1 Chimpanzee origami? Pith-folding at Toro
Transcription
1 Chimpanzee origami? Pith-folding at Toro
1 1 Chimpanzee origami? Pith-folding at Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda 2 3 William C. McGrew1 and Kevin D. Hunt2 4 5 6 7 8 1 9 2 Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Biological Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Fitzwilliam Street, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK e-mail: wcm21@cam.ac.uk telephone: 44 1223 764706 fax: 44 1223 764710 Department of Anthropology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA 10 11 Abstract All well-studied populations of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) orally 12 compress detached vegetation between tongue and palate, then discard it after juices have 13 been extracted (‘wadges’). Wadging is a form of food-processing, typically focussed on 14 fibrous bark, pith, or fruit, that yields a characteristic remnant or artefact. Most wadges 15 are jumbled, amorphous masses, but here we report a newly-discovered technique, from 16 the chimpanzees of Semliki, Uganda. Various lengths of pith stripped from wild date 17 palm (Phoenix reclinata) fronds are repeatedly folded to produce a characteristically- 18 shaped artefact. (Systematic folding of plant matter for any reason is rare in apes, e.g. 19 chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, fold leaves to extract water from tree-holes). Number of 20 folds per wadge was correlated with length of pith, i.e. longer piths had more folds, 21 presumably because it was necessary to fit the material into the confines of the mouth. 22 However, 83% of wadges unexpectedly had an odd number of folds, a highly-skewed 23 departure from an expected 50:50 distribution. There is no obvious reason why a 24 concertina-style, folded object should have an odd or even number of folds, but the 25 explanation was supplied by observations of another Ugandan population of 26 chimpanzees. The Kanyawara community at Kibale sometimes processes the pith of 27 papyrus (Cyperus papyrus) in a particular way that produces similarly-folded remnants. 28 29 30 31 32 Key words Pan troglodytes, diet, food process, material culture, object manipulation 2 33 Introduction 34 35 Chimpanzees make and use tools on a daily basis, in a variety of arenas, but most of their 36 tool manufacturing is transforming vegetation by stripping, peeling, splitting, crushing, 37 clipping, etc. In the well-known examples of termite fish, ant dip, ant fish, leaf sponge, 38 etc., the resultant tool is used in extractive foraging (McGrew 1992). In habituated 39 populations, behavioural data accompany the artefacts, so that observers see precisely 40 how the tools are made and why, e.g. twigs for fishing may be peeled off bark, in order to 41 facilitate their smooth insertion into tiny holes. 42 43 However, chimpanzees also modify vegetation when not making tools, and these 44 activities also may leave behind puzzling artefacts. Such are the ‘wadges’ of the pith of 45 the wild date palm, Phoenix reclinata, which we describe in this report from unhabituated 46 subjects. (‘Wadge’ is a somewhat imprecise English word for such a bolus; ‘quid’ is 47 probably better, as in chewing tobacco.) The artefact is obvious when encountered: A 48 straight stem is bent repeatedly to alternating sides, concertina-style, so that it has a series 49 of folds at acute angles (see Figure 1). These objects are the spat-out products of buccal 50 compression, from which juices have been extracted by squeezing them between tongue 51 and palate. Their ‘function’ is straight-forwardly nutritional, but the puzzles are why they 52 take this distinctive shape and why there are so many wadges with an odd number of 53 folds and so few with an even number of folds. We seek to study these puzzles here. 54 55 The only previous report of similar origami is the leaf-folding done by the 56 chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, who manufacture water-extracting tools (Tonooka et al. 57 1994, Tonooka 2001; Biro et al. 2006; Sousa 2009). To make these tools, the apes fold 58 sets of one to -four leaves (mostly of Hybophrynium braunianum) at about 3-cm 59 intervals, while stuffing them into the mouth. This device is inserted by hand into a tree- 60 hole containing water, then extracted and sucked, as a source of sustenance; the sequence 61 may be repeated many times before the artefact is abandoned. (Such leaf-folding differs 62 from leaf-sponging and leaf-spooning, although all three techniques yield drinking water. 63 Tonooka, 2001). 3 64 65 Wadging, in which a nutritious object is manipulated by mouth but not swallowed, 66 seems to be a universal of chimpanzee food-processing technique. Goodall (1986, p. 238) 67 described wadging at Gombe in detail, noting a variety of food-items, mostly fibrous 68 plant-parts, e.g. figs, bark, etc. In some cases, chimpanzees add leaves to fleshy, ‘rich’- 69 tasting foods, such as meat, eggs, honey, overripe fruit, to form a compound wadge. In 70 this case, the compressed, homogenised bolus may be swallowed, but the usual result is 71 an accumulation of amorphous, jumbled fibre-spheres left on the ground. (Presumably, 72 wadges are ejected rather than ingested, because the fibrous mass of foliage is of low 73 quality and would take up valuable gut space.) Humans similarly wadge the pith of 74 Cyperus papyrus, which is described as “chewy and pleasant tasting” (van der Merwe et 75 al. 2008). 76 77 We have find found no previous record of accordion-folded wadges from previous 78 studies of chimpanzees or any other species of non-human primates, either in nature or 79 captivity. The aim of this report is to document these special wadges and to seek to infer 80 how the artefacts end up with a non-random design, that is, a prevalence of odd- 81 numbered folds. We predicted that the number of folds would be positively correlated 82 with the length of the pithy stem involved; given a standard unit of folding distance, the 83 longer the stem, the more folds required. However, we had no clue about the biased 84 number of folds. 85 86 Methods 87 88 We studied the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Toro-Semliki 89 Wildlife Reserve, Uganda, from May-November, 2008 (Hunt 2000). The apes were only 90 partly habituated, but much progress was made, so that nest-to-nest follows were 91 eventually achieved. If the chimpanzees could not un-nested at the beginning of the day, 92 we searched for them, listening for calls and looking for their signs. Once contacted, we 93 stayed with them for as long as possible. These apes occupy a largely open habitat, which 94 is a mosaic of grassland, scrub, open woodland, galley forest, and swamp (Hunt and 4 95 McGrew, 2002). Their subsistence patterns are mostly the same as those of other 96 populations, but unusually among wild chimpanzees, they dig holes by hand in sandy 97 riverbeds for drinking water (Hunt and McGrew 2002, McGrew et al. 2007). 98 99 In tracking or following chimpanzees, we took note of any feeding traces left by 100 them. For wild date palm wadges, we counted the numbers in an assemblage and noted 101 the sources from whence the piths had been detached. Intact and fresh (less than 24 hr 102 old) wadges were collected in plastic ziplock bags and returned to camp for processing. 103 (In a parallel study, very fresh wadges were sealed in sterile containers for possible DNA 104 extraction, on the assumption that buccal cells from the wadgers would be found. Payne, 105 unpublished data) In camp, WCM measured wadges on a table-top, to the nearest 0.5 cm, 106 from fold to fold, and counted the number of folds. Thus, a folded-pith wadge with three 107 folds yielded four measureable segments. 108 109 Results 110 111 Remnants of the chimpanzees’ wadging of wild date palm were found on a daily basis 112 over the six-month period of the study. A typical assemblage consisted of several mature 113 fronds lying on the ground less than 5m from the presumed source plant. The presumed 114 source plant was identified by what appeared to be freshly damaged ends of the intact 115 fronds. Source plants were readily apparent because white pith revealed by the damage 116 made a stark contrast to the green outer colour of the frond. A typical assemblage 117 consisted of several mature fronds lying on the ground, havinge been torn from the 118 targeted plant, which was often less than 5m away. It was easy to recognise the damaged 119 ends of the intact fronds, as the revealed white pith was obvious in comparison with the 120 green outer colour. Detached fronds were split length-wise and clipped cross-wise, 121 producing segments of 8-50 cm length. All of the outer epithelium of the segments was 122 peeled away, leaving only the grainy, moist, fibrous pith in strips of less than 1 cm 123 diameter. There were few of these unused or incomplete strips of pith, by comparison 124 with more than 30 wadges left strewn on the ground. (See Figure 1.) These artefacts were 125 not scattered randomly, but were often concentrated in areas of less than 50 cm diameter, 5 126 sometimes in a loose pile, as if the wadger had sat still and worked through a set of 127 wadges before moving on. Fresh wadges had a distinctive pale gold colour and were 128 moist; older wadges turned white and shrank in size as they dried. Desiccated wadges 129 also expanded, changing the angles of the folds from acute to obtuse, unless the fibres 130 were tangled up with one another. 131 How did we know that these artefacts were made by chimpanzees, if we had no direct 132 observations of their making? On several occasions, observers were within 5m of 133 chimpanzees eating in thick undergrowth; we heard the distinctive sounds of fronds being 134 detached and then we recovered fresh artefacts only minutes later. We often saw the 135 chimpanzees wadging other vegetation, e.g. at least four species of tree-bark 136 (unpublished data), showing their manifest capacities (but all of these wadges were the 137 more typical jumbled balls described above). Many cases of wild date palm wadge 138 recovery took place when we were tracking chimpanzees on the trail system, only 139 minutes ahead of us. We found imprints of knuckle- and hand-prints in association with 140 the wadges. No other animals at the study-site did such wadging, nor were there resident 141 humans present to do so. 142 143 We measured 110 wadges, which had 1-14 folds; 56 of these had 3 folds, making this 144 the typical number. When fully extended, the length of the wadges ranged from 8-50 cm, 145 with the mean wadge being 15.9 (SEM) cm long. The mean distance between folds (i.e. 146 length of segment) was 3.8 cm. (SEM) (N = 563, range 1.5-6.5, median=3.5, mode=3.0) 147 Figure 2 shows a positive correlation (Spearman’s rho, n=11, rs=0.98, p<.001, two-tailed) 148 between mean length of wadge and the number of its folds, suggesting that segment- 149 length is relatively standardised. 150 151 Figure 3 shows that wadges with an odd number of folds (1,3,5,7, etc.) greatly 152 exceeded in frequency the wadges with an even number of folds (2,4,6,8, etc). For every 153 consecutive pairing, that is, 1 vs. 2 folds, 3 vs. 4 folds, etc., the prevalence of odd-number 154 folds was greater. Overall, 91 (83%) wadges had 1-13 odd-numbered folds, while only 19 155 (17%) had 2-14 even-numbered folds, which is a highly statistically significant difference 156 (Binomial test, n=110, z=6.77, p<0.001, two-tailed) (This could not have been a 6 157 collection bias, as the number of folds was obscured in the field and could not be 158 ascertained until the artefact was extended back in camp.) This unexpected finding 159 requires explanation. 160 161 Discussion 162 163 The implicit interpretation of leaf-folding at Bossou that the dimension of the fold is a 164 function of the space available in the chimpanzee’s mouth makes sense for pith-folding 165 too. The most economical way to pack a linear object into a much smaller space is to 166 compact it to the maximum permissible length allowed, and the most efficient form of 167 compaction is folding. Thus it is notable that the average length of fold of leaves at 168 Bossou was 3 cm versus 3.8 cm for pith-folding at Semliki. But why is there any 169 variation in length of wadge? One hypothesis is the bigger mouths will accommodate 170 bigger (longer) wadges, and that mouth size is a function of maturation. Thus, we predict 171 that physically mature apes make higher-volume wadges, but to test this depends on 172 gaining behavioural data. So, the correlation between number of folds and length of 173 wadge is likely just a matter of mechanical constraints. 174 175 But what about the odd-even difference? It could be that chimpanzees consistently 176 clipped lengths of pith that when folded to the optimal segment-length of about 3-4 cm 177 were somehow biased toward an odd number of folds. This seems nonsensical. If an ape 178 were sufficiently persnickety particular? fastidious? fussy? about producing exactly the 179 right number of folds to fill up the mouth, then on average, all other things being equal, 180 one would expect a 50:50 chance of odd or even, given individual variation in buccal 181 volume. 182 183 A solution to the oddness riddle emerged serendipitously from chance observations of 184 habituated chimpanzees at another Ugandan field site, Kanyawara, in Kibale National 185 Park. There, chimpanzees wadge the pith of papyrus stems; most of the time they produce 186 the standard jumbled-mass wadges, but occasionally they fashion concertina-shaped, 187 folded wadges that are identical in form to the Semliki ones (Bertolani, pers. comm.). 7 188 (See Figure 4.) The technique is as follows: After clipping the stem to the final length, the 189 ape folds it in half, with one end in the lips and the other in one hand. She then ‘feeds’ 190 the doubled-over stem into the mouth by the same hand, initial-fold first. Each new fold 191 of the doubled-stem thus produces a pair of folds, which when added to the single initial 192 fold, gives an odd-numbered total. Occasionally, the wadger does not bother with the 193 initial fold, and just ‘feeds’ the stem into the mouth; presumably these artefacts have a 194 50:50 chance of ending up with an odd or even number of folds. 195 196 To what extent does the pith-folding at Semliki resemble the leaf-folding at Bossou? 197 There are several differences (see Tonooka 2001): The folded leaf is a tool, the pleats of 198 which increase the water-holding ability of the leaves that are folded. The folded pith is 199 not a tool, and the folds seem to have no containing function. So, Semliki wadges are 200 discarded after one use, whereas Bossou’s wadges may be re-used up to 122 times. At 201 Semliki, only one species of plant is used for folding; at Bossou at least seven species are 202 used. In making the wadge, at Semliki the raw material is folded over at its centre-point 203 before being ‘fed’ into the mouth; at Bossou the raw material is stuffed directly into the 204 mouth, starting at one end of the leaf. Most of the resulting differences can be explained 205 by the differing functions of the activities. 206 207 On the other hand, there are some similarities: Both procedures fold plant materials in 208 order to fill up the buccal cavity, using folding as a technique to do so. The resulting 209 pleats are evenly and similarly spaced, yielding a concertina-like artefact with alternating 210 folds. The making of both types of artefact involves hand-mouth coordination. Most of 211 these similarities can be explained by the similar biomechanics of the task. 212 213 Why bother with such esoterica? Who cares if (whether?) the number of folds in a 214 wadge is odd or even? One answer is that this is the stuff of culture, not just of gross 215 differences across populations, but also of nuanced variation, when cultural traits are 216 basically similar but subtly different. Such trivial variants occur all the time in human 217 cultures, and may be the stuff of trivial cross-cultural misunderstandings (e.g. the 218 orientation of the upraised, two-fingered salute or ‘V’-sign); the same may be true of non- 8 219 human artefacts. This report is only a first ethnographic note, but it reminds us that 220 culture is a layered phenomenon, and that if we operate on one level only, we may miss 221 important features. Another answer is that scientists who work with artefacts, e.g. 222 archaeologists, face great challenges in inferring how those artefacts were produced. This 223 example of etho-archaeology reminds us how difficult it sometimes is to imagine the 224 absent processes that result in material culture. Whether or not pith-folding is origami 225 remains to be seen. 226 227 Acknowledgements We thank: Uganda Wildlife Authority and Uganda National 228 Council for Science and Technology for permission to work at Semliki, UWA rangers 229 Alimosi Baluku, Patrick Biryomumsho, Charles Kasaija, Feliilex Pascal, Justus 230 Orobokiritto, and Elly Rutaro for assistance in the field; Moses Comeboy, Eriikc 231 Kasutama, Edson Katswamba, Jeremiah Ndutu for assistance in camp; Linda Marchant, 232 Charlotte Payne, Timothy Webster for research collaboration and comments on the 233 manuscript; National Science Foundation, Revealing Hominid Origins Initiative, XXX 234 for funding; Lucie Salwiczek for graphics aid; Paco Bertolani for taking WCM into the 235 swamp at Kanyawara. 236 237 References 238 239 Biro D, Sousa C, Matsuzawa T (2006) Ontogeny and cultural propagation of tool use by 240 wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: Case studies in nut cracking and leaf 241 folding. In: Matsuzawa T, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M (eds) Cognitive development 242 in chimpanzees. Springer, Tokyo, pp 476-508. 243 244 245 246 247 248 Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Hunt KD (2000) Initiation of a new chimpanzee study site at Semliki-Toro Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. Pan Afr News 7:14-16. Hunt KD, McGrew WC (2002) Chimpanzees in the dry habitats of Assirik, Senegal and Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) 9 249 Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, 250 Cambridge, pp 35-51. 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture. Implications for human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Hunt KD (2007) Etho-archaeology of manual laterality: Well digging by wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol 78:240-244. Sousa C (2009) Use of leaves for drinking water. In: Matsuzawa T, Sugiyama Y (eds.) The chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba. Springer, Tokyo, pp Tonooka R (2001) Leaf-folding behavior for drinking water by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea. Anim Cogn 4:325-334. Tonooka R, Inoue N, Matsuzawa T (1994) [Leaf-folding behavior for drinking water by 260 wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: A field experiment and leaf selectivity.] 261 Primat Res 10:307-313. [Japanese, with English summary] 262 van der Merwe NJ, Masao FT, Bamford MK (2008) Isotopic evidence for contrasting 263 diets of early hominins Homo habilis and Australopithecus boisei of Tanzania. S 264 Afr J Sci 104:153-155. 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 10 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 Figure legends: 289 290 291 Fig. 1 Folded pith wadges of Phoenix reclinata made by chimpanzees at Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve. 292 293 294 Fig. 2 Average (overall) length of folded pith wadges of Phoenix reclinata as a function of number of folds. 295 296 Fig. 3 Frequency of folded pith wadges with odd versus even number of folds. 297 298 299 300 Fig 4 Jumbled (standard) and folded pith wadges of Cyperus papyrus at Kanyawara, Kibale National Park. 11 Figure 12 Figure … 13 Figure: 14 Figure: