status, quality of ownership, supervision, and the potential victim’s behaviour (American Veterinary Medical Association, 2001). We conducted a retrospective cohort study in San Francisco (SF), USA, and Kingston, Jamaica (JA) to identify human–canine environmental risk factors for non-play bites to humans. Work by a few authors has suggested that both human–canine attitudes and interactions in the Caribbean differ considerably from those in the continental United States with some studies from Caribbean territories reporting that 56–70% of dogs are kept entirely outdoors (Fielding and Mather, 2001; Davis et al., 2007; OrtegaPacheco et al., 2007). In the US, this figure is 15–20% (American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, 2005–2006). In selecting divergent cultures with respect to attitudes to human–canine relationships, we hoped to identify, if present, heterogeneity by country. present, their names were ranked alphabetically and the first ranked chosen for participation. Exposure assessment For biters and non-biters the exposure period pertained only to the time period up to the incident and interview respectively. Exposure information included respondents characteristics, canine characteristics, factors related to owner-dog habitual interactions, and factors related to the dogs’ living environment (Fig. 1 and Table 1). Except for three agetime-related questions, all responses were categorical. Identical data collection protocols were employed in both cities and 1120 (667 in Kingston and 453 in SF) interviews were conducted with 41 (11 in Kingston and 30 in SF) persons electing not to participate. One San Franciscan and seven Kingstonian questionnaires were disqualified due to participant ineligibility. In constructing the final data set, the functional forms of ‘‘age at acquisition’’, ‘‘current age’’ and ‘‘length of ownership’’ were determined using fractional polynomials (Royston et al., 1999). To create the variable ‘‘Dog breed size’’ we used breed weights listed in dog breed standards (Hart and Hart, 1988; American Kennel Club, 1997). ‘‘I don’t know’’ responses were considered missing data. Outcome determination Materials and methods Outcome categories were based on answers to the following questions: (1) ‘‘Not during play, in the last two years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound?’’, (2) ‘‘Not during play, in the last two years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body or clothes with its teeth but not cause a wound?’’ and (3) ‘‘During play, in the last two years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound?’’ A dog was considered a non-play biter (hereafter a ‘‘biter’’) if the respondent said ‘‘yes’’ to either or both of questions 1 and 2 above, and a non-biter if the respondent said ‘‘no’’ to all three questions. We were primarily concerned with factors motivating a dog to attack and bite and assumed that the factors under consideration would motivate the attack but not determine whether injury occurred. When possible, it was noted whether the victim was a family member and/or lived in the same home as the dog though no distinction was made in later multivariable analyses. Dogs that had bitten during play were excluded from analysis. Study protocol Statistical methods The study was approved by the Human Subjects Review Committee at the University of California, Davis, USA. We used directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) to create a causal diagram (Fig. 1) defining a hypothesized causal web for dog bites. This master DAG provided the basis for confounder selection (Greenland et al., 1999), and a necessary set of confounders was identified for each exposure of interest (Fig. 2 and Table 2). Relationships in the causal diagrams were determined by subject matter considerations inclusive of results of previous studies. A modified Poisson regression (Zou, 2004) was used to analyze the data in SAS/STAT version 8.2. For each exposure of interest, variables included in the relevant DAG-based subset were used in analysis (Table 2). We employed the change-inestimate procedure (Greenland, 1989) using forward selection to select confounders from each DAG-based subset with a P10% change in the estimated relative risk (RR) required for retention in the model. For each exposure of interest, we excluded all observations that had missing values for any of the variables in the DAG-based subset of potential confounders (Table 2). Differences in RR between cities were investigated by including an interaction term comprised of the exposure of interest and city in the model. The term was retained in the model if statistically significant at the 5% level. Otherwise pooled RRs were calculated. Relative risks and their associated 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) were calculated using the ‘‘estimate’’ syntax in Proc Genmod (Table 2) (Spiegelman and Hertzmark, 2005). Study participants Study participants were clients in the waiting rooms of eight veterinary clinics participating from May 30th to August 9th 2003, in Kingston and from three veterinary clinics in SF from 20th October 2003 to 10th January 2004. Both sets of clinics were located within areas 65 square miles in their respective cities. All clinics were privately owned with caseloads of >90% companion animal (dogs and cats). Clients were eligible to participate only if they had a dog present at the time of the interview, had owned the dog for P24 h, lived 7 days a week in the same home as the dog, and were P18 years of age. Data collection Respondents were approached, following clinic registration but prior to being seen. The same interviewer administered the questionnaire to over 99% of respondents and dog-related information pertained only to the dog present. Whenever more than one dog was L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Hours per day locked up Can leave premises by itself Hours per day chained 207 Means of acquisition (Dog’s origin) Age at acquisition Current age Sight/Hearing problems Allowed around visitors/strangers Length of ownership Removed when fearful Non-Play Bite Neuter/Sex Status Reason for acquisition Sleep in bedroom Removed when growls Breed size based on breed weight standard Hours per day inside Country Housing (yard space) Breed Children (5-15 years) in home Fig. 1. Master directed acyclic graph (DAG) showing hypothesized causal web of dog bites. Bold lines represent causal relationships between exposures and non-play bites. Dotted lines represent causal relationships between exposures. Results Study population characteristics Data for 161 biters and 951 non-biters were analysed. Of these, 660 (59%) were from Kingston and 452 (41%) were from SF. Most respondents were female, though more so in SF (61%) than in Kingston (54%) and 17% of SF respondents vs. 23% in Kingston answered jointly with another person. Respondents in Kingston were slightly older (53% >40 years) than in SF (43% >40 years), but more frequently had a child aged 5–15 years living with them (35% vs. 12%). Twenty-one percent of Kingstonian dogs were born at home compared to 1% in SF and more Kingston (99%) than SF (64%) dogs had yard space at their disposal. Overall 70% (55% in Kingston vs. 99% in SF) of dogs with yard space spent some portion of the day inside the house (or apartment). In the SF sample, dogs born at home and dogs acquired for protection and other reasons excluding companionship both had prevalences of approximately 1%. The 2 year incidence of non-play bites was 12.5 (Kingston) and 17.4 (SF) per 100 dogs and proportionately more SF (91%) than Kingstonian respondents (76%) witnessed the incident. In Kingston, 34 (41%) bites broke the victim’s skin compared to 26 (33%) in SF. Of the victims for which we had relevant information, 57% (49% in Kingston vs. 62% in SF) were family members and/or lived in the same home as the dog. Overall 36% of victims were family members and/or lived with the dog, while 27% were not. Most persons in this latter category were familiar with both dog and owner. The relationship of the remaining 37% of victims was not specified. In Kingston, 76% and 64% of the bites sustained by family and non-family members, respectively, were witnessed by the respondent. In SF these percentages were 95 and 87, respectively. Dogs in Kingston were acquired at younger ages than in SF. The inter-quartile ranges (IQR) were 3–11 weeks and 8–12 weeks, respectively. Dogs in Kingston were also more recently acquired (IQR = 1 month – 1.5 years vs. 4 months – 6.75 years) and younger (IQR = 10 weeks – 2 years vs. 11 months – 7.5 years). Neuter status was markedly different between cities, with intact dogs accounting for 90% of Kingston and 22% of SF dogs. Canine characteristics Being born at the respondent’s home was inversely associated with biting. Compared to spayed female dogs, all other categories of dogs had elevated risks for biting (Table 2). Intact males were 1.68 (95% CI 1.05–2.71) times more likely to bite than castrated males, but 0.80 (95% CI 0.55–1.14) times as likely to bite as intact females. Both Rottweilers and Labradors had lower risks of biting compared to German Shepherds with RR = 0.38 (95% CI 0.13–1.09) and 0.24 (95% CI 0.07–0.82), respectively. Shih Tzus had similar risks of biting to German Shepherds (Table 2). A sight or hearing problem in the dog was inversely associated with biting. Environmental factors The presence of children (5–15 years) in the home had a slight positive association with dog bites (Table 2), while having yard space was inversely associated with biting 208 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Table 1 Distribution of biting and non-biting dogs by exposure status and city of origin: Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA Exposure Exposure categories Non-play bites c By characteristics of the respondents Respondent’s age (years) Non-bites c KGN n (%) SF n (%) KGN n (%)c SF n (%)c 620 21–30 31–40 41–50 51–60 61–70 P71 6 (7) 19 (23) 21 (26) 19 (23) 12 (15) 3 (4) 2 (2) 0 (0) 13 (17) 31 (40) 14 (18) 12 (15) 7 (9) 1 (1) 30 (5) 100 (17) 136 (24) 113 (20) 91 (16) 70 (12) 35 (6) 5 (1) 94 (26) 111 (30) 79 (21) 45 (12) 26 (7) 9 (2) Total: 1104 82 78 575 369 Respondent’s gender Male Female 32 (39) 50 (61) 28 (35) 51 (65) 270 (47) 308 (53) 150 (40) 223 (60) Total: 1110 82 79 578 373 Method of response Alone Spouse/companion helped Child helped Other individual helped 63 (77) 4 (5) 12 (15) 3 (4) 67 (85) 5 (6) 4 (5) 3 (4) 444 (77) 45 (8) 59 (10) 30 (5) 309 (83) 39 (11) 11 (3) 14 (4) Total: 1112 82 79 578 373 Born at home Acquired 12 (15) 70 (85) 1 (1) 78 (99) 125 (22) 452 (78) 2 (<1) 371 (99) Total: 1112 82 79 577 373 Male (intact) Male (castrated) Female (intact) Female (spayed) 40 (49) 4 (5) 34 (41) 4 (5) 14 33 11 20 298 (52) 19 (3) 221 (38) 36 (6) 47 (13) 150 (40) 29 (8) 145 (39) Total: 1105 82 78 574 371 German Shepherd Rottweiler Labrador Shih Tzu Other 9 (11) 4 (5) 1 (1) 9 (11) 59 (72) 2 (3) 0 (0) 2 (3) 2 (3) 73 (92) 29 (5) 33 (6) 4 (1) 24 (4) 488 (84) 2 (<1) 4 (1) 27 (7) 7 (2) 333 (89) Total: 1112 82 79 578 373 P9.0 kg (20 lbs) <9.0 kg (20 lbs) Unknown 32 (39) 35 (43) 15 (18) 50 (63) 23 (29) 6 (8) 257 (44) 110 (19) 211 (37) 232 (62) 119 (32) 22 (6) Total: 1112 82 79 578 373 Yes No 2 (2) 80 (98) 2 (3) 76 (97) 18 (3) 549 (97) 34 (9) 325 (91) Total: 1086 82 78 567 359 33 (40) 49 (60) 8 (10) 71 (90) 199 (34) 379 (66) 44 (12) 329 (88) Total: 1112 82 79 578 373 Yard space No yard space 80 (98) 2 (2) 47 (60) 31 (40) 569 (99) 6 (1) 240 (64) 133 (36) Total: 1108 82 78 575 373 19–24 13–18 7–12 1–6 0 34 (41) 4 (5) 6 (7) 11 (13) 27 (33) 56 (71) 13 (17) 9 (11) 0 (0) 1 (1) 114 (20) 35 (6) 26 (4) 132 (23) 269 (47) 271 (73) 59 (16) 29 (8) 10 (3) 4 (1) Total: 1110 82 79 576 373 By characteristics of the dog Dog’s origin Dog’s sex and neuter status Breed Dog breed size (based on breed standard) Sight/hearing problems By characteristics of the dog’s living environment Children (5–15 years) in home Yes No Housing Dog in house (h/day) (18) (42) (14) (26) L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 209 Table 1 (continued) Exposure Sleep in family member’s bedroom Dog chained (h/day) Dog locked up (h/day) Can leave premises unaccompanied By characteristics of human–canine interactions Major reason for getting dog Allowed into presence of strangers Dog removed/allowed to retreat when fearful Dog removed/allowed to retreat when growls a b c Exposure categories Non-play bites Non-bites KGN n (%)c SF n (%)c KGN n (%)c SF n (%)c Yes No 34 (42) 47 (58) 61 (78) 17 (22) 84 (15) 492 (85) 273 (73) 100 (27) Total: 1108 81 78 576 373 19–24 13–18 7–12 1–6 0 1 (1) 1 (1) 2 (2) 9 (11) 69 (84) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 79 (100) 20 (4) 6 (1) 24 (4) 19 (3) 508 (88) 0 (0) 1 (<1) 2 (<1) 7 (2) 363 (97) Total: 1111 82 79 577 373 19–24 13–18 7–12 1–6 0 1 (1) 3 (4) 9 (11) 5 (6) 63 (78) 1 (1) 2 (3) 9 (12) 7 (9) 59 (76) 49 (8) 27 (5) 64 (11) 27 (5) 410 (71) 3 (<1) 5 (1) 47 (13) 19 (5) 299 (80) Total: 1110 81 78 577 373 Yes No 16 (20) 65 (80) 8 (10) 70 (90) 95 (16) 480 (84) 4 (1) 369 (99) Total: 1107 81 78 575 373 Included protection (not companionship)a Included companionship (not protection)b All other combinations 16 (20) 38 (46) 28 (34) 1 (1) 56 (80) 22 (28) 144 (25) 194 (34) 239 (41) 5 (1) 260 (70) 108 (29) Total: 1111 82 79 577 373 Yes No Sometimes 47 (62) 15 (20) 14 (18) 68 (88) 3 (4) 6 (8) 239 (48) 206 (41) 56 (11) 353 (95) 12 (3) 7 (2) Total: 1026 76 77 501 372 Yes No Sometimes Situation never occurred 7 (9) 5 (6) 0 (0) 70 (85) 8 (10) 2 (3) 5 (7) 62 (80) 67 (12) 19 (3) 1 (<1) 485 (85) 49 (13) 15 (4) 1 (<1) 302 (82) Total: 1098 82 77 572 367 Yes No Sometimes Situation never occurred 25 (31) 34 (42) 4 (5) 18 (22) 8 (10) 21 (27) 7 (9) 41 (53) 110 (20) 97 (17) 13 (2) 345 (61) 16 (4) 40 (11) 8 (2) 308 (83) Total: 1095 81 77 565 372 Acquired for protection or for protection and other reasons excluding companionship. Acquired for companionship or for companionship and other reasons excluding protection. Percentages do not add to 100 due to rounding error. (RR = 0.86; 95% CI 0.57–1.30). Dogs that spent 1–6 h per day inside were no more likely to bite than those that were not allowed inside, but dogs that spent P7 h per day inside, were twice as likely to bite than those not allowed inside (Table 2). In Kingston, dogs that slept in a family member’s bedroom were more than twice as likely to bite, while in SF they were no more likely to bite than those that did not. As no chained dogs were biters in San Francisco it was impossible to estimate an SF specific RR for biting. When the data from both cities were pooled, chaining was weakly associated with biting and the pooled RR = 1.15 (95% CI 0.66–1.99) not substantially different from the Kingston specific RR = 1.28 (95% CI 0.71–2.31). Compared to dogs that were not confined, dogs confined for 1–6 and 7–12 h per day had increased risks of biting, while those confined 210 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Hours per day inside house Removed when growls Sight/hearing problem Reason for acquisition Removed when fearful Hours per day chained Breed size based on breed weight standard Hours per day locked up Housing (yard space) Breed Current age Country Respondent’s gender Respondent’s age Allowed around visitors/strangers Method of completion Non-Play Bite Fig. 2. Directed acyclic graph (DAG) used to select a sufficient set of potential confounders for control of the effect of ‘‘Allowed around visitors/ strangers’’ on dog bites (heavy arrows). Bold arrows show confounder (ovals) relationships. Dotted lines show relationships with variables on causal pathway (rectangles). for 13–18 and 19–24 h per day had decreased risks of biting (though the estimates in the latter two categories were very imprecise) (Table 2). Leaving the owner’s premises unaccompanied was strongly associated with biting in SF but not in Kingston. Human–canine interactions Dogs acquired or kept for reasons that included companionship but not protection were 1.66 (95% CI 1.02– 2.70) times as likely to bite as those acquired or kept for reasons including protection but not companionship. Allowing a dog into the presence of strangers or visitors to the home was also associated with an increased risk of biting (RR = 1.77; 95% CI 1.03–3.04). Not routinely removing (vs. always removing) a dog and/or allowing it to retreat was associated with an elevated risk of biting both when it was fearful (RR = 2.21; 95% CI 1.14–4.28) and when it growled (RR = 1.30; 95% CI 0.90–1.90). These two categories of dogs also had elevated risks of biting compared to those for which the situation never occurred (Table 2). Discussion Epidemiological studies on dog bites have differed in their sources of study populations. Various investigators have used geographic location (Gershman et al., 1994), place of occupation (Drobatz and Smith, 2003), registration status (Reisner et al., 2005) and presence at a veterinary clinic (Guy et al., 2001c). Both reported (Gershman et al., 1994; Drobatz and Smith, 2003) and unreported (Guy et al., 2001c; Reisner et al., 2005) bites have been used as outcomes. This study differs from previous studies in including bites which did not break the skin and in excluding play bites. We reasoned that from a point of view of risk factors for biting, all dogs that attack and make contact with the teeth belong to the same source population. Play bites were excluded on the grounds that they were likely to be aetiologically distinct from non-play bites. In using two questions in parallel to determine outcome status, we increased the sensitivity of detecting instances when a dog attempted to bite and made contact. The refusal rate of 3.7% is similar to that reported by Guy et al. (2001c) and confirms the effectiveness of using veterinary clients as a data source. The inverse association between being born at home and dog bites is consistent with a previous report that dogs bred at home were under-represented among dogs showing dominance aggression and social fears Serpell and Jagoe (1995). This association might be a manifestation of the effects of the origin of the dog and/or the age at which the dog was acquired. Some evidence for the effect of origin is provided by Serpell and Jagoe (1995) who reported that among dogs found unowned and those acquired from pet shops or breeders there was a higher prevalence of dominance aggression when compared to dogs bred at home. In this data set approximately a quarter of all canine participants were 11 weeks or older when acquired and the incidence of biting was higher with increased age at acquisition for dogs acquired at up to approximately 6 months of age and remained constant thereafter (L.L.McV. Messam, unpublished Ph.D. thesis). Previously reported associations between sex-neuter status and dog bites are inconclusive, with stronger associations reported for males (Gershman et al., 1994; Drobatz L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 211 Table 2 Adjusted relative risks (RR) for associations between selected variables and non-play bites, Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA Exposure By characteristics of the dog Dog’s origin Exposure categories Born at home Acquired RR 95% CI Variablesa causing change in RR P10%b <10%c 0.71e 1 0.41–1.25 A B, C, D, O 2.56 1.52 3.22 1 1.51–4.34 0.94–2.46 1.86– 5.59 A, F B, C, D, E, L, M N 2.27 0.86 0.54 2.09 1 1.33– 3.88 0.33–2.22 0.18–1.64 1.22–3.59 A B, C, D, K, M, O 0.41 1 0.15–1.09 F A, B, C D, 1.13g 1 0.80–1.58 A B, C, D 0.86f 1 0.57–1.30 A B, C, D K 1.97e 1.90e 2.18e 1.00e 1 1.17–3.32 0.99–3.62 1.18–4.02 0.51–1.96 A, F, H I B, C, D M, O 2.54h 1.11 1 1.43–4.54 0.67–1.85 F, H, P B, C, D, I, M,O 1.15 1 0.66–1.99 F A, B, C D, H, I, 0.44 0.93 1.15 1.71 1 0.07–2.76 0.35–2.46 0.72–1.83 1.02–2.86 F, U A, B, C D, H, I, M 1.04 3.40i 1 0.63–1.72 1.98–5.85 F B, C, D M, O, Q R Total: 1100d Dog’s sex and neuter status Male (intact) Male (castrated) Female (intact) Female (spayed) Total: 1026d Breed German Shepherd Rottweiler Labrador Shih Tzu Other Total: 1100d Sight/hearing problems Yes No Total: 1025d By characteristics of the dog’s living environment Children (5–15 years) in home Yes No Total: 1104d Housing Yard space No yard space Total: 1101d Dog in house (h/day) 19–24 13–18 7–12 1–6 0 Total: 1044d Sleep in family member’s bedroom Yes (KGN) Yes (SF) No Total: 1042d Dog chained/leashed (h/day) 1–24 0 Total: 974d Dog locked up (h/day) 19–24 13–18 7–12 1–6 0 M, U Total: 973d Can leave premises unaccompanied Yes (KGN) Yes (SF) No Total: 1042d (continued on next page) 212 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Table 2 (continued) Exposure Exposure categories RR 95% CI Variablesa causing change in RR P10%b By characteristics of human–dog interactions Major reason for getting dog Included protection (not companionship)g Included companionship (not protection)h All other combinations <10%c 0.82e 1.36e 1 0.49–1.38 0.99–1.99 1.77 1 1.03–3.04 F, H, I A, B, C D, J, O, M 0.78 1.71 1 0.46–1.30 1.06–2.76 F A, B, C, D, H, I, K, M, O, U 2.97 3.55 1 1.95–4.52 2.54–4.97 F, G, U A, B, C, D H, I, J, K, M, O, V A, B, C, D, K, O, Total: 1100d Allowed into presence of strangers Yes/Sometimes No Total: 948d Dog removed/allowed to retreat when fearful Yes No/sometimes Situation never occurred Total: 961d Dog removed/allowed to retreat when growls Yes No/Sometimes Situation never occurred Total: 892d A = Country, B = Respondent’s age, C = Respondent’s gender, D = Method of response, F = Current age, G = Dog’s sex/neuter status, H = Breed, I = Dog breed weight based on breed standards, J = Sight or hearing problem, K = Children (5–15 years) in home, M = Major reason for getting dog, O = Housing, P = Dog in house, Q = Dog chained, R = Dog locked up, U = Allowed in presence of strangers, V = Dog removed/allowed to retreat when fearful. a Both sets of variables together comprise hypothesized necessary set of confounders in causal web. b Retained in final model. c Not retained in final model. d Total number of participants (1112) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one of the variables in the necessary set of confounders. e Pooled RR heavily influenced by Kingston estimate. f Pooled RR heavily influenced by SF estimate. g Acquired for protection or for protection and other reasons excluding companionship. h Acquired for companionship or for companionship and other reasons excluding protection. i Interaction with country (p = 0.002). and Smith, 2003; Reisner et al., 2005), females (Guy et al., 2001c), intact (Gershman et al., 1994; Guy et al., 2001c), and neutered dogs as well (Drobatz and Smith, 2003; Reisner et al., 2005). These conflicting results are not surprising because the relationship between sex and aggression varies with aggression type (Borchelt and Voith, 1996) and age. The present study found intact dogs more likely to bite and that neuter status modified the effect of sex. These results concur best with results from the only other study to estimate the effect of sex and neuter status while controlling for age (Guy et al., 2001c). Both studies with reference series (Gershman et al., 1994) and those without (Beck et al., 1975; Szpakowski et al., 1989; Thompson, 1997; Ozanne-Smith et al., 2001; Mendez Gallart et al., 2002; Horisberger et al., 2004) have reported German Shepherds as having among the highest frequencies within samples of aggressive and biting dogs. While our results are consistent with those findings, to the best of our knowledge it has not been previously reported that Shih Tzus have similar risks for biting as German Shepherds and higher risks than Rottweilers and Labradors. We speculate that the inverse association between a sight or hearing problem and dog bites is due to reduced interaction with humans. Owners, as a precau- tion might restrict the interactions of these dogs with humans, recognising that the dog’s diminished vision and/or hearing might render it more uncertain and thus more likely to respond to human interaction with aggression. Previous studies found more than one child (Gershman et al., 1994) and the presence of teenagers (Guy et al., 2001c) in the home to be positively associated with dog bites. Although not contradicting those findings, the association seen in the present study was weak. An inverse association between bites and having yard space is consistent with the increased risk of biting among dogs allowed inside for more than 6 h daily compared to dogs not allowed inside. Though possibly consistent with a negative correlation between problem behaviour in dogs and the size of yard space at their disposal (Kobelt et al., 2003) there is no obvious explanation for the threshold effect at 6 h or why the RRs of biting are essentially equal for dogs inside for 7–12, 13–18 and 19–24 h per day. If this relationship is causal, it might indicate that >6 h per day inside is necessary to facilitate the development of certain human–canine interactions or dynamics which facilitate dog bites. A time dependent threshold effect would be consistent with territorial aggression being at the root of many of these incidents L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 as the dog would need to be established in the area for it to then become territorial (Moyer, 1968). However we did not have information on the proportion of dog bite incidents occurring inside and/or in the context of territorial aggression. Gershman et al. (1994) found that dogs that were chained vs. not chained had increased odds of biting. After adjusting for age, our results show a slightly increased risk for biting though a negative association with chaining is also compatible with the data. This RR for chaining was heavily influenced by the data collected in Kingston as there were no chained biters in SF. Compared to dogs that are never confined, only for dogs locked up for 1–6 h daily did our results show both a substantially increased risk of biting and exclude with high probability, a protective role of confinement. Similarly, we found proportionately more biters among dogs chained from 1–6 h daily than for any other time periods. If aggression is indeed caused by poor socialisation secondary to chaining or other confinement, as has been claimed (Lockwood, 1995), these results would indicate that only in the case of shorter daily periods of restraint or confinement do the aggression-promoting influences of restraint and/or confinement counteract their obviously beneficial effect on limiting dog bite opportunity. Dogs that sleep in a family member’s bedroom were at higher risk of biting only in Kingston, while dogs that are able to leave their owner’s premises unaccompanied were at higher risks for biting only in SF. It is noteworthy that the owners of both categories of dogs were a minority (18% in Kingston and 3% in SF) and in these regards, displayed ownership characteristics atypical of their environments. These results are an indication that circumspection should be exercised in generalising results from one cultural milieu to another. The finding that dogs kept for reasons including companionship but not protection were more likely to bite than those kept for reasons including protection but not companionship might seem surprising. Having a dog for reasons including protection but not companionship was positively associated with restraint (chaining, locking up) and negatively associated with the dog being allowed inside the house and with it being allowed around visitors or strangers to the home (results not shown). It is likely, therefore, that the effect of reason for acquisition on a dog’s likelihood of biting is a result of its effect on the frequency and nature of the dog’s interactions with humans. This is given credence by a number of Kingstonian participants reporting that they restricted their dog’s interaction with non-household members to enhance its capabilities as a watchdog. Less restriction may be proffered for why dogs allowed around visitors or strangers to the home had elevated risks of biting. The higher risks of biting among dogs that are not removed, left alone or allowed to retreat from a situation after they growl or show fear can be reconciled with fear and growling being possible warning signs of impending aggression. Though we are unable to tell if the dog bites 213 actually occurred within the context of such events, these results may indicate that dogs living in homes where they are not allowed to retreat after growling or showing fear were also likely to experience other circumstances in which their management or lack thereof induced them to bite. Our estimates would be biased if potential canine participants were censored prior to study enrolment but consequent to dog bites or to exposures related to dog bites. From 2000 to 2006, 9/15 veterinarians employed at the Kingston clinics euthanized six dogs because of aggression to humans, while approximately 2% of the SF dog population are confiscated/relinquished yearly due to aggression to humans (SF Animal Care and Control: personal communication, 2006). We therefore suspect that bias due to censorship was negligible in this study. Residual confounding of RR estimates is also possible due to absence of data on potentially confounding variables. If some exposures occurred consequent to dog bites, temporal bias could occur in which an apparent causal exposure-dog bite relationship may actually be a dog bite–exposure relationship. As information was garnered only by respondent recall we could not independently verify this. Also, if recall on the part of respondents was imprecise this could result in biased estimates due to misclassification of exposures. There are restrictions on the applicability of the results of this study to the general population of dog owners. Although the percentages of dogs taken to veterinarians is not known precisely for SF and Kingston, in the US approximately 84% of dog owners report taking their dog to the veterinarian within the previous year (The American Veterinary Medical Association, 2002). While no such information is available for Jamaica, this figure is likely to be similar to the 58% reported for New Providence, Bahamas (Fielding and Plumridge, 2005). It is likely therefore that the SF sample comprises a larger percentage of the dog owning population in SF than the Kingston sample. For both cities some exposures had low prevalence and thus low statistical power may have mitigated against us detecting differences in city specific RRs. In these circumstances the pooled RR estimates were heavily influenced by the city with higher exposure prevalence (Table 2 superscripts e and f). Nevertheless, this study contributes uniquely to the epidemiological literature on dog bites; it explicitly states its analytic assumptions regarding the causal web of dog bites; it examines exposures not previously studied; it is the first dog bite study to quantify associations in terms of relative risks, and the first to compare populations from different countries. This comparison of both cities has highlighted two issues worth considering. Firstly, important samplebased differences between the distributions of human–canine environments exist between cities. The low prevalence of dogs born at home, dogs acquired for reasons which included protection but not companionship, dogs always kept outdoors, dogs chained on an average day (in SF) and dogs without yard space at their disposal (in Kingston), suggest that causal pathways of dog bites involving these 214 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 environments might not be important in these cities. Secondly the differences between SF and Kingston specific relative risks observed for ‘‘sleeping in a family member’s bedroom’’ and ‘‘being able to leave the yard unaccompanied’’ suggest that an environmental risk factor may have different effects in different countries. Conclusions This study suggests that dogs acquired for companionship, dogs allowed into the presence of strangers and visitors to the home, dogs with fewer restrictions placed on their daily freedom of movement, and, possibly, interactions with humans, are at elevated risk for biting. This study also suggests that distinct differences exist between countries with regard to both the prevalence of certain human–canine environmental exposures and their effect on the risk of dog bites. For each cultural context, prevention strategies for dog bites would benefit from a consideration of which types of human–canine interaction or environmental factors frequently place dogs in situations in which they are more or less likely to respond by biting. Being modifiable, these factors are amenable to public health intervention in the form of public education. Acknowledgements The authors are grateful for the assistance of staff of the following veterinary clinics in data collection; All Creatures, All Pets, Animal Care, Chang’s Veterinary Clinic, JSPCA, Noah’s Ark, Phoenix Veterinary Clinic, Veterinary Medical (Kingston), Pet’s Unlimited, SFSPCA, the Avenues (SF). References American Kennel Club, 1997. The Complete Dog Book. Howell Book House, New York. American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, 2005–2006. APPMA national pet owners survey. American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, Greenwich, CT. Beaver, B.V., 1997. Human–canine interactions: a summary of perspectives. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 210, 1148–1150. Beck, A.M., Jones, B.A., 1985. Unreported dog bites in children. Public Health Reports 100, 315–321. Beck, A.M., Loring, H., Lockwood, R., 1975. The ecology of dog bite injury in St. Louis, Missouri. Public Health Reports 90, 262–267. Bhanganada, K., Wilde, H., Sakolsataydorn, P., Oonsombat, P., 1993. Dog-bite injuries at a Bangkok teaching hospital. Acta Tropica 55, 249–255. Borchelt, P.L., Voith V.L., 1996. Aggressive behavior in dogs and cats. In: Voith, V.L., Borchelt, P.L. (Eds.), Readings in Companion Animal Behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton, New Jersey, pp. 217– 229. Chen, S.C., Tang, F.C., Lee, H.S., Lee, C.T., Yen, C.H., Lee, M.C., 2000. An epidemiologic study of dog bites among postmen in central Taiwan. Chang Gung Medical Journal 23, 277–283. Chomel, B.B., Trotignon, J., 1992. Epidemiologic surveys of dog and cat bites in the Lyon area, France. European Journal of Epidemiology 8, 619–624. Cornwell, J.M., 1997. Dog bite prevention: responsible pet ownership and animal safety. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 210, 1147–1148. Davis, B.W., Alie, K., Fielding, W.J., Morters, M., Galindo, F., 2007. Preliminary observations on the characteristics of the owned dog population in Roseau, Dominica. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10, 141–151. De Keuster, T., Lamoureux, J., Kahn, A., 2006. Epidemiology of dog bites: a Belgian experience of canine behaviour and public health concerns. The Veterinary Journal 172, 482–487. Drobatz, K.J., Smith, G., 2003. Evaluation of risk factors for bite wounds inflicted on caregivers by dogs and cats in a veterinary teaching hospital. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 223, 312–316. Fielding, W.J., Mather, J., 2001. Dog ownership in the West Indies: a case study from the Bahamas. Anthrozoos 14, 72–80. Fielding, W.J., Plumridge, S.J., 2005. Characteristics of owned dogs on the island of New Providence, The Bahamas. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 8, 245–260. Fishbein, D.B., Robinson, L.E., 1993. Rabies. New England Journal of Medicine 329, 1632–1638. Frangakis, C.E., Petridou, E., 2003. Modelling risk factors for injuries from dog bites in Greece: a case-only design and analysis. Accident Analysis and Prevention 35, 435–438. Gershman, K.A., Sacks, J.J., Wright, J.C., 1994. Which dogs bite? A casecontrol study of risk factors. Pediatrics 93, 913–917. Gisle, L., Buziarsist, J., Van der Heyden, J., Demarest, S., Miermans, P.J., Sartor, F., Van Oyen, H., Tafforeau, J., 2002. Health Enquiry by Interview, Belgium, 2001. Epidemiology Department. Institute of Public Health. IPH/EPI Report No. 2002-22 (Written in Dutch). Greenland, S., 1989. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic analysis. American Journal of Public Health 79, 340–349. Greenland, S., Pearl, J., Robins, J.M., 1999. Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology 10, 37–48. Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001a. A case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour, and their victims. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74, 43–57. Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001b. Demographic and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74, 15–28. Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001c. Risk factors for dog bites to owners in a general veterinary caseload. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74, 29–42. Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A., 1988. The Perfect Puppy: How to Choose Your Dog by its Behavior. W.H. Freeman, New York. Horisberger, U., Stark, K.D.C., Rufenacht, J., Pillonel, C., Steiger, A., 2004. The epidemiology of dog bite injuries in Switzerland – Characteristics of victims, biting dogs and circumstances. Anthrozoos 17, 320–339. Kahn, A., Bauche, P., Lamoureux, J., 2003. Child victims of dog bites treated in emergency departments: a prospective survey. European Journal of Pediatrics 162, 254–258. Kobelt, A.J., Hemsworth, P.H., Barnett, J.L., Coleman, G.J., 2003. A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia – conditions and behaviour problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82, 137– 148. Kumar, S., 1999. New Delhi – when the threat to public-health is in the shape of a dog. Lancet 353, 2219. Lockwood, R., 1995. The ethology and epidemiology of canine aggression. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 131–138. Mathews, J.R., Lattal, K.A., 1994. A behavioral analysis of dog bites to children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 15, 44–52. L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 177 (2008) 205–215 Mendez Gallart, R., Gomez Tellado, M., Somoza Argibay, I., Liras Munoz, J., Pais Pineiro, E., Vela Nieto, D., 2002. [Dog bite-related injuries treated in a pediatric surgery department: analysis of 654 cases in 10 years]. Anales Espanoles de Pediatria 56, 425–429. Morgan, M., Palmer, J., 2007. Dog bites. British Medical Journal 334, 413–417. Moyer, K.E., 1968. Kinds of aggression and their physiological basis. Communications in Behavioral Biology. Part A 2, 65–87. Ortega-Pacheco, A., Rodriguez-Buenfil, J.D., Bolio-Gonzalez, M.E., Sauri-Arceo, C.H., Jimenez-Coello, M., Forsberg, C.L., 2007. A survey of dog populations in urban and rural areas of Yucatan, Mexico. Anthrozoos 20, 261–274. Ozanne-Smith, J., Ashby, K., Stathakis, V.Z., 2001. Dog bite and injury prevention–analysis, critical review, and research agenda. Injury Prevention 7, 321–326. Peters, V., Sottiaux, M., Appelboom, J., Kahn, A., 2004. Posttraumatic stress disorder after dog bites in children. Journal of Pediatrics 144, 121–122. Reisner, I.R., Houpt, K.A., Shofer, F.S., 2005. National survey of ownerdirected aggression in English Springer Spaniels. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 227, 1594–1603. Royston, P., Ambler, G., Sauerbrei, W., 1999. The use of fractional polynomials to model continuous risk variables in epidemiology. International Journal of Epidemiology 28, 964–974. Savino, F., Gallo, E., Serraino, P., Oggero, R., Silvestro, L., Mussa, G.C., 2002. Dog bites in children less than fourteen years old in Turin. Minerva Pediatrica 54, 237–242. 215 Serpell, J., Jagoe, J.A., 1995. Early experience and the development of behaviour. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 79–102. Spiegelman, D., Hertzmark, E., 2005. Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences. American Journal of Epidemiology 162, 199–200. Szpakowski, N.M., Bonnett, B.N., Martin, S.W., 1989. An epidemiological investigation into the reported incidents of dog biting in the city of Guelph. Canadian Veterinary Journal-Revue Vétérinaire Canadienne 30, 937–942. The American Veterinary Medical Association, 2001. A community approach to dog bite prevention. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 218, 1732–1749. The American Veterinary Medical Association, 2002. US Pet Ownership & Demographics Sourcebook. Membership & Field Services, American Veterinary Medical Association, Schaumburg, IL. Thompson, P.G., 1997. The public health impact of dog attacks in a major Australian city. Medical Journal of Australia 167, 129–132. Van Eeckhout, G.P.A., Wylock, P., 2005. Dog bites: an overview. European Journal of Plastic Surgery 28, 233–238. Weiss, H.B., Friedman, D.I., Coben, J.H., 1998. Incidence of dog bite injuries treated in emergency departments. Journal of the American Medical Association 279, 51–53. Zou, G., 2004. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. American Journal of Epidemiology 159, 702– 706. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Preventive Veterinary Medicine journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/prevetmed Risk factors for dog bites occurring during and outside of play: Are they different? Locksley L. McV. Messam ∗ , Philip H. Kass, Bruno B. Chomel, Lynette A. Hart School of Veterinary Medicine, Department of Population Health and Reproduction, 1114 Tupper Hall, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 30 December 2011 Received in revised form 10 April 2012 Accepted 13 May 2012 Keywords: Dog Non-play bite Bites during play Human–canine contact Risk factor a b s t r a c t The aim of this study was to determine whether the effects of selected human–canine interaction/environmental factors on bites occurring when the victim was and was not playing with the dog differed from each other. A veterinary clinic-based retrospective cohort study was conducted in Kingston, Jamaica (709), and San Francisco, USA (513) to compare the effects of selected exposures on non-play bites (161) relative to bites preceded by play with the dog (110) as reported by veterinary clients. Additionally, 951 non-biting dogs were used for a risk factor analysis of bites occurring during play. Using directed acyclic graphs and the change-in-estimate procedure to select and adjust for confounders, modified Poisson regression was used to estimate (a) the ratios of proportions of non-play bites out of all bites comparing exposed to unexposed dogs (proportionate bite ratios) and (b) risk ratios for bites occurring during play for each factor of interest. Proportionate bite ratios ranged from 0.84 to 1.29, with most 95% confidence intervals including one, thus implying a lack of specificity of effects of the examined factors on nonplay bites relative to bites occurring during play with the dog. Consistent with this lack of specificity, risk ratios for bites occurring during play were similar in magnitude and direction to risk ratios previously published for non-play bites using the same non-biting dogs as a reference group. No country-specific differences in proportionate bite ratios were detected. Each human–canine environmental factor showed similar levels of association with both types of bites. One possible explanation is that both types of bites have a common causal pathway leading from each factor up to the point of human–canine contact. If the human–canine contact then leads to either play or non-play interactions with dogs and subsequently to both types of bites, the presence of such a common pathway would make the factor non-specific to either type of bite. As some of the examined factors are associated with increased frequencies of both types of bites, this could explain high percentages of bites occurring during play with the dog as reported in various case series of dog bites. If so, dog bite prevention strategies targeting these factors will simultaneously reduce the incidence of both types of bites. © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction ∗ Corresponding author. Present address: Department of Public Health Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California Davis, 1616 Da Vinci Court, Davis, CA 95616, USA. Tel.: +1 530 754 8824; fax: +1 530 752 3118. E-mail address: llmessam@ucdavis.edu (L.L.McV. Messam). 0167-5877/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2012.05.007 Research on dog bites has revealed that they can result in disfiguring and psychologically scarring injuries (Mathews and Lattal, 1994; De Keuster et al., 2006; Kesting et al., 2006; Morgan and Palmer, 2007) sometimes ending in death (Sacks et al., 2000). These L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 injuries financially burden the public health system (Weiss et al., 1998), often hurt owner–dog relationships (Reisner et al., 1994; Hart, 1995; Burt, 1997), and have a negative impact on society’s view of dog rearing (Serpell, 1995; Hunthausen, 1997). Research focusing on the circumstances of dog bites mentions that some bites occur during play (Parrish et al., 1959; Thompson, 1997; Guy et al., 2001; Ozanne-Smith et al., 2001; Horisberger et al., 2004), which is both a type and a trigger of aggression (Beaver, 1983; Wright and Nesselrote, 1987; Landsberg et al., 1997). Overall, various studies report between 5 and 29% of dog bites occurring either as a sequel to or during play with the dog (Beck et al., 1975; Szpakowski et al., 1989; Shewell and Nancarrow, 1991; Ashby, 1996; Guy et al., 2001; Maragliano et al., 2007; O’Sullivan et al., 2008). One possible explanation for the high percentages of bites occurring during play among reported dog bites is that certain exposures might be risk factors both for bites occurring during play as well as for bites occurring outside of the context of play. Nevertheless, systematic study of bites occurring while the victim is playing with the dog has largely been neglected. Distinguishing between the two types of bites in studies will help to uncover the specificity, or lack thereof, of underlying associations with potential risk factors, help to clarify aspects of the causal web of dog bites and thus provide a basis for intervention aimed at reducing total dog bite incidence. It should also help explain the high proportion of bites occurring during play among the total numbers of bites. To determine whether the effects of selected human–dog environmental factors on the risk of nonplay bites differed from their effects on the risk of bites occurring during play with the dog, a veterinary clinic-based retrospective cohort study was conducted in both San Francisco (SF), USA, and Kingston, Jamaica, directly comparing both types of bites as reported by clients. The premise was that if a particular factor specifically causes (or prevents) non-play bites but not bites occurring during play with the dog, then there should be proportionately more (or fewer) non-play biters among all biting dogs exposed to that factor when compared to those unexposed to the factor. This translates to the ratio of proportions of non-play biters among exposed and unexposed biters being greater (or less) than one. Correspondingly, the associated 95% confidence intervals should predominantly include values greater (or less) than one. Additionally, the greater (lesser) the specificity of the effects, the further (closer) will be the 95% confidence limits from (to) one. Though little has been published on cross-cultural differences in dog keeping worldwide (Wan et al., 2009), previous research points to different cultural attitudes to dogs between the United States and the Caribbean (Fielding and Mather, 2001; Davis et al., 2007; Fielding, 2008). The goal of the bi-national component of the study was to assess whether the effects of the factors being studied differed in these two countries. 111 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Study protocol The protocol for this study constitutes a part of a large cohort study on human–animal interactions as risk factors for dog bites approved by the Human Subjects Review Committee at the University of California, Davis, USA. The methods used are identical to those previously described in detail elsewhere (Messam et al., 2008) and thus only a brief description is provided here. 2.2. Study participants and study sites Study participants were clients in the waiting rooms of eight veterinary clinics in Kingston, Jamaica, from May 30 to August 9, 2003, and from three veterinary clinics in San Francisco (SF), USA, from October 20, 2003 to January 10, 2004. Persons were eligible to participate if they had owned the dog for ≥24 h, were living 7 days a week in the same home as the dog, had the dog at the clinic during the interview, and were at least 18 years old. Data were collected by interviewer-administered questionnaire and canine information pertained to only the dog present at the time of the interview. Identical data collection protocols were observed in both countries. 2.3. Outcome determination A dog bite was defined as sudden pressure from a dog’s teeth to a part of a person’s body and/or clothing. Outcome categories were determined based on responses to the following questions: (a) “During play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound?”; (b) “Not during play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound?”; and (c) “Not during play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body or clothes with its teeth but not cause a wound?” The outcome was a bite occurring during play with the dog if the respondent answered “yes” to (a) but “no” to both (b) and (c), and a non-play bite if the respondent answered “yes” to (b) and/or (c) but “no” to (a). The dog was considered a nonbiter if the respondent answered “no” to all three questions. If the respondent answered in the affirmative to (a) and (b), (a) and (c) or to all three, the event that occurred earliest was chosen as the outcome. Using two questions in parallel to determine outcome status for non-play bites increased the sensitivity of detecting instances when a dog attacked and made contact with a person with its teeth. In defining bites occurring during play, outcomes included those circumstances in which the victim was playing with the dog but were restricted to those resulting in wounds to exclude cases of playful mouthing where a dog might grasp a person’s body without applying sudden pressure. The use of the term “play bites” has been avoided in preference to “bites occurring during play with the dog” as it was not ascertained whether the dog was playing at the time of the bite. The type of bite, where only the human is playing, is hereafter referred to as a “play” bite to distinguish it from 112 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 C Exposures acting both before and after contact Reason for acquisition Means of acquisition (dog’s origin) Breed Children (5-15 years) present in home Joint humancanine play Play bite Bites during play Human play Canine non-play aggression “Play” bite Canine non-play aggression Non-play bite Human-canine contact GENETIC FACTORS A Exposures acting before contact Neuter-gender status Sight/Hearing problems Housing (yard space) Hours per day inside house Hours per day chained Hours per day locked up Sleep in bedroom Allowed around visitors/strangers Can leave premises unaccompanied Point of action B Exposures acting after contact Removed when growls Removed when fearful Causal pathway Fig. 1. Diagram indicating hypothesized points of action of exposures along the causal chain of dog bites. a situation in which the dog was also known to be playing (Fig. 1). or a verbal stimulus from the person that elicits a play bow. (Play with the dog may also be a component of certain types of contact.) 2.4. Exposure assessment 2.5. Data analysis Exposures for this analysis were identical to those previously examined for their effect on the risk of non-play bites (Messam et al., 2008) (Fig. 1). Based on their hypothesized role in leading to dog bites, they were placed into three categories. First, certain exposures were thought to exert their effects only prior to the initiation of human–canine contact (Fig. 1, box A) and thought to determine contact. Second, other exposures were thought to exert their effects only subsequent to the initiation of human–canine contact (Fig. 1, box B) and third, some exposures were thought to exert their effects both prior and subsequent to the initiation of human–canine contact (Fig. 1, box C). Because the essential etiological distinction between both types of bites hinges on whether or not the event occurred while the victim was playing with the dog, it was assumed that the causal pathway from an exposure acting prior to human–canine contact would be the same for both types of bites up to the point of the human–canine contact, and then after contact, diverge before play with the dog occurred. Human–canine contact is used in this context to mean any habitual or intermittent human–canine interaction facilitated by occupation of the same space and time and need not involve tactile contact. For example, a playful interaction may start by touching the dog, a play bow by the dog, During data collection, 41 persons (11 in Kingston and 30 in San Francisco) elected not to participate. In total, 1235 (718 in Kingston, and 517 in SF) interviews were conducted. During data entry, thirteen questionnaires were found to be ineligible and thus data from 1222 participants (110 biters during play, 161 non play biters and 951 non-biters) were used for final data analysis. Modified Poisson regression (Zou, 2004) in SAS version 8.2 was used to estimate proportionate bite ratios for non-play bites and associated 95% CIs (Spiegelman and Hertzmark, 2005). The proportionate bite ratio for E− non-play bites is given by: PBRNP = PBE+ NP /PBNP , where E− PBE+ and PB were the proportions of non-play biters NP NP among all biting dogs for exposed and unexposed categories, respectively, of the factor. Functional forms of continuous variables were determined using fractional polynomials (Royston and Altman, 1994) and directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) were used to create a subset of potential confounders to control for each exposure of interest (Greenland et al., 1999) (Table 1). The owner’s age, gender and method of response were included in each of these subsets and for each exposure of interest, identical subsets to those previously reported for analyses of non-play bites L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 113 Table 1 Variables included in each hypothesized sufficient set of confounders during the modified Poisson regression procedure analysing risk factors for non-play bites relative to bites occurring during play. Exposures Code Sufficient set of potential confoundersb Dog’s combined sex and neuter status Sight/hearing problems Housing Dog in house (h/day) Dog chained (h/day) Dog locked up (h/day) Sleep in family member’s bedrooma Allowed in the presence of strangers Can leave premises unaccompanieda Reason for acquiring dog Dog’s origin Breed Children (5–15 years) living at home Dog removed/allowed to retreat when fearful Dog removed/allowed to retreat when growlsa G J O P Q R A, B, C, D, E, F, L, M, N A, B, C, D, F A, B, C, D, K A, B, C, D, F, H, I, M, O A, B, C, D, F, H, I, M, U A, B, C, D, F, H, I, M, U A, B, C, D, F, H, I, P, M, O A, B, C, D, F, H, I, J, P, M, O A, B, C, D, F, M, O, Q, R A, B, C, D, K, O A, B, C, D, O A, B, C, D, K, M, O A, B, C, D A, B, C, D, F, H, I, K, M, O, U A, B, C, D, F, H, I, K, M, O, U, V U M N H K V A, country; B, respondent’s age; C, respondent’s gender; D, method of response; E, age at acquisition; F, current age; G, dog’s sex/neuter status; H, breed; I, dog breed weight based on breed standard; L, length of ownership. a Not included in any sufficient set of potential confounders. b Initial set of potential confounders chosen for model selection. were used (Messam et al., 2008). Initially, to detect differences in PBRNP attributable to city of residence, interaction terms consisting of the factor of interest with country were added to each model. These terms were retained if the corresponding regression coefficients were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Finally, a set of confounders for each factor of interest was selected from its respective DAG via forward selection using the change-in-estimate method (Greenland, 1989) with a ≥10% change in the estimated PBRNP required for retention in the model. Finally, the above mentioned procedures were repeated in order to estimate risk ratios (RR) and 95% CIs for bites occurring during play with the dog and informally compare them to results previously reported for non-play bites with the same non-biting dogs as a reference group (Messam et al., 2008). a home with a child 5–15 years old. Of the victims for which relevant information was collected, 70% (91% of those bitten during play vs. 57% of those bitten outside of play) were family members and/or lived in the same home as the dog. Overall, 42% of victims were family members and/or lived with the dog, while 18% were not, and most persons in this latter category were familiar with both dog and owner. The relationships of the remaining 40% of victims with the dog was not specified. Twelve percent of biters in Kingston were born at their current home compared to 1% in SF, and more biters in Kingston (97%) had access to yard space than in SF (59%). In SF, more biters were neutered (56% compared to 7% in Kingston) and fewer (1%) were acquired for protection than in Kingston (17%). 3.2. Proportionate bite ratios for non-play bites 3. Results 3.1. Study population characteristics Distributions of the exposures of interest with respect to biters and non-biters by country are presented in Tables 2–4. Forty percent of all biters, bit while being played with by the victim, with the proportion in SF (43%) slightly greater than in Kingston (37%). Most respondents were female (58%), especially among Kingstonians (62%) and owners of non-play biters (63%). Eighty percent of owners of non-play biters and 72% of owners of dogs that bit while being played with answered questions without assistance from a spouse, child or other accompanying person and similar proportions of owners of dogs that bit during (89%) and outside of play (83%) witnessed the respective biting incidents. Dogs that bit during play with the victim, were younger than non-play biters with inter-quartile ranges of ages of 10 weeks to 1 year and 11 months to 6.5 years, respectively. Fifty-eight percent of both types of biters were acquired for companionship and not protection and approximately 25% of both types of biters lived in No interaction with country of residence was found for any of the factors examined, and thus PBRNP estimates based on the pooled data from both countries are reported. Estimates of PBRNP for non-play bites were close to one (ranging from 0.84 to 1.29) and associated 95% CIs tended to be narrow (Tables 5 and 6, Fig. 2(a)–(c)). Ninety-five percent confidence intervals for effects of the exposures thought to act (i) prior to contact and (ii) the exposures thought to act both prior to and subsequent to contact all included 1 (Tables 5 and 6, Fig. 2(a)–(c)). For example, having yard space was associated with a 3% increase in the proportion of non-play bites (PBRNP = 1.03; 95% CI: 0.93–1.14), showing that the data is compatible with both increases and decreases in the proportion of non-play bites as well. Proportionate bite ratios with 95% CIs excluding one were found only for exposures thought to act exclusively subsequent to contact. For instance, the proportion of non-play bites among dogs that were always removed after growling in the presence of visitors and strangers to the home compared to the proportion of non-play biters 114 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 Fig. 2. Proportionate bite ratio estimates and associated 95% confidence intervals for the effects on non-play bites of human–canine interaction/environmental factors hypothesized to act (a) only prior to human–canine contact, (b) both prior and subsequent to human–canine contact and (c) only subsequent to human–canine contact. L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 115 Table 2 Distribution of bites and non-bites by respondent’s characteristics and country of origin: Kingston (Kgn.), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA (2003–2004). Exposure Exposure categories Total Non-play bitesa Bites during play Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b Non-bitesa SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b Respondent’s age (years) ≤20 21–30 31–40 41–50 51–60 61–70 ≥71 Total 45 2 (4) 254 14 (6) 338 14 (4) 244 8 (3) 175 9 (5) 108 2 (2) 49 0 (0) c 1213 49 2 (4) 14 (6) 25 (7) 11 (5) 6 (3) 0 (0) 2 (4) 60 6 (13) 19 (7) 21 (6) 19 (8) 12 (7) 3 (3) 2 (4) 82 0 (0) 13 (5) 31 (9) 14 (6) 12 (7) 7 (6) 1 (2) 78 30 (67) 100 (39) 136 (40) 113 (46) 91 (52) 70 (65) 35 (71) 575 5 (11) 94 (37) 111 (33) 79 (32) 45 (26) 26 (4) 9 (18) 369 Respondent’s gender Male Female Total 533 18 (3) 689 31 (4) 1222c 49 35 (7) 26 (4) 61 32 (6) 50 (7) 82 28 (5) 51 (7) 79 270 (51) 308 (45) 578 150 (28) 223 (32) 373 Method of response 962 33 (3) Alone 5 (5) Spouse/companion helped 105 99 8 (8) Child helped 56 3 (5) Other individual helped 1222c 49 Total 46 (5) 7 (7) 5 (5) 3 (5) 61 63 (7) 4 (4) 12 (12) 3 (5) 82 67 (7) 5 (5) 4 (4) 3 (5) 79 444 (46) 45 (43) 59 (59) 30 (54) 578 309 (32) 39 (39) 11 (11) 14 (25) 373 a b c Messam et al. (2008). Row percentages. Not all percentages sum to 100 due to rounding error. Differences in totals reflect differences in the number of responses to each question. among dogs that had never growled in the presence of visitors and strangers was PBRNP = 1.29 (95% CI: 1.17–1.42). Using the same comparison group, the PBRNP was 1.26 (95% CI: 1.16–1.37) for dogs that were either never or only sometimes removed after growling in the presence of visitors and strangers to the home. 3.3. Risk factors for bites occurring during play with the dog The magnitudes and directions of the RRs for bites occurring during play were similar to the RRs previously reported for the same exposures on non-play bites (Messam et al., 2008). There was also substantial overlap of their respective 95% CIs with the major difference being in the precision of the estimates (Tables 5 and 6). 4. Discussion With few exceptions (Beck et al., 1975; Messam et al., 2008), bites occurring during and outside of play are routinely pooled together for analysis. To our knowledge, this is the first report of data collected with the express purpose of distinguishing between both types of bites. Previously, in case series conducted in urban areas, Ashby (1996) (5%), Beck (1975) (9.6%), Shewell and Nancarrow (1991) (12%), Szpakowski et al. (1989) (12.5%), Guy et al. (2001) (28.6%) and Horisberger et al. (2004) (14%), reported lower percentages of dog bites occurring (among total reported bites) while the victim played with the dog than the 40% reported in this study. This difference is difficult to explain as published reports yield little information on exactly how bites occurring during play with the dog are distinguished from bites occurring outside of the context of play. Others, Georges and Adesiyun (2008) (32%) and Parrish et al. (1959) (33%) reported estimates more similar to this study’s, but combined petting and playing with the dog into one exposure category. Thus, apart from differences in study populations, differences in case definitions are likely to explain these differences in estimates. While owners should be capable of accurately identifying and reporting human play, it has been suggested that they are unreliable in distinguishing between play and non-play signals in the dog (Moss and Wright, 1987; Mathews and Lattal, 1994; Maragliano et al., 2007; Reisner and Shofer, 2008; Tami and Gallagher, 2009). This might particularly be true if the dog’s behaviour changes during the interaction. Thus, apart from indicating a situation in which a true play bite occurred, i.e., while both dog and victim were playing (Fig. 1), a dog bite “during play with the dog” might have other interpretations: at the time of the bite, unbeknownst to the respondent, the dog may not have been playing. This may have been the case for the duration of the entire person–dog interaction or just during its latter stages. Alternately, the respondent might have known that the dog was not playing but may have misrepresented (intentionally or otherwise) the dog’s actions to minimize the circumstances of the bite. This is consistent with a tendency among some owners to “display a dog positivity bias” (Rajecki et al., 1998) and excuse perceived dog misbehaviour (Sanders, 1990; Rajecki et al., 1999). The low proportionate bite ratio estimates for non-play bites, coupled with 95% confidence limits close to the null (Tables 5 and 6, Fig. 2(a)–(c)), suggest that the selected human–dog environmental exposures are associated with very slight changes in the proportions of bites occurring either during or outside of the context of play with the dog. Consequently they are not specific to either of the two types of bites. In addition, this lack of specificity of effects is consistent in both countries, though for some factors, low numbers of exposed dogs in either of the countries might have contributed to a lack of power to detect interactions. Considering that some of the exposures examined in this analysis are both positively associated with bites 116 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 Table 3 Distribution of bites and non-bites by exposures of interest: Kingston (Kgn.), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF) USA (2003–2004). Exposure Exposure categories Total Kgn. n (%)b By exposures thought to act prior to contact Dog’s sex and neuter Male intact Male castrated status Female intact Female spayed Total 441 222 336 214 1213c Non-play bitesa Bites during play Non-bitesa SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b 24 (5) 1 (<1) 23 (7) 0 (0) 48 18 (4) 15 (7) 18 (5) 9 (4) 60 40 (9) 4 (2) 34 (10) 4 (2) 82 14 (3) 33 (15) 11 (3) 20 (9) 78 298 (68) 19 (9) 221 (66) 36 (17) 574 47 (11) 150 (68) 29 (9) 145 (68) 371 Sight/hearing problems Yes No Total 57 1138 1195c 0 (0) 49 (4) 49 1 (2) 59 (5) 60 2 (4) 80 (7) 82 2 (4) 76 (7) 78 18 (32) 549 (48) 567 34 (60) 325 (29) 359 Housing Yard space No yard space Total 1017 200 1217c 47 (5) 2 (1) 49 34 (3) 26 (13) 60 80 (8) 2 (1) 82 47 (5) 31 (16) 78 569 (56) 6 (1) 575 240 (24) 133 (67) 373 Dog in house (h/day) 19–24 h 13–18 h 7–12 h 1–6 h 0h Total 537 128 80 162 312 1219c 20 (4) 4 (3) 5 (6) 9 (6) 11 (4) 49 42 (8) 13 (10) 5 (6) 0 (0) 0 (0) 60 34 (6) 4 (3) 6 (8) 11 (7) 27 (9) 82 56 (10) 13 (10) 9 (11) 0 (0) 1 (<1) 79 114 (21) 35 (27) 26 (33) 132 (81) 269 (86) 576 271 (50) 59 (46) 29 (36) 10 (6) 4 (1) 373 Dog chained/leashed 1–24 h 0h (h/day) Total 111 1108 1219c 17 (15) 32 (3) 49 2 (2) 57 (5) 59 13 (12) 69 (6) 82 0 (0) 79 (7) 79 69 (62) 508 (46) 577 10 (9) 363 (33) 373 2 (3) 4 (10) 13 (9) 10 (12) 30 (3) 59 1 (2) 3 (7) 9 (6) 5 (6) 63 (7) 81 1 (2) 2 (5) 9 (6) 7 (9) 59 (7) 78 49 (84) 27 (66) 64 (43) 27 (33) 410 (46) 577 3 (5) 5 (12) 47 (31) 19 (23) 299 (34) 373 Dog locked up (h/day) 19–24 h 13–18 h 7–12 h 1–6 h 0h Total 58 41 150 81 887 1217c 2 (3) 0 (0) 8 (5) 13 (16) 26 (3) 49 Sleep in family member’s room Yes No Total 507 709 1216c 12 (2) 37 (5) 49 43 (8) 16 (2) 59 34 (7) 47 (7) 81 61 (12) 17 (2) 78 84 (17) 492 (69) 576 273 (54) 100 (14) 373 876 251 1127c 30 (3) 12 (5) 42 56 (6) 3 (1) 59 61 (7) 15 (6) 76 74 (8) 3 (1) 77 295 (34) 206 (82) 501 360 (41) 12 (5) 372 223 36 (16) 1002 13 (1) 1215c 49 54 (24) 5 (<1) 59 16 (7) 65 (6) 81 8 (8) 70 (7) 78 95 (43) 480 (48) 575 4 (2) 369 (37) 373 Yes/sometimes Allowed in the presence of strangers No Total Can leave premises unaccompanied a b c Yes No Total Messam et al. (2008). Row percentages. Not all percentages sum to 100 due to rounding error. Differences in totals reflect differences in the number of responses to each question. occurring during play and previously have been identified as positively associated with non-play bites (Tables 5 and 6) (Messam et al., 2008), this lack of specificity of effects would explain high proportions of bites during play in case series of dog bites. Growling in the presence of visitors/strangers to the home showed some specificity of effects on non-play bites compared to bites occurring during play. Because aggression in dogs is often preceded by growling (Messent, 1983; Wright and Nesselrote, 1987; Bradshaw and Nott, 1995; AVMA, 2001; Rooney et al., 2001), this finding is not surprising. However, the low specificity may have resulted from some growls having occurred while the dog was playing as opposed to being signs of aggravation. This study has primarily focussed on environmental factors which in exerting their effects prior to human–canine contact (Fig. 1, Groups A and C) are not immediate causes of dog bites. Nevertheless, they provide the environmental context for different types of dog bites to occur by determining the type of human–canine contact which precedes the bite. As has previously been noted (Westgarth et al., 2008), it is expected that the precise nature (duration and character) of each human–canine contact be exposure specific. For instance, the excitable prancing around of some dogs, while waiting to be unleashed or let out of a kennel and the fact that some dogs come running at the sound of their owner’s car entering the driveway or the back door opening, will all depend on the behaviour of the individual dog and human as well as the type of household it lives in. Thus, for example, human contact with a dog chained for a portion of the day will likely be different from human contact with a dog that usually sleeps in a family member’s bedroom. Possible sequential pathways for bites following human–canine contact are: (a) human–canine contact, human-play with dog, joint human–canine play and then a play-bite, (b) human–canine contact, human-play with L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 117 Table 4 Distribution of bites and non-bites by exposures of interest: Kingston (Kgn.), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF) USA (2003–2004). Exposure Exposure categories Non-play bitesa Total Bites during play Kgn. n (%)b By exposures thought to act both prior and subsequent to contact 173 6 (3) Protectionc Reason for acquiring 623 31 (5) Companionshipd dog 425 12 (3) Othere 49 Total 1221f Non-bitesa SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b Kgn. n (%)b SF n (%)b 1 (1) 44 (7) 16 (4) 61 16 (9) 38 (6) 28 (7) 82 1 (1) 56 (9) 22 (5) 79 144 (83) 194 (31) 239 (56) 577 5 (3) 260 (42) 108 (25) 373 Dog’s origin Born at home Acquired Total 144 4 (3) 1077 45 (4) 1221f 49 0 (0) 61 (6) 61 12 (8) 70 (6) 82 1 (<1) 78 (7) 79 125 (87) 452 (42) 577 2 (1) 371 (34) 373 Breed German shepherd Rottweiler Labrador Shih Tzu Other Total 47 3 (6) 43 2 (5) 40 0 (0) 47 4 (9) 1045 40 (4) 1222f 49 2 (4) 0 (0) 6 (15) 1 (2) 52 (5) 61 9 (19) 4 (9) 1 (3) 9 (19) 59 (6) 82 2 (4) 0 (0) 2 (5) 2 (4) 73 (7) 79 29 (62) 33 (77) 4 (10) 24 (51) 488 (47) 578 2 (4) 4 (9) 27 (68) 7 (15) 333 (32) 373 Child (5–15 years) living in home Yes No Total 311 17 (5) 911 32 (4) 1222f 49 10 (3) 51 (6) 61 33 (11) 49 (5) 82 8 (3) 71 (8) 79 199 (64) 379 (42) 578 44 (14) 329 (36) 373 By exposures thought to act only subsequent to contact Yes 141 5 (4) Dog removed/allowed to 58 3 (5) retreat when fearful in the No/sometimes Situation never occurred 1005 40 (4) presence of visitors or 1204f 48 Total strangers to home 5 (4) 7 (12) 46 (5) 58 7 (5) 5 (9) 70 (7) 82 8 (6) 7 (12) 62 (6) 77 67 (48) 20 (34) 485 (48) 572 49 (35) 16 (28) 302 (30) 367 Yes 116 4 (2) 247 13 (5) No/sometimes Situation never occurred 790 32 (4) 1203f 49 Total 3 (2) 10 (4) 46 (6) 59 25 (15) 38 (15) 18 (2) 81 8 (5) 28 (11) 41 (5) 77 110 (66) 110 (45) 345 (44) 565 16 (10) 48 (19) 308 (39) 372 Dog removed/allowed to retreat when growls in the presence of visitors or strangers to home a b c d e f Data reproduced from Messam et al. (2008). Row percentages. Not all percentages sum to 100 due to rounding error. Acquired for protection or for protection and other reasons excluding companionship. Acquired for companionship or for companionship and other reasons excluding protection. All other combinations. Differences in totals reflect differences in the number of responses to each question. dog, joint human–canine play, canine non-play aggression and then a “play” bite, (c) human–canine contact, humanplay with dog, canine non-play aggression and then a “play” bite and (d) human–canine contact, canine non-play aggression and then a non-play bite (Fig. 1). As noted for other outcomes (Terry et al., 2000), if each type of bite can result from the same type of intermediate human–canine contact, the exposure leading to that type of human–canine contact will have the same effect on each of the resulting types of bite. Consequently, no pathway leading from the human–canine contact would be expected to result in proportionately more (or fewer) non-play bites (or bites during play) among exposed compared to unexposed dogs. Thus, a high percentage of bites during play with the victim among case series of dog bites may be a result of a given exposure creating the environment in which both play and non-play interactions with a dog could occur. This in turn leads to both types of bites. Additionally, the effects of these exposures on bites occurring while both victim and dog were playing (play bites) should not differ from their effects on bites occurring when only the victim was playing (“play” bites). This is because play by the victim serves as a common intermediate along the pathway for both these types of bites (Fig. 1). In the presence of common intermediates, neither prevarication nor misclassification of dog bites, by respondents, is likely to be the fundamental explanation for the results regarding the effects of exposures in Groups A and C. Exposures acting subsequent to human–canine contact (Fig. 1, Group B) are more likely to be immediate determinants of dog bites and therefore have separate pathways to bites occurring in and outside of the context of play. Consequently, associations of these exposures with both types of bites are more likely to be distinct and there will be either proportionately more or fewer non-play bites among exposed vs. unexposed dogs. A 95% CI for the proportionate bite ratio which includes only values greater than one for dogs which growled in the presence of visitors and strangers to the home, is consistent with this expectation. The relative importance of the causal pathway from each exposure to a dog bite depends on the exposure’s prevalence. For example, low proportions of biters born at home and acquired for protection in San Francisco, suggest that causal pathways involving these exposures result in a lower frequency of bites in San Francisco than in Kingston. Similarly, low proportions of neutered biters and biters without access to yard space in Kingston suggest that there, 118 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 Table 5 Proportionate bite ratios (PBRNP,S ), risk ratios (RRs), 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) and confounders (CF) causing ≥10% change in PBRNP s and RRs for associations of selected factors with bites occurring during play, non-play bites, and non-play bites relative to bites occurring during play, Kingston, Jamaica and San Francisco (2003–2004). Exposure Exposure categories By exposures thought to act prior to contact Male (intact) Dog’s sex and neuter Male (castrated) status Female (intact) Female (spayed) Non-play bitesd Bites during play Non-play bites vs. bites during play RR 95% CI CF RR 1.09 1.00 1.08 1 n: 257a 0.99–1.20 0.90–1.10 0.98–1.20 F 4.15 1.87 4.76 1 n: 976b 95% CI CF RR 95% CI CF 1.87–9.20 0.83–4.21 2.10–10.81 A, F M, N 2.56 1.52 3.22 1 n: 1026c 1.51–4.34 0.94–2.46 1.86–5.59 A, F Sight/hearing problems Yes No 1.14 1 n: 260a 0.93–1.39 None 0.26 1 n: 979 b 0.04–1.85 F 0.41 1 n: 1025c 0.15–1.09 F Housing Yard Space No yard space 1.03 1 n: 269a 0.93–1.14 A, K 0.77 1 n: 1050b 0.48–1.24 A, B 0.86 1 n: 1101c 0.57–1.30 A Dog in house (h/day) 19–24 h 13–18 h 7–12 h 1–6 h 0h 0.94 0.90 0.95 0.92 0.85–1.04 0.78–1.04 0.82–1.10 0.78–1.09 None 3.40 3.21 4.04 1.25 1 n: 998b 1.59–7.28 1.40–7.39 1.63–10.00 0.52–3.01 A, C F, M 1.97 1.90 2.18 1.00 1 n: 1044c 1.17–3.32 0.99–3.62 1.18–4.02 0.51–1.96 A, F H, I 0.96 1 n: 248a 0.84 0.97 0.99 0.90 1 n: 247a 0.85–1.08 F A, F F F 1.15 1 n: 974c 0.44 0.93 1.15 1.71 1 n: 973c 0.66–1.99 0.62–1.13 0.85–1.10 0.90–1.09 0.80–1.02 2.34 1 n: 926b 1.25 1.34 1.27 3.31 1 n: 926b 0.07–2.76 0.35–2.46 0.72–1.83 1.02–2.86 F, U 1.06 – 1 n: 259a 0.99–1.15 None n: 262a Dog chained (h/day) 1–24 h 0h Dog chained (h/day) 19–24 h 13–18 h 7–12 h 1–6 h 0h Sleep in family member’s bedroom Yes No 1.45–3.77 0.48–3.28 0.59–3.03 0.77–2.07 2.06–5.32 A, F H, I U 1.04 – 1 n: 997b 0.70–1.56 F, P 2.54 (JA) 1.11 (USA) 1 n: 1042c 1.43–4.54 0.67–1.85 Ae , F H, P Allowed in the presence of strangers Yes/Sometimes No 1.08 1 n: 246a 0.95–1.22 A 1.37 1 n: 902b 0.79–2.40 F, I M 1.77 1 n: 948c 1.03–3.04 F, H I Can leave premises unaccompanied Yes 1.03 0.92–1.14 None 1.56–4.24 A, F M Ae , F 1 n: 258a 1.04 (JA) 3.40 (USA) 1 n: 1042c 0.63–1.72 1.98–5.85 No 2.57 – 1 n: 998b A, country; B, respondent’s age; C, respondent’s gender; F, dog’s current age; H, breed; I, dog breed weight based on breed standards; K, children (5–15 years) living at home; M, major reason for getting dog; N, dog’s origin; P = dog in house; U, allowed in the presence of strangers. a Number of observations in final model = number of participants (271) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. b Number of observations in final model = number of participants (1061) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. c Number of observations in final model = number of participants (1112) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. d Taken from Messam et al. (2008). e Interaction of exposure of interest with country. these exposures cause fewer dog bites than in San Francisco. A limitation of this study is the use of a simple causal model which does not specifically define human–canine contact for each of the examined exposures. Additionally the study focussed primarily on environmental exposures felt to exert effects early in the causal path to a dog bite. Recent analyses of cases series of dog bites suggest that immediately preceding the bites, a high percentage of child victims interacted, with the dog, in ways which might have triggered the incidents (Kahn et al., 2003; Reisner et al., 2007, 2011). Thus it is possible that an investigation of the effects of more proximate exposures might reveal specificity in their effects on non-play bites relative to bites occurring during play. This type of investigation is particularly important as it pertains to human–canine play because of its importance to the human–canine relationship in some cultures (Messent, 1983; Rooney et al., 2001; L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 119 Table 6 Proportionate bite ratios (PBRNP ), risk ratios (RR), 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) and confounders (CF) causing ≥10% change in PBRNP s and RRs for associations of selected factors with bites occurring during play, non-play bites, and non-play bites relative to bites occurring during play, Kingston, Jamaica and San Francisco (2003–2004). Exposures Exposure categories PBRNP 95% CI By exposures thought to act both prior and subsequent to contact Protection 1.03 0.91–1.17 Reason for acquiring Companionship 0.94 0.87–1.02 dog 1 Other a n: 267 Non-play bitesd Bites during play Non-play bites vs. bites during play CF RR 95% CI CF RR 95% CI CF None 0.64 2.02 1 n: 1049b 0.28–1.44 1.32–3.09 None 0.82 1.36 1 n: 1100c 0.49–1.38 0.99–1.99 None Dog’s origin Born at home Acquired 1.11 1 n: 267a 0.99–1.26 None 0.34 1 n: 1049b 0.13–0.93 A 0.71 1 n: 1100c 0.41–1.25 A Breed German shepherd Rottweiler Labrador Shih Tzu Other 1.06 1.05 0.84 1.06 1 n: 267a 0.92–1.22 0.83–1.32 0.66–1.66 0.92–1.22 None 1.75 0.80 1.19 1.46 1 n: 1049b 0.74–4.10 0.20–3.15 0.54–2.59 0.62–3.43 A, M 2.27 0.86 0.54 2.09 1 n: 1100c 1.33–3.88 0.33–2.22 0.18–1.64 1.22–3.59 A Children (5–15 years)c living in home Yes No 0.99 1 n: 267a 0.91–1.09 A 0.98 1 n: 1053b 0.62–1.53 A, D 1.13 1 n: 1104c 0.80–1.58 A 0.87–1.13 0.87–1.15 None 0.86 1.97 1 n: 961b 0.46–1.58 1.15–3.38 A, F M 0.78 1.71 1 n: 961c 0.46–1.30 1.06–2.76 F 1.17–1.42 1.16–1.37 None 0.80 1.49 1 n: 892b 0.37–1.72 0.95–2.36 A, F 2.97 3.55 1 n: 892c 1.95–4.52 2.54–4.97 F, G U By exposures thought to act only subsequent to contact 0.99 Yes Dog removed/allowed 1.00 No/Sometimes to retreat when fearful 1 Never occurred n: 245a Dog removed/allowed to retreat when growls Yes No/Sometimes Never occurred 1.29 1.26 1 n: 236a A, country; D, method of response; F, dog’s current age; G, dog’s sex/neuter status; M, major reason for getting dog; U, allowed in the presence of strangers. a Number of observations in final model = number of participants (271) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. b Number of observations in final model = number of participants (1061) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. c Number of observations in final model = number of participants (1112) minus the number of participants with missing data for at least one variable in the necessary set of confounders. d Taken from Messam et al. (2008). Westgarth et al., 2008). In those contexts, it would be important for dog owners to know which types of playful human–canine interactions might lead first to canine non-play aggression and consequently result in human injury. 5. Conclusions This study suggests that certain human–canine factors are equally associated with dog bites occurring during and outside of the context of play with the dog. To correctly interpret these results, it is necessary to consider that, in all likelihood, only some bites occurring during play occurred while the dog was actually playing. This is likely to be the case with dog bites reported to health authorities as well. Nevertheless, if a particular exposure creates a human–canine environment and consequent human–canine contacts resulting in either play or non-play interactions with a dog, it will be a common risk factor for all types of dog bites resulting from these two interactions. This lack of specificity of effects could explain high percentages of bites occurring while the victim played with the dog and suggests that dog bite prevention strategies targeting some of these factors could simultaneously reduce the incidence of both types of bites. The study also suggests that an investigation of factors acting during or subsequent to human–canine contact might reveal more specific exposure effects on non-play bites relative to bites occurring in the context of play with the dog. This will elucidate further aspects of the causal web of dog bites including the role of human–canine play in its etiology. Given the importance of human–canine play to many human–canine relationships, it is important to understand which types of human–canine play are likely to increase the frequency of dog bites and which are not, with the ultimate goal of reducing the prevalence of the former in favour of the latter. Apart from obvious public health benefits, this type of information will contribute positively to human–canine relationships and ultimately enhance canine welfare. 120 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107 (2012) 110–120 Acknowledgements The authors expressly acknowledge the assistance of staff of the following veterinary clinics in facilitating data collection: All Creatures, All Pets, Animal Care, Chang’s Veterinary Clinic, JSPCA, Noah’s Ark, Phoenix Veterinary Clinic, Veterinary Medical (Kingston, Jamaica), Pet’s Unlimited, SFSPCA and the Avenues (SF, USA). References Ashby, K., 1996. Dog bites. Hazard 26, 7–13. AVMA, 2001. A community approach to dog bite prevention. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 218, 1732–1749. Beaver, B.V., 1983. Clinical classification of canine aggression. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 10, 35–43. Beck, A.M., Loring, H., Lockwood, R., 1975. The ecology of dog bite injury in St. Louis, Missouri. Public Health Rep. 90, 262–267. Bradshaw, J.W.S., Nott, H.M.R., 1995. Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 115–130. Burt, M.R., 1997. Canine legislation: Can dogs get a fair shake in court? J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 210, 1139–1142. Davis, B.W., Alie, K., Fielding, W.J., Morters, M., Galindo, F., 2007. Preliminary observations on the characteristics of the owned dog population in Roseau, Dominica. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 10, 141–151. De Keuster, T., Lamoureux, J., Kahn, A., 2006. Epidemiology of dog bites: A Belgian experience of canine behaviour and public health concerns. Vet. J. 172, 482–487. Fielding, W.J., 2008. Attitudes and actions of pet caregivers in New Providence, The Bahamas, in the context of those of their American counterparts. Anthrozoos 21, 351–361. Fielding, W.J., Mather, J., 2001. Dog ownership in the West Indies: a case study from the Bahamas. Anthrozoos 14, 72–80. Georges, K., Adesiyun, A., 2008. An investigation into the prevalence of dog bites to primary school children in Trinidad. BMC Public Health 8, 85. Greenland, S., 1989. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic analysis. Am. J. Public Health 79, 340–349. Greenland, S., Pearl, J., Robins, J.M., 1999. Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology 10, 37–48. Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001. A case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour, and their victims. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 74, 43–57. Hart, L.A., 1995. Dogs as human companions: a review of the relationship. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 161–178. Horisberger, U., Stark, K.D.C., Rufenacht, J., Pillonel, C., Steiger, A., 2004. The epidemiology of dog bite injuries in Switzerland—characteristics of victims, biting dogs and circumstances. Anthrozoos 17, 320–339. Hunthausen, W., 1997. Effects of aggressive behavior on canine welfare. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 210, 1134–1136. Kahn, A., Bauche, P., Lamoureux, J., 2003. Child victims of dog bites treated in emergency departments: a prospective survey. Eur. J. Pediatr. 162, 254–258. Kesting, M.R., Holzle, F., Pox, C., Thurmuller, P., Wolff, K.D., 2006. Animal bite injuries to the head: 132 cases. Br. J. Oral Maxillofac. Surg. 44, 235–239. Landsberg, G.M., Hunthausen, W.L., Ackerman, L.J., 1997. Handbook of Behaviour Problems of the Dog and Cat. Butterworth Heinenann Oxford, Boston. Maragliano, L., Ciccone, G., Fantini, C., Petrangeli, C., Saporito, G., Di Traglia, M., Natoli, E., 2007. Biting dogs in Rome (Italy). Int. J. Pest Manag. 53, 329–334. Mathews, J.R., Lattal, K.A., 1994. A behavioral analysis of dog bites to children. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 15, 44–52. Messam, L.L.M., Kass, P.H., Chomel, B.B., Hart, L.A., 2008. The human–canine environment: a risk factor for non-play bites? Vet. J. 177, 205–215. Messent, P.R., 1983. Social facilitation of contact with other people by pet dogs. In: Katcher, A.H., Beck, A.M. (Eds.), New Perspectives on Our Lives with Companion Animals. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, pp. 37–46. Morgan, M., Palmer, J., 2007. Dog bites. Br. Med. J. 334, 413–417. Moss, S.P., Wright, J.C., 1987. The effects of dog ownership on judgments of dog bite likelihood. Anthrozoos 1, 95–99. O’Sullivan, E.N., Jones, B.R., O’Sullivan, K., Hanlon, A.J., 2008. Characteristics of 234 dog bite incidents in Ireland during 2004 and 2005. Vet. Rec. 163, 37–42. Ozanne-Smith, J., Ashby, K., Stathakis, V.Z., 2001. Dog bite and injury prevention—analysis, critical review, and research agenda. Inj. Prev. 7, 321–326. Parrish, H.M., Clack, F.B., Brobst, D., Mock, J.F., 1959. Epidemiology of dog bites. Public Health Rep. 74, 891–903. Rajecki, D.W., Rasmussen, J.L., Modlin, S.J., Holder, A.M., 1998. Dog bites boy: judgments of blame and shame. Anthrozoos 11, 66–73. Rajecki, D.W., Rasmussen, J.L., Sanders, C.R., Modlin, S.J., Holder, A.M., 1999. Good dog: aspects of humans’ causal attributions for a companion animal’s social behavior. Soc. Anim. 7, 17–34. Reisner, I.R., Erb, H.N., Houpt, K.A., 1994. Risk-factors for behaviorrelated euthanasia among dominant-aggressive dogs—110 cases (1989–1992). J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 205, 855–863. Reisner, I.R., Nance, M.L., Zeller, J.S., Houseknecht, E.M., Kassam-Adams, N., Wiebe, D.J., 2011. Behavioural characteristics associated with dog bites to children presenting to an urban trauma centre. Inj. Prev. 17, 348–353. Reisner, I.R., Shofer, F.S., 2008. Effects of gender and parental status on knowledge and attitudes of dog owners regarding dog aggression toward children. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 233, 1412–1419. Reisner, I.R., Shofer, F.S., Nance, M.L., 2007. Behavioral assessment of childdirected canine aggression. Inj. Prev. 13, 348–351. Rooney, N.J., Bradshaw, J.W.S., Robinson, I.H., 2001. Do dogs respond to play signals given by humans? Anim. Behav. 61, 715–722. Royston, P., Altman, D.G., 1994. Regression using fractional polynomials of continuous covariates—parsimonious parametric modeling. Appl. Stat. J. Roy. St. C. 43, 429–467. Sacks, J.J., Sinclair, L., Gilchrist, J., Golab, G.C., Lockwood, R., 2000. Breeds of dogs involved in fatal human attacks in the United States between 1979 and 1998. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 217, 836–840. Sanders, C.R., 1990. Excusing tactics: social responses to the public misbehavior of companion animals. Anthrozoos 4, 82–90. Serpell, J., 1995. From paragon to pariah: some reflections on human attitudes to dogs. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 245–256. Shewell, P.C., Nancarrow, J.D., 1991. Dogs that bite. BMJ 303, 1512–1513. Spiegelman, D., Hertzmark, E., 2005. Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences. Am. J. Epidemiol. 162, 199–200. Szpakowski, N.M., Bonnett, B.N., Martin, S.W., 1989. An epidemiological investigation into the reported incidents of dog biting in the city of Guelph. Can. Vet. J. 30, 937–942. Tami, G., Gallagher, A., 2009. Description of the behaviour of domestic dog (Canis familiaris) by experienced and inexperienced people. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 120, 159–169. Terry, M.B., Neugut, A.I., Schwartz, S., Susser, E., 2000. Risk factors for a causal intermediate and an endpoint: reconciling differences. Am. J. Epidemiol. 151, 339–345. Thompson, P.G., 1997. The public health impact of dog attacks in a major Australian city. Med. J. Aust. 167, 129–132. Wan, M., Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Champagne, F., 2009. A cross-cultural comparison of reports by German shepherd owners in Hungary and the United States of America. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 121, 206–213. Weiss, H.B., Friedman, D.I., Coben, J.H., 1998. Incidence of dog bite injuries treated in emergency departments. JAMA 279, 51–53. Westgarth, C., Pinchbeck, G.L., Bradshaw, J.W., Dawson, S., Gaskell, R.M., Christley, R.M., 2008. Dog–human and dog–dog interactions of 260 dog-owning households in a community in Cheshire. Vet. Rec. 162, 436–442. Wright, J.C., Nesselrote, M.S., 1987. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 19, 169–178. Zou, G., 2004. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am. J. Epidemiol. 159, 702–706. The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect The Veterinary Journal journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tvjl Age-related changes in the propensity of dogs to bite L.L.McV. Messam ⇑, P.H. Kass, B.B. Chomel, L.A. Hart School of Veterinary Medicine, Department of Population Health and Reproduction, 1114 Tupper Hall, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Accepted 15 January 2013 Keywords: Age Bite Dog Non-play Play a b s t r a c t This retrospective cohort study was aimed at describing the effects of age at acquisition, age, and duration of ownership of dogs on the risk of (1) bites during play and (2) non-play bites to humans. Data were collected on 110 dogs that had bitten during play with a person, 161 dogs that had bitten outside of play and 951 non-biting dogs from veterinary clients in Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA. Modified Poisson regression was employed to model the relationships of both types of bites to each variable separately. Effects of the variables on dog bite risk (1) during and (2) outside of play with the dog, differed from each other and by type of bite. Effects varied with the dog’s age and age-related associations were strongest in dogs younger than 1 year old. Ages at acquisition of dogs at highest risk for bites during play were substantially lower than those at risk for non-play bites. Ages and durations of ownership of dogs at highest risk for bites during play were also lower than those of dogs at highest risk for non-play bites. The propensity of a dog to bite changes as it ages and relationships between dog bites occurring during and outside of play and the dog’s age at acquisition, current age, and duration of ownership, differ from each other. Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction Dog bites are frequent sequelae to human–canine interactions (Overall and Love, 2001; The American Veterinary Medical Association, 2001). This has led to much interest in identifying human and canine risk factors for both bites and aggression to humans in many parts of the world (Cornelissen and Hopster, 2010; Feddersen-Petersen, 1994; Georges and Adesiyun, 2008; Gershman et al., 1994; Guy et al., 2001; Maragliano et al., 2007; Messam et al., 2008; O’Sullivan et al., 2008; Rosado et al., 2009; Wake et al., 2009). While age is accepted as a risk factor for canine aggression (Borchelt and Voith, 1996a,b; Lockwood, 1995; Overall and Love, 2001), little is known about the age or ages at which dogs are most likely to bite (Overall and Love, 2001). Similarly, while a few studies have examined the association between the age of dogs at their acquisition and subsequent aggression (Appleby et al., 2002; Hsu and Sun, 2010; Petersen and Deddens, 2006), there is still need for an understanding of how age at acquisition is related to dog bites. Knowledge of how a dog’s age at acquisition and current age are related to its aggressive behavior will help veterinarians to contextualize properly for dog–owners both human-directed aggression in newly acquired dogs as well as aggression-related behavior changes in dogs as they age. ⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 530 754 9516. E-mail address: llmessam@ucdavis.edu (L.L.McV. Messam). 1090-0233/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.01.024 To investigate the relationships of dog age-related factors to the risk of dog bites, a retrospective cohort study was conducted in Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA. The premise of the investigation was that if the effects of age-related factors on the risk of a dog biting were not constant over a dog’s lifetime, then age–time periods corresponding to higher or lower dog bite risks should be identifiable using analytic methods which permit data to define the shape of the age–time–dog bite relationship. The goals of the study were: (1) to describe the relationships of age at acquisition, dog age, and duration of ownership to the risks of bites occurring during and outside of play; (2) to identify the ranges of these variables corresponding to the highest risks of dog bites; (3) to identify the ranges of these variables during which the change in dog bite risk is greatest, and (4) for each variable, to compare its relationship to the risk of bites occurring during play to its relationship to the risk of non-play bites. Age, age at acquisition and duration of ownership were used as surrogate measures for (1) the cumulative effect of time-related social and biological changes occurring in the dog since its birth; (2) the effect of the timing of the most recent change in the dog’s ownership and living environment occurring during this process of change, and (3) the cumulative effect of these changes in the dog since the most recent change in its ownership and living environment, respectively. The bi-national component in this study provided an opportunity to investigate if the effects of dog age-related factors on dog bite-risk differed between the two countries. Previous research points to differences in cultural attitudes to dog rearing between 379 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 the United States and the Caribbean (Davis et al., 2007; Deddens and Petersen, 2008; Fielding and Mather, 2001). Materials and methods curred 1 year prior to the date of the interview. For dogs owned for less than 2 years, it was assumed that the bite preceded the day of the interview by a time period equal to half the duration of ownership. Exposures of interest Study protocol This study constituted a part of a cohort study on dog bites approved by the University of California Davis Institutional Review Board. Most aspects of the materials and methods are identical to those previously described in detail (Messam et al., 2008, 2012) and so only a brief description is provided here. Study participants Study participants were clients interviewed in the waiting rooms of eight veterinary clinics in KGN and three veterinary clinics in SF from May 2003 to January 2004. Clients were eligible to participate if they were at least 18 years old. Additionally, the dog in question had to be present at the time of the interview, owned for at least 24 h and living 7 days/week in the same home as the client. Whenever more than one dog was present, their names were ranked alphabetically and the first ranked chosen for participation. Outcome definition Dog bite categories were determined using the following questions: (a) During play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound? (b) Not during play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body with its teeth and cause a wound? (c) Not during play, in the last 2 years, did the dog ever hold onto or catch a part of any person’s body or clothes with its teeth but not cause a wound? The outcome was considered a bite during play if the respondent answered ‘yes’ to (a) but ‘no’ to both (b) and (c); a non-play bite if the respondent answered ‘yes’ to (b) and/or (c) but ‘no’ to (a), and a non-bite if the respondent answered ‘no’ to all three questions. Bites occurring during play were restricted to those resulting in wounds to exclude cases of playful mouthing where a dog might grasp a person’s body without applying sudden pressure (Messam et al., 2012). ‘Bite during play,’ instead of ‘play bite’ was used whenever the victim was playing with the dog at the time of the bite, as no distinction was made between when the dog was and was not playing. For dogs owned for 2 years or more, it was assumed that the dog bites oc- The exposures of interest were the dog’s age at acquisition, the dog’s current age, and the duration of ownership (Table 1), with each recorded both as categorical and continuous variables. In the absence of exact dates of birth and acquisition, the following decision rules were used: when an exact age or time period was given, that number was used; when a range was provided, the midpoint of the range was used, and when fractions of weeks, months and years were given, the value was rounded to the nearest week, month or year, respectively. If a respondent could not provide one of the age or time periods, the value was estimated using the values of the other two variables of interest if possible. When no age or time period was obtained from the respondent, the value was omitted. Twenty-eight per cent of the ages at acquisition and 18% of dog ages were estimated, respectively, for the continuous variable analysis. No estimation of age–time variables was performed when these exposures were recorded as categorical variables. Statistical analysis For analyses, modified Poisson regression (Zou, 2004) in SAS version 8.2 was used. Initially, each exposure of interest was used as a continuous variable to model play and non-play bites with functional forms (of the exposures of interest) separately, determined using fractional polynomials (Royston et al., 1999). This was necessary to allow the data, in addition to the statistical model, to define the shape of each age (–time) variable–dog bite relationship. Directed acyclic graphs (DAGs; Greenland et al., 1999) were used to choose a set of potential confounders of the relationships of age at acquisition to bites occurring during and outside of play. This initial set included city of residence, presence of yard space, source of the dog and reason for the dog’s acquisition (Table 2). A priori, no canine characteristics were believed to be confounders of the relationships of current age or duration of ownership to either type of bites, as both these variables represent slightly different surrogates for aging in the dog. Since aging is an inherent characteristic of the animal, its effect was not believed to be confounded by other individual-level characteristics or variables. For model selection, the change-in-estimate criterion (Greenland, 1989) was employed to select confounders from the DAG-based subset with a P10% change in the estimated RR required for a potential confounder to be retained in the model. To detect differences in RRs attributable to city of residence, an interaction term consisting of the exposure of interest and city was added to each model and retained if the corresponding regression coefficient was statistically significant (P < 0.05). If no statisti- Table 1 Distribution of biting and non-biting dogs by selected exposures and city of origin: Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA. Exposure Age at acquisition Exposure categories Birth 62 months >2 months to 66 months >6 months to 61 year >1 year to 62 years >2 years to 65 years >5 years Total Current age 62 months >2 months to 66 months >6 months to 61 year >1 year to 62 years >2 years to 65 years >5 years Total Duration of ownership 62 months >2 months to 66 months >6 months to 61 year >1 year to 62 years >2 years to 65 years >5 years Total a b Total 149 481 317 84 48 43 34 1156b 123 326 145 153 184 233 1164b 425 183 139 95 154 193 1189b Bites during play Non-play bites Non-bites KGN n (%)a SF n (%)a KGN n (%)a SF n (%)a KGN n (%)a SF n (%)a 5 (3) 35 (7) 8 (3) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 28 (6) 21 (11) 4 (5) 2 (4) 3 (7) 3 (9) 12 (8) 38 (8) 19 (6) 4 (5) 1 (2) 0 (0) 1 (3) 1 (<1) 22 (5) 27 (9) 9 (11) 8 (17) 5 (12) 6 (18) 129 (87) 242 (50) 111 (35) 24 (29) 10 (21) 7 (16) 3 (9) 2 (1) 116 (24) 131 (41) 43 (51) 27 (56) 28 (65) 21 (62) 48 61 75 78 526 368 7 (6) 28 (9) 9 (6) 3 (2) 1 (<1) 0 (0) 2 (2) 30 (9) 7 (5) 8 (5) 6 (3) 8 (3) 2 (3) 9 (3) 15 (10) 15 (10) 23 (12) 13 (6) 1 (<1) 4 (1) 9 (6) 16 (10) 22 (12) 26 (11) 106 (86) 200 (61) 65 (45) 58 (40) 53 (29) 52 (22) 5 (4) 55 (17) 40 (28) 53 (35) 79 (43) 134 (58) 48 61 77 78 534 366 27 (6) 13 (7) 4 (3) 3 (3) 1 (<1) 0 (0) 29 (47) 12 (20) 8 (13) 2 (3) 4 (7) 6 (10) 10 (2) 8 (4) 16 (12) 7 (7) 22 (14) 13 (7) 8 (2) 8 (4) 18 (13) 7 (7) 18 (12) 20 (10) 273 (64) 97 (53) 48 (35) 42 (44) 47 (31) 48 (25) 78 (18) 45 (25) 45 (32) 34 (36) 62 (40) 106 (55) 48 61 76 79 555 370 Row percentages. Not all percentages sum to 100 due to rounding error. Differences in totals reflect differences in number of responses to each question. 380 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Table 2 Distribution of biting and non-biting dogs by selected exposures and city of origin: Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA. Exposure Respondent’s age (years) Exposure categories 45 254 338 244 175 108 49 620 21–30 31–40 41–50 51–60 61–70 P71 1213c Total Respondent’s gender Male Female 533 689 1222c Total Method of response Alone Spouse/companion helped Child helped Other individual helped Dog’s sex and neuter status Male (intact) Male (castrated) Female (intact) Female (spayed) Housing Yard space No yard space 441 222 336 214 1213c Total 1017 200 1217c Total d Reason for acquisition Included protection Included companionshipe Included protection and companionshipf Love dogs Take care of dog Otherg Dog’s origin Born at home Obtained from friend/acquaintance SPCA or shelter Purchased Stray/found Total Total b c d e f g 962 105 99 56 1222c Total a Total 173 623 75 208 49 93 1221c 144 423 158 455 39 1219c Bites during play Non-play bitesa Non-bitesa KGN n (%)b SF n (%)b KGN n (%)b SF n (%)b KGN n (%)b SF n (%)b 2 (4) 14 (6) 14 (4) 8 (3) 9 (5) 2 (2) 0 (0) 2 (4) 14 (6) 25 (7) 11 (5) 6 (3) 0 (0) 2 (4) 6 (13) 19 (7) 21 (6) 19 (8) 12 (7) 3 (3) 2 (4) 0 (0) 13 (5) 31 (9) 14 (6) 12 (7) 7 (6) 1 (2) 30 (67) 100 (39) 136 (40) 113 (46) 91 (52) 70 (65) 35 (71) 5 (11) 94 (37) 111 (33) 79 (32) 45 (26) 26 (4) 9 (18) 49 60 82 78 575 369 18 (3) 31 (4) 35 (7) 26 (4) 32 (6) 50 (7) 28 (5) 51 (7) 270 (51) 308 (45) 150 (28) 223 (32) 49 61 82 79 578 373 33 (3) 5 (5) 8 (8) 3 (5) 46 (5) 7 (7) 5 (5) 3 (5) 63 (7) 4 (4) 12 (12) 3 (5) 67 (7) 5 (5) 4 (4) 3 (5) 444 (46) 45 (43) 59 (59) 30 (54) 309 (32) 39 (39) 11 (11) 14 (25) 49 61 82 79 24 (5) 1(<1) 23 (7) 0 (0) 18 (4) 15 (75) 18 (5) 9 (4) 40 (9) 4 (2) 34 (10) 4 (2) 14 33 11 20 578 373 (3) (15) (3) (9) 298 (68) 19 (9) 221 (66) 36 (17) 47 (11) 150 (68) 29 (9) 145 (68) 48 60 82 78 574 371 47 (5) 2 (1) 34 (3) 26 (13) 80 (8) 2 (1) 47 (5) 31 (16) 569 (56) 6 (3) 240 (24) 133 (67) 49 60 82 78 575 373 6 (3) 31 (5) 5 (7) 2 (1) 1 (2) 4 (4) 1 (1) 44 (7) 1 (1) 4 (2) 5 (10) 6 (16) 16 (9) 38 (6) 7 (9) 15 (7) 0 (0) 6 (6) 1 (1) 56 (9) 1 (1) 10 (5) 5 (10) 6 (6) 144 (83) 194 (31) 56 (75) 126 (61) 8 (16) 49 (53) 5 (3) 260 (42) 5 (7) 51 (25) 30 (61) 22 (24) 49 61 82 79 577 373 4 (3) 19 (4) 1 (1) 22 (5) 3 (8) 0 (0) 14 (3) 12 (8) 33 (7) 2 (5) 12 (8) 38 (9) 0 (0) 31 (7) 1 (3) 1 (1) 22 (5) 25 (16) 28 (6) 3 (8) 125 (87) 259 (61) 11 (7) 166 (36) 15 (38) 2 (1) 71 (17) 109 (69) 175 (38) 15 (38) 49 61 82 79 576 372 Messam et al. (2008). Row percentages. Not all percentages sum to 100 due to rounding error. Differences in totals reflect differences in the number of responses to each question. Acquired for protection or for protection and other reasons excluding companionship. Acquired for companionship or for companionship and other reasons excluding protection. Acquired for both protection and companionship. Acquired for reasons which did not include those listed above. cally significant interaction was detected, pooled RRs were calculated and city of residence retained in the final model if it caused a P10% change in the estimated RR. Overall, from 110 biters during play, 161 non-play biters and 951 non-biters, data for 1061 and 1112 dogs were used for bite during play and non-play bite analyses, respectively. Thus the same group of non-biting dogs was used for both analyses. Analyses, using data for the same dogs, were then repeated using age at acquisition and current age as categorical variables to compare results with the continuous variable analysis. Additionally, Spearman’s rank correlation (r) between age of dog and duration of ownership for both play and non-play bites was estimated. Finally, a sensitivity analysis was performed in order to determine the robustness of the choices of functional forms of the age–time variables (Table 3) by omitting the observations with estimated exposure values and repeating the fractional polynomial procedure. From final models, RRs and 95% CIs for comparisons of interest (Tables 4 and 5) were estimated using model-based variances and covariances (Table 3). From each of the six final models, a range of dog bite risks corresponding to the range of its respective exposure of interest was generated. In this way, age–time values corresponding to the 95th percentile of dog bite risks were obtained for each exposure of interest. Results Approximately 50% of KGN respondents were 40 years or younger, compared to 60% of SF respondents, with most respondents in both countries being female (Table 2). Compared to dogs in SF, dogs in KGN were acquired at a younger age (92% vs. 77% <6 months of age), were younger (53% vs. 19% <6 months old) and owned for less time (46% vs. 23% owned for <2 months; Table 1). The relationships between the age–time variables and dog bites were non-linear, with the exception of the relationship of duration of ownership to bites during play (Figs. 1 and 3). Age at acquisition Dogs that bit while being played with were acquired at a younger age than non-play biters. Medians (M) and inter-quartile 381 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Table 3 Final modified Poisson regression equations modeling the natural log relative risk, (ln(RR)), of (1) bites occurring during play and (2) non-play bites, as separate functions of age at acquisition (X1), current age (X2) and duration of ownership (X3). Exposure Outcome Age at acquisition Bites during playb,c Non-play bites Current age Bites during playc,d Non-play bitese Duration of ownership Bites during playc Non-play bitesc,f,g Regression equationsa lnðRRÞ ¼ 2:86 þ 0:52ð0:14Þ 1 X 1 þ1 10 0:05ð0:01Þ 2 X 1 þ1 10 0:55ð0:18ÞðCÞ 1 lnðRRÞ ¼ 0:99 0:07ð0:03Þ X 110þ1 lnðRRÞ ¼ 3:18 þ 1:40ð0:25Þ 1 X2 10 lnðRRÞ ¼ 1:11 þ 0:57ð0:11Þ ln lnðRRÞ ¼ 1:47 0:22ð0:06Þ X2 10 X3 10 þ 0:61ð0:13Þ 1 X2 10 ln X2 10 0:96ð0:19ÞðCÞ 2 0:22ð0:06Þ ln X102 0:79ð0:18ÞC lnðRRÞ ¼ 0:76 0:17ð0:20ÞC þ 0:07ð0:07Þ ln X3 10 2 2 0:10ð0:04Þ ln X103 þ 0:30ð0:10Þ ln X103 ðCÞ 0:02ð0:06Þ ln X103 ðCÞ a Equations are of the form ln(RR) = b1 + b2A2+ +bn, An, where bn = nth regression coefficient in the equation (from left to right), An = variable in the equation, standard errors of regression coefficients are in parentheses and cov(bi, bj) = covariance of the ith and jth parameters in the regression equation. b cov(b2, b3) = 0.002. c C = City (Kingston = 1, San Francisco = 0). d cov(b2, b3) = 0.032. e cov(b2, b3) = 0.006. f cov(b3, b4) = 0.0006, cov(b3, b5) = 0.005, cov(b3, b6) = 0.0006, cov(b4, b5) = 0.0006, cov(b4, b6) = 0.002, cov(b5, b6) = 0.001. g Interaction with city (C). P = 0.0047 and P = 0.76 for first and second interaction terms, respectively. ranges (IQR) were M=2 months (IQR 6 weeks to 3 months) and M=2.25 months (IQR = 6 weeks to 5.75 months), respectively. Dogs acquired at 6 weeks of age were at higher risk for biting during play than those born into their current owner’s home. For dogs acquired between 6 weeks and approximately 1 year of age, the risk of biting while being played with decreased slightly with increasing age at acquisition, but for dogs acquired older than 1 year of age, risks were essentially the same (Fig. 1a). Correspondingly, while a dog acquired at 6 weeks was 3.4 (95% CI: 1.3–8.9) times as likely to bite while being played with, than one born into its owner’s home, dogs acquired at 3 and 6 months were 2.6 (95% CI: 1.0–8.7) and 1.8 (95% CI: 0.7–4.9) times, respectively, as likely to bite during play than those born at their current home. Dogs acquired between 1 and 1.5 months of age had estimated risks of Table 4 Adjusted relative risks (RRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the associations between 6 month increases in age at acquisition, current age, and duration of ownership and (a) non-play bites and (b) bites occurring during play with the dog when the exposures are modeled as continuous variables, Kingston (KGN), Jamaica and San Francisco (SF), USA. Exposure b c d e f g h Non-play bites Bites during play RR 95% CI RR 95% CI 6 vs. 0 8 vs. 2 12 vs. 6 18 vs. 12 24 vs. 18 1.8 1.2 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.1–3.0 1.0–1.3 1.0–1.1 1.0–1.03 1.0–1.02 n = 1033h 1.8 0.5 0.8 0.9 0.9 0.7–4.9 0.4–0.8 0.7–0.9 0.8–0.9 0.9–1.0 n = 989h Current agec,d 8 vs. 2 12 vs. 6 18 vs. 12 24 vs. 18 30 vs. 24 3.8 1.6 1.2 1.1 1.0 2.1–6.9 1.3–1.9 1.1–1.2 1.0–1.1 1.0–1.1 n = 1029h 0.6 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 0.4–1.0 0.5–0.7 0.7–0.8 0.8–0.9 0.86–0.93 n = 986h Duration of ownership (SF)e 8 vs. 2 12 vs. 6 18 vs. 12 24 vs. 18 1.4 1.1 1.0 1.0 1.2–1.7 1.0–1.2 0.9–1.1 0.9–1.0 0.9f,g 0.9f,g 0.9f,g 0.9f,g 0.8–0.9 0.8–0.9 0.8–0.9 0.8–0.9 (KGN)e 8 vs. 2 12 vs. 6 18 vs. 12 24 vs. 18 2.3 1.3 1.1 1.0 1.2–4.2 1.1–1.7 1.0–1.3 0.9–1.2 n = 1029h Age at acquisition a Months a,b Unadjusted age at acquisition–non-play bite association. No variable caused P10% change in RRs. Age at acquisition–bites during play association adjusted for city. Unadjusted current age–non-play bite association. No variable caused P10% change in RRs. Current age–bites during play association adjusted for city. Duration of ownership–non-play bite association: interaction with city. Duration of ownership–bites during play association (linear model): RR. for each additional 6 months of ownership. Duration of ownership–bites during play association adjusted for city. Differences in totals reflect missing data for each exposure of interest. n = 986h 382 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Table 5 Adjusted relative risks (RRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the associations between age at acquisition and non-play bites, age at acquisition and bites occurring during play, current age and non-play bites and current age and bites occurring during play, when the exposures are modeled as categorical variables: Kingston, Jamaica and San Francisco, USA. Exposure Non-play bites Bites during play RR RR 95% CI 95% CI a,b Age at acquisition Birth >0 months to 62 months >2 months to <6 months P6 months to <2 years P2 years to <5 years >5 years 1 1.4 1.5 1.5 1.4 1.8 Current agec,d 62 months >2 months to <6 months 6 months to <2 years P2 years to <5 years >5 years 1 1.0 3.6 5.3 4.6 0.8–2.6 0.8–2.9 0.8–3.0 0.6–3.3 0.8–4.0 n = 1032e 0.2–4.4 0.9–14.6 1.3–20.9 1.2–18.3 n = 1026e 1 3.6 2.5 1.0 1.2 2.1 1 1.9 1.3 0.5 0.3 1.4–9.4 1.0–6.8 0.3–3.4 0.3–5.2 0.6–7.2 n = 989e 0.7–4.9 0.5–3.5 0.2–1.6 0.1–0.9 n = 984e a Age at acquisition-non-play bite association adjusted for city. Age at acquisition–bites during play association adjusted for city and reason for acquisition. c Unadjusted current age-non-play bite association. No variable caused P10% change in RR. d Current age–bites during play association adjusted for city. e Differences in totals reflect missing data for each exposure of interest. b biting during play in the 95th percentile of the range of risks for bites during play. The risk of non-play bites increased sharply with increasing age at acquisition for dogs acquired younger than 6 months old and then was constant (Fig. 1a). Thus, for dogs acquired older than 6 months of age, later ages at acquisition did not appreciably change the RR of a non-play bite (Tables 4 and 5; Fig. 2b) when compared to dogs acquired at 6 months. Compared to dogs born at the respondent’s home, dogs acquired at 2 months, 6 months and 1 year old were 1.6 (95% CI: 1.1–2.4), 1.8 (95% CI: 1.1–3.0) and 1.9 (95% CI: 1.1–3.3) times as likely to bite outside of play, respectively. Dogs aged 1.5 years or older when acquired had estimated risks of non-play bites in the 95th percentile of the range of non-play bite risks. Current age Dogs that bit during play were younger than non-play biters with M = 4 months (IQR = 10 weeks to 1 year) and M = 2.5 years (IQR = 11 months to 6.5 years), respectively. The risk of bites during play increased sharply until approximately 3 months of age and declined thereafter with increasing age (Fig. 1b). Thus, dogs that were 6, 12 and 24 months old were 0.7 (95% CI: 0.6–0.8), 0.4 (95% CI: 0.3–0.5) and 0.3 (95% CI: 0.2–0.4) times as likely to bite during play as a 3 month old puppy, respectively. Two to 4 month old dogs had estimated risks of biting during play in the 95th percentile of the range of risks for bites during play. Regarding non-play bites, the risk increased with age but at a diminishing rate from 2 months to approximately 3 years, after which it declined gradually (Fig. 1b). Correspondingly, while an 8 month old dog was 3.8 (95% CI: 2.1–6.9) times as likely to bite as a 2 month old dog, a 1 year old dog was 1.6 (95% CI: 1.3–1.9) times as likely to bite as a 6 month old dog. The most rapid increases in risk occurred in the age range 2–12 months (Figs. 1b and 2d), and 1–1.5 year old dogs had essentially the same risks of biting. Thus an 18 month old dog was just 1.2 (95% CI: 1.1– 1.3) times as likely to bite as a 12 month old dog (Table 4). Dogs that were 21–65 months old had estimated risks of non-play bites in the 95th percentile of the range of non-play bite risks. Age at acquisition vs. current age For dogs acquired before 4–6 months of age, the effect of increases in age at acquisition on the magnitude of the risk of nonplay bites was greater than the effect due to an increase in the dog’s age (Fig. 3). For dogs acquired after 6 months of age, this tendency was reversed (Fig. 3). Duration of ownership Dogs that bit during play were owned for a shorter period before the bite took place than non-play biters, with M=2 months (IQR = 3 weeks to 7 months) and M = 21.5 months (IQR = 8 months to 4.5 years), respectively. Dogs owned for 3 months or less had estimated risks of biting during play in the 95th percentile of the range of risks for bites during play. The risk of non-play bites, as a function of duration of ownership, showed a similar pattern to the risk of non-play bites as a function of current age. It was highest at 24–36 months and decreased gradually thereafter (Fig. 1b and c). There was evidence of differences in the association of duration of ownership on non-play bites between the two cities (Table 4 and Fig. 1c). After being owned for 6 months in SF, a further 6 months of ownership did not change the risk of biting. In KGN, this was the case after being owned for 1 year. In SF and KGN, dogs owned for 6–33 and 20–97 months, respectively, had estimated risks of non-play bites in the 95th percentile of the range of non-play bite risk. Correlation between current age and duration of ownership There were high correlations between the current age and duration of ownership for dogs that bit during play (r = 0.88; 95% CI 0.81–0.95) and for dogs that were non-play biters (r = 0.89; 95% CI 0.82–0.95). Continuous vs. categorized exposures The sensitivity analysis confirmed the choices of functional forms of the exposures of interest used for final models in the continuous variable analysis (Table 3). Results using the exposures of interest as categorical variables (Table 5) were similar to the continuous variable analysis. This was confirmed by the overlap in 95% CIs when the RR estimates for the continuous variable analyses calculated at the midpoints of each category were used for comparison with the categorical variable analyses (Fig. 4). Discussion In this study, age at acquisition, current age and duration of ownership have been used as surrogates for unspecified socio-biologic factors believed to be associated with dog bites. Thus, for instance, while canine age (which is simply the amount of time that has transpired since the birth of a dog), cannot in itself be a causative or protective factor with respect to dog bites, it is likely to be correlated with canine socio-biological changes which might be causative or protective. Most biters during play were acquired younger than 6 months old and bites occurring during play with the dog occurred relatively soon after acquisition (75% within 6 months of ownership). If bites during play are likely to occur soon after acquisition, the increase in bite risk observed for dogs acquired at 1.5–2 months of L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 383 Fig. 1. Comparisons of the risk of non-play bites to bites during play plotted as separate functions of a dog’s (a) age at acquisition, (b) current age and (c), duration of ownership. age compared to those born at home might be attributable to more physical interaction between the owner and a newly acquired puppy than between the owner and a puppy that he/she has seen develop from birth. Additionally, increased responsiveness by a 2 month old puppy, the eruption of its teeth, its increased strength and tendency to playfully mouth are possible reasons for increasing risks of biting while being played with during the first 1.5– 2 months after birth. Progressive decreases in the risk of bites occurring during play for dogs acquired older than 1.5–2 months of age and for dogs older than 3–4 months might be a consequence of a decreasing tendency of older dogs to play, or for their owners to play with them, or both. This is consistent with dog age being inversely associated with the frequency of owner–dog play (Rooney et al., 2000), as well as with a reported decline in social play in dogs after 6–7 months (Feddersen-Petersen, 1991). Dogs that never changed homes being at the lowest risk for non-play biting is consistent with previous observations that dogs bred at home (Serpell and Jagoe, 1995), or which remained longer (adopted at 60 days vs. 30–40 days) with litter mates (Petersen and Deddens, 2006), were under-represented among dogs with behavior problems. It is also consistent with a previous report which found that while there was an overall positive association between being born outside in a kennel, garage or barn (as opposed to in the residential part of the home) and stranger-directed aggression, there was no association observed among that subset of the same dogs acquired before 8 weeks of age (Appleby et al., 2002). Recently, somewhat contradictory findings have been reported: dogs acquired as puppies (vs. as adults) were at higher odds of showing stranger-directed aggression (Hsu and Sun, 2010). However, the authors explain that people might not adopt aggressive adult dogs and also that they might be unable to recognize signs of future aggressive tendencies in puppies. This study suggests that the association between age at acquisition and the risk of dog bites (both during and outside of play with the dog) primarily occurs over a limited time window, i.e. during the first 6–12 months of a dog’s life. This lends support to the view 384 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Fig. 2. Plots of the relative risk (RR) of bites for 6 month increases in ages at acquisition for (a) age at acquisition–bites during play; (b) age at acquisition–non-play bites, and for 6 month increases in current ages for (c) current age–bites during play, and (d) current age–non-play bites associations assuming both linear and non-linear (polynomial) relationships to dog bites. For example, in (a) the risk of biting while being played with for a dog acquired at age 12 months would be 0.8 times its risk of biting had it been acquired at age 6 months and in (d) a 12 month old dog’s risk of non-play biting is 1.4 times that of a 6 month old dog. Regardless of which age categories are compared, RRs are constant when linearity is assumed (dashed lines). that the timing of events in a dog’s life is an important determinant of dog bites (Lockwood, 1995; Stein et al., 1994; Wright, 1996) and that early experiences are more important determinants of adult dog behavior than later ones (Serpell and Jagoe, 1995). For instance, it is possible that the trauma of changing both home and owner can have negative consequences on canine development and behavior, manifesting itself in an increased risk of biting outside of play. It is logical that this could still contribute to aggression in non-play biters, even if human-directed aggression caused previous relinquishment. Previous studies assuming a constant effect of age on non-play bite risk have reported odds ratios of 0.96 (95% CI: 0.89–1.03) (Guy et al., 2001) and 1.1 (95% CI: 1.0–1.2) (Drobatz and Smith, 2003) for 1 year increases in age. When constant age effects were assumed, in this study, a similar result (RR = 1.1; 95% CI: 1.0–1.1) was obtained. This result suggests that for every 1 year increase in age, there is a 1.1-fold increase in the risk of biting, thus implying that the risk of dog bites increases by a constant multiple throughout the lifetime of the dog. These results differ from, and are less plausible, than the results obtained using fractional polynomials L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 385 Fig. 3. Plot comparing the effects of age at acquisition and current age on the risks of non-play bites. Fig. 4. Plots of estimated relative risks (RR) and 95% confidence intervals from categorical- and continuous variable analyses for (a) age at acquisition–bites during play; (b) age at acquisition–non-play bites; (c) current age–bites during play; and (d) current age–non-play bites associations. RR estimates for the continuous variable analysis are calculated at the midpoints of categories used for the categorical variable analyses. Straight lines used to connect point estimates from continuous variable analyses are used for comparison purposes only. In reality lines connecting these point estimates are not straight. 386 L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 (Tables 4 and 5, Fig. 2b), which suggest that the relationship between age and risk of dog bites varies with the dog’s developmental stage. The rapid increase in non-play bite risk observed in the first year of the dog’s life corresponds to the period of most rapid sensory, motor and social development (Estep, 1996). Further increases in non-play bite risk up to approximately 3 years of age, with little change for 2–6 year old dogs (Fig. 1c), are consistent with the appearance, within the first 3 years of age, of various types of canine aggression towards humans, as noted by others (Borchelt and Voith, 1996b; Luescher and Reisner, 2008). Comparisons between the effects of age at acquisition and age at the time of biting suggest that effects of re-homing are more important than correlates of age in determining non-play bites for younger dogs. However, as the dog matures, correlates of age become more determinant in whether a dog reacts by biting in a given circumstance (Fig. 3). The high positive correlation between duration of ownership and age for both play and non-play bites explains the similarity in their relationships to dog bite risk and supports a belief that both are proxies for similar developmental processes. If so, both duration of ownership and current age should be associated with each type of bite through similar mechanisms, even if these mechanisms differ between non-play bites and bites occurring during play. These results also suggest that changes in non-play bite risk with increasing duration of ownership are greatest during the first year (SF) to 1.5 years (KGN) of ownership. As no city-related differences in RRs were detected for current age, the observed city-related differences for duration of ownership might point to underlying qualitative differences in norms for human–canine interactions between the two countries. It is possible that there was some misclassification of the age– time exposures recorded. As most dogs were unregistered, documented dates of birth and acquisition were not available and owner recall remained the only practical source of age–time information. Thus, estimated values of age–time variables based on information provided by owners are not likely to be exact. While this misclassification of the exposures of interest could cause inaccurate RR estimates, consistency between the results from the categorical and continuous variable data analyses (Fig. 4a–d) inspire some confidence that the results obtained in this study are not artefacts of the estimated values of the exposures of interest. Nevertheless, greater importance should be attached to the overall relationships that the results describe, as opposed to the precise numeric values of RR estimates. Additionally, the low prevalence of dogs born in their current home in SF (<1%) suggests that comparisons involving dogs born at home were heavily influenced by KGN data (28%) and that the conclusions apply primarily to dogs from KGN. Nevertheless, these results might still be relevant to other US localities, as one study based on US national estimates reported that 26.5% of newly acquired dogs were born in the respondent’s home (New et al., 2004). Finally, breed-related differences are also likely to exist between groups of dogs, but this was not investigated as it would require much larger breed-specific sample sizes. Conclusions This study suggests that the associations of dog age at acquisition, current age and duration of ownership with the risk of bites occurring during and outside of play differ from each other; that these associations vary during the lifetime of the dog in an agedependent manner; that the association between these age–time variables and dog bites is strongest in the first year of the dog’s life, and that the dogs most likely to bite while being played with are younger than those most likely to bite outside of play. Using fractional polynomials to model these age-time characteristics as continuous variables has been a valuable step in providing an insight into how their relationships with dog bites change over the lifetime of a dog. Pending confirmation of these findings, it is to be hoped that veterinarians can use this information to help owners develop realistic expectations regarding changes in their dogs’ behavior over time. This is important, as incongruencies between dog–owner expectations and canine aggressive behavior sometimes culminate in relinquishment and/or euthanasia. Conflict of interest statement None of the authors of this paper has a financial or personal relationship with other people or organizations that could inappropriately influence or bias the content of the paper. Acknowledgements The authors thank the staff of the following clinics for assistance in data collection – All Creatures, All Pets, Animal Care, Chang’s Veterinary Clinic, JSPCA, Noah’s Ark, Phoenix Veterinary Clinic, Veterinary Medical (KGN), Pet’s Unlimited, SFSPCA, The Avenues (SF). References Appleby, D., Bradshaw, J., Casey, R., 2002. Relationship between aggressive and avoidance behaviour by dogs and their experience in the first six months of life. Veterinary Record 150, 434–438. Borchelt, P.L., Voith, V.L., 1996a. Aggressive behavior in dogs and cats. In: Voith, V.L., Borchelt, P.L. (Eds.), Readings in Companion Animal Behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton, New Jersey, pp. 217–229. Borchelt, P.L., Voith, V.L., 1996b. Dominance aggression in dogs. In: Voith, V.L., Borchelt, P.L. (Eds.), Readings in Companion Animal Behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton, New Jersey, pp. 230–239. Cornelissen, J.M., Hopster, H., 2010. Dog bites in The Netherlands: A study of victims, injuries, circumstances and aggressors to support evaluation of breed specific legislation. The Veterinary Journal 186, 292–298. Davis, B.W., Alie, K., Fielding, W.J., Morters, M., Galindo, F., 2007. Preliminary observations on the characteristics of the owned dog population in Roseau, Dominica. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10, 141–151. Deddens, J.A., Petersen, M.R., 2008. Approaches for estimating prevalence ratios. Occupational and Environmental Medicine 65, 486–501. Drobatz, K.J., Smith, G., 2003. Evaluation of risk factors for bite wounds inflicted on caregivers by dogs and cats in a veterinary teaching hospital. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 223, 312–316. Estep, D.Q., 1996. The ontogeny of behavior. In: Voith, V.L., Borchelt, P.L. (Eds.), Readings in Companion Animal Behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton, New Jersey, pp. 19–31. Feddersen-Petersen, D., 1991. The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species. Bonner Zoologische Beiträge 42, 97–114. Feddersen-Petersen, D., 1994. Social behavior of wolves and dogs. Veterinary Quarterly 16, 51–52. Fielding, W.J., Mather, J., 2001. Dog ownership in the West Indies: A case study from the Bahamas. Anthrozoos 14, 72–80. Georges, K., Adesiyun, A., 2008. An investigation into the prevalence of dog bites to primary school children in Trinidad. BMC Public Health 8, 85. Gershman, K.A., Sacks, J.J., Wright, J.C., 1994. Which dogs bite? A case-control study of risk factors. Pediatrics 93, 913–917. Greenland, S., 1989. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic analysis. American Journal of Public Health 79, 340–349. Greenland, S., Pearl, J., Robins, J.M., 1999. Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology 10, 37–48. Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001. Risk factors for dog bites to owners in a general veterinary caseload. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74, 29–42. Hsu, Y.Y., Sun, L.C., 2010. Factors associated with aggressive responses in pet dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 123, 108–123. Lockwood, R., 1995. The ethology and epidemiology of canine aggression. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 131–138. Luescher, A.U., Reisner, I.R., 2008. Canine aggression toward familiar people: A new look at an old problem. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 38, 1107–1130. Maragliano, L., Ciccone, G., Fantini, C., Petrangeli, C., Saporito, G., Di Traglia, M., Natoli, E., 2007. Biting dogs in Rome (Italy). International Journal of Pest Management 53, 329–334. L.L.McV. Messam et al. / The Veterinary Journal 197 (2013) 378–387 Messam, L.L., Kass, P.H., Chomel, B.B., Hart, L.A., 2012. Risk factors for dog bites occurring during and outside of play: Are they different? Preventive Veterinary Medicine 107, 110–120. Messam, L.L.M., Kass, P.H., Chomel, B.B., Hart, L.A., 2008. The human–canine environment: A risk factor for non-play bites? The Veterinary Journal 177, 205– 215. New, J.C., Jr., Kelch, W.J., Hutchison, J.M., Salman, M.D., King, M., Scarlett, J.M., Kass, P.H., 2004. Birth and death rate estimates of cats and dogs in U.S. households and related factors. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 7, 229–241. O’Sullivan, E.N., Jones, B.R., O’Sullivan, K., Hanlon, A.J., 2008. Characteristics of 234 dog bite incidents in Ireland during 2004 and 2005. Veterinary Record 163, 37– 42. Overall, K.L., Love, M., 2001. Dog bites to humans – Demography, epidemiology, injury, and risk. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 218, 1923–1934. Petersen, M.R., Deddens, J.A., 2006. Re: ‘Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences’. American Journal of Epidemiology 163, 1158–1159; author reply 1159–1161. Rooney, N.J., Bradshaw, J.W.S., Robinson, I.H., 2000. A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 66, 235– 248. 387 Rosado, B., Garcia-Belenguer, S., Leon, M., Palacio, J., 2009. A comprehensive study of dog bites in Spain, 1995–2004. The Veterinary Journal 179, 383–391. Royston, P., Ambler, G., Sauerbrei, W., 1999. The use of fractional polynomials to model continuous risk variables in epidemiology. International Journal of Epidemiology 28, 964–974. Serpell, J., Jagoe, J.A., 1995. Early experience and the development of behaviour. In: Serpell, J. (Ed.), The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 79–102. Stein, D.J., Dodman, N.H., Borchelt, P., Hollander, E., 1994. Behavioral-disorders in veterinary practice – Relevance to psychiatry. Comprehensive Psychiatry 35, 275–285. The American Veterinary Medical Association, 2001. A community approach to dog bite prevention. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 218, 1732–1749. Wake, A.A.F., Minot, E.O., Stafford, K.J., Perry, P.E., 2009. A survey of adult victims of dog bites in New Zealand. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 57, 364–369. Wright, J.C., 1996. Canine aggression: Dog bites to people. In: Voith, V.L., Borchelt, P.L. (Eds.), Readings in Companion Animal Behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton, New Jersey, pp. 240–246. Zou, G., 2004. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. American Journal of Epidemiology 159, 702–706.