Having a Ball: Increasing Exploratory Behavior in Pig-nosed
Transcription
Having a Ball: Increasing Exploratory Behavior in Pig-nosed
A Quarterly Source of Ideas for Enrichment Having a Ball: Increasing Exploratory Behavior in Pig-nosed Turtles By Natalie Cook, Animal Welfare Officer, Animal Concerns Research and Education Society, Singapore P Pig-nosed turtle ACRES (Carettochelys insculpta) The rattan ball containing lotus flower and mint leaves. ig nosed turtles are at risk from the pet trade and are listed as Vulnerable in the IUCN Red List. In Singapore, ACRES (Animal Concerns Research and Education Society) rescues animals from the illegal pet trade and houses them in the ACRES Wildlife Rescue Centre until they are able to be released back into the wild or to a sanctuary in their country of origin. The Centre currently houses a number of pig-nosed turtles and other aquatic turtles and terrestrial tortoises. The Centre relies on cost effective, non-time consuming enrichment. Our enrichment options for reptiles, including aquatic animals, are somewhat limited, so the aim of the enrichment we employ is not to provoke play behavior, like enrichment for big cats or primates, for example, but instead to encourage foraging or exploratory behavior. Baseline Observations Observations were taken over three months to observe one pig-nosed turtle’s behavior and activity levels. The pre-enrichment observations showed that the turtle, named Cyclops, was active (mainly swimming) for about half of the recorded time. The other time was spent either resting or out of sight in his shelter. As pig-nosed turtles are nocturnal animals, the goal of the enrichment was not to increase the daytime activity levels but instead to provide Cyclops with something to do during the times he was active. Cyclops showed interest in investigating novel objects within his enclosure: during the baseline observations, he would mouth any objects that had been moved during cleaning and he would also investigate objects that entered his enclosure such as a leaf falling into the water. Enrichment Device A rattan ball was filled with an open lotus flower and was stuffed with mint leaves. It was weighed down with a rock so it would hang down into the water. If it merely floated on the top, Cyclops may not have noticed it, and if it was too heavy and sank to the floor, it would not move around the enclosure as designed. The moveable design of the enrichment ball allowed Cyclops to utilize more of his enclosure space as he interacted with it. Enrichment Observations When the ball was first placed into the enclo- Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 ACRES tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr ACRES Cyclops cautiously approaches the ball. After becoming familiar with the ball, Cyclops spent time interacting with it. 2 sure, Cyclops appeared to be apprehensive to approach the ball. On the first day of the enrichment, he spent a long time simply watching it. It wasn’t until the second day that he approached the device. He would swim up to the enrichment, usually approaching one of the leaves protruding out, and nudge the ball with his nose, then swim backwards away from it. He seemed to be more interested when there were lots of leaves and petals sticking out, rather than the foliage being tightly packed inside. Over time, he appeared to grow less suspicious and would approach the ball immediately on the device being placed in the water. Behavior Changes During the enrichment, the amount of time spent resting had increased slightly (41.8% preenrichment to 51.5% during enrichment), but there was a notable increase in his exploratory behavior during his activity periods, from 1.8% before the enrichment to 29% during the enrichment. Cyclops was observed interacting with the device for short intervals, approaching slowly at first, observing the ball and then touching the ball with his snout. Although he didn’t manipulate the ball as much as we initially thought he might—we had thought he might even rip the ball to pieces—he still had a level of interaction with a novel object he did not have before. Conclusion The short term goals of the enrichment were achieved. During Cyclops’ periods of activity, swimming behavior decreased and exploratory behavior increased, as was hypothesised. Although Cyclops did not spend all his active time manipulating the device, after rest periods he would reinvestigate afresh. This enrichment was easy to make and affordable. The ball was able to be reused several times, which makes it ideal for a charitable organization with limited funds. A smaller ball may incite more interaction from the turtle, as the large size may have incited an initial fear response. Changing the scented herbs inside the ball for variety may also elicit a change in behavior. G Acknowledgements Thanks to the ACRES wildlife rescue and animal care team, in particular, Anbarasi Boopal, Director of the ACRES Wildife Rescue Centre, Hafz’zan Shah, Gary Chua, Jason Goh, and Tan En for their support and help. Also thanks to Cyclops, the pig-nosed turtle. The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Primate Enrichment in the Canopy, on the Ground, and Hidden Away By Guy Waddilove, Mammals South, Zoological Society of London, United Kingdom London Zoo W London Zoo Left: Log with drilled holes; right: pine cone feeder Roofed block feeder e use a variety of enrichment devices for the primates at the London Zoo. The devices featured here have been split into three categories, depending on the nature of foraging behaviour they are designed to encourage: canopy, ground, and hidden. Most are easy to install and clean, and they can be relocated or removed and reinstalled, which can renew original levels of interest and further enhance their enrichment value. From what we have seen, most of our monkeys seem prefer to take food from enrichment feeding devices. If simultaneously offered the same food items on shelves, the floor, or platforms, they invariably choose to plunder the enrichment devices before approaching the more obvious and easily obtained food. When using enrichment devices in a group situation, especially when one animal is particularly domineering, we use an appropriate number of feeders simultaneously to ensure enrichment for all group members. CANOPY FORAGING Suspended feeding devices encourage naturalistic foraging behavior “in the canopy,” which is where many primate species would naturally obtain the majority of their food. Drilled Holes The holes of drilled branches can be filled with seeds, raisins, nuts, grapes, or small pieces of the rusual diet wedged into them, or filled with soft foods such as flavored mashed potato, porridge, rice, or squashed banana. If a branch has two or more offshoot branches on one side only, or is awkwardly curved, the branch will rotate or swing in an elliptical manner when suspended vertically. This is a sensation the monkeys seem to relish. An elliptical branch can offer environmental, motile, non-food related enrichment, but can also be drilled to double up as a feeder. Pine Cone Feeders Some species of pine, such as Monterey, retain their large cones on the bough long after a branch has been cut from the tree. After being baked in an oven, these large cones open to provide a mass of narrow crevices, perfect for holding and hiding small morsels like seeds and raisins. The branches can be drilled through and then suspended to provide enrichment for smaller species or those inclined to forage thoroughly, such as squirrel monkeys or macaques. While easily refilled, they are liable to spill small food items onto the ground, making them suited for species that forage on the ground. If cones are kept in a fridge for a while or frozen, they close up again to firmly conceal any treats within, making them interesting to more destructive species, like mangabeys. However, these cones are a limited resource, since they are not easy to come by, especially those still attached to the branch. Roofed Block Feeders An overhanging roof provides rain cover and limits access to a block of wood with variously-sized holes drilled into it to various depths. Placement is important, with the device ideally unreachable from any surrounding branches or the ground. It encourages natural foraging for the entire group, since the roof requires some coordination and dexterity to negotiate, thus restricting the amount of food any individual can take away with them to consume.The monkeys take turns collect- The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 3 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Ker-Plunk Browse Feeders Rewards are obtained by the removal of sticks that suspend food items within a vertical or horizontal tube of bamboo or elongated wooden box. The ends of the sticks can be suffused with different scents, thus combining olfactory enrichment with a puzzle that yields a food reward. Enrichment value is enhanced for browsing primates such as colobus if stems of browse are used instead of plain sticks. Hopper Feeders Hoppers require patience and dexterity to extract seeds, nuts, raisins, etc. from pieces of bamboo or wooden boxes with a small opening made at their base,thereby prolonging feeding time. Hoppers can be fixed to an upright log with holes drilled into its sides, thus combining it with a drilled branch feeder. If suspended, this device can also have a platform affixed to its base, providing a handy perch and preventing reward items from easily falling to the ground. Suspended Platforms When placed away from other furnishings, a suspended platTop: Browse feeder; middle: form combines leaping and feeding opportunihopper feeder; ties for less dextrous, arboreal species, such as bottom: bamboo colobus or Diana monkeys. Stabilizing side ropes knot feeder. reduces the likelihood of the platform tipping, Opposite: platform feeder and a rim around the edge further prevents food and tube feeder items from easily falling off. Such platforms 4 provide the enclosure with an extra and unusual feeding station that moves and swings. GROUND FORAGING Often highly intelligent primates such as gorillas, macaques and mangabeys conduct a high proportion of their foraging at ground level in the wild. Scatter feeds encourage such behavior in captivity, while enrichment devices can present more challenges. Tube Toy Feeders This device is a hollow tube with several holes drilled towards either end of it, threaded with rope. It can be made from plastic tubes or small sections of bamboo (for species unable to shatter or splinter it). Food items or live food placed in the central portion of the tube are held by the criss-crossing rope, until the device is thrown about sufficiently to release food or the ropes are untangled and pulled out by the recipient animal. Such toys are especially suited to ground foragers but can also be suspended to require swatting or knocking to release the food reward. Drilled Logs Drilled logs provide ideal enrichment for ground foraging species. Variously-sized holes can be filled with foods like flavored mashed potato, rice, porridge, or squashed banana. If small holes are drilled at an angle outwards from the base of a single row of large holes, flavoured or treat-laden ice is prevented from slipping out of the holes once it begins to melt, making a longer-lasting log lollipop. Drilled logs encourage tool use and even tool manufacture when offered to gorillas, which use bark chips to scoop out the contents of the holes. The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 London Zoo London Zoo ing food from the device. It does not, therefore, require the deployment of multiple devices when used with groups dominated by one individual. Two central pillars inserted into the block of wood prevent the roof from tipping, while food being placed in the holes in the block stops it from being easily spilled. London Zoo tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Top and middle: compartment feeders; bottom: seesaw feeder Starting with the largest holes, they then strip the cret) only occasionally, though their effectiveness bark chips down to appropriately match the diam- as enrichment tools is increased by periodically eter of the next largest holes and continue adjustchanging their location within an enclosure. ing them to the holes of smallest diameter. The enrichment value of hidden reward feeders can be greatly enhanced by combining two of False Beaches the feeders into a single device. For example, two Some monkeys regularly beach-comb in the wild bamboo knot ropes hanging from either end of a or forage in swampy areas, such as Sulawesi compartmentalised plank or platform, or a seecrested macaques and white-naped mangabeys. saw suspended by a bamboo knot rope. By providing a large, sturdy, and shallow water Compartment Feeders container and putting Lengths of split bamboo or boxes fixed to a some food in it, or mixing suspended platform or wooden plank provide food items with a layer hidden crevices to explore. Compartments can of pea shingle below the be placed beneath planks or platforms and fitted water level, monkeys are with an internal divider to warrant investigaencouraged to use foragtion from both end. Construction does require ing behaviors. The use of screws, so this is unsuitable for species likely to pea shingle provides hid- chew wood, such as mangabeys. den reward enrichment, since monkeys will investi- Bamboo Knot Feeders gate even when empty, to Another form of hidden reward device, since see if there are more food sections of bamboo must be lifted up from a knot items. Consequently, false beneath them to release any food items hidden beaches can be left in situ within. This movement can easily be restricted indefinitely. by also tying a knot just above the bamboo, thus limiting the amount of food that will fall out each False Burrows time a length of bamboo is lifted. This idea can be A suitable length of combined with a platform, which, when placed at smooth plastic pipe can the top of the knotted rope, acts as a rain cover, or be cut at a shallow angle as a convenient perch if placed at the base. and blocked at the bottom end, then buried in the See-Saw Feeders ground within an encloUsing an elongated wooden box with holes near sure and secured by plac- the ends provides a feeder of nuts, seeds, grapes ing a heavy rock over the or live food combined with a suspended see-saw. disturbed substrate. Any This provides opportunities for cooperative play tactilely interesting article or food items can then (particularly for juveniles) and the use of hand/ be left in the false burrow for ground foraging spe- eye coordination through catching falling food cies to investigate. Such false burrows encourage or searching for it on the ground. Suspending the olfactory and auditory investigation (as opposed device with two central ropes restricts the degree to visual) of a hidden reward form of enrichment. of motion somewhat, which makes the device safer, particularly if hung clear of other branches INVESTIGATIVE FORAGING and low to (but clear of) the ground. ConstrucMany species, such as squirrel monkeys and tion requires screws so device is not suitable macaques, travel through forests investigating for for those species liable to chew wood, such as potentially hidden food items. Devices that may mangabeys. G contain hidden food encourage this type of natural foraging behavior and are investigated even when empty. Because of this, such devices can be left in enclosures indefinitely and filled (preferably in seThe Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 5 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Olfactory Enrichment for Carnivores By Juliana do Carmo Padilha, Graduation in Biology, UNESP, Rio Claro-SP, Brazil; Eliana Ferraz Santos, Department of Biology, Zoo “Bosque dos Jequitibás”, Campinas-SP, Brazil; Eleonore Zulnara Freire Setz, Laboratory of Mammal Ecology and Behavior, Department of Animal Biology, UNICAMP, Campinas-SP, Brazil O lfactory stimuli influence the behavior of many species and are considered effective agents of environmental enrichment for captive Top: Puma behaviors: a) smelling, b) cheek rubbing, c) forehead rubbing, d) gular rubbing, b) and c) “salivate” behavior. Middle: Maned wolf behaviors: a) digging, b) and c) cheek rubbing, and d) back rubbing or rolling. Bottom: Bush dog behaviors: a) smelling; b) dominant male (at left) urinating over bait. 6 animals. Applying them in enclosures encourages exploratory and scent marking behavior, while not interfering with feeding and other activities (Schapp, 2002). Carnivores communicate widely via persistent odors from feces and urine or secreted by specialized glands, which are spread in the environment when animals rub their body against the substrate. Olfactory signals are used in a variety of behavioral situations, but they often appear associated with the expression of dominance and territorial control in social communication. These signs are important to keep the integrity of the territory and inform age, gender, sex, and repro- ductive condition, dominance, and individual identity. Furthermore, they may work to aid navigation and advertise ownership, thereby reducing agonistic encounters (Ewer, 1998). Scent marks are commonly deposited on rocks, branches, and vegetation, along trails, and in shelters (Gorman, Trowbridge, 1989). In the present study, we used the scent baits “Canine Call” and “Pro’s Choice” as environmental enrichment for 69 individuals of 17 species of Neotropical carnivores, representing four families: Felidae, Canidae, Procyonidae, and Mustelidae, in three zoos of São Paulo State, Brazil (Zoo “Bosque dos Jequitibás”-Campinas, Municipal Ecological Park of Americana, and Municipal Zoo of Piracicaba). Each bait scent was placed by putting one drop on the ground and another on a platform, a tree branch, or trunk already in the enclosure. The behaviors were recorded by the animal focal sampling method (Altmann, 1974). Each animal behavior, along with its duration, was recorded in relation to the bait scent over an observation period of one hour conducted during daylight hours. One run was conducted on each bait scent with each animal. For most of the species examined, the scents showed great potential for environmental enrichment, since they elicited strong inspection and scent-marking behaviors. We recorded 21 scentrelated behaviors (Table 1). Some of these seemed to be exploratory, such as lick, nibble, and dig. The time spent investigating the scents varied greatly between different individuals of the same species. The scents did not elicit responses for kinkajou, crab-eating raccoon, river otter, grison, and giant river otter. These either showed no interest at all or only briefly smelled the scents. The interactions of felids with the scent were similar across species. The animals identified where the bait was placed and began rubbing their cheeks, foreheads, and gular regions over the spot. Furthermore, they rolled over the scent, scratched the trunk, and urinated on top. The The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr puma female salivated as she rubbed the scent. The males, in general, marked the scented area with urine spray together with scaping with hind Table 1: Behaviors displayed by the animals. legs. Responses lasted about 20 minutes, and after 30 minutes the animals lost interest. Canid responses were similar to those of felids and were characterized by rubbing the face and back in the scent, digging where the scent was placed, and urinating over it. Some demonstrated species-specific behaviors, such as the bush dog female that marked the scent placed on tree branches while doing a “handstand.” With canids, interest in scents generally lasted between 5 and 10 minutes. It is noteworthy that few procyonids and mustelids responded to the scent. Among procyonids, only coatis showed some interest in the scent, and that was the species that interacted for the longest time, approximately 45 minutes. Coatis presented only two specific behaviors. Both male and female rubbed the forepaws in the ground moistened by the scent and spread it on the inside of the tail. The males also rubbed their testicles on the scent. Among mustelids, only the tayra responded to the scent, but for a short period of time, around 5 minutes. The animal rubbed its cheeks, defecated, and urinated on the scent. The results from this study support the potential use of olfactory stimuli for environmental enrichment of a number of species of carnivores. Many species spent substantial amounts of time investigating the bait scents, which stimulated the animals to exhibit a number of scent-marking behaviors previously described in the literature for wild animals. References Altmann, J. Observation study of behavior: sampling methods. Behavior, v.49, p.227-267, 1974. Ewer, R.F. The Carnivores. Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1998, 500 p. Gorman, l.m.; Trowbridge, b.j. The Role of Odor in the Social Lives of Carnivores. In: Gittleman, j. (Org.). Carnivore Behavior, Ecology and Evolution. Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1989. p. 57-89. Schapp, D. Enriching the Devil: The Tasmanian Devil. The Shape of Enrichment, v. 11, n.1, p.1-4, 2002. Female coatis rubbing the soil with scent on their tail. The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 7 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Aardvarks Live to Dig and Eat: Give Them a Little Fun By Miklos Faludi, Keeper, and Katalin Toth, Trainee, Zoo Budapest, Hungary I Zoo Budapest n May 2011, Budapest Zoo in Hungary received 1.1 aardvarks for breeding purposes. Their arrival was very special, as the last time Budapest Zoo had aardvarks was in 1912. The aardvarks Zoo Budapest An aardvark uses the feeder ball. arrived from Zoo Gyor in Hungary, where the male was born in May 2010. The female came from Tanzania, and came to us after a quarantine period at Zoo Gyor. Her birth date is estimated to be in February 2010. Both of the animals are very young, curious, and active. We built a reverse day/night enclosure to show them to the public as active animals in the dark. The night cycle is from 7:30 a.m. to 7:30 p.m. The enclosure is approximately 40 square meters. It has 60 to 70 centimeters of deep New items in the aardvark exhibit. sandy soil, which the aardvarks can dig in as much as they want. The male was used to humans, and he moved around more and allowed himself to be touched 8 immediately. The female was shy and seemed stressed. She started to pace by the walls. After the first few days, the female started to relax, and on the fifth day she left her sleeping box, the crate she had been transported in, and dug herself a hole in a corner of the exhibit. Since then, she sleeps and rests there. The male still uses his own box. After the first week, both animals moved around more and more while exploring the enclosure. The enclosure had only very basic enrichment: thick, sandy soil and big, heavy logs, which soon started to disappear in the sand due to the aardvarks’ digging activity. The exhibit had started to look poor and bare, and these young animals did not have not much to do. The highlight of their days was the feeding three times a day. After eating, they moved around the exhibit a few times and sniffed around. They then disappeared into their dirt hole or box for sleeping until the next feeding. This was not good for the animals, and not good for the public either. So we made some changes. We did some brainstorming for ideas, with a few restrictions. First, we couldn’t use objects on the sandy soil that they could move, since they would dig them loose within 10 minutes, so we decided any enrichment object or tool needed to be hung. Second, any food (worms, smashed eggs, small cuts of fruits/vegetable) could not be put on the ground, since they would ingest the sandy soil along with the food, which could damage their mouths. And third, we needed to use natural materials that we had around, rather than things we would have to purchase. Changing the Look of the Exhibit The big logs standing halfway out and the sandy soil were the basics of the exhibit, but in the wild, even the dry savanna has some trees and bushes. As living trees and plants would not survive this enclosure, we made fake trees from bamboo. The bamboo logs were placed and anchored in holes dug down to the solid ground, and they were of The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Zoo Budapest different lengths to have smaller or taller trees. We put leaves, branches, and corn husks into them. These green materials need to be changed/ replaced weekly or biweekly. We have planted four trees, which give a new look, new smells, new materials, and new colors to the exhibit. All four survived the first night, although the aardvarks did try to dig them out, unsuccessfully. Zoo Budapest Browse added to the bamboo holders. The feeder ball suspended from a “tree.” Hanging Food One of the bamboo logs was drilled on two sides to have three centimeter diameter holes, serving as a container. We put live worms, smashed boiled eggs, and small cuts of avocado and melon into these holes, and then stepped back and waited. Within a few minutes, the aardvarks discovered the feeder and tried to get the food by licking at it through the holes. This was the first time we managed to show the public how long a tongue these animals have, and how they use it to get the termites and ants out from tight places in the wild. Then we borrowed a food-ball from the babirusas. This food ball is about 20 centimeters in diameter and has four 10 millimeter-size holes. These holes seemed to be small for the aardvarks, so we enlarged them to 16 millimeters. We filled the ball with live worms, hung it from the tallest bamboo tree, and waited. This was discovered immediately. At first, the animals only pushed it and sniffed around, but when the first worms fell out, the aarvdarks understood its purpose and started to empty the ball. For precautions, a plastic collar was put around the bamboo, so the falling worms land on this collar and not on the sandy soil. Future Plans Using these two techniques, we have managed to change the exhibit and animals’ behavior without added expense. From here, we have further ideas that we plan to integrate for the aardvarks. * The sleeping boxes (in which the animals were originally transported) will be changed for wooden barrels, or metal barrels covered with natural material like mat/wattle to prevent them from being damaged by the aardvarks’ strong claws. * To reduce the dust getting into the air from the sandy soil, the surface will be partly covered with hay and straw. This can be changed easily, and the animals can dig at it without issue. * Location of the trees will be changed periodically—with five minutes work, a tree can be moved and placed in a new position. * Train the animals for a walk: these aardvarks only have an indoor exhibit. The lights have been chosen to be as close to natural as possible, but this is not enough. So if we cannot get the natural light to the animals, the animals need to get out to the natural light. A walk in the park during good weather would be good not only for the animal’s health and serve as excellent enrichment (new smells and noises, new figures, different surfaces) but also as an educational opportunity for the public since they will be able to get close to these very uniqe and special animals. G The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 9 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr A Contest of Ideas: Enrichment at Lory Park Zoo By Althea Guinsberg, Lory Park Zoo, South Africa Lory Park Zoo M Meerkats investigate the combination burrow and feeding opportunity. 10 ahatma Gandhi once said, the greatness of a society and its moral progress can be judged by the way it treats its animals. If we consider the importance of animal welfare for animals in captivity, enrichment becomes a key area. Zoo animals not only depend on their keepers to take care of them by meeting their daily basic requirements but also to provide the necessary stimulation that relieves boredom, heightens senses, and provides the opportunity for the animals to express natural behaviors. It is with this in mind that in June 2011, Lory Park Zoo held an enrichment competition between the section keepers. The goal was to stimulate out-of-thebox thinking with the staff and ensure that new ideas were formulated for enrichment that had not been previously done in the Zoo. The results were far beyond what anybody expected, and the teamwork between the keepers and staff was certainly inspiring to see. Each keeper chose two animals in the park and had a week to come up with a new enrichment activity for those animals. Robyn Ingle-Moller, the Enrichment Officer from The National Zoological Gardens of South Africa, was also in the Zoo on the day to assist with judging and graciously donated entrance for the winning team to the Animal Keepers Association of Africa (AKAA) conference being held in October. There were a multitude of ideas that came out on the day, such as papier-mache giraffes for the Siberian tigers to destroy, hanging scented balls for the white lions to knock about, and coconuts filled with grapes for the monkeys to fish out. Some examplesfrom the three winning teams are outlined below: Meerkat Enrichment (winning team) Timothy Chirwa and his team created a feeding structure that would provide more of a challenge for the meerkats to get their food, rather than being able to simply walk up to a bowl. The team made a raised box that could only be accessed through two pipes leading into it from one side and a ramp on the other. Half the box was covered to provide shelter and to act almost like a burrow filled with bedding material. From the box, there were two raised platforms going out to two sides. Both platforms had holes cut in them so that the food and water bowls could be sunk into them. The platform for the water was directly connected to the box, so that easy access to water was not compromised. The platform for the food was built to make getting to the food a little trickier. The food platform was connected to the box with a threaded rod. PVC pipes were put over the rod so that moving from the box to the platform required some thinking and a little bit of courage. The goal with the structure was to get the meerkats to work for their food as they would have to in the wild. The meerkats were quick to explore the pipes and ramp leading into the box and seemed to enjoy playing in the “burrow.” They were a little nervous about stepping on the PVC pipes to get to the food, but of course once one brave meerkat did it, the others were quick to work out that they were not in danger and followed suit. It was a very successful enrichment and the structure has now been added to their enclosure permanently since the results were so outstanding. Coati Enrichment (second place) Amos Ngwira and his team decided that the coatis would be great candidates for enrichment, since the male had been displaying some stereotypical behavior, and the team wanted to see if they could lessen the degree of this behavior. The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 Lory Park Zoo tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr With this in mind, we thought that if we could create a sleeping area that was more in the sun, it could also be used to facilitate moving the aninmals from the sleeping quarters to the trees in the enclosure without having to walk across the floor. The basis of this decision was that coatis are exceptional climbers and prefer to be up in a tree or safely ensconced in a bush than walking along the ground, where they would be more likely to be preyed upon in their natural habitat. As such, we decided to make a hammock, which they could use to sleep in and to get from their night quarters to the tree. A spider monkey Their reaction was exactly what we thought and mona monkey eat from it would be: as soon as we put it up and left the the coconuts. enclosure, they moved toward it and proceeded to walk up and down smelling every bit of it. They seemed to enjoy it so much that we have now left it in the enclosure on a permanent basis. Primate and King Vulture Enrichment (third place) For the brown-headed spider monkeys and a mona monkey, Corlia van Niekerk and her team decided to take coconuts and drill a hole through both ends and fill it with various pieces of fruit that the monkeys are quite partial to. This enrichment was to simulate more natural feeding behavior by making them work for their food. It also ensured that the primates would be busy for an extended period of time. The team put the coconuts around the enclosure for them to find. Upon letting the primates out, at first they were only interested in the group of people standing around their enclosure to see the results of the enrichment. However, hey did eventually notice the coconuts that were hidden in their enclosure. They kept busy with them throughout the afternoon, and once they had gotten all the food out, the coconuts were used as play toys. Unfortunately they were unable to break the coconuts open as the team had hoped, but they certainly gave it their best effort and pulled all the fibers off the coconuts in the process. As further enrichment, the team opened the coconuts for them the next day, and they loved eating out of the shell. The enrichment was a great success, as it not only elicited a reaction from the monkeys but also got them to eat in a fashion that closely resembled their feeding behavior in their natural environment. Our king vultures have a poor record of reacting to enrichment and rarely exhibit any interest. Corlia’s team decided to take up the challenge by creating a food-oriented enrichment activity for the vultures by building a little wagon out of wood. Meat was placed in the wagon and the wagon was covered in skin, thereby stimulating an “animal.” King vultures are New World vultures and have an extremely keen sense of smell, which means they would smell the meat under the skin. The wagon was designed so that it could be pulled by a piece of rope that could be removed from it as soon as interest was shown by the vultures. The movement of the wagon would be better suited to a bird of prey that catches its food rather than a vulture, so to combat this, the team moved the wagon ever so slightly to make it look more like a carcass. The vultures however did not show interest in the wagon when it was moving or in the meat as we had hoped. So the following day, we removed the skin from the wagon and then the vultures came down to eat the meat. Although the king vultures did not have a response that we would have liked, it was not an entire failure. It did indicate that we need to be more creative with these majestic birds. Conclusion The benefits of enrichment cannot be stressed enough. It is evident that there are many different types of enrichment for different animals—it just takes some time and creative thinking. One must be mindful, however, that when doing enrichment it is essential that there is no way for the animal to injure itself or for it to negatively affect the animal. With those precautions, the rewards of enrichment will go a long way to support captive animal welfare. This was certainly a great initiative by the Lory Park team that thrilled both staff and animals and has brought forth a host of great ideas to be incorporated into the Zoo’s operations going forward. G The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 11 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr The Shape of Enrichment’s Enrichment Idea Database Now Available! The Shape of Enrichment proudly announces our new online, searchable Enrichment Idea Database (EIDB)! Information such as Enrichment Category and Goals, Taxa, Material, Construction and Installation Time, Appearance, Cost, Durability, Method of Cleaning, Description, Safety Concerns, Assessment Method, and Results are requested of every idea entered in the database. Most of these are searchable: you can search one or any combination of a wide variety of attributes. Are you looking for low-cost, natural-appearing ideas for your animals? Or something to reduce stereotypy? Or perhaps something new that is tough enough to stand up to your most curious or destructive species? The EIDB can provide inspiration for those needs and many more. For now, the database is read-only and contains only ideas from The Shape of Enrichment, REEC and ICEE Proceedings, and Shape’s Enrichment Workshops. However, in 2012, subscribers will be able to submit their own ideas and ideas from their institutions for consideration. And here’s the best news! Access to the EIDB is FREE with all 2012 subscriptions ordered on our website by registered users. To be authorized to use the EIDB, follow these steps: • Register on www.enrichment.org • Log in when ordering • Order online That’s it! The format for your subscription (hard copy or digital) and payment method do not matter. But you MUST place the order via our online ordering system. (Note that magazine and newsletter subscription services, such as EBSCO, will not be able to offer you access to our database). Our 2011 subscribers receiving the publication in digital format currently already have access to this new feature. Just log in and click on “Enrichment Idea DB” under Services on the main menu bar. At this time, the portion of the database available for viewing is limited (about 50 ideas). But, by early 2012, literally hundreds more ideas will be visible. The ideas have been entered and are working their way through our two-tiered review system. The EIDB will continue to grow, providing information for a wide variety of species, situations, and environments. If you’d like to have access to this exciting feature, register on our website and order your 2012 subscription online. We hope this database will be helpful in your enrichment planning and implementation! 12 The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr The Enrichment /Training Video Library This library consists of video tapes donated in the interest of sharing enrichment techniques, a collaborative effort between The American Association of Zoo Keepers, Inc. (AAZK) and The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. All videos are available in VHS-NTSC (North America), -PAL, or -SECAM formats. Please do not copy these tapes or request more than three at a time.There are four geographical areas handling videos. If you live in Europe, Africa, or the Middle East, contact our video volunteers at videoEurope@enrichment.org. If you live in Australia, New Zealand, or the nearby South Asia regions, contact videoAusAsia@enrichment.org. If you live in South America, contact videoSA@enrichment.org. If you live in the U.S. or anywhere else not mentioned, contact our main office at video@enrichment.org. Be sure to include your full address and the VHS format required. Information on the library and order forms can also be found on our website at www.enrichment.org. GENERAL ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • A Look at Enrichment, The Shape of Enrichment, Inc., San Diego, California (Video or CD) 37 min. • That’s Enrichment!, The Shape of Enrichment, Inc., San Diego, California (Video or CD) 12 min. • Enrichment at the Taronga Zoo, Sydney Australia 5 min. • Behavioural Enrichment by the People for the Animals, Rotterdam Zoo, The Netherlands 18 min. • Environmental Enrichment: Advancing Animal Care, UFAW, U.K. 38 min. • Metro Washington Park Zoo Environmental Enrichment Program, Portland, Oregon 16 min. • And Enrichment For All, San Diego Zoo, California 19 min. • Menagerie’s Medium, San Diego Zoo, California 11 min. • Habitat Enrichment Ideas from the Topeka Zoo, Kansas 32 min. • Enrichment Day at Utah’s Hogle Zoo 42 min. • ABC-123 Enrichment at Hogle Zoo, Utah 10 min. • Taking Enrichment to the Next Level, Hogle Zoo, Utah 10 min. • Enrichment at THE ZOO Including Tamarins on the Loose, Gulf Breeze Zoo, Florida 40 min. • Mixed Species Exhibits by Animal Choice, Gulf Breeze Zoo, Florida 8 min. • Columbus Zoo Enrichment, Ohio 7 min. • I Get a Kick Out of You and Manatee Training and Enrichment, Ohio 5 min. • Lincoln Park Enrichment Day, Chicago, Illinois 15 min. • Sedgwick County Zoo Play Days, Wichita, Kansas 50 min. • Target Training, Brookfield Zoo, Illinois 12 min. • The Training Game, Brookfield Zoo, Illinois 16 min. • Operant Conditioning at the Memphis Zoo, Tennessee 90 min. • Animal Training for Different Species, Columbus Zoo, Ohio 58 min. PRIMATE ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • Gorilla Training at the Belo Horizonte Zoo, Brazil • Primates with Boomer Balls, Boomer Ball 83 min. • Training Program for Semen Collection in Gorillas, Henry Doorly Zoo 38 min. • Gorilla Enrichment, Columbus Zoo, Ohio 18 min. • Gorilla Enrichment, Denver Zoo, Colorado 15 min. • Gorilla Husbandry Training for Assisted Reproduction at Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Orlando, Florida 22 min. • Orangutan Medical Behavior Training, Brookfield Zoo, Illinois 10 min. • Stressless Research Monkey Capture, Animal Welfare Institute 20 min. • Enrichment at the Bronx Zoo Monkey House, New York, New York 10 min. • Primate Enrichment, Dallas Zoo, Texas 23 min. • Primate Enrichment, Burnet Park Zoo, New York 34 min. • Primate Enrichment, Columbus Zoo, Ohio 3 min. • Bungee Jumping Monkeys, Philadelphia Zoo, Pennsylvania 18 min. • Three Observations of Callitrichids Using an Artificial Gum Tree, The Lubee Foundations, Inc., Gainesville, Florida 15 min. CARNIVORE ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • Carcass Feeding at Wildlife Safari, Winston, Oregon 17 min. • Carcass Feeding at Folsom City Zoo, California 5 min. • So Many Cats, Cincinnati Zoo, Ohio 4 min. • Enrichment for Carnivores, Sunset Zoo, Manhatten, Kansas 23 min. • Carnivore Enrichment, Columbus Zoo, Ohio 5 min. • Carnivore Enrichment, Kolmarden Zoo, Sweden 74 min. • Cat Enrichment, Dallas Zoo, Texas 13 min. • Tiger Enrichment, Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Orlando, Florida 8 min. • Felid TAG DAK Training, Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Orlando, Florida 21 min. • Bears with Boomer Balls, Boomer Ball 68 min. • The Bear Necessities: Captive Enrichment for Polar Bears, UFAW, U.K. 20 min. • Building a Polar Bear Den at the Calgary Zoo, Canada 75 min. • Bear Exhibit Renovation, Woodland Park Zoo, Seattle, Washington 17 min. • Working For a Living, Folsom City Zoo, California 13 min. • Enrichment for Gray and Harbor Seals, National Aquarium in Baltimore, Maryland 6 min. • Sea Otter Enrichment, Monterey Bay Aquarium, California 5 min. OTHER MAMMAL ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • Fruit Bat Enrichment, The Lubee Foundations, Inc., Gainesville, Florida 16 min. • Olfactory Enrichment: Rodrigue’s Fling Fox, The Lubee Foundations, Inc., Gainesville, Florida 4 min. • Commercial Bat Enrichment, The Leather Elves and The Lubee Foundations, Inc., Gainesville, Florida 5 min. • Carnivorous Cravings: Great Spear-Nosed Fruit Bat, The Lubee Foundations, Inc., Gainesville, Florida 6 min. • Elephant Feeder Ball Introduction, San Diego Wild Animal Park, California 12 min. • Snow Fall for Elephants, San Diego Wild Animal Park, California 6 min. • Kansas City Giraffe and Rhino Enrichment 45 min. • Rhino Training, Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Orlando, Florida 15 min. • Mission: Enrichment for Hoofed Stock, Dallas Zoo, Texas 18 min. • Kangaroo and Wallaby Enrichment at the Philadelphia Zoo, Pennsylvania 5 min. • Tree Kangaroo Pouch-Check Training, Brookfield Zoo, Illinois 5 min. • Marine Mammal Enrichment, National Aquarium in Baltimore, Maryland BIRD ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • Positive Approach to Parrots as Pets. Tape 1: Understanding Bird Behavior; Tape 2: Training through Positive Reinforcement, Natural Encounters, Inc., Lakeville, Minnesota 69 min. each tape • Kings of the Wind, Natural Encounters, Inc., Lakeville, Minnesota 38 min. OTHER ANIMAL ENRICHMENT AND TRAINING • Octopus Enrichment at the Cleveland Zoo, Ohio 6 min. The Shape of Enrichment Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 13 tf JZPVixSEImvGjQKUTgoLwtfJZPr Editors: Karen E. Worley Valerie J. Hare Editorial Advisor: David Shepherdson, Ph.D. Oregon Zoo, Oregon Editorial Reviewers: Mollie Bloomsmith, Ph.D. Yerkes Primate Center, Georgia Kathy Carlstead, Ph.D. Honolulu Zoo, Hawaii Amy Cutting, Ph.D. Oregon Zoo, Oregon Vicky Melfi, Ph.D. Paignton Zoo, U.K. Challenges Coordinator: Else Poulsen Video Library Coordinators: Beth Rich Yvette Kemp Harald Schwammer Cynthia Cipreste Jo Walker The Shape of Enrichment is dedicated to sharing ideas, inspirations, and practical knowledge of enrichment strategies among those working in the field of animal care. It is an open forum for keepers, trainers, curators, researchers, exhibit designers, administrators, volunteers, and anyone else interested in approaches to enrichment. All of our staff are volunteers. We are always looking for new submissions, from feature-length articles to short blurbs. We accept submissions in any form, polished or not. Let us, and your colleagues, hear from you! THE SHAPE OF ENRICHMENT presents enrichment ideas of all kinds from a variety of sources. We urge you to consider, assess, and evaluate any idea carefully before applying it to your own animals and exhibits. If you have concerns or opposing views, we are happy to accept letters and articles that express them. As the editors, we present these ideas for your consideration only; we do not take responsibility for their effectiveness or feasibility. THE SHAPE OF ENRICHMENT, ISSN 10888152, is published quarterly by the non-profit The Shape of Enrichment, Inc. Subscriptions are $18 per calendar year, payable in U.S. funds only, drawn on a U.S. bank or by credit card on www.enrichment.org. Domestic and foreign air mail postage is included. Mid-year subscriptions are prorated. Back issues are available for $5 each. Send all subscription requests, article submissions, letters, comments, and questions to 1650 Minden Dr., San Diego, CA 92111-7124. Phone: (858) 279-4273. E-mail: shape@enrichment.org. Please share and photocopy any part of this newsletter, as long as you credit the source, its editors, and the authors. This publication is NOT FOR RESALE. Visit our website at www.enrichment.org! 14 Downloadable Subscriptions The Shape of Enrichment now offers downloadable subscriptions. Subscribers to the downloadable version of The Shape of Enrichment will enjoy many benefits: • E-mail notification when each issue is published and ready to download — no more waiting for the postal service! • Store your issues on your hard drive — no more lost or torn issues! • Print only what you need; re-print whenever you like To take advantage of our downloadable subscriptions, you MUST be a registered user on our new website. To register, go to www.enrichment.org, click on Register, and complete the form. Be sure to make a note of your user name and password — you will need these when you log on! Also, if your contact information changes — especially your e-mail — remember to update your user account. Complimentary subscriptions will only be downloadable; hard copies will no longer be available. Hard copies of The Shape of Enrichment will still be available for purchase; but increased postage fees will be reflected in the cost of the subscription and back issues. Very soon, all The Shape of Enrichment back issues and some of the International Conference on Environmental Enrichment (ICEE) Proceedings will also be available through our Publication Download feature. Register now to be notified when these become available! Contents: Volume 20 No. 3, 2011 Features 1 3 6 8 Having a Ball: Increasing Exploratory Behavior in Pig-nosed Turtles by Natalie Cook Primate Enrichment in the Canopy, on the Ground, and Hidden Away by Guy Waddilove Olfactory Enrichment for Carnivores by Juliana do Carmo Padiha, Eliana Ferraz Santos, and Eleonore Zulnara Freire Setz Aardvarks Live to Dig and Eat: Give Them a Little Fun by Miklos Faludi and Katalin Toth by Althea Guinsberg 10 A Contest of Ideas: Enrichment at Lory Park Zoo ...And Then Some 12 Video Library 13