Interviewing Children about Multiple Similar Events Why is it important?
Transcription
Interviewing Children about Multiple Similar Events Why is it important?
Brubacher: Interviewing about Multiple Similar, 2014 Interviewing Children about Multiple Similar Events Why is it important? • Child sexual abuse: 43 – 75% cases involve allegations of multiple events • Particularization – Episodic accounts of individual incidents Sonja P. Brubacher, PhD Central Michigan University bruba1sp@cmich.edu 28th Annual San Diego International Conference on Child and Family Maltreatment Jan 31, 2014 • Not always required (e.g., People v. Jones, 1990) – Can enhance credibility Podirsky v R., 1990; R. v B. [G.], 1990; S v. R, 1989; Burrows & Powell, 2013 Why is it difficult? • Important Terminology – Multiple Similar or Repeated Events – Script • Generic details • What happens? You can do X – Semantic vs. episodic ‘re-living’ (e.g., Tulving, 2000) Challenges in retrieving episodes Occ 2 Episode 3 • Organized structures • Makes experience predictable • E.g., restaurant script • Contain slots for alternatives • Recall of generic details easier than recall of specific episodes (Hudson & Nelson, 1986) – Episode • Specific details • What happened? She did X Occ 1 Scripts Specific versus Generic Memories Occ 4 • Source-monitoring (Johnson et al., 2003) Episodic language: “We sang ‘Happy Birthday’ and ate chocolate cake” Generic language (timeless present): “You sing songs, you eat cake” Event-specific details: “We played pin the tail on the donkey” Generalized knowledge: “You can play games” “I got a treat bag with stars on it to take home” Conditionals: “If you were good, you might get a treat bag” Specific sequences: “At the end, I opened my presents” Optionals: “You can open presents before you eat cake or after” – Source similarity • Script/gist interference (Brainerd & Reyna, 2004; Powell et al., 1999) 1 Brubacher: Interviewing about Multiple Similar, 2014 Toolbox • • • • • • • Episodic Narrative Practice • Practice is important component of many guidelines (e.g., NICHD; Lamb et al., 2007; Stepwise; Yuille, 2002) • ↑ Amount and/or Accuracy Episodic Narrative Practice Frequency Questions Open-ended prompts Utility of generic narrative Labels Differences Language (Lamb et al., 2013; Roberts et al., 2004; Sternberg et al. 1997) • Practice talking about episodes of repeated events transfers to more specific talk in substantive phase (Brubacher et al., 2011) See Roberts, Brubacher, Powell, & Price, 2011, for a review Practice: Awareness of Frequency Episodic Narrative Practice (Brubacher et al., 2011) Incident Practice Script Practice Novel Practice Time you remember best of swimming lessons & another What usually happens at swimming lessons Time you rode a horse (only once) • 5-8 year olds; 1or 4 times (N = 240) Script Practice Novel Practice Time you remember best of swimming lessons & another What usually happens at swimming lessons Time you rode a horse (only once) Percentage of Children Incident Practice “Target/Substantive” phase (Laurier Activities) Activities introduced Asked about frequency if not spontaneously provided Practice: Language use in Target 70 60 Asked 60 50 50 40 40 30 30 20 20 10 10 0 Incident Script Novel Incident 5- & 6-year olds Script Novel 7- & 8-year olds Episodic Narrative Practice • Can model to children that individual episodes are important for discussion (Brubacher et al., 2011) b – Spontaneous reports of frequency Repeated Single Repeated Single Repeated 60 Single Percent Episodic 80 Delayed a a 80 40 Immediate 70 0 Free recall narrative – episodic prompts only Specific occurrence 100 80 20 0 Incident Incident Practice Script Script Practice Novel Novel Practice Time you remember best of swimming lessons & another What usually happens at swimming lessons Time you rode a horse (only once) • Did not increase false multiple reports by children with single experience – Increases specific details – Practices thinking about what is different • Models ‘labelling’ of occurrences • Need not take longer than standard practice 2 Brubacher: Interviewing about Multiple Similar, 2014 Frequency Questions 90 • Children struggle to provide exact numbers – Can affect credibility • “One time or more than one time” (Lamb et al, 2007; Orbach & Pipe, 2011) “I don’t know. 50 times out of the whole entire year. I don’t really do numbers.” (People vs. Avila, 2001). One 80 Percent of Responses (Sharman et al., 2011) • Often use words they do not understand when referring to time, frequency and duration (Poole & Lamb, 1998) Frequency Questions: Children’s responses 70 Four 60 50 DK 40 30 3 to 5 20 10 0 Other 0 0 Single Multiple Drohan-Jennings, Roberts, Brubacher, Glisic, Powell, & Friedman (2013, April) Frequency Questions in Investigative Interviews • 15% of interviews did not contain any Frequency Qs (see Brubacher et al., 2013) • As estimated number of incidents increased, so did Qs about Frequency – Linear relationship between Abuse Severity and Number of Frequency Qs asked # Frequency Qs 3 Open-ended Prompts • Critical for all interviews • Age-related improvements • Increased complexity in cases involving allegations of multiple abuse – Suggestive open-ended prompts must be avoided: • Content 2 – “tell me everything about when he took off his t-shirt” • Temporal components 1 – “tell me everything about when he took off his shirt the time he tried to kiss you” 0 Exposure n= 4 Touch over n = 16 Touch under n = 42 Penetration n = 35 – Temporally-suggestive prompts are especially difficult (Powell et al., 2000) Table includes unpublished data from Brubacher et al., 2013 Open-ended Prompts: Temporally-linked details • Allowing children to describe “what usually happens” (see Brubacher et al., 2012) 2.5 Number of Details * 2 1.5 True Details Suggested Details 1 0.5 Utility of Generic Narrative – Appears not to be detrimental to accuracy – May increase amount of overall information – May produce “episodic leads” • More common as incidents increase (Brubacher et al. 2013; see also Brubacher & La Rooy, 2013) 0 Linked to episode Not linked Powell et al., 2000 3 Brubacher: Interviewing about Multiple Similar, 2014 Number of difference references Utility of Generic Narrative: Reporting of Episodic Leads Episodic Leads & Labels The last time • Labeling occurrences: Challenge for investigative interviewing (Powell et al., 2007) 3 2.5 • Labels: “the time/day when…” 2 The time in the shed • Episodic Leads: specific detail not used in labeling manner 1.5 • If distinctive details can be identified, occurrences should be easier to discriminate (e.g., Johnson et al., 1993) 1 0.5 0 Generic Narrative first Episodic Narrative first Order of Recall • Labels help structure reports • “Tell me about the time in the car park” • “What else happened the time you were home sick from school?” Brubacher, Roberts & Powell (2012) * 7- and 8-year olds only Labels in Forensic Interviews Label Types in Forensic Interviews Proportion of Labels Generated • Interviewers used more Temporal Labels than did children (Brubacher et al., 2013) 0.6 0.5 * • Age positively correlated with proportion of childprovided (versus interviewer-provided) labels • Interviewers, however, replaced the children’s labels with their own 35% of the time – E.g., “the time in the shed” “the last time” – Because these are field interviews, we do not know how often these replacements were inaccurate Interviewer 0.4 Child 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Temporal Locational Abuse Situational Label Type • The more often the interviewer replaced the child’s labels for occurrences, the less likely the child was to follow the interviewer’s conversational shifts (Brubacher et al., 2013) Differences • What if children do not spontaneously provide any Episodic Leads or Labels? – Little published evidence, but: • “Was there ever a time when something different happened?” coupled with “tell me about that time” • “Did (label) happen any other time?” – When this question was included in a lab-based study, 98% of the resultant labels were unique to only one episode – 66% of the total labels were generated by children (all were [eventually] unique) Brubacher, 2011; Brubacher et al., 2011 March Watch your language! • Episodic prompts – – – – “Tell me what happened that time” “You said ‘he got on the bed’. Then what happened?” “What else happened, the time mum was at the shops?” “Ok, you said there was a time he grabbed you in the shed. Tell me everything that happened” • Generic prompts – “Tell me what happens”“You said ‘he gets on the bed.’ Then what happens?” – “What else happens, when mum’s gone out?” – “Ok, you said you’ve been in the shed with him a few times. Tell me what happens.” See Brubacher 2011; 2012; 2013; Schneider et al., 2010 4