The Vision of Hsiao on Somatosensation
Transcription
The Vision of Hsiao on Somatosensation
Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (November 12, 2014). doi:10.1152/jn.00670.2014 1 2 3 The Vision of Hsiao on Somatosensation 4 Martha Flanders and John F. Soechting 5 Laboratory of Neurophysiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455 6 7 8 9 10 11 Running title: Research contributions of S.S. Hsiao 12 Keywords: somatosensory, haptics, touch, Pacinian corpuscle, neuroprosthetics 13 14 15 16 Corresponding author: 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 Martha Flanders 6-145 Jackson Hall 321 Church St SE Minneapolis, MN 55455 Phone (612) 624-6601 Fax (612) 626-5009 fland001@umn.edu 1 Copyright © 2014 by the American Physiological Society. 27 ABSTRACT 28 29 The goal of this review is to start to consolidate and distill the substantial body of 30 research that comprises the published work of the late Professor Steven S. Hsiao. The 31 studies of Hsiao began by demonstrating the receptive field properties of somatosensory 32 neurons, progressed to describing cortical feature selectivity, and then eventually elevated 33 the field to hopes of tapping into natural neural codes with artificial somatosensory 34 feedback. With ongoing analogies to contemporaneous studies in visual neuroscience, 35 the research results and writings of Hsiao have provided the fields of haptics and 36 somatosensory neurophysiology with the conceptual tools needed to allow profound 37 progress. Specifically, Hsiao suggested that slowly-adapting tactile form perception 38 could be restored with cortical microstimulation, rapidly-adapting slip reflexes should be 39 relegated to low-level, hard-wired prosthetic components, and Pacinian-corpuscle 40 spatiotemporal population responses could potentially be decoded/encoded to provide 41 information about interactions of hands and hand-held instruments with external objects. 42 Future studies will be guided by these insightful reports from Hsiao. 43 44 2 45 BEGINNINGS 46 47 Twenty-two years ago, we (Soechting and Flanders 1992) were neighbors with Steve 48 Hsiao and his mentor in a volume of the Annual Review of Neuroscience (Johnson and 49 Hsiao 1992). In their review, our contemporaries put forward the most lucid description 50 of tactile receptor sensitivities that we have encountered to date, outlining the 51 complementary nature of slowly-adapting type one (SAI) afferents (from Merkel 52 complexes), rapidly-adapting type one (RAI) afferents (from Meissner corpuscles), and 53 the rapidly-adapting signals from Pacinian corpuscles (PC). They explained the 54 preference of SAIs for isolated curves of skin indentation (LaMotte and Srinivasan 1987; 55 Phillips and Johnson 1981) as a mechanical surround-suppression effect (implicitly 56 analogous to the center-surround organization in the visual system), and went on to 57 speculate that the SAI receptor density may be matched to the skin’s low-pass filtering to 58 achieve the best possible spatial resolution. Whereas SAIs specialize in spatial 59 resolution, encapsulated receptor structures make RAIs and PCs selective for higher 60 frequency vibrations, and these latter two groups are complementary to each other in their 61 superficial location/small receptive field size (RA) compared to deep location/large 62 receptive field size (PC). With the example of tool use, Johnson and Hsiao (1992) 63 concisely summarized that SAIs would give a maintained representation of the detailed 64 form of the tool while RAs would, with high spatiotemporal fidelity, signal any slippage 65 of the instrument within the hand. The authors envisioned that the population of PCs 66 would transduce spatiotemporal patterns of transmitted vibrations to represent “transient 67 mechanical events that occur at the working surfaces of the instrument” (p. 246). 3 68 Regardless of whether these words were written by Hsiao or his mentor, this is the vision 69 of somatosensation with which Steven S. Hsiao began his career. 70 71 Hsiao’s first publication was momentous (Phillips et al. 1988). In it, the authors used 72 their new technique of characterizing the response of an individual neuron (peripheral or 73 cortical) by generating a spatial event plot (SEP). Figure 1 expands on Fig. 1 from 74 Phillips et al. (1988) in order to explain the approach. Immobilized monkeys were 75 anesthetized for peripheral recordings but alert for cortical recordings. As shown 76 schematically in Fig 1A, a raised letter was moved across a stationary finger pad. The 77 letters were raised above the background by 500 m, moved across the skin at 50 mm/s, 78 and applied with a servo-controlled normal force of 60 g. After each proximal to distal 79 movement, the letter stimulus was shifted 0.2 mm along its long axis (i.e., perpendicular 80 to the direction of the scanning movement) so that the neuron’s receptive field (dashed 81 circle) now scanned across the letter on a different row. The SEP in Fig. 1B is 82 constructed so that within each row, recorded action potentials can be positioned 83 according to the location of the receptive field relative to a raised letter stimulus at the 84 time when they occurred. In this study, there were 64 sweeps across the letter by the 85 receptive field of a somatosensory neuron, resulting in 64 rows of action potentials in the 86 SEP. 87 88 Figure 1 of Phillips et al. (1988) exhibited the action potentials of a cortical area 3b SA 89 neuron that always fired when the leading edge of the letter “K” was in its receptive field, 90 and fired with high fidelity whenever a raised portion occupied its receptive field. Figure 4 91 2 of Phillips et al. (1988) then revealed that a similarly high spatial resolution was 92 observed in recordings from SAI afferents. We exhibit this phenomenon in Fig. 2 (row 93 2), which is reprinted from a subsequent study where raised dot patterns (350 m) were 94 slowly scanned (20 mm/s) and applied with a servo-controlled normal force of 30 g 95 (Connor et al. 1990). As first shown by Phillips et al. (1988), Fig. 2 (row 3) demonstrates 96 that RA afferents also had sufficiently small receptive fields to provide an isomorphic 97 representation of the stimulus pattern. In contrast, PC afferents (Fig. 2, bottom row) 98 failed to do so because of their large receptive fields and preferred vibration rates (100- 99 300 Hz) above the speed of the scanned stimuli. 100 101 Hsiao and colleagues (Phillips et al. 1988) explained that these SEPs truly represented the 102 response of a single neuron to a stimulus moved across the skin, but also, especially in 103 the case of dense and uniform SA1 and RA afferent populations, they could be 104 interpreted as representing the population response, or the “neural image” of the stimulus. 105 They went on to discuss the sensitivities of various somatosensory cortical neurons as a 106 progression from an isomorphic to an abstracted touch representation. 107 108 As a sign of the times, this 1988 paper was entitled “Spatial pattern representation and 109 transformation…,” similar to our own titles involving sensorimotor transformations (e.g., 110 Soechting and Flanders 1989a,b), where we conceptualized neural processing as 111 occurring in successive stages with information being serially transformed from one stage 112 to the next. The framework laid by Phillips et al. (1988) also broke transformations into 113 sequential stages. They hypothesized that cortical area 3b RA neurons and area 1 5 114 neurons may exhibit intermediate representations – much like the hidden units in Zipser 115 and Anderson’s (1988) model of transformation in parietal visual cortex. 116 117 FEATURES OF SENSORY RESPONSES 118 119 In the 1990’s and beyond, Hsiao and colleagues continued to develop sophisticated 120 methods for precise stimulation (e.g., Lane et al. 2010) and analysis of receptive fields, 121 while their conceptual framework evolved toward feature selectivity and perceptual 122 constancy. The early work on SAI and RA afferents was extended and elaborated by 123 showing the precise features of the stimuli encoded by the afferents, and by deducing that 124 roughness can be encoded by the spatial variations in the firing rate of a population of 125 SAIs (Connor et al. 1990; Blake et al. 1997, see Fig 2). More recently, Hsiao’s former 126 coworker, Bensmaia, further extended this interpretation by showing that the finer 127 features of texture stimuli are represented by spike timing in RA and PC afferents, and 128 has emphasized the central convergence of these different types of peripheral afferent 129 inputs (Weber et al. 2013; Saal and Bensmaia 2014). 130 131 Just as visual cortical neurons are tuned to the orientations of bars, neurons in primary 132 (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortex are tuned to the orientation of edges 133 impressed on the hand, a feature extracted from the response of populations of SAI 134 afferents (Hsiao et al. 2002). S2 neurons have receptive fields extending over multiple 135 digits, with markedly similar tuning properties for each finger (Fitzgerald et al. 2004, 136 2006), facilitating the haptic discrimination of the shapes of large objects. Hsiao and 6 137 colleagues noted the similarities in orientation selectivity in primary visual and 138 somatosensory cortical areas and suggested that these similarities arose from analogous 139 neural mechanisms in these areas (Bensmaia et al. 2008). 140 141 In the visual system, it is well known that the representation of motion is transformed 142 from a representation of the components of a pattern (e.g., the stripes of a plaid) to one of 143 the motion of the entire pattern (the plaid) as one ascends the hierarchy of visual cortical 144 areas (Movshon et al. 1985). Furthermore, this “dorsal stream” transformation evolves 145 gradually over time (Masson and Stone 2002). Hsiao and colleagues discovered an 146 analogous phenomenon in the tactile perception of motion (Pei et al. 2008) and 147 subsequently demonstrated a similar hierarchy in the somatosensory cortical areas (Pei et 148 al. 2011). Neurons in S1 area 3b responded to local (component) motion (analogous to 149 neurons in V1 area 17). However in S1 area 1, some neurons encoded the pattern motion 150 while others were tuned to component motion or exhibited intermediate characteristics 151 (analogous to the medial temporal visual area MT). 152 153 Ascending the hierarchy of “ventral stream” areas, cortical neurons are tuned to 154 progressively more complex features of contours and objects (cf., Brincat and Connor 155 2004). In a collaboration of the Hsiao and Connor laboratories, Yau et al. (2009) 156 explicitly tested the idea that there are analogous coding schemes in visual and 157 somatosensory areas. They recorded activity from intermediate visual area (V4) for 158 visual stimuli, and somatosensory area (S2) for tactile stimuli. In each case, monkeys 159 were presented with contours whose directions of curvature were systematically varied. 7 160 Indeed, they found neurons in both areas that responded briskly to such stimuli, and 161 found very similar patterns of tuning in both areas. Thus the concept of hierarchies of 162 feature representation holds well for both vision and somatosensation. 163 164 165 GOALS FOR THE FUTURE 166 In a recent paper on neuroprosthetics, Hsiao et al. (2011) gave their forecast and 167 recommendations on the prospect of providing artificial somatosensory feedback. 168 Building on decades of his work on feature selectivity, Hsiao predicted that it will 169 eventually be possible, using electrical or optical stimulation in cortex, to find the 170 “natural neural codes” (p. 76) to provide SA-type perception of form. Regarding the 171 restoration of RA-type input, Hsiao et al. (2011) recommended hard-wiring slip reflexes 172 into the mechanical hand itself. The authors reasoned that the user need not be 173 consciously aware of slippage, as long as it is automatically corrected. They compared 174 this to the case of Dr. Strangelove, where the movie character’s funny right hand had a 175 mind of its own. The authors also saw promise in the prospect of tapping into the PC 176 channel, if we can “mimic the temporal pattern of what a PC afferent would have 177 experienced under the same circumstances” (p. 73). The idea is to use the spatiotemporal 178 patterns that naturally arise when a person uses a tool to interact with external objects. In 179 this case the authors mentioned examples of a blind person using a cane and, more 180 dramatically, Luke Skywalker using his prosthetic hand to wield his lightsaber weapon! 181 182 Another current topic that will extend beyond the work of Hsiao is research on spinal 183 circuits, where we are beginning to gain an appreciation for the processing of tactile 8 184 feedback in the dorsal horn. At the end of their detailed review of peripheral and spinal 185 “touch” representations, Abraira and Ginty (2013) thanked their close colleague Steven 186 Hsiao for his helpful comments on the manuscript. Remarkably, these authors analogized 187 the spinal dorsal horn to the retina, for its complexity and importance in early sensory 188 processing. Indeed, individual peripheral afferent neurons may serve as the first stage of 189 directional feature selectivity (Pruszynski and Johansson 2014). Furthermore, this dorsal- 190 horn spinal circuit may be essential for comparing cortical motor commands with 191 somatosensory feedback during tool use and tactile exploration (Flanders 2011; Weiss 192 and Flanders 2011). 193 194 Whereas the pioneering approach of the spatial event plot (Johnson and Hsiao 1992; 195 Phillips et al. 1988) was necessarily done with the hand immobilized, a challenge for 196 future experimentation is to put tools in the hands and let them move freely along 197 surfaces. This future research will be guided by the vision of Hsiao. 198 199 200 Acknowledgements 201 The authors were supported by a grant for the National Institute of Neurological 202 Disorders and Stroke NS027484. We thank the three reviewers for their very helpful 203 suggestions. 204 205 9 206 References 207 Abraira VE, Ginty DD. The sensory neurons of touch. Neuron 79:618-639, 2013. 208 209 Bensmaia SJ, Denchev PV, Dammann JF 3rd, Craig JC, Hsiao SS. The representation of 210 stimulus orientation in the early stages of somatosensory processing. J Neurosci 28:776- 211 786, 2008. 212 213 Blake DT, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO. Neural coding mechanisms in tactile pattern 214 recognition: the relative contributions of slowly and rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors 215 to perceived roughness. J Neurosci 17:7480-7489, 1997. 216 217 Brincat SL, Connor CE, Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior 218 inferotemporal cortex. Nat Neurosci 7: 880-886, 2004. 219 220 Connor CE, Hsiao SS, Phillips JR, Johnson KO. Tactile roughness: Neural codes that 221 account for psychophysical magnitude estimates. J Neurosci 10:3823-3836, 1990. 222 223 Fitzgerald PJ, Lane JW, Thakur PH, Hsiao SS. Receptive field properties of the macaque 224 second somatosensory cortex: evidence for multiple functional representations. J 225 Neurosci 24:11193-11204, 2004. 226 227 Fitzgerald PJ, Lane JW, Thakur PH, Hsiao SS. Receptive field properties of the macaque 228 second somatosensory cortex: representation of orientation on different finger pads. J 229 Neurosci 26:6473-6484, 2006. 230 231 Flanders M. What is the biological basis of sensorimotor integration? Biol Cybern 104:1- 232 8, 2011. 233 234 Hsiao SS, Fettiplace M, Darbandi B. Sensory feedback for upper limb prostheses. Prog 235 Brain Res 192:69-81, 2011. 236 10 237 Hsiao SS, Lane J, Fitzgerald P. Representation of orientation in the somatosensory 238 system. Behav Brain Res 135:93-103, 2002. 239 240 Johnson KO, Hsiao SS. Neural mechanisms of tactual form and texture perception. Ann 241 Rev Neurosci 15:227-250, 1992. 242 243 LaMotte RH, Srinivasan MA. Tactile discrimination of shape: Responses of slowly 244 adapting mechanoreceptive afferents to a step stroked across the monkey fingerpad. J 245 Neurosci 7:1655-1671, 1987. 246 247 Lane JW, Fitzgerald PJ, Yau JM, Pembeci I, Hsiao SS. A tactile stimulator for studying 248 passive shape perception. J Neurosci Methods. 185:221-229, 2010 249 250 Masson GS, Stone LS. From following edges to pursuing objects. J Neurophysiol 251 88:2869-2873, 2002. 252 253 Movshon JA, Adelson EH, Gizzi G, Newsome WT. The analysis of visual moving 254 patterns. In: Pattern Recognition Mechanisms, edited by. Chagas C, Gattass R, and Gross 255 C. New York: Springer, 1985. 256 257 Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Bensmaia SJ. The tactile integration of local motion cues is analogous 258 to its visual counterpart. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 105:8130-8135, 2008. 259 260 Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Craig JC, Bensmaia SJ. Neural mechanisms of tactile motion 261 integration in somatosensory cortex. Neuron 69:536-547, 2011. 262 263 Phillips JR, Johnson KO. Tactile spatial resolution: III. A continuum mechanics model of 264 skin predicting mechanoreceptive responses to bars, edges, and gratings. J Neurophysiol 265 46:1204-1225, 1981. 266 11 267 Phillips JR, Johnson KO, Hsiao SS. Spatial pattern representation and transformation in 268 monkey somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci. USA 85:1317-1321, 1988. 269 270 Pruszynski JA, and Johansson RS. Edge-orientation processing in first-order tactile 271 neurons. Nat Neurosci 17:1404-1409, 2014. 272 273 Saal HP, Bensmaia SJ. Touch is a team effort: interplay of submodalities in cutaneous 274 sensibility. Trends Neurosci (in press) 2014. 275 276 Soechting JF, Flanders M. Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three- 277 dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 62: 582-594, 1989. 278 279 Soechting JF, Flanders M. Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor 280 transformations. J Neurophysiol 62: 595-608, 1989. 281 282 Soechting JF, Flanders M. Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, 283 vectors, and coordinate systems. Ann Rev Neurosci 15:167-191, 1992. 284 285 Weber AI, Saal HP, Lieber JD, Cheng JW, Manfredi LR, Dammann JF 3rd, Bensmaia SJ. 286 Spatial and temporal codes mediate the tactile perception of natural textures. Proc Natl 287 Acad Sci USA 110:17107-17112, 2013. 288 289 Weiss EJ, Flanders M. Somatosensory comparison in haptic tracing. Cereb Cortex 290 21:425-434, 2011. 291 292 Zipser D, Anderson RA. A back-propagation network that simulates response properties 293 of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331: 679-684, 1988. 294 295 Yau JM, Pasupathy A, Fitzgerald PJ, Hsiao SS, Connor CE. Analogous intermediate 296 shape coding in vision and touch. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 106:16457-16462, 2009 297 12 298 Figure Captions 299 300 FIG. 1. Panel A shows how moving a raised letter (K) toward the distal 301 end of a finger pad, from right to left in this figure, advances the stationary skin receptive 302 field (dashed circle) from left to right across the letter. The receptive field was advanced 303 by 1 mm every 20 ms (50 mm/s). Panel B shows the conventions of a spatial event plot 304 (SEP) for the action potentials recorded at each location. 305 306 FIG. 2. Example spatial event plots (SEPs) gathered while grids of 0.5 mm 307 diameter raised dots were scanned across the distal finger pad of an anesthetized macaque 308 monkey. The top row shows the dot spacing (1.3 – 6.2 mm) of the stimuli. The middle 309 two rows show that SAI and RA peripheral afferents can provide nearly isomorphic 310 neural images of the pattern; the bottom row suggests that PC afferents cannot. 311 Reproduced with permission from Connor et al., J Neurosci 10:3826 (Fig. 4), 1990. 13 A. Schematic of receptive field B. Spatial event plot to mark scanning across a raised letter action potentials at place of occurrence on letter stimulus K 8 mm high receptive field swept across 64 rows raised 500 µm upper corner of embossed letter “K” 2 mm wide 1 mm/tick (20 ms/tick at 50 mm/s) ce. eter and dot spacinghad statistically significant essestimates(ANOVA p < 0.000 1). Perceived asedsomewhat with increasingdot size, espeerdot spacings.Dot spacinghad a much larger t diameter, roughnessincreasedwith increasing o 3.2 mm, then declined. Thus, the roughness ectionsfor typical SA, s.The corresponding stimuluspattern are w (dot diameter,0.5 omprises a setof rasm repetitive parallel ulusacrossthe recepstimulusshiftedverbetweeneachscan. . . a.-. . . ....... .......... ........... .*-, :.- . Figure 3B shows, for example, that dot spacingsof approximately 2 and 5 mm produce the sameroughnessmagnitude even though they evoke very different neural responses,as described in the next section. The remainder of the paper is concerned with discovering the coding mechanismthat extracts a constant factor in different neural responses that evoke the same subjective senseof roughness. . .......... ...... ..... . ...... ...... ‘.-.-.-.-*-, ......... . ......... Z.’ . . l : .’ PC Dot spacing (mm)