To be or not to be humorous: Does it make a
Transcription
To be or not to be humorous: Does it make a
To be or not to be humorous: Does it make a difference? LO NU , I SO P P E N H E I M E R , N E LW A R N A R S - K L E V E R L A A A N dL A R R YS H E R M A N Abstract Humor is thought to be a social instrumentor a social skill that afibct'sthe outcomesoJ'socialinteractions.The purposesof the presentstudy were (l) to address the question whether within a peer group, humor and social distances are related to a particular social status such as popularity, neglection, and rejection and (2) to replicate sherman's (1988) crosssectional sturiy. For thesepurposes,225 children and adolescentsdivided over three age groups (9-, I2-, and l5-year olds) with three complete classroomstuithin each age group participated in this study. All subjects \)ere presented with measures assessinghumor, funniness, social distance, play and work preferences,and social status. The results shotv that" (a) on the hasis from the age of 12, children evaluatetheir peers' humorousness personality characterand social specified and detailed more of increasingly diferently perceive humorousness of 9, istics;( b ) already at the age females "realistically" ( or fun is humorous being than males; c ) and perhapsmore apparentlynot a sffic'ient characteristicto erplain smQllersocial distances; "controversial" children obtained a meaningt'ulrelation between (d) only perceiveclhumorousnessand social distance;and (e) despitedffirences in strength, the patterns oJ'interrelationshipsamong perceivedhumorousness and social tlistance in the present study and those reported by Sherman (1985) showeda remarkable similarity. Already in early philosophical views, humor is perceivedas an ingredient "man good of human existence and a positive attribute that makes company" (Aristotle, qtd. in Thomson 1966:134). According to McGhee (lg7g), humor exists only in the minds of people and not in the real world. Despite these views of humor and its evolutionary and social Humor9 2 (1996),117 l4I. 0117 0933 1719/96/0009 €,Walter de Gruyter ll8 N. Warnars-Kleverlaan,L. Oppenheimer,and L. Sherman functions(Darwin 1955),little is known about the developmentof humor and its role in interpersonal relationships (cf. Bell, McGhee, and Duffey 1 9 8 6 ;S h e r m a n1 9 8 8 ) . Studying humor as a social skill presentsus with a fascinatingparadox. According to Martineau (1972), Goodman (1983) and Foot (1991) humor is important in the formation and facilitation of social relationships as well as in ending them. Consequently,humor will reduce as well "interas increasesocial distancesamong people(Sherman 1988).As an personal communication behavior, (humor) facilitates social interaction" (Sherman 1988:390), promotes friendships,relationships,and intimacy by gaining the approval of others and by sustaining the friendships we value and wish to maintain. The reduced social distances between the "humorous" individual and others (Kane, Suls, and Tedeschi 1977; Masten 1986;Sherman 1988)also affect,for instance,peer relationsand "interperpeer statusin groups of children (Masten 1986).Humor, as an sonal communication behavior," however, is also perceivedas a socially acceptable way to dissociate from people (to increase distances); by means of humor, one can safely expressnegative feelingstowards others. Efforts to define humor have not been very successful.Becausehumor is a multi-dimensionalconcept (Foot 1991),it has not yet been possible to develop a theory of humor that adequately includes all its features. The ability to use humor to achieveparticular, sometimescontradictory, . social goals is only one of its features. Other features involve (a) humor production or the ability to be humorous; (b) a senseof playfulness or the ability to have a good time, which may be related to the personality trait of being good-natured; (c) the ability to recognize humor, life's absurdities, and the self as humorous; (d) the appreciation of humor, humorous people, and humorous situations; and (e) the ability to use humor as an adaptive mechanism,that is, being able to laugh at problems or to master difficult situations through the use of humor (Thorson and Powell 1991). However, despite the absence of a clear definition for humor, an intuitive understanding of what humor is about is present (Ziv 1984\: Adults as well as children have no problems in telling which of their friends have a good senseof humor and which do not. The findings from different studiesby Sherman( 1985a,1985b;Sherman and Wolf 1984) and Masten (1986) suggestthat humor is not derived "in the form of from social status (Chapman 1973), but that humor interpersonal humor perceptions is predictive of social status and friendship preferences" (Sherman 1988: 390). That is, children perceived as l I ( l 1 ( \ I L v To be or not to be humorous I 19 humorous will evidence smaller social distances from their peers than children who are perceived as not humorous. As early as six years old, gender differences in humor have been reported (McGhee 1979; Ziv 1984): males more often create humor, females more often enjoy it. Explanations for this finding involve the relation between humor and gender-role expectations. Because humor is frequently aggressive,the initiation of humor is often consideredto be more appropriate for males than for females in Western society. Findings about perceived humor partly support this view: Generally, sociometric humor ratings of peers resultedin a higher humor scorefor males(Ziv 1984).However, gender appears to be an important factor in perceived humor. Children of the same gender are seen as more humorous than children of the opposite gender (Sherman 1988). In a study involving three 4th-grade classrooms(71 children with a mean age of 9.6 years),Sherman(1988) studiedthe role of genderin the relationship betweenhumor and social distance.The data from this study indicate that, consistentlyamong both genders,children who were rated as more humorous were also perceived as socially less distant. Children of the same gender rated each other as socially less distant as well as more humorous than children of the opposite gender.These data suggest that humor is a social instrument or a social skill that affectsthe outcomes of social interactions(cf. Krasnor and Rubin l98l). The purpose of the present study is to address the question whether v,,ithina peer group, socialdistancesare relatedto a particularsocialstatus,suchas popularity, neglection,and rejection (cf. Oppenheimer 1989;Warnars-Kleverlaanand Oppenheimer1989). According to Babad (1914), the method of sociometrichumor ratings is most appropriate to assessthe humorousnessof children as perceived by their peers. By this method children were asked to rate all other children in their class on their humorousness,by means of a five-point Likert scale (Sherman 1988). In everyday life, humor is indicated in terms of jokes, cartoons, and the behavior of others, that is, having fun or being funny is used as a justification for humorous action, especially when the social meanings of humor are analyzed (Podilchak 1992). For example, children often play with meanings just for fun. According to McGhee (1919), the word "funny" more than any other related term, is used to mean humorous. For this reason, in the present study, humor w i l l b e c o m D a r e dt o f u n n i n e s s . 120 N. Warnars-Kleverlqan, L. Oppenheimer,and L. Sherman In his study on humor and social distance,Sherman(1988) suggested that humor in the form of interpersonal humor perceptionsis predictive of social status and friendship preferences.The present study involves a replication of Sherman's cross-sectionalstudy in which, instead of one age group (9-year olds), three age groups are used (9-, l2-, and l5-yearolds). In addition to the instrumentsthat assessthe humorousnessand social distance of the children by their peers,also a measurefor funniness, play and work preferences,and social status are introduced. The purpose of this study is to examine whether the relationship between humorousnessratings and social distanceremains similar for different ages,whether social distance relates to friendship preferencesand social status, and whether gender differences reported by Sherman are replicated with a Dutch population. The results of the youngest age group (the 9-year olds) will be compared with the results reported in the study by S h e r m a n( 1 9 8 8 ) . While the present study uses a correlational design to verify the relationship between perceived humorousnessand perceived social distance, the assumption that humor will reduce social distances among people (Sherman 1988) allows a directional interpretation of the correlation coefficients. That is, perceived humorousnesscan then be examined as an explanatory variable for the observed variance in perceived social distance. Method Subjects + The subjectsin this study were 225 children and adolescentsfrom middleclass neighborhoods. They were divided among three age groups with three complete (intact) classroomswithin each age group. The age groups represented4th-graders(n:86; age-rangefrom 8.6 to 10.7 years;mean age 9.3, 46 females and 40 males) and 6th-graders from elementary schools (n--76: age-rangefrom 11.1 to 12.5 years; mean age 12.3;41 females and 35 males), and 3rd-year students from sesondary schools (n--63; age-rangefrom 14.8to l7.l years;mean age 15.8;28 femalesand 35 males). The three intact classroomswithin each age group consisted of 25,32, and 29 4th-graders,20,21, and 29 6th-graders,and 20, 19, and 23 3rd-year students. c S, LI g s a o fa (r a To be or not to be humorous 121 M aterials and procedure Funniness. The funniness measure consisted of two open-ended questions. The children were asked (a) to indicate which classmate(s)were most funny and (b) why they thought those children were funny. Besides peer-group ratings for funniness,the data were used to obtain the concurrent validity between the funniness and the humor scales. Humor. ln contrast to the funniness scale,a description of a humorous individual was presented on top of the humor scale (identical to "We want to find out how humorous people Sherman's1988procedure): are. By humorous we do not mean somebody who is looking strange or somebody who is behaving silly. By a humorous individual we mean somebodywho has a good senseof humor, tells good jokes, makesother people laugh, and can laugh at other'sjokes." This definition with small modificationswas identical to the one used by Sherman(1988:394). With the above description of a humorous individual in mind, the children were asked to rate all other children in their class on their humorousnessby means of a five-point Likert scale (Sherman 1988). With the exception of the child's own name, a list of printed names of all the children in the classwas presented.Following each child's name a five-point continuum for humor judgements was presented,ranging "very "not much." The children could then rate from (1) at all" to (5) putting peers mark in the column that best described the by a their other child. Three humor scoresbasedon theseratingswere calculated.Thesewere the mean (a) total-humor score (HUt) of a child given by all other humor children in the classroomirrespectiveof gender;(b) same-gender score(HUs; the humor ratings given by femalesto femalesand by males to males), and (c) cross-genderhumor score (HUc; the humor ratings given by femalesto males or by males to females). Socialdisttmc'e. For socialdistancea similar procedurewas used.Again a list of printed namesof all the children.with the exceptionof the child's own name, was presented.Five socialdistancejudgmentswere presented "I would like to have him/her as one of my best friends" ranging from "l (rating l) to wish he/sheweren't in our group" (rating 5). Again each 122 N. Wurnars-Kleverlaun,L. Oppenheirner,antl L. Sherman child could rate all the other children by indicatingwhich statementbest describedeach child. The mean social distancescoreswere calculatedin exactly the same way as the humor scores.That is, the mean (a) total socialdistancescore( SDt ) of a child givenby all childrenin the classroom irrespectiveof gender; (b) same-gendersocial distancescore (SDs; the social-distance ratingsgiven by femalesto femalesand by malesto males); and (c) cross-gender socialdistancescore(SDc; the social-distance ratings given by f-emales to males or by malesto females). SocictlStutLt.s.Two lists of classmateswere used to assesssocial status. For the positive-nominationthe childrenwererequiredto circlethe names o f t h r e e c h i l d r e nt h e y l i k e d t h e m o s t . F o r t h e n e g a t i v e - n o m i n a t i ot n hey were required to circle the names of three children they liked the least. Nomination scoteswere basedon the responscslrom all the pcersirrespectiveol- gcnder.To assessthe social statusof a child a method similar t o t h e o n e d e s c r i b e db y A s h e r a n d D o d g e ( 1 9 8 6 ) w a su s e d .T h i s m e t h o d involves the cornputation ol' the liequenciestbr positive and negative nominatior.rsIbr each child and the transformation of these frequencres into standerrdized z-scoresfbr liking (L) and disliking (D) wirhin each classroom.Following this transfbrmation.the social preference(SP) and socialimpact (SI )scoreswere computed.The socialpreference(SP)score is determined by subtracting the standardizeddisliking score liom the s t a n d a r d i z e dl i k i n g s c o r e ;t h e s o c i a l i m p a c t ( S I ) s c o r e b y a d d i n g t h e standardizedliking and disliking scoresfor each child. The classification procedure consistsof fitting each child into one of five groups that are characterizedby dili-erentvaluesfor SP. SI. L. and D. Popularchildren ( S P > 1 . 0 . L > 0 , a n d D < 0 ) a r e i n t e r p r e t e da s b e i n g c o o p e r a t i v ea n d show leadershipbehavior and seldom disrupt the group, fight, and ask f o r h e l p . R e j a c r e cclh i l d r e n ( S P < - 1 . 0 , L < 0 , a n d D > 0 ) a r e c h i l d r e n who cooperatelittle and show no ieadershipqualities;they often disrupt the group, fight. and seekhelp. l{eglet:tedchildren (SI< -1.0. L<0, and D<0) distinguishthemselvesfrom the rejectedchildren in that they are uot actually disliked by their peers; they are only not n.rentionedas clrildren othcrs would want to be friends with. Controversiulchildren (Sl>1.0. L>0, and D>0)combine characteristio c sf t h e r e j e c t e da n d popular children. Thesechildren are perceivedas disruptive.start fights. and frequently seekhelp. On the other hand, they are also perceivedas leaders.Though not cooperative,they are also not perceivedas lacking ( ( e c j, a a n a p To be or not to be lumrtrous 123 t h i s b e l r a v i o r .A v e r a g ec h i l d r e n ( 0 . 5< S P < 0 . 5 a n d - 0 . 5 < S l < 0 . 5 ) , normally the largestgroup of children within a peer group, are evaluated to be averageon cooperation,leadership,attraction, etc. (secAsher and D o d g e 1 9 8 6 ;C o i e . D o d g e , a n d C o p p o t e l l i 1 9 8 2 ) . s I r I e Play and v'ork pre/brences ,st'ale. Play and work preferences were obtained by asking the children to indicate the children they prel-erto work with and preferto play with in their classroom.The rating categones "I am r a n g e df r o m ( l ) ' ' I d o n ' t l i k e t o p l a y i w o r ka t a l l w i t h . . . " t o ( 5 ) ". As with humor and social distance. very eager to play/work with... separateaveragesame-and cross-genderratings were determined. instrumentswere presentedin a fixed order ir.r The various assessment four sessionswith two-weekintervalsbetweeneachsession.The questror-rnairesand scalesfor funniness.socialdistanceand play and work preferences,humor, and social status were presentedin this respectiveorder. was done with one class Afler two months. a secondhumor assessment o f 9 - y e a ro l d s ( n : 3 2 ) a n d o n e c l a s so f l 2 - y e a ro l d s ( n : 2 7 ) , p e r r n i t t i n g the calculation of test-retestreliabilitieslbr the humor measure. S l J Results tr M uteriulsand proc'edure e n n d t. ^ n )t A g IS n d S, IS ) "name the most funny chiid(ren) in their Funnines,s.To the question class." 88%,of the children respondedwith at least one child. Of the children who were not able to mention a llnny child. three children gave "nobody was lunny, all were dumb. n very negativeview of their peers: Almost eighty percent (79'f,,)of the responsesto the rvhy-question could be classifiedinto five categoriesinvolving responsesre{errin-eto joking, acting funny. laughing (either together or alone). making subtle and witty remarks, and being funny. The rnajor diff'erencebetween the "being" "acting" funny concernedthe useof behavioral explar.rations and versuspsychologicalor personalitycharacteristics. To permit Chi-square for the funnianalysisfor independentsamples.oniy the first explar.ration nessof a child was scored.In Table 1. the resultingfrequencies.percentages, and total number of responseslbr each responsecategory are present.Chi-square analysesshowed the responsecategoriesnclt to be 124 N. Warnars-Kleverlaan, L. Oppenheinrer,and L. Sherman independentof age(y) ( 10) : 52.18,p < .001). No relationbetweengender and the responsecategoriescould be demonstrated.Separate,one-sample Chi-square analysesdemonstrated that the 9-year olds perceived funniness mostly in terms of "acting funny" and joking (y.2(5):58.97; p<.001) and the l5-year olds primarily in terms of making subtle and "being witty remarks, funny," and "other" characteristics (X2(5):28.12; p <.001 ). Although for the l2-year olds the frequenciesfor the different responsecategoriesdiffered(y2(5):42.9; p <.001), this age group could be perceived as transitional when compared to the changesbetween the 9- and 15-yearolds. The responsesclassifiedas "other" may tentatively illustrate the multidimensional nature of the construct of humor. Creativity, appearances,tendentiousjokes, social distance,and the use of unique and unexpectedremarks are recurrent themesin theseresponses and together with the five major responsecategoriesclosely coincide with the features of the humor concept. These additional themes in the responsesof the children becamemore evident from the age of 12. To relate the data obtained for funninessto the humor ratings, the frequency of the funniness nominations for each child were transformed into standardizedz funniness scores (F) for each child within each classroom. Huntor. To obtain the concurrent validity between the funniness measure and the humor scalethe mean total humor score(HUt) was correlated with the standardizedfunninessscore(F) for each age group. The correlations ranged from .71 to .93 with mean correlationsof .76, .80, and .84 for the 9-,12-, and l5-year olds, respectively.Theseresultsshow a high concurrent validity and confirm the assumptionthat "funny" is used to mean humorous (McGhee 1919). The analysisof the total humor scores(HUt) by meansof a two-way A N O V A , r e s u l t e di n a m a i n e f t - e cftb r a g e ( F ( 2 . 2 1 9 ) - 4 . 1 9 . p < . 0 1 ) a s well as fbr gender (F(1 ,219): 14.17. p < .00I ). The 9-year old children r e c e i v e ds i g n i f i c a n t l yh i g h e r h u m o r s c o r e s( 2 . 5 9 ) t h a n t h e 1 2 - ( 2 . 3 3 ) a n d 1 5 - y e a ro l d s ( 2 . 3 1 ) . T h e m a l e si n t h i s s t u d y r e c e i v e da s i g n i f i c a n t l y higher (2.59) mean total humor scorethan the females(2.21). No effect for the interaction between age and gender was evident. A 3 x 2 x 2 (agex genderx humor) ANOVA, with the last variable consistingof the s a m e -a n d c r o s s - g e n d ehru m o r s c o r e s( H U s a n d H U c ) a n d t r e a t c da s a repeated measurement, revealed significant main effects lbr agc ( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) - 4 . 7 3p, < . 0 1 ) a n d g e n d e r( F ( 2 . 2 1 9 ) : 1 6 . 1 0p, < . 0 0 1 ) b u t n o t }l" To be or not to be humorous 125 Table 1. The .frequent.iesJbr the frst explanation to the question why a thild tt'us con,sidered .finn;, ( percentage.sbetu,eenbracketsI Categories joking "acting" funny laughing witty remarks "being" funny other Age 9 years (n:86) ( 3 1 . 2 ) 31 1 7 ( 2 2 . 4 \ 2r 25 ( 2 9 . 1) 2 7 I 5 ( 1 9 . 7\ ] e 12 ( 1 3 . e ) t 3 l 5 ( 1 9 . )7 1 6 (6.e) 7 10 ( r3.2) 24 6 ( 69 ) 6 9 (1r.8) s 6 (5.8) 12 10 ( 13.2) 24 5 99 15 Total S h e d h t- ]. J, is ry IS )n l) ly at 2 a o, Other creativity stupid remarks social distance general remarks tendentious appearance uniquelunexpected humor having guts I 5 years ( n: 6 3 ) I 2 years ( n- 7 6 ) 2 I 2 U 0 0 0 U 0 5 l 5 I 0 2 2 2 I 2 tl I U (t 0 0 0 (.) 2 6 9 14 16 16 t r3 63 Total (n--225) ( 3 . 2 1 -t 5l -r8 ( 9 . s) 9 4 6 5 5 ( 1 4 . 3 )1 0 3 6 39 ( 2 2 . 2 .l9 \ 50 30 ( 2 5 . 1 \1 8 3 l JJ ( 2 s . 4 )t 9 3 1 55 78 225 1 t 0 4 l J 3 5 1 4 r 5 5 r 3 t 3 2 - 11 9 9 6 6 4 l l l "other" is specificd separately.The italicized liequencies represent The responsecategory the total number of responses(involving all explanations offered by a child). for the interaction between age and gender. Also the humor measures d i f f e r e ds i g n i f i c a n t l y( , F ( 1 , 2 1 9 ) : 1 3 5 . 5 6p, < . 0 0 1 ) , a s w e l l a s t h e h u m o r . <.001) A 3 x2 (agexgender) b y a g e i n t e r a c t i o n( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 1 8 . 1 6 p ANOVA on the same-genderhumor ratings (HUs) revealed significant e f f e c t sf o r a g e ( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 1 3 . 2 2 ,p < . 0 0 1 ) a n d g e n d e r( - F ( 1 , 2 1 9 ) : 1 1 . 6 8 , p<.001). A similar analysison the cross-genderhumor ratings (HUc) resulted in a main effect for gender only for HUc (F(1,219):17.25, p <.001). In Table 2, the same-and cross-genderhumor ratings for each gender and age group are presented.Post-hocanalyses(Fisher'sprotectedLSD) indicated that the differencesin same-genderratings between the 9-year olds and the I 2- and I 5-year olds are significant (p < .00I ) ' Thesefindings indicate that the humor scoresgiven to same-genderpeersbecomesignifi"realistic" from the age of 12. For the cantly lower and apparentlymore cross-gender ratings no significant differences between the age groups 126 lV. Warnars-Kleverlaan,L. Oppenheinter,und L. Shennan Table 2. The sunu'- unrl uos.s-centlerhunutr antl soLiul-tli,stuncc raIitlg.\ ht gentler./br the tltret' ugt groups Humor ratings 9-year olds Cender of ratcr Female Male same ,l/ 2.87 3.07 6 2 2 . 13 65 2.38 cross I 2-year olds Gender of ratcr ,t sitme dJ' cross -t0 2.22 63 I .93 OE 2 . 8 3 . 7 6 2 . 4 9 I 5 - y e a ro l d s Genderol rater ,t same .JJ 2.34 .73 2.50 ,tl cross 6-l 9-l 1.99 .61 2 . 3 8 r I2 ttJ S o c i a ld i s t a n c e Female Male same dl' l.)9 |.31 65 2.42 7.t 2.72 cross ,l 56 same l,-) / 1.45 dl' cross 7t 1.93 ,5-t 1.87 dl same tt/ cross 62 50 l.19 1.88 .66 .61 l.9t l .75 tll .63 .77 Note that in this table a low social-distanceratin-s implics a small social clstancc. were evident.with respectto gender,post-hocanalysesindicatedsignificant differencesbetween males and females for the same- and crossgenderhumor ratings(p <.001 ) showingthat consistentlyover-agemales receivedsignificantlyhigher humor ratings than femalesfrom their male as well as their female peers. Sociul Distance. The total social-distancescores(SDt) were analyze<l by means of a two-way ANOVA and resulted in a main effect for age ( F ( 2 . 2 1 9 ): 5 . 6I , p < . 0 1 ) a n d g e n d e r( F ( 1 , 2 1 9 ) : 4 . 8 9 ,p < . 0 5 p e r c e i v e d ). social distancewas significantlygreaterin the youngestage group (1.94) a s c o m p a r e dt o t h e t w o o l d e r a g e g r o u p s( 1 . 6 6 a n d l . j 4 f o r t h e 1 2 - a n d l5-year olds, respectively).Femalesperceivedtheir peersto have signific a n t l y l e s s s o c i a l d i s t a n c e( 1 . 7 1 ) t h a n m a l e s ( 1 . 8 8 ) . A 3 x 2 x 2 (agex genderx humor) ANOVA, with the last variable consistingof the same-and cross-gender social-distance scores(SDs and SDc) and treated as a repeated measurement, revealed significant main effects for age ( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 5 . 8 7 ,p < . 0 1 ) a n d g e n d e r( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 3 . 6 1 p , :.05) but nor for the interaction between age and gender. Also the social distance measuresdifferedsignificantly(F( 1,219): 336.I 3, p < .001), as well as all other higher order interactions.A 3x2 (agexgender) ANOVA on the same-gendersocial-distanceratings revealedsignificant effectsfor gender TLtbe or not to be lrumorous 121 (F( I ,219) : 9.07, p < .01) and the interaction between age and gender ( F ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 5 . 3 5 , p < . 0 1 ) . t r o r t h e c r o s s - g e n d esro c i a l - d i s t a n crea t i n g s . s i g n i f i c a n et f f e c t sf o r a g e ( F ( 2 " 2 1 9 ) : 3 5 . 8 5 ,p < . 0 0 1 ) a n d t h e i n t e r a c t i o n b e t w e e na g ea n d g e n d e rc o u l d b e d e m o n s t r a t e (dF ( 2 , 2 1 9 ) : 3 . 1 2 .p < . 0 5) . ratings by gender In Table 2 the same- and cross-gendersocial-distance for the three age groups are presented.Post hoc analyses(Fisher's protected LSD) revealedthat the 9-year olds rated their peers of the same genderto have a significantlygreatersocialdistancethan did the l2- and l 5 - y e a r o l d s ( p < . 0 5 ) . W i t h r e s p e c t o t h e c r o s s - g e n d er ra t i n g s( S D c ) , i t was again the 9-year olds only who perceivedtheir peers of the other gender to have a significantlygreater social distancethan their peersof the samegender(p <.001 ). With respectto gender,only the same-gender social-distanceratings were significant.Femalesrated their female peers l o w e r i n s o c i a ld i s t a n c et h a n t h e m a l e sr a t e d t h e i r m a l e p e e r s( p < . 0 1 ) . Pearson product-moment correlation coefllcients were calculated betweenthe total humor ( HUt) and socialdistarrceratings(SDt) irrespective of the genderof the rater and ratee.Thesecoeflrcientsattained -.48, . 5 6 , a n d - . 5 7 f o r t h e 9 - , 1 2 - a n d l 5 - y e a r o l d s . r e s p e c t i v e l yT. h c hypothesisthat humor will reducesocialdistances(Sherman 1988)allows for the assumptionof a (causal) directionfor thesecorrelationcoefficients ratings showing that 23 to 32 percent of the variancein social-distance humor ratings. ln Table 3, the intercorrelations can be explainedby the are shown between the same- and cross-genderratings for humor and social distanccfor each age group and all age groups combined.As can bc observedfrom this table. the relationshipsbetweenthe different ratings, though varying in magnitude,are relativelyconsistentover the three age groups. tS3S le ,i :d l) Ld fr2 le d ge 0t ce rll : i I Reliubility. Test-retest reliability scores for the humor measurement obtainedwith 9- and l2- year olds werecalculatedby meansof Spearman rank-order coefficients.The reliability coelicients were .79 and .93 ( p < .001) for the 9- and |2-year olds, respectively.With the f'emales,the s a m e - g e n d emr e a n sc o r r e l a t e d. 1| ( p < . 0 1 ) a n d . 9 4 ( p < . 0 0 1 ) , w h i l e t h e c r o s s - g e n d em r e a n sc o r r e l a t e d. 6 9 ( p < . 0 1 ) a n d . 9 1 ( p < . 0 0 1 ) . F o r t h e m a l e s ,t l r e s a m e - g e n d em r e a n sc o r r e l a t e d. 8 9 ( p < . 0 0 1) a n d . 7 6 p < . 0 1 \ a n d t h e c r o s s - g e n d emr e a n s . 8 8 ( p < . 0 0 1 ) a n d . 7 6 ( p < . 0 1 ) . O v e r a l l . these coelicients indicated a satisfactory test-retestreliability for the humor measurement. I 128 lrl. Ifarnurs-Kleverlaan, L. Oppenheimer,ancl L. Sherntan Table 3. The intertorrelations belv'een sume- und tross-gender so(.iatl-di.stanrcantl humor ratingsJi;r eadr age group'epuratell'and all uge groups comhinetl (intluding age us variable) 9-year olds (ri : 86 ) SDc HUs HUc . 6 6 ** SDc HUs HUc Gender .37+* -.02 12-yeao r lds(n:76\ SDs -.32r* - .31** .67*4 .16 - .21* HUs HUc . 5 6 +* SDc HUs HUc Gender .26+ -.07 -.58** .06 . 7 0 ** _.41** _.41*r I 5-yearoldsft :63 ) SDs SDc HUs HUc * .49+ SDc HUs HUc Gc'nder .34** -.48++ -.43** -.60** .ll .92*+ - 10 A l l a g e s( n : 2 2 5 ) SDs SDc HUs SDc HUs HUc Genderu Age -.32** .l 8 * * .l 6 * -.43+r HUc Gender -.02 - .01 . 77 + * .01 .29** * p < . 0 5*; * p < . o l uGender wasa binarycodedvariable: girls=0andboys:L Play und n'ork preJbrences. The scores on this measure involved peer ratings with respect to play and work preferences.The intercorrelations betweenthe play and work preferencesand the total humor (HUt) and social distance (SDt) scores for each classroom peer group separately indicated that the social-distancescaleand the play and work preferences scale assessedthe same variable. The correlation coe{icients between the SDt and work and play preferencesranged from -.gl to -.94 for the nine peer groups. For the same- and cross-genderplay and work prefer_ encesidenticalresultswere found. children with whom otherswould like to play and work are also the children who are perceivedto be close to others (with small social distances).As with social distance.sienificant To be or nlt to be humorous 129 but considerably weaker relationships were evident between humor and play and work preferences.Becausethe play and work indices did not add additional information to the social distance measures,the play and work indices were omitted from the following analyses. ler )eI NS nd ty )es he he ke to rnt Social Statu.r. By means of the social status scales,the status of each individual child within the peer group was established(popular fn:59], controversial [n:14l, average ln:701, neglected[n:41], and rejected fn:4ll). Within groups ANOVAs with the five social-statusgroups as the independent variable were performed on the total social-distance (SDt) and humor scores (HUt) separatelyfor each age group. With respectto social distance, the three ANOVAs revealed significant effects ( p < . 0 0 1) f o r t h e s o c i a l - s t a t u g s r o u p s ( f ( 4 , 8 5) : 2 6 . 6 , F ( 4 . 1 5 ) : 3 2 . 0 2 , and F(4,62):13,32, p<.001 for the 9-, l2-, and l5-year olds, respectively). This finding indicates that social distance is related to social status. A similar relationship could be demonstrated between socialwere 5.59, 5.2, and I .51 ( p < .01) for the statusand humor. The -F-values 9-,72-, and l5-year olds, respectively.In Table 4, the mean social-distance and humor scores for each social-status group and age are presented. From this table it can be observed that the popular children possessthe smallestsocial distanceand the rejectedchildren the greatestsocial distance from their peers. Separate one-way ANOVAs indicated that the popular children could be distinguished from all other social-status groups with the exceptionof the l5-year old controversialgroup on the basis of low social distances(p<.05). Similarly, the rejected children could be distinguishedfrom all other social-statusgroups by signilicantlylarger social distances(p<.05). With the humor scores,a different picture emerges. The only distinctions based on the humor scoresthat could be made between the social-statusgroups were between the popular and rejectedgroups and the controversial and rejectedgroups' These tlistinctions demonstrated significantly lower humor scoresfor the rejected children as compared to the popular and controversial children (p<.05). It is interesting to note that certainly at the older ages the neglectedchildren do not distinguish themselveson the basis of their social distance, nor humor ratings from the average children. The neglected children appear to be reasonably well-liked and to possessa reasonablesenseof humor. When the same- and cross-gendersocial distanceand humor ratings are consideredseparately,complex patterns of interrelationshipsbetween 130 N. Wumur,s-Kleverlacrn, L. Oppenheirner,and L. Shernrun T a b l c 4 . T I t e n r c u ns o t i u l d i . y t u n t e( S D t ) u n d h u n t o r s u n ' e s( I t L t 1 trrttl .st tutdur d dt' t,i u t i ons aroup untl uge for cucltSotitrl-.ytutu.s ( c H u m o r( H U r ) S o c i a ld i s t a n c el S D t I r S v vears I 2 ycars l5 years 9 years 12 1'cars I 5 ycars rnean sd mean stl ntcan mean mean lnean l.lti 1.61 t . 75 r.68 l.-s7 :9 3I 17 1-1 2q 1 . 2 2 - r , 5 2 . 8 1 .1,5 2 . 7 3 . 6 5 2 . 6 6 t . 6 1 . 1 1 ) . 6 4 . 4 9 2.21 .54 2.06 l . 5 8 . 6 1 2 . 8 5 . -)i 2 . 6 4 r i l 3 . 5 5 I .91 -r.r ).61 . 7 0 2 . 0 9 .1: 2.18 l.-.]rl .-t-t 2.05 1 . 8 4 . _ 5 61 . 8 0 Popular 1.48 Average 1.90 Controv. 3.02 Neglected 2.84 Rejectcd 2.79 29 40 1I l7 19 .ut sd sd sd .71 .68 r05 .67 .59 N o t c t h a t i n t h i s t a b l e a l o u s o c i a ld i s t a n c es c o r ei m p l i e s a s r n a l lr e a l s o c i a ld i s t a n c e , age, gender,and social status becameevident. In general,however,the r e s u l t s f r o m t h e f o u r s e p a r a t e3 x 2 x 5 ( a g e x g e n d e r x s o c i a l s t a t u s ) ANOVAs repeat the previously reported findings for the effectsof age, and humor scores. -qender.and social statusfor the total social-distance Correlations computed between the total social-distanceand humor ratings (SDt and HUt) and betweenthe same-and cross-gender,social d i s t a n c e( S D s a n d S D c ) a n d h u m o r s c o r e s( H U s a n d H U c ) f o r e a c h social-statusgroup revealeda surprising pattern (seeTable 5). For the popular and rejectedgroups the correlation betweenSDt and HUt did not attain significance.With the averageand neglectedgroups.the correlations between these variableswere significant.The magnitude of the coefllcients.however,suggestedthat only 6 to 12 percentof the variance in social distance could be explained by humor. In contrast to these findings,the relationshipbetweenthe SDt and HUt with the controversial group was .78. If it is a-eainassumedthat humor will reduce social distances(Sherman 1988). then with the controversialchildren approxrmately 60 percent of the variance in social distancecould be explaincd by the humor ratings. (Note that the controversialgroup consistedof oniy 14 children.) For the controversial children in particular it was the cross-gender social-distanceratings (SDc) that related most strongly with the crossg e n d e rh u m o r r a t i n g s( H U c : - . 1 4 , p < . 0 0 1 ) . T h e r e l a t i o n s h i pb e t w e e n SDs and HUs. while lower, was still strongestwithin the controversial children as compared to the other social-statusgroups. For the other social-statusgroups,the relationshipbetweenSDc and HUc and between t S d S tl 7 S b n Tr i Pr ln SI H H C( T: ST Hr HI Re ln. ST HI HI No *p To bc or not to be lunrorous 131 and rejected SDs and HUs was more varied. While for the neglected significant groups the relationship between SDs ancl HUs was the only group it was the relationshipbetweenthesevariables,with the average was present strongest relationship. While no significant relationship between relationships the betweenSDt and HUt for the popular children, I 7t 68 .0i .67 .59 the us) 'es. 00r ial ach the did rrethe SDcandHUcandSDsandHUswereiclenticalbutweak(-.30and - . 3 2 . p < . 0 5 ) . T h e s ef i n d i n g ss u g g e stth a t t h e r e l a t i o n s h i pb e t w e e ns o c i a l distanceandhumor(eithernegativeorpositive)isdependentonsocialas the gender of status and the gender of the chilcl who is rated as well the rater. was to replicate Tltereplication. One of the purposesof the presentstudy 9 - y e a ro l d s i n t h e o f d a t a t h e S h e r m a n ' s( 1 9 8 S ) s t u d y a n d t o c o m p a r e bothstu<lies.InTable6(seealsoTable2).thesame-andcross-gender in the Sherman mean socialdistanceand humor scoresby genderreported trttss-gtnrlcr' \0(iul-di'\lun((' (sDs Table 5. corrt,ltttiDrtsL|efiitienl,s halx eut lhe sutrtc-ttntl groups thc .fivt difli'rtttt to(iol-sldtLt,t untl SDt.) drtd huntor ruring,s(HLts ttnd HLL) lor [r:59] SDs SDc HUs HUc -.05 .30* -. t0 C o n t r o v .( r I S D I ' H U t ] : rese 'sial 'cial oxi.ned lof rder 'OSS- veen rsial rther veen A v e r a g e( f l S D t . H U t l : - . 2 5 * ) P o p u l a r( / [ S D t . H U t ] : - . 1 5 ) [rr: l4l SDs SDc HUs HUc -.06 SDc HUs .13 .20 .11 N e g l e c t e d( r I S D t . H U t ]: -.78**) SDc HUs -.J7 R e j e c t e d( r l S D t . H U t l - - . 2 5 ) SDs SDc HUs HLIc .t0 - .36* * p < . 0 5 :* * P< . 0 1 SDc .0d .)0 [ r r- . { 1 ] SDs .21 .15** .19 -. -i,9 [r:41 ] SDc [rr:7t)] HUs .I0 .l-5* -.34*) SDc -.06 .1 9 HUs 132 |tl. Warnars-Kleverlaan,L. Oppenheilner,and L. Sherman Table 6. Sume- and tross-gender mean sotiul-distunte and ltLunor scores by gentler Jitt Slrcnnun'.s( 1988:396) and the present studv (underlined) Tal rati Social distance Gender of rater Humor Gender ol'rater same CTOSS Female 2.89 2.87 2.12. 2.13 I.88 L29 2.91 2 42 Male 2.89 3.07 2.37 2.38 2.06 1.31 3 . 5 6 2 . 72 (1988) and the present study are presented.Similarly, in TableT, the intercorrelations between same- and cross-gender social distance and humor are given. From Table 6, a number of interesting differencesbetween Sherman's ( 1988) findingsand the resultsof the presentstudy with a Dutch population can be observed.While Sherman(1988) does not report same-and cross-gender differences in the humor ratings, in the present study a significant same-genderdifference is present showing that males receive a significantly higher humor rating from males than femalesreceivefrom f e m a l e s( / ( 3 9 ) : 2 . 0 5 , p < . 0 5 ) . W i t h t h e s o c i a ld i s t a n c er a t i n g st h e s a m e gender ratings are considerablylower for the Dutch than for the American sample. Dutch children perceive their own gender peers to be closer (to have less social distance) than the American children. With the crossgender ratings the findings of the present study show a similar effect as "which males received reported by Sherman. That is, cross-genderratings from females were significantly higher (greater social distance) than the cross-genderratings which females received from males." Despite these differencesand the considerably higher relationshipsin the present study between same- and cross-gendersocial-distanceratings (.63 versus.3l) and between same-genderhumor ratings and cross-gendersocial distance ratings (-.32 versus .18; seeTable 7), the correlation patterns for the presentand Sherrnan'sstudy are very similar. Discussion The guiding hypothesisof the presentstudy statedthat humor as a social "interpersonal communication behavior" would facilitate competenceor social interaction (Sherman 1988: 390) and reduce the social distances SD HL HL Ger bet 19', ASS (M I chi chi "fu un( Ou prir anc nol ist reg iori bec to( psy Cor "dir teri soci Spe that alit. To be or not to be humorous 133 TableT.TlrcittterttlrrelcltitlnnrutrirJbrsante-untlr'ross-gentlerstlcial-distant'ean.dhutnor p r e s e n ts t u d l ( u n d e r l i n e d n' : 8 6 ) r a t i n g sf b r S h e r m u n ' s( 1 9 5 8 . 3 9 7 ;n - 7 4 ) a n t l t h e SDc SDs 7) Ihe ,nd .n's rlarnd y a :ive om mecan (to CSS- tas Lved the nese udy .31) lnce 'the SDc HUs HUc Gender . 3 1* * -.63** .30* .14 HUs HUc Sher Sher Pres -.18 -.31*+ .02 .40** .27* .6lxx .00 .67** -.t6 t1 Mrte * p< . 0 5 *; * p< . 0 1. "humorous" individual and others( Kane, Suls,and Tedeschi betweenthe 1977;Mastenl986;Shermanl988).Reducedsocialdistanceswere groups of children assumed to affect peer relations and peer status in ( M a s t e n1 9 8 6 ) . "good senseof humor" certainly is a characteristicthat Humor or a with other children take into accountwhen evaluatingtheir relationships the word chil<lren.Conforming with McGhee's ( 1979) assumption' "funny" was indeed conceived by children to mean humorous' The are older' understandingand perception of humor changesas children is humorousness children' Our {rndings suggestthat with the younger funny behaving primarily defined by observablebehavior, by acting or in terms that are not and by telling jokes. Often thesejokes are worded normallyuseclinthehomeandclassroomsetting.Humorousness,then' with the is perceivedas behavior (acting and jokes) that is incongruous behavon emphasis regular normative setting (cf. McGhee 1979)' This Humorousness ioral incongruity decreasesas the children are older' that ls' remarks' witty produce to ability becomesmore defined by the or a personality of in terms to ofl'er apt retorts and is thought of more psychologicalcharacteristicthanperceivedasabehavioralcharacteristic. Correspondingwiththesechangesincorrceptionso|humorousness|rom "dirty joke" to apt retort and from a behavioralto a personalitycharac- ocial litate tnces teristic,alsoexplanationsinvolvingcharacteristicssuchascreativity. "having guts" becomemore apparent' social distance,appearance,and may imply Speculatively,this increasein the variety of characteristics thatfiomtheageofl2childrencometounderstandthenrultidimensionality of the concept of humor ( Foot 199I ) ' 134 N. Warnars-Kleverlacm, L. Oppenheimer,and L. Sherman Irrespectiveof age, humorousnessis perceiveddifferentlyby girls and boys. Gender differencesin humor perceptionwere reported by McGhee ( 1 9 1 9 ) a n d Z i v ( 1 9 8 4 ) .A c c o r d i n gt o M c G h e e ( 1 9 1 9 ) , b o y s m o r e o f t e n create humor, girls more often enjoy it. A possibleexplanation for this gender differenceis sought by McGhee in the relation betweenhumor and gender-roleexpectations.That is. becausehumor is frequentlyaggressive, the initiation of humor is often consideredto be more appropriate fbr boys than for girls. ln the presentstudy. genderalso plays an important role in perceivedhumor; not only are children ol the same gender perceivedto be more humorous than children of the oppositegender(cf. Shcrman 19U8),but boys are perceivedto be more humorous by their male undfemale peers.Though this finding could be acceptedas support l b r M c G h e e ' s( 1 9 7 9 ) a s s u m p t i o ni,t i s n o t c l e a rw h e t h e rt h e h i g h e rl e v e l of perceived humorousnessis a result of a more aggressive type of humor, or that boys initiate humor more often than girls. or becausegirls and boys experiencehumor dillerently. ln the present study. the younger boys were consideredto be much rnore humorous by their male peersthan their femalepeers.While there is ample evidencethat younger children "overattribute" in their social j u d g m e n t s( c l ' .H c l l e r a n d B e r n d t 1 9 8 1 ) ,t h i s s h o u l d h a v e o c c u r r e dw i t h both boys and girls. However, while these same-genderhigher humor perceptionslbr boys decreasedwith age, with the girls, no such agerelated changesin the same-genderhumor perceptionswere observed. fhe convergenceof same-and cross-genderhumor perceptionsof boys and girls at the age of 15 suggeststhat already at the age of 9 girls perceive humorousnessdifferently, perhaps more "realistically" than males.The latter finding suggeststhat an argument could be made that gender differencesin humor perception are due to diflerencesin the experienceof humor by girls and boys. rather than the aggressivityor the frequencyof initiation of humor. Clearly more researchwill be needed to clarify this issue.In addition, il overattributionsare made with respect to peer evaluations,it is not clear whether such overattributionsare also made with respectto younger and older children.To study this issue,use could be made of mixed-age ciassrooms (for example. within the Montessori educational system), younger and older children are still sufTiciently known to the children to permit valid evaluationsof a particular characteristic( humorousness). Gender differenceswere also observedfbr social distance.While girls pcrceivedthemselvesto be closer to one another than boys (cf. Gilligan I a d p ir tl c( al Ir id di re ca fo in ab fer pe Fc hu rel ob tio col Sln pe( jud the oul Spt intr dec pla soc &8' T stal I Ttt be or not to be huntorous 135 I n IS )I S- ,rer -f :ir )rt rel 1f nd rch :ial 'ith 10r ge'ed. oys ;irls nan ;hat the /or :ded pect also , use the srill ticugirls ligan 1982;Tannenl990)'theperceivedsocialdistancesbetweengirlsaswell themselvesand boys as the social distancesperceivedby girls between the other hand' the did not change lbr different ages' With boys, on male peers perceived social distances between themselvesand their increase,linolderboys,whiletheperceivedsocialdistancebetween themselvesand girls decreased. as a social ln the introductlon to this study, it was arguedthat humor competencewillaffectsocialclistancesbetweentlrehumorousindividual andothers.Anassurnptionthatpossessesanalmoslintuitivevalidity: with! The recurrent' Indivrdualswith a good senseof humor arc fun to be humor and social perceived identicalpatternsof interrelationshipsanrong distancefortheagegroupSstudiedindicatedthalperceivedlrunrorousness correlatton pattern relatesto perceivedsocial distance.Each recurrent c a n t h e t l b c p c r e c i v c da s a r e p l i c a t i o n ' variable That is, if perceivedhumorousnessis taken as the explanatory changes for variance in social distances,then thc absenccof age-related nicely explains thc in girls' perceivedhumorousnessof girls and boys changesin girls' perccivedsocialdistancesto their absenceo1 erge-related perceivedtheir male femaleand male peers.Similarly. becauseolclerboys peerslobelesshumorous.thesocialciistirnccsbetweenboysincreased. horvever' perceived For t1.,. boys' perceivcd social distancesto girls' While no agehumorousnessfails to offer a satisfactoryexplalatiol was humorousness girls' ol' relateclchanges in thc boys' perception observed,theboys'perceivedsocialdistancestogirlsdecreased.lnaddifemale peers' as tion, the girls' low perceivedhumorousnesso1' their comparedtotheirmalepeers,doesnotcorrespondtothcgirls'perceived to their male smallersocial distanccsto their female peersas compared peers.Whileourfindingsamplydemonstratedthatchildrenareabletcr ( I 979)' judge therr peersln terms of humorousnessor funniness McGhee thelatterfin<lingsSuggestthata..goodsenseoIhttmor''orbeinghumorin social distance' ous may not be sufficient to explain differcnces Speculatively,itcouldbearguedthataschildrenbecomeoldertheir which could explainthe observed interestin the oppositegenderincreases, decreaseoftheboys'perceivcdsocialdistancestogirls"butleavesunex' girls' perceived plaine<lwhy the same decreaseclid not occur with the to assulne reason no is there socialclistancesto boys. At the age of 15. gcnder' for the oppositc a gender * B , v r e difference l a t i n g p e rin c ethe i v e attraction dhumorousnessandsocialdistancetotheSocial statuso|chilclren.additionalsuppol.tforthisSuggestionrvasobtaineci. 136 l,{. Warnars-Kleverlaan,L. Oppenheirner,and L. Sherntun The division of the Dutch children over the five social status groups (2601, popular, I 8% neglected,I 8% rejected,6%ocontroversial,and 3 loh average,) shows an approximate correspondencewith the division reported by Coie, et al. (1982) for American children (22o/opopular,24'''/oneglected, 24oh rejected, 130locontroversial, and l6ok average).When dealing with social status in relation to perceived humorousnessand social distance, it is important to note that the determination of the social status of a child is based on peer evaluations irrespectiveof gender.This implies that perceived humorousnessand social distance irrespectiveof gender only, that is, the total humorousnessand social-distance scores(HUt and SDt) can be relatedvalidly to social status. When only the relationshipsbetween total perceivedhumorousness and social distance are considered,the previously discussedrecurrent relationship betweenhumor and social distancecould not be observed for the social status groups. Notwithstanding the fact that popular children are perceivedby their peers to be cooperative,to show leadership behavior,and seldomdisrupt the group, fight, and ask for help (seeCoie, et al. 1982), and contrary to the expectation,no relationship between humor and social distancecould be demonstrated.That is. while these children were found to possessthe smallest social distance to other children, they were not perceivedto be the children with the highest humor ratings. While the rejectedchildren demonstratedthe largestsocial distanceto other children and obtained the lowest humor ratings, no relationship between perceived humorousnessand social distance was evident. ln contrast to the popular children, rejectedchildren are perceivedto show little cooperation and no leadershipqualities; they often disrupt the group, fight, and seek help. For the neglectedand average children, relationships,while moderate,were presentbetweenperceivedhumorousnessand social distance.Neglectedchildren distinguishthemselvesfrom the rejectedchildren in that they are not actually disliked by their peers; they are just not mentioned as children others would want to be friends with and are a low visibility group. The averagechildren constitutethe largest group of children within a peer group and are evaluatedto be averagein cooperation,leadership,attraction. etc. The controversial children are described as an intermediate group betweenthe popular and rejectedchildren. Thesechildren are perceived to combine characteristicsof both social-statusgroups.They are similar cl r2 g g ju lil di ra wl is b1 to fir it5 T- I To be or not to be hwnorous 131 ,o i'l by ',1 ith f a 1at rly, )t) .ESS ent ved hilhip lese :her xest eto ;hip .ln now the .ren, ousrom ends r the obe roup rived nilar to the rejected children in that they are perceivedas disruptive and start fights; they also frequently seek help. On the other hand, they are also perceivedas leaders in the group. They are not perceivedas cooperative but they are also not perceived as lacking this behavior. Coie, Dodge, "visible, active, and and coppotelli (1982) describe these children as assertive ... Sometimes this activity takes the form of leadership and sometimesit puts them in demand as leaders" (p.565). Elsewhere,the "one might speculatethat controversial children same authors note that possessmore positive social skills than they are described as having" ( p .s 6 8 ) . with the controversial children, the relationship between perceived humorousnessand social distance was substantial. If social distance is affectedby humor, as was stated in the guiding hypothesisfor this study, then for this group more than 60 percent of the variance in social distance is explained by humor. Hence, a good senseof humor may be one of "more positive skills" possessedby the controversial children that those make them a small but very special group of children within their peer groups(coie, et al. 1982).Perhapsthe controversialchildren are able to "effectively," that is, by using humor to facilitate social use humor more interaction thereby promoting friendships and relationships and by using humor as a socially acceptableway to dissociatepeople or expressin a (cf. socially acceptable manner negative feelings towards others Martineau 1972). The controversial children distinguished themselves from the other children on a secondcharacteristic.In general,higher same-genderhumor judgments' A ratings were associatedwith higher same-genderlikeability was also the be humorous girl who was judged by her lemale peers to girl who was well-liked by her female peers and similarly, a boy who was judged by his male peers to be humorous was also the boy who was wellliked by his male peers. The controversial children demonstrated a different pattern. with these children, the higher same-genderlikeability ratings related much stronger with the cross-genderhumor ratings than with the same-genderhumor ratings. This finding shows that a boy who is judged by hisfemale peers to be humorous is the boy who is best liked by h\s male peers and vice versa a girl who is judged by her nrale peers to be humorous is the girl who is best liked by her female peers. These findings suggestthat controversial children differ on a variety ofpersonality and social characteristicsfrom the other children' 138 N. Warnars-Kleverlaan,L. Oppenheimer,and L. Sherman While the rejected children were clearly less liked and were perceived to be the least humorous, the neglected children did not distinguish themselvesfrom the averagechildren in their peer group. These children were perceived to possessan average level of humorousnessand to be reasonably well liked. Speculatively, it could be argued that neglected children are not so much children with whom other children do not like "voluntarily have no to be friends, but rather that they are children who need" to be friends with other children within the peer group. This assumption may be based on differencesbetweenthe educational systems in the United States and the Netherlands. In the Dutch educational system, activities like sports, music, and arts are not part of the educational system but are offered by private clubs and institutions. Children "outside who participate in these activities the school" are often not the same children who form the peer group in a classroom and are often not from the same school. Children in the Netherlands regularly function in two or more completely different peer groups. Consequently,a child may have friends in one peer group and be perceivedas popular (for example, the soccer team) and not feel the need for the formation of additional friendships with children in another peer group (the classroom). The study of social status formation in peer groups that function outside the educationalsystem(the classroom)is required to verify this assumption. However, the nature of the groups, particularly with respect to how sports teams are formed on the basis of gender, may obviously constrain the examinationof same-and cross-genderevaluations. As was previously noted, the presenceand absence of relationships between humor and social distance, as well as the differencesin relationships among same- and cross-genderperceivedhumorousnessand social distance within the different social status groups, may be a consequence The reported findings of the way in which social status has been assessed. should, consequently,be cautiously interpreted. The observation that the relationship between social distance and humor is dependent on socialstatus and the gender of the child who is doing the evaluating, as well as the gender of the child who is being evaluated, concurs with this assumption. In other words, the recurrent relationships between humor and social distancewithin each age group, which could be consideredas replications. are not necessarilysufficient reason to interpret statistically significant relationships as relevant scientific findings. The (theoretical) assumption t s r r t ! n b s' u d o p p s1 p o g( al r2 A p( g( g( g( re al (l m aE es of di to m' To be or not to be humorous 139 red Lish ren be ted tike no 'his lms rnal rcatren the not nin nay ple, )nal The the ion. now rain hips .ion:cial ence lings t the rcialdl as this ocial ions, icant ption that humor will reduce social distance(the assumption of causaldirection; Sherman 1988) should have resulted in stronger relationshipsbetween perceived humor and social distance than observed in this study. The present findings suggestthat humor as an explanatory variable' irrespecthe tive of the social status of the children, explains at most 36% of competencies) (other social variables Other variance in social distance. may play as important a role in the formation of social distance' ihe pr"uiously discussedfindings proceed beyond the results reported by Sherman in his 1988 study which formed the basis for the present siudy. When the flndings of the present study for the 9-year-old children *"r. "o.npured to the findings reported by Sherman, similarities and differenceswere observed.However, despitedifferencesin the magnitude of the relationships, the interrelationshipsamong same-and cross-gender perceivedhumorousnessand social distance showed remarkably identical putt"rn, for the American and the Dutch children. While in Sherman's study, children who are perceived to be humorous by their same-gender peers are not perceived to possesssmaller social distancesby their peers of th. opposite gender, in the Dutch sample the peers of the opposite gender also perceivethese children to possesssmaller social distances.In uOaltion, the relationship between same-and cross-gendersocial-distance ratings with the Dutch children is considerably stronger than with the Ameiican children. This finding is probably basedon the Dutch children's perception and/or experienceof considerably smaller, same- and crosspeer group and a lesseremphasison lender social distanceswithin their gender (see, for instance, the absenceof a relationship between crossgender with the Dutch children). With lender social-distanceratings and perceptions of the Dutch cross-gender and iespect to humor, the sameand American children are almost identical' In summary, the presentstudy detailed the resultsreported by Sherman (1988) an<lamply demonstratedthat humor is a complex concept' The meaning and interpretation of humor and perceived humorousness is "good senseof humor" plays a role in the ug.- und gender-related' A establishmentof social distances among children and in the formation of the social status of a child. To be humorous does seem to make a difference.The present study, however, could not give conclusiveanswers "how?" As has been noted throughout the discussion to the question more researchis definitely needed. Universiteit van Amsterdam and Miami University, Oxford' OH 140 N. Warnars-Kleverlaan, L. Oppenheimer,and L. Slrcrman N References o Asher, S. A. and K. A. Dodge 1986 tdentifying children who are rejected by their peers. Developmental Ps1'chology 22, 444 449. Babad.E. Y. 1974 A multi-method approach to the assessmentof humor: A critical look at humor 1ests.Journal of Persctnalitl42,619 631. Bell, N. J., P. E. McGhee, and N. S. Duffey Interpersonal competence, social assertivenessand the development of I9 8 6 humor. -Britrs&Journal of DevelopmentalPst't'hobgl 4, 5l 55. Chapman,A. J. 1973 Social facilitation of laughter in children. Journul ol Experimental Social Ps1'chologl'9, 528 541. Coie, J. D., K. A. Dodge, and H. Coppotelli 1982 Dimensions and types oi social status: A cross-age perspective. DevelopntentalPsychology 18, 557 570. Darwin, C. 1873 Foot, H. 1 9 9I Gilligan, C. 1982 Goodman. J. 1983 SI Sh The Expressionof Emotions in Man and Aninral.s.[Reprint 1955.New York: Philosophical Library.l Ta The psychology of humor and laughter. In Cochrane, R. and D. Caroll (eds.), Psychologyand Social li.raes. London: Falmer Press, I 14. Tb In a Diflerent Voice; Psychological Theory and Wonten's Development Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Tb w: How to get more smileage out of your life: Making senseof humor, then serving it. In McGhee, P. E. and J. H. Goldstein (eds.), Handbook oJ Research in Humor: hl. II. Applied S/rdle"t. New York: Springer-Verlag, | 2t. Heller. K. A. and T. J. Berndt Developmental changes in the formation and organization of personality l98l attributions. Child Dewbpntent 52.683 691. K a n e . T . R . . J . S u l s .a n d J . T . T e d e s c h i Humour as a tool of social interaction. In Chapman, A. J. and H. C. Foot 1971 (eds.), ft.i a Funny Thing, Huntour. Oxford: Pergamon, l3 16. Krasnor. L. and K. Rubin The assessmentof social problem-solving skills in young children. In New Merluzzi, T., C. Glass, and M. Genest (eds.), Cognltlle l.rse.r.snlertl. York: Guilford Press. Martineau, W H . A model of the social functions of humor. In Goldstein, J. H. and P. E. 1972 McGhee (eds.), ?'fte Psy'chobgy of Hwrutr. New York: Academic Press, 101 125. M a s t e n ,A . S . I 986 Pr Humor and competence in school-aged children. Child Detelopment 57, 16t 473. Zit To be or not to be humorous t 4 l McGhee,P. E. 1979 Humor It'.s Origins and Development.San Francisco: Freeman. L. Oppenheimer, The nature of social action: Social oompetenceversus social conformism. I 989 In Schneider, B. H., G. Attili, J. Nadel, and R. Weissberg (eds.), Socral (\tmpetente in DevelopntentalPerspective.Dordrecht: Kluwer, 4l 69. .ntal kat tof ccial tlve. 'ork: aroll menl then *oJ . r l eo Podilchak, W. 1992 Fun, funny, fun-of humor and laughter. Humor: Interruttional Journal of Huntor Research5 4,375 396. Sherman. L. W. Humour and social distance ratings among elementary school children: 1985a Some differential sex and age patterns. ln MacHale. D. (ed.), Proceedings, FiJih Interruttionol ConJerenceon Humour. ERIC documenr ED 263-976. Dublin: Boole. 1985b H u m o r a n d s o c i a l d i s t a n c e .P e r t e p t u a lu n d M o t o r S k i l l s 6 1 , 1 2 7 4 . Humor and social distance in elementary school children. Humor: 1988 Internationul Journal of Humor Researchl, 389 404. Sherman,L. W. and Wolf, A. lntrapersonal perceptions of shynessand humor as related to interpersonal 1984 perceptions of social distance and humorousness.Paper presented at the 4th International Congress on Humor. Tel Aviv. Tannen, D. You Just Don't Understand: Women and Men in Conversation New York: 1990 William Morrow and Comoanv. T h o m s o n .J . A . K . 1966 The Ethics oJ Aristotle. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books. T h o r s o n .J . A . a n d F . C . P o w e l l Measurement of senseof humor. Ps,-chologitalReports 69, 691 702 l99l Warnars-Kleverlaan,N. and L. Oppenheimer 1989 Social competence and locus of control: A relationship reconsidered. In Schneider, B. H., G. Attili, J. Nadel, and R. Weissberg (eds.), Socral Competencein Devebpmental Perspet'tive.Dordrecht: Kluwer, 4l l-413. Ziv, A. I 981 rality Foot n. In New P. E. Press, n 5'1, Personality and Sen,seof Huntor. New York: Springer. HUTIUI;i 1 9 9 6V o l u m e9 - 2 CONTENTS R E V I E WA R T I C L E T o b e o r n o t t o b e h u m o r o u s :D o e s it makea difference? Nel Warnars-Kleverlaan, Louis Oppenheimer, and Larry Sherman 1 1 7 T h e p o t e n t i a lf o r u s i n g h u m o r i n g l o b a la d v e r t i s i n g Lynette S. Unger 143 W o m e n ,a g i n g ,a n d s e n s eo f h u m o r James A. Thorson and F. C. Powell 1 6 9 P u n s :S e c o n dt h o u g h t s Walter David Redfern 181 Partisan perceptionsof political numor Richard E. Weise 199 NOTES L e n n yB r u c e ' ss e m a n t i ch o l e s Sol Saporta 209 F a l l e nA n g e l o ' sd i s r e s p e c t f uwl o r d p l a y :A b l a s p h e m o u ps u n i n Measure for Measure Robert F. Fleissner 211 Humor and play in Native American life, review of lndi'n Humor: Bicultural Play in Native America, by K e n n e t hL i n c o l n Allan J. Byan zt3 B O O KR E V I E W S Gail Finney (ed.), Look Who's Laughing:Genderand Comedy Annette Stavely 221 A n a t Z a j d m a n ,H u m o r Avner Ziv 224 D e b o r a hJ . H i l l , S c h o o l D a y s , F u n Days: Creative Ways to Teach Humor Skillsin the Classroom Jordan D. Brown 225 Thomas Pughe, Comic Sense; Reading Robert Coover, Stanley Elkin, Philip Roth John G. Parks 228 NEWSLETTER tssN0933-1719 Mouton de Gruyter. Berlin . New York