Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance
Transcription
Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance
Southern Cross University ePublications@SCU Theses 2006 Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons Annette Kay Morgan Southern Cross University Publication details Morgan, AK 2006, 'Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons', Masters thesis, Southern Cross University, Lismore, NSW. Copyright AK Morgan 2006 ePublications@SCU is an electronic repository administered by Southern Cross University Library. Its goal is to capture and preserve the intellectual output of Southern Cross University authors and researchers, and to increase visibility and impact through open access to researchers around the world. For further information please contact epubs@scu.edu.au. Grey Matters: Does Bacopa monnieri Improve Memory Performance in Older Persons? Annette Morgan ND DBM BNurs A research thesis submitted in fulfilment of requirement of the degree of Master of Science Department of Nursing and Health Care Practices Southern Cross University July, 2006 Statement of Originality and Authenticity I certify that the work presented in this thesis is, to the best of my knowledge and belief, original, except as acknowledged in the text, and that the material has not been submitted, either in whole or in part, for a degree at this or any other university. I acknowledge that I have read and understood the University's rules, requirements, procedures and policy relating to my higher degree research award and to my thesis. I certify that I have complied with the rules, requirements, procedures and policy of the University (as they may be from time to time). Signed: ..................................................................... Date: ......................................................................... ii Acknowledgements Here, at the end of this thesis, the whole thing seems as though it was a straight forward process. How quickly I have forgotten those long hours, weeks and months when the end point seemed only a dim and distant point in time. I have many to thank for helping me make it here- to the final stage of an arduous, yet also inspirational, journey. Firstly, thanks to Val, Josh and Jesse for their ongoing love and support. Thanks to my supervisor Dr John Stevens for guidance, encouragement, moral support and always being available despite an excessive workload. Thanks to Ms Keelin Turner for expert guidance with neuropsychological testing- and for sharing some of her inspirational knowledge of the workings of the brain. Thanks to Dr Don McMurray for the many hours of memory testing and the professionalism, warmth and humour he brought along to the testing sessions. Thanks to Dr Joan O’Connor for always being willing to help and advise. Many thanks to Natural Remedies Pvt. Ltd. for supplying raw materials and funding. Thanks to Herbs of Gold for supply of tablets and to Tabco Pty. Ltd. for the manufacture and supply of placebo tablets. Thanks also to Michael Gepp and Miles Wayne for facilitation of these negotiations. The generosity and support of these companies made the research a feasible venture. Lastly, I’d like to thank all of the people who responded to the call for research participants- without them, especially, this research would not have been possible. iii Abstract Background This thesis investigated the efficacy and safety of Bacopa monnieri in improving memory in healthy Australians over the age of 55-years. A review of the literature showed that memory impairment and dementia are increasingly prevalent in the current demographic climate of an ageing population. As well as the pathological cognitive loss of neurodegenerative disease, many older persons are experiencing memory loss as part of the physiological process of ageing. Bacopa monnieri is a herbal medicine used since antiquity in the traditional Ayurvedic medical system of India for its cognitive enhancing effects. A number of pre-clinical and clinical studies support this traditional usage. Laboratory studies have demonstrated antioxidant and cholinergic actions in the brain as well as improved memory and cognitive performance in animal models. Human trials of Bacopa have also demonstrated improved memory performance. Some of these trials are limited by methodological flaws such as lack of blinding, small sample sizes, or use of outcome measurements which are not well validated. However, a small number of well designed human trials provide evidence for efficacy in cognitive and memory performance improvement. The current study was employed to extend on previous findings by assessing the efficacy and safety of Bacopa in the aged population specifically, as it is in this population that memory impairment becomes apparent. Aims 1. To assess the efficacy of Bacopa monnieri in improving memory in healthy Australians over the age of 55-years. 2. To assess whether the use of Bacopa is associated with side-effects Design A 12-week, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group clinical trial. iv Participants Participants were self selected from the general population. They were aged 55-years or over at the commencement of the trial. Participants were without dementia, depression or other serious health conditions and did not use psychotropic medications. Intervention Participants were randomised to one of two treatment conditions, either a tableted extract of Bacopa monnieri called Bacomind™ (300mg/day, standardised to contain at least 40% bacosides), or an identical placebo. Participants attended three clinical evaluations: the first an initial screening session, the second a baseline evaluation of neuropsychological function and subjective memory performance at the commencement of the trial and the third, an end-of-trial outcome evaluation at 12-weeks, during which neuropsychological function and subjective memory performance were again assessed along with side-effects and study compliance. Primary Outcome Measures Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT), Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q), and Trail Making Test (TMT) Results From 136 people who elected to participate, 103 people met study entry criteria and 98 of these commenced the trial. Of these, 81 participants completed the trial and provided evaluable data for the end point analysis. Bacopa monnieri versus placebo significantly improved verbal learning as well as delayed recall as measured by the AVLT (p<.05). Though improvements were noted in the CFT, MAC-Q and TMT, there were no significant differences between placebo and active groups found for these tests. The Bacopa group reported a higher incidence of gastro-intestinal (GIT) side-effects than the v placebo group, these predominantly being increased stool frequency, abdominal cramps and nausea. No other significant adverse effects were found. Conclusions A clinical trial was carried out to assess the effects of 12-weeks administration of Bacopa monnieri (300mg/day) on memory performance in people over the age of 55years. Primary outcome measures were well validated neuropsychological tests that objectively measured verbal and visual memory and a memory complaint questionnaire that measured subjective memory complaints. The results demonstrated that Bacopa significantly improved memory acquisition and retention in older Australians. This concurs with findings from previous human and animal studies, as well as supports traditional Ayurvedic claims and uses. The beneficial effects on memory observed may be due to previously demonstrated antioxidant and cholinergic effects of the herb on the central nervous system. The use of Bacopa was associated with GIT side-effects, particularly increased bowel movements, nausea and abdominal cramping, findings infrequently reported previously. Possible explanations for these side-effects include GIT irritation by the saponin constituents of the herb, or cholinergic stimulation of autonomic and motor responses in the GIT, or a combination of both of these factors. The side-effects observed in the current study provide supportive evidence that Bacopa may increase cholinergic activity in humans. A worthwhile future extension of the current study would be to assess whether the finding of Bacopa’s efficacy for improving memory performance is replicable in populations with either mild cognitive impairment or early dementia. vi List of Abbreviations AACD age-associated cognitive decline AAMI age-associated memory impairment ACh acetylcholine AChE acetylcholinesterase AD Alzheimer’s disease ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorder AIHW Australian Institute of Health & Welfare a-MCI amnestic type of mild cognitive impairment ANOVA analysis of variance ANS autonomic nervous system AVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test BM Bacopa monnieri CAM complementary and alternative medicine CFT Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test ChAT choline-acetyl transferase CIND cognitive impairment no dementia CSIRO Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation CVD cardiovascular disease DB double-blind GIT gastrointestinal tract GLM general linear model HAM-D Hamilton Depression Rating Scale HIS Hachinski Ischaemic Score HREC Human Research Ethics Committee LTP long term potentiation MAC-Q Memory Complaint Questionnaire MCI mild cognitive impairment md-MCI multiple domain mild cognitive impairment MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination MRI magnetic resonance imaging NFT neurofibrillary tangles vii O open PC placebo-controlled PET positron emission tomography R randomised RDBPC randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled SD standard deviation SPECT single photon emission computed tomography SPSS statistical package for social sciences TMT Trail Making Test TMT-A Trail Making Test part A TMT-B Trail Making Test part B WHO World Health Organisation WMH white matter hyperintensities viii Table of Contents Table of Contents ............................................................................................................ ix List of Figures................................................................................................................. xii List of Tables .................................................................................................................. xii 1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 1 2. MEMORY AND AGEING .......................................................................................... 4 2.1 Introduction ............................................................................................................ 4 2.2 Cognition and memory - an overview .................................................................... 4 2.3 Memory and cognitive changes of normal ageing ................................................. 6 2.3.1 Speed of mental processing declines with ageing ........................................... 8 2.3.2 Fluid and crystallised cognitive abilities ......................................................... 9 2.3.3 Biological changes of brain ageing ................................................................. 9 2.4 Healthy brain ageing............................................................................................. 11 2.5 Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 11 3. THE PROBLEM OF DEMENTIA ............................................................................ 13 3.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 13 3.2 Demographic background: an ageing populace.................................................... 13 3.3 Dementia prevalence and cost .............................................................................. 14 3.4 Dementia pathologies ........................................................................................... 15 3.5 Therapeutic strategies for dementia...................................................................... 16 3.6 Pre-clinical dementia states .................................................................................. 18 3.7 Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 20 4: BACOPA MONNIERI............................................................................................... 22 4.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 22 4.2 History .................................................................................................................. 22 4.3 Constituents .......................................................................................................... 23 4.4 Pre-clinical and clinical studies of the cognitive effects of Bacopa ..................... 24 4.4.1 In vitro and animal studies ............................................................................ 24 4.4.2 Human trials .................................................................................................. 27 4.5 Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 32 5. RESEARCH DESIGN................................................................................................ 33 5.1 Introduction and overview.................................................................................... 33 ix 5.2 Location and timing.............................................................................................. 33 5.3 Ethical considerations........................................................................................... 33 5.4 Recruitment of participants .................................................................................. 34 5.4.1 Sample size.................................................................................................... 34 5.5 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 34 5.6 Selection criteria ................................................................................................... 35 5.6.1 Inclusion criteria ............................................................................................ 35 5.6.2 Exclusion criteria........................................................................................... 35 5.7 Study design ......................................................................................................... 36 5.7.1 Randomisation ............................................................................................... 36 5.8 Compliance........................................................................................................... 37 5.9 Materials ............................................................................................................... 37 5.9.1 Study drugs .................................................................................................... 37 5.10 Instruments ......................................................................................................... 38 5.10.1 Screening instruments.................................................................................. 38 5.10.2 Primary outcome measures.......................................................................... 39 5.11 Statistical analysis .............................................................................................. 41 6. RESULTS................................................................................................................... 43 6.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 43 6.2 Participants ........................................................................................................... 43 6.2.1 Exclusions...................................................................................................... 43 6.2.2 Randomisation ............................................................................................... 44 6.2.3 Effects of gender, marital status, age and education on baseline measures .. 46 6.2.4 Group distribution by age and gender categories. ......................................... 48 6.2.5 Compliance.................................................................................................... 50 6.2.6 Study withdrawals ......................................................................................... 50 6.2.7 Side-effects .................................................................................................... 52 6.3 Primary outcome measurements........................................................................... 54 7. DISCUSSION............................................................................................................. 60 7.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 60 7.2 Effects of age, gender and education on baseline measurements......................... 60 7.3 Primary outcome measures................................................................................... 61 7.3.1 The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) ........................................ 61 7.3.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) and Trail Making Test (TMT) 64 x 7.3.3 The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)........................................ 65 7.4 Possible mechanisms of action for observed effect of Bacopa ............................ 65 7.5 Side-effects ........................................................................................................... 67 7.6 Limitations of the current study............................................................................ 70 7.7 Recommendations arising from the study ............................................................ 72 8. CONCLUSION .......................................................................................................... 75 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................... 78 Appendix I: Participant instruction sheet and record booklet ........................................ 92 Appendix II: Human research ethics committee approval ............................................. 96 Appendix III: Participant consent form and information sheet ...................................... 99 Appendix IV: Clinical report form ............................................................................... 103 Appendix V: Neuropsychological test administration protocol ................................... 111 Appendix VI: Specification sheets for Bacopa and placebo tablets ............................. 112 Appendix VII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) ...................................... 115 Appendix VIII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) alternate form for endpoint assessment .................................................................................................................... 117 Appendix IX: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) ......................................... 119 Appendix X: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) marking Sheet .................. 120 Appendix XI: Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B............................................... 122 Appendix XII: Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)...................................... 126 Appendix XIII: Results of repeated measures analysis for all variables...................... 127 xi List of Figures Figure 2.1: The major aspects of memory.......................................................................6 Figure 6.1: Distribution of age groups at point of randomisation (n=98)......................49 Figure 6.2: Distribution of educational levels at point of randomisation (n=98)...........49 Figure 6.3: Flow-chart depicting participant progression through phases of the clinical trial.....................................................................................................................51 Figure 6.4: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a4.............................................................................................................................56 Figure 6.5: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a5.............................................................................................................................56 Figure 6.6: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a6.............................................................................................................................57 Figure 6.7: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a7.............................................................................................................................57 Figure 6.8: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT total learning index ........................................................................................................58 Figure 6.9: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT retroactive interference index ........................................................................................58 List of Tables Table 4.1: Summary of the clinical trials of neuropsychological effects of Bacopa.... 31 Table 6.1: Dependent variables at baseline: clinical characteristics and test scores at point of randomisation (n=98), with analysis of group differences........ ..................... 45 Table 6.2: Significant effects of gender on task performance at baseline (n=98)..........46 Table 6.3: Significant effects of age on task performance at baseline (n=98) ..............47 Table 6.4: Correlation of length of education and task performance at baseline...........48 Table 6.5: Reasons for withdrawal from trial ............................................................... 50 Table 6.6: Total side-effects reported during study ......................................................52 Table 6.7: Mean (and SD) for all tasks by group and testing session........................... 54 Table 6.8: Significant group differences on primary outcome measures: results of repeated measures analysis of variance......................................................................... 55 xii 1. INTRODUCTION Some aspects of the function of memory decline throughout adulthood and into old age as part of the normal ageing process. Many older people complain of memory difficulties. As well as the memory loss of normal ageing, the pathological memory and cognitive loss of dementia occurs in many older people. Dementia has a very high, and increasing, incidence and prevalence. The escalation in dementia rates in an ageing population poses great social, personal and economic problems for society. This trend is observed in Australia and worldwide. The exploration of potential therapies for improving cognitive function is thus an important quest which could have enormous benefits for society. The greatest likelihood of successful intervention lies with treatment at the earliest possible stages of dementia. Both delayment and prevention of dementia onset would have a large impact in terms of reducing both suffering and costs. There exists an unclear transitional phase between normal age-related memory loss and the first manifestations of dementia. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is one of the conditions recognised in this grey zone, and it is an area of much research attention as it may represent a potential point of earliest intervention. Although many older people experience, and are troubled by, the memory difficulties of normal ageing, no pharmacological therapies exist to address this problem. Furthermore, the pharmacological therapies so far developed for the pathological cognitive losses of dementia are highly inadequate in terms of both efficacy and side effects. Thus there remains a serious gap in effective strategies for cognitive improvement in both age related memory loss and dementia. Bacopa monnieri is an ancient herbal remedy from the traditional Ayurvedic medical system of India where it has been used and documented for many centuries to improve mental health, intellect and memory. These traditional claims and usage have been supported by clinical and laboratory studies in recent times, however there have been only a small number of high quality clinical trials published to date, none of which have 1 targeted older age groups. Thus the aim of this study is to see whether Bacopa monnieri improves memory in healthy people over the age of fifty five years. The hypothesis to be tested is that Bacopa will improve memory performance in older persons. This study will thus help to determine whether Bacopa can offer benefit to older people without dementia whose memory difficulties currently remain unaddressed, and thereby provide an efficacious intervention where none currently exists. Furthermore, while this study examines the effects of Bacopa on the memory performance of non- demented older people, the findings may have implications for the dementia population, and confirmation of the hypothesis would suggest that a clinical trial in subjects with dementia or MCI would be warranted. Many Australians look to complementary and alternative medicines (CAM) for improvement in general health and cognitive function (Jorm et al., 2004; MacLennan et al., 1996; Eisenberg et al., 1993). This study contributes to the evidence basis for CAM usage and thereby helps to enable a more cost effective, efficacious and targeted usage of these therapies, as well as potentially widening the pool of evidence based therapeutic strategies for improvement of cognitive health. To achieve the stated aim of this study, the early chapters review the literature in relation to firstly, ageing-related memory changes, secondly, dementia and MCI, and thirdly, Bacopa monnieri. Following on from the first introductory chapter, Chapter 2 gives an overview of the major aspects of memory and the effects that age related change brings. This is discussed from the point of view of both neuropsychological effects and also biological effects, that is, the effects of age on memory related aspects of brain function and structure. Chapter 3 describes the problem and prevalence of dementia in an ageing population and introduces the concept of MCI. This chapter illustrates the importance of finding efficacious therapeutic interventions for these conditions. Chapter 4 examines the literature on Bacopa monnieri, and provides the rationale for exploring its potential as such an intervention. Following on from the background chapters, Chapter 5 explains the methodology and study design used and describes how a randomised, double-bind, placebo-controlled, design is applied to answer the research question. Chapter 6 presents the results 2 obtained from the trial, whilst Chapter 7 provides an in depth discussion and analysis of these results, particularly the effects that Bacopa had on memory performance, the side effects observed and how the findings relate to the previous literature. Arising from this discussion chapter, the conclusion is presented in Chapter 8, which demonstrates how the aim of the current study was achieved. 3 2. MEMORY AND AGEING 2.1 Introduction This chapter presents an overview and brief description of some of the basic concepts of memory and cognition. Within this framework, the cognitive and memory changes that frequently occur with normal ageing will be reviewed. Memory and the types of loss that occurs in normal ageing are important concepts to elucidate because this provides a context within which to view the current study’s exploration of Bacopa as a potentially therapeutic agent for memory and cognitive improvement. 2.2 Cognition and memory - an overview Human cognition can be divided into four main categories of functions: reception/acquisition, memory/learning, thinking/reasoning, and expression/action (Lezak et al., 2004:20). Memory is a major aspect of, and indeed is central to, all of these cognitive functions. It is important to remember that in reality these functions are not separate but are ‘inextricably bound’ and simply describe different aspects of the same activity (ibid:20), and, also, that each of the functions may share many of the same processing mechanisms (Craik & Jennings, 1992:53). Memory may be defined as the learning, storage and retrieval of information (Kimmel, 1990:173). It has variously been explained and understood in terms of storage, processes and systems depending on the perspective and orientation of the theoretician (Craik & Jennings, 1992:52). These perspectives all divide memory into different components- primary or short term and secondary or long term being two critical distinctions. Primary memory is the conscious awareness for an event or information that has only just occurred and it lasts only a short while, (Carlson, 2002:370). A key feature of primary memory is working memory (Baddeley, 1992) which involves temporary storage, manipulation and transformation of information, and is required in every-day cognitive tasks such as adding sums and making conversation. Indeed working memory is involved in nearly all cognitive tasks (Reisberg, 1997:126). Three systems comprise 4 working memory- two are ‘slave’ systems which store verbal and visual information, and these are overseen by a ‘central executive’ system which controls attention, and regulates, analyses and processes the incoming information. The central executive is a function of the frontal lobe, and it is this aspect of working memory that is affected in Alzheimer’s disease (Baddeley, 1991), and also by ageing processes (Van der Linden, et al., 1994). Overall, however, age related deterioration in most primary memory tasks is minimal (Balota, Dolan & Duchek, 2000:396). Secondary memory, the system predominantly affected by ageing, refers to the longer term storage of knowledge, which is no longer in conscious awareness but is available for retrieval. It represents a huge repository of information which remains fairly stable across time (Reisberg 1997:127). Secondary memory is further divided into declarative (explicit) and non-declarative (implicit or procedural) aspects. These dual aspects of long term memory were confirmed by studies of amnesic patients who maintained some aspects of memory whilst losing others (Lezak et al., 2004:25). Declarative memory is what is classically thought of when we speak of memory- it is the long term memory of facts, figures, experiences and events available for conscious retrieval- it refers to the ‘knowing that’ type of knowledge. Two sub-types of declarative memory have been elucidated (Tulving, 1983): firstly, episodic- the memory for specific events (and the source or context in which they occurred) and secondly, semantic- the memory for general knowledge and facts. Episodic memory demonstrates clear deficits in ageing (Craik & Jennings, 1992:96), which includes encoding, storage and retrieval stages; and furthermore the source (or context) aspect of episodic memory shows definite age-related impairment (Balota, Dolan & Duchek, 2000:395). Semantic memory, by contrast, remains largely unaffected by age (ibid: 397). Non-declarative (also called procedural or implicit) memory is not held in conscious awareness but is automatic and includes skills and behaviours learned via repetition (Craik & Jennings, 1992:54) - it is best understood as the ‘knowing how’ type of knowledge. Examples of activities requiring the use of implicit memory are riding a bicycle or playing an instrument. This aspect of long term memory is very durable and weighty evidence exists to show that it is minimally affected by ageing (ibid: 94). 5 Figure 2-1 gives a framework of the major aspects of memory as discussed above. To reiterate, it is evident that age related impairments are most pronounced in that aspect of long term, declarative memory called episodic memory; whereas semantic and nondeclarative aspects of memory show little change with age. Primary (short term) memory shows little deterioration with age, though aspects of working memory are affected. Ageing related changes will be discussed in more depth in the following section. MEMORY PRIMARY (SHORT TERM) WORKING MEMORY SECONDARY (LONG TERM) DECLARATIVE (EXPLICIT) ‘Know that’ NON-DECLARATIVE (IMPLICIT) ‘Know how’ EPISODIC SEMANTIC Figure 2.1 The major aspects of memory 2.3 Memory and cognitive changes of normal ageing Kral first classified the memory loss of normal ageing in 1962 by coining the term ‘benign senescent forgetfulness’ to differentiate it from pathological forms of memory loss. Responding to the lack of specific diagnostic criteria in Kral’s definition, Crook et al. (1986) presented classification and diagnostic criteria for ‘Age Associated Memory Impairment’ (AAMI). This term was applied to the type of memory loss that occurs with normal aging- specifically the memory loss of older people relative to their younger years, but normal relative to their age group. It is defined by subjective memory difficulties, performance on any well standardised memory test that is at least one standard deviation below that of younger people, and occurring in people over 50 years of age, without any disease affecting memory or any intellectual deficit (ibid). 6 Hanninen (1996), in an extensive population study of AAMI, found that subjects were impaired in both memory test performance as well as tests of executive functions associated with frontal lobe function. This study also found AAMI to be unlikely to progress to dementia, with only a slightly elevated incidence of dementia found in those classified as having AAMI compared to other normal elderly. It is estimated that only approximately one percent of those who experience AAMI will go on to develop dementia, a figure similar to that found in the normal population (Small, 2002). AAMI is an ambiguous entity, and its heterogeneous nature has led many researchers to question its clinical usefulness. Memory difficulties in the aged has a very high prevalence- reported as occurring in almost half of the population over sixty-five years of age (Small, 2002). Subjective memory complaints are thus a frequent phenomenon in older people, with community studies demonstrating an incidence of such complaints in 25-50% of people in the over 65 year old age group (Kawas, 2003). The mechanisms for normal age related memory loss are controversial, and not yet fully elucidated (Christensen, 2001). Individuals vary greatly in memory performance and this variability increases with age, pointing to the notion that there are a variety of factors at play to create these cognitive changes (Kawas, 2003). Even though there are many people whose cognitive performance remains intact with aging, it is most common for aspects of cognition and memory performance to decline (American Psychological Association, 1998) and Christensen concludes that cognitive changes are inevitable- only the age of onset varies (2001). Longitudinal studies demonstrate this gradual decline in memory and cognitive performance with ageing (De Carli, 2003; Christensen, 2001), and it is clearly apparent in standardised memory tests where poorer performance of older age groups compared to younger age groups is the norm (Anstey & Low, 2004). Reflecting this, it is increasingly common for people to present to their doctors with concerns about their memory and cognitive health (Small, 2002). As well as the above-mentioned variance between individuals, variance within individuals is marked in age related cognitive decline (Albert, 2002). Individuals vary within themselves in that some memory and cognitive abilities decline faster than 7 others, i.e. the loss is not consistent across abilities (Christensen, 2001). The particular aspects of memory that decline with age include aspects of long term, declarative memory, aspects of working memory (both discussed above) and particularly, the speed of cognitive processing, which shall be discussed below. When researchers at the Mayo clinic investigated acquisition (learning) and delayed recall (forgetting) in 161 normal elderly people, they concluded that a consistent and gradual decline in acquisition occurs with age (Petersen et al., 1992). The decline in acquisition is thought to be due to the slowing of processing speed rather than capability- older people take longer to learn new information (Albert, 2002). Acquisitional deficits may also reflect a decreased capacity to move information from short term (primary) into long term (secondary memory) as reported by Craik & Jennings (1992). 2.3.1 Speed of mental processing declines with ageing Luszcz & Bryan (1999) in a thorough review of mediational theories of age related cognitive change conclude that reduced speed of information processing is a fundamental mediator of age related memory loss. This is in line with mediational theories which purport the diminishment of overall cognitive functions (such as executive processes and processing speed) rather than just isolated memory specific processes (Sliwinski & Hofer, 1999). Slowing of mental processing speed with age has been long apparent from the evidence of neuropsychological test performance (Christensen, 2001), and has more recently been confirmed with brain imaging techniques (Sachdev, 2001), which have been informing cognitive psychology since the 1990’s (Tulving, 2001:18). Population studies also show that speed of mental processing declines with age, although longitudinal studies show this is a milder effect than cross-sectional studies have predicted (Kawas, 2003). Longitudinal studies are more realistic because they show individual progression rather than group means (Christensen, 2001). Slowing of processing speed may also be a factor in demonstrated age related declines in abstract thought, visual and verbal memory and recall, naming and verbal fluency (Lezak et al., 2004:297). 8 2.3.2 Fluid and crystallised cognitive abilities In an attempt to differentiate those cognitive abilities affected by ageing from those unaffected, the cognitive capacity of the brain has been categorised into two broad types- these being crystallised and fluid abilities, initially expounded by Cattel in 1963. Crystallised abilities come from accumulated knowledge- that accrued through learning, life experience, education and culture (Anstey & Low, 2004). These types of functions involve those cognitive abilities and skills which are familiar, well learned and well practiced (Lezak et al., 2004:296; Christensen, 2001), hence crystallised abilities engage long term memory (notably semantic and non-declarative aspects) (Anstey & Low, 2004). Conversely, fluid intelligence requires new learning, finding solutions to unfamiliar problems and reflects cognitive processing speed (Lezak et al., 2004: 296-7). It also involves complex attention and working memory- all of which are effected by age (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). Working memory shows age effects when mental organisation or manipulation of material is required or when trying to remember material whilst occupied with other activities (Lezak et al., 2004:298). Fluid and crystallised abilities are thus affected very differently by ageing. Crystallised abilities improve up into the 70’s age group with only slight decreases in very late old age (Christensen, 2001), whereas fluid abilities diminish from young adulthood, with accelerated decline after age 60 (Anstey & Low, 2004). Fluid abilities are affected by environmental insults, genetics and the biological changes of ageing (ibid). This decrease in fluid abilities occurs alongside structural and functional changes as evidenced by neuroimaging techniques (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). 2.3.3 Biological changes of brain ageing The ageing brain experiences structural and functional changes as well as biochemical and molecular ones. In a thorough review Trollor and Valenzuela (2001) discuss some of the biological changes that occur in the aged brain. After 40 years of age there is an approximate reduction in brain size of 5% per decade (ibid). It is now known that this atrophy is not attributable to neuronal loss as has classically been believed to be the case 9 (Horner & Gage, 2002). Recent neuron counting technology confirms that there is minimal neuronal loss in normal aging except for that which occurs in specific regions of the hippocampus; rather loss of size can be attributed to loss of synaptic connections and dendrites, as well as reductions in white matter (Trollor and Valenzuela, 2001). Age related changes in synaptic transmission result from structural and functional changes in neural networks (Albert, 2002). Brain atrophy shows a predilection for certain regions- particularly the prefrontal cortex but also the neostriatum, midbrain and medial temporal areas (Trollor and Valenzuela, 2001). The decrease in hippocampal/medial temporal volume probably explains some of the main ageing related memory deficits observed- those of declarative memory, learning and spatial skills (ibid), as this region is critical in the formation of declarative memory as well as visual and spatial memory (Zola & Squire, 2000:485-497). Furthermore, degenerative changes in the neuronal circuits between the hippocampus and key memory regions of the cortex occur (Albert, 2002). White matter hyperintensities (WMH), viewed on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), can be present in up to 70% of normal aged brains by 70 years of age, and are predominantly sub-cortical (O’Brien et al., 2003). Although researchers have scrutinized these, their significance remains inconclusive at this point. They have been correlated with decreased processing speed (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001) and may be a product of ischaemic brain changes as well as oxidative stress (O’Brien et al., 2003). WMH have also been strongly associated with subjective memory complaints as well as late-onset depression (Minett et al., 2005). As well as the occurrence of WMH, in the aged brain more white matter than grey matter is lost; hence demyelisation may contribute to age related diminishment of cognitive processing speed (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). It is known that β-amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) occur in normal aged brains as well as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (ibid). There are some differences however- firstly, in normal ageing senile plaques are diffuse, in AD they are dense. Secondly, NFT are not found in the cortex in normal ageing, whereas they are widespread throughout the cortex in AD (ibid). The occurrence of these features in normal brains is one of many factors leading some researchers to challenge the 10 hypothesis that they are pathological mediators of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). One strong argument is that AD may be yet another cardiovascular pathology, with NFT’s and plaques occurring as a result of ischaemic damage (de la Torre, 2004) in both normal older and AD brain tissue. Distinctive vascular changes occur in older brains- capillary density is reduced with tortuosities and thickening occurring. By 40-50 years of age 50% of cerebral vessels show thickening, increasing to 80% in the 80-90years age group (Klassen et al., 1968, cited in Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). Thus neurons in older brains may not receive adequate oxygen and nutrition for healthy function, possibly leading to neuronal dysfunction (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). 2.4 Healthy brain ageing It is important to remember that not all older individuals undergo cognitive decline- it is a common but not inevitable feature of advancing age (Kahn & Rowe, 1998). The fact that some older people do not experience cognitive deterioration implies that it may be preventable. The potential for plasticity (the capacity to generate more synaptic connections) is maintained well into old age (Sachdev, 2001). It is now known that self repair mechanisms exist and regeneration of neurones is feasible (Horner & Gage, 2002). This has implications that the brain may be able to benefit from brain restorative tonics, as well as nutritional and other regenerative therapies. 2.5 Conclusion To conclude, it is widely accepted that memory declines with age, but the exact nature of the changes are still unknown despite widespread literature on the topic (Luszcz & Bryan, 1999). However, it is known that different aspects of memory are affected differently, and that cognitive ageing of individuals varies from person to person. It is also agreed upon that speed of information processing is a central feature of age related changes, and there is a decrease in long term declarative memory, acquisition, aspects of working memory and fluid cognitive abilities. Whilst acknowledging these declines in performance, it is important to remember that the changes of normal ageing, unlike neuropathological changes, are mild and do not interfere with a persons’ daily functioning. However, some cognitive deficits do occur 11 in most people with ageing and this is accompanied by biological changes in the brain. Lastly, it is also important to bear in mind that not all older adults experience cognitive deterioration and that the potential for brain plasticity exists. This implies that there may be benefits to be had in therapeutic strategies aimed at improving brain function and overall neurological health. 12 3. THE PROBLEM OF DEMENTIA 3.1 Introduction This chapter features a discussion of the problem and prevalence of dementia in an ageing populus which will clarify and justify the need for the development of therapeutic strategies that can improve cognitive health, and potentially delay or prevent dementia onset. This discussion highlights the need for, and benefits to be had by, effective early intervention strategies for dementia. Secondly, the concept of transitional cognitive impairment states that lie on the continuum between normal, ageing-related cognitive decline and the pathological cognitive loss of dementia will be introduced. In this context, there is a particular focus on mild cognitive impairment (MCI), as, in many people, this condition represents the earliest signal of approaching dementia, and hence a time of potential early intervention. 3.2 Demographic background: an ageing populace In Australia’s early history (and worldwide), infectious disease was the biggest killer. When public health improvements and later, antibiotics reduced infection rates, then mortality from infectious disease decreased markedly, and cardiovascular disease (CVD) and cancer emerged as the leading causes of morbidity and mortality (Jorm, 2001). Since prevention and treatment strategies for CVD and cancer have progressed, people now have a longer life expectancy (ibid.). Coupled with longer life expectancy is the ageing of the post-World War II cohort. As this so-called ‘baby-boomer’ generation ages, Australia will increasingly experience a ‘boom’ in the older age groups, especially the ‘old-old’ age group (those over 85years) - in fact this age group is the most rapidly increasing section of the population (Jorm, 2001). This phenomenon of an ageing population is not expected to ease until the 2040’s when cohorts from lower birth rate periods will constitute the older age groups. Degenerative neurological conditions typically become evident in later life. Hence neurological illness is emerging in epidemic proportions and constitutes a threat to public health care resources as well as to the wellbeing and quality of life of older 13 Australians. One might also ponder the increased risk of neurological damage that the baby boomer generation might be prone to due to widespread psychotropic recreational drug exposure, as well as ever increasing environmental xenobiotic exposure which has neurotoxic effects and causes deleterious effects on cognitive performance (for example Ganzevles & de Geus, 1991; Mearns et al., 1994). 3.3 Dementia prevalence and cost It is in the ‘old-old’ that neurodegenerative disease and especially dementia has the highest prevalence, for example, it is estimated that one in twenty Australians over 65 years old (Australian Department of Health and Aged Care, 1999) and almost one in four over 85 years old have dementia (23.6% in 1995), (Henderson & Jorm, 1998:12). For every 5.1 years of life after age 65, dementia prevalence doubles (Jorm et al., 1987). This reflects a high incidence rate: currently, approximately 1000 Australians are diagnosed with dementia every week (CSIRO, 2005). The future outlook does not get brighter- dementia prevalence rates are expected to swell enormously. The increase in dementia will far outstrip the increase in the Australian population in the coming decades. Current figures are far from reassuringHenderson and Jorm (1998:15) calculate that while the total Australian population will increase by 40% in the years 1995-2041, the number of dementia cases will increase by 254%, explained by the aforementioned disproportionate increase in the old-old. Hence, dementia not only causes immense distress to sufferers and their families, but poses major social and economic problems for society at large. On an economic level, the costs associated with this burgeoning neurodegenerative disease epidemic are enormous and will increase. Because dementia causes chronic, severe, progressive disablement of people over a long period, it has very high care costs. A recent report by the Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW) identified dementia as the greatest cause of disease burden due to disability in the elderly as well as the largest source of expenditure in residential aged care (2004: xi). Dementia cost the Australian health and aged care systems just over 2.5 billion dollars in 2000-01, with 84% of this cost being expenditure on residential aged care (ibid: 75). 14 This issue of the increasing prevalence and cost of neurodegenerative disease is not unique to Australia, it is a worldwide phenomenon. Research based on global demographic data approximates that 25 million people had dementia in 2000, which is expected to rise to 63 million by 2030, with the majority of cases occurring in less developed regions of the world (Wimo et al., 2003). The most promising way of potentially avoiding such increases in dementia prevalence and cost will be the development of therapeutic strategies to delay, prevent and/or treat it (AIHW, 2004). 3.4 Dementia pathologies Dementia is not a disease but is a syndrome of progressive cognitive decline caused by numerous different pathologies- in fact it is estimated that more than 200 types of dementia exist (Haan & Wallace, 2004). In early stages cognitive symptomatology differs with different aetiologies, however as neurodegenerative pathology progresses, then the marked destruction of brain tissue renders similar symptoms whatever the cause (Lezak et al., 2004:207). The various pathological mediators of dementia include Alzheimer’s disease (AD), vascular dementia (and mixed forms of both of these), Lewy body dementia, Picks disease, Huntington’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, substance-induced dementia, head trauma, as well as various immune, endocrine, and systemic disorders amongst others (American Psychiatric Association, 2000:147-171). Of these, AD and vascular dementia account for the vast majority of cases. AD is the major cause of dementia, accounting for an estimated 50-70% of cases, whereas vascular dementia accounts for 20-30% (Alzheimer’s Australia, 2004). AD is a chronic degenerative condition that causes changes in structure and function of the brain. Cell numbers are diminished in specific areas of the brain, including the frontal, temporal and parietal cortex and the hippocampus. Neuritic plaques of β-amyloid and tau protein composition and neurofibrillary tangles form in the brain tissue throughout the cortex and sub-cortical grey matter. There occurs a depletion of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine and the enzyme which produces it- choline acetyltransferase, along with other neurotransmitters and neurochemicals (World Health Organisation, 1992, cited in Henderson & Jorm, 1998). These changes result in diminished cognitive function that worsens as the disease progresses. 15 The cognitive changes associated with dementia generally progress along a continuum from normality to mild, moderate and severe stages, though there are wide variations of symptoms and progression between individuals. Abrams, Beers and Berkow (1995) describe these cognitive changes as follows. There is impairment in short term and long term memory, particularly the inability to learn and recall new information. Problems with abstract thinking and judgement occur, as well as the loss of higher cortical functions causing language difficulties, motor impairments, problems with recognition, and personality changes. Eventually, neurological destruction affects all bodily systems, and people generally die of illnesses of debility such as pneumonia (Lezak et al., 2004:207). 3.5 Therapeutic strategies for dementia Unfortunately there are no effective medications available for Alzheimer’s disease or dementia. The anti-psychotic drugs frequently applied to manage the behavioural symptoms ironically can cause neurological side effects and excessive sedation (Bennett, 1999). One must question the effects of these strong drugs on an already confused mind. One longitudinal study found that these medications doubled the rate of cognitive decline in AD (McShane et al., 1997). A data survey of Sydney nursing home residents found 27.4% were on regular anti-psychotic medication, mainly those residents with greatest cognitive impairment (Snowdon et al., 1995). The widespread use of these drugs may help make the patient with dementia a little more controllable (for example see Phipps, 1999), but the cost is further impaired cognitive function. Specific pharmacotherapies developed for Alzheimer’s’ disease, such as tacrine, donepezil, rivastigmine and galantamine, inhibit cholinesterase- the enzyme that breaks down acetylcholine. Studies (for example, Knapp et al., 1994; Rogers et al., 1998; Courtney et al., 2004) have shown these agents do have modest clinical efficacy, but they do not effect the underlying pathology and this, coupled with strong side effects, results in questionable usefulness. There is evidence that these medications can slow the progression of symptoms initially, but don’t change the overall outcome or prognosis (Lopez, et al., 2002). The search for effective therapeutic strategies continues. 16 As Jorm (2002) points out, disease prevention can mean either elimination or postponement until later life, the latter being a more probable prospect for dementia. The development of therapies that can delay the onset of dementia, by even moderate degrees, could have an enormous favourable impact on both public health expenditure and personal suffering (Alzheimer’s Australia, 2004). For example, Access Economics, in a study commissioned by the Alzheimer’s Association Australia, determined that if, starting in 2005, the age of onset of Alzheimer’s disease could be delayed by just 5 months, there would be 5% less new cases per year, and if delayed by 5 years there would be 50% less new cases per year (Access Economics, 2004: i). Furthermore, these researchers demonstrate that a 5-year delayment would cumulatively save the Australian economy many billions of dollars (67.5 billion dollars by 2040). The staggering prevalence figures and the reductions in costs and suffering that effective interventions could produce indicate the urgent need for preventive and disease delay tactics. Scarpini et al. (2003) identify three promising areas of research: firstly, reduction of risk factors- such as oxidative stress, cardiovascular risk factors and inflammation; secondly, neurogenesis promotion- via stem cell and nerve growth factors and lastly, prevention and removal of amyloid plaque via vaccine and chelation agents. In the category of reduction of risk factors, much promising work exists to suggest that prevention of AD is possible (Mattson, 2000). It is in the area of risk prevention that both nutritional approaches and botanical medicines may have a lot to offer. It is becoming widely recognised that reduction of cardiovascular risk factors will also decrease the risk of dementia of both AD and vascular types. Specifically, these factors include elevated homocysteine and LDL cholesterol levels, obesity, hypertension and diabetes mellitus- all of which are modifiable (Haan & Wallace, 2004; Mattson, 2000). Dietary consumption of fish, monounsaturated oils and antioxidants have all been associated with lowered risk for dementia (Morris et al., 2005; Barberger-Gateau et al., 2002; Solfrizzi et al., 1999; Morris et al., 2002) as has caloric restriction (Mattson, 2003). As well as dietary antioxidants, research also supports a role for supplemental antioxidants, especially vitamins C and E, in dementia prevention (Larrieu et al., 2004; Zandi et al., 2004; Sano et al., 1997). 17 Plant medicines that reputedly act to improve function of the central nervous system are a promising area for research. They are most likely to offer benefits in the abovementioned ‘reduction of risk factors’ category of therapy, because many phytochemicals from plants are now known to be powerful antioxidants, and many also have antiinflammatory actions. Bacopa has been shown to possess both of these actions (Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Tripathi et al., 1996; Russo et al., 2003a; 2003b; Jain et al., 1994). Furthermore, Bacopa has also been shown to possess a cholinergic modulation effect (Bhattacharya et al., 1999). This is a significant effect because, as highlighted earlier, cholinergic deficits are a feature of dementia (World Health Organisation, 1992, cited in Henderson & Jorm, 1998). Other indications that promising therapeutic agents might be found in the plant kingdom come from studies wherein various herbs have demonstrated beneficial cognitive and neurological effects. For example, many well designed trials have shown the positive effect of Gingko biloba extract on dementia. Between 1975 and 1992 there were 34 clinical trials of Gingko involving 2326 patients, in which the incidence of side-effects was below 3% (Schulz et al., 1997). Improvements in mental speed, memory and concentration (Vesper & Hansgen 1994), cerebral perfusion (Kleijnen & Knipschild 1992), and overall cognitive function (Hofferberth, 1994) have been well demonstrated. One high quality 12-month RDBPC clinical trial found Gingko improved cognitive symptoms in dementia to a degree equivalent to the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor medications, with an absence of adverse effects (Le Bars et al., 1997). Panax ginseng is another example of a plant which has demonstrated promising neurological benefits. Blumenthal reviews a number of human studies which have demonstrated improvement in various cognitive and psychological parameters with Panax administration (2003:215-225). Thus the beneficial neurological effect of some botanical agents has been well demonstrated and this supports the exploration of Bacopa monnieri as another potentially therapeutic botanical agent. 3.6 Pre-clinical dementia states Dementia is not a natural consequence of the ageing process. People experience varying degrees of cognitive deterioration as they age- from nil in some cases of so called ‘successful ageing’ (Kahn & Rowe, 1998), through to the pathological loss of dementia. 18 Between normal ageing-related cognitive change and the pathological changes of dementia, lies a transitional zone of cognitive impairment, wherein there may be some loss of cognitive function of various manifestations and aetiologies, which is not severe enough to affect daily functioning, but will progress to dementia in some cases. The pathology of Alzheimer’s disease can be present for many years before it is diagnosed, and its earliest stages may exhibit only subtle cognitive changes (Alzheimer’s Australia, 2004:11), hence this transitional zone may represent the earliest stages of dementia for many people. Therefore, this has become an area of research focus in the attempt to enable researchers to ultimately address dementia at its earliest stages by developing strategies to treat, prevent or delay its onset (Albert, 2002). In the literature, a number of different terms have been developed to describe various states of slight cognitive impairment. These include (amongst others): age-associated cognitive decline (AACD) (Levy, 1994), cognitive impairment no dementia (CIND) (Graham et al., 1997) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Petersen et al., 1999). Unsatisfactory boundaries exist between the various syndromes, resulting in a confusing overlap between aetiology, prognosis and prevalence (De Carli, 2003). However, despite the fact that definite and unanimous criteria have not been fully established for any of these classifications, MCI is widely gaining acceptance as a meaningful defining concept, and it has been further refined over the years to provide better diagnostic and operational criteria, including the elucidation of sub-types (Petersen, 2004). Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) describes people who have cognitive impairment beyond that of normal ageing but are not demented. Unlike normal age-related changes, however, MCI is recognised as a pathological entity (Petersen, 2004). The criteria for MCI include memory complaints with preserved intellectual and daily functioning, impaired memory relative to peers and the absence of dementia (Petersen et al., 2001). The usefulness of its operational criteria in distinguishing between normality and dementia has been demonstrated in a descriptive and comparative study of participants with MCI enrolled in a large multicentre clinical trial (Grundman et al., 2004). MCI has been classified into various sub-types, some of which are now recognised as prodromal forms of various dementia types- however specific criteria are yet to be agreed upon (Petersen, 2004). The sub-types include amnestic MCI (a-MCI) wherein 19 memory is objectively and subjectively impaired, but daily functioning and other cognitive domains are preserved; multiple domain MCI (md-MCI) which describes multiple cognitive impairments of varying degrees with or without memory problems; and lastly a single domain non-memory MCI (such as language for example), which is uncommon (Petersen et al., 2001). Each of these sub-types exhibits a propensity to develop into different dementia types, with a-MCI particularly indicative of possible future AD. From a longitudinal study of 220 older people from the Mayo Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centre, researchers estimated that approximately 12% of people with MCI progress to dementia annually (in contrast to approximately 1-2% of the general population), and that if followed for 6 years, 80% will have become demented (Petersen et al., 1999; 2004). However, in the literature there is a large variation in reports of rates of progression to dementia and this probably reflects differences in subjects, diagnostic criteria used, as well as varying lengths of follow up (Dawe et al., 1992). Likewise, estimates of the prevalence and incidence rates of MCI vary widely depending on the diagnostic criteria used (Busse et al., 2003). It is widely agreed upon though, that MCI occurs frequently in the aged population (ibid.), and people with it are at greater risk than the normal population of developing dementia, even though the degree of risk has not yet been established (Schneider, 2005). Hence MCI represents a potential area for early dementia intervention strategies. 3.7 Conclusion The Australian population, following the global trend, is experiencing an ageing population, with particular increases in the ‘old-old’. It is in this age group that neurological illness occurs most frequently, and of the neurological illnesses dementia is the most prevalent. Hence there is a burgeoning epidemic of dementia. This is associated with huge costs in terms of both personal suffering and economics. Effective pharmacological therapies do not currently exist, and remain elusive. Botanical agents have shown some benefits in improving neurological health, and lifestyle, nutritional and cardiovascular risk reduction strategies appear to offer very promising outcomes for dementia risk reduction and prevention. Even small delays in dementia onset would have enormous benefits. The development of such strategies is imperative. 20 A more marked impairment of cognition and memory than occurs in normal ageing becomes apparent in some older people, and there is a recognised transitional zone between normal ageing and dementia. Various terms have been applied to this state, with MCI now recognised as a possible manifestation of very early dementia. Much attention has been focused on this area because early diagnosis and intervention present the greatest hope and potential for achieving prevention, delay and treatment strategies for dementia. Restoration of brain health and cognitive function is one of the most promising areas for potentially reducing the incidence and hence prevalence rates of dementia and thereby alleviation of suffering and costs. 21 4: BACOPA MONNIERI 4.1 Introduction Much has been written over the ages about Bacopa monnieri (Bacopa). As this research project sought to observe the effects of Bacopa on the memory of older people in a randomised controlled clinical trial, a detailed analysis of what is currently known about the herb is required. This chapter will therefore examine what is known about Bacopa including its historical and contemporary application, its constituents and its known effects. The knowledge gleaned from the traditional usage of plant medicines globally can point to therapeutic agents which potentially have a lot to offer modern health care practice, and this may be especially relevant for pathologies where effective pharmacological therapies are unavailable or inadequate. Impaired cognition in the aged is a condition for which there is a paucity of effective therapies, and additionally, it presents a large and escalating public health issue. A plant that has been shown, in the literature explored below, to hold great promise for the improvement of cognitive function is Bacopa monnieri, commonly called Brahmi in Sanskrit and in Hindi, and water hyssop in English. Bacopa belongs to the family Scrophulariaceae, and is a small succulent creeper that thrives in warm climates throughout the world, growing in moist places and along waterways. It is a plant that is native to both India and Australia. Bacopa’s botanical name has numerous synonyms, commonly encountered ones include: Bacopa monniera Wettst., Bacopa monniera Linn., and Herpestis monniera (Kapoor, 1990; Morgan & Bone, 1999; Russo & Borrelli, 2005). 4.2 History Bacopa has been regarded as a highly effective brain tonic since antiquity in the Ayurvedic medical system of India (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). According to Hackman (1998), early Hindu religious practices prior to written history required the memorisation and repetition of lengthy, orally transmitted Vedic scriptures by scholars. 22 Bacopa was reputedly used in these early times to enhance the scholars’ capacity to memorise these epic hymns and scriptures (ibid.). The herb has been described in Ayurvedic texts since around 800 BC and recorded as a treatment for a range of mental disorders in the ‘Carak Samhita’ (Singh & Dhawan, 1997), which, according to the literature, was written in the 6th century AD (Chowdhuri et al., 2002; Russo & Borrelli, 2005). In a later treatise of the 16th century, the Bhavprakasa Varg-Prakarana, Bacopa’s actions are set down as follows: bitter, laxative, astringent, brain tonic, memory enhancing, and longevity promoting. As well as brain conditions such as epilepsy, insanity and neuroses, other indications described in this treatise include anaemia, leprosy, renal disease, blood disease, poisoning and cough (Singh & Dhawan, 1982). Ayurvedic medicine classifies Bacopa as belonging to a group of plant medicinesknown as medhya rasayana- that improve mental health, intellect and memory (medhya) and promote longevity and rejuvenation (rasayana) (Singh & Singh, 1980). The Sanskrit name Brahmi stems from Brahma- the creative aspect of God. As the brain is seen as the creative centre of humans, then Bacopa, which acts primarily on the brain, is so named (Russo & Borrelli, 2005). Hence Bacopa shares its Sanskrit name, Brahmi, with another herbal nervous system restorative- Centella asiatica (Gotu kola) (Morgan & Bone, 1999). 4.3 Constituents Many of Bacopa’s constituents have been identified including numerous saponins, alkaloids designated Brahmine and Herpestine, and flavonoids. In a thorough review of the chemical composition of Brahmi, Russo and Borrelli (2005) point out that the first constituent identified was an alkaloid ‘Brahmine’ in early work by Bose and Bose (1931). Alkaloids have been found to have only a poor yield, however (Dey et al., 1964). In the 1950s and 60s research by chemists at India’s Central Drug Research Institute identified a variety of saponins, and the elucidation of constituents by various laboratories is still continuing. For example, bacopasides I to V have been identified, and also bacopasaponins A to G, as well as bacosides A1 to A3 and B. (Chakravarty et al., 2001, 2003; Hou et al., 2002; Mahato et al., 2000; Rastogi et al., 1994). Saponins are considered to be the major active constituents of the plant. 23 Saponins are glycosides, a sugar unit attached to an aglycone portion (the sapogenin). The sapogenin portion describes the type of saponin- either steroidal (4-ringed structure), or triterpenoid (5-ringed structure) (Mills & Bone, 2000:43). The main active chemical constituents of Bacopa are the dammarane-type triterpenoid saponins (Garai et al., 1996a, 1996b; Mahato et al., 2000) with jujubogenin and pseudojujubogenin as the aglycones (Deepak & Amit, 2004). The saponins consist of numerous subtypes designated as bacosides, bacopasides and bacopasaponins as mentioned above. Bacoside A is considered the major active component, first identified by Chatterji et al. in 1963, with bacoside B being an optical isomer of A (Singh et al., 1988). Morgan & Bone stated in 1999 that in herbal extracts standardised to bacoside A content, it is likely that bacoside A collectively refers to the overall content of dammarane saponins rather than a solitary saponin. This view is supported by a 1978 study that had shown bacoside A to be a mixture rather than a single saponin (Kawai & Shibata). A recent study has confirmed this by identifying four major components of bacoside A, these being: bacoside A3, bacopaside II, bacopasaponin C and the jujubogenin isomer of bacopasaponin C. The authors who identified these components also reported the presence of two well known flavonoids: luteolin and apigenin in all samples (Deepak et al., 2005). It is significant that dammarane-type saponins are also the primary active constituents in Panax ginseng (Blumenthal, 2003:215), in which they have been shown to promote memory acquisition, retention and retrieval in rats (Ma & Yu, 1993; Ma, Yu & Chen, 1991) and survival of experimentally damaged neurones in chicken and rat cerebral cortex (Himi et al., 1989). 4.4 Pre-clinical and clinical studies of the cognitive effects of Bacopa 4.4.1 In vitro and animal studies Learning ability in rats has been significantly enhanced by Bacopa extract as it facilitated acquisition, consolidation and retention of three newly learned behavioural responses at an oral dosage of 40mg/kg three times daily (Singh & Dhawan, 1982). In 24 this study, effects on cognitive function were measured by foot shock motivated brightness discrimination reaction, active conditioned flight reaction (jump to avoid shock) and continuous avoidance response (shock avoidance by lever pulling) tests. Bacopa facilitated all parameters of memory acquisition and retention. In a subsequent study the same authors investigated the constituents responsible for Bacopa’s effect and demonstrated that the isolated bacosides A and B were effective in enhancing memory in rats in learning tasks involving both positive and negative reinforcement (Singh & Dhawan, 1997; Singh et al., 1988). Additionally, this study demonstrated that the bacosides produced changes in the hippocampus, cerebral cortex (areas critical to memory function) and hypothalamus regions of the brain and caused enhanced levels of protein kinase activity and increases in protein levels in these regions. This indicated positive implications for improved neurotransmission and repair of damaged neurons via enhanced regeneration of nerve synapses (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). A recent study concurs with Singh and Dhawan’s findings regarding the effects of isolated Bacopa saponins on memory. Administration of bacosides to mice attenuated experimentally induced anterograde amnesia and improved memory as measured by a well validated learning task- the Morris Water maze test (Kishore & Singh, 2005). 4.4.1.1 Cholinergic effects The diminished ability to learn and recall new information is a strong feature of Alzheimer’s disease. In rat models of Alzheimer’s disease, Bacopa was shown to significantly promote memory as well as reversing induced reductions of acetylcholine (ACh) in the frontal cortex and hippocampus regions. The activity of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT- a key catalyst in the production of ACh), and muscarinic receptor binding of ACh were also improved (Bhattacharya, Kumar & Ghosal, 1999). ACh is a neurotransmitter which plays an important role in memory and learning functions in the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus (Carlson, 2002:106). Furthermore, depletion of ChAT and hence ACh, is one of the central neuropathological features of Alzheimer’s disease (World Health Organisation, 1992, cited in Henderson & Jorm, 1998). Bhattacharya et al. (1999), demonstrated that the mechanism of action of Bacopa is likely to be, at least in part, related to cholinergic modulation. 25 Further support for a cholinergic effect of Bacopa comes from two other animal studies. Firstly, Das and colleagues (2002) demonstrated an in-vitro, dose dependent, partial inhibition of the activity of acetylcholinesterase (AChE- the post-synaptic enzyme which breaks down ACh), as well as significantly attenuated cognitive performance observed in-vivo in rats with scopolamine-induced dementia. Secondly, an early study by Dey et al. (1964) demonstrated hypotensive and bradycardic effects in cats with administration of intravenous Bacopa, leading these researchers to postulate that the mechanism was via cholinergic activation because the effect was partly (60-70%) blocked by atropine, (atropine blocks muscarinic ACh receptors). 4.4.1.2 Antioxidant activity The antioxidant activity of Bacopa has been reported in a number of laboratory studies (Tripathi et al., 1996; Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Sairam et al., 2001; Sumathy et al., 2001, 2002; Russo et al., 2003a, 2003b). Antioxidant effects of Bacopa in areas of the brain that are key memory areas- the hippocampus, frontal cortex and striatum- have been documented by Bhattacharya et al. (2000) in rat brain. Bacopa was shown to protect the brain (Sumathy et al., 2002) and liver (Sumathy et al., 2001), from morphine-induced inhibition of antioxidant enzyme systems. Russo et al. (2005) demonstrated a free radical scavenging activity which protected against cytotoxicity and DNA damage in human fibroblasts (Russo, et al., 2003a). Further research by Russo et al. (2003b), also demonstrated that Bacopa significantly reduced oxidation and DNA damage in cultured rat astrocytes induced by a nitric oxide donor. Furthermore, Anbarasi et al. (2005) demonstrated that isolated bacoside A protected rat brain tissue from various parameters of oxidative stress caused by chronic cigarette smoke exposure. One of the foremost theories of brain ageing asserts that free radical damage results in both ageing-related changes in healthy brains (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001) and in neurodegenerative pathology, such as Alzheimer’s disease (Singh, et al., 2004). Good antioxidant status is associated with better memory performance in the aged (Perrig, 1987) and antioxidant therapy has been targeted as a promising dementia strategy by Jorm, one of Australia’s foremost authorities on dementia (Jorm, 2002). Thus, the demonstrated antioxidant effects of Bacopa, particularly in brain tissue, support its 26 potential as a therapy in neurodegenerative pathologies and age-related cognitive decline. Stress elicits a defensive response in living organisms. The defence response involves several mechanisms including stress gene expression, enhanced antioxidant protection, and enhanced toxin clearance. Bacopa has been shown to facilitate each of these adaptive resources by modulation of Hsp 70 expression, and enhancement of activity of both superoxide dismutase and cytochrome P450 enzymes in stressor exposed rat brain (Chowdhuri et al., 2002). Thus, Bacopa may facilitate the capacity of the brain to withstand stress, and help the brain to function under adverse conditions. These findings support the afore-mentioned medhya rasayana classification of Bacopa in ancient Ayurveda in that they imply a brain tonic and adaptogenic effect (adaptogenic meaning improved resistance to stress). This may indicate some similarities with Panax ginseng (another dammarane saponin-containing herb as mentioned previously), which is considered to be a major adaptogen and tonic, enhancing resistance to stress in numerous experimental situations as well as clinical trials (Blumenthal, 2003: 214-226; Mills & Bone 2000:420-427). 4.4.2 Human trials In the clinical trials described below, Bacopa has been found to improve various aspects of cognitive function in children and adults. Sharma et al. (1987) found that learning, memory, perception and reaction times improved in 20 primary school children given Bacopa in syrup form at a dosage of 350mg three times daily for three months. No side effects were reported. This study was strengthened by the use of a matched placebo control group (n=20). However, the study was limited in that it was not double-blinded, and used a small sample size. Negi et al. (2000) reported children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were found to benefit from Bacopa administration. A randomised, doubleblind, placebo-controlled trial of 36 children with ADHD was carried out by the Indian researchers. Bacopa was given at a dosage of 50mg twice daily for 12-weeks, and a battery of cognitive function tests administered at baseline, 4, 8, 12 and 16-weeks (i.e. 4-weeks post trial). Improvements were reported in the active treatment group (n=19) at 27 12-weeks, as measured by tests of sentence repetition, logical memory, and paired associate learning tasks. Interestingly, improvement was still apparent at 16-weeks, four weeks after cessation of Bacopa administration. In an open trial, 35 adults with anxiety neurosis were treated with Bacopa at the dose of 12g of dried herb daily in syrup form for 4-weeks. No significant side effects were observed and results were highly favourable as overall anxiety levels, concentration and memory span were all significantly improved along with other major anxiety-related physical symptoms and biochemical markers of anxiety (Singh & Singh, 1980). Whilst the findings of this study are encouraging, the lack of a control group is a significant design flaw, and further, the researchers do not delineate the selection criteria for inclusion in the study. Bacopa’s anxiolytic action was supported in a later animal study in which it was compared with a major pharmacological anxiolytic agent- the benzodiazepine lorazepam, in validated rat models of anxiety. It proved to be as effectual as the drug in every outcome measured without producing any motor deficits (a common side-effect of lorazepam) (Bhattacharya & Ghosal 1998). Whilst promising, these findings indicate the need for further rigorous clinical trials to establish whether they are applicable to humans. It is noteworthy that support for an anxiolytic action in humans was provided by a well designed clinical trial (discussed below in detail) in which state anxiety was significantly improved by Bacopa (Stough et al., 2001). Some recent Australian studies demonstrate that Bacopa may be effective for enhancing cognition in longer rather than shorter term administration. One study, using a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled design, tested the effects of Bacopa monnieri on cognitive function in forty six healthy adults between 18-60years of age. Participants took Bacopa (300mg daily) or placebo for 12-weeks and measurements, using a battery of well validated neuropsychological tests, were recorded at baseline, 5weeks and 12-weeks. Significant improvements were found in the active treatment group in speed of information processing (as measured by the Inspection Time task), learning rate and memory consolidation (as measured by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test) and state anxiety levels (as measured by Spielberger’s State Anxiety Inventory). Improvements were only found at 12-weeks and not earlier, suggesting that 28 chronic administration is required to elicit Bacopa’s effects (Stough et al., 2001). This study is well designed and rigorous; however, a limitation is that it does not take into account age, gender or education effects, all of which are known to effect performance on memory tests. In contrast to the findings of Stough et al., members of the same research team reported that Bacopa had no acute effect on memory, when outcomes were measured at 2-hours post-administration (Nathan et al., 2001). In this study a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial was used to assess the acute effects of Bacopa (300mg daily) on thirty eight healthy adults between 18-60 years of age. The same well validated neuropsychological test battery as employed in the Stough study (above) was used to assess the effects of Bacopa before and then 2-hours after administration. No effects were found. Additionally, in a later randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of 85 adults (aged 19-68years) which tested cognitive effects of a combined tablet of 300mg Bacopa with 120mg Gingko biloba, no significant effects were found in outcomes measured at 2-weeks and 4-weeks of treatment duration (Nathan, et al., 2004). Despite extensive searching of the literature it appears that the effects of Bacopa observed at 12-weeks have not been reported for lesser durations in well designed, larger human trials. Another Australian based randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study confirmed the efficacy of Bacopa in improving memory in chronic administration. In this study, seventy six adults 40-65years of age, were given Bacopa (dose 300mg, or 450mg for persons over 90kg) or placebo and measured on tasks of attention, memory and psychological state at baseline, 12-weeks and 6-weeks post trial (Roodenrys et al., 2002). Bacopa significantly improved the ability to retain information over time as measured by a task requiring delayed recall of word pairs. The authors commented that this may be due to less information being lost from memory, that is, the results are due to decreased forgetting, as opposed to enhanced acquisition because learning trials did not show any effect of Bacopa. Outcome measures in this study which failed to show a significant effect for Bacopa were tasks of short term memory, working memory, attention, retrieval of prior knowledge and psychological state (anxiety, stress and depression). 29 Roodenrys et al.’s finding of decreased forgetting rate as measured by a word pairs task supports the aforementioned findings of Stough et al. (2001) who also reported decreased forgetting rate, as measured by the AVLT. In contrast to Roodenrys et al., however, Stough’s study, as well as the animal study of Singh and Dhawan (1997), did find an improvement in learning rate. A further contrast between the two studies is Stough et al’s finding of Bacopa’s efficacy in reducing anxiety. Additionally, Roodenrys et al.’s findings also contrasts with the anxiolytic effect observed in aforementioned studies by Singh & Singh (1980) and Bhattacharya & Ghosal (1998). The Roodenrys et al. study demonstrates a sturdy design, using well validated instruments to measure outcomes; however as in the Stough et al. study, age and education were not controlled for nor were the data adjusted for the effects of multiple measures. Thus there is an increased chance of a type one error. The clinical trials on the neuropsychological effects of Bacopa are summarised in Table 4.1. A double-blind, placebo-controlled toxicological study in which bacosides were administered in various single doses (ranging from 20mg to 300mg) as well as multiple doses (100mg and 200mg) to healthy male volunteers for one month demonstrated an absence of any side-effects (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). The herb is TGA approved in Australia for over the counter sale as it is considered safe, and no adverse reactions are recorded in the literature. The traditional recommended dosage regimen is 5-10g of the powdered dried herb daily (Anonymous, Indian Herbal Pharmacopoeia, 2002:36). 30 Table 4.1: Summary of the clinical trials of neuropsychological effects of Bacopa Author and Design Year Nathan R, DB, PC et al., 2004 n=85 healthy adults aged 19-68yrs Dosage and Duration Results Combination of Bacopa 300mg/day and Gingko 120mg/day 4-weeks Measurements at baseline, 2 & 4-weeks. No significant effect found on cognition and memory. No difference in sideeffects compared to placebo. Roodenrys et al., 2002 R, DB, PC n=76 healthy adults aged 40-65yrs 300mg/d (standardised to min 55% bacosides) 12-weeks Significant effect on retention of new information. Attention, retrieval and anxiety measures unaffected. One withdrawal due to GIT upset in Bacopa group. Stough et al., 2001 R, DB, PC n=46 healthy adults aged 18-60yrs 300mg/d (standardised to min 55% bacosides) 12-weeks Significantly improved speed of information processing, learning rate & memory consolidation & reduced state anxiety. Adverse reactions reported: nausea, dry mouth & fatigue. Nathan et al., 2001 R, DB, PC n=36 healthy adults aged 18-60yrs 300mg/d (standardised to min 55% bacosides) 2-hours postadministration No effect on various measures of memory performance found with acute administration of Bacopa. Negi et al., 2000 R, DB, PC n=36 children with ADHD* 100mg/day 12-weeks Improvements in a range of cognitive assessments. No sideeffects reported. Sharma et al., 1987 Matched group PC n=40 healthy children aged 6-8yrs 350mg three times daily in syrup form 12-weeks Learning, memory, perception & reaction times improved. No side-effects reported. Singh & Singh, 1980 O n=35 adults with anxiety neurosis 12g/day dried Bacopa in syrup form 4-weeks Decreased anxiety, improved concentration, improved memory span. No side-effects reported. R= randomised, DB= double-blind, PC= placebo-controlled, O=open * ADHD= attention deficit hyperactivity disorder 31 4.5 Conclusion In conclusion, there are a number of in-vitro and animal studies that demonstrate Bacopa’s potential for improving cognitive and neurological function, as well as substantiating both antioxidant and cholinergic actions. These have been supported by reports of memory enhancement in human clinical trials in adults and children. Most clinical studies suggest that efficacy is evident with longer term administration (typically 3 months) rather than shorter term administration where effects are not always evident. These animal and human studies validate the traditional claims of Ayurvedic medicine in which known usage of Bacopa as a ‘brain and memory tonic’ dates back approximately 2000 to 3000 years. Despite this, there have not as yet been any studies that have examined Bacopa’s effect on memory specifically in older people, which is when memory frequently begins to decline; nor in dementia, in which memory loss is a core feature. This study, then, examined the effect of Bacopa specifically in older people and thus contributes to filling this knowledge gap. 32 5. RESEARCH DESIGN 5.1 Introduction and overview The aim of this study was to assess the efficacy of the herbal medicine, Bacopa monnieri on memory performance in healthy people over the age of 55-years. It was hypothesised that Bacopa would significantly improve memory function in older people, and a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial was used to test this hypothesis. This chapter describes the procedures, design and method of data collection and its analysis. The study commenced with an initial screening assessment. If the study criteria were met, participants were enrolled into the study and randomised into active and placebo groups. A neuropsychological test battery and subjective memory complaint questionnaire were administered at a second clinical visit, to establish baseline measurements of memory function, and participants were given tablets, instruction sheets and medication record booklets (see Appendix I) at this visit. This was followed by 12-weeks of supplementation with Bacopa or placebo tablets. At the end of 12weeks the neuropsychological test battery and subjective memory complaint questionnaire were again administered and compliance and side-effects were assessed. 5.2 Location and timing The study took place in the Natural Medicine Clinic of the School of Natural and Complementary Medicine at Southern Cross University in Lismore, Australia between February and July 2005. 5.3 Ethical considerations To ensure that the wellbeing and dignity of participants was safeguarded, this study adhered to the ethical principles and procedures as laid down by the Southern Cross University Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC) in accordance with the National Health and Medical Research Council Act, 1992, and the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Research Involving Humans. All details of the research aims, recruitment of participants and study procedures were provided to the HREC prior to the 33 commencement of the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to commencement of the study (Appendix III). All participants were given a written information sheet explaining purposes of the study and what participation would involve as well as explaining the option to withdraw consent at any time (Appendix III). Confidentiality was strictly maintained and only those directly involved in the research had access to the data collected. All data from the study has been carefully stored in a locked filing cabinet in the researcher’s office in the university premises and will be destroyed after 5-years. Ethics approval (number ECN04-141) was obtained prior to commencement of the trial; this can be found in Appendix II. 5.4 Recruitment of participants Participants of both sexes, 55-years of age and above were self-selected from the general population of the Northern Rivers region in NSW, Australia. The study was publicised via radio, television and print media and healthy people in this age group were invited to register their interest in participation by telephone. The trial was also advertised electronically via staff email on the university wide intranet at Southern Cross University. 5.4.1 Sample size Using the R2 statistical package and nominating an effect size of 0.4 with alpha at 0.05 and a power level of Beta=0.80, a power analysis determined a prospective sample size of 80 participants (40 in each group) for this study. Thus it was planned to enrol 100 participants in the trial to allow for a 20% dropout rate. 5.5 Procedures One hundred and thirty-six people responded to the media release and email. These respondents were contacted by telephone and asked brief screening questions then enrolled for an initial clinical screening assessment. The initial screening assessment included a comprehensive systems review, a brief physical examination including vital signs and urinalysis, and assessment of cognitive function and emotional state utilising the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) and the Hamilton Depression Scale (Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979). 34 All clinical data was recorded on standard Clinical Report Forms (Appendix IV). This first clinical screening session was completed in approximately 1 hour per subject and was carried out by a registered nurse and naturopathic clinician. If selection criteria for the study were met (see below), the subject was then scheduled for a baseline assessment of neuropsychological function and subjective memory complaints (lasting approximately 1 hour). At this visit a 12-week supply of tablets was provided and the subject was scheduled for a second, end-of-trial, assessment of neuropsychological function and subjective memory complaints (also lasting 1 hour) which took place twelve weeks later. All of the neuropsychological assessments at both baseline and at end point were conducted by the same psychologist. The neuropsychological test administration and scoring were supervised by a consultant clinical neuropsychologist. The order and timing of the neuropsychological tests administration was the same for each testing session, and this protocol is detailed in Appendix V. 5.6 Selection criteria Following the screening assessment, 103 healthy, cognitively normal men and women over the age of 55-years were initially included in the study, with 98 of these commencing the study drugs. Selection criteria (given below) were partly adapted from Crook et al.’s diagnostic criteria for Age Associated Memory Impairment (1986) and expanded to incorporate usage of either herbal medicines or recreational drugs as exclusionary criteria. 5.6.1 Inclusion criteria • Age 55-years or over at commencement of trial • Absence of dementia as determined by a score of 24 or greater on the MiniMental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975). • Absence of depression as determined by a score of 12 or less on the Hamilton Depression Scale (Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979) 5.6.2 Exclusion criteria • Diagnosed psychiatric or neurological disorder 35 • History of brain inflammation or infection or previous head injury resulting in unconsciousness • Cerebral ischaemia as determined by a score of 4 or greater on the modified Hachinski Ischaemia Scale (Rosen et al., 1980) • History of disease of any of the following bodily systems: cardiovascular, renal, endocrine, liver, kidney, respiratory • Systemic disease or malignancy • Psychoactive medication usage including anti-depressants, anxiolytics, sedatives or stimulants • Current herbal medicine usage (unless willing to discontinue for a 4-week washout period and the duration of the trial) • Recreational drug use • Alcohol abuse (defined by consumption of more than 4 standard drinks per day) 5.7 Study design A 12-week randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial was designed to provide maximum outcomes with the resources available to undertake the study. Participants were randomly assigned to active or placebo groups and neuropsychological tests as well as the subjective memory complaint questionnaire were administered at baseline and again at 12-weeks to measure the effects of Bacopa compared to placebo on memory and cognitive function. 5.7.1 Randomisation Following the exclusion of respondents, participants were randomly allocated into two equal groups to receive either active treatment or placebo. Randomisation to groups was carried out by a research academic at Southern Cross University, who had no involvement with the study. Randomisation was generated via the following internet randomisation website: http://www.randomization.com. This process randomised each subject to treatment groups using the method of randomly permuted blocks. Neither the participants nor anyone involved in conducting the research knew which group participants were in until the data analysis stage of the research was completed. Randomisation codes were stored electronically by the research academic who carried out the randomisation process. 36 5.8 Compliance At the second clinical visit (baseline assessment), participants were provided with enough tablets to last for the twelve weeks study duration, and also ten extra tablets were provided in case of loss or damage. Participants were also given an instruction sheet and record booklet in which they were instructed to record their daily ingestion of the trial medication, as well as any side-effects or symptoms experienced during the trial (Appendix I). These booklets were collected either at the last clinical visit (end-of-trial) or, otherwise, at the time of withdrawal from the trial. Participants were supplied with verbal and written instructions to contact the researcher if any side-effects occurred, or if they needed to withdraw for any reason. All tablets remaining at the end-of-trial visit were collected and counted by the researcher. A 20% or greater discrepancy in the tablet count was designated as constituting non-compliance (based on a similar psychopharmacological trial by LeBars et al., 1997), and would lead to exclusion from analysis. 5.9 Materials 5.9.1 Study drugs Bacopa monnieri was given in the form of a tablet and was derived from an alcoholic extract of the herb (herb to extract ratio, 20:1). The extract was standardised to contain total Bacosides 40.0 – 50.0%, along with a number of chemical constituents viz., Bacoside A3 (>2.7%), Bacopaside II (>3.6%), Jujubogenin isomer of Bacopa saponin C (>4.5%), Bacopa saponin C (>3.0%), Bacopaside I (>4.5%), Apigenin (0.1 – 0.5%) and Luteolin (0.1 – 0.8%). The extract was supplied by Natural Remedies Pvt. Ltd. in Bangalore, India, following a proprietary manufacturing method. The name of this standardised extract is Bacomind™, code number NRBME40, tableted by Tabco Pty Ltd and supplied by Herbs of Gold Pty Ltd, Australia. Each 300mg of Bacomind™ in a tablet contains 6000mg equivalent of the dried herb. The dosage instructed was 300mg of Bacomind™ in one tablet daily, after a meal. Film-coated placebo tablets were identical in size, colour and shape to that of the Bacopa tablets. Specification sheets for Bacomind™ and placebo tablets can be found in Appendix VI. The dosage of Bacopa was based on the 37 manufacturers’ recommendation and was the same as that used in previous clinical trials by Stough et al. (2001) and Roodenrys et al. (2002). 5.10 Instruments 5.10.1 Screening instruments The following instruments were utilised in the pre-trial screening session, to measure selection criteria. 5.10.1.1 The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) This is a widely used test to screen for cognitive dysfunction. This test was developed by Folstein et al. in 1975 to determine degrees of cognitive impairment. There are 12 items with a total possible score of 30, with higher scores reflecting better cognitive function. Scores above 21 reflect normal function and mild impairment, scores ranging from 10-20 indicate moderate impairment, and severe impairment is indicated by scores of 9 or less. A copy of the MMSE form used can be found in the Clinical Report Form in Appendix IV. 5.10.1.2 The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D) A 21-item version of the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (adapted from Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979 by GlaxoWellcome) was utilised from the following website: http://healthnet.umassmed.edu/mhealth/HAMD.pdf. A copy of this form can be found in the Clinical Report Form in Appendix IV. The possible scores on this scale range from 0-66, with higher scores reflecting worse depression. Scores above 13 indicate depression, and below 13 imply absence of depression. 5.10.1.3 Modified Hachinski Ischemic Score (HIS) The Hachinski Ischaemic Score was developed in 1975 to enable clinicians to distinguish between multi-infarct aetiology and degenerative aetiology in patients presenting with dementia (Hachinski et al., 1975). The modified version of the scale, utilised in the current study was introduced by Rosen et al. in 1980. This scale rates symptoms of ischaemic cerebrovascular events. There are eight items, with a total possible score of 12-points. A score above 3 indicates multi-infarct dementia. A copy of the scale can be found in the Clinical Report Form (Appendix IV). In the current study 38 the Modified Hachinski Ischaemic scale was used to screen for cognitive deficits related to ischaemia and stroke. 5.10.2 Primary outcome measures The following instruments were used to give both objective and subjective measurements of memory at baseline and end-of-trial clinical sessions. 5.10.2.1 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Rey, 1964), is a word list learning test that is a validated and widely used test for assessing memory in both clinical practice and research (Lezak et al., 2004:421). A copy of the AVLT can be found in Appendix VII. Its usage has been extensively reported in the literature (Schmidt, 1996). Numerous publications have established norms for the AVLT (for example Ivnik et al., 1990), and Australian norms for the test have also been established (Forrester &Geffen, 1991; Geffen et al., 1990; Rickert & Senior, 1998). The AVLT assesses various aspects of memory, as outlined below. In the AVLT the same 15-word list (List A) is read to the participant for five repetitions (AVLT a1-5). After each repetition, the subject recalls as many words as possible from the list. During a sixth trial (AVLT b), words are presented from a different fifteen word list, an interference list, (List B), which must be recalled by the subject, followed immediately by a sixth recall of the original list, List A (AVLT a6). A seventh recall of List A (AVLT a7) occurs after a 20-minute interval. The last part of the test consists of a recognition task (AVLT recognition) wherein a list of fifty words is read to the subject who must identify the 15-words from List A embedded amongst 35 other words (including semantically and phonetically similar words as well as the 15-words from List B). Specific instructions for the administration of the AVLT are given in Lezak et al. (2004: 421-426) and these were adhered to in the trial. Alternate word lists for the AVLT were used at the end-of-trial assessment to avoid learning effects. These lists were developed by Jones-Gotman, Szilkas & Majdan (cited in Lezak et al., 2004: 423). The alternate AVLT word lists used at the end-of-trial assessment can be viewed in Appendix VIII. 39 From the raw scores obtained on the AVLT a number of measures of memory function were obtained as follows (Lezak et al., 2004:422-426): • AVLT trial a1: immediate recall • AVLT trial a6: recall post intrusion • AVLT trial a7: delayed recall (assesses long term retention) • AVLT trials a1-a5, summed scores: total learning • AVLT trial a5 minus trial a6: retroactive interference • AVLT trial a1 minus trial b: proactive interference • AVLT recognition hit rate (correctly identified list A words) • AVLT recognition false positives (words incorrectly identified as list A words) • AVLT true recognition rate (recognition hit rate minus false positives) • AVLT trial a6 minus trial a7: forgetting rate 5.10.2.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Rey, 1941; Osterrieth, 1944; Corwin & Bylsma, 1993) is used to assess visuospatial ability and visual memory (Spreen & Strauss, 1991:157). A complicated geometrical figure (the Rey-Osterrieth figure) (Appendix IX), is presented to the subject, who is asked to copy it initially and then reproduce it from memory twice- firstly 3-minutes, and then 30-minutes, later. Scoring involves giving marks for both placement and accuracy of 18 different components of the drawing. Total possible score equals 36 for each trial. A copy of the CFT marking sheet is in Appendix X. 5.10.2.3 Trail Making Test (TMT) The Trail Making Test (Reitan, 1958), is a test which measures scanning and visuomotor tracking abilities, and involves cognitive processing (incorporating memory) as well as psychomotor speed (Lezak et al., 2004: 371). There are two separate parts of the test- part A and part B (Appendix XI). In part A (Trails A), the subject must draw a line connecting circles containing consecutive numbers (from number 1 to number 25). In part B (Trails B), the subject again draws a line between circles, though now alternating between consecutive numbers and letters, i.e. from 1 to A to 2 to B and so on up until the number 13 and the letter L. The subject performs the task as fast as he/she can, 40 while their pen does not lose contact with the paper. The scores obtained are the times taken (in seconds) to complete the two tasks. 5.10.2.4 Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q) The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (Crook et al., 1992) was designed as a brief screening tool to measure experiential memory deterioration with aging, thereby quantifying subjective memory complaints. Participants answer six questions comparing current everyday memory to that of earlier life. The total score is the sum of the six questions, scored on a 5-point Likert scale, with options ranging from ‘much better now’ to ‘much worse now’. The possible score range is 7-35, with scores over 25 indicating subjective memory impairment. This Memory Complaint Questionnaire can be found in Appendix XII. 5.11 Statistical analysis All data in this study were analysed using the computer software package- Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS version 11.5 for Windows). For the primary efficacy analysis, neuropsychological test scores and subjective memory complaints scores were analysed using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) employing group (Bacopa and placebo) and time (baseline and week 12) as between and within subject factors. To test the successfulness of randomisation, potential difference between groups (Bacopa and placebo) on all variables at baseline was analysed using independent samples t-test for continuous variables and chi-square test for categorical variables. To determine whether gender, age, education and marital status affected memory in both groups, baseline test scores were analysed by independent t-tests for age and education, and by Pearson’s product-moment correlations for gender and marital status. Side-effects as reported either verbally or in the participants’ record booklets, were analysed using an independent samples t-test for significant differences between active and placebo groups. 41 In summary, this chapter has described the research carried out, including participants and procedures, selection criteria, study design, study drugs and the instruments used to measure outcomes. Finally, the statistical analysis techniques used in the study were described. Chapter 6 will report on the results obtained from this analysis. 42 6. RESULTS 6.1 Introduction Following on from the previous chapter which gave an in-depth description of the design of the research, this chapter will report the research results. Firstly, analysis of the flow of participants through the clinical trial will be presented. Next, the clinical characteristics and baseline measurements will be reported with analysis of baseline group differences and the effects of gender, marital status, age and education on performance. The analysis of study compliance, side-effects and withdrawals will be elucidated and, lastly, the results obtained from the primary outcomes analysis will be presented. 6.2 Participants 136 people volunteered for participation in the trial. Of these, 103 met the study selection criteria and 98 commenced the trial. Of those commencing, 52 (53.1%) were females and 46 (46.9%) were males. The average age of participants was 65-years (range 55-86, SD 7.53) and they had an average of 13-years of education (range 5-22, SD 4.01). 26 participants were single and 72 were married or defacto. 81 participants completed the study and 17 withdrew. Of the completers, there were 42 (51.9%) females and 39 (48.1%) males, with an average age of 65.4-years (range 55-86, SD 7.67) and an average of 13-years of education (range 5-22, SD 4.08). 18 participants were single and 63 were married or defacto. 6.2.1 Exclusions Of the 136 applicants, a total of 33 people were initially excluded from the trial for the following reasons (for some respondents more than one reason applied): • Psychiatric illness: depression (n=1) • Neurological disease: Parkinson’s disease (n=2), essential tremor (n=1), recent loss of consciousness (n=1), chronic back pain (n=1) • Cardiovascular disease: ischaemic heart disease (n=4), atrial fibrillation (n=2), valvular heart disease (n=1), stroke (n=1) 43 • Other systemic illness: thyroid disease (n=1), systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE) (n=1) • Medication usage: antidepressants (n=10), anxiolytic (n=1), opiate use (n=1), sleep medication (n=1) • Herbal medicine usage (n=2) • Mini Mental Status Examination score under 24 point cut-off (n=2) • Under 55 years of age (n=3) • Exclusion of partner (n=1) • Unwilling to be on placebo (n=1) • Work commitments that would effect compliance (n=1) 6.2.2 Randomisation Following exclusion, participants were randomised into groups using the protocol previously described in section 5.7.1. To assess whether equal distribution into groups was achieved by randomisation, the means were calculated and group differences analysed by independent t-test for the following continuous variables recorded at baseline: age, education, MMSE scores, Hamilton Depression Scale scores, neuropsychological test scores and the memory complaint questionnaire scores. The categorical variables of gender and marital status were analysed for group differences using the chi-squared test. The clinical characteristics and baseline neuropsychological test scores of those who commenced the trial are summarised in Table 6.1. As indicated in this table, there were significant differences found between the active and placebo groups mean scores on the Complex Figure Test (CFT) in both the 3-minute and the 30-minute delayed recall tasks. The Bacopa group performed better than the placebo group on these tasks at baseline. However, no interactions between the active and placebo groups on gender, age group and education level were found. The mean scores on the CFT tasks were fairly low and the means varied between groups by only 4 marks out of a total possible score of 36 marks. Following consultation with the university’s professional statistician it was considered that this is not a large difference and since the scores were normally distributed it is likely that the differences found on these tasks was due to chance. On all other neuropsychological measures and demographic characteristics, there were no 44 significant differences found between groups. With these findings and minor limitations it seems randomisation was generally successful. Table 6.1: Dependent variables at baseline: clinical characteristics and test scores at point of randomisation (n=98), with analysis of group differences. Total Sample Number of Subjects Bacopa Group Placebo Group p 98 49 49 - 52(53%)/46(47%) 24(49%)/25(51%) 28(57%)/21(43%) 0.54 65+/-7.53 65.41+/-6.87 65.39+/-8.20 0.989 55-86 55-77 55-86 - 13+/-4.01 13.37+/-3.97 12.82+/-4.07 0.5 5-22 5-20 6-22 - 72(73%)/26(27%) 35(71%)/14(29%) 37(75%)/12(25%) 0.81 28.18+/-1.56 28.05+/-1.63 28.30+/-1.50 0.423 Hamilton Depression Scale 3.28+/-2.89 3.07+/-2.73 3.48+/-3.05 0.488 AVLT a1 5.91+/- 1.62 5.94+/-1.63 5.88+/-1.62 0.853 AVLT a2 7.63+/-2.25 7.8+/-2.48 7.47+/-2.02 0.477 AVLT a3 8.68+/-2.30 8.73+/-2.29 8.63+/-2.32 0.828 AVLT a4 9.36+/-2.08 9.33+/-2.13 9.39+/-2.06 0.885 AVLT a5 10.17+/-2.48 10.37+/-2.44 9.98+/-2.53 0.443 AVLT b (interference list) 4.28+/-1.90 4.33+/-2.01 4.22+/-1.81 0.793 AVLT a6 7.85+/-2.82 8+/-2.83 7.69+/-2.83 0.594 AVLT a7 (20minute recall) 7.58+/-2.79 7.86+/-2.52 7.31+/-3.05 0.332 AVLT recognition hit rate 12.45+/-2.39 12.71+/-2.09 12.18+/-2.65 0.274 AVLT false positive rate 3.64+/-3.20 3.43+/-3.27 3.86+/-3.15 0.511 Gender (female/male) Age (years) -range Education (years) -range Marital Status (partner/single) MMSE AVLT true recognition rate 8.81+/-3.84 9.29+/-3.71 8.33+/-3.94 0.218 AVLT total learning (Σ1-5) 41.67+/-8.91 42.16+/-9.42 41.18+/-8.44 0.589 AVLT retroactive interference 2.33+/-1.97 2.37+/-1.99 2.29+/-1.96 0.839 AVLT proactive interference 1.63+/-1.98 1.61+/-1.95 1.65+/-2.03 0.92 .27+/-1.62 0.14+/-1.70 0.39+/-1.53 0.458 CFT copy 34.24+/-2.85 34.45+/-2.21 34.03+/-3.38 0.471 CFT 3min 16.45+/-6.41 18.24+/-6.43 14.65+/-5.93 0.005** CFT 30min 16.58+/-6.37 18.26+/-5.92 14.89+/-6.42 0.008** AVLT forgetting rate MAC-Q 26.07+/-4.56 25.84+/-3.78 26.31+/-5.25 0.613 Trail Making Test A 36.05+/-9.48 34.27+/-7.95 37.84+/-10.59 0.062 Trail Making Test B 87.07+/-33.54 88.02+/-31.61 86.12+/-35.66 0.781 Results are mean+/-SD unless otherwise specified. Chi-square test for gender and marital status. Independent t-test for age, education and neuropsychological tasks. p = two-tailed significance, for differences between groups. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, MAC-Q = Memory Complaint Questionnaire. **p<.01 45 6.2.3 Effects of gender, marital status, age and education on baseline measures The effects of the independent variables on baseline neuropsychological and subjective memory complaints scores were analysed using independent samples t- test for the effects of gender and marital status and using Pearson’s product moment correlation for the effects of age and education. Results are outlined below. 6.2.3.1 Gender An independent samples t-test was applied to compare all the baseline memory test scores for males and females. Females performed significantly better than males in some of the AVLT tasks as shown in Table 6.2 below. Table 6.2: Significant effects of gender on task performance at baseline (n=98) TASK t Sig. AVLT 2 2.012 .047 AVLT 3 3.123 .002 AVLT 4 2.323 .022 AVLT 5 3.429 .001 AVLT 7 2.349 .003 AVLT total 1-5 3.000 .003 t=independent samples t-test value for differences between females and males Sig. = two–tailed significance 6.2.3.2 Marital status Using an independent samples t-test, there were no significant differences found for performance of married/defacto participants compared to single/widowed participants on baseline test scores. 6.2.3.3 Age The relationship between age and all the baseline neuropsychological test scores was investigated using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient. Advancing age led to reductions in performance as evidenced by the significant correlations between age and various dependent variables as shown in Table 6.3 below. 46 Table 6.3: Significant effects of age on task performance at baseline (n=98) TASK r Sig. Strength & direction of correlation AVLT 2 -.228 .024 small negative AVLT 3 -.234 .021 small negative AVLT 4 -.201 .047 small negative AVLT 5 -.239 .018 small negative AVLT b -.394 000 medium negative AVLT 6 -.210 .038 small negative AVLT 7 -.223 .027 small negative AVLT total 1-5 -.237 .019 small negative AVLT proactive* .312 .002 medium positive CFT a (3min) -.300 .003 medium negative CFT b (30min) -.247 .014 small negative TMT A* .279 .005 small positive TMT B* .392 .000 medium positive r= Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient; Sig. = two–tailed significance *on these tasks higher scores = worse performance 6.2.3.4 Education The relationship between education and all baseline test scores was investigated using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient. Higher levels of education significantly improved test scores as evidenced by significant correlations between education and the following dependent variables as shown in Table 6.4 below. 47 Table 6.4: Correlation of length of education and task performance at baseline (n=98) TASK r Sig. Strength & direction of correlation AVLT 1 .223 .028 small positive AVLT 2 .307 .002 medium positive AVLT 3 .284 .005 small positive AVLT 5 .265 .008 small positive AVLT b .238 .019 small positive AVLT 6 .331 .001 medium positive AVLT 7 .241 .017 small positive AVLT total 1-5 .295 .003 small positive AVLT falspos* -.243 .016 small negative AVLT truerec .239 .018 small positive CFT copy .325 .001 medium positive CFT 3min .411 .000 medium positive CFT 30min .383 .000 medium positive TMT B* -.342 .001 medium negative *on these tasks higher scores = worse performance r= Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient Sig. = two–tailed significance 6.2.4 Group distribution by age and gender categories. The age of participants was categorised into three groups: 55-65years, 66-75years and 76-86years. Education was categorised into three levels: primary (5-7years), secondary (8-13years) and tertiary (14-22years). The distribution of age groups and educational levels at the point of randomisation are given in Figures 6.1 and 6.2. 48 30 Number of cases 20 Group 10 Bacopa (n=49) Placebo 0 (n=49) 55-65 66-75 76-86 Age group Figure 6.1: Distribution of age groups at point of randomisation (n=98) 30 25 20 Number of cases 15 Group 10 Bacopa (n=49) 5 Placebo 0 (n=49) primary secondary tertiary Educational level attained Figure 6.2: Distribution of educational levels at point of randomisation (n=98) 49 6.2.5 Compliance All participants who completed the trial complied with medication use. This was assessed by a count of tablets remaining in containers returned at the end of the trial. A discrepancy from the study regimen of 20% or greater constituted as non-compliance. Additionally, compliance was monitored by checking the completed medication record booklet handed over by participants (see Appendix I) and also by verbal questioning at the end point clinic session. In a small number of cases (n=11) participants forgot to return their tablets, in these cases only the written record and verbal questioning were used to assess medication compliance. 6.2.6 Study withdrawals Of the 98 participants who commenced the study, 81 participants completed the trial. A total of 17 (10 females and 7 males) withdrew after the baseline testing session, 13 from the Bacopa group and 4 from the placebo group. The reasons for withdrawal from the trial for the Bacopa and placebo groups as shown in Table 6.5 includes side-effects n= 9 for Bacopa group and n= 2 for placebo group. Table 6.5: Reasons for withdrawal from trial Reason for withdrawal Total Bacopa Placebo Group Group Side-effects 11 9 2 Hospitalisation for elective surgery 1 0 1 Concurrent illness 2 1 1 Accidental injury 1 1 0 Lost to follow-up 2 2 0 A flow chart which illustrates the progression of participants through the phases of the clinical trial is presented in Figure 6.3 (below). This flow chart is based on a model designed by Moher et al. (2001). 50 Assessed for eligibility (n=136) Excluded (n=33) -selection criteria not met (n=30) -declined consent (n=1) -other reasons (n=2) Randomised (n=103) Allocated to receive Bacopa (n=51) Allocated to receive Placebo (n=52) Received intervention (n=49) Received intervention (n=49) Did not receive intervention: Did not receive intervention: -work commitments (n=1) -work commitments (n=1) -death in family (n=1) -travel (n=1) -lost to follow up (n=1) Followed up at 12-week visit Followed up at 12-week visit (n=36) (n=45) Withdrawn after baseline (n=13): Withdrawn after baseline (n=4): -side effects (n=9) -side effects (n=2) -lost to follow up (n=2) -elective surgery (n=1) -concurrent illness (n=1) -concurrent illness (n=1) -accident (n=1) Analysed (n=36) Analysed (n=45) Figure 6.3: Flow-chart depicting participant progression through phases of the clinical trial. 51 6.2.7 Side-effects Participants recorded any possible side-effects in the record booklet (Appendix I), and were also instructed to inform the researcher immediately in the event of any suspected reaction occurring. There were a total of 52 possible side-effects reported during the trial, 41 of these occurred in the Bacopa group, and 11 occurred in the placebo group (note that more than one event could be reported by participants.) Table 6.6 documents the total number of side-effects reported. Table 6.6: Total side-effects reported during study Symptoms reported* Bacopa Placebo Group Group Increased stool frequency 15 1 GIT cramps 8 0 Nausea 9 1 Reflux 0 2 Flatulence 1 0 Bloating 1 2 Decreased appetite 1 0 Constipation 0 1 Headache 1 1 Hypertension 0 1 Insomnia 1 0 Vivid dreams 2 0 Increased sense of wellbeing 2 2 *Participants could report more than one symptom; includes study withdrawals 51% (25/49) of participants in the Bacopa group reported at least one side-effect compared with 18% (9/49) in the placebo group. The severity of symptoms reported was mild to moderate and transient, with only one serious event reported- that of hypertension (in the placebo group). However, symptoms were severe enough to warrant withdrawal from the study in 18% (9/49) of the Bacopa group and 4% (2/49) of the placebo group. All side-effect related withdrawals in the Bacopa group were due to GIT symptoms. 52 Overall, GIT symptoms occurred predominantly in the Bacopa group, with 45% (22/49) of subjects reporting at least one GIT symptom as compared to the placebo group, in which 10% (5/49) of subjects reported at least one GIT symptom. The predominant GIT symptoms reported were increased stool frequency, abdominal cramps and nausea. These were transient and ceased on discontinuation of tablets. Other bodily systems in which side-effects were reported were the nervous system (headache, insomnia, vivid dreams, enhanced sense of wellbeing), and the cardiovascular system (hypertension). Due to unequal drop-out rates, only 73% (36/49) of the Bacopa group completed the study, compared to 92% (45/49) of the placebo group. An independent samples t-test was applied to test for significant differences in side effects experienced between treatment groups. The following side-effects occurred significantly more often in the Bacopa group compared to placebo (at 95% confidence interval): increased stool frequency (t=4.106, p=.000), nausea (t=2.744, p=.007), abdominal cramps (t=3.060, p=.003), and total GIT side-effects (t=4.128, p=.000). 53 6.3 Primary outcome measurements The means and standard deviations for all dependent variables by group and testing session are presented in Table 6.7 below Table 6.7: Mean (and SD) for all tasks by group and testing session Bacopa Group Task Placebo Group Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test (n=36) (n=36) (n=45) (n=45) AVLTa1 5.75 (1.70) 5.64 (1.62) 5.72 (1.52) 5.22 (1.56) AVLTa2 7.47 (2.39) 7.92 (1.98) 7.51 (2.05) 7.40 (2.18) AVLTa3 8.64 (2.22) 9.28 (2.17) 8.69 (2.29) 8.49 (2.38) AVLTa4 9.22 (2.11) 10.69 (1.90) 9.33 (2.07) 8.96 (2.18) AVLTa5 10.33 (2.51) 11.06 (2.18) 9.96 (2.50) 9.38 (2.36) AVLTb 4.33 (1.99) 4.64 (1.78) 4.29 (1.83) 4.76 (1.58) AVLTa6 7.94 (3.07) 9.83 (2.04) 7.73 (2.91) 7.16 (2.04) AVLTa7 7.94 (2.75) 9.58 (2.17) 7.36 (3.10) 6.84 (2.66) AVLTrec 12.61 (2.22) 13.86 (1.15) 12.38 (2.52) 13.13 (2.12) AVLTfalspos 3.47 (3.53) 1.92 (2.34) 4.02 (3.18) 3.49 (3.61) AVLTtrue rec 9.14 (3.93) 11.94 (2.62) 8.36 (3.91) 9.64 (4.11) AVLTtotal1-5 41.42 (9.25) 44.31 (8.52) 41.20 (8.44) 39.42 (9.22) AVLTretroactive 2.39 (2.10) 1.22 (1.57) 2.22 (2.01) 2.22 (1.61) AVLTproactive 1.42 (1.90) 1.00 (1.93) 1.60 (2.07) 0.47 (1.53) AVLTforget 0.00 (1.79) 0.25 (1.27) 0.38 (1.60) 0.31 (1.55) CFTcopy 34.43 (2.33) 32.80 (4.99) 33.90 (3.50) 33.54 (2.77) CFTa 18.08 (5.84) 19.57 (5.85) 14.14 (5.73) 17.77 (6.93) CFTb 18.40 (5.30) 20.46 (6.10) 14.49 (6.41) 18.11 (6.32) MAC-Q 25.89 (3.52) 22.39 (5.35) 26.38 (5.37) 24.69 (4.57) Trail Making A 33.56 (7.58) 30.72 (8.98) 37.87 (10.74) 35.58 (14.70) Trail Making B 89.89 (32.25) 71.81 (27.47) 86.29 (36.92) 75.47 (22.32) AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, AVLTa1-a7 = repetitions of word list A (possible range 015), AVLT b = interference word list B (possible range 0-15), AVLT rec = recognition list hit rate (possible range 0-15) , AVLT falspos = recognition list false positives (possible range 0-35), AVLT true rec = true recognition (recognition list hit rate minus false positives, possible range -35 to +15) , AVLT total 1-5 = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, range 0-75), AVLT retroactive = retroactive interference score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance, ), AVLT proactive = proactive interference score (trial a1 minus trial b, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance), AVLT forget = forgetting rate (trial a6 minus trial a7, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance); CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (possible range 036 on all tasks), CFTcopy=copy task, CFTa=3-minute recall task, CFTb=30-minute recall task; MAC-Q = Memory Complaint Questionnaire (possible range 7-35, lower scores = better performance); Trail Making Test A & B- scores=time taken to complete task (in seconds). 54 Initially, the normality of the distribution of scores for each of the continuous variables was tested and it was found that the assumptions of normality were met. A General Linear Model (GLM) was run to test for significant differences between the Bacopa and placebo groups on all dependent variables (memory complaint questionnaire and neuropsychological test scores) from baseline to end-of-trial. A Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used with time (baseline and end point scores) as the within subjects factor, and treatment group (Bacopa and placebo) as the between subjects factor. Type 1 sum of squares was employed. At the 0.05 probability level, Bacopa significantly improved memory function as measured by performance on the following AVLT tasks: trial a4, trial a5, trial a6 (post distraction trial), trial a7 (delayed recall trial), total learning (Σ trials a1-a5), and retroactive interference index. Table 6.8 summarises these results. Figures 6.4 to 6.9 inclusive illustrate the treatment effects of Bacopa compared to placebo for each of these outcome measures. Improved performance was also noted on the CFT and the TMT - however on these two tests there were no significant effects for Bacopa compared to placebo- both groups improved. Likewise, on the subjective memory complaint questionnaire, both placebo and Bacopa groups had improved scores. For a summary of the Repeated Measures Analyses results for all tasks refer to Appendix XIII. Table 6.8: Significant group differences on primary outcome measures: results of repeated measures analysis of variance Task F df Error Sig. df AVLT trial a4 13.204 1 79 .000 AVLT trial a5 6.097 1 79 .016 AVLT trial a6 (recall post-intrusion) 18.830 1 79 .000 AVLT trial a7 (delayed recall) 12.021 1 79 .001 AVLT trial Total Learning (Σ trials 1-5) 6.761 1 79 .011 AVLT retroactive interference index 4.020 1 79 .048 F= Fisher value for significance of group contrasts. df=degrees of freedom for the two treatment groups, error df=degrees of freedom for error. Sig.=one-tailed significance. AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, AVLTa4-a7 = repetitions of word list A recall task (possible range 0-15), AVLT Total Learning = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, possible range 0-75), AVLT retroactive = retroactive interference score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance). 55 Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a4 11.0 Estimated Marginal Means 10.5 10.0 Group 9.5 Bacopa (n=36) 9.0 Placebo (n=45) 8.5 Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.4: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a4 Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a5 11.5 Estimated Marginal Means 11.0 10.5 Group 10.0 Bacopa (n=36) 9.5 Placebo (n=45) 9.0 Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.5: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a5 56 Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a6 (post distraction trial) 10.0 Estimated Marginal Means 9.5 9.0 8.5 Group 8.0 Bacopa 7.5 (n=36) 7.0 Placebo (n=45) 6.5 Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.6: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a6 Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a7 (delayed recall trial) 10.0 Estimated Marginal Means 9.5 9.0 8.5 Group 8.0 Bacopa 7.5 (n=36) 7.0 Placebo (n=45) 6.5 Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.7: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a7 57 Estimated Marginal Means of Total Learning (sum of scores of AVLT trials 1-5) 45 Estimated Marginal Means 44 43 Group 42 Bacopa 41 (n=36) 40 Placebo (n=45) 39 Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.8: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT total learning index (sum of trials a1-a5) Estimated Marginal Means, low scores=best performance Estimated Marginal Means of retroactive interference index 2.6 2.4 2.2 2.0 Group 1.8 1.6 Bacopa 1.4 (n=36) 1.2 Placebo 1.0 (n=45) Baseline End-of-trial TIME Figure 6.9: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT retroactive interference index (lower scores = better performance) 58 To conclude, this chapter has outlined the results obtained from the statistical analysis of the study data. The flow of participants through the trial was documented, the effects of gender, marital status, education and age on baseline scores were delineated and the distribution of age and education levels was presented. Study compliance, withdrawals and side-effects were documented and, finally, the results of the primary outcome measurements analyses were reported. The next chapter will examine these results in relation to the previous literature and explore the implications of the findings obtained. 59 7. DISCUSSION 7.1 Introduction This study was designed to test firstly, whether previously reported findings of the beneficial effects of Bacopa monnieri on human memory (Stough et al., 2001; Roodenrys et al., 2002), were replicable in the elderly population, and secondly, whether Bacopa would produce side-effects. Previously validated neuropsychological tests were used to objectively measure verbal and visual memory, and a memory complaint questionnaire was used to obtain a subjective measurement of memory function. Analysis of the data revealed a significant effect of Bacopa on memory performance in Australians over the age of 55 years. This effect was objectively measured by performance outcomes on the test of auditory verbal learning- the AVLT. Performances on tests measuring visuospatial memory- the CFT and the TMT- improved across the total sample population, however the differences found between active and placebo groups on these tests did not reach significance. Likewise, the whole study population demonstrated subjective memory improvement as measured by the Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q), but the Bacopa group not significantly more so than placebo. Additionally, Bacopa use was significantly associated with gastrointestinal (GIT) sideeffects of mild to moderate degrees of intensity in almost half of the participants who were on it. 7.2 Effects of age, gender and education on baseline measurements As noted in the results section significant correlations were found between baseline neuropsychological test scores and gender, age and education level. Higher education levels improved performance on all three tests, whereas advancing age decreased performance on all three tests and women performed better than men on AVLT tasks (but not CFT or TMT). These findings concur with the bulk of research literature. 60 For example, the AVLT scores significantly worsen with age as demonstrated in numerous correlational and normative studies (Schmidt, 1996:31). Ivnik et al. (1990) comment that effects of ageing on AVLT scores is accentuated in older rather than younger groups. Age effects on the CFT recall tasks also have been consistently documented (Lezak et al., 2004:459), and poorer scores with ageing have also been demonstrated on the TMT (ibid: 373). Education has the opposite effect on scores: the more educated, the better the performance on the AVLT (Schmidt, 1996:32) as well as the CFT (Lezak et al., 2004:459) and the TMT in which education effects are especially evident in Part B rather than Part A (ibid: 373). For example, Geffen et al., who published Australian norms for the AVLT which included older age groups, showed enhanced performance on trials 2-5 with higher education levels (Geffen et al., 1990). Other Australian norms for the AVLT published by Rickert and Senior also reported significant effects for age and education level (1998). However, the effect of education on AVLT performance is generally weaker and less consistent than that of age (Schmidt, 1996:32). A gender effect has been found but has not been consistently evident for the AVLT (Schmidt, 1996:32), though where it has been reported the same pattern emerges as noted in the current study, i.e. women outperform men. For example, in Geffen et al.’s Australian study, women consistently performed significantly better than men on the AVLT (1990). Conversely, gender differences on the CFT tend to favour males, though inconsistently (Lezak et al., 2004:459), and men have been shown to perform better on the TMT part B, particularly in older age groups (Lezak et al., 2004:373). Gender effects on the CFT and TMT were not noted in the current trial. 7.3 Primary outcome measures 7.3.1 The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) Compared to placebo, Bacopa significantly improved performance on the AVLT in the following tasks: trials a4 (p=.000), a5 (p=.016), a6 (p=.000), a7 (p=.001), sum of trials 15 (i.e. the total learning score) (p=.011), and retroactive interference (p=.048). Performance on these trials demonstrated that Bacopa had a significant effect on memory acquisition and retention. Participants on Bacopa showed a significant 61 improvement in their capacity for verbal learning with repetition, unlike those on placebo whose performance either plateaued or marginally decreased on these measures. Improvement in memory acquisition was demonstrated by an increasing amount of words recalled over the 5 learning trials that were retained at the delayed recall trial, a7 (Lezak et al., 2004:428). Performance on the delayed recall trial also demonstrates improved memory retention. Furthermore, retention of the learned material was less affected by the introduction of an interference word list (B), as evidenced by significantly improved retroactive interference scores in the Bacopa group. Attention span is considered to be a significant element contributing to performance on the first AVLT word list recall trial al., compared to the subsequent learning trials (Macartney-Filgate and Vriezen, 1988). In the current study, the improvement in memory acquisition contrasts with attention span performance, as measured by the first trial, a1, where no effect for Bacopa was found. Therefore, these findings imply that Bacopa improves memory acquisition and retention as opposed to immediate attention span. The finding of improved memory retention concurs with the findings of Roodenrys et al. (2002) who also found a significant effect of Bacopa on retention of new information. Unlike Roodenrys et al., who suggest that Bacopa improves retention by decreasing forgetting rate rather than by improving learning rate, however, the current study demonstrated an improved learning rate and no effect on forgetting rate. It is notable that Roodenrys et al.’s measurement of retention was based on performance on a word pair test which involved three learning trials and a fourth delayed recall trial of six unrelated word pairs. It was only in the delayed recall trial that an effect was found. On the test used in the current study, no significant enhancement of learning occurred in the first three learning trials either, it was not until the fourth, fifth, sixth and seventh trials that significant effects were observed. Thus, it may be that insufficient learning trials were applied in the Roodenrys et al. study to demonstrate that learning had occurred, rather than that a learning effect did not occur as they had surmised. The current study also concurs with the findings of Stough and colleagues (2001), who reported improvements on various measures from the AVLT, in that Bacopa 62 significantly improved learning rate (acquisition) and memory consolidation (assessed by decreased proactive interference and decreased forgetting rate). Memory consolidation refers to the complex molecular processes by which information is moved from short term memory into long term memory, i.e. how memories become ‘solid’ (Carlson, 2002:383). The current study concurs with Stough et al.’s finding of improved learning and consolidation, however differs in that improvement in memory consolidation was not associated with improvements in proactive interference or forgetting rate for which no effects were found. Rather, the current study suggests that the observed enhancement of memory consolidation may be related to reduced retroactive interference (p=0.048), as explained below. Interference effects on memory are categorised into two types- proactive and retroactive (Lezak et al., 2004:428). Proactive interference occurs when earlier learning disrupts later learning, for example- learning words from an original word list interferes with learning words from a second list. Retroactive interference occurs when later learning disrupts earlier learning, for example- learning words from a second list interferes with recall from an original word list. As noted above, the capacity to withstand retroactive interference was significantly improved by Bacopa in this study. As aforementioned, this contrasts with the findings of Stough et al. (2001) who found an improvement in proactive interference, but not retroactive interference. As well as concurring with human trials, this study’s finding of improved memory acquisition and consolidation concurs with earlier animal studies. For example, Singh & Dhawan (1997) demonstrated improved acquisition, consolidation and retention of three newly learnt behaviours in rats. Additionally, it is interesting to note that the improvement in auditory-verbal learning demonstrated by the current study supports the reputed ancient usage of the herb for helping Vedic scholars memorise lengthy scriptural hymns (Hackman, 1998). Such memorisation exemplifies an activity that is a function of auditory-verbal memory and learning. The results obtained in the AVLT in this study demonstrate that Bacopa improves verbal memory in non-demented older persons. It is verbal memory that is impaired early in AD, often years prior to diagnosis, with deficits in acquisition and retention of new information evident from the onset of the disease (Lezak et al., 2004:214). 63 Learning (acquisition) and delayed recall are parts of memory that are sensitive to ageing effects as well as to AD (Petersen, 1992). Tests that measure delayed recall are the most sensitive tests to detect the effects on memory of both normal ageing and AD (Albert, 2002). Hence, benefits detected in delayed recall and acquisition in the current study of healthy older people has implications that Bacopa may hold benefits for AD sufferers as well. Future research to establish the efficacy of Bacopa for cognitive improvement in early AD patients would thus be warranted. The current trial found that beneficial effects of Bacopa were apparent after 12-weeks duration. This, in concurrence with other human trials (Stough et al., 2001; Roodenrys et al., 2002; Negi et al., 2000; Sharma et al., 1987), supports the notion that longer term administration of Bacopa is required for its effects to become apparent. Acute administration (2 hours) did not produce measurable cognitive effects in one study (Nathan et al., 2001), and in Stough et al.’s study effects were found only at the 12week follow up and not at the 5-week follow-up. Likewise, in Negi et al.’s (2000) study, significant improvement in memory tasks occurred at 12-weeks, but not at 4 or 8-weeks of Bacopa administration. An extra follow-up visit in the current study, at the 6-week mark, would have helped to verify the benefits of chronic over acute administration; however availability of resources was a constraining factor. 7.3.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) and Trail Making Test (TMT): While the current study demonstrated that Bacopa facilitates memory acquisition and retention on an auditory-verbal learning test (the AVLT), the effects were not significantly greater than placebo on tests assessing visuo-spatial memory, i.e. the CFT and the TMT. In the CFT in both the 3-minute recall and 30-minute recall tasks, and in TMT parts A and B, both placebo and Bacopa groups performed better at the 12-week end-of-trial session than at baseline, with no significant differences between the groups. Better performance for the whole sample on the CFT and TMT may reflect practice effects, as the same CFT figure and the same TMT task were used at both baseline and end-of-trial testing sessions, (unlike the AVLT test in which an equivalent alternate word list was used at the follow-up visit). Parallel test figures are available for the CFT 64 (Lezak et al., 2004:537-40), whereas in the TMT a parallel form is not feasible as it comprises alphabet and number joining tasks. This must be considered a limitation of the current study as practice effects may constitute a threat to internal validity, although the inclusion of a control group attenuated this threat to some degree (Polgar & Thomas, 1995:56-61). As discussed in the results section, there was also an inexplicable difference between the two treatment groups means at baseline on the CFT only. The group means were normally distributed and the difference was not accountable for by effects of gender, age, or education. The reason for the difference remains unaccounted for and could have indicated a difference in a variable not measured, for example eyesight or motor skills, (as visual-motor but not audio-verbal tasks showed group difference). Retrospectively, assessment of visual and auditory acuity would have been pertinent data to collect in the pre-trial screening session. 7.3.3 The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q) The MAC-Q scores showed subjective improvement in memory in both active and placebo groups, however, the scores did not show any difference between groups. This may reflect an improved attitude to memory because of increased attentional monitoring of it due to study participation, that is, a Hawthorne effect; or it may reflect a desire to give the researcher positive feedback, that is, a Rosenthal effect. These effects were controlled for in this study by double-blinding and the use of a control group. 7.4 Possible mechanisms of action for observed effect of Bacopa Free radical damage impairs the function of neurons and is associated with the cognitive deterioration seen in neurological disease (Halliwell & Gutteridge, 1985). The brain is particularly susceptible to oxidative damage because it is very metabolically active (thus high oxygen consumption), has high levels of iron (a pro-oxidant) and is a lipid rich organ with high levels of unsaturated fat (prone to lipid peroxidation). The antioxidant activity of Bacopa has been reported in a number of laboratory studies (Tripathi et al., 1996; Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Sairam et al., 2001; Sumathy et al., 2001, 2002; Russo et al., 2003a, 2003b). Bacopa’s antioxidant action and free radical scavenging activity, especially in memory related structures in the brain including the hippocampus 65 (Bhattacharya et al., 2000), as well as astrocytes (Russo et al., 2003b), may explain some of the memory improvement effect seen in the current study. In addition to antioxidant effects, the memory facilitation effects found in the current study may be due to enhanced cholinergic modulation in the central nervous system. Bacopa has been shown to improve cholinergic function and enhance levels of acetylcholine (ACh) in rat models of Alzheimer’s disease (Bhattacharya, et al., 1999; Das et al., 2002). In the Bhattacharya et al. study, Bacopa improved induced cognitive deficits and also countered colchicine-induced reductions of acetylcholine, choline acetyltransferase activity and muscarinic cholinergic receptor binding in the hippocampus. Das et al. demonstrated a dose dependent decrease in the enzymatic breakdown of ACh in neuronal synapses. Furthermore, in an early study, Dey et al. (1964) reported that marked hypotensive and bradycardic effects observed in cats given Bacopa intravenously were partly modified by atropine- suggesting a cholinergic effect of Bacopa. Cholinergic neurotransmission plays a crucial role in memory function- it is required for synaptic modification processes involved in long term memory formation in the hippocampus (Hasselmo & Bower, 1993). A moderate decrease in cholinergic function is associated with ageing, and a dramatic depletion of ACh is a feature of Alzheimer’s disease (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). This is the rationale for the use of acetylcholinesterase (AChE) inhibitors which can help to improve symptoms of dementia in early stages of the disease. The cognitive benefits observed in the current study may thus find application in Alzheimer’s disease, and a clinical trial of the effects of Bacopa on dementia would seem worthwhile. Beyond its antioxidant and cholinergic actions, Bacopa may act on the brain in other ways as yet to be elucidated. The research thus far supports a nootropic action (i.e. nonspecific facilitation of cognitive function), rather than either a stimulant or sedative action in the central nervous system (Singh et al., 1988). Furthermore, increased levels of hippocampal protein kinase activity and enhanced protein levels have been reported by Dhawan and Singh (1996). This is relevant because protein kinases are actively involved in the synaptic modulation which accompanies long term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampus. Extensive research in this area of synaptic plasticity is providing a 66 physiological basis underpinning important mechanisms of memory and learning (Carlson, 2002:360-367). The research showing protein kinase activity for Bacopa is promising but remains to be replicated, and more research is needed to further elucidate possible mechanisms of action for the cognitive enhancing effects observed in this trial. 7.5 Side-effects There were significant GIT side-effects reported in the current trial, with 45% of the Bacopa group reporting at least one GIT effect, compared to only 5% of the placebo group reporting any GIT effect. The GIT effects reported by the Bacopa group consisted of increased stool frequency (15/49; 30%), nausea (9/49; 18%) and abdominal cramps (8/49; 16%). This finding contrasts with much of the literature on Bacopa; for example a doubleblind, placebo-controlled toxicological study in which bacosides were administered to healthy male volunteers for one month demonstrated an absence of any side-effects (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). Recent human studies of Bacopa have either not documented any side-effects or reported an absence of them, with the exception of the Stough et al. study (2001) which reported the occurrence in the Bacopa vs. placebo group of the following symptoms: nausea (18% vs. 4%), dry mouth (23% vs. 16%) and muscle fatigue (14% vs. 4%). Roodenrys et al. (2002) reported one adverse gastrointestinal effect (unspecified) of Bacopa. Additionally, one early human trial of 24 patients aged 7-34 years, reported that 20% of patients receiving a crude aqueous extract of Bacopa (50g fresh plant decocted in water, taken once daily), experienced abdominal pain and ‘slight gastro-intestinal disturbance’ although the exact nature of the disturbance was not delineated by the authors (Mukherjee & Dey, 1966). The side-effects observed in the current study cannot be put down to excessive dosage as the dosage used was equivalent to 6g of dried herb material daily, this being in accordance with the traditional recommended dosage regimen of 5-10g of the powdered dried herb daily (Kapoor, 1990:61), as well as being the dosage recommended by the manufacturer and that used in other human studies. Nor were the effects due to heavy metal or microbiological contamination as analytical testing was carried out on the study drug by the manufacturer, and excluded these possibilities. 67 The observed side-effects may have been related to the saponins in Bacopa. As previously discussed, Bacopa’s main active constituents are its saponins. Saponins are glycosides (i.e. plant constituents which yield sugar on hydrolysis), that are of two types- steroidal and triterpene (Lacaille-Dubois & Wagner, 1996). These are widely distributed throughout the plant kingdom and occur in many foods and beverages. Mills and Bone caution that herbs with high saponin content can irritate the gastric mucosa (2005:252). In the upper digestive tract the gastric mucosal irritant effect has been linked to a reflexive expectorant action, (probably mediated via the vagal nerve), characteristic of saponin-rich expectorant herbs (which are also emetic in higher doses) (Mills & Bone, 2000: 45). Saponins interact with cell membranes by binding with sterols to create ‘pore-like’ openings in them; in red blood cells this eventually causes rupture and hence haemolysis (Gee & Johnson, 1988). When this happens in the small intestine, an increase in intestinal permeability can eventuate which may disrupt function and absorption, as has been shown to occur in rat small intestine (ibid.). Saponins are poorly absorbed in the digestive tract and usually then, only the aglycone (i.e. sapogenin) component, hence the aforementioned haemolytic reaction will only occur with injected and not orally ingested saponins (Mills & Bone, 2000:43). Thus saponins can be safely ingested, though may react with the GIT mucosa. It is feasible that one of the contributing causal factors of the GIT side-effects observed in this trial was the high concentration of saponins in the herbal extract used due to the standardisation process applied in its manufacture. The standardisation process in herbal medicines ensures consistently high levels of the nominated active constituent/s by measuring and maintaining levels of chemical marker compounds (Mills & Bone, 2000:123-4). Notably, minimum but not maximum amounts of marker compounds are stipulated. Given the high level of saponins (Bacosides) in the study drug used- which was standardised to contain at least 40%- coupled with a high concentration of the herb per tablet (one 300mg tablet is equivalent to 6g of dried herb), it is postulated that this resulted in a very high saponin content per tablet which may have caused GIT irritation, whereas a less concentrated traditional extract may not have been enough to elicit a reaction. 68 Another important possible reason for the observed side-effects of Bacopa is that its cholinergic action, as demonstrated in animal models (Das et al., 2002; Bhattacharya et al., 1999; Dey et al., 1964), may be mediating adverse effects on the GIT, a reaction that is a well documented side-effect of cholinergic therapy such as AChE inhibitor medications used in dementia (Small et al., 1997; Flicker, 1999). The neurotransmitter ACh is found centrally in the brain and spinal cord, peripherally in skeletal muscle junctions, in ganglia of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), and in organs supplied by the parasympathetic branch of the ANS (Carlson, 2002:107). AChE is the enzyme in postsynaptic membranes which breaks down ACh. Inhibitors of this enzyme thus cause the accumulation of ACh in synapses, prolonging its effects and stimulating cholinergic transmission throughout the central and peripheral nervous systems. GIT side-effects are caused both by parasympathomimetic effects- increased tone, peristalsis and secretions of the stomach and intestines, and by motor effectsnausea, vomiting, belching, abdominal cramps and increased bowel movements (Koelle, 1975: 467-470). The side-effects noted in the current study could thus all be explained by a cholinergic effect of Bacopa. In addition to having cautionary implications for Bacopa usage, these observations also lend support to the hypothesis that Bacopa has a cholinergic effect in humans, and that the cognitive enhancing effects are, at least in part, mediated via enhanced cholinergic modulation. Finally, regarding the side-effects observed in this study, it is noteworthy that the findings are in accordance with occasional traditional references to Bacopa as having a laxative and even a mild purgative effect (Sivarajan, 1994: 97; Macmillan, 1991:424), and an effect of diuresis, especially when urinary retention is accompanied with constipation (Kapoor, 1990:61), (notably – both diuresis and laxation can be elicited by cholinergic stimulation). Singh and Dhawan (1982) reproduce and translate Sanskrit text from a 16th century Ayurvedic medical treatise- the Bhavprakasa Varg-Prakaranawhich, significantly, lists laxation amongst Bacopa’s actions. This raises an important issue concerning traditional versus modern usage of plant medicines which merits mention here. In Ayurvedic medicine, Bacopa is not used 69 singly, but rather in complex formulae, with specific combinations indicated for various pathologies and for various individual ‘constitutional’ types. For example, other herbs in a Bacopa containing formula might attenuate GIT irritant effects. Furthermore, people who react to Bacopa might be the type of person (constitution) for whom it would not be indicated or prescribed according to Ayurvedic tradition. This raises the interesting question of how we can integrate traditional knowledge of botanical actions into the modern health care context when the wisdom on which it is based is not fully understood or appreciated- can the knowledge be fully translated without this underpinning basis? This is a question that warrants much consideration, however it is outside the scope of the current study. In this context, the reductionist methodology employed in trials such as the present one, may not truly reflect the potential for herbal medicines like Bacopa, when applied in their traditional context. However, the testing of complex formula brings in confounding variables to the extent that such a trial would be methodologically flawed. This issue (amongst others) presents a dilemma for complementary medicine research, which as yet remains unresolved. 7.6 Limitations of the current study A number of limitations are apparent in the current trial and are summarised below. Firstly, a broader range of neuropsychological tests could have been used to elicit more information about cognitive function. The neuropsychological assessments used were far from exhaustive. Some widely used memory tests are not available to nonpsychologists and were also cost-prohibitive for this study, for example the Wechsler Scales of Memory and Intelligence, (or component tests), are commonly included as a core instrument in memory and cognitive testing (Lezak et al., 2004:480). Resources available in this study restricted test selection to those that were firstly available to the non-psychologist and secondly, affordable on a limited budget. Whilst appropriate and broad spectrum, testing was far from exhaustive and could have been expanded to include for example, a test of prose recall which imitates closely the demands on memory of everyday events such as conversations, news items and other media communications (Lezak et al., 2004:414). Furthermore, the use of single tests for verbal and visual memory may not be a thorough enough measure on their own. For 70 example, in one study in which different verbal memory tests were compared, high levels of variability between tests was demonstrated, suggesting that a single test should not be relied upon as a measure of verbal memory (Macartney-Filgate & Vriezen, 1988). A second limitation of the current study was that sensory deficits are common in older age groups and could have affected test performance. A common impairment in elderly people is sight and hearing deficits, which could directly affect visual and verbalauditory test performance. In hindsight, visual and auditory acuity should have been measured as part of the screening procedure, and use of visual or auditory aids documented. Patient information should also have included that eyeglasses or hearing devices should be brought in to memory assessment sessions. Furthermore, marked auditory and visual deficits could have been incorporated into the exclusionary criteria for study participation. A third limitation of the current study is that a 6-week follow-up neuropsychological assessment was unable to be included due to lack of resources. Hence assessment of memory occurred only at baseline and at end-point (12-weeks). A 6-week measure would have provided useful information about the time period required for Bacopa to achieve an effect. Likewise, a 4-week post-trial assessment (at 16-weeks) would have been worthwhile to assess whether the benefits achieved were maintained after finishing Bacopa. A fourth limitation of the current study is that the group baseline difference on CFT scores remains unresolved. As previously discussed, this difference could not be accounted for by differences on any independent variables including gender, age, or education. Nor was inter-rater reliability an issue because the task was scored by the same neuropsychologist. There could have been a difference between groups in a variable not measured, for example acuity of eyesight (the CFT being a visual task), or level of agility (as motor skills are required on the CFT). Alternatively, the Bacopa group may have had better visual memory at baseline, (in which case those with better visual memory might benefit more from Bacopa than those with poorer visual memory). This issue remains unresolved. 71 A fifth limitation of the study is that whilst for the AVLT an alternate word list was used for the 12-week end-of-trial assessment, this was not the case for the CFT and the TMT in which the same forms were utilised for pre and post testing. Thus, practice effects may have occurred on these tests, which would limit the interpretation of significant findings on these tasks, had any had been found. Practice effects are especially evident in memory tests- so much so that their absence may point to cognitive deficits (Lezak et al., 2004: 114). The effect can be eliminated for verbal tests with the use of parallel forms on repeated testing; however with visuospatial tests there will still be some practice effects even with the use of an alternate form because learning about how to perform the task occurs (as well as learning the actual content) (Lezak et al., 2004: 415). Practice effects on the CFT and TMT were evidenced by the improved scores after 12-weeks in both treatment and placebo groups. Alternative figures have been developed for the CFT; the most widely used being the Taylor figure, though scoring is generally a little higher on this figure than on the CFT (Lezak et al., 2004: 458). The use of a control group in the current study controlled for practice effects, however alternate forms on the CFT would have strengthened the study. Lastly, another limitation noted in the current study is the failure to adjust for the use of multiple dependent variables. Neuropsychological assessment cannot be achieved with singular outcome measures, necessitating the use of multiple measures. The large number of analyses performed may have increased the risk of a type one error, that is, of finding a significant effect when none exists. The current study did not adjust for multiple measures. The alpha value could have been set at .01 rather than .05 to counteract this increased risk. 7.7 Recommendations arising from the study One outcome of this study is recommendations to help counteract the GIT side-effects observed with Bacopa administration. It is advised that Bacopa doses equivalent to 6g of dried plant should be given in divided doses; and that manufacturers might thus ideally restrict the amount of herb per tablet to 3g (dry weight equivalent). Enteric coating of Bacopa tablets would protect the upper GI mucosa from exposure to the saponins, and thus would seem warranted. It is also suggested, in the light of these findings, that Bacopa tablets be taken with food, again to help buffer the GI mucosa 72 from the irritant effect of saponins. Furthermore, it would be pertinent for further research on Bacopa to see if the side-effects observed in this study are replicable. Further research to establish cholinergic effects in humans and to elucidate other possible mechanisms of action is warranted. Human trials of Bacopa in the dementia population would seem warranted to see if the cognitive enhancement observed in nondemented older persons might also benefit dementia sufferers, particularly given the support for evidence of cholinergic activity observed, as well as documented antioxidant activity. Effective pharmacological treatment of dementia remains largely elusive and there is a need to explore all possible therapies. This study has demonstrated that Bacopa produces cognitive benefits in older people, lending weight to previous evidence of cognitive function enhancement. These demonstrated nootropic effects may be of benefit to dementia sufferers too. Because early intervention is likely to be the most effective point of intervention in neurodegenerative pathologies, and because delaying onset of dementia would have such a dramatic impact as discussed in chapter 3, then trials of Bacopa in people meeting criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) would also be indicated, in an effort to reduce or delay progression to dementia. In any further studies looking at verbal and visual memory, it is recommended that visual and auditory acuity testing is included at screening to account for potential effects of deficits. Participants should also be advised to bring visual and hearing aids to assessment sessions. Additional neuropsychological testing is advisable in future clinical studies to give a broader appraisal of the potential effects of Bacopa. Additionally, it would be pertinent to employ alternative figures for the CFT if used. All of the human studies on Bacopa have looked at effects of up to 12-weeks duration of administration and doses of 300mg/day. It would be interesting for further studies to assess the effects of longer administration periods, and different dosage regimens. 73 A final recommendation is that when non-psychologists design future clinical trials involving assessment of neuropsychological function, it is important that budgeting should factor in adequate resources to cover the costs of the neuropsychological tests employed as well as the neuropsychological expertise required to administer them- both of these can be very costly. 74 8. CONCLUSION This thesis reported the results of a 12-week double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial of the effects of Bacopa monnieri on memory performance in healthy older Australians. The purpose of this trial was to determine whether the beneficial cognitive effects of Bacopa as reported in earlier studies were replicable in the elderly, as this is the demographic most affected by memory difficulties; and also to document the presence or absence of adverse events. Demographically, Australia, along with other nations globally, has an ageing population. Neurological illnesses are widely referred to as the new epidemic, as longer life-spans allow more neuropathology to emerge. Dementia rates in the Western world, and indeed globally, are high and on the increase. This is of great concern as it places escalating demands on limited health care resources, as well as causing enormous personal suffering. Non-pathological memory loss in the aged population also has a very high incidence, which has been reported as occurring in almost half of the over 65years age group (Small, 2002). The identification of agents that can improve neurological health and cognitive function is an important endeavour, firstly because of the potential to improve quality of life for sufferers of cognitive impairment and their families, and secondly, to reduce the burden of neurological illness on health care resources. The exploration of medicines used in traditional cultures for improving neurological function seems a valid starting point in the quest for treatment strategies, especially because highly effective pharmacological treatments for cognitive decline do not exist. Evidence is accumulating for the cognitive benefits of some therapies found in the complementary medicine field, such as various nutritional supplements and herbal medicines, especially in relation to preventing and reducing neurological oxidative damage and improving neurotransmission. Bacopa monnieri is a herb with a long traditional usage in Ayurvedic medicine for its beneficial effects on mental functioning. This traditional usage is supported by both preclinical and clinical research. This study tested the hypothesis that Bacopa would 75 improve memory in healthy older Australians. This extended previous findings that have shown evidence of a beneficial effect on memory in younger age groups. It was also postulated that the use of Bacopa would not be associated with any side-effects. A clinical trial was carried out to assess the effects of 12-weeks administration of Bacopa monnieri (300mg/day) on memory in people over 55-years of age. Primary outcome measures were well validated neuropsychological tests to objectively measure audio-verbal and visual memory, and a memory complaint questionnaire to measure subjective memory complaints. The results demonstrated that Bacopa versus placebo significantly improved memory acquisition and retention in older Australians. This concurs with findings from previous human and animal studies, as well as supports traditional Ayurvedic claims and uses. Contrary to a postulated absence of side-effects, Bacopa’s use was associated with GIT side-effects, specifically increased bowel movements, nausea and abdominal cramping. These side-effects have been infrequently reported in previous literature, with most literature reporting an absence of side-effects. These symptoms may have been due to either GIT irritation by the saponin component of the herb, or to possible cholinergic stimulation of autonomic and motor responses in the GIT, or to a combination of both of these factors. And whilst these factors offer the most likely explanations for the observed side-effects, other, as yet not understood mechanisms must also be considered. This study’s replicated findings of beneficial effects on human memory have promising implications for Bacopa’s potential use in dementia. This is especially so, given its demonstrated antioxidant and cholinergic actions, because dementia is associated with both oxidative damage and depletion of acetylcholine. The side-effects observed in this study lend further weight to previous findings of a cholinergic effect of Bacopa. Animal studies have demonstrated a cholinergic action, though more research is needed to further establish a cholinergic effect for Bacopa in humans, and to identify other possible mechanisms of action as well. Given that even modest postponement of dementia symptoms would have enormous benefits for sufferers and for society, the identification of cognitive enhancing agents is an important pursuit. Thus, a clinical trial of Bacopa in dementia patients would seem a worthwhile extension of the current study. 76 Many older people report memory deficits and many look to complementary medicines for solutions to health problems, including cognitive enhancement. Research such as the current study contributes efficacy and safety data that will assist Australians to choose more effective therapies and may identify agents that can improve quality of life and ultimately reduce the health care costs associated with degenerative illnesses. An evidence basis for complementary medicine usage will enable these therapies to take their place alongside mainstream medical approaches in the interest of better health care for all. 77 REFERENCES Abrams, B., Beers, M., & Berkow, R.E. (1995). The Merck Manual of Geriatrics, 2nd ed. New Jersey: Merck & Co. Access Economics Pty Ltd. (2004). 'Delaying the Onset of Alzheimer's Disease; Projections and Issues, Report to Alzheimer's Australia’. Canberra. Albert, M. (2002). 'Memory decline: The boundary between aging and age-related disease'. Annals of Neurology, 51(3), 282-284. Alzheimer's Australia. (2004). 'Dementia Research: A Vision for Australia'. Canberra: Alzheimer's Australia. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: APA American Psychological Association. (1998). 'Guidelines for the evaluation of dementia and age-related cognitive decline'. American Psychologist, 53(12), 1298-1303. Anbarasi, K., Vani, G., Balakrishna, K., & Devi, C. (2005). 'Effect of bacoside A on brain antioxidant status in cigarette smoke exposed rats'. Life Science (Abstract), October 11 [epub ahead of print]. Anonymous. (2002). Indian Herbal Pharmacopoeia. Mumbai. Anstey, K. J., & Low, L. F. (2004). 'Normal cognitive changes in aging'. Australian Family Physician, 33(10), 783-787. Australian Department of Health and Aged Care. (1999). Fact Sheet: caring for people with dementia, cognitive and behavioural difficulties. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW): Jenkins, A., Goss, J., Hales, C. & Karmel, R. (2004). 'The impact of dementia on the health and aged care systems'. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Baddeley, A. (1991). 'The decline in working memory in Alzheimer's disease'. Brain, 114, 2521-2542. Baddeley, A. (1992). 'Working Memory'. Science, 255(5044), 556-559. Balota, D., Dolan, P., & Duchek, J. (2000). ‘Memory Changes in Healthy Older Adults’. In E. C. Tulving, FM. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory (pp. 395-410). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 78 Barberger-Gateau, P., Letteneur, L., Deschamps, V., Peres, K., Dartigues, J.F., & Renaud, S. (2002). 'Fish, meat & risk of dementia'. British Medical Journal, 325(7370), 932-935. Bennett, J. (1999). 'Antipsychotic drug treatment'. Nursing Standard, 13(24), 49-53. Bhattacharya, S.K., Bhattacharya, A., Kumar, A., & Ghosal, S. (2000). 'Antioxidant activity of Bacopa monniera in rat frontal cortex, striatum and hippocampus.' Phytotherapy Research, 14(3), 174-179. Bhattacharya, S.K., & Ghosal, S. (1998). 'Anxiolytic activity of a standardized extract of Bacopa monniera- an experimental study'. Phytomedicine, 5(2), 77-82. Bhattacharya, S.K., Kumar, A., & Ghosal, S. (1999). 'Effect of Bacopa monniera on animal models of Alzheimer's disease and perturbed central cholinergic markers of cognition in rats'. Research Communications in Pharmacology and Toxicology, 4(3 & 4), ii1 -ii12. Blumenthal, M.E. (2003). The American Botanical Council (ABC) Clinical Guide to Herbs. Austin, Texas: The American Botanical Council. Bose, K.C., & Bose, N.K. (1931). 'Observations on the actions and uses of Herpestis monniera'. Journal of the Indian Medical Association, 1, 60. Busse, A., Bischkopf, J., Riedel-Heller, S. G., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2003). 'Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and incidence according to different criteria: Results of the Leipzig longitudinal study of the aged (LEILA75+)'. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(5), 449-454. Carlson, N. (2002). Physiological Psychology, 5th edn. Boston: A Pearson Education Company. Cattel, R. (1963). 'Theory of fluid and crystallised intelligence: a critical experiment'. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1-22. Chakravarty, A.K., Garai, S., Masuda, K., Nakane, T., & Kawahara, N. (2003). 'Bacopasides III-V: three new triterpenoid glycosides form Bacopa monniera'. Chem Pharm Bulletin, 51, 215-217. Chakravarty, A.K., Sarkar, T., Masuda, K., Shiojima, K., Nakane, T., & Kawahara, N. (2001). 'Bacopaside I and II: two pseudojujubogenin glycosides from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 58, 553-556. Chatterji, N., Rastogi, R.P., & Dhar, M.L. (1963). 'Chemical examination of Bacopa monniera Wettst.: Part I - Isolation of chemical constituents'. Indian Journal of Chemistry, 1, 212-215. 79 Chowdhuri, D.K., Parmar, D., Kakkar, P., Shukla, R., Seth, P.K.,, & Srimal, R.C. (2002). 'Antistress effects of bacosides of Bacopa monnierei: Modulation of Hsp70 expression, superoxide dismutase and cytochrome P450 activity in rat brain'. Phytotherapy Research, 16, 639-645. Christensen, H. (2001). 'What cognitive changes can be expected with normal ageing?' Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 768-775. Cohen, N.J., & Squire, L.R. (1980). 'Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that'. Science, 210, 207-210. Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO). (2005). 'Preventative health flagship tackles dementia', Media release. Corwin, J., & Bylsma, F.W. (1993). Translation: Rey 'Psychological examination of traumatic encephalopathy' and Osterrieth 'The complex figure copy test'. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7(1), 3-18. Courtney, C., Farrell, D., Gray, R., Hills, R., Lynch, L., Sellwood, E., et al. (2004). 'Long term donepezil treatment in 565 patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD 2000): randomised double-blind trial'. Lancet, 363(9427), 2105-2115. Craik, F.M., & Jennings, J.M. (1992). 'Human memory', in Craik, F.M. & Salthouse, T.A. (eds), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, pp51-109. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Crook, T.H., Bartus, R.T., Ferris, S.H., Whitehouse, P., Cohen, G.D., & Gershon, S. (1986). 'Age-associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change: Report of a National Institute of Mental Health work group'. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 261-276. Crook, T.H., Feher, E.P., & Larrabee, G.J. (1992). ‘Assessment of memory complaints in age-associated memory impairment: the MAC-Q’. International Journal of Psychogeriatrics, 4, 165-176. Das, G.S., Nath, C., Pal, R., Singh, S., & Singh, H. (2002). 'A comparative study in rodents of standardized extracts of Bacopa monniera and Gingko biloba anticholinesterase and cognitive enhancing activities'. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behaviour, 73, 893-900. Dawe, B., Procter, A., & Philpot, M. (1992). 'Concepts of mild memory impairment in the elderly and their relationship to dementia- a review'. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 473-479. 80 De Carli, C. (2003). 'Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology and treatment'. Lancet Neurology, 2, 15-21. de la Torre, J.C. (2004). 'Is Alzheimer's disease a neurodegenerative or a vascular disorder? Data, dogma and dialectics'. The Lancet Neurology, 3, 184-190. Deepak, M., & Amit, A. (2004). 'The need for establishing identities of 'bacoside A and B', the putative major bioactive saponins of Indian medicinal plant Bacopa monnieri'. Phytomedicine, 11(2/3), 264-268. Deepak, M., Sangli, G.K., Arun, P.C., & Amit, A. (2005). 'Quantitative determination of the major saponin mixture bacoside A in Bacopa monnieri by HPLC'. Phytochemical Analysis, 16(1), 24-29. Dey, C. D., Koley, P. N., & Dutta, C. P. (1964). 'Chemical and pharmacological properties of Brahmi’. Journal of Experimental Medical Sciences, 8(1), 1-13. Dhawan, B.N., & Singh, H.K. (1996). 'Pharmacological studies on Bacopa monniera, an Ayurvedic nootropic agent'. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 6 (Suppl.3), 144. Eisenberg, D.M., Kessler, R.C., Foster, C., Norlock, F.E., Calkins, D.R., & Delbanco, T.L. (1993). 'Unconventional medicine in the United States: prevalence, costs and patterns of use'. New England Journal of Medicine, 328, 246-252. Flicker, L. (1999). 'Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease'. British Medical Journal, 318, 615-616. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). 'Mini Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician'. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Forrester, G., & Geffen, G. (1991). 'Performance measures of 7-to 15-year-old children on the Auditory-Verbal Learning Test'. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5(4), 345-359. Ganzevles, P.G., & de Geus, B.W.J. (1991). 'Clinical neuropsychological assessment of patients chronically exposed to organic solvents'. Archives of Toxicology Supplement, 15, 54-57. Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K., & Yamasaki, K. (1996a). 'Dammarane-type triterpenoid saponins from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 42(3), 815-820. Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K. & Yamasaki, K. (1996b). 'Bacopasaponin D- a pseudojujubogenin glycoside from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 43, 24472449. 81 Gee, J. M., & Johnson, L. T. (1988). 'Interactions between hemolytic saponins, bile salts and small intestinal mucosa in the rat'. Journal of Nutrition, 118, 1391-1397. Geffen, G., Moar, K.J., O'Hanlon, A.P., Clark, C.R., & Geffen, L.B. (1990). 'Performance measures of 16-to 86 year old males and females on the Auditory Verbal Learning Test'. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4(1), 45-63. GlaxoWellcome Incorporated. (1997). The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression Questionnaire, 2003, from www.glaxowellcome.com Graham, J.E., Rockwood, K., Beattie, B.L., Eastwood, R., Gauthier, S., Tuokko, H., et al. (1997). 'Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly population'. Lancet, 349(9068), 1793-1796. Grundman, M., Petersen, R., Ferris, S.H., Thomas, R.G., Aisen, P.S., Bennett, D.A., et al. (2004). 'Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished form Alzheimer disease and normal aging for clinical trials'. Archives of Neurology, 61(1), 59-66. Haan, M.N., & Wallace, R. (2004). 'Can dementia be prevented? Brain aging in a population-based context'. Annual Review of Public Health, 25, 1-24. Hachinski, V., Illif, L., Zilhka, E., du Boulay, G.H., McAllister, V.L., Marshall, J., et al. (1975). 'Cerebral blood flow in dementia'. Archives of Neurology, 32, 632-637. Hackman, R. (1998). 'Antioxidants that entertain the brain'. Nutrition Science News, 10, 530-538. Halliwell, B., & Gutteridge, J.M.C. (1985). 'Oxygen free radicals and the nervous system'. Trends in Neuroscience, 8, 22-26. Hanninen, T. (1996). 'Age-Associated Memory Impairment: A neuropsychological and epidemiological study'. University of Kuopio, Finland. Hasselmo, M.E., & Bower, J.M. (1993). 'Acetylcholine and memory'. Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 218-221. Hedlung, J.L., & Vieweg, M.S. (1979). 'The Hamilton rating scale for depression'. Journal of Operational Psychiatry, 10(2), 149-165. Henderson, A.S., & Jorm, A.F. (1998). Dementia in Australia. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service. Himi, T., Saito, H., & Nishiyama, N. (1989). 'Effect of Ginseng saponins on the survival of cerebral cortex neurons in cell cultures'. Chemical & Pharmaceutical Bulletin, 37(2), 481-484. 82 Hofferberth, B. (1994). 'The efficacy of EGb 761 in patients with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type, a double blind, placebo controlled study on different levels of investigation'. Human Psychopharmacology, 9, 215-222. Horner, P.J., & Gage, F.H. (2002). 'Regeneration in the adult and aging brain'. Archives of Neurology, 59(11), 1717-1720. Hou, C.C., Lin, S.J., Cheng, J.T., & Hsu, F.L. (2002). 'Bacopaside HI, bacopasaponin G, and bacopasides A, B and C from Bacopa monniera'. Journal of Natural Products, 65, 1759-1763. Ivnik, R.J., Malec, J.F., Tangalos, E.G., Petersen, R.G., Kokmen, E., & Kurland, L.T. (1990). 'The Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): Norms for ages 55 years and older'. Psychological Assessment, 2, 303-304. Jain, P., Khanna, N.K., Trehan, T., Pendse, V.K., & Godhwani, J.L. (1994). 'Antiinflammatory effects of an Ayurvedic preparation, Brahmi Rasayan, in rodents'. Indian Journal of Experimental Biology, 32, 633-636. Jorm, A. (2002). 'Prospects for the prevention of dementia'. Australasian Journal on Ageing, 21(1), 9-13. Jorm, A., Rodgers, B., & Christensen, H. (2004). 'Use of medications to enhance memory in a large community sample of 60-64year olds'. International Psychogeriatrics, 16(2), 209-217. Jorm, A.F. (2001). 'Dementia: a major health problem for Australia' Position Paper 1. Alzheimer's Association Australia, September. Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., & Henderson, A. S. (1987). 'The prevalence of dementia: A quantitative integration of the literature'. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76, 465-479. Kahn, R.L., & Rowe, J.W. (1998). Successful Aging. New York: Pantheon. Kapoor, L.D. (1990). CRC Handbook of Ayurvedic Medicinal Plants. Florida: CRC Press, Inc. Kawai, K.I., & Shibata, S. (1978). 'Pseudojujubogenin, a new sapogenin from Bacopa monnieri'. Phytochemistry, 17, 287-289. Kawas, C.H. (2003). 'Early Alzheimer's disease'. New England Journal of Medicine, 349(11), 1056-1063. Kimmel, D.C. (1990). Adulthood and Aging: An Interdisciplinary, Developmental View. New York: John Wiley & sons. 83 Kishore, M., & Singh, M. (2005). 'Effect of bacosides, alcoholic extract of Bacopa monniera Linn. (brahmi), on experimental amnesia in mice'. Indian Journal of Experimental Biology, 43(7), 640-645. Klassen, A., Sung, J., & Sradlan, E. (1968). 'Histological changes in cerebral arteries with increasing age'. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 27, 607-623. Kleijnen, J., & Knipschild, P. (1992). 'Gingko biloba for cerebral insufficiency'. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 34, 352-358. Knapp, M.J., Knopman, D.S., Solomon, P.R., & et al. (1994). 'A 30 week randomized controlled trial of high dose tacrine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease'. Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, 985-991. Koelle, G.B. (1975). ‘Parasympathomimetic Agents’ in Goodman, L.S & Gilman, A. (Eds.), The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics 5th ed. pp. 467-476. New York: Macmillan. Kral, V.A. (1962). 'Senescent forgetfulness: benign and malignant'. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 86, 257-260. Lacaille-Dubois, M., & Wagner, H. (1996). 'A review of the biological and pharmacological activities of saponins'. Phytomedicine, 2(4), 363-386. Larrieu, L., Leenneur, C., Helmer, J.F., Dartigues, P., & Barberger-Gateau, P. (2004). 'Nutritional risk factors and risk of incident dementia in the PAQUID longitudinal cohort'. The Jounal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 8(3), 150-154. LeBars, P.L., Katz, M.M., Berman, N., Turan, M., Freedman, A.M., & Schatzberg, A.F. (1997). 'A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of Gingko biloba for dementia'. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278(16), 1327-1332. Levy, R. (1994). 'Aging-associated cognitive decline'. International Psychogeriatrics, 6, 63-68. Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., & Loring, D.W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press. Lopez, O.L., Becker, J.T., Wisniewski, S., Saxton, J., Kaufer, D.I., & DeKosky, S.T. (2002). 'Cholinesterase inhibitor treatment alters the natural history of Alzheimer's disease'. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72(3), 310-315. 84 Luszcz, M., & Bryan, J. (1999). 'Challenges to understanding age-related memory loss in late adulthood: an introduction'. Gerontology, 45(6), 343-344. Ma, T.C., & Yu, Q.H. (1993). 'Effect of 20-(S)-Ginsenoside-R2 and cyproheptadine on two-way active avoidance learning and memory in rats'. Arzneim-Forsch/Drug Research, 43(11), 1049-1052. Ma, T.C., Yu, Q.H., & Chen, M.H. (1991). 'Effects of ginseng stem-leaves saponins on one way avoidance behaviour in rats'. Zhongguo Yao Li Xue Bao, 12(5), 403406. Macartney-Filgate, M.S., & Vriezen, E.R. (1988). 'Intercorrelation of clinical tests of verbal memory'. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 121-126. MacLennan, A., Wilson, D., & Taylor, B. (1996). 'Prevalence and cost of alternative medicine in Australia'. The Lancet, 347(March 2), 569-573. Macmillan, H.F. (1991). Tropical Planting and Gardening (6 ed.). Kuala Lumpur: Malayan Nature Society. Mahato, S.B., Garai, S., & Chakravarty, A.K. (2000). 'Bacopasaponins E and F: two jujubogenin bisdesmosides from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 53(6), 711714. Malhotra, C.L., & Das, P.K. (1959). 'Pharmacological studies of Herpestis monniera Linn. (Brahmi)'. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 47, 294-305. Markowitsch, H.J. (2000). ‘Neuroanatomy of Memory’, in E.C. Tulving, F.M. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory. (pp. 465-484). Oxford University Press. Mattson, M.P. (2000). 'Existing data suggest that Alzheimer's disease is preventable'. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 924, 153-159. Mattson, M.P. (2003). 'Gene-diet interactions in brain aging and neurodegenerative disorders'. Annals of Internal Medicine, 139(5), 441-444. McShane, R., Keene, J., Gedling, K., Fairburn, C., Jacoby, R., & Hope, T. (1997). 'Do neuroleptic drugs hasten cognitive decline in dementia? Prospective study with necropsy follow up'. British Medical Journal, 314, 266-275. Mearns, J., Dunn, J., & Lees-Haley, P.R. (1994). 'Psychological effects of organophosphate pesticides: a review and call for research by psychologists'. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 286-294. Mills, S., & Bone, K. (2000). Principles and Practice of Phytotherapy. London: Harcourt Ltd. Mills, S., & Bone, K. (2005). The Essential Guide to Herbal Safety. St Louis: Elsevier. 85 Minett, T.S., Dean, J.L., Firbank, M., English, P., & O'Brien, J.T. (2005). 'Subjective memory complaints, white matter lesions, depressive symptoms and cognition in elderly patients'. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 665-671. Moher, D., Schulz, K.F., & Altman, D.G. (2001). 'The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group randomised trials'. The Lancet, 357(9263), 1191-1194. Morgan, M., & Bone, K. (1999). 'Bacopa',. Modern Phytotherapist, September, 21-27. Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A. & Tangney, C.C. (2005). 'Fish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community study'. Archives of Neurology, 62, 1-5. Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A., Bienias, J.L., Tangney, C.C., Bennett, D.A., Aggarwal, N., et al. (2002). 'Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in a biracial community study'. Journal of American Medical Association (JAMA), 287(24), 3230-3237. Mukherjee, G.D., & Dey, C.D. (1966). 'Clinical Trial on Brahmi- Part I'. Journal of Experimental Medical Science, June- Sept, 5-11. Nathan, P.J., Clarke, J., Lloyd, J., Hutchison, C.W., Downey, L., & Stough, C. (2001). 'The acute effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive function in healthy normal subjects.' Human Psychopharmacology, 16, 345-351. Nathan, P.J., Tanner, S., Lloyd, J., Harrison, B., Curran, L., Oliver, C., et al. (2004). 'Effects of a combined extract of Gingko biloba and Bacopa monniera on cognitive function in healthy humans'. Human Psychopharmacology, 19, 91-96. Negi, K.S., Singh, Y.D., Kushwaha, K. P., Rastogi, C. K., Rathi, A. K., Srivastava, J. S., et al. (2000). 'Clinical evaluation of memory enhancing properties of Memory Plus in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder'. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 42, Supplement. O'Brien, J.T., Erkinjuntti, T., Reisberg, B., Roman, G., Sawada, T., Pantoni, L., et al. (2003). 'Vascular cognitive impairment'. The Lancet Neurology, 2, 89-98. Osterrieth, P.A. (1944). 'Le test de copie d'une figure complex: Contribution a l'etude de la perception et de la memoire'. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286-340. Perrig, W.J., Perrig, P., & Stahelin, H.P. (1997). 'The relation between antioxidants and memory performance in the old and very old'. Journal of American Geriatric Society, 45(6), 718-724. 86 Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R..J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). 'Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome'. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303-308. Petersen, R.C., Stevens, J.C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E.G., Cummings, J.L. & DeKosky, S.T. (2001). 'Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review)'. Neurology, 56(9), 11331142. Petersen, R.C. (2004). 'Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity'. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183-194. Petersen, R.C., Smith G.E., Kokmen, E., Ivnik, R.J., & Tangalos, E.G. (1992). 'Memory function in normal aging'. Neurology, 42, 396-401. Phipps, L. (1999). 'Effects and side effects of antipsychotic drugs'. Nursing Standard, 13(48), 56. Polgar, S., & Thomas, S.A. (1995). Introduction to Research in the Health Sciences (3 ed.). Melbourne: Churchill Livingstone. Rastogi, R.P. (1990). Compendium of Indian Medicinal Plants (Vol. 1; pp 118-122). New Delhi: CSIR. Rastogi, R.P., Pal, R., & Kulshreshtha, D.K. (1994). 'Bacoside A3: a triterpenoid saponin from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 36, 133-137. Reisberg, D. (1997). Cognition: Exploring the Science of Mind, 1st ed. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. Reitan, R.M. (1958). ‘Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage.’ Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271-276. Rey, A. (1941). 'L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique'. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286-340. Rey, A. (1964). L'examen clinique in psychologie. Paris: Press Universitaire de France. Rickert, P. & Senior, G. (1998). 'WMS-III List Learning Test and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: comparisons and Australian normative data',. Poster presented at the 4th Annual Conference of the College of Clinical Neuropsychologists, Lorne, Victoria, Australia. Rogers, S.L., Farlow, M.R., Doody, R.S., et al. (1998). 'A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease',. Neurology, 50, 136-145. 87 Roodenrys, S., Booth, D., Bulzomi, S., Phipps, A., Micallef, C., & Smoker, J. (2002). 'Chronic Effects of Brahmi (Bacopa monnieri) on Human Memory'. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(2), 279-281. Rosen, W.G., Terry, R.D., Fuld, P.A., Katzman, R., & Peck, A. (1980). 'Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias'. Annals of Neurology, 7, 486-488. Russo, A., & Borrelli, F. (2005). 'Bacopa monniera, a reputed nootropic plant: an overview'. Phytomedicine, 12, 305-317. Russo, A., Izzo, A.A., Borrelli, F., Renis, M., & Vanella, A. (2003a). 'Free radical scavenging capacity and protective effect on DNA damage of Bacopa monniera'. Phytotherapy Research, 17, 870-875. Russo, A., Borrelli, F., Campisi, A., Aquaviva, R., Raciti, G., & Vanella, A. (2003b). 'Nitric oxide-related toxicity in cultured astrocytes: effects of Bacopa monniera.' Life Science, 73, 1517-1526. Sachdev, P.S. (2001). 'The ageing brain: a symposium. An introduction'. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 765-767. Sairam, K., Rao, V., Dora Babu, M., & Goel, R.K. (2001). 'Prophylactic and curative effects of Bacopa monniera in gastric ulcer models'. Phytomedicine, 8(6), 423430. Sano, M., Ernesto, C., Thomas, R.G., Klauber, M.R., Schafer, K., & Grundman, M. (1997). 'A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol or both as treatment for Alzheimer's disease' The Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study. New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 1216-1222. Scarpini, E., Scheltens, P., & Feldman, H. (2003). 'Treatment of Alzheimer's Disease: Current status and new perspectives'. Lancet Neurology, 2, 539-547. Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. A Handbook. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Schneider, L.S. (2005). 'Mild cognitive impairment'. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(8), 629-632. Schulz, V., Hubner, W.D., & Ploch, M. (1997). 'Clinical trials with phytopsychopharmacological agents'. Phytomedicine, 4(4), 379-387. 88 Sharma, R., Chaturvedi, C., & Tewari, P.V. (1987). 'Efficacy of Bacopa Monnieri in revitalizing intellectual functions in children'. Journal of Research and Education in Indian Medicine, 6, 1-10. Singh, H.K., & Dhawan, B.N. (1982). 'Effect of Bacopa monniera Linn. (Brahmi) extract on avoidance responses in rats'. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 5, 205214. Singh, H.K., & Dhawan, B.N. (1997). 'Neuropsychopharmacological effects of the ayurvedic nootropic Bacopa monniera linn. (brahmi)'. Indian Journal of Pharmacology, 29, s359-s365. Singh, H.K., Rastogi, R.P., Srimal, R.C., & Dhawan, B. N. (1988). 'Effect of Bacosides A and B on Avoidance Responses in Rats'. Phytotherapy Research, 2(2), 70-75. Singh, P.S., Sharad, S., & Kapur, S. (2004). 'Free radicals and oxidative stress in neurodegenerative diseases: Relevance of dietary antioxidants'. Journal of the Indian Academy of Clinical Medicine, 5(3), 218-225. Singh, R.H., & Singh, L. (1980). 'Studies on the anti-anxiety effect of the medhya rasayana drug, Brahmi (Bacopa monniera Wettst.) -part 1'. Journal of Research in Ayurveda and Siddha, 1, 133-148. Sivarajan, V.V. (1994). Ayurvedic Drugs and their Plant Sources. Lebanon: NH: International Science Publisher. Sliwinski, M., & Hofer, S. (1999). 'How strong is the evidence for mediational hypotheses of age related memory loss?' Gerontology, 45(6), 351-354. Small, G.R., Rabins, P.V., Barry, P.B., Buckholtz, N.S., DeKosky, S.T., Ferris, T.H., Finkel, S.I., Gwyther, L.P., Khachaturian, Z.S., Lebowitz, B.D., McRae, T.D., Morris, J.C., Oakley, F., Schneider, M.S., Streim, J.E., Sunderland, T., Teri, L.A., & Tune, L.E. (1997). 'Diagnosis and Treatment of Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders Consensus Statement'. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278(16), 1363-1371. Small, G.W. (2002). 'What we need to know about age related memory loss'. British Medical Journal, 324(22 June), 1502-1505. Snowdon, J., Vaughan, R., Miller, R., Burgess, E., & Tremlett, P. (1995). 'Psychotropic drug use in Sydney nursing homes'. The Medical Journal of Australia, 163, 7072. 89 Solfrizzi, V., Panza, F., Torres, F., Mastroianni, F., Del Parigi, L., Vanezia, A., et al. (1999). 'High monounsaturated fatty acids intake protects against age-related cognitive decline'. Neurology, 52, 1563-1569. Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1991). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press. Stough, C., Lloyd, J., Clarke, J., Downey, L.A., Hutchison, C.W., Rodgers, T., et al. (2001). 'The chronic effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive function in healthy human subjects'. Psychopharmacology, 156(4), 481-484. Sumathy, T., Subramanian, S., Govindasamy, S., Balakrishna, K., & Veluchamy, G. (2001). 'Protective effect of Bacopa monniera on morphine induced hepatotoxicity in rats'. Phytotherapy Research, 15, 643-645. Sumathy, T.S., Govindasamy, S., Balakrishna, K., & Veluchamy, G. (2002). 'Protective role of Bacopa monniera on morphine- induced brain mitochondrial enzyme activity in rats'. Fitoterapia, 73, 381-385. Tripathi, Y.B., Chaurasia, S., Tripathi, E., Upadhyay, A., & Dubey, G.P. (1996). 'Bacopa monniera Linn. as an antioxidant: mechanism of action.' Indian Journal of Experimental Biology, 34(6), 523-526. Trollor, J.N., & Valenzuela, M.J. (2001). 'Brain ageing in the new millenium'. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 788-805. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford University Press. Tulving, E. (2001). 'Does Memory Encoding Exist?' In Naveh-Benjamin, M., Moscovitch, M. & Roediger III, H.L. (Eds.), Perspectives on Human Memory and Cognitive Aging: Essays in Honour of Fergus Craik (pp. 6-27). New York: Psychology Press. Van der Linden, M., Bredart, S., & Beerten, A. (1994). 'Age related differences in updating working memory'. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 145-152. Vesper, J., & Hansgen, K.D. (1994). 'Efficacy of Gingko biloba in 90 outpatients with cerebral insufficiency caused by old age: Results of a placebo-controlled doubleblind trial'. Phytomedicine, 1, 9-16. Wimo, A., Winblad, B., Aguero-Torres, H., & von Strauss, E. (2003). 'The magnitude of dementia occurrence in the world'. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 17(2), 63-67. 90 World Health Organisation (WHO). (1992). The International Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders, 10th Revision, (ICD-10). Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organisation. Zandi, P.P., Anthony, J.C., Khachaturian, A.S., Stone S.V., Gustafson, D., Tschansz, J.T., et al. (2004). 'Reduced risk of Alzheimer disease in users of antioxidant vitamin supplements: The Cache County Study'. Archives of Neurology, 61(1), 82-88. Zola, S.M., & Squire, L.R. (2000). 'The Medial Temporal Lobe and the Hippocampus'. In Tulving, E.C., & Craik, F.M. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory. (pp. 485-500). Oxford University Press. 91 Appendix I: Participant instruction sheet and record booklet Clinical Trial: ‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older Australians?’ Participant Instruction Sheet and Record Booklet Participant name: ______________________________________ Follow up appointment: _________________________________ Instructions: • Take one tablet daily after food • Store in a cool dry place • Bring container and any left over tablets to next appointment • Record any comments, or possible side effects in comments section • Please contact Annette Morgan with any questions or comments phone: (02) 6620 3155 or amorgan@scu.edu.au Annette Morgan and Dr John Stevens Southern Cross University School of Nursing & Health Care Practices 92 Participant record booklet: please complete daily and submit at last appointment (end of study). Day number Date Tablet taken Comments, record any symptoms or (tick) possible side effects experienced 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 93 Day number Date Tablet Comments, record any symptoms or taken (tick) possible side effects experienced 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 Well done, two thirds there! 94 Day number Date Tablet Comments, record any symptoms or taken (tick) possible side effects experienced 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 END OF TRIAL! 95 Appendix II: Human research ethics committee approval SOUTHERN CROSS UNIVERSITY ~ MEMORANDUM ~ To: J. Stevens/A. Morgan NHCP jstevens@scu.edu.au amorgan@scu.edu.au From: Secretary, Human Research Ethics Committee Date: 17 September 2004 Project: Does Bacopa monnieri improve cognitive function in Age Associated memory Impairment (AAMI) Status: Protocol change and renewal of project. New Approval Number ECN-04-141 HUMAN RESEARCH ETHICS COMMITTEE (HREC) At the meeting of the HREC on 13 September, the change of protocol to your project to the population of participants has been noted by the Committee. The renewal of this project has also been approved and a new approval number issued. In the Informed Consent information, you do not require the statement about parent or guardian if subject is under 18 years of age, as the population is the ‘elderly. Please correct this Informed Consent carefully before it is given to participants. 96 The approval is subject to the standard conditions of approval below. Standard Conditions (in accordance with National Health and Medical Research Council Act 1992 and the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Research Involving Humans): 1. That the person responsible (usually the Supervisor) provide a report every 12 months during the conduct of the research project specifically including: 2. (a) The security of the records (b) Compliance with the approved consents procedures and documentation (c) Compliance with other special conditions. That the person responsible and/or associates report and present to the Committee for approval any change in protocol or when the project has been completed. 3. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately anything that might affect ethical acceptance of the research protocol. 4. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately any adverse effects on participants. 5. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately any unforeseen events that might affect continued ethical acceptability of the project. 6. That subjects be advised in writing that: “Any complaints or queries regarding this project that cannot be answered by the person responsible for this research project should be forwarded to: Ms Suze Kelly, Secretary, HREC. Graduate Research College, Southern Cross University PO Box 157, Lismore, 2480 Ph: (02) 6626 9139 Fax: (02) 6626 9145 Email: skelly1@scu.edu.au” 97 Suzanne Kelly Secretary, HREC (02) 6626 9139 skelly1@scu.edu.au Dr Baden Offord The Chair, HREC Ph: (02) 6620 3162 rofford@scu.edu.au 98 Appendix III: Participant consent form and information sheet PARTICIPANT INFORMATION SHEET A clinical study assessing the effects of Bacopa monnieri on memory performance in older people Purpose of Study: The School of Nursing and Health Care Practices invites you to participate in a study to assess whether a herbal medicine called Bacopa monnieri improves memory in people over the age of 55 years. This study is being carried out by Annette Morgan, as part of a Masters of Health Science research thesis through Southern Cross University. The supervisor of the research is Dr John Stevens, Lecturer, School of Nursing, Southern Cross University. Approval for the study has been given by Southern Cross University’s Human Research Ethics Committee. Aims of the Study: Bacopa monnieri, popularly known as Brahmi, is a herb with an ancient reputation of improving mental functions such as memory and concentration, and it is used as a general ‘brain tonic’. We are interested to see whether this herb will improve aspects of memory in the over 55 age group, thereby improving quality of life. Procedures to be followed: The study will involve three one hour visits to the Southern Cross University Natural Medicine Clinic located at 6 Industry Drive, East Lismore. The first visit will consist of an initial consultation with the researcher, who will take a brief medical history and perform brief screening of memory, mood and cognition. If 99 you meet the entry criteria of the study you will be booked in for a more thorough memory assessment with a research assistant trained in psychology. Participants will be randomly allocated into two groups. Following the first memory assessment (second visit) which provides a baseline measure of memory function, one group will be given Bacopa monnieri tablets, and the second group will be given identical placebo tablets. Dosage is one tablet daily after food for 12 weeks. Neither participants nor the researchers will know which participants are on Bacopa or placebo tablets, until the end of the trial. At the end of week 12, participants will come to the university for a third and final visit to have the same memory assessment repeated. Scores obtained in the two memory assessments will be analysed. This should provide an objective assessment of the efficacy of the herb. Possible discomforts and risks Bacopa monnieri is approved for over the counter sale in Australia, and is freely and widely available in retail outlets. It is a well researched herb, with a long history of traditional usage and has an excellent safety profile. One four week clinical trial to assess safety demonstrated an absence of side effects. Thus, it is unlikely that you will experience any adverse effects. It is recommended that the tablets be taken after food. Although it is unlikely that you will experience any adverse effects from taking Bacopa, please discontinue usage in the event any occur and notify the study co-ordinator immediately. Confidentiality: The researchers accept responsibility to maintain anonymity and confidentiality, and will not disclose any information that will identify participants. All data obtained will be stored in a locked cupboard in offices of the researcher at Southern Cross University. Only those directly involved in the research will have access to the data. 100 Freedom of Consent: If you agree to participate, you are free to withdraw your consent and to discontinue participation at any time. However, informing us of your decision would be appreciated. Inquiries: We are available to answer any questions or comments you may have at any time. Please contact: Study Co-ordinator: Ms Annette Morgan ph: (02) 6620 3155, email: amorgan@scu.edu.au If unavailable, please contact: Study Supervisor: Dr John Stevens: (02) 6620 3306 Any complaints or queries regarding this research that cannot be answered by the researchers (above) should be forwarded to: Mr John Russell Graduate Research College Southern Cross University Ph: (02) 6620 3705, Fax: (02) 6626 9145, email: jrussell@scu.edu.au 101 PARTICIPANT CONSENT FORM Name of Project: ‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older people?’ I have read the information sheet and agree to participate in this study. Name of Participant: ____________________________________________ Signature of Participant: _________________________________________ Date: _________________________________________________________ Name of witness (who shall be independent of the project) I,____________________________________________________ certify that the terms of the form have been verbally explained to the subject, that the subject appears to understand the terms prior to signing the form, and that proper arrangements have been made for an interpreter where English is not the subject's first language. On advice given I asked the subject if she/he needed to discuss the project with an independent person before signing and she/he declined (or has done so). Signature of Witness Date___________ I certify that the terms of the form have been verbally explained to the participant, that the participant appears to understand the terms prior to signing the form, and that proper arrangements have been made for an interpreter where English is not the participants first language. Signature of Researcher: __________________________Date: _______________ 102 Appendix IV: Clinical report form Master of Health Science Research Thesis ‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older people?’ PARTICIPANT DATA BOOKLET Participant code:______________________ Annette Morgan and Dr John Stevens Southern Cross University School of Nursing & Health Care Practices 103 CLINICAL REPORT FORM 1 Demographic Information Name Date Address Phone DOB Age (yrs) Marital status Gender Education (yrs) Occupation 2 Medical History Head Injury/ Infection/ Inflammation Neurological disorder TIA Stroke Psychiatric Illness Cardiovascular disease Renal disease Hepatic disease Diabetes Haematological disorder Endocrine illness Malignancy Psychotropic drug use Alcohol / drug use Other Illness 3 Current medications, herbal & nutritional supplements 4 Physical examination BP Pulse Respiratory rate Temperature Urine Analysis Notes 104 5. Modified Hachinski Scale (Rosen et al., 1980) Characteristic Point score Abrupt onset of symptoms 2 Stepwise deterioration 1 Somatic complaints 1 Emotional incontinence 1 Hypertension history / current 1 Stroke history 2 Focal neurological symptoms 2 Focal neurological signs 2 Total score= (Score of 4 or more suggests multi-infarct dementia) 6. Current memory/cognitive difficulties experienced: Memory loss Forgets recent events Forgets things just said Forgets names of people Forgets words Gets lost Asks questions or tells story repeatedly Confused about date or place Cant do simple calculations Cant understand what is read or said Impairment of other cognitive functions Anxiety or agitation Paranoia Delusions or hallucinations Wandering Disruptive behaviour Incontinence 105 7. The Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) NAME: ________________________________ DATE: ___________________ Orientation: (10 points) What is the year? What is the season? What day of the week is it? What is the month? Can you tell me where we are? (residence or street name) What city/ town are we in? What state are we in? What country are we in? What are the names of two streets nearby? What floor of the building are we on? Registration: (3 points) I am going to name three objects. After I have said them, I want you to repeat them. Remember what they are because I am going to ask you to repeat them again in a few minutes. ‘Apple.....Table......Penny’ Attention and calculation: (5 points) Can you subtract 7 from 100, and then subtract 7 from the answer you get and keep subtracting 7 until I ask you to stop? Now I am going to spell a word forwards and I want you to spell it backwards. The word is WORLD. W-O-R-L-D. Recall: (3 points) Now, what were the three objects I asked you to remember? Language: (9 points) What is this called (show watch) What is this called (show pencil) Now I would like you to repeat a phrase after me: ‘No ifs and or buts’ Read the words on this page then do what it says. (The page says in large letters ‘CLOSE YOUR EYES’) Take this paper in your right hand, fold the paper in half using both hands and put the paper down using your left hand Pick up the paper and write a short sentence on it. (Sentence must have a subject and a verb and make sense) Now copy the design that you see printed on the page. (Design is interlocking pentagons. The result must have five-sided figures with intersection forming a four sided figure). Score 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 3 2 3 3 1 1 1 1 3 1 1 TOTAL: 30 106 CLOSE YOUR EYES WRITE A SENTENCE: 107 8. The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression Ref: Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979; adapted by GlaxoWellcome, 1997 Participant Name:_________________________________________________ Date of Assessment_______________________________________________ For each item, write the correct score adjacent to item (only one response per item) Score ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ 1. DEPRESSED MOOD 0= Absent 1= Indicated only on questioning 2= Spontaneously reported 3= Communicates non-verbally through facial expression, posture, voice, tendency to weep 4= Reports virtually only these feeling states in spontaneous verbal and non-verbal expression 2. FEELINGS OF GUILT 0= Absent 1= Self reproach, feels he has let people down 2= Ideas of guilt or rumination over past errors or sinful deeds 3= Sees present illness as a punishment. Delusions of guilt. 4= Hears accusatory or denunciatory voices and/or experiences threatening visual hallucinations 3. SUICIDE 0= Absent 1= Feels life is not worth living 2= Wishes he were dead, or any thoughts of possible death to self 3= Suicidal ideas or gesture 4= Attempts at suicide 4. INSOMNIA EARLY 0= No difficulty in falling asleep 1= Complains of occasional difficulty falling asleep- more than ½ hour 2= Complains of nightly difficulty falling asleep 5. INSOMNIA MIDDLE 0= No difficulty 1= Complains of being restless and disturbed during the night 2= Waking during night 6. INSOMNIA LATE 0= No difficulty 1= Waking in early hours of morning but goes back to sleep 2= Unable to fall asleep again if gets out of bed 108 ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ 7. WORK AND ACTIVITIES 0= No difficulty 1= Thoughts and feelings of incapacity, fatigue or weakness related to activities, work or hobbies 2= Loss of interest in activities, hobbies or work 3= Decrease in actual time spent in activities or decrease in productivity 4= Stopped work because of current illness/feeling state 8. RETARDATION: PSYCHOMOTOR (Slowness of thought and impaired ability to concentrate; decreased motor activity) 0= Normal speech and thought 1= Slight retardation at interview 2= Obvious retardation at interview 3= Interview difficult 4= Complete stupor 9. AGITATION 0= None 1= Fidgetiness 2= Playing with hands, hair, etc. 3= Moving about, can’t sit still 4= Hand wringing, nail biting, hair-pulling, biting of lips 10. ANXIETY (PSYCHOLOGICAL) 0= No difficulty 1= Subjective tension and irritability 2= Worrying about minor matters 3= Apprehensive attitude apparent in face or speech 4= Fears expressed without questioning 11. ANXIETY (SOMATIC) 0= Absent 1= Mild 2= Moderate 3= Severe 4= Incapacitating 12. SOMATIC SYMPTOMS (GASTROINTESTINAL) 0= None 1= Loss of appetite but eats without encouragement 2= Difficulty eating without encouragement, reduction of food intake 13. SOMATIC SYMPTOMS (GENERAL) 0= None 1= Heaviness in limbs, back, head. Aching, low energy, fatigability. 2= Any clear cut symptoms 14. GENITAL SYMPTOMS (loss of libido, impaired sexual performance, menstrual disturbances) 0= Absent 1= Mild 2= Severe 109 ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ ______ 15. HYPOCHONDRIASIS 0= Not present 1= Self-absorption (bodily) 2= Preoccupation with health 3= Frequent complaints, requests for help etc 4= Hypochondriacal delusions 16. LOSS OF WEIGHT 0= Not present 1= Probably weight loss associated with present illness 2= Definite weight loss reported 3= Not assessed 17. INSIGHT 0= Acknowledges being depressed and ill 1= Acknowledges illness but attributes cause to bad food, climate, over-work, virus etc. 2= Denies being ill at all 18. DIURNAL VARIATION A. Note whether symptoms are worse morning or evening 0= No variation 1= Worse in A.M. 2= Worse in P.M. B. When present, mark the severity of the variation 0= None 1= Mild 2= Severe 19. DEPERSONALISATION AND DEREALIZATION (Such as feelings of unreality; nihilistic ideas) 0= Absent 1= Mild 2= Moderate 3= Severe 4= Incapacitating 20. PARANOID SYMPTOMS 0= None 1= Suspicious 2= Ideas of reference 3= Delusions of reference and persecution 21. OBSESSIONAL AND COMPULSIVE SYMPTOMS 0= Absent 1= Mild 2= Severe Total Score ___________ 110 Appendix V: Neuropsychological test administration protocol Timing of Test Administration 1. Complex Figure Test Copy Task (5-minutes) 2. Memory complaint questionnaire (MAC-Q) (3-minutes only) 3. Complex Figure Test 3-minute recall (5-minutes - unlimited) 4. RAVLT trials A1- A5, B1 and A6 (10-minutes) 5. Complete and/or discuss MAC-Q (10-minutes) 6. Trail A and Trail B 7. Complex Figure Test 30-minute Recall Trial (10-minutes) 8. Complex Figure Test Recognition Task 9. RAVLT Recall Trial = A7 (3-minutes) 10. RAVLT Recognition Trial (5-minutes) RAVLT abbreviations • Repeated words= R • Repeated & self-corrected= RC • Questions whether repeated but unsure= RQ • Words not on list are errors= E 111 Appendix VI: Specification sheets for Bacopa and placebo tablets 112 113 114 Appendix VII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) AVLT 1st Administration- baseline assessment (Lezak et al., 2004:422-426) NAME: __________________________________DATE: ______________________ LIST A 1 2 3 4 5 LIST B DRUM DESK CURTAIN RANGER BELL BIRD COFFEE SHOE SCHOOL STOVE PARENT MOUNTAIN MOON GLASSES GARDEN TOWEL HAT CLOUD FARMER BOAT NOSE LAMB TURKEY GUN COLOUR PENCIL HOUSE CHURCH RIVER FISH TOTAL TOTAL B A 20 ′ Rec 115 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recognition Trial (1st administration) The participant must respond Yes / No to each word that is read out according to whether they think the word was on list A. The examiner circles every word that has a Yes response. ANSWERS Bell (A) Home (sa) Towel (B) Boat (B) Glasses (B) Window (sa) Fish (B) Curtain (A) Hot (pa) Stocking (sb) Hat (A) Moon(A) Flower Parent (A) Shoe (B) Barn (sa) Tree (pa) Colour (A) Water (sa) Teacher (sa) Ranger (B) Balloon (pa) Desk (B) Farmer (A) Stove (B) Nose (A) Bird (B) Gun (B) Rose (spa) Nest (spa) Weather (sb) Mountain (B) Crayon (sa) Cloud (B) Children (sa) School (A) Coffee (A) Church (B) House (A) Drum (A) Hand (pa) Mouse (pa) Turkey (A) Stranger (pb) Toffee (pa) Pencil (B) River (A) Fountain (pb) Garden (A) Lamb (B) KEY List A words = ________(A) List B words = (B) s = word with a semantic association to a word on List A or List B p = word phonemically similar to a word on List A or List B 116 Appendix VIII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) alternate form for endpoint assessment AVLT 2nd administration: week-12 assessment (alternate word list by JonesGotman, Szilkas & Majdan, cited in Lezak et al., 2004: 423) PATIENT NAME: ______________________________DATE:_________________ LIST A 1 2 3 4 5 LIST B VIOLIN ORANGE TREE ARMCHAIR SCARF TOAD HAM CORK SUITCASE BUS COUSIN CHIN EARTH BEACH KNIFE SOAP STAIR HOTEL DOG DONKEY BANANA SPIDER RADIO BATHROOM BUCKET CASSEROLE HUNTER SOLDIER FIELD LOCK TOTAL TOTAL B A 20 ′ Rec 117 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recognition Trial (2nd administration- week 12) The participant must respond Yes / No to each word that is read out according to whether they think the word was on list A. The examiner circles every word that has a Yes response. ANSWERS Rock (pb) Star (sa) Soap (B) Television (sa) Violin (A) Corn (pb) Peel (sa) Frog (sb) Hotel (B) Beach (B) Pear (sa) Lock (B) Dog (A) Piano (sa) Radio (A) Tree (A) Banana (A) Orange (B) Spider (B) Bus (B) Cork (B) Toad (B) Cousin (A) Bucket (A) Doctor Bread Uncle (sa) Bathroom(B) Soldier Chest Sofa (sb) Earth (A) Gloves (sb) Scarf (A) Knife (A) Stair (A) Hospital (sb) Field (A) Wife (sa) Donkey (B) Ham (A) Grass (sa) Armchair (B) Train (sb) Hunter (A) Casserole (B) Lunchbox (sb) Blanket (pa) Suitcase (A) Chin (B) KEY List A words = ________(A) List B words = (B) s = word with a semantic association to a word on List A or List B p = word phonemically similar to a word on List A or List B 118 Appendix IX: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) Ref: adapted from Osterrieth, 1944 cited in Lezak et al., 2004. 119 Appendix X: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) marking Sheet Ref: Scoring system for CFT taken from Taylor 1959, adapted from Osterrieth, 1944 cited in Lezak et al., 2004. Units COPY IMMEDIATE 30 MINUTE RECALL RECALL 1. Cross, upper left corner, outside of rectangle 2. Large rectangle 3. Diagonal cross 4. Horizontal midline of 2 5. Vertical midline 6. Small rectangle, within 2 to the left 7. Small segment above 6 8. Four parallel lines within 2, upper left 9. Triangle above 2, upper right 10. Small vertical line within 2, below 9 11. Circle with 3 dots, within 2 12. Five parallel lines within 2 crossing 3, lower right 13. Sides of triangle attached to 2 on right 14. Diamond attached to 13 15. Vertical line within triangle 13 parallel to right vertical of 2 16. Horizontal line within 13, continuing 4 to right 17. Cross attached to 5 below 2 18. Square attached to 2, lower left TOTAL SCORE (out of 36) Scoring: Consider each of the units separately. Appraise accuracy of each unit and relative position within the whole of the design. For each unit count as follows: Correct Distorted or incomplete but recognisable Absent or not recognisable Maximum Placed properly Placed poorly 2 points 1 point Placed properly Placed poorly 1 points ½ point 0 points 36points 120 CFT scoring sheet continued. Use figure below to guide scoring. 121 Appendix XI: Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B Ref: Reitan, 1958; test instructions from Spreen & Strauss, 1991. TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART A NAME: ............................................................................ DATE: ............................................................................. Sample: join numbers together in correct order without lifting pen from the page, and in fastest time possible; e.g. 1 to 2 to 3 and so on until finished. End 7 2 8 Begin 4 1 3 6 5 122 TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART A 123 TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART B NAME: ........................................................................... DATE: ............................................................................ Sample: join numbers and letters together in correct order without lifting pen from the page, and in fastest time possible; e.g. from 1 to A to 2 to B to 3 to C and so on until finished End 4 A D Begin B 1 2 C 3 124 TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART B 125 Appendix XII: Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q) From: Crook et al., 1992 Name: ....................................................................Date: ................................................. As compared to when you were in school, how would you describe your ability to perform the following tasks involving you memory? Much Somewhat About Somewhat Much better now better now the same poorer now poorer now (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) 1. Remembering the name of a person just introduced to you 2. Recalling telephone numbers or postcodes that you use on a daily or weekly basis 3. Recalling where you have put objects (such as keys) in your home or office 4. Remembering specific facts from a newspaper or magazine you have just finished reading 5. Remembering the items you intended to buy when you arrive at the grocery store or pharmacy 6. In general, how would you describe your memory compared to when you were in high school TOTAL SCORE_______________________ 126 Appendix XIII: Results of repeated measures analysis for all variables TASK AVLT a1 AVLT a2 AVLT a3 AVLT a4 AVLT a5 AVLT b (interference list) AVLT a6 AVLT a7 (delayed recall) AVLT recognition hit rate AVLT recognition false positives AVLT true recognition rate AVLT total learning a1-a5 AVLT retroactive interference AVLT proactive interference AVLT forgetting rate CFT copy CFT 3min recall CFT 30min recall MACQ Trail Making Test A Trail Making Test B F 1.823 .894 2.348 13.204 6.094 .143 18.830 12.021 1.242 2.555 3.539 6.761 4.020 1.353 .365 .649 1.101 1.887 2.525 .038 1.280 df 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 Error df 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 79 Sig. .181 .347 .129 .000 .016 .706 .000 .001 .269 .114 .064 .011 .048 .248 .547 .423 .297 .173 .116 .847 .261 General Linear Model, Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance employing time as within subjects factor and group as between subjects factor was used. F= Fisher value for significance of group contrasts. df =degrees of freedom for the two treatment groups, error df=degrees of freedom for error. Sig.=one-tailed significance. AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, AVLTa1-a7 = repetitions of word list A (possible range 0-15), AVLT b = interference word list B (possible range 0-15), AVLT rec = recognition list hit rate (possible range 0-15) , AVLT recognition false positives (possible range 0-35), AVLT true recognition rate = recognition list hit rate minus false positives, (possible range -35 to +15) , AVLT total learning a1-a5 = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, range 0-75), AVLT retroactive interference score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance, ), AVLT proactive interference score (trial a1 minus trial b, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance), AVLT forgetting rate (trial a6 minus trial a7, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance); CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (possible range 0-36 on all tasks), CFTcopy=copy task, CFT 3-minute recall task, CFT 30-minute recall task; MAC-Q = Memory Complaint Questionnaire (possible range 7-35, lower scores = better performance); Trail Making Test A and B scores=time taken to complete task (in seconds). 127