Cognitive Neuroscience
Transcription
Cognitive Neuroscience
Gazzaniga • Ivry • Mangun Cognitive Neuroscience THIRD EDITION Chapter 1 A Brief History of Cognitive Neuroscience ©2009 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. A historical perspective of cognitive neuroscience To study how the brain enables the mind The term coined by Michael Gazzaniga and George Miller in 1970s Cognition: the process of knowing (i.e., what arises from awareness, perception, and reasoning) Neuroscience: the study of the nervous system Thomas Willis was the first to relate human behavior to brain structure Drs. Thomas Willis & William Petty, physicians in Oxford, examined the body of Anne Green, and found her miraculous resurrection after hanging for death on Dec 14, 1650 Thomas Willis (1621–1675), a founder of clinical neuroscience, coined the term neurology, also the first anatomist to link abnormal human behaviors to changes in brain structure The human brain (ventral view) drawn by Christopher Wren for Thomas Willis, published in Willis’s The Anatomy of the Brain and Nerves. The central issue in cognitive neuroscience Whether the mind is enabled by the whole brain working in concert, or by specialized parts of the brain working at least partly independently Thomas Willis foreshadowed cognitive neuroscience with the notion that isolated brain damage could affect behavior In the 19th century, people who studied phrenology, called phrenologists, declared that the brain was organized around some 35 or more specific functions Franz Joseph Gall (1758–1828), one of the founders of phrenology The right hemisphere of the brain, according to Gall and Spurzheim in 1810 Anatomical personology * Alimentiveness, 1. Destructiveness, 2. Amativeness, 3. Philoprogenitiveness, 4. Adhesiveness, 5. Inhabitiveness, 6. Combativeness, 7. Secretiveness, 8. Acquisitiveness, 9. Constructiveness, 10.Cautiousness, 11. Approbativeness, 12. Self-esteem, 13. Benevolence, 14. Reverence, 15. Firmness, 16. Conscientiousness, 17. Hope, 18. Marvelousness, 19. Ideality, 20. Mirthfulness, 21. Imitation, 22. Individuality, 23. Configuration, 24. Size, 25. Weight and resistance, 26. Coloring, 27. Locality, 28. Order, 29. Calculation, 30. Eventuality, 31. Time, 32. Tune, 33. Language, 34. Comparison, 35. Causality. The phrenological map of personal characteristics on the skull, from the American Phrenological Journal, March 1848 An analysis of Presidents Washington, Jackson, Taylor, and McKinley by Jessie A. Fowler, from the Phrenological Journal, June 1898 Fowler & Wells Co. publication on marriage compatibility in connection with phrenology, 1888 The central issue in cognitive neuroscience Whether the mind is enabled by the whole brain working in concert, or by specialized parts of the brain working at least partly independently Thomas Willis foreshadowed cognitive neuroscience with the notion that isolated brain damage could affect behavior In the 19th century, people who studied phrenology, called phrenologists, declared that the brain was organized around some 35 or more specific functions However, Marie-Jean-Pierre Flourens (1794-1867) suggested the aggregate field theory, and wrote “All sensations, all perceptions, and all volitions occupy the same seat in these (cerebral) organs. The faculty of sensation, perception, and volition is then essentially one faculty.” The posture of a pigeon deprived of its cerebral hemispheres, as described by Flourens The localizationist view of the brain function Jackson proposed a topographic organization of the cerebral cortex, in which a map of the body is represented in a particular cortical area However, he did not maintain the strict view of localizationist, rather he concluded that many regions of the brain contribute to a given behavior John Hughlings Jackson (1835–1911), an English neurologist who was one of the first to recognize the localizationist view Focal brain damage causes specific behavioral deficits Paul Broca (1824–1880) The connections between the speech centers, from Wernicke’s 1876 article on aphasia A = Wernicke’s sensory speech center B = Broca’s area for speech Pc = Wernicke’s area concerned with language comprehension and meaning The preserved brain of Leborgne (Broca’s patient “Tan”) Speech problems in Broca’s aphasia Speech problems in Wernicke’s aphasia Fritsch and Hitzig electrically stimulates discrete parts of a dog brain and observed that this stimulation produced characteristic movements in the dog Gustav Theodor Fritsch (1838–1927), physiologist and anatomist Eduard Hitzig (1838–1907), professor of psychiatry The original illustration of the dog’s cortex by Fritsch and Hitzig Sampling of the 52 distinct areas described by Brodmann on the basis of cell structure and arrangement Early scientists contribute in our understanding of the nervous system Antoni van Leeuwenhoek (1632–1723), and one of the original microscopes used by Leeuwenhoek, composed of two brass plates holding the lens René Descartes (1596–1650) cogito ergo sum (English: "I think, therefore I am") Nerves contained fluid or “spirits”, and these spirits were responsible for the flow of sensory and motor information in the body Father of modern neuroscience Camillo Golgi (1843–1926), cowinner of the Nobel Prize in 1906 Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1852–1934), cowinner of the Nobel Prize in 1906 Golgi’s drawings of different types of ganglion cells in dog and cat Ramón y Cajal’s drawing of the afferent inflow to the A bipolar retinal cell, illustrating the mammalian cortex dendrites and axon of the neuron The neuron doctrine Jan Evangelista Purkinje (1787–1869), who described the first nerve cell in the nervous system, the Purkinje cell in the cerebellum Even Freud had come up with the idea of the neuron as a separate and distinct physiological unit Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) From his work with crayfishes, Freud published this illustration as an example of how two nerve fibers connect—a concept that Ramón y Cajal disproved Helmholtz was the first to suggest that invertebrates would be good models for studying vertebrate brains Hermann Ludwig von Helmholtz (1821–1894) Helmholtz’s apparatus for measuring the velocity of nerve conduction Sir Charles Sherrington coined the term synapse to describe the junction between two neurons Sir Charles Sherrington (1857–1952) cowinner of the Nobel Prize in 1932 with Edgar Adrian Sherrington received the prize for showing that reflexes require integrated activation and demonstrated reciprocal innervation of muscles Female pioneers in neuroscience Ida Hyde (1857–1945), the first woman elected to the American Physiological Society, 1902 Ida Hyde’s microelectrode Arvanitaki (1939) was the first to demonstrate that spontaneous, rhythmically recurring activity could be an inherent property of a single nerve without the requirement of an entire neuronal circuit to generate it 1986 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine Stanley Cohen Rita Levi-Montalcini "for their discoveries of growth factors" 2004 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine Richard Axel Linda B. Buck "for their discoveries of odorant receptors and the organization of the olfactory system" The Italian Nobel prize-winning neurologist Rita Levi-Montalcini has died at the age of 103 (30 Dec, 2012) French biologist Claude Bernard (1865) wrote, If it is possible to dissect all the parts of the body, to isolate them in order to study them in their structure, form and connections, it is mot the same in life, where all parts cooperate at the same time in a common aim. An organ does not live on its own, one could often say it did not exist anatomically, as the boundary established is sometimes purely arbitrary. What lives, what exists, is the whole, and if one studies all the parts of any mechanisms separately, one does not know the way they work. The localizationist view vs. the connectionist view Connectomes The associationism Edward L. Thorndike (1874–1949) Animal Intelligence: An Experimental Study of the Associative Processes in Animals A response that was followed by a reward would be stamped into the organism as a habitual response. If no reward followed a response, the response would disappear. Thus, rewards provided a mechanism for establishing a more adaptive response. Learning is the key JB Watson could turn any baby into anything John B. Watson (1878–1958) Watson and “Little Albert” during one of Watson’s fear-conditioning experiments Although behaviorism had important theories to offer, it could not explain all learning George A. Miller The Magic Number Seven, Plus-or-Minus Two Noam Chomsky The language is universal 1956, a big year in cognitive neuroscience