Iva Brunec , Jason Ozubko , Morris Moscovitch , Morgan Barense
Transcription
Iva Brunec , Jason Ozubko , Morris Moscovitch , Morgan Barense
Memory for temporal information during spatial navigation Iva 1,2 Brunec , Jason 1,2 Ozubko , 1,2 Moscovitch , Morris Morgan 1,2 Barense (1) Department of Psychology, University of Toronto; (2) Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre Introduction • Explore participants’ memory for time in a spatial navigation task with closely controlled spatial and temporal parameters • How good is our capacity for temporal pattern separation? • What is the relationship between memory for temporal duration and spatial location? • Is information about time encoded automatically or does it require a rich sense of re-experiencing an event? Re-experience/Know/New All 12 intersections + 12 new previously unseen intersections from the same area of the city. Participants respond R/K/N and provide a confidence judgment. Temporal duration discrimination All 12 intersections shown pairwise. Participants respond Left/Right and provide a confidence judgment. Distance discrimination All 12 intersections shown pairwise. Participants respond Left/Right and provide a confidence judgment. Temporal Duration Discrimination • Overall proportion correct: 0.6 (SD = 0.48) Proportion Correct per Temporal Interval 1 * 0.8 0.7 * www.PosterPresentations.co m * 0.8 * 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.3 0.2 0.2 0.1 0.1 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Difference in seconds between compared pair Re-experience vs. Know 1 0.9 * 0.8 0.6 0.5 RESEARCH POSTER PRESENTATION DESIGN © 2012 1 0.9 0.7 • 7 repetitions of time-constrained passively guided navigation in a virtual rendering of the city of Chicago, USA • Stops in 12 intersections, each associated with a different temporal duration (range 112 seconds). Proportion Correct per Degree of Spatial Separation * * sign. above chance at p < 0.05 Design and Stimuli • Overall proportion correct: 0.84 (SD = 0.34) 0.9 Participants • 14 participants (6 female) • Average age 19.2 (SD = 2.5) years • Average Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale score 4.2 (SD = 2.0) Distance Discrimination * 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Re-experience Know 11 • Spatial information is encoded much more readily than temporal duration information. Memory for time appears to be more sensitive to interference and requires a greater degree of separation. This suggests that temporal deficits might precede spatial deficits in disorders such as dementia • Reliable temporal duration discrimination requires a rich sense of re-experiencing and is significantly lower when participants report ‘knowing’ an intersection with little associated perceptual and spatial detail • While hippocampal time cells may track time continuously, this information may only be available to conscious evaluation when the memory trace for the spatiotemporal context is strong and the subjective experience is vivid. Future Directions Results Proportion Aims & Questions Summary and Conclusions • Participants complete 7 repetitions of the same route where each of the 12 intersections is associated with a unique temporal duration. The temporal durations of different intersections are randomized across participants. Proportion • Damage to the hippocampus results in profound episodic memory impairments and disorientation in space and time1,2 • The hippocampus is crucial for creating unique, non-overlapping memory representations by the mechanism of pattern separation3. We appear to use spatiotemporal contextual information to construct unique memory representations for individual events • Traditionally, hippocampal function has been studied in the spatial domain. However, recent research points to the existence of ‘time cells’ which track successively elapsed moments during an empty temporal gap4 • Space and time are inextricably linked and understanding the complex interplay of memory for space and time may offer insight into the principles of hippocampal function Task and Procedure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Number of intersections between compared pair • Significantly greater accuracy overall on distance discrimination relative to temporal discrimination judgments • No significant correlation between distance and temporal judgments • Sign. greater proportion of accurate temporal judgments when both intersections were “re-experienced” rather than “known” 11 All sign. above chance at p < 0.05 Re-experience vs. Know 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Re-experience Know • Passive experience of navigation could have contributed to low performance – active navigation with slightly less constrained timing might yield different results • A different pattern of results might be observed in a familiar environment where participants would not need to encode the spatial features of the route • Comparison of each individual duration with a pre-learned duration • Elapsed time during motion vs. pause References 1. Moscovitch, M., Nadel, L., Winocur, G., Gilboa, A., & Rosenbaum, R. (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 179-190. 2. Bellassen, V., Iglói, K., de Souza, L. C., Dubois, B., & Rondi-Reig, L. (2012). Temporal order memory assessed during spatiotemporal navigation as a behavioral cognitive marker for differential Alzheimer's disease diagnosis. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(6), 1942-1952. 3. Bakker, A., Kirwan, C., Miller, M., & Stark, C. (2008). Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science, 319(5870), 1640-1642. 4. MacDonald, C., Lepage, K., Eden, U., & Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron, 71(4), 737-749.