peer reviewed published articles

Transcription

peer reviewed published articles
PEER REVIEWED PUBLISHED
ARTICLES
2009
Isabel Healthcare Inc
3120 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 200, Falls Church, VA 22042
T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
LIST OF PEER REVIEWED PUBLISHED ARTICLES
2009
1. HOW ACCURATELY DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS DECISION
SUPPORT SYSTEM SUGGEST THE CORRECT DIAGNOSIS IN ADULT
ER AND PEDIATRIC CASES ?
a. Ramnarayan P, Natalie Cronje, Ruth Brown et al. Validation of a
diagnostic reminder system in emergency medicine: a multi-centre
study. Emergency Medicine Journal 2007;24:619-624;
doi:10.1136/emj.2006.044107
b. Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, Coren M, Winrow A, Britto
J. ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic aid for pediatrics:
results from an initial performance evaluation. Arch Dis Child. 2003
May;88 (5):408-13.
2. WHAT IS THE IMPACT ON CHANGE OF DIAGNOSIS DECISION
MAKING BEFORE AND AFTER USING A DIAGNOSIS DECISION
SUPPORT SYSTEM?
a. Ramnarayan P, Winrow A, Coren M, Nanduri V, Buchdahl R, Jacobs
B, Fisher H, Taylor PM, Wyatt JC, Britto JF. Diagnostic omission
errors in acute paediatric practice: impact of a reminder system on
decision-making. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2006 Nov 6;6(1):37
b. Ramnarayan P, Roberts GC, Coren M et al. Assessment of the
potential impact of a reminder system on the reduction of
diagnostic errors: a quasi-experimental study. BMC Med Inform
Decis Mak. 2006 Apr 28;6:22
3. HOW ACCURATELY DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS DECISION
SUPPORT SYSTEM SUGGEST THE CORRECT DIAGNOSIS IN 50
ADULT NEJM CPC CASES?
Graber ML, Mathews A. Performance of a web-based clinical
diagnosis support system for internists. J Gen Intern Med. 2008
Jan;23 Suppl 1:85-7.
4. HOW DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS DECISION SUPPORT
SYSTEM IMPROVE DIAGNOSIS ACCURACY IN A CRITICAL CARE
SETTING ?
Thomas NJ, Ramnarayan P, Bell P Et al. An international
assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children.
Technology and Health Care 16 (2008) 103–110.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
3120 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 200, Falls Church, VA 22042
T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
LIST OF PEER REVIEWED PUBLISHED ARTICLES
2009
5. WHAT IS THE MAGNITUDE OF DIAGNOSIS ERROR?
Gordon D. Schiff, Seijeoung Kim,Richard Abrams et al. Diagnosing
Diagnosis Errors: Lessons from a Multi-institutional Collaborative
Project. Advances in Patient Safety 2005; 2:255-278
6. WHAT ARE THE PRIMARY & SECONDARY OUTCOMES OF
DIAGNOSTIC PROCESS - THE LEADING PHASE OF WORK IN
PATIENT CARE MANAGEMENT PROBLEMS IN EMERGENCY
DEPARTMENT ?
Cosby KS, Roberts R, Palivos L et al. Characteristics of Patient Care
Management Problems Identified in Emergency Department
Morbidity and Mortality Investigations During 15 Years. Ann Emerg
Med. 2008;51:251-261.
7. WHAT ARE THE CAUSES OF MISSED AND DELAYED DIAGNOSES IN
THE AMBULATORY SETTING?
Gandhi TK, Kachalia A, Thomas EJ et al. Missed and Delayed
Diagnoses in the Ambulatory Setting: A Study of Closed Malpractice
Claims. Ann Intern Med.2006;145:488-496.
8. WHAT ARE THE FACTORS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO DIAGNOSIS
ERROR?
a. Mark Graber, MD et al. Diagnostic Error in internal Medicine. Arch
Intern Med. 2005 Jul 11; 165(13):1493-9
b. Mark Graber MD. Diagnostic errors in medicine: a case of neglect.
Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf. 2005 Feb;31(2):106-13
9. DO PHYSICIANS KNOW WHEN THEIR DIAGNOSES ARE CORRECT?
Charles P. Friedman, PhD et al. Do Physicians Know When Their
Diagnoses Are Correct? Implications for Decision Support and Error
Reduction. J Gen Intern Med 2005; 20:334-9.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Isabel Healthcare Inc
3120 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 200, Falls Church, VA 22042
T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
HOW ACCURATELY DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS
DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEM SUGGEST THE
CORRECT DIAGNOSIS IN ADULT ER AND
PEDIATRIC CASES?
Ramnarayan P, Natalie Cronje, Ruth Brown et al.
Validation of a diagnostic reminder system in emergency medicine:
a multi-centre study.
Emergency Medicine Journal 2007;24:619-624; doi:10.1136/emj.2006.044107
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
619
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Validation of a diagnostic reminder system in emergency
medicine: a multi-centre study
Padmanabhan Ramnarayan, Natalie Cronje, Ruth Brown, Rupert Negus, Bill Coode, Philip Moss, Taj
Hassan, Wayne Hamer, Joseph Britto
...................................................................................................................................
Emerg Med J 2007;24:619–624. doi: 10.1136/emj.2006.044107
Information on the Isabel
system can be found at
www.isabelhealthcare.com
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
........................
Correspondence to:
Padmanabhan Ramnarayan,
44 B Bedford Row,
Children’s Acute Transport
Service, London WC1R 4LL,
UK; ram@isabelhealthcare.
com
Accepted 22 March 2007
........................
E
Background: Diagnostic error is a significant problem in emergency medicine, where initial clinical
assessment and decision making is often based on incomplete clinical information. Traditional computerised
diagnostic systems have been of limited use in the acute setting, mainly due to the need for lengthy system
consultation. We evaluated a novel web-based reminder system, which provides rapid diagnostic advice to
users based on free text search terms.
Methods: Clinical data collected from patients presenting to three emergency departments with acute medical
problems were entered into the diagnostic system. The displayed results were assessed against the final
discharge diagnoses for patients who were admitted to hospital (diagnostic accuracy) and against a set of
‘‘appropriate’’ diagnoses for each case provided by an expert panel (potential utility).
Results: Data were collected from 594 patients (53.4% of screened attendances). Mean age was 49.4 years
(95% CI 47.7 to 51.1) and the majority had significant past illnesses. Most were assessed first by junior
doctors (70%) and 266/594 (44.6%) were admitted to hospital. Overall, the diagnostic system displayed the
final discharge diagnosis in 95% of inpatients and 90% of ‘‘must-not-miss’’ diagnoses suggested by the expert
panel. The discharge diagnosis appeared within the first 10 suggestions in 78% of cases.
Conclusions: The Isabel diagnostic aid has been shown to be of potential use in reminding junior doctors of
key diagnoses in the emergency department. The effects of its widespread use on decision making and
diagnostic error can be clarified by evaluating its impact on routine clinical decision making.
mergency departments (EDs) have been shown to be high
risk clinical areas for the occurrence of medical adverse
events.1 2 In contrast to the inpatient setting where
diagnostic delays and missed diagnoses account for only 10–
15% of adverse events, diagnostic errors are more frequent and
assume greater significance in primary care, emergency
medicine and critical care, resulting in a large number of
successful negligence claims.3–6 Various reasons may account
for this difference. In EDs, acute clinical presentations are often
characterised by incomplete and poor quality information at
initial assessment leading to considerable diagnostic uncertainty. Systematic factors such as frequent shift changes,
overwork, time pressure and high patient throughput may
further contribute to diagnostic errors.7 Cognitive biases
inherent within diagnostic reasoning have also been shown to
play a crucial role in perpetuating patient safety breaches
during diagnostic assessment.8 9 Despite easier access to health
information for the public through NHS Direct and NHS
Online, the demand for emergency care is steadily rising,10 and
it is possible that greater pressure on emergency staff to cut
waiting times and increase efficiency with limited resources
will lead to a higher incidence of patient safety incidents during
diagnostic assessment.
The use of computerised diagnostic decision support systems
(DDSS) has been proposed as a technological solution to
minimise diagnostic error.11 A number of DDSS have been
developed over the past few decades, but most have not shown
promise in EDs, either because they focussed on a narrow
clinical problem (eg, chest pain) or because computerised aids
intended for general use were used only for consultation in rare
and complex diagnostic dilemmas. In order to enter complete
clinical data into these systems using system-specific medical
terminology, considerable user motivation and time was
required (often 20–40 min of data entry time).12 Isabel
(www.isabelhealthcare.com) is a novel DDSS which was
primarily developed for acute paediatrics. Isabel users enter
their search terms in natural language free text, and are shown
a list of diagnostic suggestions (up to a maximum of 30,
displayed on three consecutive pages, 10 diagnoses per page),
which are arranged by body system (eg, cardiovascular,
gastrointestinal) rather than by clinical probability.13 14 These
suggestions are intended only as reminders to prompt clinicians
to consider them in their diagnostic investigation, not as
‘‘correct’’ choices or ‘‘likely’’ diagnoses. The system uses
statistical natural language processing techniques to search
an underlying knowledge base containing textual descriptions
of .10 000 diseases. In clinical trials performed in acute
paediatrics, mean Isabel usage time was ,3 min, and the
system reminded junior doctors to consider clinically significant
diagnoses in 12.5% of cases.15 The DDSS was extended to cover
adult disease conditions in January 2005.16
Although the extended Isabel system was closely modelled
on the paediatric version, a large scale validation of the newly
developed system was felt necessary to establish its diagnostic
accuracy and potential utility for a range of clinical scenarios
among adult patients in EDs. This was especially important
since acute presentations in adults differ significantly from
those in children. Adults may have multiple pre-morbid
conditions that confound their acute illness. Further, the
spectrum of diseases encountered is quite distinct, and the
relative importance of diagnoses during initial assessment may
be different from that in paediatric cases. It was also postulated
that the greater amount of clinical detail available on adults
presenting to EDs may prolong data entry time. In addition,
since the Isabel system relies on extracting key concepts from
Abbreviations: DDSS, diagnostic decision support systems; ED,
emergency department; RA, research assistant
www.emjonline.com
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
620
Ramnarayan, Cronje, Brown, et al
Table 1 Characteristics of participating emergency
departments
Nature
Annual attendances
(majors)
Inpatient admission rate
Clinical decision unit
Number of consultants
Centre A
Centre B
Centre C
Overall
Teaching
74 000
Teaching
80 000
Teaching
44 000
198 000
16%
Yes
5
29.8%
Yes
5
19%
No
4
21.6%
its knowledge base (textbooks and journal articles), variations
in natural language textual patterns in the medical sources
used for the adult system may significantly influence its
diagnostic suggestions.
METHODS
This preliminary validation study was designed purely to
examine the clinical performance of the Isabel system and
identify its potential utility in adult EDs, not to assess its impact
on clinical practice or diagnostic errors. Therefore, clinicians
were not allowed real-time access to the system during patient
assessment in this study. Clinical data from ED patients
presenting with a range of acute medical problems were used
to validate the results of the DDSS. The study was approved by
the London multi-regional ethics committee (04/MREC02/41)
and relevant local research governance committees.
Study centres
A convenience sample of four UK EDs were selected for data
collection during the study. Due to significant delay in
completing research governance procedures at one centre, data
were finally only collected from three participating sites. The
characteristics of the study sites are summarised in table 1.
Study patient data
Data were collected from all consecutive patients over 16 years
old presenting to the ‘‘majors area’’ or resuscitation rooms with
an acute medical problem. Patients presenting to ‘‘minors’’ or a
similar area, patients with surgical complaints (including
trauma, orthopaedics, ENT, ophthalmology and gynaecology),
post-operative surgical problems, psychiatric problems (including substance and alcohol abuse) and complaints directly
related to pregnancy were excluded. Patients presenting for
reassessment of the same clinical problem within a week of an
earlier visit to the ED were also excluded.
Study procedure
This study was a prospective, multi-centre observational study
utilising medical case note review. No interventions were
performed on patients.
Screening for eligibility
The attendance records of all consecutive patients presenting to
an ED ‘‘majors area’’ within a pre-designated 2-week period
were screened for eligibility by the primary research assistant
(RA). Screening was performed at study sites one after the
other, ie, the 2-week period was different for each centre. The
complete medical and nursing notes of patients with presenting
complaints that fitted study criteria were selected for further
review. Reasons for exclusion were recorded for the remainder
using specific codes established a priori. Where the RA was
unsure of study eligibility, patients were included and data
were collected. Such notes were reviewed at regular intervals by
the principal investigator, and a final decision regarding study
eligibility was made. To assess inter-rater reliability during
www.emjonline.com
Table 2
panel
Template of patient summary report provided to
Patient characteristics
Age, gender, ethnic origin
Clinical presentation
Registering complaint(s)
Triage category
Vital signs at triage
Presenting clinical symptoms
Relevant co-morbidities and family history
Current medications
Positive clinical signs
Initial tests performed and results (if available)
screening for eligibility, a second RA examined patient notes
from two randomly chosen dates within the specified 2-week
period at each study ED. The primary RA was blinded to these
dates. Concordance between the two RAs was calculated using
the kappa statistic (k 0.65, 95% CI 0.62 to 0.68).
Data collection
From eligible patient notes, the primary RA extracted data
regarding patient details; date and time of patient assessment;
details of clinical presentation such as symptoms, past medical
and family history; examination findings, tests performed and
results available at the end of complete assessment by the first
examining clinician; differential diagnosis and management
plan of the first examining clinician; referral for specialist or
senior opinion; and outcome of ED assessment. This was
entered directly into an Access database (Microsoft, Reading,
UK) by means of pre-designed electronic forms. During data
entry, both positive and negative symptoms, signs and test
results were collected as recorded in the patient notes.
Following complete data collection at all centres, final
discharge diagnoses for patients at the end of ED assessment,
recorded either on discharge letters or on ED electronic systems,
were ascertained. For patients admitted as inpatients, final
primary diagnoses at hospital discharge were obtained from
hospital electronic coding systems.
Data quality assurance was achieved by multiple means
including: use of a training manual created before study
commencement containing standardised case examples to
practise screening, data collection and abstraction; the use of
25 medical records to practise data collection, with doubts
being clarified by the study investigator at study outset; weekly
meetings to discuss data abstraction issues and examine
collected data; and a log of discussions for ready reference.
Reliability of the data collection process was established by
randomly assigning 50% of eligible patient notes from two
randomly chosen dates to a second RA. Concordance for key
variables was analysed using the kappa statistic (k 0.58, 95% CI
0.52 to 0.64).
Expert panel
An expert panel was set up at each study site consisting of two
consultants (attending physicians), which met regularly to
provide gold standard diagnoses for a randomly selected subset
of study patients. At each panel meeting, moderated by the
primary RA, data collected during initial ED assessment for
each patient were provided in the form of a pre-formatted
clinical summary report generated from the Access database
(table 2).
Presenting clinical symptoms were provided as recorded in
the patient notes. Only relevant co-morbidities, family history,
positive clinical signs and results of initial tests (if performed by
the examining clinician) were provided. The panel were blinded
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
Validation of a diagnostic reminder system
621
Figure 1 Clinical data extracted from patient medical notes is entered into the diagnostic reminder system in free text natural language. Filters for age,
gender, pregnancy status and geographical region are selected as drop-down choices.
to the ED where the patient was assessed, clinical decisions
made and eventual patient outcome. The panel were instructed
to provide, for each case following mutual consensus, a set of
‘‘must-not-miss’’ diagnoses, defined as key diagnoses that
would influence patient management, ie, result in a specific
action, either eliciting further history or physical examination
or initiating new tests and/or treatments. To establish
concordance between the three expert panels, a random
selection of 5% of study patients was assigned to each panel
in blinded fashion. For this subset, gold standard diagnoses
were defined as those suggested by two or more panels.
DDSS data entry
Concurrent with the data collection process, an Isabel prototype
was created from the original paediatric version. In order to
generate a mature DDSS for use in adult patients, this prototype
needed refinement. A total of 130 notes randomly drawn
from patients not admitted to hospital were used for this final
fine-tuning (development set). In an iterative process, the
system’s results for each case were critically examined by
clinicians in the development team. To generate a focussed list
of diagnoses, each diagnosis in the Isabel database had to be
tagged to particular age group(s), gender and regions in which
the disease was commonly seen (paediatric tags could not be
used for adult patients). Once a fully developed Isabel system
was available, the remainder of the cases (464/594) were used
to test its performance (validation set).
During the validation stage, the RA exported the clinical
summary report (as presented to the panel) for each case from
the Access database in the form of individual text files. Since the
Isabel system accepted search terms only as text and negative
findings could not be searched within its medical content,
information from the patient summary report needed modification by the RA during data entry into Isabel. Patient characteristics (eg, age and gender) were input using a drop-down menu,
numerical values from vital signs and test results were converted
Figure 2 Diagnostic reminders are grouped under body system. Ten suggestions are displayed on the first page with an option to view more diagnoses.
Clicking RD leads directly to other Related Diagnoses.
www.emjonline.com
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
622
Ramnarayan, Cronje, Brown, et al
Table 3 Breakdown of patients screened and reasons for exclusion
Patient notes screened
Eligible patient notes (%)
Exclusions (%)
Trauma
Surgical/post-surgical
Pregnancy-related
Psychiatric/psychosomatic
Substance abuse
Orthopaedic
Incomplete data
Centre A
Centre B
Centre C
Total
344
200 (58.1)
144
50 (34.7)
15 (10.4)
18 (12.5)
13 (9.0)
15 (10.4)
2 (1.4)
31 (21.5)
403
194 (48.1)
209
96 (45.9)
10 (4.8)
14 (6.7)
12 (5.7)
23 (11.0)
19 (9.1)
35 (16.7)
366
200 (54.6)
166
47 (28.3)
20 (12.0)
25 (15.0)
12 (7.2)
21 (12.6)
5 (3.0)
36 (21.7)
1113
594
519
193
45
57
37
59
26
102
to text terms (eg, temperature 38.8˚C into ‘‘fever’’), and only
positive findings and test results (when available) were entered.
This procedure was standardised prior to data entry by establishing normal ranges for vital signs and test results. The patient
summary was aggregated into a single block of text with each
symptom, positive clinical finding, salient past illness and the
result of each test placed on a separate line, and then pasted into
the search box. Any amount of clinical information could be
entered, although it was accepted that specificity of search results
would improve with detailed data entry. Figure 1 illustrates this
procedure. In fig 2, results as displayed in Isabel are shown.
p Value
0.02
0.5
0.03
0.03
0.5
0.8
0.001
0.4
Outcome measures
Two separate outcome measures were assessed. Diagnostic
accuracy was used to provide an indication of the system’s
clinical performance and was defined as the proportion of cases
admitted to hospital in which the final discharge diagnosis
appeared among the DDSS suggestions. However, this did not
provide an indication of the system’s utility in terms of guiding
appropriate patient management in clinical practice. Therefore,
the proportion of cases in which the DDSS included the entire
set of key diagnoses that were deemed as ‘‘must-not-miss’’ by
the expert panel indicated its utility in an ED setting.
Table 4 Patient characteristics
Average age (years)
Male/female ratio
Mode of arrival (%)
Self
Ambulance
Other
Triage category (%)
Unknown
1
2
3
4
5
Examining clinician (%)
SHO (resident)
Registrar (fellow)
Consultant (attending)
Unclear/other
Time of assessment (%)
0800–1800
1800–0800
Significant co-morbidities
Significant family history
Primary assessment outcome (%)
Admit
Review after tests
Senior opinion
Specialist opinion
Discharge
Unclear/other
Final outcome (%)
Hospital admission
Discharge (no follow-up)
Discharge (GP follow-up)
Discharge (OP follow-up)
Transferred out
Other/unclear
Total
Centre A
Centre B
Centre C
Overall
47.57
1.06
50.5
0.83
50.23
1.08
49.4
0.99
95 (47.5)
100 (50.0)
5
98 (50.5)
96 (49.5)
0
101 (49.9)
96 (48.0)
3
294 (49.5)
292 (49.2)
8
0.92
7
4
30 (15.1)
110 (55.6)
44 (22.2)
3
0
3
18 (11.4)
98 (62.0)
38 (24.0)
1
2
4
53 (26.9)
97 (49.2)
41 (20.8)
0
9
11
101
305 (55.0)
123
4
0.93
,0.001
0.41
0.83
162 (81.0)
23
2
13
154 (79.4)
19
4
17
129 (64.5)
22
2
47
445 (74.9)
64
8
77
118 (59.0)
82
180
29
126 (64.9)
68
180
10
111 (55.5)
89
183
32
355 (59.8)
239
543
71
19 (9.5)
2
18
63
81
17
80 (41.2)
7
9
23
72
3
29 (14.5)
3
19
63
82
4
128
12
46
149
235
24
80 (40.0)
45
43
13
6
13
200
101 (52.0)
90
NR
NR
NR
3
194
85 (42.5)
59
38
9
2
7
200
266 (44.8)
194
81
22
8
23
594
GP, general practitioner; NR, not recorded; OP, out patient.
www.emjonline.com
p Value
0.32
0.001
0.15
0.61
0.001
,0.001
0.16
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
Validation of a diagnostic reminder system
623
Table 5
Figure 3 Diagnostic accuracy plotted for final diagnosis as well as expert
panel ‘‘must-not-miss’’ diagnoses using 10, 20 and 30 results.
Sample size and statistical analysis
Using an estimated diagnostic accuracy of 85% (from the
paediatric system) and an acceptable error rate of 3.5%, data
were required from 450 patients to ensure adequate power.
Differences between study EDs were analysed using the x2 test
for proportions and ANOVA for continuous variables. Statistical
significance was set at p value ,0.05.
RESULTS
During the study period, 1113 consecutive patient notes were
screened for study eligibility at the three centres. A total of 489
patients were excluded by the RA and a further 30 patients
were judged as ineligible on secondary review (overall 46.6%).
Therefore, 594 medical notes were reviewed in detail. There
were significant differences between centres with respect to the
proportion of patient notes excluded and reasons for exclusion
(table 3).
Patient characteristics are summarised in table 4.
The mean age of patients included in the study was
49.4 years (95% CI 47.7 to 51.1) with an equal male to female
ratio. A significant number of eligible patients were brought
into EDs by ambulance (49.2%), and the majority were triaged
into level 3 (55%). Most patients were seen by an SHO in the
first instance (74.9%), and 40% were seen out of working hours
(1800–0800). The majority of patients had past medical
illnesses of note (91.4%). In addition, significant family history
was present in a number of patients (12%). The primary
examining clinician indicated having sought senior opinion in
7.7% and specialist opinion in nearly 25% of patients. Overall,
44.8% were admitted to hospital as inpatients, of whom 33%
were discharged without further follow-up arrangements.
Diagnostic accuracy was measured using 217 inpatient
discharge diagnoses available from 266 admissions. Overall,
the DDSS displayed 206/217 diagnoses, with an accuracy rate of
95% (CI 92% to 98%). Seventy eight per cent of the discharge
diagnoses were displayed on the first page (first ten suggestions).
Panel members examined 129 cases to provide ‘‘must-notmiss’’ diagnoses. A total of 30 cases were assessed by all three
expert panels and 99 others by a single panel. In the former set,
52 diagnoses were suggested (mean 1.7 per case). Isabel
displayed 50/52 suggestions (96%) in the list of its diagnostic
reminders, with the majority present on the first page (35/50,
70%). For the latter set, 100 ‘‘must-not-miss’’ diagnoses were
provided by the panel (mean 1 per case). Isabel displayed 90/
100 suggestions; 53/100 were present on the first page (first 10
Case assessment and ‘‘must-not-miss’’ diagnoses
Patient characteristics
69 years old, male, white, Irish
Clinical presentation
Registering complaint(s): abdominal pain
Triage category: 3
Vital signs at triage: temp 38.8, BP 108/65 mm Hg,
heart rate 120/min, respiratory rate 20 bpm, saturations
96% in room air, GCS 15/15
Presenting clinical symptoms
Fever
Rigors
Right iliac fossa pain radiating to central abdomen
Vomited
Nausea
Chronic blood per rectum
Reduced appetite
Constipated
Passing flatus
Chronic productive cough
Relevant co-morbidities and family history
Peripheral vascular disease
Diabetes mellitus
Ischaemic heart disease
Smoker
Current medications
Simvastatin
Metformin
Positive clinical signs
Bibasal crackles
Abdominal tenderness with percussion
Tender rectum
Pallor
Dehydrated
Fever
Tachycardia
Initial tests performed and results (if available)
Leucocytosis
High serum bilirubin
Elevated CRP
Sinus tachycardia on ECG
Thickened right colon, suspected mass on ultrasound scan abdomen
Panel assessment – ‘‘must not miss’’ diagnoses to consider
Acute appendicitis
Carcinoma colon
Ischaemic bowel
reminders). Comprehensiveness improved significantly when
all three pages were examined (fig 3). An example of one expert
panel’s assessment of a case and relevant ‘‘must-not-miss’’
diagnoses is shown in table 5.
DISCUSSION
We have demonstrated in this large validation study that the
Isabel diagnostic aid demonstrates significant accuracy for
hospital discharge diagnosis among patients presenting to EDs
with acute medical problems. The system also showed potential
utility in the ED setting by including all key diagnoses among
its diagnostic reminders.
The Isabel system has been previously evaluated in acute
paediatrics, and shown to display the final diagnosis on the first
page in .90% cases drawn from real life practice.17 The
heterogeneous nature of acute presentations among adult patients
and their lengthy past history may have resulted in more complex
clinical data entry accounting for some decrease in accuracy in this
study. Most adult patients had significant past medical illnesses
and were on numerous medications. In addition, textual
descriptions of diseases in the current Isabel knowledge base
may be quite different between adult and paediatric sources
resulting in a poorer match between the clinical data entered and
the diagnostic results generated. Despite these findings, Isabel’s
diagnostic performance is better than that of previously described
www.emjonline.com
Downloaded from emj.bmj.com on 22 August 2007
624
Ramnarayan, Cronje, Brown, et al
.......................
DDSS.18 VanScoy et al showed in an ED setting using two
diagnostic systems, QMR and ILIAD, that the final diagnosis was
present in the top five choices in only 30% of cases. Data entry
time was prolonged since detailed case descriptions needed to be
matched to system-specific terminology and specific training was
required in the use of the DDSS.19 In our study, clinical assessment
was entered into Isabel in the examining clinicians’ own words in
free text, leading to the conclusion that rapid and easy use of the
system is possible without prolonged data entry. In this context,
Graber et al have also recently shown that merely pasting the
entire history and physical examination section from the Case
Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital series as text
without any modification into Isabel resulted in a diagnostic
accuracy rate of 74%.20
Identifying the precise patient population in which DDSS might
prove useful is a major challenge. Expert systems such as QMR
were intended to be used in a diagnostic dilemma. However, there
is sufficient evidence that diagnostic errors occur during routine
practice, and that there is poor correlation between physicians’
diagnostic accuracy and their own perception of the need for
diagnostic assistance.21 We chose to focus on validating Isabel
against a pre-selected subset of ED patients (acute medical
problems seen in the ‘‘majors area’’). Nearly half of all patients
screened qualified using our liberal study criteria, although it is
improbable that in practice medical staff would have used Isabel
in all these patients. Experience from our paediatric clinical study
indicates that clinicians may seek diagnostic advice in 5–7% of
acute medical presentations (three to five ED patients per day).22
Identifying the optimal parameter against which DDSS performance can be measured also remains controversial.23 We used
hospital discharge diagnoses and an expert panel’s opinion of key
diagnoses to provide a combined view of system accuracy and
utility. During this initial evaluation, we deliberately denied
clinicians access to Isabel. Yet, by extrapolation from these results,
it seems likely that clinicians will use and benefit from its
diagnostic advice in situations of uncertainty, especially since
minimal data entry time was required. Integration of the DDSS
into an electronic medical record may allow active diagnostic
advice to be delivered to staff with minimal effort. Such an
interface has been developed recently.24
LIMITATIONS
The main limitation of this study was the fact that users did not
interact with the system making it difficult to estimate its true
utility in practice. The impact of a DDSS is best measured by its
ability to improve clinicians’ diagnostic assessment; in addition,
unexpected negative effects might be seen. We used electronic
systems or discharge summaries to provide data on final
diagnoses, but these sources have been shown to be unrepresentative and of variable quality. However, due to logistical
reasons, we could not follow up all patients seen in this study.
Also, as ED discharge diagnoses on patients were also missing
in a number of patients, we used inpatient discharge diagnoses
for our main outcome analysis.
CONCLUSIONS
Diagnostic assistance may be useful in a large proportion of
patients seen in an emergency department. The Isabel
diagnostic aid performs with an acceptable degree of clinical
accuracy in this setting. Further studies to elucidate its effects
on decision making and diagnostic error are essential in order
to clarify its role in routine practice.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We gratefully acknowledge the advice during study design provided by
Dr Paul Taylor.
www.emjonline.com
Authors’ affiliations
Padmanabhan Ramnarayan, Children’s Acute Transport Service, London,
UK
Natalie Cronje, Isabel Healthcare, London, UK
Ruth Brown, Rupert Negus, St Mary’s Hospital, London, UK
Bill Coode, Philip Moss, Newham General Hospital, London, UK
Taj Hassan, Wayne Hamer, Leeds General Infirmary, Leeds, UK
Joseph Britto, Isabel Healthcare, Reston, VA, USA
Funding: This study was supported by a research grant from the National
Health Service (NHS) Research & Development Unit, London. The sponsor
did not influence the study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; the writing of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the
manuscript for publication.
Competing interests: The Isabel system is currently managed by Isabel
Healthcare, and is available only to subscribed individual and institutional
users. Dr Ramnarayan is a part-time research advisor for Isabel
Healthcare, Ms Cronje was employed as a research assistant by Isabel
Healthcare for this study, and Dr Britto is Clinical Director of Isabel
Healthcare. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
REFERENCES
1 Weingart SN, Wilson RM, Gibberd RW, et al. Epidemiology of medical error.
BMJ 2000;320(7237):774–7.
2 Burroughs TE, Waterman AD, Gallagher TH, et al. Patient concerns about
medical errors in emergency departments. Acad Emerg Med 2005;12(1):57–64.
3 Leape L, Brennan TA, Laird N, et al. The nature of adverse events in hospitalized
patients. Results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study II. N Engl J Med
1991;324:377–84.
4 Davis P, Lay-Yee R, Briant R, et al. Adverse events in New Zealand public
hospitals II: preventability and clinical context. N Z Med J
2003;116(1183):U624.
5 Sandars J, Esmail A. The frequency and nature of medical error in primary care:
understanding the diversity across studies. Fam Pract 2003;20(3):231–6.
6 Bartlett EE. Physicians’ cognitive errors and their liability consequences. J Healthc
Risk Manage 1998;18:62–9.
7 Driscoll P, Thomas M, Touquet R, et al. Risk management in accident and
emergency medicine. In: Vincent CA, ed. Clinical risk management. Enhancing
patient safety. London: BMJ Publications, 2001.
8 Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to
minimize them. Acad Med 2003;78(8):775–80.
9 Kovacs G, Croskerry P. Clinical decision making: an emergency medicine
perspective. Acad Emerg Med 1999;6(9):947–52.
10 Bache J. Emergency medicine: past, present, and future. J R Soc Med
2005;98(6):255–8.
11 Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing diagnostic errors in medicine: what’s
the goal? Acad Med 2002;77(10):981–92.
12 Lemaire JB, Schaefer JP, Martin LA, et al. Effectiveness of the Quick Medical
Reference as a diagnostic tool. CMAJ 1999;161(6):725–8.
13 Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Kulkarni G, et al. A novel diagnostic aid (ISABEL):
development and preliminary evaluation of clinical performance. Medinfo
2004;11(Pt 2):1091–5.
14 Greenough A. Help from ISABEL for pediatric diagnoses. Lancet
2002;360(9341):1259.
15 Ramnarayan P, Roberts GC, Coren M, et al. Assessment of the potential impact
of a reminder system on the reduction of diagnostic errors: a quasi-experimental
study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2006;6:22.
16 E-health insider. http://www.e-health-insider.com/news/item.cfm?ID = 1009
(accessed 9 July 2007).
17 Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, et al. ISABEL: a web-based differential
diagnostic aid for paediatrics: results from an initial performance evaluation.
Arch Dis Child 2003;88(5):408–13.
18 Berner ES, Webster GD, Shugerman AA, et al. Performance of four computerbased diagnostic systems. N Engl J Med 1994;330(25):1792–6.
19 Graber MA, VanScoy D. How well does decision support software perform in the
emergency department? Emerg Med J 2003;20(5):426–8.
20 Graber ML, Mathews A. Performance of a web-based clinical diagnosis support
system using whole text data entry. http://www.isabelhealthcare.com/info/
independentq1.doc (accessed 9 July 2007).
21 Friedman CP, Gatti GG, Franz TM, et al. Do physicians know when their
diagnoses are correct? Implications for decision support and error reduction.
J Gen Intern Med 2005;20(4):334–9.
22 Ramnarayan P, Winrow A, Coren M, et al. Diagnostic omission errors in acute
paediatric practice: impact of a reminder system on decision-making. BMC Med
Inform Decis Mak 2006;6(1):37.
23 Berner ES. Diagnostic decision support systems: how to determine the gold
standard? J Am Med Inform Assoc 2003;10(6):608–10.
24 PatientKeeper. http://www.patientkeeper.com/success_partners.html (accessed
9 July 2007).
HOW ACCURATELY DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS
DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEM SUGGEST THE
CORRECT DIAGNOSIS IN ADULT ER AND
PEDIATRIC CASES?
Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, Coren M, Winrow A, Britto J.
ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic aid for pediatrics: results from an initial
performance evaluation.
Arch Dis Child. 2003 May;88 (5):408-13.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
408
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic aid for
paediatrics: results from an initial performance
evaluation
P Ramnarayan, A Tomlinson, A Rao, M Coren, A Winrow, J Britto
.............................................................................................................................
Arch Dis Child 2003;88:408–413
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
.......................
Correspondence to:
Dr J Britto, Department of
Paediatrics, Imperial
College School of
Medicine, St Mary’s
Hospital, Norfolk Place,
London W2 1PG, UK;
j.britto@ic.ac.uk
Accepted
4 October 2002
.......................
A
Aims: To test the clinical accuracy of a web based differential diagnostic tool (ISABEL) for a set of case
histories collected during a two stage evaluation.
Methods: Setting: acute paediatric units in two teaching and two district general hospitals in the southeast of England. Materials: sets of summary clinical features from both stages, and the diagnoses
expected for these features from stage I (hypothetical cases provided by participating clinicians in
August 2000) and final diagnoses for cases in stage II (children presenting to participating acute paediatric units between October and December 2000). Main outcome measure: presence of the expected
or final diagnosis in the ISABEL output list.
Results: A total of 99 hypothetical cases from stage I and 100 real life cases from stage II were
included in the study. Cases from stage II covered a range of paediatric specialties (n = 14) and final
diagnoses (n = 55). ISABEL displayed the diagnosis expected by the clinician in 90/99 hypothetical
cases (91%). In stage II evaluation, ISABEL displayed the final diagnosis in 83/87 real cases (95%).
Conclusion: ISABEL showed acceptable clinical accuracy in producing the final diagnosis for a variety of real as well as hypothetical case scenarios.
large proportion of routine clinical decision making
depends on the availability of good quality clinical
information and instant access to up to date medical
knowledge.1 There is increasing evidence that the use of computer aided clinical decision support to manage medical
knowledge results in better healthcare processes and patient
outcomes.2 However, the use of computer based knowledge
management techniques in medical decision making has
remained poor.3 4
Considerable advances have been made in making latest
processed information from clinical trials available, exemplified by the Cochrane database and the Clinical Evidence
series.5 The free accessibility of Medline on the internet
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PubMed) has enabled easy and
universal search of the medical literature. However, these
endeavours do not primarily serve the busy clinician at the
bedside seeking bottom line answers to routine clinical questions. These questions involve diagnosis and immediate management for a patient in general practice6 or in the emergency
department.7 Recent initiatives such as the ATTRACT project
have attempted to provide quick, up to date answers to clinical queries for the bedside physician, but involve a substantial
financial and resource commitment.8
In addition, the current model of healthcare delivery within
the UK National Health Service (NHS) accentuates these
problems by providing care in the form of an “inverted pyramid of knowledge”. Clinical wisdom and knowledge are concentrated at the top among senior staff, in many cases distant
from the patient who is first seen by a junior doctor at the bottom of the pyramid. This may contribute to a significant proportion of medical error,9 resulting in extra bed days and a
preventable financial burden.10
ISABEL (Isabel Medical Charity, UK) is a computerised differential diagnostic aid for paediatrics that is delivered via the
world wide web. It was developed following a missed diagnosis on a 3 year old girl with necrotising fasciitis complicating
chicken pox. It has currently over 9000 registered users
(including doctors, nurses, and other healthcare profession-
www.archdischild.com
als) and receives over 100 000 page requests every month.
Powered by Autonomy, proprietary software that serves as an
efficient information retrieval engine by matching patterns
within unformatted text, the tool produces differential
diagnoses for any set of clinical features by searching text
from standard paediatric textbooks. Rather than provide a
single diagnosis (a diagnostic tool), ISABEL is primarily
intended to suggest only a differential diagnosis and serve as
a “reminder” system to remind the clinician of potentially
important diagnoses that might have been missed.
During the development of ISABEL, text from each
textbook pertaining to each of 3500 different diagnostic labels
(for example, measles, migraine) was added to the ISABEL
database. These textbooks included Nelson’s textbook of pediatrics
(16th edition, 2000, WB Saunders), Forfar and Arneil’s textbook of
paediatrics (5th edition, 1998, Churchill Livingstone, UK), Jones
and Dargan Churchill’s pocket book of toxicology (2001, Churchill
Livingstone, UK), and Rennie and Roberton’s textbook of neonatology (3rd edition, 1999, Churchill Livingstone, UK). Each diagnostic label was allocated an age group classification
(“newborn”, “infant”, “child”, or “adolescent”) to prevent
inappropriate diagnoses being presented to the user for a specific patient age group.
In order to examine ISABEL’s utility, an evaluation
programme was planned in a stepwise fashion: initial system
performance to establish the safety of the tool, subsequent
evaluation of impact in a simulated setting, and evaluation of
impact in a real life clinical setting. This paper describes only
the initial evaluation of ISABEL’s capability, and focuses on
system performance. The tool was isolated from intended
users (clinicians); its impact on clinical practice and decision
making was not examined. This study was planned in two
stages in two different settings, with the following considerations:
• The two stages to be conducted in series for ease of data
collection.
• They were intended to provide a variety of hypothetical as
well as real cases for the investigators to test the tool.
ISABEL
409
Figure 1 Outcome of all cases
collected for both stages of
evaluation.
• ISABEL is useful primarily as a reminder tool and not as an
“oracle”. However, in reminding clinicians of diagnoses in a
given clinical setting, it is imperative that the final diagnosis is also one of the “reminders” suggested. ISABEL would
be considered “unsafe” for clinical use if many plausible
diagnoses were suggested to the junior doctor, but the final
diagnosis was not. For this reason, it is crucial that ISABEL
showed acceptable clinical accuracy by displaying the final
diagnosis (especially for inexperienced junior doctors who
may not have considered the “correct” diagnosis).
METHODS
Differential diagnostic tool
ISABEL was delivered on the internet free of charge
(www.isabel.org.uk). During the development phase, only the
investigators had access to ISABEL, enabled by a secure log-in
procedure. This would ensure that clinicians participating in
the data collection would not be able to use ISABEL and
impact on clinical management. On accessing the tool, the
patient age group had to be chosen first (newborn, infant,
child, or adolescent). Following this, the summary clinical
features of the case were entered into a free text box. These
features would normally be gathered from the history, physical examination, and results of initial investigations. Any
additional findings could be subsequently entered into the
tool to focus the differential diagnosis further.
To maximise the tool’s performance, clinical features had to
be entered in appropriate medical terminology (rather than
lay terms, as the database consisted of textbooks), the spelling
had to be accurate (British or American), and laboratory
results needed interpretation in words (“leucocytosis” or
“increased white cell count” for a white cell count of 36.7 ×
106/µl). These data were then submitted to the ISABEL
database, searched by Autonomy, and a fresh web page
displaying the results from ISABEL was produced. This list
included around 10–15 unique diagnoses for consideration,
classified on the basis of the systems from which they were
drawn (respiratory, metabolic, etc). The diagnoses were not
ranked in order of probability, thus reinforcing the function of
the tool as a “reminder” system.
Study design and conduct
Stage I: We undertook this study in August 2000 within the
Department of Paediatrics at St Mary’s Hospital, London,
which has both general paediatric as well as specialist services
in infectious diseases, neonatology, and intensive care.
Clinicians with varying levels of experience (consultant, registrar, and senior house officer) were contacted to provide hypothetical case histories of acute paediatric presentations. For
each case, they specified the age group category, summarised
the clinical features, and listed the expected diagnosis(es).
Stage II: This study was undertaken in four acute paediatric
units, two teaching hospitals (St Mary’s Hospital, London and
Addenbrookes’ Hospital, Cambridge), and two large district
general hospitals (Kingston Hospital, Surrey and Royal Alexandra Hospital, Brighton). This study was done from October
to December 2000. Junior doctors working within these
departments prospectively collected data on children presenting to the acute paediatric unit. Only data regarding the age
group, a summary of clinical features at initial presentation,
and the working diagnosis(es) were collected from the
doctors. The final diagnosis for each patient, as decided by the
clinical team at the end of the hospital stay (or at the end of
the clinical assessment), was collected from the discharge
summary.
Guidance was provided to the doctors to specify clinical
features in medical terminology and to interpret results of initial investigations in words. This was done so that the investigators would not need to modify the content provided before
entering the clinical features into ISABEL. One investigator
(AT) collected these data in a structured form. In one sitting at
the end of the data collection, she then entered the age group
and the clinical features of each case into ISABEL as provided
by the junior doctors, without modifying the content or spelling. This preserved the inherent user variability in summarising clinical features. Results generated by the tool for each
case were collected for analysis.
Outcome measures
The main outcome measure used for both stages of the study
was the presence of the expected or final diagnosis(es) within
the results generated by ISABEL. This was a measure of the
www.archdischild.com
410
Figure 2 Sample screen from ISABEL: input of clinical features.
Figure 3 Sample screen from ISABEL: output of diagnostic reminders classified by system.
www.archdischild.com
Ramnarayan, Tomlinson, Rao, et al
ISABEL
411
Table 1
Clinical details of cases tested in stage I evaluation of ISABEL
Primary diagnosis expected by paediatrician and number of forms
Acute graft versus host disease
Acute pyelonephritis
AIDS
Aspergillosis
Bacterial meningitis
Bacterial pneumonia
Blastomycosis
Brucellosis
Campylobacter
Cat scratch disease
Cellulitis
Chlamydia pneumoniae infection
Chlamydia trachomatis infection
Chronic granulomatous disease
Congenital cytomegalovirus infection
Crohn’s disease
Croup
Dermatomyositis
E coli 0157 infection and haemolytic uraemic
syndrome
Epstein-Barr virus infection
Endocarditis
Endotracheal tube obstruction
Enteric fever
Enteroviral infection
Erythema infectiosum
Gastroenteritis
Gram negative septicaemia
Haemolytic disease of newborn
Hand, foot, and mouth disease
Herpes simplex gingivostomatitis
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
3
1
1
Juvenile psoriatic arthritis
Juvenile spondyloarthropathy
Kawasaki disease
Leptospirosis
Lyme disease
Malaria
Measles
Meconium aspiration syndrome
Meningitis
Meningococcal disease
Meningococcus
Mumps
Mycoplasma infection
Necrotising enterocolitis
Nonpolio enterovirus infection
Osteomyelitis
Parotitis
Pauciarticular juvenile chronic arthritis
Periorbital cellulitis
1
1
3
1
2
7
1
1
2
3
1
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
Herpes zoster (shingles)
HIV
Human herpes virus infection
Iatrogenic blood loss
Impetigo
Infant botulism
Infant of a diabetic mother
Infection mononucleosis (glandular fever)
Infectious mononucleosis
1
2
1
1
1
2
1
1
1
Pneumonia
Polio
Rheumatic fever
Respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis
Respiratory syncytial virus infection
Scarlet fever
Sepsis
Shigella infection
Staphylococcal scalded skin syndrome
Staphylococcal toxic shock
Still’s disease (systemic juvenile chronic
arthritis)
Subacute sclerosing panencephalitis
Systemic sclerosis
Tuberculous pneumonia
Toxic shock syndrome
Tuberculosis
Urinary tract infection
Varicella zoster (chicken pox)
Viral encephalitis
Viral urinary respiratory tract infection
clinical accuracy of the tool, defined as reminding clinicians of
the “correct” diagnosis.
RESULTS
Figure 1 summarises the outcome of all data that were
collected for both stages of the evaluation. On the whole, 99
hypothetical cases (provided by 13 clinicians) were eligible for
analysis in stage I, and 100 cases in stage II. Forms in which no
diagnoses were entered and where the clinical features section
included the final diagnosis in the wording were excluded
from testing. Figures 2 and 3 show sample screens from
ISABEL for a real case in stage II testing.
Tables 1 and 2 summarise the clinical characteristics of
cases collected in stages I and II, providing an indication of the
spectrum and frequency of final diagnoses as well as specialties covered during the evaluation.
Stage I: Presence of the expected diagnosis/es in the ISABEL
differential diagnosis list: Of the 99 hypothetical cases that were
used to test ISABEL, the expected diagnosis (if it was a single
diagnosis) and all the expected diagnoses (if the clinician
expected to see multiple diagnoses) were present in 90 cases
(91%).
Stage II: Presence of the final diagnosis in the ISABEL differential
diagnosis list: One hundred real cases were used to test ISABEL.
Each of these cases had a primary final diagnosis which was
used as the outcome variable (11 cases also had a supplementary diagnosis, which was not used to test the tool). Table 3
shows an example from stage II evaluation data.
1
1
1
4
1
2
1
1
1
In 13/100 cases, the final diagnosis was non-specific (such
as “viral illness”). Since textbooks do not describe such nonspecific diagnoses, and the ISABEL database was itself created
from textbooks, these were not recognised as distinct final
diagnoses. In the remaining 87 cases, the final diagnosis was
present in the ISABEL output in 83 cases (95%). The four cases
in which the final diagnosis was absent in the ISABEL output
were: Stevens-Johnson syndrome, respiratory syncitial virus
bronchiolitis, erythema multiforme, and staphylococcal cellulitis.
Recognising that non-specific final diagnoses are often
made in routine clinical practice, ISABEL was separately
tested against these diagnoses. In 10/13 such cases, ISABEL
suggested diagnoses that were nearly synonymous. For a nonspecific final diagnosis such as “viral illness”, ISABEL
suggested alternatives such as roseola infantum, Epstein-Barr
virus, and enteroviruses.
For each case, the ISABEL differential diagnostic tool
suggested more than 10 diagnoses (mode 13, range 10–15).
DISCUSSION
Presence of the expected/final diagnosis
This outcome measure is akin to the “sensitivity” of a test. In
this respect, ISABEL showed a level of sensitivity between 83%
and 91%. More importantly, ISABEL displayed the final diagnosis in stage II cases even when only the presenting clinical
features were entered. Although the primary function of the
tool is to remind clinicians to consider other reasonable
www.archdischild.com
412
Ramnarayan, Tomlinson, Rao, et al
Table 2
Clinical characteristics of cases collected during stage II evaluation
Primary
diagnosis*
System
Final diagnosis (n=55)
Allergy
Allergic reaction
Egg allergy
Urticarial reaction
Erythema multiforme
Staphylococcal infection
Diabetes
Diabetic ketoacidosis
Foreign body ingestion
Non-specific gastritis
Non-specific abdominal pain
Pyloric stenosis
Sickle cell crisis
Acute gastroenteritis
Cellulitis
Cervical lymphadenitis
Enteroviral infection
Infected insect bite
Mycoplasma infection
Osteomyelitis
Otitis media
Pneumococcal meningitis
Pneumonia
Preauricular abscess
Preseptal cellulitis
Roseola infantum
Rotavirus infection
Rubella
Scalded skin syndrome
Septic arthritis
Stevens-Johnson syndrome
Streptococcal infection
Tonsillitis
Toxic shock syndrome
Upper respiratory tract infection
Viral illness
Viral meningitis
Opiate withdrawal
Nephrotic syndrome
Urinary tract infection
Febrile convulsion
Vasovagal attacks
Acute severe asthma
Acute sinusitis
Asthma
Bronchiolitis
Croup
Laryngomalacia
Respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis
Viral induced wheeze
Acute exacerbation of systemic lupus
erythematosus
Henoch-Schönlein purpura
Kawasaki disease
Diskitis of L1/L2
Slipped upper femoral epiphysis
Appendicitis
Dermatology
Endocrine
Gastroenterology
Haematology
Infection
Neonate
Nephrology
Neurology
Respiratory
Rheumatic
Skeletal
Surgical
Supplementary
diagnosis*
2
1
1
2
1
1
2
1
1
2
1
2
1
1
1
2
1
1
1
2
2
7
1
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
2
1
2
9
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
5
5
2
1
12
5
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
2
1
1
1
1
*Some patients had more than one final diagnosis (for example, one child had a primary diagnosis of
urinary tract infection as well as a supplementary diagnosis of asthma).
diagnoses in their diagnostic plan and thus influence their
management plan, it is important that the tool also generates
accurate diagnoses in its output. The use of a final diagnosis as
the gold standard is useful in this regard, to ensure that even
inexperienced clinicians using the system remain “safe”. In
this context, diagnostic accuracy rates of clinicians, in an
unselected patient population, have been around 60%.11 These
studies used necropsy findings or results of specific “diagnostic” tests as the gold standard. In a prospective evaluation of a
prototype of the medical diagnostic aid Quick Medical Reference (QMR), the unaided diagnostic accuracy rate of
physicians (an entire ward team considered as a single unit)
was 60% for diagnostically challenging cases.12 In this highly
selective patient population, it was possible to establish a final
www.archdischild.com
diagnosis only in 20 out of 31 cases, even after follow up for six
months and extensive investigation.
In general, published reports of similar evaluations of other
diagnostic systems are sparse. In one study, some commonly
used systems for adult medicine (Dxplain, Iliad, QMR, and
Meditel) had an accuracy of between 50% and 70% when
tested against a set of challenging cases.13 Our figures compare
favourably with results obtained from testing other diagnostic
systems in clinical use.14–16 Most such systems are expert
systems, and use a combination of rule based and Bayesian
approaches to model diagnostic decision making.17 Most
systems are also oriented towards adult medicine. Even the
few existing paediatric diagnostic aids offer support related to
very specific areas such as rheumatic diseases and abdominal
ISABEL
413
Table 3 Example from stage II evaluation data
showing the clinical features, the ISABEL differential
diagnosis list, and the final diagnosis
Age
group
Clinical features
Final diagnosis
Infant
Cough
Respiratory syncytial virus
bronchiolitis
Wheeze
Increased work of
breathing
Subcostal recession
Wheeze and crackles on
examination
Tachypnoea
Poor feeding
The spectrum of cases tested is broad enough to ensure that
the tool is not limited to a special subset of users. Delivered via
the world wide web, ISABEL could impact on patient
management on a global level. Because of a potential synergy
between the doctor and ISABEL, the results of this study cannot be directly extrapolated to estimate the clinical impact of
the tool. Further studies are underway to evaluate the effects
of clinicians using ISABEL to aid clinical decision making in
simulated as well as real settings. These studies are essential to
explore the positive as well as negative impact ISABEL might
produce on healthcare processes, health economics, as well as
patient outcomes.
.....................
Authors’ affiliations
pain, making them difficult to use in routine clinical
practice.16 18 The evaluation of one such system for paediatric
rheumatic diseases suggested 80% accuracy (92% when only
diagnoses included in its knowledge base were used).16
Limitations of the study
This study was designed to evaluate only the presence of the
final diagnosis in the reminder list. It did not address the
plausibility of the other diagnoses suggested by ISABEL. In
this respect, it is possible that although the final diagnosis was
present in the differential diagnosis list, the other suggestions
made by ISABEL were misleading. However, in order to verify
the safety of the tool, it was crucial to show that the final
diagnosis formed part of the “reminder” list. Further evaluation is underway to test the relevance of the alternate
diagnoses.
The hypothetical cases did not represent the complete
spectrum of paediatrics and may not have tested the true
limits of the tool. Similarly, the number of real cases used to
test the tool may have been insufficient to ensure a
complete evaluation of the system’s capability, especially in
the specialities of neonatal paediatrics, oncology, and
paediatric surgery. In addition, this study was conducted in
secondary hospital care. Further testing is planned in a
primary care setting to assess the safety of the tool.
Considering that this study was only a preliminary evaluation of the performance of the tool, an attempt to extrapolate
these results to routine clinical use is not easily possible. This
evaluation separates the tool from the intended user (the clinician). This tool, like other similar tools, will only be used as
an adjunct to the existing system of clinical decision making19
Despite this, it is possible that inexperienced doctors using the
tool either in a primary care or hospital setting might cause an
increase in referrals and investigations. The true measure of
the tool’s benefits and risks will be clear only when tested by
real clinicians uninvolved in the development of the tool.
These questions will be answered in subsequent clinical
impact evaluations.
CONCLUSIONS
Our study shows that in a large proportion of cases, ISABEL
has the potential to remind the clinician of the final diagnosis
in a variety of hypothetical as well as real clinical situations.
P Ramnarayan, A Tomlinson, M Coren, J Britto, Department of
Paediatrics, Imperial College School of Medicine, St Mary’s Hospital,
Norfolk Place, London, UK
A Rao, Department of Haematology, Chase Farm Hospital, The
Ridgeway, Enfield, UK
A Winrow, Department of Paediatrics, Kingstone Hospital, Galsworthy
Road, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, UK
REFERENCES
1 Wyatt JC. Clinical knowledge and practice in the information age: a
handbook for health professionals. London: The Royal Society of
Medicine Press, 2001.
2 Hunt DL, Haynes RB, Hanna SE, et al. Effects of computer-based clinical
decision support systems on physician performance and patient
outcomes: a systematic review. JAMA 1998;280:1339–46.
3 Ely JW, Osheroff JA, Ebell MH, et al. Analysis of questions asked by
family doctors regarding patient care. BMJ 1999;319:358–61.
4 Hersh WR, Hickam DH. How well do physicians use electronic
information retrieval systems? A framework for investigation and
systematic review. JAMA 1998;280:1347–52.
5 Godlee F, Smith R, Goldmann D. Clinical evidence. BMJ
1999;318:1570–1.
6 Gorman PN, Ash J, Wykoff L. Can primary care physicians’ questions
be answered using the medical journal literature? Bull Med Libr Assoc
1994;82:140–6.
7 Wyer PC, Rowe BH, Guyatt GH, et al. Evidence-based emergency
medicine. The clinician and the medical literature: when can we take a
shortcut? Ann Emerg Med 2000;36:149–55.
8 Brassey J, Elwyn G, Price C, et al. Just in time information for clinicians:
a questionnaire evaluation of the ATTRACT project. BMJ
2001;322:529–30.
9 Neale G, Woloshynowych M, Vincent C. Exploring the causes of
adverse events in NHS hospital practice. J R Soc Med 2001;94:322–30.
10 Alberti KG. Medical errors: a common problem. BMJ 2001;322:501–2.
11 Sadegh-Zadeh K. Fundamentals of clinical methodology. 4. Diagnosis.
Artif Intell Med 2000;20:227–41.
12 Bankowitz RA, McNeil MA, Challinor SM, et al. A computer-assisted
medical diagnostic consultation service. Implementation and prospective
evaluation of a prototype. Ann Intern Med 1989;110:824–32.
13 Berner ES, Webster GD, Shugerman AA, et al. Performance of four
computer-based diagnostic systems. N Engl J Med 1994;330:1792–6.
14 Feldman M, Barnett GO. Pediatric computer-based diagnostic decision
support. Scientific Program, Section on computers and other
technologies. American Academy of Pediatrics, New Orleans, LA, 27
October 1991.
15 Swender PT, Tunnessen WW Jr, Oski FA. Computer-assisted diagnosis.
Am J Dis Child 1974;127:859–61.
16 Athreya BH, Cheh ML, Kingsland LC. Computer-assisted diagnosis of
pediatric rheumatic diseases. Pediatrics 1998;102.
17 Shortliffe EH, Perreault LE. Medical informatics. New York:
Springer-Verlag, 2001.
18 De Dombal FT, Leaper DJ, Staniland JR, et al. Computer-aided
diagnosis of acute abdominal pain. BMJ 1972;2:9–13.
19 Ramnarayan P, Britto J. Paediatric clinical decision support systems
Arch Dis Child 2002;87:361–2.
www.archdischild.com
WHAT IS THE IMPACT ON CHANGE OF DIAGNOSIS
DECISION MAKING BEFORE AND AFTER USING A
DIAGNOSIS DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEM?
Ramnarayan P, Winrow A, Coren M, Nanduri V, Buchdahl R, Jacobs B, Fisher H,
Taylor PM, Wyatt JC, Britto JF.
Diagnostic omission errors in acute paediatric practice: impact of a reminder system
on decision-making.
BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2006 Nov 6;6(1):37
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
BMC Medical Informatics and
Decision Making
BioMed Central
Open Access
Research article
Diagnostic omission errors in acute paediatric practice: impact of a
reminder system on decision-making
Padmanabhan Ramnarayan*1, Andrew Winrow2, Michael Coren3,
Vasanta Nanduri4, Roger Buchdahl5, Benjamin Jacobs6, Helen Fisher7,
Paul M Taylor8, Jeremy C Wyatt9 and Joseph Britto7
Address: 1Children's Acute Transport Service (CATS), 44B Bedford Row, London, WC1H 4LL, UK, 2Department of Paediatrics, Kingston General
Hospital, Galsworthy Road, Kingston-upon-Thames, KT2 7QB, UK, 3Department of Paediatrics, St Mary's Hospital, Paddington, London, W2 1NY,
UK, 4Department of Paediatrics, Watford General Hospital, Vicarage Road, Watford, WD18 0HB, UK, 5Department of Paediatrics, Hillingdon
Hospital, Pield Heath Road, Middlesex, UB8 3NN, UK, 6Department of Paediatrics, Northwick Park Hospital, Watford Road, Harrow, Middlesex,
HA1 3UJ, UK, 7Isabel Healthcare Ltd, Po Box 244, Haslemere, Surrey, GU27 1WU, UK, 8Centre for Health Informatics and Multiprofessional
Education (CHIME), Archway Campus, Highgate Hill, London, N19 5LW, UK and 9Health Informatics Centre, The Mackenzie Building, University
of Dundee, Dundee, DD2 4BF, UK
Email: Padmanabhan Ramnarayan* - ramnarayan@msn.com; Andrew Winrow - andrew.winrow@kingstonhospital.nhs.uk;
Michael Coren - michael.coren@st-marys.nhs.uk; Vasanta Nanduri - v_nanduri@hotmail.com; Roger Buchdahl - rbuc@easynet.co.uk;
Benjamin Jacobs - benjamin.jacobs@nwlh.nhs.uk; Helen Fisher - helen.fisher@st-marys.nhs.uk; Paul M Taylor - p.taylor@chime.ucl.ac.uk;
Jeremy C Wyatt - j.wyatt@chs.dundee.ac.uk; Joseph Britto - joseph@isabelhealthcare.com
* Corresponding author
Published: 06 November 2006
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
doi:10.1186/1472-6947-6-37
Received: 15 July 2006
Accepted: 06 November 2006
This article is available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
© 2006 Ramnarayan et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Abstract
Background: Diagnostic error is a significant problem in specialities characterised by diagnostic uncertainty such as
primary care, emergency medicine and paediatrics. Despite wide-spread availability, computerised aids have not been
shown to significantly improve diagnostic decision-making in a real world environment, mainly due to the need for
prolonged system consultation. In this study performed in the clinical environment, we used a Web-based diagnostic
reminder system that provided rapid advice with free text data entry to examine its impact on clinicians' decisions in an
acute paediatric setting during assessments characterised by diagnostic uncertainty.
Methods: Junior doctors working over a 5-month period at four paediatric ambulatory units consulted the Web-based
diagnostic aid when they felt the need for diagnostic assistance. Subjects recorded their clinical decisions for patients
(differential diagnosis, test-ordering and treatment) before and after system consultation. An expert panel of four
paediatric consultants independently suggested clinically significant decisions indicating an appropriate and 'safe'
assessment. The primary outcome measure was change in the proportion of 'unsafe' workups by subjects during patient
assessment. A more sensitive evaluation of impact was performed using specific validated quality scores. Adverse effects
of consultation on decision-making, as well as the additional time spent on system use were examined.
Results: Subjects attempted to access the diagnostic aid on 595 occasions during the study period (8.6% of all medical
assessments); subjects examined diagnostic advice only in 177 episodes (30%). Senior House Officers at hospitals with
greater number of available computer workstations in the clinical area were most likely to consult the system, especially
out of working hours. Diagnostic workups construed as 'unsafe' occurred in 47/104 cases (45.2%); this reduced to 32.7%
following system consultation (McNemar test, p < 0.001). Subjects' mean 'unsafe' workups per case decreased from 0.49
to 0.32 (p < 0.001). System advice prompted the clinician to consider the 'correct' diagnosis (established at discharge)
during initial assessment in 3/104 patients. Median usage time was 1 min 38 sec (IQR 50 sec – 3 min 21 sec). Despite a
modest increase in the number of diagnostic possibilities entertained by the clinician, no adverse effects were
demonstrable on patient management following system use. Numerous technical barriers prevented subjects from
accessing the diagnostic aid in the majority of eligible patients in whom they sought diagnostic assistance.
Page 1 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Conclusion: We have shown that junior doctors used a Web-based diagnostic reminder system during acute paediatric
assessments to significantly improve the quality of their diagnostic workup and reduce diagnostic omission errors. These
benefits were achieved without any adverse effects on patient management following a quick consultation.
Background
Studies suggest that a significant proportion of adverse
events in primary as well as in secondary care result from
errors in medical diagnosis [1-3]; diagnostic errors also
constitute the second leading cause for malpractice suits
against hospitals [4]. Specialities such as primary care and
emergency medicine have specifically been identified as
high risk areas for diagnostic mishaps, where cognitive
biases in decision making contribute to errors of omission, resulting in incomplete workup and 'missed diagnoses' [5-7]. Adverse events are also commoner in
extremes of age, such as paediatric patients and the elderly
[8,9]. Diagnostic decision support systems (DDSS), computerised tools that provide accurate and useful patientand situation-specific advice have been proposed as a
technological solution for the reduction of diagnostic
errors in practice [10]. Although a number of 'expert diagnostic systems' exist currently, a recent systematic review
showed that these systems were less effective in practice
than systems that provided preventive care reminders and
prescription advice [11]. To a large extent, this may be
because most latter systems were integrated into an existing electronic medical record (EMR), enabling effortless
and frequent use by clinicians; in contrast, expert DDSS
such as Quick Medical Reference (QMR), ILIAD and
MEDITEL-PEDS were typically used in stand-alone fashion [12-14]. Due to a lengthy data input process, considerable clinician motivation and effort was required for
their regular use, leading to infrequent usage [15]. As a
result, busy clinical areas have been poorly served by existing DDSS.
Attempts to integrate diagnostic decision support into an
EMR have been sporadic [16,17], mainly limited by the
difficulties associated with converting a complex clinical
narrative into structured clinical data in a standard EMR,
especially for specialities such as paediatrics and emergency medicine. It appears likely that in the medium term,
effortless provision of decision support for busy clinical
areas at high-risk for diagnostic error seems possible only
through alternative approaches. A Web-based paediatric
DDSS that permits rapid use in a busy clinical environment by using natural language free text data entry has
been recently described [18,19]. Its underlying knowledge
base consists of textual descriptions of diseases; using statistical natural language processing, the DDSS matches
clinical features to disease descriptions in the database.
This approach is similar to that adopted by the RECONSIDER program [20]. Diagnostic suggestions are displayed
in sets of 10 up to a maximum of 30, and arranged by
body system (e.g. cardiology) rather than by clinical probability. Between 2001 and 2003, >15,000 users registered
for its use, 10% of whom used it on >10 separate occasions, resulting in >60,000 distinct user log-ins (personal
communication). Thus, although poor usage has been a
major confounding factor during evaluations of the clinical benefits of a number of DDSS [21], Isabel usage statistics led us to believe that a study evaluating its clinical
impact would permit the assessment of its benefits and
risks to be interpreted with confidence, and provide useful
insights into the user-DDSS dynamic. Results from an
independent email questionnaire survey also suggested
that most regular users in the UK found it helpful during
patient management [22].
In this study, we aimed to measure the clinical impact of
the Isabel system on diagnostic decision making. We
hypothesised that lessons learnt from our evaluation
study could be generalised to the design, implementation
and evaluation of other stand-alone DDSS, and clarify the
risks associated with the use of such a system in real life.
Diagnostic suggestions were provided to junior doctors
during acute paediatric assessments in which they experienced diagnostic uncertainty.
Methods
The study was co-ordinated from St Mary's Hospital,
Imperial College London, and was approved by the London multi-centre research ethics committee (MREC/02/2/
70) and relevant local research ethics committees.
Study centres
From a list of non-London district general hospitals
(DGH) at which >4 individual users were registered in the
Isabel database, four paediatric departments (two university-affiliated DGHs and two DGHs without official academic links) were enrolled, based on logistical and
clinical reasons – all sites were <100 miles driving distance from London; three were geographically clustered in
the Eastern Region and two sites were separated by large
distances from regional tertiary centres. The baseline characteristics of each of the participating centres are detailed
in table 1. None of the study centres were involved in the
development of the DDSS.
Study participants
All junior doctors (Senior House Officers [interns] and
Registrars [residents]) in substantive posts at each of the
Page 2 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Table 1: Characteristics of participating paediatric departments
Nature
24 hours dedicated PAU
Annual PAU attendance
Number of junior doctors
Number of consultants (acute)
Computers in PAU
Metropolitan area
Dist to tertiary centre (miles)
Clinical activity (PAU attendances per hour PAU open)
Computer accessibility index (available computers per unit clinical activity)
Senior support (number of acute consultants per subject enrolled)
Centre A
Centre B
Centre C
Centre D
university
no*
4194
31
10 (6)
2
yes
<25
1.1
1.8
0.2
dgh
yes
4560
21
7 (4)
1
mixed
26
0.53
1.9
0.2
dgh
yes
4780
12
7 (4)
1
no
71
0.54
1.85
0.3
university
yes
4800
16
11 (7)
3
no
69
0.67
4.5
0.4
Abbreviations: dgh = district general hospital, PAU = paediatric assessment unit
* PAU open from 0800 to 0000 only
participating paediatric departments between December
2002 and April 2003 were enrolled after informed verbal
consent. Consultants (attending physicians) and locum
doctors were excluded.
Study patients
All children (age 0–16 years) presenting with an acute
medical complaint, and assessed by a junior doctor in a
designated Paediatric Assessment Area/Ambulatory Unit
(PAU), were eligible for DDSS use. Outpatients, re-attendances for ward follow up, and day cases were ineligible.
Based on subjects' feedback collected prior to the study
start date, we made a pragmatic decision to allow junior
doctors to selectively consult the DDSS only for patients
in whom they experienced diagnostic uncertainty. This
latter subset formed the actual study population.
Study design and power
Our study was a within-subject 'before and after' evaluation in which each study subject acted as their own control. Participants explicitly recorded their diagnostic
workup and clinical plans (tests and treatment) for cases
before seeking DDSS advice. Following real-time use, subjects either decided to act on system advice (by recording
their revised diagnostic workup and clinical plans) or
chose not to, thus ending the consultation. On the basis
of a pilot study in an experimental setting, we calculated
that the trial required data from 180 cases to detect a 33%
reduction in clinically 'unsafe' diagnostic workups (80%
power; type I error 5%). We defined diagnostic workups
as being 'unsafe' if they deviated from a 'minimum gold
standard' provided by an independent expert panel.
Intervention
Decision support system
A limited, secure (128-bit SSL encryption) and passwordprotected trial version was used. This differed from the
publicly accessible DDSS – only the first 10 diagnostic
suggestions were displayed, and reading material related
to each diagnosis was not made available. The trial version was accessible only via a designated shortcut icon
placed on each computer present at study commencement
within the participating PAUs. This mechanism utilised
cookies, and automatically facilitated identification of the
centre at which the request originated, allowing subjects
to access the system without an additional log-in procedure. On the trial DDSS, data were captured in two consecutive screens (figures 1 and 2). On screen 1, subjects
recorded the patient's clinical details in their own words,
and their own diagnostic workup and clinical plans (preDDSS). Based on the clinical details submitted in screen 1,
diagnostic suggestions were displayed on screen 2. Subjects had the choice to revise their original diagnostic
workup and clinical plans by adding or deleting decisions
at this stage. It was not possible to go back from screen 2
to screen 1. The consultation ended when clinical decisions from screen 2 (unmodified or revised) were finally
submitted, or after >2 hours of inactivity.
Training
Three separate group training sessions were organised by
one investigator (PR) at each centre one month before the
study start date, coinciding with weekly mandatory
departmental teaching sessions. At each session, subjects
used the trial DDSS with practice cases created for the
study. Sessions were repeated twice during the study
period to recruit and train new post-holders.
Outcome measures
The primary outcome measure was change in the proportion of 'unsafe' diagnostic workups following DDSS consultation. We defined 'unsafe' workups as instances in
which subjects' diagnostic workup (pre- and post-DDSS
consultation) deviated from a 'minimum gold standard'
provided by an independent expert panel of clinicians.
Page 3 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Figure
This
figure
1 shows screen 1 during the DDSS usage during the study
This figure shows screen 1 during the DDSS usage during the study. This page is loaded following a successful log-in, which is
initiated by clicking a designated icon on each study computer at each participating centre. Information collected automatically
at this stage includes date and time of screen 1 display; patient identifiers; subject identifiers; clinical features of patient, and initial diagnostic workup and tests as well as treatments.
Inclusion of the correct discharge diagnosis in the diagnostic workup (pre- and post-Isabel consultation), quality
scores for diagnostic workup and clinical action plans;
time taken by subjects to complete system usage; number
of diagnoses included in the diagnostic assessment preand post-DDSS; inappropriate tests and treatments
ordered by subjects following system advice and significant decisions deleted following consultation were examined as secondary outcome measures.
Data collection
Clinical data were collected during the study period
(December 2002-April 2003) by two complementary
methods – automatically from trial DDSS logs, and manually by a single research assistant (HF). Data collected by
the trial website during system use is shown in table 2. At
the end of each clinical consultation, the user indicated
how useful the diagnostic advice provided had been for a)
patient management and b) as an educational exercise.
Page 4 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Figure
This
figure
2 shows screen 2, which is displayed following submission of the information entered on screen 1
This figure shows screen 2, which is displayed following submission of the information entered on screen 1. The subject has the
opportunity to revise their decisions, including diagnoses and tests and treatments. A brief survey attempts to capture the
user's satisfaction with respect to educational value as well as clinical utility. Information submitted following this page completes the episode of data collection.
Page 5 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
This was recorded on a Likert-scale from 0–5 (not useful
to extremely useful). All subjects were sent two rounds of
email and postal questionnaires each to collect feedback
at trial completion.
The research assistant obtained study patients' medical
records that matched the patient identifiers collected
automatically from the trial website during system use. It
was not possible to use partial or invalid entries to match
medical records. Copies of available medical records were
made such that information was available only up to the
point of DDSS use. Copies were anonymised by masking
patient and centre details. Diagnostic workup and clinical
plans recorded by subjects on the trial website were verified for each case against entries in the medical records
and in hospital laboratory systems. Discharge diagnoses
were collected from routinely collected coding data for all
study patients, and additionally from discharge summaries where available. Discharge diagnoses were validated
by a consultant involved in study conduct at each centre.
In addition, limited demographic and clinical details of
all eligible patients at each study centre were collected
from hospital administrative data.
A panel of four consultant paediatricians independently
examined study medical records, in which subjects' clinical decisions were masked to ensure blinding. In the first
instance, each panel member provided a list of 'clinically
significant' diagnoses, tests and treatments (the latter two
were collectively termed 'clinical action plans') for each
Table 2: Study data automatically collected by the DDSS logs
Patient details
Surname
Date of birth
Age group (neonate, infant, child or adolescent)
Sex
User details
Centre code (based on identity of icon clicked)
Subject identity (including an option for anonymous)
Subject grade
Operational details
Date and time of usage (log in, submission of each page of data)
Unique study ID assigned at log in
Clinical details
Patient clinical features at assessment
Doctor's differential diagnosis (pre-ISABEL)
Doctor's investigation plan (pre-ISABEL)
Doctor's management plan (pre-ISABEL)
Isabel list of differential diagnoses
Diagnoses selected from Isabel list by user as being relevant
Doctor's differential diagnosis (post-ISABEL)
Doctor's investigation plan (post-ISABEL)
Doctor's management plan (post-ISABEL)
Survey details
Satisfaction score for patient management
Satisfaction score for educational use
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
case that would ensure a safe clinical assessment. The
absence of 'clinically significant' items in a subject's
workup was explicitly defined during panel review to represent inappropriate clinical care. For this reason, the
panel members did not include all plausible diagnoses for
each case as part of their assessment, and instead focused
on the minimum gold standard. Using this list as a template, the appropriateness of each decision suggested by
subjects for each case was subsequently scored by the
panel in blinded fashion using a previously validated
scoring system [23]. This score rewarded decision plans
for being comprehensive (sensitive) as well as focussed
(specific). 25% of medical records were randomly
assigned to all four panel members for review; a further
20% was assigned to one of the six possible pairs (i.e.
slightly more than half the records were assessed by a single panel member). Clinically significant decisions (diagnoses, tests and treatments) were collated as a 'minimum
gold standard' set for each case. For cases assessed by multiple panel members, significant decisions provided by a
majority of assessors were used to form the gold standard
set. Concordance between panel members for clinical
decisions was moderate to good, as assessed by the intraclass correlation co-efficient for decisions examined by all
four members (0.70 for diagnoses, 0.47 for tests and 0.57
for treatments).
Analysis
We analysed study data from two main perspectives: operational and clinical. For operational purposes, we defined
each attempt by a subject to log into the DDSS by clicking
on the icon as a 'DDSS attempt'. Each successful display of
screen 1 was defined as a 'successful log in'; a unique study
identifier was automatically generated by the trial website
for each successful log in. Following log in, DDSS usage
data was either 'complete' (data were available from
screens 1 and 2) or 'incomplete' (data were available from
screen 1 only, although screen 2 may have been displayed
to the subject). Time spent by the user processing system
advice was calculated as the difference between the time
screen 2 was displayed and the end of the consultation (or
session time out).
In the first instance, we used McNemar's test for paired
proportions to analyse the change in proportion of
'unsafe' diagnostic workups. In order to account for the
clustering effects resulting from the same subject assessing
a number of cases, we also calculated a mean number of
'unsafe' diagnostic workups per case attempted for each
subject. Change in this variable following DDSS consultation was analysed using two-way mixed-model analysis of
variance (subject grade being between-subjects factor and
occasion being within-subjects factor). In order to exclude
re-thinking effect as an explanation for change in the primary outcome variable, all episodes in which there was a
Page 6 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
difference between the workup pre- and post-DDSS consultation were examined. If diagnoses that were changed
by subjects were present in the Isabel suggestion list, it
could be inferred that the DDSS was responsible for the
change. A more objective marker of clinical benefit was
assessed by examining whether the post-Isabel diagnostic
workup (but not the pre-Isabel workup) included the discharge diagnosis. We analysed changes in pre- and postDDSS diagnostic quality scores, as well as clinical action
plan scores, using subjects as the unit of analysis. We
tested for statistical significance using one way analysis of
variance (grade was the between-subjects factor) to provide a sensitive measure of changes in diagnostic workup,
and tests and treatments. The median test was used to
examine differences between grades in system usage time.
Diversity of suggestions displayed by the DDSS during the
study, and therefore its dynamic nature, was assessed by
calculating the number of unique diagnoses suggested by
the system across all episodes of completed usage (i.e. if
the diagnostic suggestions remained constant irrespective
of case characteristics, this number would be 10). Statistical significance was set for all tests at p value <0.05.
Subjects were expected to use the DDSS in only a subset of
eligible patients. In order to fully understand the characteristics of patients in whom junior doctors experienced
diagnostic difficulty and consulted the DDSS, we examined this group in more detail. We analysed available data
on patient factors (age, discharge diagnosis, outcome of
assessment and length of inpatient stay if admitted), user
factors (grade of subject), and other details. These
included the time of system usage (daytime: 0800–1800;
out-of-hours: 1800-0800), centre of use and its nature
(DGH vs university-affiliated hospital), an index of PAU
activity (number of acute assessments per 60 min period
the PAU was functional), computer accessibility index
(number of available computers per unit PAU activity)
and level of senior support (number of acute consultants
per subject). We subsequently aimed to identify factors
that favoured completion of DDSS usage using a multiple
logistic regression analysis. Significant variables were
identified by univariate analysis and entered in forward
step-wise fashion into the regression model. Characteristics of patients where subjects derived clinical benefit with
DDSS usage were also analysed in similar fashion. We correlated subjects' own perception of system benefit (Likert
style response from user survey) with actual benefit
(improvement in diagnostic quality score) using the Pearson test. Qualitative analysis of feedback from subjects
provided at the end of the study period was performed to
provide insights into system design and user interface.
Results
During the 5-month study period, 8995 children were
assessed in the 4 PAUs; 76.7% (6903/8995 children) pre-
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
sented with medical problems and were eligible for inclusion in the study. Subjects attempted to seek diagnostic
advice on 595 separate occasions across all centres
(8.6%). Subjects successfully logged in only in 226 episodes. Data were available for analysis in 177 cases (complete data in 125 cases; screen 1 data only in an additional
52 cases). The summary of flow of patients and data
through the study is illustrated in figure 3. Centre-wise
distribution of attrition in DDSS usage is demonstrated in
table 3. Medical records were available for 104 patients,
and were examined by the panel according the allocation
scheme shown in figure 4.
The largest subset of patients in whom the DDSS was consulted belonged to the 1–6 year age group (61/177,
34.5%). The mean age of patients was 5.1 years (median
3.3 years). The DDSS was most frequently used by SHOs
(126/177, 71.2%). Although 25% of all eligible patients
were seen on PAUs outside the hours of 08:00 to 18:00,
more than a third of episodes of system usage fell outside
the hours of 0800–1800 (64/177, 36.2%). Subjects at
Centre D used the DDSS most frequently (59/177,
33.3%). In general, usage was greater at university-affiliated hospitals than at DGHs (106/177, 59.9%). Discharge
diagnosis was available in 77 patients. The commonest
diagnosis was non-specific viral infection; however, a
wide spread of diagnoses was seen in the patient population. 58 patients on whom the DDSS was consulted were
admitted to hospital wards, the rest were discharged home
following initial assessment. Inpatients stayed in hospital
for an average of 3.7 days (range: 0.7–26 days). Clinical
characteristics of patients on whom Isabel was consulted
are summarised in table 4. A list of discharge diagnoses in
table 5 indicates the diverse case mix represented in the
study population.
80 subjects enrolled during the study. 63/80 used the system to record some patient data (mean 2, range: 1–12);
56/80 provided complete patient information and their
revised decisions post-DDSS consultation (mean 2, range:
1–6). Due to limitations in the trial website design, it was
unclear how many of the subjects who did not use the system during the study period (17/80) had attempted to
access the DDSS and failed. It was evident that a small
number of subjects used the DDSS on multiple (>9) occasions but did not provide their revised clinical decisions,
leading to incomplete system use. System usage data with
respect to subjects is shown in figure 5.
'Unsafe' diagnostic workups
104 cases in which medical records were available were
analysed. Before DDSS consultation, 'unsafe' diagnostic
workups occurred in 47/104 cases (45.2%); they constituted episodes in which all clinically significant diagnoses
were not considered by subjects during initial decision
Page 7 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Figure
Flow
diagram
3
of patients and data through the study
Flow diagram of patients and data through the study.
Page 8 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Table 3: Centre-wise attrition of DDSS usage and study data
Patients seen in PAU
Medical patients seen in PAU
Number eligible for diagnostic decision support
DDSS attempts
DDSS successful log in
Step 1 completed†
Steps 1&2 completed
Medical records available
Centre A
Centre B
Centre C
Centre D
Total
2679
2201
unknown
338
unknown
47
30
24
1905
1383
unknown
118
unknown
26
25
24
1974
1405
unknown
52
unknown
45
20
16
2437
1914
unknown
87
unknown
59
50
40
8995
6903
unknown
595
226*
177
125
104
* Each successful log in was automatically provided a unique study identifier which was not centre-specific. The number of successful log-ins was
thus calculated as the total number of study identifiers issued by the trial website.
† Step 1 completed indicates that following successful log-in, the subject entered data on the first screen, i.e. patient details.
making. Overall, the proportion of 'unsafe' workups
reduced to 32.7% (34/104 cases) following DDSS consultation, an absolute reduction of 12.5% (McNemar test p
value <0.001, table 6). In a further 5 cases, appropriate
diagnoses that were missing in subjects' workup did form
part of Isabel suggestions, but were ignored by subjects
during their review of DDSS advice. In 11/13 cases in
which 'unsafe' diagnostic workups were eliminated, the
DDSS was used by SHOs. Mean number of 'unsafe'
workups per case reduced from 0.49 to 0.32 post-DDSS
consultation among subjects (p < 0.001); a significant
interaction was demonstrated with grade (p < 0.01).
Examination of the Isabel suggestion list for cases in
which there was a difference between pre- and post-DDSS
workup showed that in all cases, the additional diagnoses
recorded by subjects formed part of the system's advice.
Similar results were obtained for clinical plans, but
smaller reductions were observed. In 3/104 cases, the final
Allocation
Figure 4 of medical records for expert panel assessment consisting of four raters
Allocation of medical records for expert panel assessment consisting of four raters. All panel members rated 25% of cases and
two raters (six possible pairs) rated an additional 20% of cases.
Page 9 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Table 4: Characteristics of patients in whom Isabel was consulted
Factor
PATIENT FACTORS
Age (n = 177)
Neonate
Infant
Young child (1–6 yrs)
Older child (6–12 yrs)
Adolescent
Number of DDSS consultation episodes (completed episodes)
19
33
61
38
26
Primary diagnostic group (n = 77)
Respiratory
Cardiac
Neurological
Surgical
Rheumatology
Infections
Haematology
Other
9
0
6
3
5
31
3
20
Outcome (n = 104)
IP admission
Discharge
58
46
USER FACTORS
Grade (n = 177)
SHO
Registrar
126 (79)
51 (46)
OPERATIONAL FACTORS (n = 177)
Time of use
In hours (0800–1800)
Out of hours (1800-0800)
113 (84)
64 (41)
Centre
A
B
C
D
diagnosis for the patient was present in the post-DDSS list
but not in the pre-DDSS list, indicating that diagnostic
errors of omission were averted following Isabel advice.
Diagnostic quality scores
104 cases assessed by 51 subjects in whom medical
records were available were analysed. Mean diagnostic
quality score across all subjects increased by 6.86 (95% CI
4.0–9.7) after DDSS consultation. The analysis of variance
model indicated that there was no significant effect of
grade on this improvement (p 0.15). Similar changes in
clinical plan scores were smaller in magnitude (table 7).
Usage time
Reliable time data were available in 122 episodes. Median
time spent on system advice was 1 min 38 sec (IQR 50 sec
– 3 min 21 sec). There was no significant difference
47 (30)
26 (25)
45 (20)
59 (50)
between grades with respect to time spent on screen 2
(median test, p = 0.9). This included the time taken to
process DDSS diagnostic suggestions, record changes to
original diagnostic workup and clinical plans, and to
complete the user satisfaction survey.
Impact on clinical decision making
Pre-DDSS, a mean of 2.2 diagnoses were included in subjects' workup; this rose to 3.2 post-DDSS. Similarly, the
number of tests ordered also rose from 2.7 to 2.9; there
was no change in the number of treatment steps. Despite
these increases, no deleterious tests or treatment steps
were added by subjects to their plans following DDSS
consultation. In addition, no clinically significant diagnoses were deleted from their original workup after Isabel
advice.
Page 10 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
Table 5: Discharge diagnoses in children in whom the diagnostic
aid was consulted
Diagnosis
Viral infection
Acute lymphadenitis
Viral gastroenteritis
Pelvic region and thigh infection
Epilepsy
Acute lower respiratory infection
Allergic purpura
Acute inflammation of orbit
Chickenpox with cellulitis
Gastroenteritis
Rotaviral enteritis
Feeding problem of newborn
Syncope and collapse
Lobar pneumonia
Kawasaki disease
Abdominal pain
Angioneurotic oedema
Erythema multiforme
Constipation
Irritable bladder and bowel syndrome
Coagulation defect
G6PD deficiency
Sickle cell dactylitis
Cellulitis
Clavicle osteomyelitis
Kerion
Labyrinthitis
Meningitis
Myositis
Purpura
Scarlet fever
Staphylococcal scalded skin syndrome
Mitochondrial complex 1 deficiency
Adverse drug effect
Eye disorder
Musculoskeletal back pain
Trauma to eye
Disorders of bilirubin metabolism
Foetal alcohol syndrome
Neonatal erythema toxicum
Physiological jaundice
Stroke
Acute bronchiolitis
Acute upper respiratory infection
Asthma
Hyperventilation
Juvenile arthritis with systemic onset
Polyarthritis
Reactive arthropathy
Anorectal anomaly
Number of patients
8
3
3
3
3
3
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
Using forward step-wise regression analysis, grade of subject (registrar), time of system usage (in-hours), centre
identity, senior support and computer accessibility index
were identified as independent factors predicting completion of DDSS usage. Patients in whom actual benefit was
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
demonstrated on diagnostic decision making were more
likely to stay longer in hospital.
469 unique diagnostic suggestions were generated by the
DDSS during its use on 125 cases. This represented a high
degree of diversity of responses appropriate for the diverse
case mix seen in this study – a static list would have consisted of the same 10 diagnoses, and a unique set of suggestions for each single episode of use would have
generated 1250 distinct suggestions.
User perception
Data were available in 125 cases in which subjects completed DDSS usage. Mean satisfaction score for patient
management was 1.6 (95% CI 1.4–1.96); for Isabel use as
an educational adjunct, this was higher (2.4, 95% CI
1.98–2.82). There was moderate correlation between subjects' perception of DDSS usefulness in patient management and actual increment in diagnostic quality score (r
value 0.28, p value 0.0038, figure 6). Feedback from questionnaires indicated that many subjects found the trial
website cumbersome to use in real time since it forced
them to record all their decisions prior to advice, thus taking up time during patient assessment. This was especially
problematic since many subjects had used Isabel in its
original form. A number of subjects were dissatisfied with
computer access during the trial; these related to unavailability of passwords to access the Internet, slow computer
connections, unavailability of adequate workstations at
the point of clinical use and lack of infrastructure support.
Another theme that emerged from user feedback involved
the lack of access to reading material on diagnoses during
the trial period – most users felt this was an important part
of the system and the advice provided.
Discussion
This study demonstrates that diagnostic uncertainty
occurs frequently in clinical practice, and that it is feasible
for a DDSS, unintegrated into an EMR, to improve the
process of diagnostic assessment when used by clinicians
in real life practice. We have also shown that this improvement prevented a small but significant number of diagnostic errors of omission. A number of barriers to
computer and Internet access in the clinical setting prevented system use in a significant proportion of eligible
patients in whom subjects sought diagnostic assistance.
The DDSS studied provided advice in the field of diagnosis, an area in which computerised systems have rarely
been shown to be effective. In an early clinical study, Wexler et al showed that consultation of MEDITEL-PEDS, a
DDSS for paediatric practice, resulted in a decrease in the
number of incorrect diagnoses made by residents [24].
However, subjects did not interact with the DDSS themselves; advice generated by the system was provided to cli-
Page 11 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Figureusage
DDSS
5 data shown as distribution of number of subjects by episodes of system use
DDSS usage data shown as distribution of number of subjects by episodes of system use.
nicians, and diagnostic decisions were amended by
subjects on the basis of the information provided. The
impact of QMR was studied in similar consultative fashion: a beneficial impact was demonstrated on diagnostic
decisions as well as test ordering [25]. In a subsequent laboratory study examining the impact of two different systems (QMR and ILIAD) on simulated cases, a correct
diagnosis was added by subjects to their diagnostic
workup in 6.5% episodes [26]. Diagnostically challenging
cases were deliberately used; it was not clear that junior
clinicians would seek diagnostic advice on similar cases in
routine practice. Since the user-DDSS dynamic plays a key
role in whether these systems are used and the extent of
benefit derived from them [27,28] the above-mentioned
studies provide limited information on how clinicians
would interact with computerised DDSS to derive clinical
benefits in practice, especially in a busy environment.
Our study was notable for utilising a naturalistic design, in
which subjects used the Isabel system without extensive
training or monitoring, allowing results to be generalised
to the clinical setting. This design allowed us to explore
the complex interplay between user-DDSS interaction,
user decisions in the face of diagnostic advice, and barriers
to usage. The DDSS selected was already being used frequently in practice; a number of previous system evaluations have been confounded by inadequate usage. The
clinical performance of the DDSS studied has also been
previously validated [29]. A preliminary assessment of Isabel impact on subjects' diagnostic decisions has already
been made in a simulated environment, results of which
closely mirror our current findings [30]. Although the
nature and frequency of clinicians' information needs
have been previously described, we were able to estimate
the need for diagnostic decision support, and characterise
the subgroup of patients in whom junior clinicians sought
diagnostic advice. Since diagnostic uncertainty only
occurs in a subset of acutely ill patients, similar interventions in the future will need to be targeted, rather than
being universally applied. However, this has to be balanced against our finding that there was poor correlation
between subjects' own perception of system utility and
actual clinical benefit, which suggests that a universal
approach to usage may be more beneficial. This phenomenon has been previously described [31]. We have also
identified that junior doctors, such as SHOs, are more
Table 7: Changes in mean quality scores for diagnostic workup and clinical action plans
Diagnostic quality score change (SD)
Clinical action plan score change (SD)
SHO
Registrar
Overall
8.3 (11.6)
1.4 (6.3)
3.8 (6.1)
1.7 (7.4)
6.9 (10.3)
1.5 (6.7)
Page 12 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
Table 6: Reduction in unsafe diagnostic workups following DDSS consultation (n = 104)
Unsafe diagnostic workup
SHO
Registrar
Pre-DDSS consultation
Post-DDSS consultation
Relative Reduction (%)
28
19
17
17
39.3
10.5
likely to use and benefit from DDSS, including in an educational role. Cognitive biases, of which 'premature closure' and faulty context generation are key examples,
contribute significantly to diagnostic errors of omission
[32], and it is likely that in combination with cognitive
forcing strategies adopted during decision making, DDSS
may act as 'safety nets' for junior clinicians in practice
[33].
Fundamental deviation in function and interface design
from other expert systems may have contributed to the
observed DDSS impact on decision-making in this study.
The provision of reminders has proved highly effective in
improving the process of care in other settings [34]. Rapid
access to relevant and valid advice is crucial in ensuring
usability in busy settings prone to errors of omission –
average DDSS consultation time during this study was <2
minutes. It also appears that system adoption is possible
during clinical assessment in real time with current computer infrastructure, providing an opportunity for reduction in diagnostic error. EMR integration would allow
further control on the quality of the clinical input data as
well as provision of active decision support with minimum extra effort; such an interface has currently been
developed for Isabel and tested with four commercial
EMRs [35]. Such integration facilitates iterative use of the
Figure 6 between user perception of system utility and change in diagnostic quality score
Correlation
Correlation between user perception of system utility and change in diagnostic quality score.
Page 13 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
system during the evolution of a patient's condition, leading to increasingly specific diagnostic advice. A number of
other observations are worthy of note: despite an increase
in the number of diagnoses considered, no inappropriate
tests were triggered by the advice provided; the quality of
data input differed widely between users; the system
dynamically generated a diverse set of suggestions based
on case characteristics; the interpretation of DDSS advice
itself was user-dependent, leading to variable individual
benefit; and finally, on some occasions even useful advice
was rejected by users. User variability in data input cannot
be solely attributed to the natural language data entry
process; considerable user variation in data entry has been
demonstrated even in DDSS that employ controlled
vocabularies for input [36]. Further potential benefit from
system usage was compromised in this study due to many
reasons: unavailability of computers, poor Internet access,
and slow network connections frequently prevented subjects from accessing the DDSS. Paradoxically, the need to
enter detailed information including subjects' own clinical decisions into the trial website (not required during
real life usage) may itself have compromised system usage
during the study, limiting the extent to which usage data
from the study can be extrapolated to real life.
This study had a number of limitations. Our study was
compromised by the lack of detailed qualitative data to
fully explore issues related to why users sometimes
ignored DDSS advice, or specific cases in which users
found the DDSS useful. The comparison of system versus
a panel gold standard had its own drawbacks – Isabel was
provided variable amount of patient detail depending on
the subject who used it, while the panel were provided
detailed clinical information from medical notes.
Changes in decision making were also assessed at one
fixed point during the clinical assessment, preventing us
from examining the impact of iterative use of the DDSS
with evolving and sometimes rapidly changing clinical
information. Due to the before-after design, it could also
be argued that any improvement observed resulted purely
from subjects rethinking the case; since all appropriate
diagnoses included after system consultation were present
in the DDSS advice, this seems unlikely. Subjects also
spent negligible time between their initial assessment of
cases and processing the system's diagnostic suggestions.
Our choice of primary outcome focused on improvements in process, although we were also able to demonstrate a small but significant prevention of diagnostic
error based on the discharge diagnosis. The link between
improvements in diagnostic process and patient outcome
may be difficult to illustrate, although model developed
by Schiff et al suggests that avoiding process errors will
lead to actual errors in some instances, as we have demonstrated in this study [37]. However, in our study design, it
was not possible to test whether an 'unsafe' diagnostic
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
workup would directly lead to patient harm. Finally, due
to barriers associated with computer access and usage, we
were not able to reach the target number of cases on
whom complete medical data were available.
Conclusion
This clinical study demonstrates that it is possible for a
stand-alone diagnostic system based on the reminder
model to be used in routine practice to improve the process of diagnostic decision making among junior clinicians. Elimination of barriers to computer access is
essential to fulfil the significant need for diagnostic assistance demonstrated in this study.
Competing interests
This study was conducted when the Isabel system was
available free to users, funded by the Isabel Medical Charity. Dr Ramnarayan performed this research as part of his
MD thesis at Imperial College London. Dr Britto was a
Trustee of the medical charity in a non-remunerative post.
Ms Fisher was employed by the Isabel Medical Charity as
a Research Nurse for this study.
Since June 2004, Isabel has been managed by a commercial subsidiary of the Medical Charity called Isabel Healthcare. The system is now available only to subscribed users.
Dr Ramnarayan now advises Isabel Healthcare on
research activities on a part-time basis; Dr Britto is now
Clinical Director of Isabel Healthcare. Both hold restricted
shares in Isabel Healthcare. All other authors declare that
they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
PR conceived the study, contributed to the study design,
analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript.
BJ assisted with data collection, validation of discharge
diagnoses, and data analysis.
MC assisted with the study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and revised the draft manuscript
VN assisted with study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and helped with data analysis
RB assisted with the study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and revised the draft manuscript
AW assisted with the study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and revised the draft manuscript
HF assisted with data collection and analysis
PT assisted with study conception, study design and
revised the draft manuscript
Page 14 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:37
JW assisted with study conception, provided advice
regarding study design, and revised the draft manuscript
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/37
19.
20.
JB assisted with study conception, study design and data
analysis.
21.
All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Acknowledgements
A number of clinicians and ward administrators were involved in assisting
in study conduct at each participating centre. The authors would like to
thank Nandu Thalange, Michael Bamford, and Elmo Thambapillai for their
help in this regard. Statistical assistance was provided by Henry Potts.
22.
Financial support: This study was supported by a research grant from the
National Health Service (NHS) Research & Development Unit, London.
The sponsor did not influence the study design; the collection, analysis, and
interpretation of data; the writing of the manuscript; and the decision to
submit the manuscript for publication.
24.
References
26.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
Sandars J, Esmail A: The frequency and nature of medical error
in primary care: understanding the diversity across studies.
Fam Pract 2003, 20:231-6.
Leape L, Brennan TA, Laird N, Lawthers AG, Localio AR, Barnes BA,
Hebert L, Newhouse JP, Weiler PC, Hiatt H: The nature of
adverse events in hospitalized patients. Results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study II. N Engl J Med 1991, 324:377-84.
Davis P, Lay-Yee R, Briant R, Ali W, Scott A, Schug S: Adverse
events in New Zealand public hospitals II: preventability and
clinical context. N Z Med J 116(1183):U624. 2003 Oct 10
Bartlett EE: Physicians' cognitive errors and their liability consequences. J Healthcare Risk Manage Fall 1998:62-9.
Croskerry P: The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis
and strategies to minimize them. Acad Med 2003, 78(8):775-80.
Graber M, Frankilin N, Gordon R: Diagnostic error in internal
medicine. Arch Intern Med 165(13):1493-9. 2005 Jul 11
Burroughs TE, Waterman AD, Gallagher TH, Waterman B, Adams D,
Jeffe DB, Dunagan WC, Garbutt J, Cohen MM, Cira J, Inguanzo J, Fraser VJ: Patient concerns about medical errors in emergency
departments. Acad Emerg Med 2005, 12(1):57-64.
Weingart SN, Wilson RM, Gibberd RW, Harrison B: Epidemiology
of medical error. BMJ 320(7237):774-7. 2000 Mar 18
Rothschild JM, Bates DW, Leape LL: Preventable medical injuries
in older patients. Arch Intern Med 160(18):2717-28. 2000 Oct 9
Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N: Reducing diagnostic errors in
medicine: what's the goal? Acad Med 2002, 77(10):981-92.
Garg AX, Adhikari NK, McDonald H, Rosas-Arellano MP, Devereaux
PJ, Beyene J, Sam J, Haynes RB: Effects of computerized clinical
decision support systems on practitioner performance and
patient outcomes: a systematic review.
JAMA
293(10):1223-38. 2005 Mar 9
Miller R, Masarie FE, Myers JD: Quick medical reference (QMR)
for diagnostic assistance. MD Comput 1986, 3(5):34-48.
Warner HR Jr: Iliad: moving medical decision-making into new
frontiers. Methods Inf Med 1989, 28(4):370-2.
Barness LA, Tunnessen WW Jr, Worley WE, Simmons TL, Ringe TB
Jr: Computer-assisted diagnosis in pediatrics. Am J Dis Child
1974, 127(6):852-8.
Graber MA, VanScoy D: How well does decision support software perform in the emergency department? Emerg Med J
2003, 20(5):426-8.
Welford CR: A comprehensive computerized patient record
with automated linkage to QMR. Proc Annu Symp Comput Appl
Med Care 1994:814-8.
Elhanan G, Socratous SA, Cimino JJ: Integrating DXplain into a
clinical information system using the World Wide Web. Proc
AMIA Annu Fall Symp 1996:348-52.
Greenough A: Help from ISABEL for pediatric diagnoses. Lancet 360(9341):1259. 2002 Oct 19
23.
25.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Kulkarni G, Rao A, Britto J: A Novel
Diagnostic Aid (ISABEL): Development and Preliminary
Evaluation of Clinical Performance. Medinfo 2004:1091-5.
Nelson SJ, Blois MS, Tuttle MS, Erlbaum M, Harrison P, Kim H, Winkelmann B, Yamashita D: Evaluating RECONSIDER. A computer program for diagnostic prompting. J Med Syst 1985, 9(5–
6):379-88.
Eccles M, McColl E, Steen N, Rousseau N, Grimshaw J, Parkin D,
Purves I: Effect of computerised evidence based guidelines on
management of asthma and angina in adults in primary care:
cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 325(7370):941. 2002
Oct 26
Briggs JS, Fitch CJ: The ISABEL user survey. Med Inform Internet
Med 2005, 30(2):167-72.
Ramnarayan P, Kapoor RR, Coren M, Nanduri V, Tomlinson AL, Taylor PM, Wyatt JC, Britto JF: Measuring the impact of diagnostic
decision support on the quality of clinical decision-making:
development of a reliable and valid composite score. J Am
Med Inform Assoc 2003, 10(6):563-72.
Wexler JR, Swender PT, Tunnessen WW Jr, Oski FA: Impact of a
system of computer-assisted diagnosis. Initial evaluation of
the hospitalized patient. Am J Dis Child 1975, 129(2):203-5.
Bankowitz RA, McNeil MA, Challinor SM, Miller RA: Effect of a
computer-assisted general medicine diagnostic consultation
service on housestaff diagnostic strategy. Methods Inf Med
1989, 28(4):352-6.
Friedman CP, Elstein AS, Wolf FM, Murphy GC, Franz TM, Heckerling
PS, Fine PL, Miller TM, Abraham V: Enhancement of clinicians'
diagnostic reasoning by computer-based consultation: a
multisite study of 2 systems. JAMA 282(19):1851-6. 1999 Nov 17
Miller RA: Evaluating evaluations of medical diagnostic systems. J Am Med Inform Assoc 1996, 3(6):429-31.
Berner ES, Maisiak RS: Influence of case and physician characteristics on perceptions of decision support systems. J Am
Med Inform Assoc 1999, 6(5):428-34.
Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, Coren M, Winrow A, Britto J:
ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic aid for pediatrics: results from an initial performance evaluation. Arch Dis
Child 2003, 88:408-13.
Ramnarayan P, Roberts GC, Coren M, Nanduri V, Tomlinson A, Taylor PM, Wyatt JC, Britto JF: Assessment of the potential impact
of a reminder system on the reduction of diagnostic errors:
a quasi-experimental study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 6(1):22.
2006 Apr 28
Friedman CP, Gatti GG, Franz TM, Murphy GC, Wolf FM, Heckerling
PS, Fine PL, Miller TM, Elstein AS: Do physicians know when their
diagnoses are correct? Implications for decision support and
error reduction. J Gen Intern Med 2005, 20(4):334-9.
Croskerry P: Achieving quality in clinical decision making:
cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med
2002, 9(11):1184-204.
Mamede S, Schmidt HG: The structure of reflective practice in
medicine. Med Educ 2004, 38(12):1302-8.
Dexter PR, Perkins S, Overhage JM, Maharry K, Kohler RB, McDonald
CJ: A computerized reminder to increase the use of preventive care for hospitalized patients. N Engl J Med 2001,
345:965-970.
[http://www.healthcareitnews.com/NewsArticleView.aspx?Conten
tID=3030&ContentTypeID=3&IssueID=19].
Bankowitz RA, Blumenfeld BH, Guise Bettinsoli N: User variability
in abstracting and entering printed case histories with Quick
Medical Reference (QMR). In Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual
Symposium on Computer Applications in Medical Care New York: IEEE
Computer Society Press; 1987:68-73.
Gordon Schiff D, Seijeoung Kim, Richard Abrams, Karen Cosby,
Bruce Lambert, Arthur Elstein S, Scott Hasler, Nela Krosnjar, Richard
Odwazny, Mary Wisniewski F, Robert McNutt A: Diagnostic diagnosis errors: lessons from a multi-institutional collaborative
project.
[http://www.ahrq.gov/downloads/pub/advances/vol2/
Schiff.pdf]. Accessed 10 July 2006
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed
here:
Page 15 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
WHAT IS THE IMPACT ON CHANGE OF DIAGNOSIS
DECISION MAKING BEFORE AND AFTER USING A
DIAGNOSIS DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEM?
Ramnarayan P, Roberts GC, Coren M et al.
Assessment of the potential impact of a reminder system on the reduction of
diagnostic errors: a quasi-experimental study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak.
2006 Apr 28;6:22
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
BMC Medical Informatics and
Decision Making
BioMed Central
Open Access
Research article
Assessment of the potential impact of a reminder system on the
reduction of diagnostic errors: a quasi-experimental study
Padmanabhan Ramnarayan*1, Graham C Roberts2, Michael Coren3,
Vasantha Nanduri4, Amanda Tomlinson5, Paul M Taylor6, Jeremy C Wyatt7
and Joseph F Britto5
Address: 1Children's Acute Transport Service (CATS), 44B Bedford Row, London, WC1H 4LL, UK, 2Department of Paediatric Allergy and
Respiratory Medicine, Southampton University Hospital Trust, Tremona Road, Southampton, SO16 6YD, UK, 3Department of Paediatrics, St
Mary's Hospital, Paddington, London, W2 1NY, UK, 4Department of Paediatrics, Watford General Hospital, Vicarage Road, Watford, WD18 0HB,
UK, 5Isabel Healthcare Ltd, Po Box 244, Haslemere, Surrey, GU27 1WU, UK, 6Centre for Health Informatics and Multiprofessional Education
(CHIME), Archway Campus, Highgate Hill, London, N19 5LW, UK and 7Health Informatics Centre, The Mackenzie Building, University of
Dundee, Dundee, DD2 4BF, UK
Email: Padmanabhan Ramnarayan* - ramnarayan@msn.com; Graham C Roberts - g.c.roberts@soton.ac.uk; Michael Coren - michael.coren@stmarys.nhs.uk; Vasantha Nanduri - v_nanduri@hotmail.com; Amanda Tomlinson - mandy@isabelhealthcare.com;
Paul M Taylor - p.taylor@chime.ucl.ac.uk; Jeremy C Wyatt - j.wyatt@chs.dundee.ac.uk; Joseph F Britto - joseph@isabelhealthcare.com
* Corresponding author
Published: 28 April 2006
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
doi:10.1186/1472-6947-6-22
Received: 25 September 2005
Accepted: 28 April 2006
This article is available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
© 2006 Ramnarayan et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Abstract
Background: Computerized decision support systems (DSS) have mainly focused on improving clinicians' diagnostic accuracy
in unusual and challenging cases. However, since diagnostic omission errors may predominantly result from incomplete workup
in routine clinical practice, the provision of appropriate patient- and context-specific reminders may result in greater impact on
patient safety. In this experimental study, a mix of easy and difficult simulated cases were used to assess the impact of a novel
diagnostic reminder system (ISABEL) on the quality of clinical decisions made by various grades of clinicians during acute
assessment.
Methods: Subjects of different grades (consultants, registrars, senior house officers and medical students), assessed a balanced
set of 24 simulated cases on a trial website. Subjects recorded their clinical decisions for the cases (differential diagnosis, testordering and treatment), before and after system consultation. A panel of two pediatric consultants independently provided gold
standard responses for each case, against which subjects' quality of decisions was measured. The primary outcome measure was
change in the count of diagnostic errors of omission (DEO). A more sensitive assessment of the system's impact was achieved
using specific quality scores; additional consultation time resulting from DSS use was also calculated.
Results: 76 subjects (18 consultants, 24 registrars, 19 senior house officers and 15 students) completed a total of 751 case
episodes. The mean count of DEO fell from 5.5 to 5.0 across all subjects (repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.001); no significant
interaction was seen with subject grade. Mean diagnostic quality score increased after system consultation (0.044; 95%
confidence interval 0.032, 0.054). ISABEL reminded subjects to consider at least one clinically important diagnosis in 1 in 8 case
episodes, and prompted them to order an important test in 1 in 10 case episodes. Median extra time taken for DSS consultation
was 1 min (IQR: 30 sec to 2 min).
Conclusion: The provision of patient- and context-specific reminders has the potential to reduce diagnostic omissions across
all subject grades for a range of cases. This study suggests a promising role for the use of future reminder-based DSS in the
reduction of diagnostic error.
Page 1 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
Background
A recent Institute of Medicine report has brought the
problem of medical error under intense scrutiny[1]. While
the use of computerized prescription software has been
shown to substantially reduce the incidence of medication-related error [2,3], few solutions have demonstrated
a similar impact on diagnostic error. Diagnostic errors
impose a significant burden on modern healthcare: they
account for a large proportion of medical adverse events
in general [4-6], and form the second leading cause for
malpractice suits against hospitals [7]. In particular, diagnostic errors of omission (DEO) during acute medical
assessment, resulting from cognitive biases such as 'premature closure' and 'confirmation bias', lead to incomplete diagnostic workup and 'missed diagnoses' [8]. This
is especially relevant in settings such as family practice [9],
as well as hospital areas such as the emergency room and
critical care [11,12], 20% of patients discharged from
emergency rooms raised concerns in a recent survey that
their clinical assessment had been complicated by diagnostic error [12]. The use of clinical decision-support systems (DSS) has been one of many strategies proposed for
the reduction of diagnostic errors in practice [13]. Consequently, a number of DSS have been developed over the
past few years to assist clinicians during the process of
medical diagnosis [14-16].
Even though studies of several diagnostic DSS have demonstrated improved physician performance in simulated
(and rarely real) patient encounters [17,18], two specific
characteristics may have contributed to their infrequent
use in routine practice: intended purpose and design.
Many general diagnostic DSS were built as 'expert systems'
to solve diagnostic conundrums and provide the correct
diagnosis during a 'clinical dead-end' [19]. Since true
diagnostic dilemmas are rare in practice [20], and the initiative for DSS use had to originate from the physician,
diagnostic advice was not sought routinely, particularly
since clinicians prefer to store the patterns needed to solve
medical problems in their heads [21]. There is, however,
evidence that clinicians frequently underestimate their
need for diagnostic assistance, and that the perception of
diagnostic difficulty does not correlate with their clinical
performance [22]. In addition, due to the demands of the
information era [23]. diagnostic errors may not be
restricted to cases perceived as being difficult, and might
occur even when dealing with common problems in a
stressful environment under time pressure [24]. Further,
most 'expert systems' utilized a design in which clinical
data entry was achieved through a controlled vocabulary
specific to each DSS. This process frequently took > 15
minutes, contributing to infrequent use in a busy clinical
environment [25]. These 'expert systems' also provided
between 20 and 30 diagnostic possibilities [26], with
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
detailed explanations, leading to a lengthy DSS consultation process.
In order to significantly affect the occurrence of diagnostic
error, it seems reasonable to conclude that DSS advice
must therefore be readily available, and sought, during
most clinical encounters, even if the perceived need for
diagnostic assistance is minor. Ideally, real-time advice for
diagnosis can be actively provided by integrating a diagnostic DSS into an existing electronic medical record
(EMR), as has been attempted in the past [27,28]. However, the limited uptake of EMRs capable of recording sufficient narrative clinical detail currently in clinical practice
indicates that a stand-alone system may prove much more
practical in the medium term [29]. The key characteristic
of a successful system would be the ability to deliver reliable diagnostic reminders rapidly following a brief data
entry process in most clinical situations. ISABEL (ISABEL
Healthcare, UK) is a novel Web-based pediatric diagnostic
reminder system that suggests important diagnoses during clinical assessment [30,31]. The development of the
system and its underlying structure have been described in
detail previously [32,33]. The main hypotheses underlying the development of ISABEL were that the provision of
diagnostic reminders generated following a brief data
entry session in free text would promote user uptake, and
lead to improvement in the quality of diagnostic decision
making in acute medical settings. The reminders provided
(a set of 10 in the first instance) aimed to remind clinicians of important diagnoses that they might have missed
in the workup. Data entry is by means of natural language
descriptions of the patient's clinical features, including
any combination of symptoms, signs and test results. The
system's knowledge base consists of natural language text
descriptions of > 5000 diseases, in contrast to most 'expert
systems' that use complex disease databases [34-36]. The
advantages and trade-offs of these differences in system
design have been discussed in detail elsewhere [37]. In
summary, although the ability to rapidly enter patient features in natural language to derive a short-list of diagnostic suggestions may allow frequent use by clinicians
during most patient encounters, variability resulting from
the use of natural language for data entry, and the absence
of probability ranking, may compromise the accuracy and
usefulness of the diagnostic suggestions.
The overall evaluation of the ISABEL system was planned
in systematic fashion in a series of consecutive studies
[38].
a) An initial clinical performance evaluation: This would
evaluate the feasibility of providing relevant diagnostic
suggestions for a range of cases when data is entered in
natural language. System accuracy, speed and relevance of
suggestions were studied.
Page 2 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
b) An assessment of the impact of the system in a quasiexperimental setting: This would examine the effects of
diagnostic decision support on subjects using simulated
cases.
c) An assessment of the impact of the system in a real life
setting: This would examine the effects of diagnostic
advice on clinicians in real patients in their natural environment.
In the initial performance evaluation, the ISABEL system
formed the unit of intervention, and the quality of its
diagnostic suggestions was validated against data drawn
from 99 hypothetical cases and 100 real patients. Key
findings from cases were entered into the system in free
text by one of the developers. The system included the
final diagnosis in 95% of the cases [39]. This design was
similar to early evaluations of a number of other individual diagnostic DSS [40-42], as well as a large study assessing the performance characteristics of four expert
diagnostic systems [43]. Since this step studied ISABEL in
isolation, and did not include users uninvolved in the
development of the system, it was vital to examine DSS
impact on decision making by demonstrating in the subsequent step that the clinician-DSS combination functioned
better than either the clinician or the system working in
isolation [44,45]. Evaluation of impact is especially relevant to ISABEL: despite good system performance when
tested in isolation, clinicians may not benefit from its
advice either due to variability associated with user data
entry leading to poor results, or the inability to distinguish
between diagnostic suggestions due to the lack of ranking
[46]. A previous evaluation of Quick Medical Reference
(QMR) assessed a group of clinicians working a set of difficult cases, and suggested that the extent of benefit gained
by different users varied with their level of experience
[47].
In this study, we aimed to perform an impact evaluation
of ISABEL in a quasi-experimental setting in order to
quantify the effects of diagnostic advice on the quality of
clinical decisions made by various grades of clinicians
during acute assessment, using a mix of easy and difficult
simulated cases drawn from all pediatric sub-specialties.
Study design was based on an earlier evaluation of the
impact of ILIAD and QMR on diagnostic reasoning in a
simulated environment [48]. Our key outcome measure
focused on appropriateness of decisions during diagnostic
workup rather than accuracy in identifying the correct
diagnosis. The validity of textual case simulations has previously been demonstrated in medical education exercises
[49], and during the assessment of mock clinical decision
making [50,51].
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Methods
The simulated field study involved recording subjects'
clinical decisions regarding diagnoses, test-ordering and
treatment for a set of simulated cases, both before and
immediately after DSS consultation. The impact of diagnostic reminders was determined by measuring changes
in the quality of decisions made by subjects. In this study,
the quality of ISABEL's diagnostic suggestion list per se was
not examined. The study was coordinated at Imperial College School of Medicine, St Mary's Hospital, London, UK
between February and August 2002. The study was
approved by the Local Research Ethics Committee.
Subjects
A convenience sample consisting of pediatricians of different grades (senior house officers [interns], registrars [residents] and consultants [attending physicians] from
different geographical locations across the UK), and final
year medical students, was enrolled for the study. All students were drawn from one medical school (Imperial College School of Medicine, London, UK). Clinicians were
recruited by invitation from the ISABEL registered user
database which consisted of a mixture of regular users as
well as pediatricians who had never used the system after
registration. After a short explanation of the study procedure, all subjects who consented for the study were
included within the sample.
Cases
Cases were drawn from a pool of 72 textual case simulations, constructed by one investigator, based on case histories of real children presenting to emergency
departments (data collected during earlier evaluation).
Each case was limited to between 150 and 200 words, and
only described the initial presenting symptoms, clinical
signs and basic laboratory test results in separate sections.
Since the clinical data were collected from pediatric emergency rooms, the amount of clinical information available at assessment was limited but typical for this setting.
Ample negative features were included in order to prevent
the reader from picking up positive cues from the text.
These cases were then classified into one of 12 different
pediatric sub-specialties (e.g. cardiology, respiratory) and
to one of 3 case difficulty levels within each specialty (1unusual, 2-not unusual, and 3-common clinical presentation, with reference to UK general hospital pediatric practice) by the author. This allocation process was duplicated
by a pediatric consultant working independently. Both
investigators assigned 57 cases to the same sub-specialty
and 42 cases to both the same sub-specialty and the same
level of difficulty (raw agreement 0.79 and 0.58 respectively). From the 42 cases in which both investigators
agreed regarding the allocation of both specialty and level
of difficulty, 24 cases were drawn such that a pair of cases
per sub-specialty representing two different levels of diffi-
Page 3 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
culty (level 1 & 2, 1 & 3 or 2 & 3) was chosen for the final
case mix. This process ensured a balanced set of cases representing all sub-specialties and comprising easy as well as
difficult cases.
Data collection website
A customized, password protected version of ISABEL was
used to collect data during the study. This differed from
the main website in that it automatically displayed the
study cases to each subject in sequence, assigned each case
episode a unique study number, and recorded time data
in addition to displaying ten diagnostic suggestions. Three
separate text boxes were provided to record subjects' clinical decisions (diagnoses, tests and treatment) pre- and
post-DSS consultation. The use of the customized trial
website ensured that subjects proceeded from one step to
the next without being able to skip steps or revise clinical
decisions already submitted.
Training
Training was intended only to familiarize subjects with
the trial website. During training, all subjects were
assigned unique log-in and passwords, and one sample
case as practice material. Practice sessions involving medical students were supervised by one investigator in group
sessions of 2–3 subjects each. Pediatricians (being from
geographically disparate locations) were not supervised
during training, but received detailed instructions regarding the use of the trial website by email. Context-specific
help was provided at each step on the website for assistance during the practice session. All subjects completed
their assigned practice case, and were recruited for the
study.
Study procedure
Subjects were allowed to complete their assessments of
the simulated cases from any computer connected to the
Internet at any time (i.e. they were not supervised). After
logging into the trial website, subjects were presented with
text from a case simulation. They assessed the case,
abstracted the salient clinical features according to their
own interpretation of the case, and entered them into the
designated search query box in free text. Following this,
they keyed in their decisions regarding diagnostic workup,
test-ordering and treatment into the designated textboxes.
These constituted pre-DSS clinical decisions. See figure 1
for an illustration of this step of the study procedure. On
submitting this information, a list of diagnostic suggestions was instantly presented to the subject based on the
abstracted clinical features. The subjects could not read
the case text again at this stage, preventing them from
processing the case a second time, thus avoiding 'secondlook' bias. Diagnostic suggestions were different for different users since the search query was unique for each subject, depending on their understanding of the case and
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
how they expressed it in natural language. On the basis of
the diagnostic suggestions, subjects could modify their
pre-DSS clinical decisions by adding or deleting items:
these constituted post-DSS clinical decisions. All clinical
decisions, and the time taken to complete each step, were
recorded automatically. See figure 2 for an illustration of
this step of the study procedure. The text from one case,
and the variability associated with its interpretation during the study, is depicted in figure 3.
Each subject was presented with 12 cases such that one of
the pair drawn from each sub-specialty was displayed.
Cases were presented in random order (in no particular
order of sub-specialty). Subjects could terminate their session at any time and return to complete the remainder of
cases. If a session was terminated midway through a case,
that case was presented again on the subject's return. If the
website detected no activity for > 2 hours, the subject was
automatically logged off, and the session was continued
on their return. All subjects had 3 weeks to complete their
assigned 12 cases. Since each case was used more than
once, by different subjects, we termed each attempt by a
subject at a case as a 'case episode'.
Scoring metrics
We aimed to assess if the provision of key diagnostic
reminders would reduce errors of omission in the simulated environment. For the purposes of this study, a subject was defined to have committed a DEO for a case
episode if they failed to include all 'clinically important
diagnoses' in the diagnostic workup (rather than failing to
include the 'correct diagnosis'). A diagnosis was judged
'clinically important' if an expert panel working the case
independently decided that the particular diagnosis had
to be included in the workup in order to ensure safe and
appropriate clinical decision making, i.e. they would significantly affect patient management and/or course, and
failure to do so would be construed clinically inadequate.
The expert panel comprised two general pediatricians
with > 3 years consultant level experience. 'Clinically
important' diagnoses suggested by the panel thus
included the 'most likely diagnosis/es' and other key diagnoses; they did not constitute a full differential containing
all plausible diagnoses. This outcome variable was quantified by a binary measure (for each case episode, a subject
either committed a DEO or not). Errors of omission were
defined for tests and treatments in similar fashion.
We also sought a more sensitive assessment of changes in
the quality of clinical decisions made by subjects in this
study. Since an appropriate and validated instrument was
essential for this purpose, a measurement study was first
undertaken to develop and validate such an instrument.
The measurement study, including the development and
validation of a diagnostic quality score and a management
Page 4 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Figure 1 of ISABEL simulated study procedure – step 1
Screenshot
Screenshot of ISABEL simulated study procedure – step 1. This figure shows how one subject was presented with the
text of a case simulation, how he could search ISABEL by using summary clinical features, and record his clinical decisions prior
to viewing ISABEL's results. For this case, clinically important diagnoses provided by the expert panel are: nasopharyngitis (OR
viral upper respiratory tract infection) and meningitis/encephalitis. This subject has committed a DEO (failed to include both
clinically important diagnoses in his diagnostic workup).
plan quality score, has previously been reported in detail
[52]. The scoring process, tested using a subset of cases
worked on by clinicians during this study (190 case episodes), was reliable (intraclass correlation coefficient
0.79) and valid (face, construct and concurrent validity).
During the scoring process, the expert panel was provided
an aggregate list of decisions drawn from all subjects (preand post-DDSS consultation) for each case. They provided
Page 5 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Figure 2 of ISABEL simulated study procedure – step 2
Screenshot
Screenshot of ISABEL simulated study procedure – step 2. This figure shows how the same subject was provided the
results of ISABEL's search in one click, and how he was provided the opportunity to modify the clinical decisions made in step
1. It was not possible to go back from this step to step 1 to modify the clinical decisions made earlier. Notice that the subject
has not identified meningitis/encephalitis from the ISABEL suggestions as clinically important. He has made no changes to his
workup, and has committed a DEO despite system advice.
measurements of quality for each of the clinical decisions
in addition to identifying 'clinically important' decisions
for each case. Prior to scoring, one investigator (PR)
mapped diagnoses proposed by subjects and the expert
panel to the nearest equivalent diagnoses in the ISABEL
database. Quality of each diagnostic decision was scored
Page 6 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
A 6-week old infant is admitted with a week’s history of poor feeding. Whereas previously the
infant had been growing along the 25th centile, he has now fallen below the 10th centile. In the
past week, his parents also think he is breathing quite quickly. He has a cough and is vomiting
most feeds.
On examination, he is tachypnoeic, has moderate respiratory distress, and a cough. A grade 3
murmur is heard all over his precordium. He also has a 3 cm liver palpable.
Initial lab results show Haemoglobin 9 g/dL, White cell count 12.4 x 106/µL, Platelets 180 x
106/µL.
*This case was assigned to Cardiology and case level 2 by both investigators
User A
User B
User C
User D
User E
failure to thrive, growth below 10th centile, previous growth on 25th centile,
tachypnoeic, moderate respiratory distress, cough, precordial grade 3 murmur,
palpable liver 3cm, neutrophilia, thrombocytophilia
poor feeding for 1/52, FTT, DIB + COUGH + vomiting, HEART MURMUR, liver
palpable
Weight loss, cough, vomiting, respiratory distress, heart murmur, hepatomegaly
poor feeding, failure to thrive, tachypnoeic, cough, vomiting, respiratory distress,
heart murmur, large liver, anaemia
poor feeding, wt loss, tafchypneic, cardiac murmur
‘Most likely diagnosis’
‘Clinically important’ diagnoses (panel)
Ventricular septal defect
Ventricular septal defect (or other congenital
heart disease with left to right shunt)
Nasopharyngitis (or Bronchiolitis)
Figure
and
Example
clinically
3of one
important
simulated
diagnoses
case used
as in
judged
study*,
by the
panel
variability in clinical features as abstracted by five different users (verbatim),
Example of one simulated case used in study*, the variability in clinical features as abstracted by five different users (verbatim),
and clinically important diagnoses as judged by panel.
for the degree of plausibility, likelihood in the clinical setting, and its impact on further patient management. These
measurements were used to derive scores for each set of
subjects' clinical decisions (diagnostic workup, tests and
treatments). As per the scoring system, subjects' decision
plans were awarded the highest score (score range: 0 to 1)
only if they were both comprehensive (contained all
important clinical decisions), and focused (contained only
important decisions). Scores were calculated for each subject's diagnostic, test-ordering and treatment plans both
Page 7 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
Cases
Subject
decisions
Panel
suggests
System
generates list
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
consultation and interaction with grade was assessed by
two-way mixed-model analysis of variance (grade being
between-subjects factor and time being within-subjects
factor). Mean number of DEOs was calculated for each
subject grade, and DEOs were additionally analyzed
according to level of case difficulty. Statistical significance
was set at a p value of 0.05.
Responses from all
Scored by panel on scale
for appropriateness
Subject decides
again
Subjects’ pre-system and postsystem list of suggestions
Reference standard
(responses scored >0)
Compare
Subjects’ performance
Figure 4 of scoring procedure
Schematic
Schematic of scoring procedure.
pre- and post-ISABEL consultation. Figure 4 provides a
schematic diagram of the complete scoring procedure.
Primary outcome
1. Change in the number of diagnostic errors of omission
among subjects.
Secondary outcomes
1. Mean change in subjects' diagnostic, test-ordering and
treatment plan quality scores.
2. Change in the number of irrelevant diagnoses contained within the diagnostic workup.
3. Proportion of case episodes in which at least one additional 'important' diagnosis, test or treatment decision
was considered by the subject after DSS consultation.
4. Additional time taken for DSS consultation.
Analysis
Subjects were used as the unit of analysis for the primary
outcome measure. For each subject, the total number of
DEOs was counted separately for pre- and post-DSS diagnostic workup plans; only subjects who had completed all
assigned cases were included in this calculation. Statistically significant changes in DEO count following DDSS
Subjects were used as the unit of analysis for the change in
mean quality scores (the development of quality scores
and their validation has been previously described; however, the scores have never been used as an outcome measure prior to this evaluation). In the first step, subjects'
quality score (pre- and post-DSS) was calculated for each
case episode. For each subject, a mean quality score across
all 12 cases was computed. Only case episodes from subjects who completed all 12 assigned cases were used during this calculation. A two-way mixed model ANOVA
(grade as between-subjects factor; time as within-subjects
factor) was used to examine statistically significant differences in quality scores. This analysis was performed for
diagnostic quality scores as well as test ordering and treatment plan scores. Data from a pilot study suggested that
data from 64 subjects were needed to demonstrate a mean
diagnostic quality score change of 0.03 (standard deviation 0.06, power 80%, level of significance 5%).
Using subjects as the unit of analysis, the mean count of
diagnoses (and irrelevant diagnoses) included in the
workup was calculated pre- and post-DSS consultation for
each subject as an average across all case attempts. Only
subjects who attempted all assigned cases were included
in this analysis. Using this data, a mean count for diagnoses (and irrelevant diagnoses) was calculated for each
subject grade. A two-way mixed model ANOVA was used
to assess statistically significant differences in this outcome with respect to grade as well as occasion. Using case
episodes as the unit of analysis, the proportion of case episodes in which at least one additional 'important' diagnosis, test or treatment was prompted by ISABEL was
determined. The proportion of case episodes in which at
least one clinically significant decision was deleted, and at
least one inappropriate decision was added, after system
consultation, was also computed. All data were analyzed
separately for the subjects' grades.
Two further analyses were conducted to enable the interpretation of our results. First, in order to provide a direct
comparison of our results with other studies, we used case
episodes as the unit of analysis and examined the presence
of the 'most likely diagnosis' in the diagnostic workup.
The 'most likely diagnosis' was part of the set of 'clinically
important' diagnoses provided by the panel, and represented the closest match to a 'correct' diagnosis in our
study design. This analysis was conducted separately for
Page 8 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Table 1: Study participants, cases and case episodes*
Grade of subject
Consultant (%)
Registrar (%)
SHO (%)
Subjects invited to participate
27 (27.8)
33 (34)
20 (20.6)
17 (17.5)
97
Subjects who attempted at least one case
(attempters)
Subjects who attempted at least six cases
Subjects who completed all 12 cases (completers)
18 (23.7)
24 (31.6)
19 (25)
15 (19.7)
76
751
24
16 (25.8)
15 (28.8)
18 (29)
14 (26.9)
15 (24.2)
10 (19.2)
13 (20.9)
13 (25)
62
52
715
624
24
24
2
1
3
0
6
Regular DSS users (usage > once/week)
Student (%) Total Case episodes
Cases
* Since each case was assessed more than once, each attempt by a subject at a case was termed as a 'case episode'.
each grade. Second, since it was important to verify
whether any reduction of omission errors was directly
prompted by ISABEL, or simply by subjects re-thinking
about the assigned cases, all case episodes in which at least
one additional significant diagnosis was added by the user
were examined. If the diagnostic suggestion added by the
user had been displayed in the DSS list of suggestions, it
strongly suggested that the system, rather than subjects' rethinking, prompted these additions.
Results
The characteristics of subjects, cases and case episodes are
summarized in table 1. Ninety seven subjects were invited
for the study. Although all subjects consented and completed their training, only seventy six subjects attempted
at least one case (attempters) during their allocated three
weeks. This group consisted of 15 medical students, 19
SHOs, 24 registrars and 18 consultants. There was no significant difference between attempters and non-attempters with respect to grade (Chi square test, p 0.07). Only 6/
76 subjects had used ISABEL regularly (at least once a
week) prior to the study period (3 SHOs, 1 Registrar and
2 Consultants); all the others had registered for the service, but never used the DSS previously. A total of 751 case
episodes were completed by the end of the study period.
Fifty two subjects completed all assigned 12 cases to produce 624 case episodes (completers); 24 other subjects
did not complete all their assigned cases (non-completers). Completers and non-completers did not differ significantly with respect to grade (Chi square test, p 0.06).
However, more subjects were trained remotely in the noncompleters group (Chi square test, p 0.003). The majority
of non-completers had worked at least two cases (75%);
slightly less than half (42%) had worked at least 6 cases.
Forty-seven diagnoses were considered 'clinically important' by the panel across all 24 cases (average ~ 2 per case).
For 21/24 cases, the panel had specified a single 'most
likely diagnosis'; for 3 cases, two diagnoses were included
in this definition.
Diagnostic errors of omission
624 case episodes generated by 52 subjects were used to
examine DEO. During the pre-DSS consultation phase, all
subjects performed a DEO in at least one of their cases,
and 21.1% (11/52) in more than half their cases. In the
pre-ISABEL consultation stage, medical students and
SHOs committed the most and least number of DEO
respectively (6.6 vs. 4.4); this gradient was maintained
post-ISABEL consultation (5.9 vs. 4.1). Overall, 5.5 DEO
were noted per subject pre-DSS consultation; this reduced
to 5.0 DEO after DSS advice (p < 0.001). No significant
interaction was noticed with grade (F3, 48 = 0.71, p = 0.55).
Reduction in DEO following DSS advice within each
grade is shown in table 2. Overall, more DEOs were noted
Table 2: Mean count of diagnostic errors of omission (DEO) pre-ISABEL and post-ISABEL consultation
Grade of subject
DEO pre-ISABEL (SD)
DEO post-ISABEL (SD)
Reduction (SD)
Consultant
Registrar
SHO
Medical student
5.13 (1.3)
5.64 (1.5)
4.4 (1.6)
6.61 (1.3)
4.6 (1.4)
5.14 (1.6)
4.1 (1.6)
5.92 (1.4)
0.53 (0.7)
0.5 (0.5)
0.3 (0.5)
0.69 (0.7)
Mean DEO across all subjects (n = 52)*
5.50 (1.6)
4.98 (1.5)
0.52 (0.6)
*Total number of subjects who completed all 12 assigned cases
Page 9 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
Table 3: Mean DEO count analyzed by level of case and subject
grade
Grade
Consultant
Registrar
SHO
Medical student
Difficult cases
Easy cases
Pre-DSS
Post-DSS
Pre-DSS
Post-DSS
1.66
2.21
1.3
2.92
1.47
1.93
1.2
2.54
2.0
2.14
2.0
2.54
1.87
1.92
1.8
2.30
for easy cases compared to difficult cases pre- and postDSS advice (2.17 vs. 2.05 and 2.0 vs. 1.8); however, this
was not true for medical students as a subgroup (2.5 vs.
2.9). Improvement following DSS advice seemed greater
for difficult cases for all subjects, although this was not
statistically significant. These findings are summarized in
table 3.
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
workup pre-DSS advice was 3.9. This increased to 5.7
post-DSS consultation (an increase of 1.8 diagnoses). The
increase was largest for medical students (a mean increase
of 2.6 diagnoses) and least for consultants (1.4 diagnoses). The ANOVA showed significant interaction
between grade and occasion (F3,58 = 3.14, p = 0.034). The
number of irrelevant diagnoses in the workup changed
from 0.7 pre-DSS to 1.4 post-DSS advice (an increase of
0.7 irrelevant diagnoses, 95% CI 0.5–0.75). There was a
significant difference in this increase across grades (most
for medical students and least for consultants; 1.1 vs. 0.3
irrelevant diagnoses, F3, 48 = 6.33, p < 0.01). The increase
in irrelevant diagnoses did not result in a corresponding
increase in the number of irrelevant or deleterious tests
and treatments (an increase of 0.09 tests and 0.03 treatment decisions).
Mean quality score changes
624 case episodes from 52 subjects who had completed
all assigned 12 cases were used for this analysis. Table 4
summarizes mean diagnostic quality scores pre- and postISABEL consultation, and the change in mean quality
score for diagnoses, for each grade of subject. There was a
significant change in the weighted mean of the diagnostic
quality score (0.044; 95% confidence interval: 0.032,
0.054; p < 0.001). No significant interaction between
grade and occasion was demonstrated. In 9/52 subjects
(17.3%), the pre-DSS score for diagnostic quality was
higher than the post-DSS score, indicating that subjects
had lengthened their diagnostic workup without substantially improving its quality. Overall, the mean score for
test-ordering plans increased significantly from 0.345 to
0.364 (an increase of 0.019, 95% CI 0.011–0.027, t51 =
4.91, p < 0.001); this increase was smaller for treatment
plans (0.01, 95% CI 0.007–0.012, t51 = 7.15, p < 0.001).
Additional diagnoses, tests and treatment decisions
At least one 'clinically important' diagnosis was added by
the subject to their differential diagnosis after ISABEL consultation in 94/751 case episodes (12.5%, 95% CI 10.1%14.9%). 47/76 (61.8%) subjects added at least one 'clinically important' diagnosis to their diagnostic workup after
consultation. Overall, 130 'clinically important' diagnoses
were added after DSS advice during the experiment. In
general, students were reminded to consider many more
important diagnoses than consultants, although this was
not statistically significant (44 vs. 26, Chi square p >
0.05); a similar gradient was seen for difficult cases, but
DSS consultation seemed helpful even for easy cases. Similar proportions for tests and treatment items were smaller
in magnitude (table 6). No clinically significant diagnoses
were deleted after consultation. Important tests included
by subjects in their pre-DSS plan were sometimes deleted
from the post-DSS plan (64 individual items from 44 case
episodes). A similar effect was seen for treatment steps (34
individual items from 24 case episodes). An inappropriate
test was added to the post-ISABEL list in 7/751 cases.
Number of irrelevant diagnoses
624 case episodes from 52 subjects were used for this
analysis. The results are illustrated in table 5. Overall, the
mean count of diagnoses included by subjects in their
751 case episodes were used to examine the presence of
the 'most likely diagnosis'. Overall, the 'most likely diagnosis/es' were included in the pre-DSS diagnostic workup
by subjects in 507/751 (67.5%) case episodes. This
Table 4: Mean quality scores for diagnoses broken down by grade of subject
Consultant
Registrar
SHO
Medical student
Weighted average (all subjects)†
Mean pre-ISABEL score
Mean post-ISABEL score
Mean score change*
0.39
0.40
0.45
0.31
0.43
0.44
0.48
0.37
0.044
0.038
0.032
0.059
0.383
0.426
0.044
* There was no significant difference between grades in terms of change in diagnosis quality score (one-way ANOVA p > 0.05)
Page 10 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Table 5: Increase in the average number of diagnoses and irrelevant diagnoses before and after DSS advice, broken down by grade
Grade
No. of diagnoses
Pre-DSS
Post-DSS
Increase
Pre-DSS
Post-DSS
Increase
3.3
4.3
4.4
4.0
4.6
5.9
6.1
6.6
1.3
1.6
1.7
2.6
0.4
0.8
0.6
1.1
0.7
1.3
1.4
2.2
0.3
0.5
0.8
1.1
Consultant
Registrar
SHO
Medical student
increased to 561/751 (74.7%) case episodes after DSS
advice. The improvement was fully attributable to positive
consultation effects (where the 'most likely diagnosis' was
absent pre-DSS but was present post-DSS); no negative
consultations were observed. Diagnostic accuracy pre-ISABEL was greatest for consultants (73%) and least for medical students (57%). Medical students gained the most
after DSS advice (an absolute increase of 10%). Analysis
performed to elucidate whether ISABEL was responsible
for the changes seen in the rate of diagnostic error indicated that all additional diagnoses were indeed present in
the system's list of diagnostic suggestions.
Time intervals
Reliable time data was available for 633/751 episodes
(table 7). Median time taken for subjects to abstract clinical features and record their initial clinical decisions on
the trial website was 6 min (IQR 4–10 min); median time
taken to examine ISABEL's suggestions and make changes
to clinical decisions was 1 min (IQR 30 sec-2 min). Time
taken for ISABEL to display its suggestions was less than 2
sec on all occasions.
Table 6: Number of case episodes in which clinically 'important'
decisions were prompted by ISABEL consultation
Number of 'important'
decisions prompted by
ISABEL
No. of irrelevant diagnoses
Diagnoses
Tests
Treatment steps
1
2
3
4
5
69
19
2
3
1
56
12
2
0
0
42
5
2
0
0
None
657
637
678
No. of case episodes in
which at least ONE
'significant' decision was
prompted by ISABEL
94 (12.5%)
70 (9.3%)
49 (6.5%)
Total number of
individual 'significant'
decisions prompted by
ISABEL
130
86
58
Discussion
We have shown in this study that errors of omission occur
frequently during diagnostic workup in an experimental
setting, including in cases perceived as being common in
routine practice. Such errors seem to occur in most subjects, irrespective of their level of experience. We have also
demonstrated that it is possible to influence clinicians'
diagnostic workup and reduce errors of omission using a
stand-alone diagnostic reminder system. Following DSS
consultation, the quality of diagnostic, test-ordering and
treatment decisions made by various grades of clinicians
improved for a range of cases, such that a clinically important alteration in diagnostic decision-making resulted in
12.5% of all consultations (1 in 8 episodes of system use).
In a previous study assessing the impact of ILIAD and
QMR, in which only diagnostically challenging cases were
used in an experimental setting, Friedman et al showed
that the 'correct diagnosis' was prompted by DSS use in
approximately 1 in 16 consultations [48]. Although we
used a similar experimental design, we used a mix of easy
as well as difficult cases to test the hypothesis that incomplete workup was encountered in diagnostic conundrums
as well as routine clinical problems. Previous evaluations
of expert systems used the presence of the 'correct' diagnosis as the main outcome. We focused on clinical safety as
the key outcome, preferring to use the inclusion of all
'clinically important' diagnoses in the workup as the main
variable of interest. In acute settings such as emergency
rooms and primary care, where an incomplete and evolving clinical picture results in considerable diagnostic
uncertainty at assessment, the ability to generate a focused
and 'safe' workup is a more clinically relevant outcome,
and one which accurately reflects the nature of decision
making in this environment [53]. Consequently, we
defined diagnostic errors of omission at assessment as the
'failure to consider all clinically important diagnoses (as
judged by an expert panel working the same cases)'. This
definition resulted in the 'correct' diagnosis, as well as
other significant diagnoses, being included within the
'minimum' workup. Further, changes in test-ordering and
treatment decisions were uniquely measured in this study
as a more concrete marker of the impact of diagnostic
decision support on the patient's clinical management; we
Page 11 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
Table 7: Time taken to process case simulations broken down by
grade of subject
Consultant
Registrar
SHO
Medical student
Overall
Median time preISABEL
Median time postISABEL
5 min 5 sec
5 min 45 sec
5 min 54 sec
8 min 36 sec
42 sec
57 sec
53 sec
3 min 42 sec
6 min 2 sec (IQR: 4:03 –
9:47)
1 min (IQR: 30 sec –
2:04)
were able to demonstrate an improvement in test-ordering in 1 in 10 system consultations, indicating that diagnostic DSS may strongly influence patient management,
despite only offering diagnosis-related advice. Finally, the
time expended during DSS consultation is an important
aspect that has not been fully explored in previous studies.
In our study, subjects spent a median of 6 minutes for
clinical data entry (including typing in their unaided decisions), and a median of 1 minute to process the advice
provided and make changes to their clinical decisions.
The research design employed in this study allowed us to
confirm a number of observations previously reported, as
well as to generate numerous unique ones. These findings
relate to the operational consequences of providing diagnostic assistance in practice. In keeping with other DSS
evaluations, different subject grades processed system
advice in different ways, depending on their prior knowledge and clinical experience, leading to variable benefit.
Since ISABEL merely offered diagnostic suggestions, and
allowed the clinician to make the final decisions (acting as
the 'learned intermediary') [54], in some cases, subjects
ignored even important advice. In some other cases, they
added irrelevant decisions or deleted important decisions
after DSS consultation, leading to reduced net positive
effect of the DDSS on decision making. For some subjects
whose pre-DSS performance was high, a ceiling effect prevailed, and no further improvement could be demonstrated. These findings complement the results of our
earlier system performance evaluation which solely
focused on system accuracy and not on user interaction
with DDSS. One of the main findings from this study was
that consultants tended to generate shorter diagnostic
workup lists containing the 'most likely' diagnoses, with a
predilection to omit other 'important' diagnoses that
might account for the patient's clinical features, resulting
in a high incidence of DEO. Medical students generated
long diagnostic workup lists, but missed many key diagnoses leading to a high DEO rate. Interestingly, all subject
grades gained from the use of ISABEL in terms of a reduction in the number of DEO, although to varying degrees.
Despite more DEOs occurring in cases considered to be
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
routine in practice than in rare and difficult ones in the
pre-DSS consultation phase, ISABEL advice seemed to
mainly improve decision making for difficult cases, with a
smaller effect on easy cases. The impact of DSS advice
showed a decreasing level of beneficial effect from diagnostic to test-ordering to treatment decisions. Finally,
although the time taken to process cases without DSS
advice in this study compared favorably with the Friedman evaluation of QMR and ILIAD (6 min vs. 8 min), the
time taken to generate a revised workup with DSS assistance was dramatically shorter (1 min vs. 22 min).
We propose a number of explanations for our findings.
There is sufficient evidence to suggest that clinicians with
more clinical experience resort to established pattern-recognition techniques and the use of heuristics while making diagnostic decisions [55]. While these shortcuts
enable quick decision making in practice, and work successfully on most occasions, they involve a number of
cognitive biases such as 'premature closure' and 'confirmation bias' that may lead to incomplete assessment on
some occasions. On the other hand, medical students may
not have developed adequate pattern-recognition techniques or acquired sufficient knowledge of heuristics to
make sound diagnostic decisions. It may well be that
grades at an intermediate level are able to process cases in
an acute setting with a greater emphasis on clinical safety.
This explanation may also account for the finding that
subjects failed to include 'important' diagnoses during the
assessment of easy cases. Recognition that a case was unusual may trigger a departure from the use of established
pattern-recognition techniques and clinical shortcuts to a
more considered cognitive assessment, leading to fewer
DEO in these cases. We have shown that it is possible to
reduce DEOs by the use of diagnostic reminders, including in easy cases, although subjects appeared to be more
willing to revise their decisions for difficult cases on the
basis of ISABEL suggestions. It is also possible that some
subjects ignored relevant advice because the system's
explanatory capacity was inadequate and did not allow
subjects to sufficiently discriminate between the suggestions offered. User variability in summarizing cases may
also explain why variable benefits were derived from ISABEL usage – subjects may have obtained different results
depending on how they abstracted and entered clinical
features. This user variability during clinical data entry has
been demonstrated even with use of a controlled vocabulary in QMR [56]. We observed marked differences
between users' search terms for the same textual case;
however, diagnostic suggestions did not seem to vary
noticeably. This observation could be partially explained
by the enormous diversity associated with various natural
language disease descriptions contained within the ISABEL database, as well as by the system's use of a thesaurus
Page 12 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
that converts medical slang into recognized medical
terms.
The diminishing level of impact from diagnostic to testordering to treatment decisions may be a result of system
design – ISABEL does not explicitly state which tests and
treatments to perform for each of its diagnostic suggestions. This advice is usually embedded within the textual
description of the disease provided to the user. Study and
system design may both account for the differences in
time taken to process the cases. In previous evaluations,
subjects processed cases without using the DSS in the first
instance; in a subsequent step, they used the DSS to enter
clinical data, record their clinical decisions, and processed
system advice to generate a second diagnostic hypothesis
list. In our study, subjects processed the case and recorded
their own clinical decisions while using the DSS for clinical data entry. The second stage of the procedure only
involved processing ISABEL advice and modifying previous clinical decisions. As such, direct comparison between
the studies can be made only by the total time involved
per case (30 min vs. 7 min). This difference could be
explained by features in the system's design that resulted
in shorter times to enter clinical data and to easily process
the advice provided.
The findings from this study have implications specifically
for ISABEL as well as other diagnostic DSS design, evaluation and implementation. It is well recognized that the
dynamic interaction between user and DSS plays a major
role in their acceptance by physicians [57]. We feel that
adoption of the ISABEL system during clinical assessment
in real time is possible even with current computer infrastructure, providing an opportunity for reduction in DEO.
Its integration into an EMR would allow further control
on the quality of the clinical input data as well as provision of active decision support with minimum extra
effort. Such an ISABEL interface has currently been developed and tested with four commercial EMRs [58]; this
integration also facilitates iterative use of the system during the evolution of a patient's condition, leading to
increasingly specific diagnostic advice. The reminder system model aims to enable clinicians to generate 'safe'
diagnostic workups in busy environments at high risk for
diagnostic errors. This model has been successfully used
to alter physician behavior by reducing errors of omission
in preventive care [59]. It is clear from recent studies that
diagnostic errors occur in the emergency room for a
number of reasons. Cognitive biases, of which 'premature
closure' and faulty context generation are key examples,
contribute significantly [60]. Use of a reminder system
may minimize the impact of some of these cognitive
biases. When combined with cognitive forcing strategies
during decision making, DDSS may act as 'safety nets' to
reduce the incidence of omission errors in practice [61].
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Reminders to perform important tests and treatment steps
may also allow a greater impact on patient outcome [62].
A Web-based system model in our study allowed users
from disparate parts of the country to participate in this
study without need for additional infrastructure or financial resources, an implementation model that would minimize the cost associated with deployment in practice.
Finally, the role of DDSS in medical education and training needs formal evaluation. In our study, medical students gained significantly from the advice provided,
suggesting that use of DDSS during specific diagnostic
tasks (e.g. problem-based case exercises) might be a valuable adjunct to current educational strategies. Familiarity
with DDSS will also predispose to greater adoption of
computerized aids during future clinical decision making.
The limitations of this study stem mainly from its experimental design. The repeated measures design raises the
possibility that some of the beneficial effects seen in the
study are a result of subjects 'rethinking' the case, or the
consequence of a reflective process [63]. Consequently,
ISABEL's effects in practice could be related to the extra
time taken by users in processing cases. We believe that
any such effects are likely to be minimal since subjects did
not actually process the cases twice during the study – a
summary of the clinical features was generated by subjects
when the case was displayed for the first time, and subjects could not review the cases while processing ISABEL
suggestions in the next step. Subjects also spent negligible
time between their first assessment of the cases and
processing the diagnostic suggestions from the DSS. The
repeated measures design provided the power to detect
differences between users with minimal resources; a randomized design using study and control groups of subjects
would have necessitated the involvement of over 200 subjects. The cases used in our study contained only basic
clinical data gained at the time of acute assessment, and
may have proved too concise or easy to process. However,
this seems unlikely since subjects only took an average of
8 min to process even diagnostic conundrums prior to
DSS use when 'expert systems' were tested. Our cases pertained to emergency assessments, making it difficult to
generalize the results to other ambulatory settings. The
ability to extract clinical features from textual cases may
not accurately simulate a real patient encounter where
missed data or 'red herrings' are quite common. The
inherent complexity involved in patient assessment and
summarizing clinical findings in words may lead to
poorer performance of the ISABEL system in real life, since
its diagnostic output depends on the quality of user input.
As a corollary, some of our encouraging results may be
explained by our choice of subjects: a few were already
familiar with summarizing clinical features into the DSS.
Subjects were not supervised during their case exercises
since they may have performed differently under scrutiny,
Page 13 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
raising the prospect of a Hawthorne effect [64]. The use of
a structured website to explicitly record clinical decisions
may have invoked the check-list effect, as illustrated in the
Leeds abdominal pain system study [65]. The check list
effect might also be invoked during the process of summarizing clinical features for ISABEL input; this may have
worked in conjunction with 'rethinking' to promote better
decision making pre-ISABEL. We also measured decision
making at a single point in time, making it difficult to
assess the effects of iterative usage of the DSS on the same
patient. Finally, our definition of diagnostic error aimed
to identify inadequate diagnostic workup at initial assessment that might result in a poor patient outcome. We recognize the absence of an evidence-based link between
omission errors and diagnostic adverse events in practice,
although according to the Schiff model [53], it seems logical to assume that avoiding process errors will prevent
actual errors at least in some instances. In the simulated
setting, it was not possible to test whether inadequate
diagnostic workup would directly lead to a diagnostic
error and cause patient harm. Our planned clinical impact
assessment in real life would help clarify many of the
questions raised during this experimental study.
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Authors' contributions
PR conceived the study, contributed to the study design,
analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript.
GR assisted with the design of the study and data analysis.
MC assisted with the study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and revised the draft manuscript
VN assisted with study design, served as gold standard
panel member, and helped with data analysis
MT assisted with data collection and analysis
PT assisted with study conception, study design and
revised the draft manuscript
JW assisted with study conception, provided advice
regarding study design, and revised the draft manuscript
JB assisted with study conception, study design and data
analysis.
All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Conclusion
This experimental study demonstrates that diagnostic
omission errors are common during the assessment of
easy as well as difficult cases. The provision of patient- and
context-specific diagnostic reminders has the potential to
reduce these errors across all subject grades. Our study
suggests a promising role for the use of future reminderbased DSS in the reduction of diagnostic error. An impact
evaluation, utilizing a naturalistic design and conducted
in real life clinical practice, is underway to verify the conclusions derived from this simulation.
Acknowledgements
Competing interests
References
This study was conducted when the ISABEL system was
available free to users, funded by the ISABEL Medical
Charity (2001–2002). Dr Ramnarayan performed this
research as part of his MD thesis at Imperial College London. Dr Britto was a Trustee of the ISABEL medical charity
(non-remunerative post). Ms Tomlinson was employed
by the ISABEL Medical Charity as a Research Nurse.
Since June 2004, ISABEL is managed by a commercial subsidiary of the Medical Charity called ISABEL Healthcare.
The system is now available only to subscribers. Dr Ramnarayan now advises ISABEL Healthcare on research activities on a part-time basis; Dr Britto is now Clinical
Director of ISABEL Healthcare; Ms Tomlinson is responsible for Content Management within ISABEL Healthcare.
All three hold stock options in ISABEL Healthcare. All
other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
The authors would like to thank Tina Sajjanhar for her help in allocating
cases to specialties and levels of difficulty; Helen Fisher for data analysis; and
Jason Maude of the ISABEL Medical Charity for help rendered during the
design of the study.
Financial support: This study was supported by a research grant from the
National Health Service (NHS) Research & Development Unit, London.
The sponsor did not influence the study design; the collection, analysis, and
interpretation of data; the writing of the manuscript; and the decision to
submit the manuscript for publication.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
Institute of Medicine: To Err is Human; Building a Safer Health System
Washington DC: National Academy Press; 1999.
Fortescue EB, Kaushal R, Landrigan CP, McKenna KJ, Clapp MD, Federico F, Goldmann DA, Bates DW: Prioritizing strategies for preventing medication errors and adverse drug events in
pediatric inpatients. Pediatrics 2003, 111(4 Pt 1):722-9.
Kaushal R, Bates DW: Information technology and medication
safety: what is the benefit? Qual Saf Health Care 2002,
11(3):261-5.
Leape L, Brennan TA, Laird N, Lawthers AG, Localio AR, Barnes BA,
Hebert L, Newhouse JP, Weiler PC, Hiatt H: The nature of
adverse events in hospitalized patients. Results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study II. N Engl J Med 1991, 324:377-84.
Wilson RM, Harrison BT, Gibberd RW, Harrison JD: An analysis of
the causes of adverse events from the quality in Australian
health care study. Med J Aust 1999, 170:4115.
Davis P, Lay-Yee R, Briant R, Ali W, Scott A, Schug S: Adverse
events in New Zealand public hospitals II: preventability and
clinical context. N Z Med J 116(1183):U624. 2003 Oct 10
Bartlett EE: Physicians' cognitive errors and their liability consequences. J Healthcare Risk Manage Fall 1998:62-9.
Croskerry P: The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis
and strategies to minimize them. Acad Med 2003, 78(8):775-80.
Green S, Lee R, Moss J: Problems in general practice. Delays in diagnosis
Manchester, UK: Medical Defence Union; 1998.
Page 14 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
Thomas EJ, Studdert DM, Burstin HR, Orav EJ, Zeena T, Williams EJ,
Howard KM, Weiler PC, Brennan TA: Incidence and types of
adverse events and negligent care in Utah and Colorado.
Med Care 2000, 38:261-71.
Rothschild JM, Landrigan CP, Cronin JW, Kaushal R, Lockley SW,
Burdick E, Stone PH, Lilly CM, Katz JT, Czeisler CA, Bates DW: The
Critical Care Safety Study: The incidence and nature of
adverse events and serious medical errors in intensive care.
Crit Care Med 2005, 33(8):1694-700.
Burroughs TE, Waterman AD, Gallagher TH, Waterman B, Adams D,
Jeffe DB, Dunagan WC, Garbutt J, Cohen MM, Cira J, Inguanzo J, Fraser VJ: Patient concerns about medical errors in emergency
departments. Acad Emerg Med 2005, 12(1):57-64.
Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N: Reducing diagnostic errors in
medicine: what's the goal? Acad Med 2002, 77(10):981-92.
Barnett GO, Cimino JJ, Hupp JA, Hoffer EP: DXplain: an evolving
diagnostic decision-support system. JAMA 1987, 258:67-74.
Miller R, Masarie FE, Myers JD: Quick medical reference (QMR)
for diagnostic assistance. MD Comput 1986, 3(5):34-48.
Warner HR Jr: Iliad: moving medical decision-making into new
frontiers. Methods Inf Med 1989, 28(4):370-2.
Barness LA, Tunnessen WW Jr, Worley WE, Simmons TL, Ringe TB
Jr: Computer-assisted diagnosis in pediatrics. Am J Dis Child
1974, 127(6):852-8.
Wilson DH, Wilson PD, Walmsley RG, Horrocks JC, De Dombal FT:
Diagnosis of acute abdominal pain in the accident and emergency department. Br J Surg 1977, 64(4):250-4.
Miller RA: Medical diagnostic decision support systems – past,
present, and future: a threaded bibliography and brief commentary. J Am Med Inform Assoc 1994, 1(1):8-27.
Bankowitz RA, McNeil MA, Challinor SM, Miller RA: Effect of a
computer-assisted general medicine diagnostic consultation
service on housestaff diagnostic strategy. Methods Inf Med
1989, 28(4):352-6.
Tannenbaum SJ: Knowing and acting in medical practice: the
epistemiological politics of outcomes research. J Health Polit
1994, 19:27-44.
Friedman CP, Gatti GG, Franz TM, Murphy GC, Wolf FM, Heckerling
PS, Fine PL, Miller TM, Elstein AS: Do physicians know when their
diagnoses are correct? Implications for decision support and
error reduction. J Gen Intern Med 2005, 20(4):334-9.
Wyatt JC: Clinical Knowledge and Practice in the Information Age: a handbook for health professionals London: The Royal Society of Medicine
Press; 2001.
Smith R: What clinical information do doctors need? BMJ 1996,
313:1062-68.
Graber MA, VanScoy D: How well does decision support software perform in the emergency department? Emerg Med J
2003, 20(5):426-8.
Lemaire JB, Schaefer JP, Martin LA, Faris P, Ainslie MD, Hull RD:
Effectiveness of the Quick Medical Reference as a diagnostic
tool. CMAJ 161(6):725-8. 1999 Sep 21
Welford CR: A comprehensive computerized patient record
with automated linkage to QMR. Proc Annu Symp Comput Appl
Med Care 1994:814-8.
Elhanan G, Socratous SA, Cimino JJ: Integrating DXplain into a
clinical information system using the World Wide Web. Proc
AMIA Annu Fall Symp 1996:348-52.
Berner ES, Detmer DE, Simborg D: Will the wave finally break?
A brief view of the adoption of electronic medical records in
the United States. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2005, 12(1):3-7. Epub
2004 Oct 18
Greenough A: Help from ISABEL for pediatric diagnoses. Lancet 360(9341):1259. 2002 Oct 19
Thomas NJ: ISABEL. Critical Care 2002, 7(1):99-100.
Ramnarayan P, Britto J: Pediatric clinical decision support systems. Arch Dis Child 2002, 87(5):361-2.
Fisher H, Tomlinson A, Ramnarayan P, Britto J: ISABEL: support
with clinical decision making. Paediatr Nurs 2003, 15(7):34-5.
Giuse DA, Giuse NB, Miller RA: A tool for the computer-assisted
creation of QMR medical knowledge base disease profiles.
Proc Annu Symp Comput Appl Med Care 1991:978-9.
Johnson KB, Feldman MJ: Medical informatics and pediatrics.
Decision-support systems. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1995,
149(12):1371-80.
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
41.
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
Giuse DA, Giuse NB, Miller RA: Evaluation of long-term maintenance of a large medical knowledge base. J Am Med Inform Assoc
1995, 2(5):297-306.
Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Kulkarni G, Rao A, Britto J: A Novel
Diagnostic Aid (ISABEL): Development and Preliminary
Evaluation of Clinical Performance. Medinfo 2004:1091-5.
Wyatt J: Quantitative evaluation of clinical software, exemplified by decision support systems. Int J Med Inform 1997,
47(3):165-73.
Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, Coren M, Winrow A, Britto J:
ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic aid for pediatrics: results from an initial performance evaluation. Arch Dis
Child 2003, 88:408-13.
Feldman MJ, Barnett GO: An approach to evaluating the accuracy of DXplain.
Comput Methods Programs Biomed 1991,
35(4):261-6.
Middleton B, Shwe MA, Heckerman DE, Henrion M, Horvitz EJ, Lehmann HP, Cooper GF: Probabilistic diagnosis using a reformulation of the INTERNIST-1/QMR knowledge base. II.
Evaluation of diagnostic performance. Methods Inf Med 1991,
30(4):256-67.
Nelson SJ, Blois MS, Tuttle MS, Erlbaum M, Harrison P, Kim H, Winkelmann B, Yamashita D: Evaluating Reconsider: a computer
program for diagnostic prompting. J Med Syst 1985, 9:379-388.
Berner ES, Webster GD, Shugerman AA, Jackson JR, Algina J, Baker
AL, Ball EV, Cobbs CG, Dennis VW, Frenkel EP: Performance of
four computer-based diagnostic systems. N Engl J Med
330(25):1792-6. 1994 Jun 23
Miller RA: Evaluating evaluations of medical diagnostic systems. J Am Med Inform Assoc 1996, 3(6):429-31.
Miller RA, Masarie FE: The demise of the "Greek Oracle" model
for medical diagnostic systems. Methods Inf Med 1990, 29:1-2.
Maisiak RS, Berner ES: Comparison of measures to assess
change in diagnostic performance due to a decision support
system. Proc AMIA Symp 2000:532-6.
Berner ES, Maisiak RS: Influence of case and physician characteristics on perceptions of decision support systems. J Am
Med Inform Assoc 1999, 6(5):428-34.
Friedman CP, Elstein AS, Wolf FM, Murphy GC, Franz TM, Heckerling
PS, Fine PL, Miller TM, Abraham V: Enhancement of clinicians'
diagnostic reasoning by computer-based consultation: a
multisite study of 2 systems. JAMA 282(19):1851-6. 1999 Nov 17
Issenberg SB, McGaghie WC, Hart IR, Mayer JW, Felner JM, Petrusa
ER, Waugh RA, Brown DD, Safford RR, Gessner IH, Gordon DL, Ewy
GA: Simulation technology for health care professional skills
training and assessment. JAMA 282(9):861-6. 1999 Sep 1
Clauser BE, Margolis MJ, Swanson DB: An examination of the contribution of computer-based case simulations to the USMLE
step 3 examination. Acad Med 2002, 77(10 Suppl):S80-2.
Dillon GF, Clyman SG, Clauser BE, Margolis MJ: The introduction
of computer-based simulations into the Unites States medical licensing examination. Acad Med 2002, 77(10 Suppl):S94-6.
Ramnarayan P, Kapoor RR, Coren M, Nanduri V, Tomlinson AL, Taylor PM, Wyatt JC, Britto JF: Measuring the impact of diagnostic
decision support on the quality of clinical decision-making:
development of a reliable and valid composite score. J Am
Med Inform Assoc 2003, 10(6):563-72.
Gordon D Schiff, Seijeoung Kim, Richard Abrams, Karen Cosby,
Bruce Lambert, Arthur S Elstein, Scott Hasler, Nela Krosnjar, Richard
Odwazny, Mary F Wisniewski, Robert A McNutt: Diagnostic diagnosis errors: lessons from a multi-institutional collaborative
project.
[http://www.ahrq.gov/downloads/pub/advances/vol2/
Schiff.pdf]. Accessed 10 May 2005
Brahams D, Wyatt J: Decision-aids and the law. Lancet 1989,
2:632-4.
Croskerry P: Achieving quality in clinical decision making:
cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med
2002, 9(11):1184-204.
Bankowitz RA, Blumenfeld BH, Guis Bettinsoli N: User variability
in abstracting and entering printed case histories with Quick
Medical Reference (QMR). In Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual
Symposium on Computer Applications in Medical Care New York: IEEE
Computer Society Press; 1987:68-73.
Kassirer JP: A report card on computer-assisted diagnosis –
the grade: C. N Engl J Med 330(25):1824-5. 1994 Jun 23
[http://www.healthcareitnews.com/story.cms?ID=3030].
Page 15 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making 2006, 6:22
59.
60.
61.
62.
63.
64.
65.
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22
Dexter PR, Perkins S, Overhage JM, Maharry K, Kohler RB, McDonald
CJ: A computerized reminder to increase the use of preventive care for hospitalized patients. N Engl J Med 2001,
345:965-970.
Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R: Diagnostic error in internal
medicine. Arch Intern Med 2005, 165(13):1493-9.
Croskerry P: Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decision
making. Ann Emerg Med 2003, 41(1):110-20.
Berner ES: Diagnostic decision support systems: how to determine the gold standard?
J Am Med Inform Assoc 2003,
10(6):608-10.
Mamede S, Schmidt HG: The structure of reflective practice in
medicine. Med Educ 2004, 38(12):1302-8.
Randolph AG, Haynes RB, Wyatt JC, Cook DJ, Guyatt GH: Users'
Guides to the Medical Literature: XVIII. How to use an article evaluating the clinical impact of a computer-based clinical decision support system. JAMA 282(1):67-74. 1999 Jul 7
Adams ID, Chan M, Clifford PC, Cooke WM, Dallos V, de Dombal FT,
Edwards MH, Hancock DM, Hewett DJ, McIntyre N: Computer
aided diagnosis of acute abdominal pain: a multicentre study.
BMJ 1986, 293:800-4.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed
here:
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6947/6/22/prepub
Publish with Bio Med Central and every
scientist can read your work free of charge
"BioMed Central will be the most significant development for
disseminating the results of biomedical researc h in our lifetime."
Sir Paul Nurse, Cancer Research UK
Your research papers will be:
available free of charge to the entire biomedical community
peer reviewed and published immediately upon acceptance
cited in PubMed and archived on PubMed Central
yours — you keep the copyright
BioMedcentral
Submit your manuscript here:
http://www.biomedcentral.com/info/publishing_adv.asp
Page 16 of 16
(page number not for citation purposes)
HOW ACCURATELY DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS
DECISION SUPPORT SYSTEM SUGGEST THE
CORRECT DIAGNOSIS IN 50 ADULT NEJM CPC
CASES?
Graber ML, Mathews A.
Performance of a web-based clinical diagnosis support system for internists.
J Gen Intern Med. 2008 Jan;23 Suppl 1:85-7.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Performance of a Web-Based Clinical Diagnosis Support System
for Internists
Mark L. Graber, MD1,2 and Ashlei Mathew1,2
1
Medical Service-111, VA Medical Center, Northport, NY 11768, USA; 2Department of Medicine, SUNY at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY, USA.
BACKGROUND: Clinical decision support systems can
improve medical diagnosis and reduce diagnostic
errors. Older systems, however, were cumbersome to
use and had limited success in identifying the correct
diagnosis in complicated cases.
OBJECTIVE: To measure the sensitivity and speed of
“Isabel” (Isabel Healthcare Inc., USA), a new web-based
clinical decision support system designed to suggest the
correct diagnosis in complex medical cases involving
adults.
METHODS: We tested 50 consecutive Internal Medicine
case records published in the New England Journal of
Medicine. We first either manually entered 3 to 6 key
clinical findings from the case (recommended approach)
or pasted in the entire case history. The investigator
entering key words was aware of the correct diagnosis.
We then determined how often the correct diagnosis
was suggested in the list of 30 differential diagnoses
generated by the clinical decision support system. We
also evaluated the speed of data entry and results
recovery.
RESULTS: The clinical decision support system suggested the correct diagnosis in 48 of 50 cases (96%) with
key findings entry, and in 37 of the 50 cases (74%) if the
entire case history was pasted in. Pasting took seconds,
manual entry less than a minute, and results were
provided within 2–3 seconds with either approach.
CONCLUSIONS: The Isabel clinical decision support
system quickly suggested the correct diagnosis in
almost all of these complex cases, particularly with
key finding entry. The system performed well in this
experimental setting and merits evaluation in more
natural settings and clinical practice.
KEY WORDS: clinical diagnosis support systems; Isabel; internal
medicine; diagnostic error; Google; decision support.
J Gen Intern Med 23(Suppl 1):37–40
DOI: 10.1007/s11606-007-0271-8
© Society of General Internal Medicine 2007
INTRODUCTION
The best clinicians excel in their ability to discern the correct
diagnosis in perplexing cases. This skill requires an extensive
knowledge base, keen interviewing and examination skills, and
the ability to synthesize coherently all of the available information. Unfortunately, the level of expertise varies among
clinicians, and even the most expert can sometimes fail. There
is also a growing appreciation that diagnostic errors can be
made just as easily in simple cases as in the most complex.
Given this dilemma and the fact that diagnostic error rates are
not trivial, clinicians are well-advised to explore tools that can
help them establish correct diagnoses.
Clinical diagnosis support systems (CDSS) can direct physicians to the correct diagnosis and have the potential to reduce
the rate of diagnostic errors in medicine.1,2 The first-generation
computer-based products (e.g., QMR—First Databank, Inc, CA;
Iliad—University of Utah; DXplain—Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA) used precompiled knowledge bases of
syndromes and diseases with their characteristic symptoms,
signs, and laboratory findings. The user would enter findings
from their own patients selected from a menu of choices, and
the programs would use Bayesian logic or pattern-matching
algorithms to suggest diagnostic possibilities. Typically, the
suggestions were judged to be helpful in clinical settings, even
when used by expert clinicians.3 These diagnosis support
systems were also useful in teaching clinical reasoning.4,5
Surprisingly and despite their demonstrated utility in experimental settings, none of these earlier systems gained widespread acceptance for clinical use, apparently related to the
considerable time needed to input clinical data and their
somewhat limited sensitivity and specificity.3,6 A study of Iliad
and QMR in an emergency department setting, for example,
found that the final impression of the attending physician was
found among the suggested diagnoses only 72% and 52% of the
time, respectively, and data input required 20 to 40 minutes for
each case.7
In this study, we evaluated the clinical performance of
“Isabel” (Isabel Healthcare Inc, USA), a new, second generation, web-based CDSS that accepts either key findings or
whole-text entry and uses a novel search strategy to identify
candidate diagnoses from the clinical findings. The clinician
first enters the key findings from the case using free-text entry
(see Fig 1). There is no limit on the number of terms entered,
although excellent results are typically obtained with entering
just a few key findings. The program includes a thesaurus
that facilitates recognition of terms. The program then uses
natural language processing and search algorithms to compare these terms to those used in a selected reference library.
For Internal Medicine cases, the library includes 6 key textbooks anchored around the Oxford Textbooks of Medicine, 4th
Edition (2003) and the Oxford Textbook of Geriatric Medicine
and 46 major journals in general and subspecialty medicine
and toxicology. The search domain and results are filtered to
take into account the patient’s age, sex, geographic location,
37
38
Graber and Mathew: A Web-Based Clinical Diagnosis Support System
JGIM
Figure 1. The data-entry screen of the Isabel diagnosis support software. The clinician manually enters the age, gender, locality, and
specialty. The query terms can be entered manually from the key findings or findings can be pasted from an existing write-up. This patient
was ultimately found to have a B-cell lymphoma secreting a cryo-paraprotein
pregnancy status, and other clinical parameters that are either
preselected by the clinician or automatically entered if the
system is integrated with the clinician’s electronic medical
record. The system then displays a total of 30 suggested
diagnoses, with 10 diagnoses presented on each web page
(see Fig. 2). The order of listing reflects an indication of the
matching between the findings selected and the reference
materials searched but is not meant to suggest a ranked order
of clinical probabilities. As in the first generation systems,
more detailed information on each diagnosis can be obtained
by links to authoritative texts.
The Isabel CDSS was originally developed for use in
pediatrics. In an initial evaluation, 13 clinicians (trainees and
staff) at St Mary’s Hospital, London submitted a total of 99
case scenarios of hypothetical case presentations for different
diagnoses, and Isabel displayed the expected diagnosis in 91%
of these cases.8 Out of 100 real case scenarios gathered from 4
major teaching hospitals in the UK, Isabel suggested the
correct diagnosis in 83 of 87 cases (95%). In a separate
evaluation of 24 case scenarios in which the gold standard
differential diagnosis was established by two senior academic
pediatricians, Isabel decreased the chance of clinicians (a mix
of trainees and staff clinicians) omitting a key diagnosis by
suggesting a highly relevant new diagnosis in 1 of every 8 cases.
In this study, the time to enter data and obtain diagnostic
suggestions averaged less than 1 minute.9
The Isabel clinical diagnosis support system has now been
adapted for adult medicine. The goal of this study was to
evaluate the speed and accuracy of this product in suggesting
the correct diagnosis in a series of complex cases in Internal
Medicine.
METHOD
We considered 61 consecutive “Case Records of Massachusetts
General Hospital” (New England Journal of Medicine, vol.
350:166–176, 2004 and 353:189–198, 2005). Each case had
an anatomical or final diagnosis, which was considered to be
correct by the discussants. We excluded 11 cases (patients
under the age of 10 and cases that focused solely on
management issues). The 50 remaining case histories were
copied and pasted into the Isabel data-entry field. The pasted
material typically included the history, physical examination
findings, and laboratory test results, but data from tables and
figures were not submitted. Beyond entering the patient’s age,
sex, and nationality, the investigators did not attempt to
otherwise tailor the search strategy. Findings were compared
to the recommended (but slightly more time consuming)
strategy of entering discrete key findings, as compiled by a
senior internist (MLG). Because the correct diagnosis is
presented at the end of each case, data entry was not blinded.
RESULTS
Using the recommended method of manually entering key
findings, the list of diagnoses suggested by Isabel contained the
correct diagnosis in 48 of the 50 cases (96%). Typically 3–6 key
findings from each case were used. The 2 diagnoses that were
not suggested (progressive multifocal encephalopathy and
nephrogenic fibrosing dermopathy) were not included in the
Isabel database at the time of the study; thus, these 2 cases
would never have been suggested, even with different keywords.
JGIM
Graber and Mathew: A Web-Based Clinical Diagnosis Support System
39
Figure 2. The first page of results of the Isabel diagnosis support software. Additional diagnoses are presented by selecting the ‘more
diagnoses’ box
Using the copy/paste method for entering the whole text,
the list of diagnoses suggested by Isabel contained the correct
diagnosis in 37 of the 50 cases (76%). Isabel presented 10
diagnoses on the first web page and 10 additional diagnoses on
subsequent pages up to a total of 30 diagnoses. Because users
may tend to disregard suggestions not shown on later web
pages, we tracked this parameter for the copy/paste method of
data entry: The correct diagnosis was presented on the first
page in 19 of the 37 cases (51%) or first two pages in 28 of the
37 cases (77%). Similar data were not collected for manual
data entry because the order of presentation depended on
which key findings were entered.
Both data entry approaches were fast: Manually entering
data and obtaining diagnostic suggestions typically required
less than 1 minute per case, and the copy/paste method
typically required less than 5 seconds.
DISCUSSION
Diagnostic errors are an underappreciated cause of medical
error,10 and any intervention that has the potential to produce
correct and timely medical diagnosis is worthy of serious
consideration. Our recent analysis of diagnostic errors in
Internal Medicine found that clinicians often stop thinking
after arriving at a preliminary diagnosis that explains all the
key findings, leading to context errors and ‘premature closure’,
where further possibilities are not considered.11 These and
other errors contribute to diagnoses that are wrong or delayed,
causing substantial harm in the patients affected. Systems
that help clinicians explore a more complete range of diagnostic possibilities could conceivably reduce these types of error.
Many different CDSSs have been developed over the years,
and these typically matched the manually entered features of
the case in question to a database of key findings abstracted
from experts or the clinical literature. The sensitivity of these
systems was in the range of 50%–60%, and the time needed to
access and query the database was often several minutes.3
More recently, the possibility of using Google to search for
clinical diagnoses has been suggested. However, a formal
evaluation of this approach on a subset of the same “Case
Records” cases used in our study found a sensitivity of 58%,12
in the range of the first-generation CDSSs and unacceptably
low for clinical use.
The findings of our study indicate that CDSS products have
evolved substantially. Using the Isabel CDSS, we found that
data entry takes under 1 minute, and the sensitivity in a series
of highly complex cases approached 100% using entry of key
findings. Entry of entire case histories using copy/paste
functionality allowed even faster data entry but reduced
sensitivity. The loss of sensitivity seemed primarily related to
negative findings included in the pasted history and physical
(e.g., “the patient denies chest pain”), which are treated as
positive findings (chest pain) by the search algorithm.
There are several relevant limitations of this study that
make it difficult to predict how Isabel might perform as a
diagnostic aid in clinical practice. First, the results obtained
here reflect the theoretical upper limit of performance, given
that an investigator who was aware of the correct diagnosis
entered the key findings. Further, clinicians in real life
seldom have the wealth of reliable and organized information
that is presented in the Case Records or the time needed to
use a CDSS in every case. To the extent that Isabel functions
as a ‘learned intermediary’,13 success in using the program
40
Graber and Mathew: A Web-Based Clinical Diagnosis Support System
will also clearly depend on the clinical expertise of the user
and their facility in working with Isabel. A serious existential
concern is whether presenting a clinician with dozens of
diagnostic suggestions might be a distraction or lead to
unnecessary testing. We have previously identified these
trade-offs as an unavoidable cost of improving patient safety:
The price of improving the odds of reaching a correct
diagnosis is the extra time and resources consumed in using
the CDSS and considering alternative diagnoses that might
turn out to be irrelevant.14
In summary, the Isabel CDSS performed quickly and
accurately in suggesting correct diagnoses for complex adult
medicine cases. However, the test setting was artificial, and the
CDSS should be evaluated in more natural environments for
its potential to support clinical diagnosis and reduce the rate of
diagnostic error in medicine.
Acknowledgements: The administrative and library-related support of Ms. Grace Garey and Ms. Mary Lou Glazer is gratefully
acknowledged. Funding was provided by the National Patient
Safety Foundation. This study was approved by the Institutional
Review Board (IRB).
Conflict of interest statement: None disclosed.
Corresponding Author: Mark L. Graber, MD; Medical Service-111,
VA Medical Center, Northport, NY 11768, USA (e-mail: mark.
graber@va.gov).
JGIM
REFERENCES
1. Berner ES. Clinical Decision Support Systems. New York: Springer;
1999.
2. Garg AX, Adhikari NKJ, McDonald H, et al. Effects of computerized
clinical decision support systems on practitioner performance and
patient outcomes. JAMA. 2005;293:1223–38.
3. Berner ES, Webster GD, Shugerman AA, et al. Performance of four
computer-based diagnostic systems. N Engl J Med. 1994;330:1792–6.
4. Friedman CP, Elstein AS, Wolf FM, et al. Enhancement of clinicians’
diagnostic reasoning by computer-based consultation: a multisite study
of 2 systems. JAMA. 1999;282:1851–6.
5. Lincoln MJ, Turner CW, Haug PJ, et al. Iliad’s role in the generalization
of learning across a medical domain. Proc Annu Symp Comput Appl Med
Care. 1992;174–8.
6. Kassirer JP. A report card on computer-assisted diagnosis—the grade:
C. N Engl J Med. 1994;330:1824–5.
7. Graber MA, VanScoy D. How well does decision support software
perform in the emergency department? Emerg Med J. 2003;20:426–8.
8. Ramnarayan P, Tomlinson A, Rao A, Coren M, Winrow A, Britto J.
ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnosis aid for paediatrics: results
from an initial performance evaluation. Arch Dis Child. 2003;88:408–13.
9. Ramnarayan P, Roberts GC, Coren M, et al. Assessment of the
potential impact of a reminder system on the reduction of diagnostic
errors: a quasi-experimental study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak.
2006;6:22.
10. Graber ML. Diagnostic error in medicine—a case of neglect. Joint Comm
J Saf Qual Improv. 2004;31:112–19.
11. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon RR. Diagnostic error in internal
medicine. Arch Int Med. 2005;165:1493–9.
12. Tang H, Ng JHK. Googling for a diagnosis—use of Google as a diagnostic
aid: internet based study. Br Med J. 2006.
13. Kantor G. Guest software review: Isabel diagnosis software. 2006;1–31.
14. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Reducing diagnostic error in
medicine: what’s the goal? Acad Med. 2002;77:981–92.
HOW DOES A WEB-BASED DIAGNOSIS DECISION
SUPPORT SYSTEM IMPROVE DIAGNOSIS ACCURACY
IN A CRITICAL CARE SETTING?
Thomas NJ, Ramnarayan P, Bell P Et al.
An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children.
Technology and Health Care 16 (2008) 103–110.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Technology and Health Care 16 (2008) 103–110
IOS Press
103
An international assessment of a web-based
diagnostic tool in critically ill children
Neal J. Thomasa,∗ , Padmanabhan Ramnarayanb , Michael J. Bellc , Prabhat Maheshwarid ,
Shaun Wilsone, Emily B. Nazarianf , Lorri M. Phippsa , David C. Stockwellc , Michael Engelc,
Frank A. Maffeif , Harish G. Vyase and Joseph Brittog
a Penn
State Children’s Hospital and The Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine, Hershey,
PA, USA
b Children’s Acute Transport Service, London, UK
c Pediatric Critical Care Medicine, Children’s National Medical Center, Washington DC, USA
d Pediatric Intensive Care Unit, St Mary’s Hospital, Paddington, London, UK
e Pediatric Intensive Care Unit, Queen’s Medical Centre, Nottingham, UK
f Pediatric Critical Care Medicine, Golisano Children’s Hospital, University of Rochester at Strong,
Rochester, NY, USA
g Isabel Healthcare Inc., Reston, VA, USA
Received 5 September 2007
Revised /Accepted 30 October 2007
Abstract. Improving diagnostic accuracy is essential. The extent of diagnostic uncertainty at patient admission is not
well described in critically ill children. Therefore, we studied the extent that pediatric trainee diagnostic performance could be
improved with the aid of a computerized diagnostic tool. Data regarding patient admissions to five Pediatric Intensive Care Units
were collected. Information included patients’ clinical details, admitting team’s diagnostic workup and discharge diagnosis. An
attending physician assessed each case independently and suggested additional diagnostic possibilities. Diagnostic accuracy
was calculated using the discharge diagnosis as the gold standard. 206 out of 927 patients (22.2%) admitted to the PICUs did
not have an established diagnosis at admission. The trainee teams considered a median of three diagnoses in their workup (IQR
3–5) and made an accurate diagnosis in 89.4% cases (95% CI 84.6%–94.2%). Diagnostic accuracy improved to 92.5% with
use of the diagnostic tool alone, and to 95% with the addition of attending physicians’ diagnostic suggestions. We conclude
that a modest proportion of admissions to these PICUs were characterized by diagnostic uncertainty during initial assessment.
Although there was a relatively high accuracy rate of initial assessment in our clinical setting, it was further improved by both
the diagnostic tool and the physicians’ diagnostic suggestions. It is plausible that the tool’s utility would be even greater in
clinical settings with less expertise in critical illness assessment, such as community hospitals, or emergency departments of
non-training institutions. The role of diagnostic aids in the care of critically ill children merits further study.
Keywords: Diagnosis, diagnostic reminder system, internet, pediatrics
∗
Address for correspondence: Neal J. Thomas, M.D., M.Sc, Pediatric Critical Care Medicine, Penn State Children’s
Hospital, 500 University Drive, MC H085, Hershey, PA 17033, USA. Tel.: +1 717 531 5337; Fax: +1 717 531 0809; E-mail:
nthomas@psu.edu.
0928-7329/08/$17.00  2008 – IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved
104
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
1. Introduction
Accurate diagnosis of medical conditions has been of paramount importance for centuries and faulty
diagnostic evaluation can lead to patient harm and poor medical management. Three etiologies of
diagnostic errors have been recognized within the medical community: no fault errors, system-related
errors and cognitive errors [20]. No-fault errors occur either when disease presentation is unusual and
unpredictable or when patients are uncooperative. System-related errors occur as a result of either
organizational flaws or mechanical problems within the institution. Cognitive errors occur due to lack
of adequate medical knowledge or inadequate data collection.
With the explosion of medical literature at exponential rates, avoidance of cognitive errors using
emerging technology may be the most efficient method for improving diagnostic skills. A large body of
evidence suggests that diagnostic error by clinicians poses a significant problem across a wide variety of
settings, comprising 10–30% of adverse medical events (AME) [9,22]. One recent observational study
suggested that 10% of AME in adult critical care patients were related to misdiagnosis [16]. Moreover,
autopsy studies have demonstrated that major diagnoses are missed in critically ill adults and children
who die in intensive care units [2,4,8,17].
We hypothesized that a web-based diagnostic reminder system would produce additional differential
diagnosis possibilities for children admitted to five international PICUs who were admitted without an established diagnosis. We further hypothesized that the web-based tool would increase diagnostic accuracy
by input of the findings recorded in the medical record admission history and physical examination. To
do this, we compared the accuracy of determining the discharge diagnosis for (i) the admitting team, (ii)
an unbiased board-certified/board eligible pediatric intensivist and (iii) ISABEL, a web-based diagnostic
reminder system (www.isabelhealthcare.com) [14,21].
2. Materials and methods
This prospective study was conducted during a three-month period at five pediatric intensive care
units (PICUs), two from the United Kingdom (UK) and three from the United States (USA). The
selection of ICUs was based on working relationships between the five primary investigators in each
site, with a goal of studying institutions that had diverse patient populations, different patient volumes,
and a diverse culture. All patient data collection was approved by the research ethics committees
(institutional review boards) at the participating centers prior to initiation of the study. The requirement
for informed consent was waived. All admissions to the PICU during the study period were screened by
a designated study investigator at each center. Medical admissions to the PICU without an established
diagnosis were eligible for study. All surgical admissions and medical admissions with known primary
diagnoses (such as culture-proven sepsis, diabetic ketoacidosis and others) were excluded. All PICUs
utilize various medical personnel (including residents from pediatrics or pediatric subspecialties, clinical
fellows, advanced practice nurses (APN), physician assistants and pediatric intensivists) as their daily
clinical team.
2.1. Diagnosis by admitting team
At each PICU, the screening investigator (pediatric critical care medicine fellow, pediatric resident or
pediatric APN) reviewed all admissions and enrolled eligible patients. Importantly, these investigators
neither admitted the child to the PICU nor participated in the clinical care given to the child so that
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
105
the utility of the tool itself could be tested without bias from difficulties with the system or changes in
routine practice. The medical records of eligible patients were reviewed and presenting symptoms and
physical findings and the differential diagnosis (including all diagnostic possibilities) was extracted. The
screening investigator further determined the discharge diagnosis based on review of the medical record
at discharge.
2.2. Diagnosis by ISABEL
The screening investigator at each center used a customized study version of ISABEL to securely log
in, enter, and store data on eligible patients. Each patient was assigned a unique study number, and the
date and time of electronic access was logged. Clinical data from the chart were entered and was used
to drive the diagnostic tool’s algorithm for production of diagnostic possibilities.
ISABEL is a Web based diagnostic decision support system (DSS). Computerized decision support
tools utilize two or more pieces of clinical data to provide patient-specific recommendations; assist
in diverse clinical processes such as diagnosis, prescribing or clinical management; and consist of an
underlying knowledge base, user interface for data input, and an inference engine [23]. The ISABEL
knowledge base consists of > 100,000 raw text documents describing > 10,000 diseases. Documents
are drawn from multiple resources such as reputed textbooks (e.g. Oxford Textbook of Medicine) and
review articles in journals. The user interface permits data entry in the form of a free text description
of clinical findings, without the need for a specific terminology. The inference engine utilizes statistical
natural language processing software to search through the entire database of text documents and returns
all disease descriptions that match the clinical features entered. This process is similar to a search
engine, although much more sensitive and specific. The software indexes each document in the database
daily, and extracts relevant concepts based on frequency of their occurrence, uniqueness and proximity
to other concepts. Each diagnosis in the database is assigned clinical weighting scores, derived from
expert opinion, to reflect its prevalence by age group (e.g. child), gender and geographical region (e.g.
North America). In the current study, when investigators entered historical data and physical findings
noted at admission for each patient as search terms, and applied appropriate age, gender and region
filters, 10–12 diagnoses that were matched within the database were displayed, arranged by body system
(e.g. cardiovascular, respiratory) rather than by clinical probability. Hyperlinks from each diagnostic
suggestion led to text documents that matched the search query. There was no limit on the number of
search terms that could be entered, although the tool performed well even with 3–5 terms.
A number of different diagnostic DSS have been studied in the past. Tools such as De Dombal’s
abdominal pain system were developed using data from > 6000 patients with proven surgical causes of
abdominal pain and their clinical findings. Using probabilistic techniques, this tool assisted clinicians
in differentiating surgical from non-surgical causes of abdominal pain [5]. Other DSS such as Dxplain,
Quick Medical Reference, and ILIAD utilized a complex database of relationships between hundreds
of diseases and thousands of clinical findings (e.g. rheumatoid arthritis and fever) derived from expert
opinion to provide diagnostic hypotheses (i.e. quasi-probabilistic systems) [1]. Keeping such knowledge
bases up to date was a challenge - an expert panel needed to frequently appraise new information, and
make significant changes to existing relationships within the database. The ISABEL system utilizes
a novel mechanism to keep its knowledge base current. When updated textbooks or reviews became
available on a topic, old documents are simply replaced with new text in the database without any
expert input. A small content management team searches monthly for newer editions of textbooks and
reviews and updates the database on a quarterly basis. Expert input is only necessary to modify clinical
106
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
weighting scores, and is undertaken as soon as important new information is available (e.g. SARS virus
outbreak in Hong Kong). Since the tool is web based, updates are instantly available to users. A more
detailed description of the methodology involved in the development and maintenance of this tool is
available [13].
2.3. Diagnosis by board certified/board eligible pediatric intensivist
At each center, a pediatric intensivist independently examined the clinical data available at the time of
admission for each patient and the admitting PICU team’s diagnostic workup; the discharge diagnosis
was withheld from the attending physician during this process. They then highlighted further diagnoses
from within the computerized tool’s list that they considered clinically relevant or significant for patient
assessment. This judgment was based on whether consideration of that particular diagnosis in the
workup would have triggered a specific action (either performing further focused clinical examination or
a particular test or treatment). If two closely related (or nearly synonymous) diagnoses were displayed in
the list, only one was taken into account. This investigator was then able to propose additional diagnoses
they felt were relevant to generate an additional list of potential diagnoses.
2.4. Diagnostic accuracy assessment
Diagnostic accuracy was examined by matching the discharge diagnosis with lists generated by (i)
the admitting team, (ii) ISABEL and (iii) the study intensivist. The diagnosis was considered accurate
if the discharge diagnosis was located within the list of diagnoses generated by these three methods.
An a priori calculation demonstrated that 250 patients would be required to identify the frequency of
additional significant diagnoses in the diagnostic tool’s list assuming 20% of the patients were without an
established diagnosis (with 80% power, type I error 5%). Data collection was planned for three months
at each unit, based on an estimated annual admission rate of approximately 4000 patients. Diagnostic
accuracy rates of admission teams, ISABEL and attending physicians were calculated based on the
discharge diagnosis. Results were calculated as proportions, and expressed as percentages with 95%
confidence interval limits. The Chi-square test was used to detect statistically significant differences
between proportions (defined as p value < 0.05). Factors influencing diagnostic accuracy were analyzed
by entering the variables into a multiple logistic regression model.
3. Results
Data were collected from all five participating centers during the study. All centers admitted a general
mix of medical and surgical pediatric patients. The characteristics of each of the participating centers are
shown in Table 1. A total of 927 patients were admitted to the five PICUs between May and July 2003.
The majority of patients was either surgical/post-surgical or had an established diagnosis by the time
they were admitted to the PICU (721/927, 77.8%). A total of 206 patients (22.2%) had no established
diagnosis at the time of PICU admission, and were therefore eligible for study; 69 were from UK units
and 137 from the US units. Admission data for each center are presented in Table 2. Of these, 45 lacked
discharge diagnoses and were thereby excluded. The remaining 161 patients were therefore analyzed.
There were significant differences between the various centers. The number of patients without an
established diagnosis at admission varied between center, ranging from 19.6% to 77.8% (mean 38.9%;
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
107
Table 1
Key baseline characteristics of the participating critical care units
C1
C2
C3
C4
C5
Country
UK
UK
USA
USA
USA
Total number of beds
6
10
12
12
26
Medical admissions in 2003 (%) 143 (35) 363 (77) 289 (37) 402 (62) 1001 (67)
Surgical admissions in 2003 (%) 267 (65) 106 (23) 483 (63) 248 (38)
492 (33)
Total annual admissions*
410
469
772
650
1493
* Total annual admissions at all participating centers: 3794. (C = center; UK = United
Kingdom; US = United States).
Table 2
Breakdown of patient admissions according to diagnostic groups: MayJuly 2003
Known surgical Known medical
Unknown
Total
diagnosis (%)
diagnosis (%)
diagnosis (%)*
C1
67 (65.0)
8 (7.8)
28 (27.2)
103
C2
22 (22.7)
33 (34)
42 (43.3)
97
C3
117 (61.3)
30 (15.7)
44 (23)
191
C4
60 (38.2)
78 (49.7)
19 (12.1)
157
C5
132 (34.8)
174 (45.9)
73 (19.2)
379
Total
398 (42.9)
323 (34.8)
206 (22.2)
927
* Difference between centers was statistically significant (chi-square
test, p < 0.05). (C = center).
Chi-square test, p < 0.05). The proportion of surgical admissions on the units ranged from 23.7% to
65% (mean 42.9%).
The admitting team’s differential list contained a median of three diagnoses per case (IQR 3–5) with a
range of 1–12 diagnoses and contained the discharge diagnosis in 144/161 cases (89.4%). In five of the
remaining 17 cases in which the admitting team did not include the discharge diagnosis in their initial
workup, the diagnostic tool displayed the final diagnosis in the list of suggestions. ISABEL generated
a differential list including between 10 and 12 diagnoses and this list contained the final diagnosis in
149/161 cases (92.5%). The attending intensivist accurately provided the final diagnosis in a further
four cases, leading to an overall accuracy rate of 92% (when combined with the admitting team) and
95% (combined with the admitting team and the diagnostic tool). Despite the fact that the diagnostic
tool displayed the discharge diagnosis in five patients where the admitting team had failed to consider
it, the attending physician was able to identify only two of these as being clinically relevant to the case.
The admitting team’s diagnostic accuracy appeared to be a function of the age of the patient, but not the
number of diagnoses considered at admission or the diagnostic group.
4. Discussion
In summary, we have demonstrated that within tertiary care PICUs, approximately 20% of critically ill
children will be admitted without an established diagnosis and that the clinical team makes an accurate
preliminary diagnosis almost 90% of the time. This rate is improved by use of the ISABEL tool as well
as a higher degree of clinical training and experience from an intensivist. As we chose to examine every
PICU admission during the study period, this tool may have some added utility in a variety of patient
populations, particularly those that are complex diagnostic challenges.
108
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
In general, it is believed that improvements in technology will ultimately lead to improved outcomes
for patients in the health care system of all countries. While this might be true in some instances,
such as the prevention of medication errors [11], there has also been some evidence that increased need
for technology may also lead to increased patient harm [7]. To our knowledge, ISABEL is a unique
system in that it collates data from multiple sources including textbooks, journal articles and reviews to
generate a potential diagnostic list for the user. However, its ability to improve outcome has yet to be
demonstrated, and the direct impact on patient outcome related to use of the system is an area of research
that requires further clinical study. Estimates of diagnostic uncertainty in critical care have come from
various sources. In extreme circumstances, persistent diagnostic uncertainty is often only resolved by
post mortem studies. A number of autopsy studies have concluded that the rate of clinically significant
missed diagnoses in intensive care patients ranges from 2.1% to 21.2% [10,15,19]. In addition, it is
reported that in 17% of pediatric autopsies in critical care, knowledge of the diagnosis pre-mortem
would have influenced survival by affecting decision making [3]. Therefore, it is crucial to suspect
and confirm the correct diagnosis early in order to improve outcome in these children. The rate of
missed diagnoses directly leading to death is higher in patients with a shorter stay in critical care, while
unexpected minor findings are common in patients with a longer ICU stay, presumably due to the addition
of new medical problems while undergoing critical care treatments, either nosocomial or iatrogenic [6].
These post-mortem studies suggest a significant burden of missed diagnoses, and imply that diagnosis
constitutes an important part of critical care decision making. We failed to find such a critical burden of
missed diagnoses in our study and it did not appear that the missed diagnoses in our patients significantly
altered patient care or outcome. However, prospective evaluation of diagnostic errors in the critical care
environment suggests that they constitute only a small proportion of medical error; medication-related
errors are the most common cause for AME in this setting [16]. A more rigorous evaluation of the
ISABEL tool in a variety of clinical situations may lead to improved knowledge about missed diagnoses
as well as a more thorough evaluation of ISABEL’s usefulness.
We aimed to explore the role of a diagnostic tool in critical illness as part of this preliminary study.
Recently, ISABEL has been shown to be an effective reminding system in adult emergency departments,
with 95% accuracy in presenting the user with the final diagnosis and 90% accuracy in obtaining
“must-not-miss” diagnoses [12]. We undertook the current study in the Pediatric Intensive Care Units
of the five centers for several reasons. First, we wanted to demonstrate the utility of ISABEL in a
complex population of children with complex medical conditions, thereby collecting information on a
very challenging population for study. Second, we believe it is important to demonstrate the utility of
a tool in diverse patient groups. As three of the PICUs were in the USA and two were in the UK and
all units showed similar ISABEL performance, we believe that this highlights the potential utility and
generalizability of the tool. Moreover, the patient volumes at the five sites differed by more than 300%,
again arguing for the utility of the tool in a diverse population.
The most interesting question that can be asked regarding our data and device is whether a clinician can
improve his/her own diagnostic ability in collaboration with the use of ISABEL. It is not intended that
ISABEL is to become a substitute for clinical acumen, but rather an ancillary tool to establish potential
diagnoses. We believe that this is consistent with others who believe that the performance of the
intended user working in combination with the tool is greater than the performance of the user alone [18].
Obviously, this necessitates that the user-computer interaction must be seamless and user-friendly. None
of the investigators in our subset of more than a dozen had difficulty in utilizing the ISABEL tool to
generate useful diagnostic lists. Again, further studies with more diverse clinician populations may also
demonstrate whether this interface is sufficient for all users’ needs.
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
109
This study has a number of limitations. Patient selection of only cases without an established diagnosis
at admission to ICU may not accurately indicate the requirement or need for diagnostic decision making.
Since diagnoses are being constantly made and adjusted within the first hours of PICU admission, it
is possible that ISABEL might have other uses than the method we tested. Our methods also did not
allow for testing in other settings, such as the emergency department, where less data are available and
diagnostic uncertainty might be considerably greater. Second, we only studied this tool in tertiary care
facilities with well-trained clinicians and highly-productive trainees. The results of a similar study to this
in a smaller unit are unpredictable at this time. It is possible that the experience of the trainees masked
the true utility of the ISABEL device. Conversely, it is possible that in a smaller unit the diagnoses might
be less challenging and this might make ISABEL’s utility even less. Only future studies can answer
this question. Lastly, we intentionally excluded surgical cases in our study population even though they
made up almost over 50% of total admissions to the PICUs. Therefore, it is not possible to assess the
utility of ISABEL in diagnosing common pediatric surgical emergencies such as ruptured appendix or
other disorders.
In conclusion, ISABEL improved the diagnostic accuracy of the clinical team caring for children
without an established diagnosis upon PICU arrival. Future studies demonstrating ISABEL’s utility in
other patient populations will be required to fully assess whether web-based tools can improve children’s
outcome after critical illness. It will be important to determine if implementation of tools such as
ISABEL can be seamlessly integrated within clinical practice and if subsequent versions of the tool can
be sufficiently rigorous to advance as the field of pediatrics evolves and changes. This challenge must
be met by constant update and maintenance of diagnostic tools, as well as ongoing evaluation of the
efficacy of these tools in practice.
Acknowledgements
The computerized diagnostic tool studied was provided free for registered users by the Isabel Medical
Charity at the time of this study. It is now managed by a commercial entity called Isabel Healthcare
and is available only to subscribers. Dr P Ramnarayan currently advises Isabel Healthcare on research
activities on a part-time basis. Dr J Britto is Clinical Director of Isabel Healthcare Inc, USA. All the
other authors have no competing interests.
References
[1]
[2]
[3]
[4]
[5]
[6]
E.S. Berner, G.D. Webster, A.A. Shugerman, J.R. Jackson, J. Algina, A.L. Baker et al., Performance of four computerbased diagnostic systems, N Engl J Med 330 (1994), 1792–1796.
S.A. Blosser, H.E. Zimmerman and J.L. Stauffer, Do autopsies of critically ill patients reveal important findings that were
clinically undetected? Crit Care Med 26 (1998), 1332–1336.
M.P. Cardoso, D.C. Bourguignon, M.M. Gomes, P.H. Saldiva, C.R. Pereira and E.J. Troster, Comparison between clinical
diagnoses and autopsy findings in a pediatric intensive care unit in Sao Paulo, Brazil, Pediatr Crit Care Med 7 (2006),
423–427.
A.M. Combes, A. Mokhtari, A. Couvelard, J.L. Trouillet, J. Baudot, D. Henin et al., Clinical and autopsy diagnoses in
the intensive care unit: a prospective study, Arch Intern Med 164 (2004), 389–392.
F.T. De Dombal, D.J. Leaper, J.R. Staniland, A.P. McCann and J.C. Horrocks, Computer-aided diagnosis of acute
abdominal pain, Br Med J 2 (1972), 9–13.
G.M. Dimopoulos, M. Piagnerelli, J. Berre, I. Salmon and J.L. Vincent, Post mortem examination in the intensive care
unit: still useful? Intensive Care Med 30 (2004), 2080–2085.
110
[7]
[8]
[9]
[10]
[11]
[12]
[13]
[14]
[15]
[16]
[17]
[18]
[19]
[20]
[21]
[22]
[23]
N.J. Thomas et al. / An international assessment of a web-based diagnostic tool in critically ill children
Y.Y. Han, J.A. Carcillo, S.T. Venkataraman, R.S. Clark, R.S. Watson, T.C. Nguyen et al., Unexpected increased mortality
after implementation of a commercially sold computerized physician order entry system, Pediatrics 116 (2005), 1506–
1512.
P. Kumar, J. Taxy, D.B. Angst and H.H. Mangurten, Autopsies in children: are they still useful? Arch Pediatr Adolesc
Med 152 (1998), 558–563.
L.L. Leape, T.A. Brennan, N. Laird, A.G. Lawthers, A.R. Localio, B.A. Barnes et al., The nature of adverse events in
hospitalized patients. Results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study II, N Engl J Med 324 (1991), 377–384.
G.D. Perkins, D.F. McAuley, S. Davies and F. Gao, Discrepancies between clinical and postmortem diagnoses in critically
ill patients: an observational study, Crit Care 7 (2003), R129–R132.
A.L. Potts, F.E. Barr, D.F. Gregory, L. Wright and N.R. Patel, Computerized physician order entry and medication errors
in a pediatric critical care unit, Pediatrics 113 (2004), 59–63.
P. Ramnarayan, N. Cronje, R. Brown, R. Negus, B. Coode, P. Moss et al., Validation of a diagnostic reminder system in
emergency medicine: a multi-centre study, Emerg Med J 24 (2007), 619–624.
P. Ramnarayan, A. Tomlinson, G. Kulkarni, A. Rao and J. Britto, A novel diagnostic aid (ISABEL): development and
preliminary evaluation of clinical performance, Medinfo 11 (2004), 1091–1095.
P. Ramnarayan, A. Tomlinson, A. Rao, M. Coren, A. Winrow and J. Britto, ISABEL: a web-based differential diagnostic
aid for paediatrics: results from an initial performance evaluation, Arch Dis Child 88 (2003), 408–413.
J. Roosen, E. Frans, A. Wilmer, D.C. Knockaert and H. Bobbaers, Comparison of premortem clinical diagnoses in
critically iII patients and subsequent autopsy findings, Mayo Clin Proc 75 (2000), 562–567.
J.M. Rothschild, C.P. Landrigan, J.W. Cronin, R. Kaushal, S.W. Lockley, E. Burdick et al., The Critical Care Safety
Study: The incidence and nature of adverse events and serious medical errors in intensive care, Crit Care Med 33 (2005),
1694–1700.
K.G. Shojania, E.C. Burton, K.M. McDonald and L. Goldman, Changes in rates of autopsy-detected diagnostic errors
over time: a systematic review, JAMA 289 (2003), 2849–2856.
K.G. Shojania, E.C. Burton, K.M. McDonald, and L. Goldman, Overestimation of clinical diagnostic performance caused
by low necropsy rates, Qual Saf Health Care 14 (2005), 408–413.
J.J. Stambouly, E. Kahn and R.A. Boxer, Correlation between clinical diagnoses and autopsy findings in critically ill
children, Pediatrics 92 (1993), 248–251.
D.C. Sutherland, Improving medical diagnoses by understanding how they are made, Intern. Med J 32 (2002), 277–280.
N.J. Thomas, Isabel, Crit Care 7 (2002), 99–100.
C. Vincent, G. Neale and M. Woloshynowych, Adverse events in British hospitals: preliminary retrospective record
review, BMJ 322 (2001), 517–519.
J.C. Wyatt and J.L. Liu, Basic concepts in medical informatics, J Epidemiol Community Health 56 (2002), 808–812.
WHAT IS THE MAGNITUDE OF DIAGNOSIS ERROR?
Gordon D. Schiff, Seijeoung Kim,Richard Abrams et al.
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors: Lessons from a Multi-institutional Collaborative Project.
Advances in Patient Safety 2005; 2:255-278
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors: Lessons from
a Multi-institutional Collaborative Project
Gordon D. Schiff, Seijeoung Kim, Richard Abrams, Karen Cosby,
Bruce Lambert, Arthur S. Elstein, Scott Hasler, Nela Krosnjar,
Richard Odwazny, Mary F. Wisniewski, Robert A. McNutt
Abstract
Background: Diagnosis errors are frequent and important, but represent an
underemphasized and understudied area of patient safety. Diagnosis errors are
challenging to detect and dissect. It is often difficult to agree whether an error has
occurred, and even harder to determine with certainty its causes and consequence.
The authors applied four safety paradigms: (1) diagnosis as part of a system, (2)
less reliance on human memory, (3) need to open “breathing space” to reflect and
discuss, (4) multidisciplinary perspectives and collaboration. Methods: The
authors reviewed literature on diagnosis errors and developed a taxonomy
delineating stages in the diagnostic process: (1) access and presentation, (2)
history taking/collection, (3) the physical exam, (4) testing, (5) assessment, (6)
referral, and (7) followup. The taxonomy identifies where in the diagnostic
process the failures occur. The authors used this approach to analyze diagnosis
errors collected over a 3-year period of weekly case conferences and by a survey
of physicians. Results: The authors summarize challenges encountered from their
review of diagnosis error cases, presenting lessons learned using four prototypical
cases. A recurring issue is the sorting-out of relationships among errors in the
diagnostic process, delay and misdiagnosis, and adverse patient outcomes. To
help understand these relationships, the authors present a model that identifies
four key challenges in assessing potential diagnosis error cases: (1) uncertainties
about diagnosis and findings, (2) the relationship between diagnosis failure and
adverse outcomes, (3) challenges in reconstructing clinician assessment of the
patient and clinician actions, and (4) global assessment of improvement
opportunities. Conclusions and recommendations: Finally the authors catalogue
a series of ideas for change. These include: reengineering followup of abnormal
test results; standardizing protocols for reading x-rays/lab tests, particularly in
training programs and after hours; identifying “red flag” and “don’t miss”
diagnoses and situations and use of manual and automated check-lists; engaging
patients on multiple levels to become “coproducers” of safer medical diagnosis
practices; and weaving “safety nets” to mitigate harm from uncertainties and
errors in diagnosis. These change ideas need to be tested and implemented for
more timely and error-free diagnoses.
255
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
Introduction
Diagnosis errors are frequent and important, but represent an
underemphasized and understudied area of patient-safety.1–8 This belief led us to
embark on a 3-year project, funded by the Agency for Healthcare Research and
Quality (AHRQ), to better understand where and how diagnosis fails and explore
ways to target interventions that might prevent such failures. It is known that
diagnosis errors are common and underemphasized, but they are also challenging
to detect and dissect. It is often difficult even to agree whether or not a diagnosis
error has occurred.
In this article we describe how we have applied patient safety paradigms
(blame-free reporting/reviewing/learning, attention to process and systems, an
emphasis on communication and information technology) to better understand
diagnosis error.2, 7–9
We review evidence about the types and importance of diagnosis errors and
summarize challenges we have encountered in our review of more than 300 cases
of diagnosis error. In the second half of the article, we present lessons learned
through analysis of four prototypical cases. We conclude with suggested “change
ideas”—interventions for improvement, testing, and future research.
Although much of the patient safety spotlight has focused on medication
errors, two recent studies of malpractice claims revealed that diagnosis errors far
outnumber medication errors as a cause of claims lodged (26 percent versus 12
percent in one study;10 32 percent versus 8 percent in another study11). A Harris
poll commissioned by the National Patient Safety Foundation found that one in
six people had personally experienced a medical error related to misdiagnosis.12
Most medical error studies find that 10–30 percent (range = 0.6–56.8 percent) of
errors are errors in diagnosis (Table 1).1–3, 5, 11, 13–21 A recent review of 53 autopsy
studies found an average rate of 23.5 percent major missed diagnoses (range =
4.1–49.8 percent). Selected disease-specific studies (Table 2),6, 22–32 also show
that substantial percentages of patients (range = 2.1 – 61 percent) experienced
missed or delayed diagnoses. Thus, while these studies view the problem from
varying vantage points using heterogeneous methodologies (some nonsystematic
and lacking in standardized definitions), what emerges is compelling evidence for
the frequency and impact of diagnosis error and delay.
Of the 93 safety projects funded by AHRQ, only 1 is focused on diagnosis
error, and none of the 20 evidence-based AHRQ Patient Safety Indicators directly
measures failure to diagnose.33 Nonetheless, for each of AHRQ’s 26 “sentinel
complications” (e.g., decubitus ulcer, iatrogenic pneumothorax, postoperative
septicemia, accidental puncture/laceration), timely diagnosis can be decisive in
determining whether patients experience major adverse outcomes. Hence, while
diagnosis error remains more in the shadows than in the spotlight of patient
safety, this aspect of clinical medicine is clearly vulnerable to well-documented
failures and warrants an examination through the lens of modern patient safety
and quality improvement principles.
256
257
Mailed survey questionnaires regarding
patient safety issues, Canadian health care
facilities, and colleges and associations
Baker GR (2002)
Wilson RM (1999)
AE in England hospitals
The Quality in Australian Health Care Study
Bhasale AL (1998)
Neale G (2001)
Medical practice study, 1984, AE in NY
hospitals (Leape)
Austrian GP, Diagnostic incidents
Bogner M (1994)
Potential adverse events
AE in hospitalized patients
Leape LL (1991)
Weingart S (2000)
Context/Design
Physicians and surgeons update, St. Paul,
MN. Malpractice claims data reviewed
Author, Year
Flannery FT (1991)
1/25 (4%) of health care errors in
health care facilities, 9/21 (39%) in
colleges/associations
5/118 (0.6%)
29/110 (26.4%) process of care
problems associated w/ diagnosis
267/470 (56.8%) of AEs due to delays
were delays in diagnosis, 191 (40.6%)
treatment delay. 198/252 (78.6%)
investigation category were
investigation not performed, 39
(15.5%) not acted on, 9 (3.6%)
inappropriate.
275/805 (34.2%)
11731/68645 (17.1%)
168/1276 (13.8%)
Diagnosis errors/Total errors
(%)
1126/7233 (27.4%) failure to
diagnose
Table 1. General medical error studies that reported errors in diagnosis
Comments/Types of Diagnostic Errors
Top 5 diagnoses (cancer,
circulatory/thrombosis, fracture/dislocation,
lack of attendance, infection)
Failure to use indicated tests, act on test,
appropriate test, delay in diagnosis, practicing
outside area of expertise
17.1% preventable errors due to diagnostic
errors. 71% of them were negligent.
Calculated rates (N = 275): missed diagnosis
(40 per 100), delayed (34), misdiagnosed (23),
and diagnostic procedural (14)
1922/2351 AEs (81.8%) associated w/ human
errors, 470 (20%) delays in diagnosis or
treatment, 460 (19.6%) treatment, and 252
(10.7%) investigation. 1922 AEs w/ 2940
causes identified. Causes of human errors:
465/2940 (15.8%) failure to
synthesis/decide/act on information, 346
(11.8%) failure to request, arrange an
investigation.
18/110 (16.4%) inadequate evaluation, 7
(6.4%) diagnostic error, 4 (3.6%) delayed
consultation.
118/840 (14%) had AE. 57% of all AE,
cognitive. 5/118 (0.6%) of admissions were
associated with incorrect diagnoses. 2 missed
heart failures, 2 incorrect assessment of
abdominal pain, and 1 missed fracture.
Human factors, 3 (12%), 9 (39%); competency,
5 (20%), 2 (9%)
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
258
26126 claims peer reviewed,
5921/26126 (23%) related with
negligent
1371 claims, Failure to appropriate diagnostic
testing/monitoring
2003/5921 (34%) associated with diagnosis
error, 16% failure to monitor case, 4%
failure/delay in referral, 2% failure to recognize
a complication
55/528 (10.4%) patients admitted to the
hospitalists had at least 1 error
Comments/Types of Diagnostic Errors
55 delays in treatment, due to misdiagnosis
(42%), test results availability (13%), delayed
initial assessment (7%). Most frequent missed
diagnosis: meningitis 7/23 (30%), cardiac
disease, PE, trauma, asthma, neurologic
disorder
Process errors 104/134 (78%) in Austria,
235/301 (78%), Of the process errors,
investigation errors (lab errors, diagnostic
imaging errors, and others) were 13%, 19%
Failure-to-diagnose, failed to diagnosis in
timely fashion
AE = adverse events; GP = general practitioners; JCAHO = Joint Commission for Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations; ED = emergency department
Malpractice claims in primary care
Error detection by attending physicians,
general medicine inpatients
Phillips R (2004)
12/63 (19.1%) of all errors, 8/39
(20.5%) of near misses, and 4/24
(16.7%) of AEs were related with
diagnostic errors
3–8%
Medical malpractice
Medical malpractice
lawyers and attorneys
online (2002)
Chaudhry S (2003)
Malpractice claims data, 4 specialties
40%
GP in 6 countries
Makeham MA (2002)
Kravitz RL (2003)
17/104 (13%) in Austria, 55/236
(19%) in other countries
Context/Design
ED sentinel event
Author, Year
JCAHO Sentinel event
advisory group (2002)
Diagnosis errors/Total errors
(%)
55/23 (42%)
Table 1. General medical error studies that reported errors in diagnosis, cont.
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
Table 2. Illustrative disease-specific studies of diagnosis errors
Author
Disease/Design
Findings
Steere AC
(1993)
Lyme disease
Overdiagnosis of Lyme disease: patients given the
diagnosis but on review did not meet criteria for
diagnosis. - 452/788 (57%)
Cravan ER
(1994)
Glaucoma
Diagnosis error in glaucoma claims/lawsuits - 42/194
(21.7%)
Lederle FA
(1994)
Ruptured
abdominal aortic
aneurysm
Ruptured aneurysm diagnosis initially missed - 14/23
(61%)
Mayer PL
(1996)
Symptomatic
cerebral
aneurysm
Patients initially misdiagnosed - 54/217 (25%, ranging
from 13% to 35% at 4 sites); Of these, 26/54 (48%)
deteriorated before definite treatment; erroneous working
diagnoses in 54 pts; 8 (15%) viral meningitis, 7 (13%)
migraine, 7 (13%) headache of uncertain etiology
Williams V
(1997)
Brain and spinal
cord biopsies
Errors based on “second opinion” reviews - 214/500
(43%); 44/214 (20%) w/ serious complications, 96 (45%)
w/ substantial, 50 (23%) w/ minor errors
Clark S
(1998)
Pyrogenic spinal
infection
Delay in diagnosis - 41/69 (59.4%); more than 1 month of
back/neck pain before specialist referral
Arbiser ZK
(2000)
Soft tissue
pathology
Second option reviews: minor discrepancy - 20/266
(7.5%); major discrepancy - 65 (25%)
Pope JH
(2000)
Acute cardiac
ischemia in ED
Mistakenly discharged from ED; MI - 894/889 (2.1%);
Edeldman D
(2002)
Diabetes in an
outpatient clinic
Blood glucoses meeting criteria for DM with no diagnosis
of diabetes in records - 258/1426 (18%)
Goodson WH
(2002)
Breast Cancer
Inappropriately reassured to have benign lesions - 21/435
(5%); 14 (3%) misread mammogram, 4 (1%) misread
pathologic finding, 5 (1%) missed by poor fine-needle
biopsy
Kawalski RG
(2004)
Aneurysmal
subarachnoid
hemorrhage
(SAH)
Patients w/ SAH initially misdiagnosed - 56/482 (12%);
migraine/tension headache most common incorrect dx –
20/56 (36%); failure to obtain a CT was most common
error - 41/56 (73%)
Traditional and innovative approaches
to learning from diagnosis error
Traditionally, studying missed diagnoses or incorrect diagnoses had a central
role in medical education, research, and quality assurance in the form of
autopsies.34–36 Other traditional methods of learning about misdiagnosed cases
include malpractice litigation, morbidity and mortality (M&M) conferences,
unsystematic feedback from patients, other providers, or simply from patients’
illnesses as they evolved over time.3, 14, 15, 32 Beyond the negative aspects of being
grounded in patients’ deaths or malpractice accusations, there are other limitations
of these historical approaches, including
259
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
•
Lack of systematic approaches to surveillance, reporting, and learning
from errors, with nonrandom sample of cases subjected to such
review37, 39
•
Lack of timeliness, with cases often reviewed months or years after the
event38
•
Examinations that rarely dig to the root of problems: not focused on
the “Five Whys”40
•
Postmortems that seldom go beyond the case-at-hand, with minimal
linkages to formal quality improvement activities41
•
Atrophy of the value of even these suboptimal approaches, with
autopsy rates in the single digits (in many hospitals, zero), many
malpractice experiences sealed by nondisclosure agreements, and
shorter hospitalizations limiting opportunities for followup to ultimate
diagnosis34, 41, 42
What is needed to overcome these limitations is not only a more systematic
method for examining cases of diagnosis failure, but also a fresh approach.
Therefore, our team approached diagnosis error with the following perspectives:
Diagnosis as part of a system. Diagnostic accuracy should be viewed as a
system property rather than simply what happens between the doctor’s two
ears.2, 43–45 While cognitive issues figure heavily in the diagnostic process, a quite
from Don Berwick summarizes46 a much lacking and needed perspective: “Genius
diagnosticians make great stories, but they don’t make great health care. The idea
is to make accuracy reliable, not heroic.”
Less reliance on human memory. Relying on clinicians’ memory—to trigger
consideration of a particular diagnosis, recall a disease’s signs/symptoms/pattern
from a textbook or experience—or simply to remember to check on a patient’s lab
result—is an invitation to variations and failures. This lesson from other error
research resonates powerfully with clinicians, who are losing the battle to keep up
to date.9, 45, 47
Need for “space” to allow open reflection and discussion. Transforming an
adversarial atmosphere into one conducive to honest reflection is an essential first
step.48, 49 However, an equally important and difficult challenge is creating venues
that allow clinicians (and patients) to discuss concerns in an efficient and
productive manner.37 Cases need to be reviewed in sufficient detail to make them
“real.” Firsthand clinical information often radically changes our understanding
from what the more superficial “first story” suggested. As complex clinical
circumstances are better understood, new light is often shed on what at first
appeared to be indefensible diagnostic decisions and actions. Unsuspected
additional errors also emerge. Equally important is not to get mired in details or
making judgments (whether to label a case as a diagnosis error). Instead, it is
more valuable to focus on generalizable lessons of how to ensure better treatment
of similar future patients.16
260
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
Adopting multidisciplinary perspectives and collaboration. A broad range
of skills and vantage points are valuable in understanding the complex diagnostic
problems that we encountered. We considered input from specialists and primary
care physicians to be essential. In addition, specialists in emergency medicine
(where many patients first present) offered a vital perspective, both for their
diagnostic expertise and their pivotal interface with system constraints (resource
limits mean that not every patient with a confusing diagnosis can be hospitalized).
Even more valuable has been the role of non-MDs, including nursing quality
specialists, information scientists, and social scientists (cognitive psychologist,
decision theory specialist) in forging a team to broadly examine diagnosis errors.
Innovative screening approaches. Developing new ways to uncover errors is
a priority. We cannot afford to wait for a death, lawsuit, or manual review.
Approaches we have been exploring include electronic screening that links
pharmacy and lab data (e.g., to screen for abnormal results, such as elevated
thyroid stimulating hormone [TSH], unaddressed by thyroxin therapy), trajectory
studies (retrospectively probing delays in a series of cases with a particular
diagnosis), and screening for discrepancies between admitting and discharge
diagnoses. A related approach is to survey specialists (who are poised to see
diagnoses missed in referred patients), primary care physicians (about their own
missed diagnoses), or patients themselves (who frequently have stories to share
about incorrect diagnoses), in addition to various ad hoc queries and self-reports.
Where does the diagnostic process fail?
One of the most powerful heuristics in medication safety has been delineation
of the steps in the medication-use process (prescribing, transcribing, dispensing,
administering, and monitoring) to help localize where an error has occurred.
Diagnosis, while more difficult to neatly classify (because compared to
medications, stages are more concurrent, recurrent, and complex), nonetheless can
be divided into seven stages: (1) access/presentation, (2) history taking/collection,
(3) the physical exam, (4) testing, (5) assessment, (6) referral, and (7) followup.
We have found this framework helpful for organizing discussions, aggregating
cases, and targeting areas for improvement and research. It identifies what went
wrong, and situates where in the diagnostic process the failure occurred (Table 3).
We have used it for a preliminary analysis of several hundred diagnosis error
cases we collected by surveying physicians.
This taxonomy for categorizing diagnostic “assessment” draws on work of
Kassirer and others,53 highlighting the two key steps of (a) hypothesis generation,
and (b) differential diagnosis or hypothesis weighing/prioritization. We add
another aspect of diagnostic assessment, one that connects to other medical and
iatrogenic error work—the need to recognize the urgency of diagnoses and
complications. This addition underscores the fact that failure to make the exact
diagnosis is often less important than correctly assessing the urgency of the
patient’s illness. We divide the “testing” stage into three components—ordering,
performing, and clinician processing (similar but not identical to the laboratory
literature classification of the phases of lab testing as preanalytic, analytic, and
261
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
Table 3. Taxonomy of where and what errors occurred
What Went Wrong
Where in Diagnostic
Process
(~Anatomic localization)
(~Lesion)
1. Access/presentation
Denied care
2. History
Failure/delay in eliciting critical piece of history data
Delayed presentation
Inaccurate/misinterpretation
Suboptimal weighing
Failure/delay to followup
3. Physical exam
“
“
“
Failure/delay in eliciting critical physical exam finding
Inaccurate/misinterpreted
“
Suboptimal weighing “
Failure/delay to followup
4. Tests (lab/radiology)
“
Ordering
Failure/delay in ordering needed test(s)
Failure/delay in performing ordered test(s)
Suboptimal test sequencing
Ordering of wrong test(s)
Performance
Sample mix-up/mislabeled (e.g., wrong patient)
Technical errors/poor processing of specimen/test
Erroneous lab/radiol reading of test
Failed/delayed transmission of result to clinician
Clinician processing
Failed/delayed followup action on test result
Erroneous clinician interpretation of test
5. Assessment
Hypothesis generation
Failure/delay in considering the correct diagnosis
Suboptimal weighing/prioritizing
Too much weight to low(er) probability/priority dx
Too little consideration of high(er) probability/priority dx
Too much weight on competing diagnosis
Recognizing urgency/complications
Failure to appreciate urgency/acuity of illness
Failure/delay in recognizing complication(s)
6. Referral/consultation
Failure/delay in ordering needed referral
Inappropriate/unneeded referral
Suboptimal consultation diagnostic performance
Failed/delayed communication/followup of consultation
7. Followup
Failure to refer to setting for close monitoring
Failure/delay in timely followup/rechecking of patient
262
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
postanalytic).50, 51 For each broad category, we specified the types of problems we
observed.
A recurring theme running through our reviews of potential diagnosis error
cases pertains to the relationship between errors in the diagnostic process, delay
and misdiagnosis, and adverse patient outcomes. Bates52 has promulgated a useful
model for depicting the relationships between medication errors and outcomes.
Similarly, we find that most errors in the diagnostic process do not adversely
impact patient outcomes. And, many adverse outcomes associated with
misdiagnosis or delay do not necessarily result from any error in the diagnostic
process—the cancer may simply be undiagnosable at that stage, the illness
presentation too atypical, rare, or unlikely even for the best of clinicians to
diagnose early. These situations are often referred to as “no fault” or “forgivable”
errors—terms best avoided because they imply fault or blame for preventable
errors (Figure 1).7, 53
While deceptively simple, the model raises a series of extremely challenging
questions—questions we found ourselves repeatedly returning to in our weekly
discussions. We hope these questions can provide insights into recurring themes
and challenges we faced, and perhaps even serve as a checklist for others to
structure their own patient care reviews. While humbled by our own inability to
provide more conclusive answers to these questions, we believe researchers and
practitioners will be forced to grapple with them before we can make significant
progress.
Questions for consideration by diagnosis error evaluation
and research (DEER) investigators in assessing cases
Uncertainties about diagnosis and findings
1. What is the correct diagnosis? How much certainty do we have, even
now, about what the correct diagnosis is?
2. What were the findings at the various points in time when the patient
was being seen; how much certainty do we have that a particular finding
and diagnosis was actually present at the time(s) we are positing an
error?
Relationship between diagnosis failure and adverse outcomes
3. What is the probability that the error resulted in the adverse outcome?
How treatable is the condition, and how critical is timely diagnosis and
treatment for impacting on the outcome—both in general and in this
case?
4. How did the error in the diagnostic process contribute to making the
wrong diagnosis and wrong treatment?
263
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
Figure 1. Relationships between diagnostic process errors, misdiagnosis, and adverse
events
Diagnostic
Process Errors
Adverse
Events
G
F
D
C
A
B
Diagnosis*
Errors
E
*Delayed, missed, or misdiagnosis
Caption
Group A = Errors in diagnostic process (blood sample switched between two patients, MD
doesn’t do a physical exam for patient with abdominal pain)
Group B = Diagnostic process error with resulting misdiagnosis (patient given wrong diagnosis
because blood samples switched)
Group C = Adverse outcome resulting from error-related misdiagnosis (Patient is given toxic
treatment and has adverse effect as result of switched samples. Fail to diagnose
appendicitis because of failure to examine abdomen, and it ruptures and patient dies)
Group D = Harm from error in diagnostic process (colon perforation from colonoscopy done on
wrong patient)
Group E = Misdiagnosis, delayed diagnosis or missed diagnosis, but no error in care or harm
(incidental prostate cancer found on autopsy)
Group F = Adverse event due to misdiagnosis but no identifiable process error (death from
acute MI but no chest pain or other symptoms that were missed)
Group G = Adverse events but not related to misdiagnosis, delay, or error in diagnostic process,
e.g., death from correctly diagnosed disease complication, or nonpreventable drug
reaction (PCN anaphylaxis in patient never previously exposed)
264
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
Clinician assessment and actions
5. What was the physican’s diagnostic assessment? How much
consideration was given to the correct diagnosis? (This is usually
difficult to reconstruct because differential diagnosis often is not well
documented.)
6. How good or bad was the diagnostic assessment based on evidence
clinicians had on hand at that time (should have been obvious from
available data vs. no way anyone could have suspected)?
7. How erroneous was the diagnostic assessment, based on the difficulty
in making the diagnosis at this point? (Was there a difficult “signal-tonoise” situation, a rare low-probability diagnosis, or an atypical
presentation?)
8. How justifiable was the failure to obtain additional information (i.e.,
history, tests) at a particular point in time? How can this be analyzed
absolutely, as well as relative to the difficulties and constraints in
obtaining this missing data? (Did the patient withhold or refuse to give
accurate/additional history; were there backlogs and delays that made
it impossible to obtain the desired test?)
9. Was there a problem in diagnostic assessment of the severity of the
illness, with resulting failure to observe or follow up the patient more
closely? (Again, both absolutely and relative to constraints.)
Global assessment of improvement opportunities
10. To what extent did the clinicians’ actions deviate from the standard-ofcare (i.e., was there negligent care with failure to follow accepted
diagnostic guidelines and expected practices, or to pursue abnormal
finding that should never be ignored)?
11. How preventable was the error? How ameliorable or amenable to
change are the factors/problems that contributed to the error? How
much would such changes, designed to prevent this error in the future,
cost?
12. What should we do better the next time we encounter a similar patient
or situation? Is there a general rule, or are there measures that can be
implemented to ensure this is reliably done each time?
Diagnosis error case vignettes
Case 1
A 25-year-old woman presents with crampy abdominal pain, vaginal bleeding,
and amenorrhea for 6 weeks. Her serum human choriogonadotropin (HCG) level
is markedly elevated. A pelvic ultrasound is read by the on-call radiology chief
resident and obstetrics (OB) attending physician as showing an empty uterus,
265
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
suggesting ectopic pregnancy. The patient is informed of the findings and treated
with methotrexate. The following morning the radiology attending reviews the
ultrasound and amends the report, officially reading it as “normal intrauterine
pregnancy.”
Case 2
A 49-year-old, previously healthy man presents to the emergency department
(ED) with nonproductive cough, “chest congestion,” and dyspnea lasting 2 weeks;
he has a history of smoking. The patient is afebrile, with pulse = 105, respiration
rate (RR) = 22, and white blood count (WBC) = 6.4. Chest x-ray shows “marked
cardiomegaly, diffuse interstitial and reticulonodular densities with blunting of the
right costophrenic angle; impression—congestive heart failure (CHF)/pneumonia.
Rule out (R/O) cardiomyopathy, valve disease or pericardial effusion.” The
patient is sent home with the diagnosis of pneumonia, with an oral antibiotic.
The patient returns 1 week later with worsening symptoms. He is found to
have pulsus paradoxicus, and an emergency echocardiogram shows massive
pericardial effusion. Pericardiocentesis obtains 350 cc fluid with cytology positive
for adenocarcinoma. Computed tomography of the chest suggests “lymphangitic
carcinomatosis.”
Case 3
A 50-year-old woman with frequent ED visits for asthma (four visits in the
preceding month) presents to the ED with a chief complaint of dyspnea and new
back pain. She is treated for asthma exacerbation and discharged with
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAID) for back pain.
She returns 2 days later with acutely worsening back pain, which started when
reaching for something in her cupboard. A chest x-ray shows a “tortuous and
slightly ectatic aorta,” and the radiologist’s impression concludes, “If aortic
dissection is suspected, further evaluation with chest CT with intravenous (IV)
contrast is recommended.” The ED resident proceeds to order a chest CT, which
concludes “no evidence of aneurysm or dissection.” The patient is discharged.
She returns to the ED 3 days later, again complaining of worsening asthma
and back pain. While waiting to be seen, she collapses in the waiting room and is
unable to be resuscitated. Autopsy shows a ruptured aneurysm of the ascending
aorta.
Case 4
A 50-year-old woman with a past history diabetes and alcohol and IV drug
abuse, presents with symptoms of abdominal pain and vomiting and is diagnosed
as having “acute chronic pancreatitis.” Her amylase and lipase levels are normal.
She is admitted and treated with IV fluids and analgesics. On hospital day 2 she
begins having spiking fevers and antibiotics are administered. The next day, blood
cultures are growing gram negative organisms.
266
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
At this point, the service is clueless about the patient’s correct diagnosis. It
only becomes evident the following day when (a) review of laboratory data over
the past year shows that patient had four prior blood cultures, each positive with
different gram negative organisms; (b) a nurse reports patient was “behaving
suspiciously,” rummaging through the supply room where syringes were kept;
and (c) a medical student looks up posthospital outpatient records from 4 months
earlier and finds several notes stating that “the patient has probable Munchausen
syndrome rather than pancreatitis.” Upon discovering these findings, the patient’s
IVs are discontinued and sensitive, appropriate followup primary and psychiatric
care are arranged.
A postscript to this admission: 3 months later, the patient was again
readmitted to the same hospital for “pancreatitis” and an unusual “massive leg
abscess.” The physicians caring for her were unaware of her past diagnoses and
never suspected or discovered the likely etiology of her abscess (self-induced
from unsterile injections).
Lessons and issues raised
by the diagnosis error cases
Difficulties in sorting out “don’t miss” diagnoses
Before starting our project, we compiled a list of “don’t miss” diagnoses
(available from the authors). These are diagnoses that are considered critical, but
often difficult to make—critical because timely diagnosis and treatment can have
major impact (for the patient or the public’s health, or both), and difficult because
they either are rare or pose diagnostic challenges. Diagnoses such as spinal
epidural abscess (where paraplegia can result from delayed diagnosis), or active
pulmonary tuberculosis (TB) (where preventing spread of infection is critical) are
examples of “don’t miss” diagnoses. While there is a scant evidence base to
definitively compile and prioritize such a list, three of our cases—ectopic
pregnancy, dissecting aortic aneurysm, and pericardial effusion with tamponade—
are diagnoses that would unquestionably be considered as life-threatening
diagnoses that ought not be delayed or missed.25
Although numerous issues concerning diagnosis error are raised by these
cases, they also illustrate problems relating to uncertainties, lack of gold standards
(for both testing and standard of care), and difficulties reaching consensus about
best ways to prevent future errors and harmful delays. Below we briefly discuss
some of these issues and controversies.
Diagnostic criteria and strategies for diagnosing ectopic pregnancy are
controversial, and our patient’s findings were particularly confusing. Even after
careful review of all aspects of the case, we were still not certain who was
“right”—the physicians who read the initial images and interpreted them as
consistent with ectopic pregnancy, or the attending physician rereading the films
the next day as normal. The literature is unclear about criteria for establishing this
267
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
diagnosis.54–57 In addition, there is a lack of standards in performance and
interpretation of ultrasound exams, plus controversies about timing of
interventions. Thus this “obvious” error is obviously more complex, highlighting
a problem-prone clinical situation.
The patient who was found to have a malignant pericardial effusion illustrates
problems in determining the appropriate course of action for patients with
unexplained cardiomegaly, which the ED physicians failed to address on his first
presentation: Was he hemodynamically stable at this time? If so, did he require an
urgent echocardiogram? What criteria should have mandated this be done
immediately? How did his cardiomegaly get “lost” as his diagnosis prematurely
“closed” on pneumonia when endorsed from the day to the night ED shift? How
should one assess the empiric treatment for pneumonia given his abnormal chest
x-ray? Was this patient with metastatic lung cancer harmed by the 1 week delay?
The diagnosis of aneurysms (e.g., aortic, intracranial) arises repeatedly in
discussions of misdiagnosis. Every physician seems to recall a case of a missed
aneurysm with catastrophic outcomes where, in retrospect, warnings may have
been overlooked. A Wall Street Journal article recently won a Pulitzer Prize for
publicizing such aneurysm cases.58 Our patient’s back pain was initially
dismissed. Because of frequent visits, she had been labeled as a “frequent flyer”—
and back pain is an extremely common and nonspecific symptom. A review of
literature on the frequency of dissecting aortic aneurysm reveals that it is
surprisingly rare, perhaps less than 1 out of 50,000 ED visits for chest pain, and
likely an equally rare cause for back pain.59–62 She did ultimately undergo a
recommended imaging study after a suspicious plain chest x-ray, however it was
deemed “negative.”
Thus, in each case, seemingly egregious and unequivocal errors were found to
be more complex and uncertain.
Issues related to limitations of diagnostic testing
Even during our 3 years of diagnosis case reviews, clinicians have been
confronted with rapid changes in diagnostic testing. New imaging modalities, lab
tests, and testing recommendations have been introduced, often leaving clinicians
confused about which tests to order and how to interpret their confusing and, at
times, contradictory (from one radiologist to the next) results.63
If diagnosis errors are to be avoided, clinicians must be aware of the
limitations of the diagnostic tests they are using. It is well known that a normal
mammogram in a woman with a breast lump does not rule out the diagnosis of
breast cancer, because the sensitivity of test is only 70 to 85 percent.13, 26, 64, 65
A recurring theme in our cases is failure to appreciate pitfalls in weighing test
results in the context of the patient’s pretest disease probabilities. Local factors,
such as the variation in quality of test performance and readings, combined with
communication failures between radiology/laboratory and ordering physicians
(either no direct communication or interactions where complex interpretations get
268
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
reduced to “positive” or “negative,” overlooking subtleties and limitations)
provide further sources of error.66
The woman with the suspected ectopic pregnancy, whose emergency
ultrasound was initially interpreted as being “positive,” illustrates the pitfalls of
taking irreversible therapeutic actions without carefully weighing test reading
limitations. Perhaps an impending rupture of an ectopic pregnancy warrants
urgent action. However, it is also imperative for decisions of this sort that
institutions have fail-safe protocols that anticipate such emergencies and
associated test limitations.
The patient with a dissecting aortic aneurysm clearly had a missed diagnosis,
as confirmed by autopsy. This diagnosis was suspected premortem, but
considered to be “ruled out” by a CT scan that did not show a dissection. Studies
of the role of chest CT, particularly when earlier CT scanning technology was
used, show sensitivity for dissecting aneurysm of only 83 percent.67 When we
reexamined the old films for this patient, several radiologists questioned the
adequacy of the study (quality of the infusion plus question of motion artifact).
Newer, faster scanners reportedly are less prone to these errors, but the experience
is variable. We identified another patient where a “known” artifact on a spiral CT
nearly led to an unnecessary aneurysm surgery; it was only prevented by the fact
that she was a Jehovah’s Witness and, because her religious beliefs precluded
transfusions, surgery was considered too risky.62
The role of information transfer and the
communication of critical laboratory information
Failure of diagnosis because of missing information is another theme in our
weekly case reviews and the medical literature. Critical information can be missed
because of failures in history-taking, lack of access to medical records, failures in
the transmission of diagnostic test results, or faulty records organization (either
paper or electronic) that created problems for quickly reviewing or finding needed
information.
For the patient with the self-induced illness, all of the “missing” information
was available online. Ironically, although many patients with a diagnosis of
Munchausen’s often go to great lengths to conceal information (i.e., giving false
names, using multiple hospitals), in our case, there was so much data in the
computer from previous admissions and outpatient visits that the condition was
“lost” in a sea of information overload—a problem certain to grow as more and
more clinical information is stored online. While this patient is an unusual
example of the general problems related to information transfer, this case
illustrates important principles related to the need for conscientious review, the
synthesizing of information, and continuity (both of physicians and information)
to avoid errors.
Simply creating and maintaining a patient problem list can help prevent
diagnosis errors. It can ensure that each active problem is being addressed,
helping all caregivers to be aware of diagnoses, allergies, and unexplained
269
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
findings. Had our patient with “unexplained cardiomegaly” been discharged
listing this as one of his problems, instead of only “pneumonia,” perhaps this
problem would not have been overlooked. However, making this seemingly
simple documentation tool operational has been unsuccessful in most institutions,
even ones with advanced electronic information systems, and thus represents a
challenge as much as a panacea.
One area of information transfer, the followup of abnormal laboratory test
results, represents an important example of this information transfer paradigm in
diagnostic patient safety.68–73 We identified failure rates of more than 1 in 50 for
both followup abnormal thyroid tests (where the diagnosis of hypothyroidism was
missed in 23 out of 982, or 2.3 percent, of patients with markedly elevated TSH
results), and our earlier study on failure to act on elevated potassium levels (674
out of 32,563, or 2.0 percent, of potassium prescriptions were written for
hyperkalemic patients).74 Issues of communication, teamwork, systems design,
and information technology stand as areas for improvement.75–77 Recognizing
this, the Massachusetts Safety Coalition has launched a statewide initiative on
Communicating Critical Test Results.78
Physician time, test availability,
and other system constraints
Our project was based in two busy urban hospitals, including a public hospital
with serious constraints on bed availability and access to certain diagnostic tests.
An important recurring theme in our case discussions (and in health care
generally) is the interaction between diagnostic imperatives and these resource
limitations.
To what extent is failure to obtain an echocardiogram, or even a more
thorough history or physical exam, understandable and justified by the
circumstances under which physicians find themselves practicing? Certainly our
patient with the massive cardiomegaly needed an echocardiogram at some time.
Was it reasonable for the ED to defer the test (meaning a wait of perhaps several
months in the clinic), or would a more “just-in-time” approach be more efficient,
as well as safer in minimizing diagnosis error and delay?79 Since, by definition,
we expect ED physicians to triage and treat emergencies, not thoroughly work up
every problem patients have, we find complex trade-offs operating at multiple
levels.
Similar trade-offs impact whether a physician had time to review all of the
past records of our factitious-illness patient (only the medical student did), or how
much radiology expertise is available around-the-clock to read ultrasound or CT
exams, to diagnose ectopic pregnancy or aortic dissection. This is perhaps the
most profound and poorly explored aspect of diagnosis error and delay, but one
that will increasingly be front-and-center in health care.
270
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
Cognitive issues in diagnosis error
We briefly conclude where most diagnosis error discussions begin, with
cognitive errors.45, 53, 80–84
Hindsight bias and the difficulty of weighing prior probabilities of the
possible diagnoses bedeviled our efforts to assess decisions and actions
retrospectively. Many “don’t miss” diagnoses are rare; it would be an error to
pursue each one for every patient. We struggled to delineate guidelines that would
accurately identify high-risk patients and to design strategies to prevent missing
these diagnoses.
Our case reviews and firsthand interviews often found that each physician had
his or her own individual way of approaching patients and their problems. Such
differences made for lively conference discussions, but have disturbing
implications for developing more standardized approaches to diagnosis.
The putative dichotomy between “cognitive” and “process” errors is in many
ways an artificial distinction.7, 8 If a physician is interrupted while talking to the
patient or thinking about a diagnosis and forgets to ask a critical question or
consider a critical diagnosis, is this a process or cognitive error?
Conclusion
Because of their complexity, there are no quick fixes for diagnosis errors. As
we review what we learned from a variety of approaches and cases, certain areas
stood out as ripe for improvement—both small-scale improvements that can be
tested locally, as well as larger improvements that need more rigorous, formal
research. Table 4 summarizes these change ideas, which harvest the lessons of our
3-year project.
As outlined in the table, there needs to be a commitment to build learning
organizations, in which feedback to earlier providers who may have failed to
make a correct diagnosis becomes routine, so that institutions can learn from this
aggregated feedback data. To better protect patients, we will need to
conceptualize and construct safety nets to mitigate harm from uncertainties and
errors in diagnosis. The followup of abnormal test results is a prime candidate for
reengineering, to ensure low “defect” rates that are comparable to those achieved
in other fields. More standardized and reliable protocols for reading x-rays and
laboratory tests (such as pathology specimens), particularly in residency training
programs and “after hours,” could minimize the errors we observed. In addition,
we need to better delineate “red flag” and “don’t miss” diagnoses and situations,
based on better understanding and data regarding pitfalls in diagnosis and ways to
avoid them.
271
Safety nets to mitigate harm
from diagnostic uncertainty and
error
Change Idea
Upstream feedback to earlier
providers who have may have
failed to make correct diagnosis
272
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Educating and empowering patients to have lower threshold
for seeking followup care or advice, including better defining
and specifying particular warning symptoms
•
•
•
•
•
Well designed observation and followup venues and
systems (e.g., admit for observation, followup calls or e-mail
from MD in 48 hours, automated 2 wk phone followup to
ensure tests obtained) for high-risk, uncertain diagnoses
Permits aggregation for tracking, uncovering patterns,
learning across cases, elucidating pitfalls, measuring
improvements
Build in “feedback from the feedback” to capture reflective
practitioner assessment of why errors may have occurred
and considerations for future prevention
Feedback from specialists poised to see missed diagnosis
could be especially useful
“Hard-wires” organizational and practitioner learning from
diagnosis evolution and delays
Promotes culture of safety, accountability, continuous and
blame-free learning, and communication
Rationale/Description
Table 4. Change ideas for preventing and minimizing diagnostic error
Not creating excessive patient worry/anxiety
Avoiding false positive errors, inappropriate use of
scarce/costly resources
Resource constraints (bed, test availability, clinician time)
Logistics
To be highest leverage needs to be coupled with
reporting, case conferences
Ways to extend to previous hospitals and physicians
outside of own institution
Protecting confidentiality, legal liabilities, blame-free
atmosphere
Avoiding “tampering,” from availability bias that neglects
base rates (ordering aortogram on every chest pain
patient to “rule out” dissection)
Logistical requirements for implementation and
surveillance screening to identify errors
Challenges
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
273
Prospectively defining red flag
diagnoses and situations and
instituting prospective readiness
Fail-safe protocols for
preliminary/resident and
definitive readings of tests
Change Idea
Timely and reliable abnormal
test result followup systems
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Embodies/requires coordinated multidisciplinary approach
(e.g., pharmacy, radiology, specialists)
Like AMI thrombolytic “clot box,” in-place for ready activation
the moment patient first presents
Create “pull” systems for patients with particular medical
problems to ensure standardized, expedited diagnostic
evaluations (so don’t have to “push” to quickly obtain)
“After-hours” systems for readings, amending
Need for system for amending and alerting critical changes
to clinicians
Must be well-defined and organized system for supervision,
creating and communicating final reports
Modeled on Massachusetts Patient Safety Coalition
program 78
Involve patients to ensure timely notification of results, and
contacting provider when this fails
Leverage information technologies to automate and
increase reliability
Uniform approach across various test types and disciplines
(lab, pathology, radiology, cardiology)
Fail-safe, prospectively designed systems to identify which
results are critical abnormals (panic and routine), who to
communicate result to, how to deliver
Rationale/Description
Table 4. Change ideas for preventing and minimizing diagnostic error, cont.
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Avoiding excessive work-up, diverting resources from
other problems
Difficulties in evidence-based delineation of diagnoses,
situations, patient selection, criteria, and standardized
actions
Quality control poses major unmet challenges
How to best recognize and convey variations in expertise
of attendings who write final reports
Practitioner efficiency issues: avoiding excess duplicate
work; efficient documentation
Defining responsibilities: e.g., for ED patients, for lab
personnel
On-call, cross-coverage issues for critical panic results
Achieving consensus on test types and cut-off thresholds
Challenges
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
Test and leverage information
technology tools to avoid
known cognitive and care
process pitfalls
High-level patient education
and engagement with diagnosis
probabilities and uncertainties
Change Idea
Automated screening check
lists to avoid missing key
history, physical, lab data
274
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Prompts to suggest consideration of medication effects in
differential, based on linkages to patient’s medication profile,
lab results
Sophisticated decision support tools that use complex rules
and individualized patients data
Facilitating real-time access to medical knowledge sources
Easing documentation time demands to give practitioners
more time to talk to patients and think about their problems
Better design of ways to streamline documentation
(including differential diagnosis) and access/display of
historical data
Support and enhance patients taking initiative to question
diagnosis, particularly if not responding as expected
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Could be customized based on presenting problem (i.e.,
work exposures for lung symptoms, travel history for fever)
Since diagnosis often so complex and difficult, this needs to
be shared with patients in ways to minimize
disappointments and surprises
•
•
Queries triggered by individual patient features
Less reliance on human memory for more thorough
questioning
Rationale/Description
Table 4. Change ideas for preventing and minimizing diagnostic error, cont.
Alleged atrophy of unaided cognitive skills
New errors and distractions introduced by intrusion of
computer into clinical encounter
Paucity of standardized, accepted, sharable clinical
alerts/rules
Challenges coupling knowledge bases with individual
patient characteristics
Shortcomings of first generation of “artificial intelligence”
diagnosis software
Doing in way that balances need for preserving patient
confidence in their physicians and advice, with education
and recognition of diagnosis fallabilities
Avoiding unnecessary testing
Potentially more time-consuming for practitioners
Effectively implementing with teamwork and integrated
information technology
Sorting out, avoiding false positive errors from data with
poor signal:noise ratio
Evidence of efficacy to-date unconvincing; unclear value
of unselective “review of systems”
Challenges
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
To achieve many of these advances, automated (and manual) checklists and
reminders will be needed to overcome current reliance on human memory. But
information technology must also be deployed and reengineered to overcome
growing problems associated with information overload. Finally, and most
importantly, patients will have to be engaged on multiple levels to become
“coproducers” in a safer practice of medical diagnosis.85 It is our hope that these
change ideas can be tested and implemented to ensure safer treatment based on
better diagnoses—diagnosis with fewer delays, mistakes, and process errors.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by AHRQ Patient Safety Grant #11552, the Cook
County–Rush Developmental Center for Research in Patient Safety (DCERPS)
Diagnostic Error Evaluation and Research (DEER) Project.
Author affiliations
Cook County John H. Stroger Hospital and Bureau of Health Services, Chicago (GDS, KC, MFW).
Rush University Medical Center, Chicago (RA, SH, RO, RAM). Hektoen Research Institute, Chicago (SK,
NK). University of Illinois at Chicago, College of Pharmacy (BL). University of Illinois at Chicago
Medical School (ASE).
Address correspondence to: Gordon D. Schiff; Cook County John H. Stroger Hospital and Bureau of
Health Services, 1900 W. Polk Street, Administration Building #901, Chicago, IL 60612. Phone: 312-8644949; fax: 312-864-9594; e-mail: gdschiff@aol.com.
References
1.
Baker GR, Norton P. Patient safety and healthcare
error in the Canadian healthcare system. Ottawa,
Canada: Health Canada; 2002. pp. 1–167.
(http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/
english/care/report. Accessed 12/24/04.)
2.
Leape L, Brennan T, Laird N, et al. The nature of
adverse events in hospitalized patients: Results of the
Harvard medical practice study II. NEJM
1991;324:377–84.
3.
Medical Malpractice Lawyers and Attorneys Online.
Failure to diagnose. http://www.medical-malpracticeattorneys-lawsuits.com/pages/failure-todiagnose.html. 2004.
7.
Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing diagnostic
errors in medicine: what’s the goal? Acad Med
2002;77:981–92.
8.
Kuhn G. Diagnostic errors. Acad Emerg Med
2002;9:740–50.
9.
Kohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS, editors. To err
is human: building a safer health system. A report of
the Committee on Quality of Health Care in America,
Institute of Medicine. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press; 2000.
10. Sato L. Evidence-based patient safety and risk
management technology. J Qual Improv 2001;27:435.
4.
Ramsay A. Errors in histopathology reporting:
detection and avoidance. Histopathology
1999;34:481–90.
11. Phillips R, Bartholomew L, Dovey S, et al. Learning
from malpractice claims about negligent, adverse
events in primary care in the United States. Qual Saf
Health Care 2004;13:121–6.
5.
JCAHO Sentinel event alert advisory group. Sentinel
event alert.
http://www.jcaho.org/about+us/news+letters/sentinel+
event+alert/sea_26.htm. Jun 17, 2002.
12. Golodner L. How the public perceives patient safety.
Newsletter of the National Patient Safety Foundation
2004;1997:1–6.
6.
Edelman D. Outpatient diagnostic errors:
unrecognized hyperglycemia. Eff Clin Pract
2002;5:11–6.
13. Bhasale A, Miller G, Reid S, et al. Analyzing potential
harm in Australian general practice: an incidentmonitoring study. Med J Aust 1998;169:73–9.
275
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
14. Kravitz R, Rolph J, McGuigan K. Malpractice claims
data as a quality improvement tool, I: Epidemiology of
error in four specialties. JAMA 1991;266:2087–92.
31. Steere A, Taylor E, McHugh G, et al. The overdiagnosis of lyme disease. JAMA 1993;269:1812–6.
32. American Society for Gastrointestinal Endoscopy.
Medical malpractice claims and risk management in
gastroenterology and gastrointestinal endoscopy.
http://www.asge.org/gui/resources/manual/gea_risk_u
pdate_on_endoscopic.asp, Jan 8, 2002.
15. Flannery F. Utilizing malpractice claims data for
quality improvement. Legal Medicine 1992;92:1–3.
16. Neale G, Woloshynowych M, Vincent C. Exploring
the causes of adverse events in NHS hospital practice.
J Royal Soc Med 2001;94:322–30.
33. Zhan C, Miller MR. Administrative data based patient
safety research: a critical review. Qual Saf Health
Care 2003;12(Suppl 2):ii58–ii63.
17. Bogner MS, editor. Human error in medicine.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.; 1994.
18. Makeham M, Dovey S, County M, et al. An
international taxonomy for errors in general practice: a
pilot study. Med J Aust 2002;177:68–72.
34. Shojania K, Burton E, McDonald K, et al. Changes in
rates of autopsy-detected diagnostic errors over time: a
systematic review. JAMA 2003;289:2849–56.
35. Gruver R, Fries E. A study of diagnostic errors. Ann
Intern Med 1957;47:108–20.
19. Wilson R, Harrison B, Gibberd R, et al. An analysis of
the causes of adverse events from the quality in
Australian health care study. Med J Aust
1999;170:411–5.
36. Kirch W, Schafii C. Misdiagnosis at a university
hospital in 4 medical eras. Medicine 1996;75:29–40.
20. Weingart S, Ship A, Aronson M. Confidential
clinician-reported surveillance of adverse events
among medical inpatients. J Gen Intern Med
2000;15:470–7.
37. Thomas E, Petersen L. Measuring errors and adverse
events in health care. J Gen Intern Med 2003;18:61–7.
38. Orlander J, Barber T, Fincke B. The morbidity and
mortality conference: the delicate nature of learning
from error. Acad Med 2002;77:1001–6.
21. Chaudhry S, Olofinboda K, Krumholz H. Detection of
errors by attending physicians on a general medicine
service. J Gen Intern Med 2003;18:595–600.
39. Bagian J, Gosbee J, Lee C, et al. The Veterans Affairs
root cause analysis system in action. J Qual Improv
2002;28:531–45.
22. Kowalski R, Claassen J, Kreiter K, et al. Initial
misdiagnosis and outcome after subarachnoid
hemorrhage. JAMA 2004;291:866–9.
40. Spear S, Bowen H. Decoding the DNA of the Toyota
production system. Harvard Bus Rev 1999;106.
23. Mayer P, Awad I, Todor R, et al. Misdiagnosis of
symptomatic cerebral aneurysm. Prevalence and
correlation with outcome at four institutions. Stroke
1996;27:1558–63.
41. Anderson R, Hill R. The current status of the autopsy
in academic medical centers in the United States. Am
J Clin Pathol 1989;92:S31–S37.
24. Craven, ER. Risk management issues in glaucoma
diagnosis and treatment. Surv Opthalmol 1996 May–
Jun;40(6):459–62.
42. The Royal College of Pathologists of Australasia
Autopsy Working Party. The decline of the hospital
autopsy: a safety and quality issue for health care in
Australia. Med J Aust 2004;180:281–5.
25. Lederle F, Parenti C, Chute E. Ruptured abdominal
aortic aneurysm: the internist as diagnostician. Am J
Med 1994;96:163–7.
43. Plsek P. Section 1: evidence-based quality
improvement, principles, and perspectives. Pediatrics
1999;103:203–14.
26. Goodson W, Moore D. Causes of physician delay in
the diagnosis of breast cancer. Arch Intern Med
2002;162:1343–8.
44. Leape L. Error in medicine. JAMA 1994;272:1851–7.
45. Reason J. Human error. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press; 1990.
27. Clark S. Spinal infections go undetected. Lancet
1998;351:1108.
46. Gaither C. What your doctor doesn’t know could kill
you. The Boston Globe. Jul 14, 2002.
28. Williams VJ. Second-opinion consultations assist
community physicians in diagnosing brain and spinal
cord biopsies.
http://www3.mdanderson.org/~oncolog/brunder.html.
Oncol 1997. 11-29-0001.
47. Lambert B, Chang K, Lin S. Effect of orthographic
and phonological similarity on false recognition of
drug names. Soc Sci Med 2001;52:1843–57.
48. Woloshynowych M, Neale G, Vincent C. Care record
review of adverse events: a new approach. Qual Saf
Health Care 2003;12:411–5.
29. Arbiser Z, Folpe A, Weiss S. Consultative (expert)
second opinions in soft tissue pathology. Amer J Clin
Pathol 2001;116:473–6.
49. Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive errors in
diagnosis and strategies to minimize them. Acad Med
2003;78:775–80.
30. Pope J, Aufderheide T, Ruthazer R, et al. Missed
diagnoses of acute cardiac ischemia in the emergency
department. NEJM 2000;342:1163–70.
276
Diagnosing Diagnosis Errors
50. Pansini N, Di Serio F, Tampoia M. Total testing
process: appropriateness in laboratory medicine. Clin
Chim Acta 2003;333:141–5.
67. Cigarroa J, Isselbacher E, DeSanctis R, et al.
Diagnostic imaging in the evaluation of suspected
aortic dissection—old standards and new directions.
NEJM 1993;328:35–43.
51. Stroobants A, Goldschmidt H, Plebani M. Error
budget calculations in laboratory medicine: linking the
concepts of biological variation and allowable medical
errors. Clin Chim Acta 2003;333:169–76.
68. McCarthy B, Yood M, Boohaker E, et al. Inadequate
follow-up of abnormal mammograms. Am J Prev Med
1996;12:282–8.
52. Bates D. Medication errors. How common are they
and what can be done to prevent them? Drug Saf
1996;15:303–10.
69. McCarthy B, Yood M, Janz N, et al. Evaluation of
factors potentially associated with inadequate followup of mammographic abnormalities. Cancer
1996;77:2070–6.
53. Kassirer J, Kopelman R. Learning clinical research.
Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1991.
70. Boohaker E, Ward R, Uman J, et al. Patient
notification and follow-up of abnormal test results: a
physician survey. Arch Intern Med 1996;156:327–31.
54. Pisarska M, Carson S, Buster J. Ectopic pregnancy.
Lancet 1998;351:1115–20.
55. Soderstrom, RM. Obstetrics/gynecology: ectopic
pregnancy remains a malpractice dilemma.
http://www.thedoctors.com/risk/specialty/obgyn/J4209
.asp. 1999. Apr 26, 2004.
71. Murff H, Gandhi T, Karson A, et al. Primary care
physician attitudes concerning follow-up of abnormal
test results and ambulatory decision support systems.
Int J Med Inf 2003;71:137–49.
72. Iordache S, Orso D, Zelingher J. A comprehensive
computerized critical laboratory results alerting
system for ambulatory and hospitalized patients.
Medinfo 2001;10:469–73.
56. Tay J, Moore J, Walker J. Ectopic pregnancy. BMJ
2000;320:916–9.
57. Gracia C, Barnhart K. Diagnosing ectopic pregnancy:
decision analysis comparing six strategies. Obstet
Gynecol 2001;97:464–70.
58. Helliker K, Burton TM. Medical ignorance contributes
to toll from aortic illness. Wall Street Journal; Nov 4,
2003.
59. Hagan P, Nienaber C, Isselbacher E, et al. The
international registry of acute aortic dissection
(IRAD): new insights into an old disease. JAMA
2000;283:897–903.
73. Kuperman G, Boyle D, Jha A, et al. How promptly are
inpatients treated for critical laboratory results?
JAMIA 1998;5:112–9.
74. Schiff, G, Kim, S, Wisniewski, M, et al. Every system
is perfectly designed to: missed diagnosis of
hypothyroidism uncovered by linking lab and
pharmacy data. J Gen Intern Med 2003;18(suppl
1):295.
60. Kodolitsch Y, Schwartz A, Nienaber C. Clinical
prediction of acute aortic dissection. Arch Intern Med
2000;160:2977–82.
75. Kuperman G, Teich J, Tanasijevic M, et al. Improving
response to critical laboratory results with automation:
results of a randomized controlled trial. JAMIA
1999;6:512–22.
61. Spittell P, Spittell J, Joyce J, et al. Clinical features
and differential diagnosis of aortic dissection:
experience with 236 cases (1980 through 1990). Mayo
Clin Proc 1993;68:642–51.
76. Poon E, Kuperman G, Fiskio J, et al. Real-time
notification of laboratory data requested by users
through alphanumeric pagers. J Am Med Inform
Assoc 2002;9:217–22.
62. Loubeyre P, Angelie E, Grozel F, et al. Spiral CT
artifact that simulates aortic dissection: image
reconstruction with use of 180 degrees and 360
degrees linear-interpolation algorithms. Radiology
1997;205:153–7.
77. Schiff GD, Klass D, Peterson J, et al. Linking
laboratory and pharmacy: opportunities for reducing
errors and improving care. Arch Intern Med
2003;163:893–900.
78. Massachusetts Coalition for the Prevention of Medical
Error. Communicating critical test results safe
practice. JCAHO J Qual Saf 2005;31(2) [In press]
63. Cabot R. Diagnostic pitfalls identified during a study
of 3000 autopsies. JAMA 1912;59:2295–8.
79. Berwick D. Eleven worthy aims for clinical leadership
of health system reform. JAMA 1994;272:797–802.
64. Conveney E, Geraghty J, O’Laoide R, et al. Reasons
underlying negative mammography in patients with
palpable breast cancer. Clin Radiol 1994;49:123–5.
80. Dawson N, Arkes H. Systematic errors in medical
decision making: judgment limitations. J Gen Intern
Med 1987;2:183–7.
65. Burstein, HJ. Highlights of the 2nd European breast
cancer conference.
http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/408459. 2000.
81. Bartlett E. Physicians’ cognitive errors and their
liability consequences. J Health Care Risk Manag
1998;18:62–9.
66. Berlin L. Malpractice issues in radiology: defending
the “missed” radiographic diagnosis. Amer J Roent
2001;176:317–22.
277
Advances in Patient Safety: Vol. 2
82. McDonald J. Computer reminders, the quality of care
and the nonperfectability of man. NEJM
1976;295:1351–5.
84. Fischhoff B. Hindsight [does not equal] foresight: the
effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under
uncertainty. J Exp Psychol: Hum Perc Perform
1975;1:288–99.
83. Smetzer J, Cohen M. Lesson from the Denver
medication error/criminal negligence case: look
beyond blaming individuals. Hosp Pharm
1998;33:640–57.
85. Hart JT. Cochrane Lecture 1997. What evidence do
we need for evidence based medicine? J Epidemiol
Community Health 1997 Dec;51(6):623–9.
278
WHAT ARE THE PRIMARY & SECONDARY
OUTCOMES OF DIAGNOSTIC PROCESS - THE
LEADING PHASE OF WORK IN PATIENT CARE
MANAGEMENT PROBLEMS IN EMERGENCY
DEPARTMENT ?
Cosby KS, Roberts R, Palivos L et al.
Characteristics of Patient Care Management Problems Identified in Emergency
Department Morbidity and Mortality Investigations During 15 Years.
Ann Emerg Med. 2008;51:251-261
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
CLINICAL PRACTICE AND PATIENT SAFETY/ORIGINAL RESEARCH
Characteristics of Patient Care Management Problems Identified
in Emergency Department Morbidity and Mortality Investigations
During 15 Years
Karen S. Cosby, MD
Rebecca Roberts, MD
Lisa Palivos, MD
Christopher Ross, MD
Jeffrey Schaider, MD
Scott Sherman, MD
Isam Nasr, MD
Eileen Couture, DO
Moses Lee, MD
Shari Schabowski, MD
Ibrar Ahmad, BS
R. Douglas Scott II, PhD
From the Department of Emergency Medicine, Cook County Hospital, Rush Medical School,
Chicago, IL.
Study objective: We describe cases referred for physician review because of concern about quality of
patient care and identify factors that contributed to patient care management problems.
Methods: We performed a retrospective review of 636 cases investigated by an emergency
department physician review committee at an urban public teaching hospital over a 15-year period. At
referral, cases were initially investigated and analyzed, and specific patient care management
problems were noted. Two independent physicians subsequently classified problems into 1 or more
of 4 major categories according to the phase of work in which each occurred (diagnosis, treatment,
disposition, and public health) and identified contributing factors that likely affected outcome (patient
factors, triage, clinical tasks, teamwork, and system). Primary outcome measures were death and
disability. Secondary outcome measures included specific life-threatening events and adverse events.
Patient outcomes were compared with the expected outcome with ideal care and the likely outcome
of no care.
Results: Physician reviewers identified multiple problems and contributing factors in the majority of
cases (92%). The diagnostic process was the leading phase of work in which problems were
observed (71%). Three leading contributing factors were identified: clinical tasks (99%), patient
factors (61%), and teamwork (61%). Despite imperfections in care, half of all patients received some
benefit from their medical care compared with the likely outcome with no care.
Conclusion: These reviews suggest that physicians would be especially interested in strategies to
improve the diagnostic process and clinical tasks, address patient factors, and develop more
effective medical teams. Our investigation allowed us to demonstrate the practical application of a
framework for case analysis. We discuss the limitations of retrospective cases analyses and
recommend future directions in safety research. [Ann Emerg Med. 2008;51:251-261.]
0196-0644/$-see front matter
Copyright © 2008 by the American College of Emergency Physicians.
doi:10.1016/j.annemergmed.2007.06.483
Volume , .  : March 
Annals of Emergency Medicine 251
Patient Care Management Problems
Editor’s Capsule Summary
What is already known on this topic
Morbidity and mortality conferences and other forms of
quality review are commonplace. Their content has not
been well studied.
What question this study addressed
What types of events and what causal and contributing
factors are common in morbidity and mortality reviews?
What this study adds to our knowledge
The diagnostic process was judged the most common
locus of failure in more than 600 cases, spanning 15
years. Despite imperfections in care, more than half the
patients still received some benefit compared with the
likely outcome with no care at all.
How this might change clinical practice
Detailed case reviews provide useful information for
practice improvement but have selection bias in the
choice of cases and biases produced by the retrospective
interpretation of incomplete information.
SEE EDITORIAL, P. 262.
INTRODUCTION
Background
Several major studies have reported the incidence of medical
error and risks associated with health care.1-6 These studies have
raised awareness of imperfections in care but offer little
understanding of the nature of the work or solutions to improve
safety. Although the focus on patient safety is relatively new,
processes for improvement have existed for decades. We suggest
that cumulative reviews of existing data, available in most health
care organizations, can be used to guide current efforts to
improve safety.
Our study was initially aimed at detecting “medical errors.”
Although the concept of error has been widely disseminated in
the medical literature during the last decade, the term “error”
carries with it connotations of carelessness, implies blame, and
often focuses unduly on humans as the source of harm. As our
understanding of these problems grew, we modified our study
to describe events once labeled errors as “patient care
management problems.” This subtle change is intended to
promote a healthier perspective and constructive analysis. The
term “problem” implies something to be solved and states more
directly our intent to seek strategies and solutions for
improvement.
Importance
Demand for improvement has led to changes throughout
health care, including new safety regulations for health care
organizations, proposed legislation for error reporting and
252 Annals of Emergency Medicine
Cosby et al
malpractice reform, and advancements in information
technology applications for medical settings.7-10 Reforms in
medical education have led to restrictions in house staff working
hours, development of curriculums for patient safety, increased
rigor in requirements for lifelong learning for physicians, and
new emphasis on competency standards.11,12 Although we are
anxious to improve, recommended changes should be founded
on a thorough understanding of the types of patient care
management problems that occur in medicine and factors that
may increase risk.
Goals of This Investigation
The objective of this study was to characterize the types of
cases referred to a physician review committee of an urban
emergency department (ED) and identify the phase of work in
which problems were detected and specific factors that affected
quality of patient care. Our long-term goal is to use our existing
morbidity and mortality investigations to guide safety
interventions and to develop valid methods for future study of
patient safety targets. The ED has been described as a high-risk
practice environment, particularly under conditions of
crowding, and serves as a natural setting to study safety in health
care.13,14
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study Design and Setting
This was a retrospective study to characterize patient care
management problems identified by routine mortality and
morbidity surveillance at an urban public teaching hospital
during the 15-year period spanning 1989 through 2003. The
annual adult ED census during those years ranged from
109,000 to 128,000. All cases referred for emergency physician
review between 1989 and 2003 were eligible. Our institution
has separate reporting mechanisms for pediatric and trauma
care; these populations are not included in our study. Case
referrals came from ED staff, admitting services, consultants, or
quality assurance managers. Two independent emergency
physician reviewers assessed each case by using an existing
framework described previously.15,16 The lead author designed
the data collection form and performed review 1 in every case.
Cases were randomly assigned to one of 8 coinvestigators for
review 2. Whenever possible, reviewers were disqualified from
cases in which they had direct involvement or firsthand
knowledge. The study was exempted from review by our
institutional review board.
Our department actively sought referrals from hospital staff
of cases in which there were concerns about quality of care and
encouraged reports of administrative and system problems, as
well as questions about medical management. This process
began as routine surveillance for morbidity and mortality cases
but became a significant part of the quality process in our
department. Cases underwent an investigation at referral that
included interviews with medical staff involved in each case and
a review of medical records and clinical data. Medical staff were
invited to record details of their cases to explain circumstances
Volume , .  : March 
Cosby et al
as they existed at the time. Unlike many retrospective medical
record reviews, these investigations yielded thick files rich in
context and content. Each case was typically presented for
discussion at 1 or more forums, including physician review
committees, morbidity and mortality conferences, and faculty
meetings. Once investigations were complete, the original
medical records, notes from interviews and committee
discussions, formal recommendations, and final case summaries
were placed in an investigations library. Throughout the 15-year
period of case collection summarized in this study, a common
philosophy guided case review: cases were scrutinized with the
intent to detect all patient care management problems, their
potential causes, and contributing factors, with the ultimate goal
to find solutions and seek improvement. These records
constitute the source data reviewed for this study.
Data Collection
At the start of this study, a research assistant deidentified the
source data from the investigations library. Study investigators
reviewed the case files, identified management problems, and
then classified the problems by the phase of work in which they
occurred, their contributing factors, and patient outcome.
Investigators were allowed to use summary statements and
conclusions from the initial reviews but were also encouraged to
record any other relevant factors they identified during their
reviews of file documents. To ensure internal validity
throughout the study, specific criteria were established for each
data field before data collection. Reviewers were asked to
consider each study category. If they selected a category, they
were required to check at least 1 of the category criteria to
justify their selection. Reviewers participated in educational
sessions and a pilot study, with feedback to verify consistent
application of study definitions.
The framework that guided this investigation applies a
variety of approaches to categorizing problems in care.15 We
determine the stage at which a problem occurred and what
factors likely contributed. Identified problems were classified
into 1 or more broad categories according to the phase of work
in which they occurred: diagnostic, treatment, disposition, and
public health (Table 1). Reviewers were then asked to determine
whether any of the following contributing factors added risk,
contributed to a problem in care, or had a negative impact on
patient outcome: patient factors, triage, clinical factors,
teamwork, and system. Clinical factors included medical
reasoning, specific interpretive and procedural skill sets, taskbased problems, and affective influences. The concept of taskbased problems has been described previously: they involve
specific tasks that are so routine and basic to care that they are
relegated to lower-order thinking, that is, they are done as a
matter of habit and typically with little conscious thought.15,17
Problems in these basic tasks are observed as a failed human
behavior but likely reflect a system weakness. Such events tend
to occur when the system is overloaded, workers are distracted,
or altered staffing patterns affect work flow. They can be used as
a marker of system overload or disorganization. The category of
Volume , .  : March 
Patient Care Management Problems
affective influences includes the human tendency to be
influenced by factors other than objective facts; it includes the
sometimes unconscious effects of personality, external pressures,
and conflict on reasoning.18 Individual assessments were made
for each team of clinicians. System problems included any
process, service, equipment, or supplies that failed or were
unavailable, broken, or faulty. System problems were further
classified by location: ED microsystem, hospital-wide failure,
administrative factors, and community resources. Specific
problems were also characterized according to Reason’s17
scheme as problems in “planning,” “execution,” or both. This
classification distinguishes problems that are primarily cognitive
from those that are primarily related to process failure; this
distinction offers additional insight into potential solutions.
None of these descriptors or classifications was mutually
exclusive. Judgments about care were based on whether actions
and decisions were accurate, timely, and effective. An example
of a training case analysis is demonstrated in Figure 1.
Outcome Measures
Primary patient outcome was categorized as death,
permanent disability, long-term disability (between 30 days and
12 months), short-term disability (up to 30 days), or no
disability. These outcomes were mutually exclusive. Final
outcomes, however, do not always reflect significant events.
Thus, we added 2 additional secondary outcome measures to
capture events that deserve attention: acute but short-lived lifethreatening events and adverse events (harm caused by medical
management itself, independent of the patient’s disease). These
secondary outcome measures were not mutually exclusive and
could also overlap with the primary outcomes. Finally, each
reviewer was asked to compare the actual patient outcome for
each patient in this series to the outcome expected for optimal
care and the likely outcome of no care to determine to what
extent our management met expectations or caused harm.
Primary Data Analysis
The results for both reviewers were entered in a SAS data set.
To increase data entry accuracy, 2 personnel collaborated: one
to read aloud results from the data collection forms and validate
the keystroke entry and the other to perform keystrokes.
Observer agreement between reviewers 1 and 2 was quantified
using the ␬ statistic. For rates and proportions, 95% confidence
intervals were calculated. All analyses were performed with SAS
version 7 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
RESULTS
Of the original library of 673 cases, 37 were excluded from
the study because of incomplete or illegible records, inadequate
30-day follow-up, incomplete investigations, or inconclusive
summaries (4 unexplained deaths). The final study group of 636
patients includes 353 male patients, 276 female patients, and 7
with undesignated sex. (Seven cases had sex designation
inadvertently removed from the original source data.)
Annals of Emergency Medicine 253
Patient Care Management Problems
Cosby et al
Table 1. Study definitions.
Term
Definition
Example
Phases of work in which problems were observed
Diagnosis
Failure to make a correct diagnosis or
A delay in diagnosis that affects outcome or
Treatment
Disposition
Public health
Reason’s classification
Problem with planning
Problem with execution
Contributing factors
Patient factors
An unnecessary or excessive diagnostic evaluation that itself
caused harm
Failure to provide appropriate treatment (error of omission) or
An inappropriate treatment (error of commission)
Failure to provide appropriate level of care according to acuity
or
Inadequate plan for follow-up care
Care that poses no specific threat to the patient but places
others at risk
A poor plan at the outset
Failure to carry out the plan as intended
A specific patient characteristic that poses risk
Inability to communicate
Specific anatomy poses risk
Clinical factors/tasks
Medical reasoning
Specific skill set
Task based
Affective influences
Teamwork factors
System factors
Characteristic elicits destructive affective bias in care
provider
Failure to apply triage protocol or failure of criteria to
accurately assess severity of illness. May be cognitive
(knowledge deficit), design (inadequate criteria), or
situational (excessive demand)
Failure in a specific clinical task
Incorrect clinical impression or a flawed decision
Inaccurate interpretation of clinical data, such as ECGs,
laboratory data, imaging studies; or
Complication from invasive procedures
Failure in routine bedside tasks such as measurement of
vital signs
Personal bias, evidence of interpersonal conflict, or the
influence of factors other than objective facts in clinical
decisions
Miscommunication, poor team transition, authority gradients,
or failure to supervise
Failure due to inadequate supplies, equipment, services, or
policies
ED microsystem
Hospital-wide system
Administration
Community
There was significant overlap between the main categories of
work in which problems were noted, with diagnosis being the
most commonly identified classification (451; 71%), followed
by disposition (280; 44%) and then treatment (265; 42%).
Public health decisions comprised the smallest fraction of cases
254 Annals of Emergency Medicine
Failure to give antibiotics for meningitis
Treating a viral infection with antibiotics
Patient moved to higher level of care after admission;
deterioration in condition not anticipated
Neck mass not referred for biopsy; inadequate followup plan leads to delay in care and poor prognosis
Failing to isolate tuberculosis
Failure of contact tracing for meningitis
Failing to recognize risk to relatives of victims of
carbon monoxide exposure
Failing to recognize and thus treat a condition
Intending to deliver a treatment but mistakenly
delivering the wrong drug
Inability to cooperate
Triage factors
Failure to diagnose pneumonia
Delay in recognizing a cord compression leads to
paralysis
Contrast used in unnecessary test causes renal failure
Unconscious or altered mental status
Language barrier
Delirium, dementia, or under the influence of
substances
Anatomically difficult airway, morbid obesity, difficult
venous access
Borderline personality disorder alienates medical staff
Failure to detect acute coronary syndrome
Failing to recognize a clinical pattern of illness
Missing a relevant radiographic abnormality
Pneumothorax from central line placement
Failing to detect a change in vital signs when staffing
ratio exceeds established margins of safety
Interpersonal conflict interferes with clinical judgment
Confusion about treatment given in ED leads to failure
to give medication
Airway supplies not restocked
Hospital call system not updated
Paging system down
Consultants not available
Community dialysis center closes, limiting access for
care
(23; 4%). Multiple categories were selected in more than half
the cases (322; 51%) (Figure 2).
Problems were most likely to occur in the planning stage
(591; 93%) compared with the execution stage (170; 27%),
although some events had problems in both (144, 23%).
Volume , .  : March 
Cosby et al
Patient Care Management Problems
TRAINING CASE EXAMPLE DEMONSTRATING THE FRAMEWORK FOR
ANALYSIS
Case Summary
A prominent business man with crushing chest pain and severe hypertension arrives in a
remote rural community ED that lacks an interventional cardiologist. By triage policy
he is rapidly moved to an acute care area where an EKG is promptly interpreted as
evidence of an acute myocardial infarction. A chest x-ray is viewed by the treating
physician, but the radiologist is unavailable to review it. The nearest hospital with
cardiology support is more than an hour away. A decision is made to offer the patient
thrombolytic therapy. Consent is obtained and the medication delivered. The patient
later dies of an aortic dissection.
After interviews with medical staff and further investigation, additional details were
obtained and the case analyzed.
The critique of this case concluded that failures occurred in two main categories of work:
1. Diagnosis (failure to diagnose aortic dissection), and
2. Treatment (thrombolytic therapy given in the face of severe, uncontrolled
hypertension).
Contributing factors included:
1. Flawed medical reasoning (uncontrolled hypertension not treated;
thrombolytics given despite uncontrolled blood pressure).
2. Specific skill-set failure (failure to recognize radiographic evidence for
dissection, findings later verified by expert review).
3. Ineffective team communication (the medical team did not communicate to
the radiologist the urgent need for his assistance).
4. Task-based failure (the radiologist noted that he was distracted by competing
demands).
5. Adverse affective influences (the physician later noted that he was
influenced by his desire to act aggressively because he recognized the patient to
be an influential community leader, as well as his desire to conform to local
standards to decrease “door-to-needle time.”)
A limited group discussion centered around the question of whether or not the
community should support local chest pain centers and improve resources for similar
cases, but ultimately no system factors were identified.
Figure 1. Training case example demonstrating the framework for case analysis.
Problems in specific clinical tasks were the most common
contributing factor identified (632; 99%) (Table 2). Problems
in clinical tasks most commonly involved medical reasoning
(595; 94%) but also included specific skill sets (212; 33%),
routine task-based problems (173; 27%), and destructive
affective influences (38; 6%). In almost half of all cases, more
than 1 group of clinicians participated in specific management
problems (289; 45%). Thus, many clinical problems did not
occur in isolation and were often not the result of a single
person, team, or process. Other leading factors that affected
outcome included patient factors (391; 61%) and teamwork
(387; 61%). System and triage factors were selected to a lesser
Volume , .  : March 
extent (41% and 16%, respectively). The distribution of
contributing factors was similar across all phases of work.
More than 1 contributing factor was identified for most
cases. In fact, 2 or more factors were identified in all but 56
cases (92%); 420 cases (66%) had 3 or more factors identified;
113 cases (18%) had 5 or more factors identified. Specific
problems in care were typically not the result of failure by 1
factor (person or process) alone but rather the result of the
complex interplay of multiple factors that ultimately influenced
clinical work.
Two thirds (66%) of patients in this series had death, major
permanent disability, or disability exceeding 30 days; 34%
Annals of Emergency Medicine 255
Patient Care Management Problems
Figure 2. Major categories of work in which problems were
observed. This Venn diagram illustrates the number of
events identified in each phase of work and their overlap.
Numbers in parentheses indicate total numbers for each
major heading. Public health decisions, not shown in this
diagram, accounted for an additional 23 events (4% of all
events).
returned to their baseline state of health within 30 days or had
no disability (Table 2).
Acute life-threatening events occurred sometime during the
ED or hospital course in 66 cases (10%), including cardiac
arrest, airway emergency, respiratory failure, shock, anaphylaxis,
or major dysrhythmia. Adverse events (harm from medical
management itself) occurred in 86 cases (14%). Of all adverse
events, 24 (28%) resulted in death and 23 (27%) were
associated with an acute life-threatening event.
Despite these identified events, 50% of all patients in this
series received net benefit from their care; 40% had an outcome
similar to that expected for no care. The remaining 11% had an
outcome that was worse than expected according to the natural
history of their disease. The majority of cases in this data set
failed to achieve the full benefit expected from optimal care
(590; 93%).
Observer agreement was measured with the ␬ statistic.
Observer agreement was highest in categories with well-defined,
objective criteria such as phases of work (86% to 100%
agreement; ␬ 0.71 to 1.00) and patient outcomes (100%
agreement; ␬ 0.98 to 1.00). When reviewers were asked to assess
more subjective factors such as destructive affective influences,
the observer agreement was only fair (92% agreement; ␬ 0.34)
(Table 3).
LIMITATIONS
This study is a retrospective review of investigations
completed on cases referred because of poor outcome, flawed
processes, or the perception of imperfections in care. There are
several limitations inherent to the study design.
256 Annals of Emergency Medicine
Cosby et al
First, the study has selection bias. Because our cases came
largely from physician referrals, our data are weighted toward
the types of problems that physicians find interesting or
important and do not necessarily reflect a broad sampling of all
types of cases. This does not mean that they represent the most
common or even the “worst” management problems; our cases
probably represent the most visible events or those most likely
to have immediate impact on patient outcome. There is no
control group of randomly selected patients during the same
study period. Thus, many of the same factors identified in the
study sample may have been present in other cases without
harm. Cases with significant problems or harm may have
occurred during the same period but gone unreported.
Second, post hoc critiques of care are inevitably subject to
bias. Our cases were typically referred after outcome was known;
thus, they have outcome bias, the tendency to judge similar
events more harshly when the outcome is poor.19 In addition,
our investigations were challenged by our ability to judge the
events as they actually occurred, not as we might imagine them.
Hindsight bias, the reconstruction of events in a way that makes
sense to the investigator, can mislead judgments about events.20
Even clinicians present at the time, in an effort to explain their
actions, may reconstruct memories that are only partially true.
We attempted to minimize the influence of bias by encouraging
staff to record their impressions and recall specific information
they had or lacked at the time of their decisions, what factors
influenced their decisions, and what the ambient conditions
were in the ED at the time. Timelines of actions were generated
from electronic records and added to information from
interviews to reconstruct, as much as possible, the actual events.
The thick descriptions in our study files permit detailed analysis
aimed at providing contextually rich detail to explain the
circumstances surrounding care.
There are several other limitations to our study. The type of
factors identified is constrained by the expertise of the reviewers
and the quality of information they preserved. Although we
observe the presence of factors that may have contributed to
outcome, we do not make conclusions of causality.
Although our study attempted to define discrete phases of
work in which problems occurred, we found overlap between
the major categories, which suggests that our classifications (and
perhaps the events themselves) were not independent of one
another; clinical decisions in one phase of work likely affect
other phases of work. Failure to discriminate between these
categories also reflects several natural limitations in quality
reviews. First, it is difficult to separate a single flawed step in the
continuum of care. Second, a critical appraisal of many cases,
with good or bad outcome, can often find multiple flaws. Last,
such distinctions require judgments about what was in the mind
of the clinician, facts that may be difficult for the involved
clinicians to recall accurately and even more difficult for an
independent reviewer remote from the event to determine.
The language of safety science and models of causation are
controversial, particularly when applied to health care.21 Terms
Volume , .  : March 
Cosby et al
Patient Care Management Problems
Table 2a. Association between contributing factors and primary outcome measures.
Primary Outcome Measures, No. (%)
Disability
Contributing Factors*
All Cases
nⴝ636
Death
nⴝ226
Permanent,
nⴝ24
Long Term,
nⴝ171
Short Term,
nⴝ178
None,
nⴝ37
Patient
Triage
Clinical (all subsets)
Reasoning
Skill set
Task based
Affective influences
Teamwork
System
391 (61)
103 (16)
632 (99)
595 (94)
212 (33)
173 (27)
38 (6)
387 (61)
261 (41)
170 (75)
47 (21)
225 (99)
212 (94)
77 (34)
91 (40)
11 (5)
155 (66)
103 (46)
16 (67)
2 (8)
23 (96)
21 (88)
12 (50)
7 (29)
2 (8)
13 (54)
9 (38)
86 (50)
20 (12)
171 (100)
164 (96)
47 (27)
29 (17)
9 (5)
92 (54)
79 (46)
102 (57)
31 (17)
177 (99)
169 (95)
58 (33)
37 (21)
14 (8)
100 (56)
60 (34)
17 (46)
3 (8)
36 (97)
29 (78)
18 (49)
9 (24)
2 (5)
27 (73)
10 (27)
*Contributing factors are not mutually exclusive; thus, percentages do not sum to 100.
Table 2b. Association between contributing factors and secondary outcome measures.
Secondary Outcome Measures, No. (%)*
Contributing Factors†
All Cases, No. (%),
nⴝ636
Life Threats,
nⴝ66
Adverse Events,
nⴝ86
Patient
Triage
Clinical (all subsets)
Reasoning
Skill set
Task based
Affective influences
Teamwork
System
391 (61)
103 (16)
632 (99)
595 (94)
212 (33)
173 (27)
38 (6)
387 (61)
261 (41)
47 (71)
11 (17)
66 (100)
60 (91)
28 (42)
23 (35)
7 (11)
40 (61)
22 (33)
59 (69)
10 (12)
84 (98)
69 (80)
49 (57)
30 (35)
5 (6)
50 (58)
30 (35)
*Secondary outcomes note specific events that are a subset of all cases; thus, percentages do not sum to 100.
†
Contributing factors are not mutually exclusive; thus, percentages do not sum to 100.
such as “error” tend to evoke accusations of blame, whereas
terms such as “problems” or “failures” may be vague. Judgments
about quality and causality are flawed by outcome knowledge.
From a simple human perspective, we want patients to have the
best possible outcome. When they do not, there is a natural
tendency to cite any imperfection in their care as a contributing
(if not causal) factor. No doubt, similar problems in the care of
patients with satisfactory results are more easily forgiven and
dismissed. This study is ultimately a summary of the aspects of
clinical work that our reviewers found to be most prone to
problems and thus most likely to contribute to risk. The
development of safety systems in other fields has started by
analysis of accidents or events and then progressed to more
sophisticated, systematic, and prospective design. Studies such
as ours provide a framework from which to begin.
DISCUSSION
Our data demonstrate that cases referred because of patient
care management problems are often complex and not the result
of the failure of any single individual or process at any one
moment in time. The typical course of a “medical error” has
been described as a cascade, in which one failure leads to
Volume , .  : March 
another.22 However, our study went beyond simply recognizing
complexity to identifying specific factors that can be targeted to
improve safety.
Our reviewers selected “diagnosis” as the leading phase of
work in which problems were noticed, consistent with results
from other major studies,2,4,23-25 which is not unexpected,
because diagnosis is at the heart of clinical work and is the
foundation on which all other actions are predicated. That
judgment is likely influenced by the perspective of our
reviewers, who may be inclined to focus on their own expertise
and reluctant to judge factors outside their usual sphere of
influence. Our results provide some clues about why the
diagnostic process is difficult and at times imprecise. Two major
contributing factors were consistently identified in most cases in
our study and likely created an environment in which rapid
decisions may be problematic: patient factors and teamwork.
Patient factors include conditions that impair the ability of
patients to communicate or cooperate with medical staff,
anatomic problems that add difficulty to the ability to provide
care, and conditions that elicit affective bias in care providers.
Patients who present to the ED with altered mental status or in
distress may be unable to offer important details of their medical
Annals of Emergency Medicine 257
Patient Care Management Problems
Cosby et al
Table 3. Summary of descriptive study statistics and observer agreement.*
Variable
Number
of Cases
Phases of work in which problems were observed
Diagnosis
451
Treatment
265
Disposition
280
Public health
23
Reason’s classification
Planning
591
Execution
170
Planning and execution
144
Contributing factors
Patient
391
Triage
103
Clinical (all subsets)
632
Reasoning
595
Specific skill set
212
Task based
173
Affective influences
38
Teamwork
387
System
261
Primary outcomes
Death
226
Permanent disability
24
Long-term disability
171
Short-term disability
178
No disability
37
Secondary outcomes
Life-threatening events
66
Adverse events
86
Observer Agreement
Total, %
(95% CI)
Agreement, %
␬ (95% CI)
71 (67–75)
42 (38–46)
44 (40–48)
4 (2–6)
98
93
86
100
0.95 (0.93–0.98)
0.85 (0.81–0.89)
0.71 (0.66–0.77)
1.00 (1.00–1.00)
93 (96–98)
27 (24–30)
23 (20–26)
92
79
79
0.43 (0.30–0.56)
0.50 (0.43–0.58)
0.47 (0.39–0.54)
61 (57–65)
16 (13–19)
99 (98–100)
94 (92–96)
33 (29–37)
27 (24–30)
6 (4–8)
61 (57–65)
41 (37–45)
77
93
99
94
90
82
92
72
83
0.57 (0.50–0.63)
0.74 (0.67–0.81)
0.54 (0.19–0.90)
0.45 (0.31–0.60)
0.77 (0.73–0.83)
0.47 (0.39–0.55)
0.34 (0.20–0.48)
0.41 (0.34–0.48)
0.49 (0.38–0.55)
36 (32–40)
4 (2–5)
27 (23–30)
28 (24–31)
6 (4–8)
100
100
100
100
100
0.99 (0.98–1.10)
1.00 (1.00–1.00)
0.99 (0.98–1.00)
0.99 (0.98–1.00)
0.98 (0.96–1.01)
10 (8–12)
14 (11–17)
100
100
0.99 (0.98–1.01)
0.99 (0.98–1.01)
CI, Confidence interval.
*Categories are not mutually exclusive; thus, percentages do not sum to 100.
history, which contributes to the “information gap” described
by Stiell et al,26 in which physicians must act despite a paucity
of reliable facts. Some of these factors are inherent to the
practice of emergency medicine and contribute substantially to
risk.
Other acute care specialties have recognized the importance
of teamwork; our study confirmed that problems in care often
involve ineffective team functioning.27,28 ED care tends to be
punctuated by numerous transitions that can lead to dropped
tasks and lost information. In addition, handoffs in care may
hamper the ability of clinicians to recognize dynamic changes in
a patient’s condition.29
There are implications from this study that can be used to
design safer care. First, clinicians need reliable access to
information and strategies for making decisions in the face of
uncertainty. Cognitive psychologists can offer specific training
in cognition, critical thinking, and decisionmaking.30-38 Clinical
decision support systems and cognitive aids can reduce cognitive
load and standardize clinical protocols.39,40 Medical simulation
can provide opportunities to practice and refine performance.41
Second, our study reveals that patient factors contribute
substantially to management problems. Vincent et al16,42 have
observed that patient factors may contribute to the likelihood of
258 Annals of Emergency Medicine
an adverse event; their framework for analyzing clinical events
includes consideration of patient factors. Patient factors are
often beyond our immediate control; harm caused by some
patient factors has even been labeled “no fault.”38,43 Although a
no-fault judgment may be fair to those present at the time, it
prematurely limits discussions about how to improve care. We
argue that all information from investigations can be used to
improve care, even if problems are judged to be unavoidable at
the time. Creative use of resources can address common patient
factors, such as providing 24-hour access to interpreter services,
improving electronic medical record databases for access to
reliable medical information, and using ultrasonographic
guidance to improve our ability to care for patients with
difficult venous access, to name just a few.
Third, organization and teamwork in acute care medicine
present specific challenges. Reliable and consistent processes for
transitions and exchange of patient care responsibility and
accurate communication of test results and patient information
are fundamental to the design of safe health care systems,
particularly in the dynamic setting of the ED.
The fact that system factors were not identified as a leading
contributor to problems identified in this study contrasts
sharply with current views in safety science.8,44 Our study was
Volume , .  : March 
Cosby et al
performed by physicians, not safety scientists; thus, their reviews
focus on medical judgments, not system design. Human tasks
and the systems that support them are inextricably linked. The
fact that our reviewers consistently selected human aspects of
tasks over system factors may be due to fundamental attribution
error, the tendency for organizational actors to attribute
undesirable outcomes to the perceived character flaws of people
rather than to the processes engendered by the organization’s
structure.45 This tendency is reinforced by cultural pressures
that promote an overriding sense of professional accountability
but create “system blindness,” that is, clinicians are largely
unaware of the potential for system design to improve care.46
The failure to detect a similar proportion of human and system
factors may also reflect an imbalance in health care design that
encourages reliance on individuals over development of support
systems. If so, the recent focus on design and development of
safe systems to support clinical work offers significant promise
for improvement.
Future safety research will likely focus on prospective error
reporting systems. The development of a national error
reporting mechanism in the United States is under way but
faces challenges to define process: how to collect case
information, what information to collect, and how to analyze
and then disseminate useful results. Once in place, reporting
systems are often slow to produce actionable items. Meanwhile,
studies such as ours can use historical cases to identify major
problems needing correction and identify the types of factors
that are worth tracking prospectively.
In the course of this study, we learned several lessons that can
affect the success of future error reporting systems. During the
early phases of the study, we attempted to identify a variety of
potential contributing factors based on safety research, including
cognitive bias, human factors, ergonomics, and ED design. Our
efforts failed, primarily because physician reviewers were not
familiar with the concepts and routine clinical investigations do
not typically address them. We recommend that future studies
and error reporting systems engage nonclinician experts from
safety disciplines to provide fresh insight and perspective.
We also found that observer agreement was best for
categories that were described by detailed checklists and
definitions. Reviewers were less likely to agree when asked to
make subjective assessments. Large-scale reporting systems need
a workable reporting mechanism and may struggle between the
need for uniformity and objective definitions and the desire for
free-text reports. An ideal system may need both and may need
to be modified periodically to allow for evolving standards of
care and improved understanding of causes of medical failure.
For future work, there is a variety of sources of data that can
be used to probe problems in patient care. Dramatic, visible,
and highly publicized accounts of individual cases have been
used successfully to drive safety intitiatives.47 In contrast,
aggregate data from existing databases of cases may provide a
broad overview of risk and help define particular tasks and
moments in patient care that are most vulnerable and most in
Volume , .  : March 
Patient Care Management Problems
need of system support. Several studies have used closed
malpractice claims and risk management files to identify causes
and contributing factors to adverse events.48,49 We suggest that
collective reviews of existing mortality and morbidity
investigations and internal review processes, available in most
health care organizations, offer an additional source of data to
guide improvement (Appendix E1, available online at www.
annemergmed.com). Finally, we suggest that collaboration with
safety experts in other disciplines can expand our perspective on
safety concepts and the potential for system improvements in
health care.
In summary, the primary goal for understanding patient
care management problems is to prevent them or at least
mitigate harm. However, our efforts to improve care have
been frustrated by the realization that health care and patient
care processes are complex.5,16,36-38 Our data confirm that
most problems are multifactorial. The leading factors that
contributed to problems detected by physician reviewers in
this study include medical reasoning, patient factors, and
teamwork, although these problems occurred in a
background of system imperfections. Efforts to improve
safety in medicine should be directed at improving access to
accurate medical information and communication across the
continuum of medical care; coordination of care among
health care teams; and improved processes, education, and
support for decisionmaking in the uncertain environment of
acute care medicine. Advances in system design to support
these aspects of clinical care offer opportunities for
improvement in health care safety.
We wish to thank Mr. Geoffrey Andrade for assistance with data
entry and Mr. Donald Bockenfeld, BS, for assistance with article
preparation.
Supervising editor: Robert L. Wears, MD, MS
Author contributions: KSC and RR conceived the study and
obtained funding. KSC, RR, JS, and RDS designed the study.
KSC, LP, CR, JS, S Sherman, IN, EC, ML, and S Schabowski
were responsible for data acquisition. KSC, RR, IA, and RDS
analyzed and interpreted the data. KSC and RR drafted the
article and all authors contributed substantially to its revision.
KSC, RR, IA, and RDS were responsible for statistical
analysis. RR, LP, CR, JS, S Sherman, IN, EC, ML, S
Schabowski, IA, and RDS provided administrative, technical,
or material support. KSC and RR were responsible for overall
study supervision. KSC and RR had full access to all the data
and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the
accuracy of the data analysis. KSC takes responsibility for the
paper as a whole.
Funding and support: By Annals policy, all authors are required
to disclose any and all commercial, financial, and other
relationships in any way related to the subject of this article,
that might create any potential conflict of interest. See the
Manuscript Submission Agreement in this issue for examples
of specific conflicts covered by this statement. This study was
Annals of Emergency Medicine 259
Patient Care Management Problems
funded in part by the Agency for Healthcare Research and
Quality, grant number 5 P20 HS011552, and the Department
of Emergency Medicine, Cook County Hospital (Stroger).
Publication dates: Received for publication November 18,
2006. Revisions received March 8, 2007; May 21, 2007; and
June 14, 2007. Accepted for publication June 25, 2007.
Available online October 15, 2007.
Address for reprints: Karen Cosby, MD, Department of
Emergency Medicine, Cook County Hospital (Stroger), 10th
floor Administration Building, 1900 W Polk St, Chicago, IL
60612; 312-864-1986 or 312-864-0060, fax 312-864-9656;
E-mail kcosby@ccbh.org.
REFERENCES
1. Brennan TA, Leape LL, Laird NM, et al. Incidence of adverse
events and negligence in hospitalized patients: results of the
Harvard Medical Practice Study I. N Engl J Med. 1991;324:370376.
2. Leape LL, Brennan TA, Laird N, et al. The nature of adverse
events in hospitalized patients: results of the Harvard Medical
Practice Study II. N Engl J Med. 1991;324:377-384.
3. Thomas EJ, Studdert DM, Burstin HR, et al. Incidence and types
of adverse events and negligent care in Utah and Colorado. Med
Care. 2000;38:261-271.
4. Wilson RM, Runciman WB, Gibberd RW, et al. The Quality in
Australian Health Care Study. Med J Aust. 1995;163:458-471.
5. Vincent C, Neale G, Woloshynowych M. Adverse events in British
hospitals: preliminary retrospective record review. BMJ. 2001;
322:517-519.
6. Forster AJ, Asmis TR, Clark HD, et al. Ottawa Hospital Patient
Safety Study: incidence and timing of adverse events in patients
admitted to a Canadian teaching hospital. CMAJ. 2004;170:
1235-1240.
7. Kohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS, eds. To Err Is Human:
Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press; 2000.
8. Committee on Quality of Health Care in America, Institute of
Medicine. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for
the 21st Century. Washington, DC: National Academy Press;
2001.
9. Fassett WE. Patient Safety and Quality Improvement Act of 2005.
Ann Pharmacother. 2006;40:917-924.
10. Clinton HR, Obama B. Making patient safety the centerpiece of
medical liability reform. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:2205-2208.
11. Landrigan CP, Rothschild JM, Cronin JW, et al. Effect of reducing
interns’ work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care
units. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:1838-1848.
12. Cosby KS, Croskerry P. Patient safety: a curriculum for teaching
patient safety in emergency medicine. Acad Emerg Med. 2003;
10:69-78.
13. Croskerry P, Sinclair D. Emergency medicine: a practice prone to
error? Can J Emerg Med. 2001;3:271-276.
14. Committee on the Future of Emergency Care in the United States
Health System, Institute of Medicine. Hospital-Based Emergency
Care: At the Breaking Point. Washington, DC: National Academies
Press; 2006.
15. Cosby KS. A framework for classifying factors that contribute to
error in the emergency department. Ann Emerg Med. 2003; 42:
815-823.
16. Vincent C, Taylor-Adams S, Stanhope N. Framework for analysing
risk and safety in clinical medicine. BMJ. 1998;316:1154-1157.
17. Reason J. Human Error. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press; 1990.
260 Annals of Emergency Medicine
Cosby et al
18. Croskerry P. Diagnostic failure: a cognitive and affective
approach. In: Henriksen K, Battles JB, Marks ES, et al, eds.
Advances in Patient Safety: From Research to Implementation.
Vol 2. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality;
2005:241-254. AHRQ Publication No. 05-0021-2.
19. Caplan RA, Posner KL, Cheney FW. Effect of outcome on
physician judgments of the appropriateness of care. JAMA. 1991;
265:1957-1960.
20. Henriksen K, Kaplan H. Hindsight bias, outcome knowledge and
adaptive learning. Qual Saf Health Care. 2003;12(suppl II):ii46ii50.
21. Senders JW, Moray NP. Human Error (Cause, Prediction, and
Reduction): Analysis and Synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1991.
22. Woolf SH, Kuzel AJ, Dovey SM, et al. A string of mistakes: the
importance of cascade analysis in describing, counting, and
preventing medical errors. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:317-326.
23. Kuhn GJ. Diagnostic errors. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9:740-750.
24. Schiff GD, Kim S, Abrams R, et al. Diagnosing diagnostic errors:
lessons from a multi-institutional collaborative project. In:
Henriksen K, Battles JB, Marks ES, et al, eds. Advances in
Patient Safety: From Research to Implementation. Vol 2.
Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality;
2005:255-278. AHRQ Publication No. 05-0021-2.
25. Croskerry P. Achilles heels of the ED: delayed or missed
diagnoses. ED Leg Lett. 2003;14:109-120.
26. Stiell A, Forster AJ, Stiell IG, et al. Prevalence of information gaps
in the emergency department and the effect on patient outcomes.
CMAJ. 2003;169:1023-1028.
27. Davies JM. Team communication in the operating room. Acta
Anaesthesiol Scand. 2005;49:898-901.
28. Jain M, Miller L, Belt D, et al. Decline in ICU adverse events,
nosocomial infections and cost through a quality improvement
initiative focusing on teamwork and culture change. Qual Saf
Health Care. 2006;15:235-239.
29. Behara R, Wears RL, Perry SJ, et al. A conceptual framework for
studying the safety of transitions in emergency care. In:
Henriksen K, Battles JB, Marks ES, et al, eds. Advances in
Patient Safety: From Research to Implementation. Vol 2.
Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality;
2005:309-3321. AHRQ Publication No. 05-0021-2.
30. Croskerry P. Achieving quality in clinical decision making:
cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med.
2002;9:1184-1204.
31. Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and
strategies to minimize them. Acad Med. 2003;78:775-780.
32. Croskerry P. Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical
decisionmaking. Ann Emerg Med. 2003;41:110-120.
33. Croskerry P. The cognitive imperative: thinking about how we
think. Acad Emerg Med. 2000;7:1223-1231.
34. Graber M. Metacognitive training to reduce diagnostic errors:
ready for prime time? Acad Med. 2003;78:781.
35. Elstein AS, Schwarz A. Clinical problem solving and diagnostic
decision making: selective review of the cognitive literature. BMJ.
2002;324:729-732.
36. Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. Learning Clinical Reasoning. Baltimore,
MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1991.
37. Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing diagnostic errors in
medicine: what’s the goal? Acad Med. 2002;77:981-992.
38. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Diagnostic error in internal
medicine. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:1493-1499.
39. Garg AX, Adhikari NK, McDonald H, et al. Effects of computerized
clinical decision support systems on practitioner performance and
patient outcomes: a systematic review. JAMA. 2005;293:12231238.
Volume , .  : March 
Cosby et al
Patient Care Management Problems
40. Bates DW, Kuperman GJ, Wang S, et al. Ten commandments for
effective clinical decision support: making the practice of
evidence-based medicine a reality. J Am Med Inform Assoc.
2003;10:523-530.
41. Bond WF, Deitrick LM, Arnold DC, et al. Using simulation to
instruct emergency medicine residents in cognitive forcing
strategies. Acad Med. 2004;79:438-446.
42. Vincent C, Taylor-Adams S, Chapman EJ. How to investigate and
analyse clinical incidents: Clinical Risk Management and
Association of Litigation and Risk Management protocol. BMJ.
2000;320:777-781.
43. Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. Cognitive errors in diagnosis: instantiation,
classification, and consequences. Am J Med. 1989;86:433-441.
44. Wang MC, Hyun JK, Harrison M, et al. Redesigning health
systems for quality: lessons from emerging practices. Jt Comm J
Qual Patient Saf. 2006;32:599-611.
45. Ross L. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings:
46.
47.
48.
49.
distortions in the attribution process. In: Berkowitz L, ed.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York, NY:
Academic Press; 1977:174-221.
Banja J. Medical Errors and Medical Narcissism. Sudbury, MA:
Jones and Bartlett Publishers; 2005.
Conway JB, Weingart SN. Organizational change in the face of highly
public errors. I. The Dana-Farber Cancer Institute experience. Agency
for Healthcare Research and Quality Morbidity and Mortality Rounds
Web site. Available at: http://www.webmm.ahrq.gov/perspective.
aspx?perspectiveID⫽3. Accessed January 26, 2007.
Kachalia A, Gandhi TK, Poupolo AL, et al. Missed and delayed
diagnoses in the emergency department: a study of closed
malpractice claims from 4 liability insurers. Ann Emerg Med.
2007;49:196-205.
White AA, Wright SW, Blanco R, et al. Cause-and-effect analysis
of risk management files to assess patient care in the emergency
department. Acad Emerg Med. 2004;11:1035-1041.
IMAGES IN EMERGENCY MEDICINE
(continued from p. 230)
DIAGNOSIS:
Emphysematous cystitis. Emphysematous cystitis is a rare, necrotizing infection characterized by gas collection in
the urinary bladder wall and lumen, resulting from gas-producing pathogen infection.1 The risk factors are
diabetes (up to 80%), bladder outlet obstruction, recurrent urinary tract infection, urinary stasis, neurogenic
bladder, immunosuppression, female sex, and being a transplant recipient.2 The mechanism and pathogenesis of
emphysematous cystitis are still unknown. The gas is suggested to be produced by the infected organism by the
fermentation of albumin or glucose in urine. The most common organisms are Escherichia coli,3 Enterobacter
aerogenes, and Klebsiella pneumoniae. Emphysematous cystitis has nonspecific clinical features and is often
misdiagnosed. Clinically, emphysematous cystitis is often diagnosed by the unanticipated imaging findings. Plain
abdominal radiograph usually makes the diagnosis, with high sensitivity (97.4%),4 but abdominal computed
tomography scan was the most sensitive and specific diagnostic tool.5
About 18.8% of emphysematous cystitis cases have complicated courses.4 Emphysematous cystitis demands
prompt diagnosis and intervention,6 including aggressive parenteral antibiotics and even bladder drainage.7
Generally, emphysematous cystitis has favorable prognosis, whereas delays in diagnosis and treatment may
contribute to high mortality rate, which approaches 20%.
A favorable prognosis may be achieved by early recognition of emphysematous cystitis, by clinical and
radiologic assessment, by appropriate antibiotic use, and by timely surgical intervention when indicated.
This patient was administrated empiric antibiotic and promoted surgical drainage. Escherichia coli was isolated
subsequently from both urine and drainage pus cultures. The patient was discharged after a 2-week hospitalization.
REFERENCES
1. Quint HJ, Drach GW, Rappaport WD, et al. Emphysematous cystitis: a review of the spectrum of disease. J Urol. 1992;
147:134-137.
2. Akalin E, Hyde C, Schmitt G, et al. Emphysematous cystitis and pyelitis in a diabetic renal transplant recipient.
Transplantation. 1996;62:1024-1026.
3. Bailey H. Cystitis emphysematosa: 19 cases with intraluminal and interstitial collections of gas. Am J Roentgenol Radium
Ther Nucl Med. 1961;86:850-862.
4. Grupper M, Kravtsov A, Potasman I. Emphysematous cystitis: illustrative case report and review of the literature.
Medicine. 2007;86:47-53.
5. Perlemoine C, Neau D, Ragnaud JM, et al. Emphysematous cystitis. Diabetes Metab. 2004;30:377-379.
6. Quint HJ, Drach GW, Rappaport WD, et al. Emphysematous cystitis: a review of the spectrum of disease. J Urol. 1992;
147:134-137.
7. Yasumoto R, Asakawa M, Nishisaka N. Emphysematous cystitis. Br J Urol. 1989;63:644.
Volume , .  : March 
Annals of Emergency Medicine 261
APPENDIX E1.
Examples of actions resulting from morbidity and
mortality investigations.
Uncommon Diagnosis Requiring Complex Evaluations and
Multispecialty Interventions
The Problem: After observing difficulties in the timely
diagnosis and treatment of aortic dissections, we reviewed a
series of cases of aortic dissections from our morbidity and
mortality database and identified key problem areas. The
diagnosis was observed to be difficult, often overlapping more
common chest pain entities. The evaluation for dissection could
involve a variety of approaches (echocardiography, computed
tomography, or angiography) and involved multiple consultants
(chest surgeons, cardiologists, vascular surgeons, intensivists,
and interventional radiologists). Because of the variation in
practice, there was often conflict between specialists and services
about the optimal testing and management of these cases; care
was often delayed by conflict and indecision.
Actions Taken: After review of a series of problematic cases,
the ED staff convened a multidisciplinary conference with all
the involved specialties, eventually developing a standard
approach agreed on by all disciplines. Once the policy was
developed, hospital staff worked to ensure that services were
available to follow the protocol. The result was a much more
simplified and direct approach to caring for similar cases.
Cases With Evolving Standards of Care and Controversy
About Optimal Management: Ectopic Pregnancy
The Problem: Multiple problems were observed in the
timely diagnosis of ectopic pregnancy and in management of
patients with first trimester bleeding. Because of evolving
standards in the care of patients with first trimester pregnancy,
the ED staff experienced conflict and inconsistent standards
261.e1 Annals of Emergency Medicine
within their own group, as well as consultants. In the process,
the diagnosis of several ectopic pregnancies was delayed.
Actions Taken: The ED organized a literature review and
grand rounds conference on ectopic pregnancy, developed a
treatment protocol for first trimester pregnancy, and then met
with a working group with obstetrics to standardize criteria for
consultation, admission, and follow-up. Meanwhile, the ED
improved its proficiency in bedside ultrasonography and began
more liberal screening of all first trimester pregnancy bleeding.
Dealing With a Local Infectious Disease Threat:
Tuberculosis
The Problem: A resurgence of tuberculosis in our city led to
an increase in number of patients with active tuberculosis in our
ED. There were a limited number of isolation beds available
within the hospital, and medical staff had too few beds to isolate
all potential cases. A number of patients admitted to general
medical wards were found to have active tuberculosis; at the
same time, there was an increase in the number of house staff
who converted to positive tuberculin skin tests.
The Action: Cases with positive tuberculosis culture results
were identified and their ED records tracked. The ED convened
a multidisciplinary conference with the Infectious Disease and
Radiology Departments to review the cases. Ultimately, we
found that many active cases of tuberculosis could not be
predicted according to clinical and radiographic criteria.
Eventually, the medical staff obtained additional isolation
rooms in the ED and in the hospital, applied new screening at
triage to prioritize chest radiographs in patients with respiratory
symptoms, prioritized movement of patients with suspected
cases from triage to isolation beds in the ED, and successfully
argued for increased resources to handle the challenge.
Volume , .  : March 
WHAT ARE THE CAUSES OF MISSED AND DELAYED
DIAGNOSES IN THE AMBULATORY SETTING?
Gandhi TK, Kachalia A, Thomas EJ et al.
Missed and Delayed Diagnoses in the Ambulatory Setting:
A Study of Closed Malpractice Claims.
Ann Intern Med.2006;145:488-496
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Article
Annals of Internal Medicine
Missed and Delayed Diagnoses in the Ambulatory Setting:
A Study of Closed Malpractice Claims
Tejal K. Gandhi, MD, MPH; Allen Kachalia, MD, JD; Eric J. Thomas, MD, MPH; Ann Louise Puopolo, BSN, RN; Catherine Yoon, MS;
Troyen A. Brennan, MD, JD; and David M. Studdert, LLB, ScD
Background: Although missed and delayed diagnoses have become
an important patient safety concern, they remain largely unstudied,
especially in the outpatient setting.
Objective: To develop a framework for investigating missed and
delayed diagnoses, advance understanding of their causes, and
identify opportunities for prevention.
Design: Retrospective review of 307 closed malpractice claims in
which patients alleged a missed or delayed diagnosis in the ambulatory setting.
Setting: 4 malpractice insurance companies.
Measurements: Diagnostic errors associated with adverse outcomes for patients, process breakdowns, and contributing factors.
Results: A total of 181 claims (59%) involved diagnostic errors that
harmed patients. Fifty-nine percent (106 of 181) of these errors
were associated with serious harm, and 30% (55 of 181) resulted
in death. For 59% (106 of 181) of the errors, cancer was the
diagnosis involved, chiefly breast (44 claims [24%]) and colorectal
(13 claims [7%]) cancer. The most common breakdowns in the
M
issed and delayed diagnoses in the ambulatory setting are an important patient safety problem. The
current diagnostic process in health care is complex, chaotic, and vulnerable to failures and breakdowns. For example, one third of women with abnormal results on mammography or Papanicolaou smears do not receive follow-up
care that is consistent with well-established guidelines (1,
2), and primary care providers often report delays in reviewing test results (3). Recognition of systemic problems
in this area has prompted urgent calls for improvements (4).
However, this type of error remains largely unstudied
(4). At least part of the reason is technical: Because omissions characterize missed diagnoses, they are difficult to
identify; there is no standard reporting mechanism; and
when they are identified, documentation in medical
records is usually insufficiently detailed to support detailed
causal analyses. The result is a relatively thin evidence base
from which to launch efforts to combat diagnostic errors.
Moreover, conceptions of the problem tend to remain
rooted in the notion of physicians failing to be vigilant or
up-to-date. This is a less nuanced view of error causation
than careful analysis of other major patient safety problems, such as medication errors (5, 6), has revealed.
Several considerations highlight malpractice claims as
a potentially rich source of information about missed and
delayed diagnoses. First, misdiagnosis is a common allegation. Over the past decade, lawsuits alleging negligent misdiagnoses have become the most prevalent type of claim in
488 © 2006 American College of Physicians
diagnostic process were failure to order an appropriate diagnostic
test (100 of 181 [55%]), failure to create a proper follow-up plan
(81 of 181 [45%]), failure to obtain an adequate history or perform
an adequate physical examination (76 of 181 [42%]), and incorrect
interpretation of diagnostic tests (67 of 181 [37%]). The leading
factors that contributed to the errors were failures in judgment (143
of 181 [79%]), vigilance or memory (106 of 181 [59%]), knowledge (86 of 181 [48%]), patient-related factors (84 of 181 [46%]),
and handoffs (36 of 181 [20%]). The median number of process
breakdowns and contributing factors per error was 3 for both
(interquartile range, 2 to 4).
Limitations: Reviewers were not blinded to the litigation outcomes,
and the reliability of the error determination was moderate.
Conclusions: Diagnostic errors that harm patients are typically the
result of multiple breakdowns and individual and system factors.
Awareness of the most common types of breakdowns and factors
could help efforts to identify and prioritize strategies to prevent
diagnostic errors.
Ann Intern Med. 2006;145:488-496.
For author affiliations, see end of text.
www.annals.org
the United States (7, 8). Second, diagnostic breakdowns
that lead to claims tend to be associated with especially
severe outcomes. Third, relatively thorough documentation on what happened is available in malpractice insurers’
claim files. In addition to the medical record, these files
include depositions, expert opinions, and sometimes the
results of internal investigations.
Previous attempts to use data from malpractice claims
to study patient safety have had various methodologic constraints, including small sample size (9, 10), a focus on
single insurers (11) or verdicts (9, 10) (which constitute
⬍10% of claims), limited information on the claims (8 –
11), reliance on internal case review by insurers rather than
by independent experts (8, 11), and a general absence of
See also:
Print
Editors’ Notes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489
Editorial comment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Summary for Patients. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I-12
Web-Only
Appendix
Appendix Tables
Appendix Figures
Conversion of tables and figures into slides
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
robust frameworks for classifying types and causes of failures. To address these issues, we analyzed data from closed
malpractice claims at 4 liability insurance companies. Our
goals were to develop a framework for investigating missed
and delayed diagnoses, advance understanding of their
causes, and identify opportunities for prevention.
METHODS
Study Sites
Article
Context
Efforts to reduce medical errors and improve patient safety
have not generally addressed errors in diagnosis. As with
treatment, diagnosis involves complex, fragmented processes within health care systems that are vulnerable to
failures and breakdowns.
Contributions
Four malpractice insurance companies based in 3 regions (northeastern, southwestern, and western United
States) participated in the study. Collectively, the participating companies insured approximately 21 000 physicians, 46 acute care hospitals (20 academic and 26 nonacademic), and 390 outpatient facilities, including a wide
variety of primary care and outpatient specialty practices.
The ethics review boards at the investigators’ institutions
and at each review site approved the study.
The authors reviewed malpractice claims alleging injury
from a missed or delayed diagnosis. In 181 cases in which
there was a high likelihood that error led to the missed
diagnosis, the authors analyzed where the diagnostic process broke down and why. The most common missed diagnosis was cancer, and the most common breakdowns
were failure to order appropriate tests and inadequate follow-up of test results. A median of 3 process breakdowns
occurred per error, and 2 or more clinicians were involved
in 43% of cases.
Claims Sample
Cautions
Data were extracted from random samples of closed
claim files from each insurer. A claim is classified as closed
when it has been dropped, dismissed, paid by settlement,
or resolved by verdict. The claim file is the repository of
information accumulated by the insurer during the life of a
claim. It captures a wide variety of data, including the
statement of claim, depositions, interrogatories, and other
litigation documents; reports of internal investigations,
such as risk management evaluations and sometimes rootcause analyses; expert opinions from both sides; medical
reports detailing the plaintiff ’s preevent and postevent condition; and, while the claim is open, medical records pertaining to the episode of care at issue. We reacquired the
relevant medical records for sampled claims.
Following previous studies, we defined a claim as a
written demand for compensation for medical injury (12,
13). Claims involving missed or delayed diagnoses were
defined as those alleging an error in diagnosis or testing
that caused a delay in appropriate treatment or a failure to
act or follow up on results of diagnostic tests. We excluded
allegations related to pregnancy and those pertaining to
care rendered solely in the inpatient setting.
We reviewed 429 diagnostic claims alleging injury due
to missed or delayed diagnoses. Insurers contributed to the
study sample in proportion to their annual claims volume
(Appendix, available at www.annals.org). The claims were
divided into 2 main categories based on the primary setting
of the outpatient care involved in the allegation: the emergency department (122 claims) and all other locations (for
example, physician’s office, ambulatory surgery, pathology
laboratory, or radiology suites) (307 claims). The latter
group, which we call ambulatory claims, is the focus of this
analysis.
Study Instruments and Claim File Review
Physicians who were board-certified attendings, fellows, or third-year residents in internal medicine reviewed
www.annals.org
The study relied on malpractice claims, which are not representative of all diagnostic errors that occur. There was
only moderate agreement among the authors in their subjective judgments about errors and their causes.
Implications
Like other medical errors, diagnostic errors are multifactorial. They arise from multiple process breakdowns, usually
involving multiple providers. The results highlight the challenge of finding effective ways to reduce diagnostic errors
as a component of improving health care quality.
—The Editors
sampled claim files at the insurers’ offices or insured facilities. Physician-investigators trained the reviewers in the
content of claim files, use of the study instruments, and
confidentiality procedures in 1-day sessions at each site.
The reviewers also used a detailed manual. Reviews took
on average 1.4 hours per file. To test review reliability, a
second reviewer reviewed a random selection of 10% (42
of 429) of the files. Thirty-three of the 307 ambulatory
claims that are the focus of this analysis were included in
the random blinded re-review. A sequence of 4 instruments
guided the review. For all claims, insurance staff recorded
administrative details of the case (Appendix Figure 1,
available at www.annals.org), and clinical reviewers recorded details of the adverse outcome the patient experienced, if any (Appendix Figure 2, available at www.annals
.org). Reviewers scored adverse outcomes on a 9-point
severity scale ranging from emotional injury only to death.
This scale was developed by the National Association of
Insurance Commissioners (14) and has been used in previous research (15). If the patient had multiple adverse
outcomes, reviewers scored the most severe outcome. To
simplify presentation of our results, we grouped scores on
3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7 489
Article
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
this scale into 5 categories (emotional, minor, significant,
major, and death).
Next, reviewers considered the potential role of a series
of contributing factors (Appendix Figure 3, available at
www.annals.org) in causing the adverse outcome. The factors covered cognitive-, system-, and patient-related causes
that were related to the episode of care as a whole, not to
particular steps in the diagnostic process. The factors were
selected on the basis of a review of the patient safety literature performed in 2001 by 5 of the authors in consultation with physician-collaborators from surgery and obstetrics and gynecology.
Reviewers then judged, in light of available information and their decisions about contributing factors,
whether the adverse outcome was due to diagnostic error.
We used the Institute of Medicine’s definition of error,
namely, “the failure of a planned action to be completed as
intended (i.e. error of execution) or the use of a wrong plan
to achieve an aim (i.e. error of planning)” (16). Reviewers
recorded their judgment on a 6-point confidence scale
ranging from “1. Little or no evidence that adverse outcome resulted from error/errors” to “6. Virtually certain
evidence that adverse outcome resulted from error/errors.”
Claims that scored 4 (“More likely than not that adverse
outcome resulted from error/errors; more than 50-50 but a
close call”) or higher were classified as having an error. The
confidence scale and cutoff point were adapted from instruments used in previous studies of medical injury (15, 17).
Reviewers were not blinded to the litigation outcomes
but were instructed to ignore them and rely on their own
clinical judgment in making decisions about errors. Training sessions stressed that the study definition of error is not
synonymous with the legal definition of negligence and
that a mix of factors extrinsic to merit influence whether
claims are paid during litigation.
Finally, for the subset of claims judged to involve errors, reviewers completed an additional form (Appendix
Figure 4, available at www.annals.org) that collected additional clinical information about the missed diagnosis. Specifically, reviewers considered a defined sequence of diagnostic
steps (for example, history and physical examination, test
ordering, and creation of a follow-up plan) and were asked
to grade their confidence that a breakdown had occurred at
each step (5-point Likert scale ranging from “highly unlikely” to “highly likely”). If a breakdown was judged to
have been at least “somewhat likely” (score of ⱖ3), the
form elicited additional information on the particular
breakdown, including a non–mutually exclusive list of reasons for the breakdown.
Statistical Analysis
The primary unit of analysis was the sequence of care
in claims judged to involve a diagnostic error that led to an
adverse outcome. For ease of exposition, we henceforth
refer to such sequences as errors. The hand-filled data
forms were electronically entered and verified by a profes490 3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7
sional data entry vendor and sent to the Harvard School of
Public Health for analysis. Additional validity checks and
data cleaning were performed by study programmers. Analyses were conducted by using SAS, version 8.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina) and Stata SE, version 8.0
(Stata Corp., College Station, Texas). We examined characteristics of the claims, patients, and injuries in our sample and the frequency of the various contributing factors.
We compared characteristics of error subgroups using
Pearson chi-square tests and used percentage agreement
and ␬ scores (18) to measure interrater reliability of the
injury and error determinations.
Role of the Funding Sources
This study was funded by grants from the Agency for
Healthcare Research and Quality (HS011886-03) and the
Harvard Risk Management Foundation. These organizations did not play a role in the design, conduct, or analysis
of the study, and the decision to submit the manuscript for
publication was that of the authors.
RESULTS
The 307 diagnosis-related ambulatory claims closed
between 1984 and 2004. Eighty-five percent (262 of 307)
of the alleged errors occurred in 1990 or later, and 80%
(245 of 307 claims) closed in 1997 or later. In 2% (7 of
307) of claims, no adverse outcome or change in the patient’s clinical course was evident; in 3% (9 of 307), the
reviewer was unable to judge the severity of the adverse
outcome from the information available; and in 36% (110
of 307), the claim was judged not to involve a diagnostic
error. The remaining group of 181 claims, 59% (181 of
307) of the sample, were judged to involve diagnostic errors that led to adverse outcomes. This group of errors is
the focus of further analyses.
In 40 of the 42 re-reviewed claims, reviewers agreed
about whether an adverse outcome had occurred (95%
agreement). The reliability of the determination of whether
an error had occurred (a score ⬍4 vs. ⱖ4 on the confidence scale) was moderate (72% agreement; ␬ ⫽ 0.42
[95% CI, ⫺0.05 to 0.66]).
Errors and Diagnoses
Fifty-nine percent (106 of 181) of the errors were associated with significant or major physical adverse outcomes and 30% (55 of 181) were associated with death
(Table 1). For 59% (106 of 181) of errors, cancer was the
diagnosis missed, chiefly breast (44 of 181 [24%]), colorectal (13 of 181 [7%]), and skin (8 of 181 [4%]) cancer.
The next most commonly missed diagnoses were infections
(9 of 181 [5%]), fractures (8 of 181 [4%]), and myocardial
infarctions (7 of 181 [4%]). Most errors occurred in physicians’ offices (154 of 181 [85%]), and primary care physicians were the providers most commonly involved (76 of
181 [42%]). The mean interval between when diagnoses
should have been made (that is, in the absence of error)
www.annals.org
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
Table 1. Characteristics of Patients, Involved Clinicians,
Adverse Outcomes, and Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
among 181 Diagnostic Errors in Ambulatory Care
Characteristics
Errors, n (%)
Female patient
110 (61)
Patient age (mean, 44 y [SD, 17])
⬍1 y
1–18 y
18–34 y
35–49 y
50–64 y
⬎64 y
Health insurance (n ⴝ 117)
Private
Medicaid
Uninsured
Medicare
Other
Clinicians involved in error*
Specialty
Primary care†
Radiology
General surgery
Pathology
Physician’s assistant
Registered nurse or nurse practitioner
Trainee (resident, fellow, or intern)
Setting
Physician’s office
Ambulatory surgery facility
Pathology or clinical laboratory
Radiology suite
Other
Missed or delayed diagnosis
Cancer
Breast
Colorectal
Skin
Hematologic
Gynecologic
Lung
Brain
Prostate
Liver or gastric
Other
Infection
Fracture
Myocardial infarction
Embolism
Appendicitis
Cerebral vascular disease
Other neurologic condition
Aneurysm
Other cardiac condition
Gynecologic disease (noncancer)
Endocrine disorder
Birth defect
Ophthalmologic disease
Peripheral vascular disease
Abdominal disease
Psychiatric illness
Other
Adverse outcome‡
Psychological or emotional
5 (3)
9 (5)
34 (19)
64 (35)
50 (28)
19 (10)
103 (88)
3 (3)
7 (6)
1 (1)
3 (3)
88 (49)
31 (17)
23 (13)
13 (7)
13 (7)
14 (8)
20 (11)
154 (85)
8 (4)
8 (4)
5 (3)
6 (3)
106 (59)
44 (24)
13 (7)
8 (4)
7 (4)
7 (4)
6 (3)
5 (3)
5 (3)
2 (1)
9 (5)
9 (5)
8 (4)
7 (4)
6 (3)
5 (3)
4 (2)
4 (2)
3 (2)
3 (2)
3 (2)
3 (2)
3 (2)
3 (2)
2 (1)
2 (1)
2 (1)
8 (4)
9 (5)
Continued
www.annals.org
Article
Table 1—Continued
Characteristics
Minor physical
Significant physical
Major physical
Death
Errors, n (%)
11 (6)
72 (40)
34 (19)
55 (30)
* Percentages do not sum to 100% because multiple providers were involved in
some errors.
† Includes 12 pediatricians, therefore pediatrics accounts for an absolute 7% of the
49%.
‡ Categories correspond to the following scores on the National Association of
Insurance Commissioners’ 9-point severity scale: psychiatric or emotional (score
1), minor physical (scores 2 and 3), significant physical (scores 4 to 6), major
physical (scores 7 and 8), and death (score 9). For details, see Appendix Figure 2,
available at www.annals.org.
and when they actually were made was 465 days (SD, 571
days), and the median was 303 days (interquartile range,
36 to 681 days). Appendix Table 1 (available at www
.annals.org) outlines several examples of missed and delayed diagnoses in the study sample.
Breakdowns in the Diagnostic Process
The leading breakdown points in the diagnostic process were failure to order an appropriate diagnostic test
(100 of 181 [55%]), failure to create a proper follow-up
plan (81 of 181 [45%]), failure to obtain an adequate history or to perform an adequate physical examination (76 of
181 [42%]), and incorrect interpretation of a diagnostic
test (67 of 181 [37%]) (Table 2). Missed cancer diagnoses
were significantly more likely to involve diagnostic tests
being performed incorrectly (14 of 106 [13%] vs. 1 of 75
[1%]; P ⫽ 0.004) and being interpreted incorrectly (49 of
106 [46%] vs. 18 of 75 [24%]; P ⫽ 0.002), whereas
missed noncancer diagnoses were significantly more likely
to involve delays by patients in seeking care (4 of 106 [4%]
vs. 12 of 75 [16%]; P ⫽ 0.004), inadequate history or
physical examination (24 of 106 [23%] vs. 52 of 75
[69%]; P ⬍ 0.001), and failure to refer (19 of 106 [18%]
vs. 28 of 75 [37%]; P ⫽ 0.003).
Table 3 details the 4 most common process breakdowns. Among failures to order diagnostic tests, most tests
involved were from the imaging (45 of 100 [45%]) or
other test (50 of 100 [50%]) categories. With respect to
the specific tests involved, biopsies were most frequently at
issue (25 of 100 [25%]), followed by computed tomography scans, mammography, ultrasonography, and colonoscopy (11 of 100 for each [11%]). The most common explanation for the failures to order was that the physician
seemed to lack knowledge of the appropriate test in the
clinical circumstances. Among misinterpretations of test results, 27% (18 of 67) related to results on mammography
and 15% (10 of 67) related to results on radiography. The
main reasons for inadequate follow-up plans were that the
physician did not think follow-up was necessary (32 of 81
[40%]), selected an inappropriate follow-up interval (29 of
3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7 491
Article
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
Table 2. Breakdown Points in the Diagnostic Process
Process Breakdown
Initial delay by the patient in seeking care
Failure to obtain adequate medical history or physical
examination
Failure to order appropriate diagnostic or laboratory
tests
Adequate diagnostic or laboratory tests ordered but not
performed
Diagnostic or laboratory tests performed incorrectly
Incorrect interpretation of diagnostic or laboratory tests
Responsible provider did not receive diagnostic or
laboratory test results
Diagnostic or laboratory test results were not
transmitted to patient
Inappropriate or inadequate follow-up plan
Failure to refer
Failure of a requested referral to occur
Failure of the referred-to clinician to convey relevant
results to the referring clinician
Patient nonadherence to the follow-up plan
Missed Cancer
Diagnoses
(n ⴝ 106), n (%)
Missed Noncancer
Diagnoses
(n ⴝ 75), n (%)
P Value*
16 (9)
76 (42)
4 (4)
24 (23)
12 (16)
52 (69)
0.004
⬍0.001
100 (55)
63 (59)
37 (49)
0.178
17 (9)
10 (9)
7 (9)
0.98
15 (8)
67 (37)
23 (13)
14 (13)
49 (46)
17 (16)
1 (1)
18 (24)
6 (8)
0.004
0.002
0.110
22 (12)
15 (14)
7 (9)
0.33
81 (45)
47 (26)
9 (5)
3 (2)
51 (48)
19 (18)
8 (8)
2 (2)
30 (40)
28 (37)
1 (1)
1 (1)
0.28
0.003
0.058
0.77
31 (17)
21 (20)
10 (13)
0.25
All Missed
Diagnoses
(n ⴝ 181), n (%)
* Pearson chi-square test for differences between cancer and noncancer categories.
81 [36%]), or did not document the plan correctly (22 of
81 [27%]).
Contributing Factors
The leading factors that contibuted to errors were failures in judgment (143 of 181 [79%]), vigilance or memory
(106 of 181 [59%]), knowledge (86 of 181 [48%]), patient-related factors (84 of 181 [46%]), and handoffs (36
of 181 [20%]) (Table 4). The patient-related factors included nonadherence (40 of 181 [22%]), atypical clinical
presentation (28 of 181 [15%]), and complicated medical
history (18 of 181 [10%]). There were no significant differences in the prevalence of the various contributing factors when missed cancer and missed noncancer diagnoses
were compared, with the exception of lack of supervision,
which was significantly more likely to have occurred in
missed noncancer diagnoses (10 of 75 [13%] vs. 5 of 106
[5%]; P ⫽ 0.038). The higher prevalence of lack of supervision as a contributing factor to missed noncancer diagnoses was accompanied by greater involvement of trainees
in these cases (13 of 75 [17%] vs. 7 of 106 [7%]; P ⫽
0.023).
Multifactorial Nature of Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
The diagnostic errors were complex and frequently involved multiple process breakdowns, contributing factors,
and contributing clinicians (Table 5). In 43% (78 of 181)
of errors, 2 or more clinicians contributed to the missed
diagnosis, and in 16% (29 of 181), 3 or more clinicians
contributed. There was a median of 3 (interquartile range,
2 to 4) process breakdowns per error; 54% (97 of 181) of
errors had 3 or more process breakdowns and 29% (52
of 181) had 4 or more. Thirty-five diagnostic errors (35 of
181 [19%]) involved a breakdown at only 1 point in the
care process (single-point breakdowns). Twenty-four (24 of
492 3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7
35 [69%]) of these single-point breakdowns were either
failures to order tests (n ⫽ 10) or incorrect interpretations
of tests (n ⫽ 14); only 3 involved trainees.
The median number of contributing factors involved
in diagnostic errors was 3 (interquartile range, 2 to 4); 59%
(107 of 181) had 3 or more contributing factors, 27% (48
of 181) had 4 or more, and 13% (23 of 181) had 5 or
more. Thus, although virtually all diagnostic errors were
linked to cognitive factors, especially judgment errors, cognitive factors operated alone in a minority of cases. They
were usually accompanied by communication factors, patient-related factors, or other system factors.
Specifically, 36% (66 of 181) of errors involved cognitive factors alone, 16% (29 of 181) involved judgment or
vigilance and memory factors alone, and 9% (16 of 181)
involved only judgment factors. The likelihood that cognitive factors alone led to the error was significantly lower
among errors that involved an inadequate medical history
or physical examination (21 of 66 [32%] vs. 55 of 115
[48%]; P ⫽ 0.036), lack of receipt of ordered tests by the
responsible provider (3 of 66 [5%] vs. 20 of 115 [17%];
P ⫽ 0.013), and inappropriate or inadequate follow-up
planning (21 of 66 [32%] vs. 60 of 115 [52%]; P ⫽
0.008). In other words, the role of communication and
other systems factors was especially prominent in these 3
types of breakdowns.
DISCUSSION
Our study of closed malpractice claims identified a
group of missed diagnoses in the ambulatory setting that
were associated with dire outcomes for patients. Over half
of the missed diagnoses were cancer, primarily breast and
colorectal cancer; no other diagnosis accounted for more
www.annals.org
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
than 5% of the sample. The main breakdowns in the diagnostic process were failure to order appropriate diagnostic tests, inappropriate or inadequate follow-up planning,
failure to obtain an adequate medical history or perform an
adequate physical examination, and incorrect interpretation of diagnostic test results. Cognitive factors, patientrelated factors, and handoffs were the most prominent contributing factors overall. However, relatively few diagnostic
errors could be linked to single-point breakdowns or lone
contributing factors. Most missed diagnoses involved manifold breakdowns and a potent combination of individual
and system factors. The resultant delays tended to be long,
setting diagnosis back more than 1 year on average.
The threshold question of what constitutes a harmful
Article
diagnostic error is extremely challenging. Indeed, the nebulous nature of missed diagnoses probably helps to explain
why patient safety research in this area has lagged. Our
approach was 2-dimensional. We considered the potential
role of a range of contributing factors drawn from the fields
of human factors and systems analysis (16, 19) in causing
the patient’s injury. We also disaggregated the diagnostic
process into discrete steps and then examined the prevalence of problems within particular steps. Trained physician-reviewers had the benefit of information from the
claim file and from the medical record in deciding whether
diagnoses were missed. Even so, agreement among reviewers was only fair, highlighting how difficult and subjective
clinical judgments regarding missed diagnoses are.
Table 3. Details of the 4 Most Frequent Process Breakdowns*
Process Breakdown
Leading Reasons for Breakdown
Value,
n
Within Process
Breakdown
Category, %
Failure to order appropriate diagnostic or laboratory
tests (n ⫽ 100)
Provider lacked knowledge of appropriate test
Poor documentation
Failure of communication
Among providers
Between provider and patient
Patient did not schedule or keep appointment
Patient declined tests
Imaging
Computed tomography scanning
Mammography
Ultrasonography
Blood
Other†
Biopsy
Colonoscopy or flexible sigmoidoscopy
Provider did not think follow-up was necessary
Provider selected inappropriate follow-up
interval
Follow-up plan not documented
Follow-up appointment not scheduled
Miscommunication between patient and
provider
Miscommunication between providers
Incomplete physical examination
Failure to elicit relevant information
Poor documentation
Patient provided inaccurate history
Error in clinical judgment
Failure of communication among providers
Inexperience
Whose misinterpretation?
Radiologist
Primary care physician
Pathologist
Imaging
Mammography
Radiography
Ultrasonography
Blood
Other‡
Biopsy
37
10
15
7
8
9
5
45
11
11
11
14
50
25
11
32
29
37
10
15
7
8
9
5
45
11
11
11
14
50
25
11
40
36
22
20
20
27
25
25
10
39
35
20
13
52
5
3
12
50
46
26
17
78
7
5
27
19
10
36
18
10
5
9
21
8
40
28
15
54
27
15
7
13
31
12
Type of test not ordered
Inappropriate or inadequate follow-up plan (n ⫽ 81)
Failure to obtain adequate medical history or physical
examination (n ⫽ 76)
Incorrect interpretation of diagnostic or laboratory
tests (n ⫽ 67)
Type of test interpreted incorrectly
* Category and subcategory totals may exceed 100% because of non–mutually exclusive reasons and multiple tests. Only leading reasons and tests are presented.
† The tests not shown in this category are echocardiography (n ⫽ 3), cardiac catheterization (n ⫽ 3), electrocardiography (n ⫽ 2), treadmill (n ⫽ 2), urinalysis (n ⫽ 1),
esophagogastroduodenoscopy/barium swallow (n ⫽ 1), colposcopy (n ⫽ 1), urine pregnancy (n ⫽ 1), and joint aspirate (n ⫽ 1).
‡ The tests not shown in this category are electrocardiography (n ⫽ 4), urinalysis (n ⫽ 1), echocardiography (n ⫽ 1), stool guaiac (n ⫽ 2), Papanicolaou smear (n ⫽ 3),
cystoscopy (n ⫽ 1), and treadmill (n ⫽ 1).
www.annals.org
3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7 493
Article
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
Table 4. Factors That Contributed to Missed or Delayed
Diagnoses
Variable
Value, n (%)*
Provider- and systems-related factors
Cognitive factors
Judgment
Vigilance or memory
Lack of knowledge
Communication factors
Handoffs
Failure to establish clear lines of responsibility
Conflict
Some other failure of communication
Other systems factors
Lack of supervision
Workload
Interruptions
Technology problems
Fatigue
179 (99)
143 (79)
106 (59)
86 (48)
55 (30)
36 (20)
17 (9)
3 (2)
16 (9)
31 (17)
15 (8)
12 (7)
6 (3)
5 (3)
1 (1)
Patient-related factors†
Nonadherence
Atypical presentation
Complicated medical history
Information about medical history of poor quality
Other
84 (46)
40 (22)
28 (15)
18 (10)
8 (4)
15 (8)
* Percentages were calculated by using the denominator of 181 missed or delayed
diagnoses.
† Sum of specific patient-related factors exceeds total percentage because some
errors had multiple contributing factors.
Troublesome Diagnoses
The prevalence of missed cancer diagnoses in our sample is consistent with previous recognition of this problem
as a major quality concern for ambulatory care (8). Missed
cancer diagnoses were more likely than other missed diagnoses to involve errors in the performance and interpretation of tests. Lack of adherence to guidelines for cancer
screening and test ordering is a well-recognized problem,
and there are ongoing efforts to address it (1, 20). Our data
did not allow assessment of the extent to which guideline
adherence would have prevented errors.
The next most commonly missed diagnoses were infections, fractures, and myocardial infarctions, although
there was also a broad range of other missed diagnoses.
Primary care physicians were centrally involved in most of
these errors. Most primary care physicians work under considerable time pressure, handle a wide range of clinical
Table 5. Distribution of Contributing Clinicians, Process
Breakdowns, and Contributing Factors*
Number
per Error
Contributing
Clinicians, %
Process
Breakdowns, %
Contributing
Factors, %
1
ⱖ2
ⱖ3
ⱖ4
ⱖ5
57
43
16
6
2
19
81
54
29
11
14
86
59
27
13
* The total number of errors was 181.
494 3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7
problems, and treat predominantly healthy patients. They
practice in a health care system in which test results are not
easily tracked, patients are sometimes poor informants,
multiple handoffs exist, and information gaps are the
norm. For even the most stellar practitioners, clinical processes and judgments are bound to fail occasionally under
such circumstances.
Cognitive and Other Factors
Although there were several statistically significant differences in the patterns of process breakdowns between
missed cancer diagnoses, the patterns of contributing factors were remarkably similar. This suggests that although
vulnerable points may vary according to the idiosyncratic
pathways of different diagnoses, a core set of human and
system vulnerabilities permeates diagnostic work. For example, relying exclusively on a physician’s knowledge or
memory to ensure that the correct test is ordered, practicing in a clinic with poor internal communication strategies,
or having a high prevalence of patients with complex disorders may be risky regardless of the potential diagnosis.
Such cognitive factors as judgment and knowledge
were nearly ubiquitous but rarely were the sole contributing factors. Previous research has highlighted the importance of cognitive factors in misdiagnosis and has proposed
frameworks for better understanding the underlying causes
of such errors (21–23). Detecting these cognitive failures
and determining why they occur is difficult, but the design
of robust diagnostic processes will probably depend on it.
Follow-Up
Nearly half of all errors involved an inadequate follow-up plan, a failure that was split fairly evenly between
situations in which the physician determined that follow-up was not necessary and those in which the need for
follow-up was recognized but the wrong interval was selected. Poor documentation, scheduling problems, and
miscommunication among providers also played a role. A
recent survey (24) of patients in 5 countries found that 8%
to 20% of adults who were treated as outpatients did not
receive their test results, and 9% to 15% received incorrect
results or delayed notification of abnormal results. In addition, in our study, 56% (45 of 81) of the errors that
occurred during follow-up also had patient-related factors
that contributed to the poor outcome, a finding that highlights the need for risk reduction initiatives that address
potential breakdowns on both sides of the patient–physician relationship during follow-up.
Interventions
In general, our findings reinforce the need for systems
interventions that mitigate the potential impact of cognitive errors by reducing reliance on memory, forcing consideration of alternative diagnostic plans or second opinions, and providing clinical decision support systems (21,
22). However, cognitive factors rarely appear alone, so interventions must also address other contributing factors,
such as handoffs and communication. To be clinically usewww.annals.org
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
ful and maximally effective, such interventions must be
easy to use and well integrated into clinicians’ workflows
(25). For example, incorporating clinical decision support
into the electronic medical record of a patient with breast
symptoms should help guard against failures in history taking or test ordering by bolstering physicians’ knowledge
and reducing their reliance on vigilance and memory.
An important feature of interventions is that their use
should not rely on voluntary decisions to access them, because physicians are often unaware of their need for help
(26). Rather, the use of an intervention should be automatically triggered by certain predetermined and explicit characteristics of the clinical encounter. For example, strategies
to combat misinterpretation could mandate second reviews
of test results in designated circumstances or require rapid
expert reviews when physicians interpret test results outside
of their areas of expertise.
After follow-up plans are made, improvements in
scheduling procedures, tickler systems, and test result
tracking systems could help keep patients and physicians
on track. Nationwide attention is being given to the issue
of follow-up (27, 28). Research and quality improvement
efforts that equip physicians with tools to reliably perform
follow-up must be a priority.
Selecting just 1 of the interventions we have outlined
may not be sufficient. The multifactorial and complex nature of diagnostic errors suggests that meaningful reductions will require prevention strategies that target multiple
levels in the diagnostic process and multiple contributing
factors. Nevertheless, the resource constraints most health
care institutions face demand priorities. Attention to the 3
vulnerable points we identified— ordering decisions, test
interpretation, and follow-up planning—is a useful starting
point and promises high yields, especially in reducing the
number of missed cancer diagnoses.
Study Limitations
Our study has several limitations. First, unlike prospective observational studies or root-cause analyses, retrospective review of records, even the detailed records found
in malpractice claim files, will miss certain breakdowns (for
example, patient nonadherence) and contributing factors
(for example, fatigue and workload), unless they emerged
as issues during litigation. This measurement problem
means that prevalence findings for such estimates will be
lower bounds, and the multifactorial causality we observed
probably understates the true complexity of diagnostic errors.
An additional measurement problem relates to the
process breakdowns. Although reviewers considered breakdowns as independent events, in some situations breakdowns may have been prompted or influenced by earlier
breakdowns in the diagnostic sequence. For example, a
failure to order appropriate tests may stem from oversights
in the physical examination. Such interdependence would
tend to inflate the frequency of some breakdowns.
www.annals.org
Article
Second, awareness of the litigation outcome may have
biased reviewers toward finding errors in claims that received compensation and vice versa (29, 30). Several factors militate against this bias: Reviewers were instructed to
ignore the litigation outcome; physicians, who as a group
tend to be skeptical of the malpractice system, may have
been disinclined to credit the system’s findings; and, in
fact, one quarter of error judgments diverged from the
litigation outcomes.
Third, the reliability of the error determination was
not high, and the CIs around the ␬ score are statistically
compatible with poor or marginal reliability. Diagnostic
errors are one of the most difficult types of errors to detect
reliably (31). Although reviewers may have included some
episodes of care in the study sample that were not “true”
errors, and may have overlooked some that were, we know
of no reason why the characteristics of such false-positive
results and false-negative results would differ systematically
from those we analyzed.
Fourth, malpractice claims data generally, and in our
sample in particular, have several other biases. Severe injuries and younger patients are overrepresented in the subset
of patients with medical injuries that trigger litigation (32,
33). It is possible that the factors that lead to errors in
litigated cases may differ systematically from the factors
that lead to errors in nonlitigated cases, although we know
of no reason why they would. In addition, outpatient clinics associated with teaching hospitals are overrepresented in
our sample, so the diagnostic errors we identified may not
be generalizable outside this setting.
Conclusions
Our findings highlight the complexity of diagnostic
errors in ambulatory care. Just as Reason’s “Swiss cheese”
model of accident causation suggests (19) diagnostic errors
that harm patients seem to result from the alignment of
multiple breakdowns, which in turn stem from a confluence of contributing factors. The task of effecting meaningful improvements to the diagnostic process—with its
numerous clinical steps, stretched across multiple providers
and months or years, and the heavy reliance on patient
initiative—looms as a formidable challenge. The prospects
for “silver bullets” in this area seem remote.
Are meaningful gains achievable in the short to medium term? The answer will probably turn on whether
simple interventions that target 2 or 3 critical breakdown
points are sufficient to disrupt the causal chain or whether
interventions at a wider range of points are necessary to
avert harm. In this sense, our findings are humbling, and
they underscore the need for continuing efforts to develop
the “basic science” of error prevention in medicine (34),
which remains in its infancy.
From Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard School of Public
Health, Boston, Massachusetts; University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas; and the Harvard Risk Management Foundation,
Cambridge, Massachusetts.
3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7 495
Article
Missed or Delayed Diagnoses
Note: Drs. Gandhi, Kachalia, and Studdert had full access to all of the
data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and
the accuracy of the data analysis.
Grant Support: This study was supported by grants from the Agency for
Healthcare Research and Quality (HS011886-03) and the Harvard Risk
Management Foundation. Dr. Studdert was also supported by the
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (KO2HS11285).
Potential Financial Conflicts of Interest: Employment: T.A. Brennan
(Aetna); Stock ownership or options (other than mutual funds): T.A. Brennan (Aetna); Expert testimony: T.A. Brennan.
Requests for Single Reprints: David M. Studdert, LLB, ScD, Depart-
ment of Health Policy and Management, Harvard School of Public
Health, 677 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115; e-mail,
studdert@hsph.harvard.edu.
Current author addresses and author contributions are available at www
.annals.org.
References
1. Haas JS, Cook EF, Puopolo AL, Burstin HR, Brennan TA. Differences in the
quality of care for women with an abnormal mammogram or breast complaint. J
Gen Intern Med. 2000;15:321-8. [PMID: 10840267]
2. Marcus AC, Crane LA, Kaplan CP, Reading AE, Savage E, Gunning J, et al.
Improving adherence to screening follow-up among women with abnormal Pap
smears: results from a large clinic-based trial of three intervention strategies. Med
Care. 1992;30:216-30. [PMID: 1538610]
3. Poon EG, Gandhi TK, Sequist TD, Murff HJ, Karson AS, Bates DW. “I
wish I had seen this test result earlier!”: dissatisfaction with test result management
systems in primary care. Arch Intern Med. 2004;164:2223-8. [PMID:
15534158]
4. Margo CE. A pilot study in ophthalmology of inter-rater reliability in classifying diagnostic errors: an underinvestigated area of medical error. Qual Saf
Health Care. 2003;12:416-20. [PMID: 14645756]
5. Bates DW, Cullen DJ, Laird N, Petersen LA, Small SD, Servi D, et al.
Incidence of adverse drug events and potential adverse drug events. Implications
for prevention. ADE Prevention Study Group. JAMA. 1995;274:29-34. [PMID:
7791255]
6. Leape LL, Bates DW, Cullen DJ, Cooper J, Demonaco HJ, Gallivan T, et al.
Systems analysis of adverse drug events. ADE Prevention Study Group. JAMA.
1995;274:35-43. [PMID: 7791256]
7. Chandra A, Nundy S, Seabury SA. The growth of physician medical malpractice payments: evidence from the national practitioner data bank. Health Aff
(Millwood). 2005;W5240-9. [PMID: 15928255]
8. Phillips RL Jr, Bartholomew LA, Dovey SM, Fryer GE Jr, Miyoshi TJ,
Green LA. Learning from malpractice claims about negligent, adverse events in
primary care in the United States. Qual Saf Health Care. 2004;13:121-6.
[PMID: 15069219]
9. Kern KA. Medicolegal analysis of bile duct injury during open cholecystectomy and abdominal surgery. Am J Surg. 1994;168:217-22. [PMID: 8080055]
10. Kern KA. Malpractice litigation involving laparoscopic cholecystectomy.
Cost, cause, and consequences. Arch Surg. 1997;132:392-8. [PMID: 9108760]
11. Kravitz RL, Rolph JE, McGuigan K. Malpractice claims data as a quality
improvement tool. I. Epidemiology of error in four specialties. JAMA. 1991;266:
2087-92. [PMID: 1920696]
12. Weiler PC, Hiatt HH, Newhouse JP, Johnson WG, Brennan T, Leape LL.
A Measure of Malpractice: Medical Injury, Malpractice Litigation, and Patient
496 3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7
Compensation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ Pr; 1993.
13. Studdert DM, Brennan TA, Thomas EJ. Beyond dead reckoning: measures
of medical injury burden, malpractice litigation, and alternative compensation
models from Utah and Colorado. Indiana Law Rev. 2000;33:1643-86.
14. Sowka M, ed. National Association of Insurance Commissioners. Malpractice Claims: Final Compilation. Brookfield, WI: National Association of Insurance Commissioners; 1980.
15. Thomas EJ, Studdert DM, Burstin HR, Orav EJ, Zeena T, Williams EJ, et
al. Incidence and types of adverse events and negligent care in Utah and Colorado. Med Care. 2000;38:261-71. [PMID: 10718351]
16. Kohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS. To Err Is Human. Building a Safer
Health System. Institute of Medicine. Washington, DC: National Academy Pr;
1999.
17. Brennan TA, Leape LL, Laird NM, Hebert L, Localio AR, Lawthers AG, et
al. Incidence of adverse events and negligence in hospitalized patients. Results of
the Harvard Medical Practice Study I. N Engl J Med. 1991;324:370-6. [PMID:
1987460]
18. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33:159-74. [PMID: 843571]
19. Reason J. Managing the Risks of Organizational Accidents. Brookfield, VT:
Ashgate; 1997.
20. Harvard Risk Management Foundation. Breast care management algorithm:
improving breast patient safety. Accessed at www.rmf.harvard.edu/files/flash
/breast-care-algorithm/index.htm on 9 August 2006.
21. Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to
minimize them. Acad Med. 2003;78:775-80. [PMID: 12915363]
22. Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing diagnostic errors in medicine:
what’s the goal? Acad Med. 2002;77:981-92. [PMID: 12377672]
23. Elstein AS, Schwarz A. Clinical problem solving and diagnostic decision
making: selective review of the cognitive literature. BMJ. 2002;324:729-32.
[PMID: 11909793]
24. Schoen C, Osborn R, Huynh PT, Doty M, Davis K, Zapert K, et al.
Primary care and health system performance: adults’ experiences in five countries.
Health Aff (Millwood). 2004;Suppl:W4-487-503. [PMID: 15513956]
25. Kawamoto K, Houlihan CA, Balas EA, Lobach DF. Improving clinical
practice using clinical decision support systems: a systematic review of trials to
identify features critical to success. BMJ. 2005;330:765. [PMID: 15767266]
26. Friedman CP, Gatti GG, Franz TM, Murphy GC, Wolf FM, Heckerling
PS, et al. Do physicians know when their diagnoses are correct? Implications for
decision support and error reduction. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:334-9.
[PMID: 15857490]
27. Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations. National
Patient Safety Goals. Accessed at www.jointcommission.org/PatientSafety/NationalPatientSafetyGoals/07_amb_obs_npsgs.htm on 9 August 2006.
28. Gandhi TK. Fumbled handoffs: one dropped ball after another. Ann Intern
Med. 2005;142:352-8. [PMID: 15738454]
29. Guthrie C, Rachlinski JJ, Wistrich AJ. Inside the judicial mind. Cornell Law
Rev. 2001;86:777-830.
30. LaBine SJ, LaBine G. Determinations of negligence and the hindsight bias.
Law Hum Behav. 1996;20:501-16.
31. Studdert DM, Mello MM, Gawande AA, Gandhi TK, Kachalia A, Yoon C,
et al. Claims, errors, and compensation payments in medical malpractice litigation. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:2024-33. [PMID: 16687715]
32. Burstin HR, Johnson WG, Lipsitz SR, Brennan TA. Do the poor sue more?
A case-control study of malpractice claims and socioeconomic status. JAMA.
1993;270:1697-701. [PMID: 8411499]
33. Studdert DM, Thomas EJ, Burstin HR, Zbar BI, Orav EJ, Brennan TA.
Negligent care and malpractice claiming behavior in Utah and Colorado. Med
Care. 2000;38:250-60. [PMID: 10718350]
34. Brennan TA, Gawande A, Thomas E, Studdert D. Accidental deaths, saved
lives, and improved quality. N Engl J Med. 2005;353:1405-9. [PMID:
16192489]
www.annals.org
Annals of Internal Medicine
Current Author Addresses: Drs. Gandhi and Kachalia and Ms. Yoon:
Division of General Internal Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital,
75 Francis Street, Boston, MA 02114.
Dr. Thomas: University of Texas Medical School at Houston, 6431
Fannin, MSB 1.122, Houston, TX 77030.
Ms. Puopolo: CRICO/RMF, 101 Main Street, Cambridge, MA 02142.
Dr. Brennan: 151 Farmington Avenue, RC5A, Hartford, CT 06156.
Dr. Studdert: Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115.
Author Contributions: Conception and design: T.K. Gandhi, E.J.
Thomas, T.A. Brennan, D.M. Studdert.
Analysis and interpretation of the data: T.K. Gandhi, A. Kachalia, C.
Yoon, D.M. Studdert.
W-136 3 October 2006 Annals of Internal Medicine Volume 145 • Number 7
Drafting of the article: T.K. Gandhi, A. Kachalia, T.A. Brennan, D.M.
Studdert.
Critical revision of the article for important intellectual content: T.K.
Gandhi, A. Kachalia, E.J. Thomas, T.A. Brennan, D.M. Studdert.
Final approval of the article: T.K. Gandhi, A. Kachalia, E.J. Thomas,
D.M. Studdert.
Provision of study materials or patients: A.L. Puopolo.
Statistical expertise: D.M. Studdert.
Obtaining of funding: D.M. Studdert.
Administrative, technical, or logistic support: A. Kachalia, A.L. Puopolo,
D.M. Studdert.
Collection and assembly of data: A. Kachalia, A.L. Puopolo, D.M.
Studdert.
www.annals.org
WHAT ARE THE FACTORS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO
DIAGNOSIS ERROR?
Mark Graber, MD et al.
Diagnostic Error in internal Medicine.
Arch Intern Med. 2005 Jul 11; 165(13):1493-9
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION
Diagnostic Error in Internal Medicine
Mark L. Graber, MD; Nancy Franklin, PhD; Ruthanna Gordon, PhD
Background: The goal of this study was to determine
the relative contribution of system-related and cognitive components to diagnostic error and to develop a comprehensive working taxonomy.
Methods: One hundred cases of diagnostic error in-
volving internists were identified through autopsy discrepancies, quality assurance activities, and voluntary reports. Each case was evaluated to identify systemrelated and cognitive factors underlying error using record
reviews and, if possible, provider interviews.
Results: Ninety cases involved injury, including 33
deaths. The underlying contributions to error fell into 3
natural categories: “no fault,” system-related, and cognitive. Seven cases reflected no-fault errors alone. In the
remaining 93 cases, we identified 548 different systemrelated or cognitive factors (5.9 per case). Systemrelated factors contributed to the diagnostic error in 65%
of the cases and cognitive factors in 74%. The most common system-related factors involved problems with poli-
cies and procedures, inefficient processes, teamwork, and
communication. The most common cognitive problems
involved faulty synthesis. Premature closure, ie, the failure to continue considering reasonable alternatives after an initial diagnosis was reached, was the single most
common cause. Other common causes included faulty
context generation, misjudging the salience of findings,
faulty perception, and errors arising from the use of heuristics. Faulty or inadequate knowledge was uncommon.
Conclusions: Diagnostic error is commonly multifac-
torial in origin, typically involving both system-related
and cognitive factors. The results identify the dominant
problems that should be targeted for additional research and early reduction; they also further the development of a comprehensive taxonomy for classifying diagnostic errors.
Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:1493-1499
Once we realize that imperfect understanding is the
human condition, there is no shame in being wrong,
only in failing to correct our mistakes.
George Soros
I
Author Affiliations:
Department of Veterans Affairs
Medical Center, Northport, NY
(Dr Graber); Departments of
Medicine (Dr Graber) and
Psychology (Dr Franklin), State
University of New York at Stony
Brook; and Department of
Psychology, Illinois Institute of
Technology, Chicago
(Dr Gordon).
Financial Disclosure: None.
In his classic studies of clinical
reasoning, Elstein1 estimated the
rate of diagnostic error to be approximately 15%, in reasonable
agreement with the 10% to 15%
error rate determined in autopsy studies.2-4 Considering the frequency and impact of diagnostic errors, one is struck by
how little is known about this type of medical error.5 Data on the types and causes of
errors encountered in the practice of internal medicine are scant, and the field
lacks both a standardized definition of diagnostic error and a comprehensive taxonomy, although preliminary versions
have been proposed.6-12
According to the Institute of Medicine, the most powerful way to reduce error in medicine is to focus on systemlevel improvements, 1 3 , 1 4 but these
interventions are typically discussed in regard to patient treatment issues. The possibility that system-level dysfunction could
also contribute to diagnostic errors has received little attention. Typically, diagnos-
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1493
tic error is viewed as a cognitive failing.7,15-17 Diagnosis reflects the clinician’s
knowledge, clinical acumen, and problemsolving skills.1,18 In everyday practice, clinicians use expert skills to arrive at a diagnosis, often taking advantage of various
mental shortcuts known as heuristics.16,18-21 These strategies are highly efficient, relatively effortless, and generally accurate, but they are not infallible.
The goal of this study was to clarify the
basic etiology of diagnostic errors in internal medicine and to develop a working taxonomy. To understand how these
errors arise and how they might be prevented in the future, we systematically examined the etiology of error using root
cause analysis to classify both systemrelated and cognitive components.
METHODS
Based on a classification used by the Australian Patient Safety Foundation, we defined diagnostic error operationally as a diagnosis that
was unintentionally delayed (sufficient information was available earlier), wrong (another
diagnosis was made before the correct one), or
missed (no diagnosis was ever made), as judged
from the eventual appreciation of more definitive information.
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Cases of suspected diagnostic error were collected from 5
large academic tertiary care medical centers over 5 years. To
obtain a broad sampling of errors, we reviewed all eligible cases
from 3 sources:
1. Performance improvement and risk management coordinators and peer review committees.
2. Voluntary reports from staff physicians and resident trainees.
3. Discrepancies between clinical impressions and autopsy findings.
Cases were included if internists (staff specialists or generalists or trainees) were primarily responsible for the diagnosis and
if sufficient details about the case and the decision-making process could be obtained to allow analysis. In all cases, details were
gathered from a review of the medical record, from fact-finding
information obtained in the course of quality assurance activities when available, and in 42 cases, involved practitioners were
interviewed, typically within 1 month of error identification. To
minimize hindsight bias, reviews of medical records and interviews used a combination of open-ended queries and a root
cause checklist developed by the Veterans Health Administration (VHA).22,23 The VHA instrument24,25 identifies specific flaws
in the standard dimensions of organizational performance.26,27 and
is well suited to exploring system-related factors. We developed
the cognitive factors portion of the taxonomy by incorporating
and expanding on the categories suggested by Chimowitz et al,11
Kassirer and Kopelman,12 and Bordage.28 These categories differentiate flaws in the clinician’s knowledge and skills, ability to gather
data, and ability to synthesize all available information into verifiable hypotheses. Criteria and definitions for each category were
refined, and new categories added, as the study progressed.
Case histories were redacted of identifying information and
analyzed as a team to the point of consensus by 1 internist and 2
cognitive psychologists to confirm the existence of a diagnostic
error and to assign the error type (delayed, wrong, or missed) and
both the system-related and the cognitive factors contributing to
the error.29 To identify 100 usable cases, 129 cases of suspected
error were reviewed, 29 of which were rejected. Definitive confirmation of an error was lacking in 19 cases. In 6 cases, the diagnosiswassomewhatdelayedbutjudgedtohavebeenmadewithin
an acceptable time frame. In 3 cases, the data were inadequate for
analysis, and 1 case was rejected because the error reflected an intentional act that violated local policies (wrong diagnosis of hyponatremia from blood drawn above an intravenous line).
Impact was judged by an internist using a VHA scale that
multiplies the likelihood of recurrence (1, remote; up to 4, frequent) by the severity of harm (1, minor injury; up to 4, catastrophic injury).24,25 A minor injury with a remote chance of
recurrence received an impact score of 1, and a catastrophic
event with frequent recurrence received an impact score of 16.
Close-call errors were assigned an impact score of 0, and psychological impact was similarly discounted. The relative frequency of error types was compared using the Fisher exact test.
The impact scores of different groups were compared by 1-way
analysis of variance, and if a significant difference was found,
group means were compared by a t test.
The study was approved by the institutional review board(s)
at the participating institutions. Confidentiality protections were
provided by the federal Privacy Act and the Tort Claims Act,
by New York State statutes, and by a certificate of confidentiality from the US Department of Health and Human Services.
RESULTS
We analyzed 100 cases of diagnostic error: 57 from quality assurance activities, 33 from voluntary reports, and
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1494
10 from autopsy discrepancies. The error was revealed
by tissue in 53 cases (19 autopsy specimens and 34 surgical or biopsy specimens), by definitive tests in 44 cases
(24 x-ray studies and 20 laboratory investigations), and
from pathognomonic clinical findings or procedure results in the remaining 3 cases. The diagnosis was wrong
in 38 cases, missed in 34 cases, and delayed in 28 cases.
IMPACT
Ten cases were classified as close calls, and 90 cases involved some degree of harm, including 33 deaths. The
clinical impact averaged 3.80±0.28 (mean±SEM) on the
VHA impact scale, indicating substantial levels of harm,
on average. The impact score tended to be lower in cases
of delayed diagnosis than in cases that were missed or
wrong (3.79±0.52 vs 4.76±0.40 and 4.47±0.46; P=.34).
Cases with solely cognitive factors or with mixed cognitive and system-related factors had significantly higher
impact scores than cases with only system-related factors (4.11±0.46 and 4.27±0.47 vs 2.54±0.55; P=.03 for
both comparisons). These 2 effects may be related, as delays were the type of error most likely to result from system-related factors alone.
ETIOLOGY OF DIAGNOSTIC ERROR
Our results suggested that diagnostic error in medicine
could best be described using a taxonomy that includes
no-fault, system-related ( Table 1 ), and cognitive
(Table 2) factors.
v No-fault errors
Masked or unusual presentation of disease
Patient-related error (uncooperative, deceptive)
v System-related errors
Technical failure and equipment problems
Organizational flaws
v Cognitive errors
Faulty knowledge
Faulty data gathering
Faulty synthesis
In 46% of the cases, both system-related and cognitive
factors contributed to diagnostic error. Cases involving only
cognitive factors (28%) or only system-related factors (19%)
were less common, and 7 cases were found to reflect solely
no-fault factors, without any other system-related or cognitive factors. Combining the pure and the mixed cases,
system-related factors contributed to the diagnostic error
in 65% of the 100 cases and cognitive factors contributed
in 74% (Figure). Overall, we identified 228 system-related
factors and 320 cognitive factors, averaging 5.9 per case.
The relative frequency of both system-related and cognitive factors varied with the source of the case and the
type of error involved. Cases identified from quality assurance reports and from voluntary reports had a similar prevalence of system-related factors (72% and 76%,
respectively) and cognitive factors (65% and 85%, respectively). In contrast, cases identified from autopsy discrepancies involved cognitive factors 90% of the time
(P⬎.50) and system-related factors only 10% of the time
(P⬍.001). Cases of delayed diagnosis had relatively more
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 1. System-Related Contributions to Diagnostic Error
No. of
Encounters
Type
Definition
Example
Technical (n = 13)
Test instruments are faulty, miscalibrated, or unavailable
13
Technical failure and
equipment problems
35
Clustering
33
Policy and procedures
32
Inefficient processes
27
Teamwork or
communications
Failure to share needed information or skills
23
Patient neglect
Failure to provide necessary care
20
Management
Failed oversight of system issues
18
Coordination of care
Organizational (n = 215)
Repeating instances of the same error type
Policies that fail to account for certain conditions or that
actively create error-prone situations
Standardized processes resulting in unnecessary delay;
absence of expedited pathways
8
Supervision
Clumsy interactions between caregivers or sites of care;
hand-off problems
Failed oversight of trainees
8
Expertise unavailable
Required specialist not available in a timely manner
7
Training and orientation
4
Personnel
0
External interference
Clinicians not made aware of correct processes, policy, or
procedures
Clinician laziness, rude behavior, or known to have
recurring problems with communication or teamwork
Interference with proper care by corporate or government
institutions
Wrong glucose reading on incorrectly calibrated
glucometer
Repeated instances of incorrect readings of x-ray studies
in the emergency department, by covering physicians;
radiologists not available; administration aware
Recurrent colon cancer missed: no policy to ensure
regular follow-up of patients after colon cancer surgery
9-Month delay in diagnosis of colon cancer, reflecting
additive delays in scheduling clinic visits, procedures,
and surgery
Alarming increase in PSA levels never effectively
communicated to a provider who had changed clinic
sites
Biopsy report of cancer never communicated to patient
who missed a clinic appointment
Multiple x-ray studies not read in a timely manner; films
repeatedly lost or misplaced
Delayed workup of pulmonary nodule; consultation
request lost and, when found, not acted on promptly
Delayed diagnosis of peritonitis; reinsertion of a
percutaneous gastric feeding tube not appropriately
supervised
Radiologist not available to read a key film on a holiday
evening
Delay in diagnosis of Wegener granulomatosis; staff not
aware that ANCA testing required special approvals
Clinician known to commonly skip elements of the physical
examination missed detection of gangrenous toes
None encountered
Abbreviations: ANCA, antineutrophil cytoplasmic autoantibody; PSA, prostate-specific antigen.
system-related errors (89%) and fewer cognitive errors
(36%) on average, and cases of wrong diagnosis involved more cognitive errors (92%) and fewer systemrelated errors (50%, P⬍.01 for both pairs).
NO-FAULT ERRORS
No-fault factors were identified in 44 of the 100 cases and
constituted the sole explanation in 7 cases. Eleven of these
cases involved patient-related factors, including 2 instances of deception (surreptitious self-injection of saliva, mimicking sepsis and denial of high-risk sexual activity, which delayed diagnosis of Pneumocystis carinii
pneumonia) and 9 cases involving delayed diagnoses related to missed appointments or instances in which patient statements were unintentionally misleading or incomplete. By far, the most common no-fault factor was
an atypical or masked disease presentation, encountered in 33 cases.
SYSTEM-RELATED CONTRIBUTIONS TO ERROR
In 65 cases, system-related factors contributed to diagnostic error (Table 1). The vast majority of these (215
instances) were related to organizational problems, and
a small fraction (13 instances) involved technical and
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1495
equipment problems. The factors encountered most often related to policies and procedures, inefficient processes, and difficulty with teamwork and communication, especially communication of test results. Many error
types were encountered more than twice in the same institution, an event we referred to as clustering.
COGNITIVE CONTRIBUTIONS TO ERROR
We identified 320 cognitive factors in 74 cases (Table 2).
The most common category of factors was faulty synthesis (264 instances), or flawed processing of the available information. Faulty data gathering was identified in
45 instances. Inadequate or faulty knowledge or skills were
identified in only 11 instances.
FAULTY KNOWLEDGE OR SKILLS
Inadequate knowledge was identified in only 4 cases,
each concerning a rare condition: (1) a case of missed
Fournier gangrene; (2) a missed diagnosis of calciphylaxis in a patient undergoing dialysis with normal levels of serum calcium and phosphorus; (3) a case of
chronic thrombotic thrombocytopenic purpura; and
(4) a wrong diagnosis of disseminated intravascular
coagulation in a patient ultimately thought to have
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 2. Cognitive Contributions to Diagnostic Error
No. of
Encounters
4
7
24
10
7
3
Type
Definition
Knowledge base
inadequate or
defective
Skills inadequate or
defective
Ineffective, incomplete,
or faulty workup
Ineffective, incomplete,
or faulty history and
physical examination
Faulty test or procedure
techniques
Failure to screen
(prehypothesis)
Example
Faulty Knowledge (n = 11)
Insufficient knowledge of relevant condition
Providers not aware of Fournier gangrene
Insufficient diagnostic skill for relevant
condition
Missed diagnosis of complete heart block:
clinician misread electrocardiogram
Faulty Data Gathering (n = 45)
Problems in organizing or coordinating
patient’s tests and consultations
Failure to collect appropriate information from
the initial interview and examination
Delayed diagosis of drug-related lupus: failure
to consult patient’s old medical records
Delayed diagnosis of abdominal aortic
aneurysm: incomplete history questioning
Standard test/procedure is conducted
incorrectly
Failure to perform indicated screening
procedures
Failure to collect required information owing
to poor interaction with patient
Wrong diagnosis of myocardial infarction:
electrocardiographic leads reversed
Missed prostate cancer: rectal examination
and PSA testing never performed in a
55-year-old man
Missed CNS contusion after very abbreviated
history; perjorative questioning
Faulty Synthesis: Faulty Information Processing (n = 159)
Lack of awareness/consideration of aspects of
patient’s situation that are relevant to
diagnosis
Clinician is aware of symptom but either
focuses too closely on it to the exclusion of
others or fails to appreciate its relevance
Missed perforated ulcer in a patient
presenting with chest pain and laboratory
evidence of myocardial infarction
Wrong diagnosis of sepsis in a patient with
stable leukocytosis in the setting of
myelodysplastic syndrome
1
Poor etiquette leading to
poor data quality
26
Faulty context generation
25
23
Overestimating or
underestimating
usefulness or salience
of a finding
Faulty detection or
perception
Failed heuristics
15
Failure to act sooner
14
Faulty triggering
11
Misidentification of a
symptom or sign
10
Distraction by other
goals or issues
10
Faulty interpretation of a
test result
Other aspects of patient treatment (eg, dealing
with an earlier condition) are allowed to
obscure diagnostic process for current
condition
Test results are read correctly, but incorrect
conclusions are drawn
0
Reporting or
remembering findings
not gathered
Symptoms or signs reported that do not exist,
often findings that are typically present in
the suspected illness
25
39
Premature closure
18
Failure to order or follow
up on appropriate test
Symptom, sign, or finding should be
noticeable, but clinician misses it
Failure to apply appropriate rule of thumb, or
overapplication of such a rule under
inappropriate/atypical circumstances
Delay in appropriate data-analysis activity
Clinician considers inappropriate conclusion
based on current data or fails to consider
conclusion reasonable from data
One symptom is mistaken for another
Faulty Synthesis: Faulty Verification (n = 106)
Failure to consider other possibilities once an
initial diagnosis has been reached
Clinician does not use an appropriate test to
confirm a diagnosis or does not take
appropriate next step after test
Missed pneumothorax on chest radiograph
Wrong diagnosis of bronchitis in a patient
later found to have pulmonary embolism
Missed diagnosis of ischemic bowel in a
patient with a 12-week history of bloody
diarrhea
Wrong diagnosis of pneumonia in a patient
with hemoptysis: never considered the
eventual diagnosis of vasculitis
Missed cancer of the pancreas in a patient
with pain radiating to the back, attributed to
GERD
Wrong diagnosis of panic disorder: patient
with a history of schizophrenia presenting
with abnormal mental status and found to
have CNS metastases
Missed diagnosis of Clostridium difficile
enteritis in a patient with a negative stool
test result
None encountered
Wrong diagnosis of musculoskeletal pain after
a car crash: ruptured spleen ultimately
found
Wrong diagnosis of urosepsis in a patient:
bedside urinalysis never performed
Continued
clopidogrel-associated thrombotic thrombocytopenic
purpura. The 7 cases involving inadequate skills involved misinterpretations of x-ray studies and electrocardiograms by nonexperts.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1496
FAULTY DATA GATHERING
The dominant cause of error in the faulty datagathering category lay in the subcategory of ineffective,
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table 2. Cognitive Contributions to Diagnostic Error (cont)
No. of
Encounters
Type
Definition
Example
15
Failure to consult
Appropriate expert is not contacted
10
Failure to periodically
review the situation
Failure to gather other
useful information to
verify diagnosis
Overreliance on
someone else’s
findings or opinion
Failure to validate
findings with patient
Failure to gather new data to determine whether
situation has changed since initial diagnosis
Appropriate steps to verify diagnosis are not taken
10
8
5
1
Confirmation bias
Failure to check previous clinician’s diagnosis
against current findings
Clinician does not check with patient concerning
additional symptoms that might
confirm/disconfirm diagnosis
Tendency to interpret new results in a way that
supports one’s previous diagnosis
Hyponatremia inappropriately ascribed to diuretics
in a patient later found to have lung cancer; no
consultations requested
Missed colon cancer in a patient with progressively
declining hematocrit attributed to gastritis
Wrong diagnosis of osteoarthritis in a patient found
to have drug-induced lupus after ANA testing
Outpatient followed up with diagnosis of CHF,
admitted with increased shortness of breath, later
found to have lung cancer as the cause
Wrong diagnosis of bone metastases in a patient
with many prior broken ribs
Wrong diagnosis of pulmonary embolism: positive
results on D-dimer test taken to support this
diagnosis in a patient with respiratory failure due
to ARDS and gram-negative sepsis
Abbreviations: ANA, antinuclear antibody; ARDS, adult respiratory distress syndrome; CHF, congestive heart failure; CNS, central nervous system;
GERD, gastroesophageal reflux disease; PSA, prostate-specific antigen.
incomplete, or faulty workup (24 instances). For example, the diagnosis of subdural hematoma was missed
in a patient who was seen after a motor vehicle crash because the physical examination was incomplete. Problems with ordering the appropriate tests and interpreting test results were also common in this group.
No-Fault Factors Only
(7%)
Both System-Related
and Cognitive
Factors
(46%)
System-Related
Error Only
(19%)
FAULTY INFORMATION SYNTHESIS
Faulty information synthesis, which includes a wide
range of factors, was the most common cause of cognitive-based errors. The single most common phenomenon was premature closure: the tendency to stop considering other possibilities after reaching a diagnosis.
Other common synthesis factors included faulty context generation, misjudging the salience of a finding,
faulty perception, and failed use of heuristics. Faulty
context generation and misjudging the salience of a
finding often occurred in the same case (15 of 25 instances). Perceptual failures most commonly involved
incorrect readings of x-ray studies by internists and
emergency department staff before official reading by a
radiologist. Of the 23 instances related to heuristics, 14
reflected the bias to assume that all findings were related to a single cause when a patient actually had more
than 1 condition. In 7 cases, the most common condition was chosen as the likely diagnosis, although a less
common condition was responsible.
COVARIATION AMONG FACTORS
Cognitive and system-related factors were found to often co-occur, and these factors may have led, directly or
indirectly, to each other. For example, a mistake relatively early on (eg, an inadequate history or physical examination) is likely to lead to subsequent mistakes (eg,
in interpreting test results, considering appropriate can(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1497
Cognitive Error Only
(28%)
Figure. The categories of factors contributing to diagnostic error in 100
patients.
didate diagnoses, or calling in appropriate specialists).
We examined the patterns of factors identified in these
100 cases to identify clusters of cognitive factors that
tended to co-occur. Using Pearson r tests and correcting
for the use of multiple pairwise analyses, we found several such clusters of cognitive factors. The more common clusters of 3 factors, all of which have significant
pairwise correlations within a cluster, were as follows:
• Incomplete/faulty history and physical examination; failure to consider the correct candidate diagnosis;
and premature closure
• Incomplete/excessive data gathering; bias toward a
single explanation; and premature closure
• Underestimating the usefulness of a finding; premature closure; and failure to consult
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
COMMENT
In classifying the underlying factors contributing to error, 3 natural categories emerged: no fault, systemrelated, and cognitive. This classification validates the cognitive and no-fault distinctions described by Chimowitz
et al,11 Kassirer and Kopelman,12 and Bordage28 and adds
a third major category of system-level factors.
A second objective was to assess the relative contributions of system-related and cognitive root cause factors. The results allow 3 major conclusions regarding diagnostic error in internal medicine settings.
DIAGNOSTIC ERROR IS TYPICALLY
MULTIFACTORIAL IN ORIGIN
Excluding the 7 cases of pure no-fault error, we identified an average of 5.9 factors contributing to error in each
case. Reason’s6 “Swiss cheese” model of error suggests
that harm results from multiple breakdowns in the series of barriers that normally prevent injury. This phenomenon was identified in many of our cases, in which
the ultimate diagnostic failure involved separate factors
at multiple levels of both the system-related and the cognitive pathways.
A second reason for encountering multiple factors in
a single case is the tendency for one type of error to lead
to another. For example, a patient with retrosternal and
upper epigastric pain was given a diagnosis of myocardial infarction on the basis of new Q waves in his electrocardiogram and elevated levels of troponin. The clinicians missed a coexisting perforated ulcer, illustrating
that if a case is viewed in the wrong context, clinicians
may miss relevant clues and may not consider the correct diagnosis.
SYSTEM FLAWS CONTRIBUTE COMMONLY
TO DIAGNOSTIC ERROR
System-related factors were identified in 65% of cases.
This finding supports a previous study linking diagnostic errors to system issues30 but contrasts with the prevailing belief that diagnostic errors overwhelmingly reflect defective cognition. The system flaws identified in
our study reflected far more organizational issues than
technical problems. Errors related to suboptimal supervision of trainees occurred, but uncommonly.
COGNITIVE ERRORS ARE A COMMON CAUSE
OF DIAGNOSTIC ERROR AND PREDOMINANTLY
REFLECT PROBLEMS WITH SYNTHESIS
OF THE AVAILABLE INFORMATION
Faulty data gathering was much less commonly encountered, and defective knowledge was rare. These results
are consistent with conclusions from earlier studies28,30
and from autopsy data almost 50 years ago: “ . . . mistakes were due not so much to lack of knowledge of factual data as to certain deficiencies of approach and judgment.”31 This finding may distinguish medical diagnosis
from other types of expert decision making, in which
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1498
knowledge deficits are more commonly encountered as
the cause of error.32
As predicted by other authors,1,9,15,33 premature closure was encountered more commonly than any other
type of cognitive error. Simon34 described the initial stages
of problem solving as a search for an explanation that
best fits the known facts, at which point one stops searching for additional explanations, a process he termed satisficing. Experienced clinicians are as likely as more junior colleagues to exhibit premature closure,15 and elderly
physicians may be particularly predisposed.35
STUDY LIMITATIONS
This study has a variety of limitations that restrict the
generality of the conclusions. First, because the types of
error are dependent on the source of the cases,36-38 a different spectrum of case types would be expected outside internal medicine. Also, selection bias might be expected in cases that are reported voluntarily. Distortions
could also result from our nonrandomized method of case
selection if they are not representative of the errors that
actually occurred.
A second limitation is the difficulty in discerning exactly how a given diagnosis was reached. Clinical reasoning is hidden from direct examination, and may be
just as mysterious to the clinician involved. A related problem is our limited ability to identify other factors that likely
affect many clinical decision-making situations, such as
stress, fatigue, and distractions. Clinicians had difficulty recalling such factors, which undoubtedly existed. Their recollections might also be distorted because of the unavoidable lag time between the experience
and the interview, and by their knowledge of the clinical outcomes.
A third weakness is the subjective assignment of root
causes. The field as it evolves will benefit from further
clarification and standardization for each of these causes.
A final concern is the inevitable bias that is introduced
in a retrospective analysis in which the outcomes are
known.23,39,40 With this in mind, we did not attempt to
evaluate the appropriateness of care or the preventability of adverse events, judgments that are highly sensitive to hindsight bias.
STRATEGIES TO DECREASE DIAGNOSTIC ERROR
Although diagnostic error can never be eliminated,41 our
results identify the common causes of diagnostic error in
medicine, ideal targets for future efforts to reduce the
incidence of these errors. The high prevalence of
system-related factors offers the opportunity to reduce
diagnostic errors if health care institutions accept the
responsibility of addressing these factors. For example,
errors could be avoided if radiologists were reliably
available to interpret x-ray studies and if abnormal test
results were reliably communicated. Institutions should
be especially sensitive to clusters of errors of the same
type. Although these institutions may simply excel at
error detection, clustering could also indicate misdirected resources or a culture of tolerating suboptimal
performance.
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Devising strategies for reducing cognitive error is a more
complex problem. Our study suggests that internists generally have sufficient medical knowledge and that errors
of clinical reasoning overwhelmingly reflect inappropriate cognitive processing and/or poor skills in monitoring
one’s own cognitive processes (metacognition).42 Croskerry43 and others44 have argued that clinicians who are oriented to the common pitfalls of clinical reasoning would
be better able to avoid them. High-fidelity simulations may
be one way to provide this training.45,46 Elstein1 has suggested the value of compiling a complete differential diagnosis to combat the tendency to premature closure, the most
common cognitive factor we identified. A complementary
strategy for considering alternatives involves the technique of prospective hindsight: the crystal ball experience: The clinician would be told to assume that his or her
working diagnosis is incorrect, and asked, “What alternatives should be considered?”47 A final strategy is to augment a clinician’s inherent metacognitive skills by using
expert systems, an approach currently under active research and development.48-50
Accepted for Publication: February 21, 2005.
Correspondence: Mark L. Graber, MD, Medical Service
111, Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Northport, NY
11768 (mark.graber@med.va.gov).
Funding/Support: This work was supported by a research
support grant honoring James S. Todd, MD, from the
National Patient Safety Foundation, North Adams, Mass.
Acknowledgment: We thank Grace Garey and Kathy Kessel for their assistance with the manuscript and references.
REFERENCES
1. Elstein AS. Clinical reasoning in medicine. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, eds. Clinical
Reasoning in the Health Professions. Woburn, Mass: Butterworth-Heinemann;
1995:49-59.
2. Kirch W, Schafii C. Misdiagnosis at a university hospital in 4 medical eras. Medicine (Baltimore). 1996;75:29-40.
3. Shojania KG, Burton EC, McDonald KM, Goldman L. Changes in rates of autopsydetected diagnostic errors over time. JAMA. 2003;289:2849-2856.
4. Goldman L, Sayson R, Robbins S, Cohn LH, Bettmann M, Weisberg M. The value
of the autopsy in three different eras. N Engl J Med. 1983;308:1000-1005.
5. Graber M. Diagnostic error in medicine: a case of neglect. Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf. 2005;31:106-113.
6. Reason J. Human Error. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
7. Zhang J, Patel VL, Johnson TR. Medical error: is the solution medical or cognitive?
J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2002;9(suppl):S75-S77.
8. Leape L, Lawthers AG, Brennan TA, Johnson WG. Preventing medical injury. QRB
Qual Rev Bull. 1993;19:144-149.
9. Voytovich AE, Rippey RM, Suffredini A. Premature conclusions in diagnostic
reasoning. J Med Educ. 1985;60:302-307.
10. Friedman MH, Connell KJ, Olthoff AJ, Sinacore JM, Bordage G. Medical student
errors in making a diagnosis. Acad Med. 1998;73(suppl):S19-S21.
11. Chimowitz MI, Logigian EL, Caplan LR. The accuracy of bedside neurological
diagnosis. Ann Neurol. 1990;28:78-85.
12. Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. Cognitive errors in diagnosis: instantiation, classification, and consequences. Am J Med. 1989;86:433-441.
13. Institute of Medicine. To Err is Human; Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1999.
14. Spath PL. Reducing errors through work system improvements. In: Error Reduction in Health Care: A Systems Approach to Improving Patient Safety. San
Francisco, Calif: Jossey-Bass; 2000:199-234.
15. Kuhn GJ. Diagnostic errors. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9:740-750.
16. Croskerry P. Achieving quality in clinical decision making: cognitive strategies
and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9:1184-1204.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 165, JULY 11, 2005
1499
17. Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to
minimize them. Acad Med. 2003;78:775-780.
18. Kassirer JP. Diagnostic reasoning. Ann Intern Med. 1989;110:893-900.
19. Elstein AS. Heuristics and biases: selected errors in clinical reasoning. Acad Med.
1999;74:791-794.
20. McDonald CJ. Medical heuristics, the silent adjudicators of clinical practice. Ann
Intern Med. 1996;124:56-62.
21. Gigerenzer G, Goldstein DG. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded
rationality. Psychol Rev. 1996;103:650-669.
22. Johnson C. Designing forms to support the elicitation of information about incidents involving human error. Available at: http://www.dcs.gla.ac.uk/~johnson
/papers/incident_forms/. Accessed May 1, 2005.
23. Henriksen K, Kaplan H. Hindsight bias, outcome knowledge and adaptive learning.
Qual Saf Health Care. 2003;12(suppl):ii46-ii50.
24. Bagian JP, Gosbee J, Lee CZ, Williams L, McKnight SD, Mannos DM. The Veterans Affairs root cause analysis system in action. Jt Comm J Qual Improv. 2002;
28:531-545.
25. VA National Center for Patient Safety. Root cause analysis. Available at: www
.patientsafety.gov/tools.html. Accessed May 1, 2005.
26. Battles JB, Kaplan HS, Van der Schaaf TW, Shea CE. The attributes of medical
event-reporting systems. Arch Pathol Lab Med. 1998;122:231-238.
27. Reason J. Managing the Risks of Organizational Accidents. Brookfield, Vt: Ashgate Publishing Co; 1997.
28. Bordage G. Why did I miss the diagnosis? some cognitive explanations and educational implications. Acad Med. 1999;74(suppl):S128-S143.
29. Graber M. Expanding the goals of peer review to detect both practitioner and system error. Jt Comm J Qual Improv. 1999;25:396-407.
30. Weinberg NS, Stason WB. Managing quality in hospital practice. Int J Qual Health
Care. 1998;10:295-302.
31. Gruver RH, Freis ED. A study of diagnostic errors. Ann Intern Med. 1957;47:108-120.
32. Klein G. Sources of error in naturalistic decision making tasks. In: Proceedings
of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society: Designing for Diversity; October 11-15, 1993; Seattle, Wash.
33. Dubeau CE, Voytovich AE, Rippey RM. Premature conclusions in the diagnosis of
iron-deficiency anemia: cause and effect. Med Decis Making. 1986;6:169-173.
34. Simon HA. Invariants of human behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:1-19.
35. Eva KW. The aging physician: changes in cognitive processing and their impact
on medical practice. Acad Med. 2002;77(suppl):S1-S6.
36. Thomas EJ, Petersen LA. Measuring errors and adverse events in health care.
J Gen Intern Med. 2003;18:61-67.
37. O’Neil AC, Petersen LA, Cook F, Bates DW, Lee TH, Brennan TA. Physician reporting compared with medical-record review to identify adverse medical events.
Ann Intern Med. 1993;119:370-376.
38. Heinrich J. Adverse Events: Surveillance Systems for Adverse Events and Medical Errors. Washington, DC: US Government Accounting Office; February 9, 2000.
39. Caplan RA, Posner KL, Cheney FW. Effect of outcome on physician judgments
of appropriateness of care. JAMA. 1991;265:1957-1960.
40. Fischoff B. Hindsight does not equal foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge
on judgment under uncertainty. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1975;
1:288-299.
41. Graber M,. Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing diagnostic error in medicine: what’s
the goal? Acad Med. 2002;77:981-992.
42. Croskerry P. The cognitive imperative: thinking about how we think. Acad Emerg
Med. 2000;7:1223-1231.
43. Croskerry P. Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decision making. Ann Emerg
Med. 2003;41:10-120 1.
44. Hall KH. Reviewing intuitive decision-making and uncertainty: the implications
for medical education. Med Educ. 2002;36:216-224.
45. Satish U, Streufert S. Value of a cognitive simulation in medicine: towards optimizing decision making performance of healthcare professionals. Qual Saf Health
Care. 2002;11:163-167.
46. Bond WF, Deitrick LM, Arnold DC, et al. Using simulation to instruct emergency medicine residents in cognitive forcing strategies. Acad Med. 2004;79:438-446.
47. Mitchell DJ, Russo JE, Pennington N. Back to the future: temporal perspective
in the explanation of events. J Behav Decis Making. 1989;2:25-38.
48. Trowbridge R, Weingarten S. Clinical decision support systems. In: Shojania KG,
Duncan BW, McDonald KM, Wachter RM, eds. Making Health Care Safer: A Critical Analysis of Patient Safety Practices. Rockville, Md: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2001.
49. Hunt DL, Haynes RB, Hanna SE, Smith K. Effects of computer-based clinical decision support systems on physician performance and patient outcomes: a systematic review. JAMA. 1998;280:1339-1346.
50. Sim I, Gorman P, Greenes RA, et al. Clinical decision support systems for the practice of evidence-based medicine. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2001;8:527-534.
WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
Downloaded from www.archinternmed.com on August 12, 2008
©2005 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
WHAT ARE THE FACTORS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO
DIAGNOSIS ERROR?
Mark Graber MD.
Diagnostic errors in medicine: a case of neglect. Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf.
2005 Feb;31(2):106-13
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
Forum
Diagnostic Errors in Medicine:
A Case of Neglect
Mark Graber, M.D.
ore than 90 years have passed since Harvard’s
Dr. Richard Cabot illustrated the value of
autopsies to reveal diagnostic error.1 At his
weekly meetings with medical students, Cabot would
compare the clinical impressions before death with the
findings at autopsy, creating the powerful and enduring
teaching format known as the “CPC” (clinical-pathological correlation). From his experience with more than
3,000 cases, Cabot estimated that the clinical diagnosis
was correct less than half the time in patients dying of
thoracic aneurysms, cirrhosis, pericarditis, nephritis,
and a variety of other conditions. The reaction at the
time was one of incredulity.2 Dr. Alfred Croftan, a
Chicago physician, responded that “the overwhelming
majority of cases are today diagnosed correctly…” and
that the errors noted by Dr. Cabot could only reflect the
“unpardonably careless work” by the physicians of
Boston!3(p. 145)
Modern clinicians still struggle to recognize and
accept the possibility of diagnostic error. Although
many of the diseases on Dr. Cabot’s list can now be diagnosed with routine blood tests or imaging, diagnostic
error still exists and always will.4 Medical diagnoses that
are wrong, missed, or delayed make up a large fraction
of all medical errors and cause substantial suffering and
injury. Compared with other types of medical error,
however, diagnostic errors receive little attention. We
and others have previously proposed that this neglect is
a major factor in perpetuating unacceptable rates of
diagnostic error.4–6 In this article, I explore the reasons
for this neglect in detail. I propose that three factors
explain why diagnostic errors tend to be ignored. First,
these errors are fundamentally obscure. Diagnostic
errors are difficult to identify and when found are less
M
106
February 2005
Article-at-a-Glance
Background: Medical diagnoses that are wrong,
missed, or delayed make up a large fraction of all medical
errors and cause substantial suffering and injury.
Compared with other types of medical error, however,
diagnostic errors receive little attention—a major factor
in perpetuating unacceptable rates of diagnostic error.
Diagnostic errors are fundamentally obscure, health care
organizations have not viewed them as a system problem,
and physicians responsible for making medical decisions
seldom perceive their own error rates as problematic.
The safety of modern health care can be improved if
these three issues are understood and addressed.
Solutions: Opportunities to improve the visibility of
diagnostic errors are evident. Diagnostic error needs to
be included in the normal spectrum of quality assurance
surveillance and review. The system properties that contribute to diagnostic errors need to be systematically
identified and addressed, including issues related to reliable diagnostic testing processes. Even for cases entirely
dependent on the skill of the clinician for accurate diagnosis, health care organizations could minimize errors by
using system-level interventions to aid the clinician, such
as second readings of key diagnostic tests and providing
resources for clinical decision support. Physicians need
to improve their calibration by getting feedback on the
diagnoses they make. Finally, clinicians need to learn
about overconfidence and other innate cognitive tendencies that detract from optimal reasoning and learning.
Conclusion: Clinicians and their health care organizations need to take active steps to discover, analyze,
and prevent diagnostic errors.
Volume 31 Number 2
easily understood than other types of medical error.
Second, health care organizations have not viewed diagnostic errors as a system problem. Third, physicians
responsible for making medical decisions seldom perceive their own error rates as problematic. The safety of
modern health care can be improved if we understand
and address these three issues in an effort to make diagnostic errors visible.
Neglected Errors
Although clinicians routinely arrive at most of their diagnoses with ease and accuracy, diagnostic errors occur at
non-negligible rates. Diagnostic errors are encountered
in every specialty and every type of practice. It is estimated that mistakes are made in roughly 5% of radiology
and pathology diagnoses.7,8 Error rates in clinical practice are not known with certainty but might be as high as
12%, for example in the emergency department where
complex decision making is involved in settings of
above-average uncertainty and stress.9 Diagnostic errors
are easily demonstrated when physicians are tested with
standardized patients or scenarios.10,11 There is excess
variation between providers who analyze the same
case,12,13 and physicians at times even disagree with themselves when re-presented with a case they have previously diagnosed.14
Medical residents report that diagnostic errors are the
most common medical errors they encounter, and only a
minority of these are ever discussed with their mentors.15
In the Harvard Medical Practice Study, diagnostic errors
were the second leading cause of adverse events.16
Autopsies are considered the gold standard for detecting
diagnostic errors, and, indeed, discrepancies from the
clinical diagnosis are found in approximately one quarter of cases.17 Not all these discrepancies have clinical
relevance, but errors that potentially could have changed
the outcome are found in 5% to 10% of all autopsies.17,18
Despite this evidence that diagnostic errors are a
major issue in patient safety, they receive scant attention. This may reflect the general tendency for clinicians
to be unaware of medical error,19 but even within the
patient safety community diagnostic errors are barely on
the radar screen. Clinical decision making is an underfunded research area, and the topic is generally neglected in patient safety symposia and in medical school
February 2005
curricula. If we are ever to optimize patient safety, it will
require that we minimize diagnostic errors. As a start, we
need to understand why these errors are neglected.
Fundamental Issues
Although clinical decision making has been described
in general terms, at a fundamental level the process is
complicated, hidden, and only partially understood.20,21
Making a diagnosis is primarily a cognitive process and
is subject to influence by the affective state of the clinician.22 These processes are difficult to study and
quantify and to understand. Even the decision maker
may not be aware of how or why a given diagnosis was
reached. Experts in this field work in the area of cognitive psychology and discuss specialized concepts such
as “metacognition,” “heuristics,” and “debiasing.” It is
little wonder that practicing clinicians and patient safety staff have difficulty acquiring a comprehensive
understanding of clinical decision making, the first step
in trying to understand diagnostic errors. Root cause
analysis, so powerful in understanding other types of
medical error, is less easily applied when the root causes are cognitive.
Diagnostic errors often escape detection. It has been
estimated that for every error we detect, scores are
missed.23 Compared with adverse events related to surgery or other “process” and treatment errors, which are
often glaring and obvious, diagnostic errors tend to be
more subtle and difficult to pinpoint in time and place.
Detecting unacceptable delays in diagnosis is especially
difficult, because clear standards are lacking, and the
perceptions of the clinician regarding the existence of a
delay may differ substantially from the impressions of an
affected patient.
Physicians are uncomfortable discussing diagnostic
error. It is unsettling to consider the possibility that one’s
own diagnostic capabilities might be in question. This
reflects, in part, the real concern that diagnostic errors
can lead to career-threatening malpractice suits. A substantial proportion of these suits are provoked by diagnostic errors. In the Veterans Health Administration, tort
claims related to diagnostic errors were twice as common as claims related to medication errors.24 Malpractice
suits related to diagnostic errors involve all specialties
and are the most difficult to defend.25
Volume 31 Number 2
107
System Issues
Health care organizations view diagnostic error as
physician failure rather than as an institutional problem.
The available data, however, suggests that systemrelated factors commonly contribute to diagnostic error.
Emergency departments are particularly predisposed to
system-related diagnostic errors relating to workloadrelated stress, constant distractions, nonstandardized
processes, and a variety of associated issues.26 Similarly,
when diagnostic errors involve internists, both cognitive
and latent system flaws are typically identified as root
causes.27 The system-level processes most commonly
found in these cases involve cultural acceptance of suboptimal systems, inadequate policies and procedures,
inefficient processes, and problems with teamwork and
communication.
Diagnostic errors are simply not a priority for health
care organizations. No one has told them they should be.
Patient safety is an enormous universe, and many organizations look to expert advisory groups for advice on which
areas need attention. Unfortunately, the guidance provided
by advisory organizations has yet to identify diagnostic
error as an area in need of attention. The Joint
Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations
encourages facilities to develop their own patient safety
priorities using the tools of root cause analysis to investigate serious injuries.28 In practice, however, organizations
tend to focus on the initial seven National Patient Safety
Goals, none of which directly involves diagnostic error
(but see page 110). The Joint Commission also requires
organizations to study and report on 13 categories of sentinel event, but none of these address diagnostic accuracy,28 nor do the 27 reportable events advocated by the
National Quality Forum.29 Similarly, the Leapfrog Group, a
consortium of 145 organizations providing healthcare benefits, has issued patient safety priorities for healthcare
organizations, none directly involving medical diagnosis.30
The Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality recently
compiled evidence-based patient safety practices.31 Of the
25 measures with the strongest evidence base, none are
measures to improve diagnostic accuracy or timeliness.
Health care organizations have many incentives to
bypass thorny issues, such as diagnostic error, and
focus instead on “low-hanging fruit,” such as medication
errors and wrong-site surgery. These errors are easier to
108
February 2005
understand, interventions are more obvious, and the
organization can show progress towards achieving
patient safety goals.
Provider Issues
Although the act of making a medical diagnosis is a
defining skill of physicians, clinicians seldom think about
how well they carry out this function. Two questions
immediately arise: (1) Are clinicians aware that diagnostic
errors can occur? (2) Are they themselves susceptible to
this problem? The answer to the first question is unequivocally yes: Physicians are keenly aware that diagnostic
errors are made. Throughout their training and on a regular basis in their practice years, physicians learn of diagnostic errors via quality assurance proceedings and
malpractice suits. The fear of a malpractice suit relating to
diagnostic error is a major impetus for the practice of
defensive medicine—excessive referrals to specialists and
the use of expensive and redundant tests and procedures.32
Defensive practice is ubiquitous, and some estimate that it
accounts for 5% to 9% of health care expenditures in the
United States.33 Of direct relevance, this malpractice pressure likely has more significant impact on diagnostic decisions than management decisions.34
The answer to the second question is more interesting:
Although clinicians are aware of the possibility for diagnostic error and that they are personally at risk, they
believe that their own diagnoses are correct. Errors are
made by someone else. Clinicians can perhaps remember
a diagnostic error or two they’ve made in their career, but
when asked about their current patients and their current
diagnoses, my personal observation is that most believe,
like Dr. Croftan, that virtually all of these are accurate.
Indeed only 29% of physicians reported encountering any
medical error in the past year.35 Given the evidence that
the true error rate is in the range of 5% to 15%, how is it
possible that individual physicians believe that their own
error rates approach zero? Inadequate calibration and
overconfidence may help expain this contradiction.
Inadequate calibration. Across a wide spectrum
of skills and professions, receiving feedback on one’s
performance is an essential requirement for developing
expertise. Feedback provides the follow-up information
necessary to assess the accuracy of initial predications,
a process referred to as calibration. Professionals who
Volume 31 Number 2
are well calibrated agree with each other and correctly
interpret “gold standard” cases.
Physicians occasionally receive feedback regarding
some of their diagnoses as patients move through a diagnostic evaluation or return for follow-up after treatment.
Feedback is too sporadic, however, and lack of feedback
contributes to the physician’s sincere belief that the
great majority (or all) of their diagnoses are correct. A
variety of factors limit feedback.36 Some of the more
important factors follow:
■ Availability of definitive diagnostic information. The
true diagnosis is not known in every case. This would
include, for example, most diagnoses regarding less serious ailments; if a patient’s knee pain resolves after two
weeks of treatment with aspirin, the clinician may never
know if the correct diagnosis was arthritis, bursitis, or
tendonitis.
■ Communication barriers. If definitive diagnostic
information does become available at some later time,
the clinician making the initial diagnosis may no longer
be in the information loop. The clinician in an outpatient
setting may not be routinely informed if his or her initial
diagnosis is later modified in the hospital, or vice versa.
Even if the opportunity does arise to convey knowledge
of an error, clinicians feel awkward informing colleagues
their diagnoses were wrong.
■ Paucity of planned feedback. The autopsy rate in the
United States is now estimated to be less than 5%,
depriving clinicians of a unique calibration tool.
Although some medical specialty organizations provide
formal programs that provide and encourage feedback,
this is more the exception than the rule. Physicians are
rarely expected to show evidence that they have tracked
or processed feedback in any organized way.
Overconfidence. Lack of calibration contributes to
overconfidence, a second factor causing clinicians to
overestimate their diagnostic accuracy. There is substantial evidence that humans are overconfident in a
variety of settings; we routinely overestimate our ability
to function flawlessly.37 A classic example is eyewitness
testimony—the correlation between the accuracy of eyewitness identification and the confidence level of the
witness is generally less than 0.25.38 Another sobering
statistic is that only 1% of drivers rate their driving skills
below those of the average driver.39
February 2005
Just as we overestimate our skills and knowledge, we
are overconfident that our decisions, such as medical
diagnoses, are correct.37,40 Overconfidence in the setting
of medical decision making, however, is particularly
inappropriate given the general uncertainty that surrounds most diagnostic endeavors.
A chilling observation in studies of overconfidence is
that the least skilled are the most overconfident. In tests
of grammar, logic, and humor, the subjects scoring the
lowest by objective criteria are substantially more overconfident of their performance than subjects who score
well on these tests.41 Exactly the same phenomenon is
seen when medical residents estimate their skill in communicating with patients.42 Those residents least skilled
in communicating were exactly the ones least able to
judge their skill level and the most likely to overestimate
it. In the same vein, trainees dismissed from residency
training programs believed that they rarely made any
mistakes.43 This phenomenon fits well with current theories regarding the development of expertise. Experts not
only possess superior knowledge of their field but are
highly aware of their performance level and are less likely to exhibit overconfidence in regard to their performance. Superior metacognition and accurate calibration
are the hallmarks of an expert.
Overconfidence and poor calibration become selfperpetuating if our (inappropriate) certainty about our
diagnoses dissuades us from asking for autopsies.44 Is
our diagnostic acumen sharp enough to predict that an
autopsy will simply confirm our suspicions? The available evidence would suggest otherwise.45 For example,
in a recent study, clinicians were asked about the certainty of the diagnosis; fatal but potentially treatable
errors were found at autopsy in 10% of the cases
regardless of whether the clinicians were certain or
not.46
Physicians are uncomfortable with uncertainty. Partly
in response to pressures from our patients and partly to
satisfy our own unease, we assign a “working” diagnosis.
Typically, this is produced by a “cognitive disposition to
respond” without always ensuring that the process is rigorous and the product is accurate.47 Physicians are
expected to present a professional air of confidence and
expertise. Diagnostic errors fall in our blind spot—we
ignore them.
Volume 31 Number 2
109
Solutions
Opportunities to improve the visibility of diagnostic
errors are evident.
Fundamental Issues
Diagnostic error needs to be included in the normal
spectrum of quality assurance surveillance and review.
Staff responsible for organizational performance needs
simple definitions and working rules to identify, classify,
and study these errors. A simple working definition of
diagnostic error is those diagnoses that are missed,
wrong, or delayed, as detected by some subsequent definitive test or finding. The origins of these errors can be
classified by considering the provider-specific (cognitive)
elements, the system-related contributions, and “no fault”
elements reflecting diseases that present atypically or
involve excessive patient noncompliance.4,27 Medical
schools need to teach principles of optimal decision making and cognitive debiasing.47,48
More research on the nature of clinical decision making is needed to understand how errors arise and how
they can be prevented. Research specifically targeted at
diagnostic errors is currently being sponsored by the
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality,49 as well as
the National Patient Safety Foundation, and hopefully
future research funding can be identified to build on this
foundation.
System Issues
A host of system properties contributes to diagnostic
errors, and these need to be systematically identified and
addressed, including issues related to reliable processes
related to diagnostic testing, which are addressed in this
issue of the Joint Commission Journal on Quality and
Patient Safety.
Health care organizations need to accept partial
responsibility for diagnostic errors,50 and diagnostic
accuracy should be a concern of regulatory groups and
policy-guiding organizations. The Joint Commission has
taken a positive step in this direction by adding two
requirements to the 2005 National Patient Safety Goals
which are directed at improving the timeliness of reporting abnormal laboratory results and ensuring that critical abnormalities are communicated to a licensed
caregiver.51 These requirements address one of the more
110
February 2005
common system-related diagnostic errors—failure to
appropriately communicate abnormal test results.
Health care organizations would also be well advised to
address another common system-related condition that
predisposes to error: ensuring that specialty expertise is
available when needed, at all times and on all days.
Mandatory second opinions and computer-assisted
diagnosis have the potential to reduce errors in pathology
and radiology.52–56 Health care organizations need to evaluate the costs versus benefits of these novel interventions.
Even for those cases entirely dependent on the skill of
the clinician for accurate diagnosis, health care organizations could help minimize errors by using system-level
interventions to aid the clinician. For example, second
readings of key diagnostic tests improve diagnostic
accuracy,57 and clinicial decision support tools may be
helpful in a variety of settings. Similarly, efforts to
enhance feedback to clinicians regarding their diagnoses
would be beneficial. Organizations also have responsibility for training their clinicians in both the fundamentals of their science and in the diagnostic pitfalls that
contribute to errors.58
Provider Issues
Physicians need to improve their calibration by getting feedback on the diagnoses they make. Cognitive
feedback improves judgment and decisions made under
conditions of uncertainty.59 For example, feedback
regarding their diagnostic accuracy in reading standardized cases is thought to be a major factor explaining how
radiologists in the United Kingdom have reduced their
rate of diagnostic error.60
For the perceptual specialties (radiology, pathology,
and dermatology), feedback can be provided through
voluntary or mandated participation in quality assurance
procedures. For primary care physicians and specialists,
the pathway to enhanced feedback is not so clear-cut,
and creative ideas are needed to identify how to obtain
and provide this information.
The value of an autopsy as a feedback tool needs to be
rediscovered.61 Beyond identifying what diagnoses were
missed or wrong, the autopsy is the best tool we have to
combat the overconfidence that seems to be ubiquitous
in modern medicine. If providers and organizations are
unable to increase the autopsy rate voluntarily, autopsies
Volume 31 Number 2
could be required as a condition of Medicare participation or for Joint Commission certification.62 A novel strategy just emerging is the use of postmortem magnetic
resonance imaging examination as a supplement or alternative to autopsy.63 Grieving family members may view
this noninvasive procedure as a more acceptable alternative to autopsy.
Although they are less likely to feature autopsy findings than in Cabot’s day, conferences dedicated to reconciling clinical and pathological findings (CPCs)
continue to be warranted. Morbidity and mortality conferences, while ubiquitous today in virtually all medical
schools, need to be better structured to distill and learn
from diagnostic mistakes.64 An extension on this theme
is the growing number of sections in leading medical
journals featuring quality and safety discussions, and
the creative, multimedia “Web M&M” conferences
sponsored by the Agency for Healthcare Research and
Quality, presenting monthly cases of medical error in
internal medicine, surgery and anesthesiology, critical
care, and emergency medicine.65 These new resources,
like autopsies, can increase awareness of medical
error. It remains to be seen, however, whether they are
as effective a feedback tool, insofar as the autopsy
focuses on one’s own case while the literature and Web
resources focuses on the errors of others: “. . . the goal
of autopsy is not to uncover clinician’s mistakes or
judge them, but rather to instruct clinicians in the
sense of errando discimus (to be taught by one’s own
mistakes).”66(p. 37)
Finally, clinicians need to learn about overconfidence
and the many other innate cognitive tendencies that
detract from optimal reasoning and learning.6,22,37
Principles and pitfalls of clinical decision making need
to be incorporated into the medical school curriculum,48
and education must also reach the legions of practicing
February 2005
clinicians who have had inadequate exposure to these
concepts.
A potential concern is that feedback, though likely to
improve calibration, may not be well received. An interesting and relevant experiment is currently underway in
Major League Baseball, exploring the possibility that
umpires can be improve the consistency of their strike
calls by viewing videos of each pitch at the completion
of a game.67 The umpires have filed suit to stop the
experiment, viewing the process as an objectionable
intrusion. Similarly, some radiologists have resisted
mandatory self-assessment meant to improve the accuracy of reading mammograms.68 This tension between
autonomy and the need to improve calibration via feedback challenges efforts to increase diagnostic accuracy
through this approach, but perhaps new incentives can
be offered to make clinicians more interested in selfimprovement.
Conclusion
A bright light is now being focused on patient safety, but
diagnostic errors lie in the shadow. Clinicians and their
health care organizations both need to take active steps
to discover and analyze these errors. Diagnostic error
rates will fall as remediable problems are identified, but
the first step is to make these errors visible. J
The author thanks Pat Croskerry and Sam Campbell for suggesting this
topic; Nancy Franklin, Pat Croskerry, and Gordon Schiff for editorial
suggestions; and Grace Garey and Kathy Kessel for help obtaining and
organizing the references. This work was supported by a grant from the
National Patient Safety Foundation.
Mark Graber, M.D., is Chief, Medical Service, VA Medical
Center, Northport, New York, and Professor and Vice Chair,
Department of Medicine, State University of New York at
Stony Brook, New York. Please address requests for reprints
to Mark Graber, M.D., mark.graber@med.va.gov.
Volume 31 Number 2
111
References
1. Cabot R.C.: Diagnostic pitfalls identified during a study of three thousand autopsies. JAMA 59:2295–2298, Dec. 28, 1912.
2. Gore D.C., Gregory S.R.: Historical perspective on medical errors:
Richard Cabot and the Institute of Medicine. J Am Coll Surg
197:609–611, Oct. 2003.
3. Croftan A.C.: The diagnostic doubts of Dr. Cabot. JAMA 60:145, 1913.
4. Graber M., et al.: Reducing diagnostic error in medicine: What’s the
goal? Acad Med 77:981–992, Oct. 2002.
5. Kuhn G.J.: Diagnostic errors. Acad Emerg Med 9:740–750, Jul. 2002.
6. Croskerry P.: The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and
strategies to minimize them. Acad Med 78:775–780, Aug. 2003.
7. Foucar E., Foucar M.K.: Error in anatomic pathology. In: Foucar M.K.
(ed.): Bone Marrow Pathology, 2nd ed. Chicago: ASCP Press, 2000.
8. Fitzgerald R.: Error in Radiology. Clin Radiol 56:938–946, Dec. 2001.
9. O’Connor P.M., et al.: Unnecessary delays in accident and emergency
departments: Do medical and surgical senior house officers need to vet
admissions? J Accid Emerg Med 12:251–254, Dec. 1995.
10. Gorter S., et al.: Psoriatic arthritis: Performance of rheumatologists
in daily practice. Ann Rheum Dis 61:219–224, Mar. 2002.
11. Christensen-Szalinski J.J.J., Bushyhead J.B.: Physicians’ use of
probabalistic information in a real clinical setting. J Exp Psychol Hum
Percept Perform 7:928–935, Aug. 1981.
12. Margo C.E.: A pilot study in ophthalmology of inter-rater related
reliability in classifying diagnostic errors: An under-investigated area of
medical error. Qual Saf Health Care 12:416–420, Dec. 2003.
13. Beam C.A., et al.: Variability in the interpretation of screening mammograms by US radiologists: Findings from a national sample. Arch
Intern Med 156:209–213, Jan. 22, 1996.
14. Hoffman P.J., et al.: An analysis-of-variance model for the assessment of configural cue utilization in clinical judgment. Psychol Bull
69:338–349, May 1968.
15. Wu A.W. et al.: Do house officers learn from their mistakes? JAMA
265:2089–2094, Apr. 24, 1991.
16. Leape L. et al.: The nature of adverse events in hospitalized patients:
Results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study II. N Engl J Med
324:377–384, Feb. 7, 1991.
17. Shojania K.G., et al.: Changes in rates of autopsy-detected diagnostic errors over time. JAMA 289:2849–2856, Jun. 4, 2003.
18. The Autopsy as an Outcome and Performance Measure. Evidence
report/Technology assessment #58. AHRQ Publication No 03-E002.
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD, 2003.
19. Berwick D.M.: Errors today and errors tomorrow. N Engl J Med
348:2570–2572, Jun. 19, 2003.
20. Kassirer J.P.: Diagnostic reasoning. Ann Intern Med 110:893–900,
Jun. 1, 1989.
21. Elstein A.S.: Clinical reasoning in medicine. In: Higgs J.M. (ed.):
Clinical Reasoning in the Health Professions. Oxford, U.K.:
Butterworth-Heinemann, 1995, pp. 49-59.
22. Croskerry P.: The cognitive imperative: Thinking about how we
think. Acad Emerg Med 7:1223–1231, Nov. 2000.
23. Croskerry P., Wears R.L.: Safety errors in emergency medicine. In:
Markovchick V.J., Pons P.T. (eds.): Emergency Medicine Secrets.
Philadelphia: Hanley & Belfus, 2003, pp. 29–37.
24. Weeks W.B., et al.: Tort claims analysis in the Veterans Health
Administration for quality improvement. J Law Med Ethics 29:335–345,
Fall–Winter 2001.
25. Bartlett E.E.: Physicians’ cognitive errors and their liability consequences. J Healthc Risk Manag 18: 62–69, Fall 1998.
26. Adams J.G., Bohan J.S.: System contributions to error. Acad Emerg
Med 7:1189–1193, Nov. 2000.
112
February 2005
27. Graber M.L., et al.: Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Poster presented at the National Patient Safety Foundation Annual Meeting,
Boston, May 3–7, 2004.
28. Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations:
Reporting of Medical/Health Care Errors. A position statement of the
Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations. Nov.
8, 2003.
29. National Quality Forum: Serious Reportable Events in Healthcare:
A Consensus Report. National Quality Forum Publication # CR01-02.
Washington, D.C.: National Quality Forum, 2002.
30. The Leapfrog Group for Patient Safety. http://www.leapfroggroup.org
(last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
31. Shojania K.G., et al.: Making Health Care Safer: A Critical
Analysis of Patient Safety Practices. Evidence Report/Technology
Assessment No. 43 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and
Quality: AHRQ Publication No. 01-E058. Rockville, MD, 2001.
http://www.ahrq.gov/clinic/ptsafety/ (last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
32. Anderson R.E.: Billions for defense: The pervasive nature of defensive medicine. Arch Intern Med 159:2399–2402, Nov. 8, 1999.
33. Kessler D.P., McClellan M.B.: Do doctors practice defensive medicine? Q J Econ 111:353–390, 1996.
34. Kessler D.P., McClellan M.B.: How liability law affects medical productivity. J Health Econ 21:931–955, Nov. 2002.
35. Blendon R.J., et al.: Views of practicing physicians and the public on
medical errors. N Engl J Med 347:1933–1940, Dec. 12, 2002.
36. Croskerry P.: The feedback sanction. Acad Emerg Med 7:1232–1238,
Nov. 2000.
37. Parker D., Lawton R.: Psychological contribution to the understanding of adverse events in health care. Qual Saf Health Care
12:453–457, Dec. 2003.
38. Sporer L.E., et al.: Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A metaanalysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychol Bulletin 118:315–327, Nov. 1995.
39. Reason J.T., et al.: Errors and violation on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics 33:1315–1332, 1990.
40. Russo J.E., Schoemaker P.J.H.: Decision Traps: Ten Barriers to
Brilliant Decision-Making and How to Overcome Them. New York:
Doubleday/Currency, 1989.
41. Kruger J., Dunning D.: Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties
in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J Pers Soc Psychol 77:1121–1134, Dec. 1999.
42. Hodges B., et al.: Difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence: Novice physicians who are unskilled and unaware of it. Acad
Med 76(10 Suppl):S87–S89, Oct. 2001.
43. Hall J.C., et al.: Surgeons and cognitive processes. Br J Surg
90:10–16, Jan. 2003.
44. Gawande A. Final cut: Medical arrogance and the decline of the
autopsy. The New Yorker. 19, 2001, pp. 94–99.
45. Shojania K.G.: Diagnostic surprises and the persistent value of the
autopsy. AHRQ Web M&M 2004. http://webmm.ahrq.gov/cases.aspx?ic=54
(last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
46. Podbregar M. et al.: Should we confirm our clinical diagnostic certainty by autopsies? Intensive Care Med 27:1750–1755, Nov. 2001.
47. Croskerry P.: Achieving quality in clinical decision making:
Cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med
9:1184–1204, Nov. 2002.
48. Cosby K.S., Croskerry P.: Patient safety: A curriculum for teaching
patient safety in emergency medicine. Acad Emerg Med 10:69–78, Jan.
2003.
continued
Volume 31 Number 2
References, continued
49. Schiff G., et al.: Diagnostic Errors: Lessons from a MultiInstitutional Collaborative Project for the Diagnostic Error
Evaluation and Research Project Investigators. Rockville, MD:
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2004.
50. Nolan T.W.: System changes to improve patient safety. BMJ
320:771–773, Mar. 18, 2000.
51. Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations:
2005 Laboratory Service National Patient Safety Goals.
http://www.jcaho.org/accredited+organizations/patient+safety/
05+npsg/05_npsg_lab.htm (last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
52. Hunt D.L., et al.: Effects of computer-based clinical decision support systems on physician performance and patient outcomes: A systematic review. JAMA 280:1339–1346, Oct. 21, 1998.
53. Trowbridge R., Weingarten S.: Clinical decision support systems. In:
Shojania K.G., et al. (eds.): Making Health Care Safer: A Critical
Analysis of Patient Safety Practices. Evidence Report/Technology
Assessment No. 43 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and
Quality: AHRQ Publication No. 01-E058. Rockville, MD, 2001.
http://www.ahrq.gov/clinic/ptsafety/ (last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
54. Espinosa J.A., Nolan T.W.: Reducing errors made by emergency
physicians in interpreting radiographs: Longitudinal study. BMJ
320:737–740, Mar. 18, 2000.
55. Kripalani S., et al.: Reducing errors in the interpretation of
plain radiographs and computed tomography scans. In: Shojania K.G.,
et al. (eds.): Making Health Care Safer: A Critical Analysis
of Patient Safety Practices. Evidence Report/Technology Assessment
No. 43 from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality: AHRQ
Publication No. 01-E058. Rockville, MD, 2001. http://www.ahrq.gov/
clinic/ptsafety/ (last accessed Dec. 8, 2004).
56. Westra W.H., et al.: The impact of second opinion surgical pathology on the practice of head and neck surgery: A decade experience at a
large referral hospital. Head and Neck 24:684–693, Jul. 2002.
February 2005
57. Ruchlin H.S., et al.: The efficiency of second-opinion consultation
programs: A cost-benefit perspective. Med Care 20:3–19, Jan.
1982.
58. Croskerry P.: Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decision making. Ann Emerg Med 41:110–120, Jan. 2003.
59. Balzer W.K., et al.: Effects of cognitive feedback on performance.
Psychol Bull 106:410–433, 1989.
60. Smith-Bindman R., et al.: Comparison of screening mammography
in the United States and the United Kingdom. JAMA 290:2129–2137,
Oct. 22, 2003.
61. Hill R.B., Anderson R.E.: Autopsy: Medical Practice and Public
Policy. Oxford, U.K.: Butterworth-Heinemann, 1988.
62. Lundberg G.D.: Low-tech autopsies in the era of high-tech medicine:
Continued value for quality assurance and patient safety. JAMA
280:1273–1274, Oct. 14, 1998.
63. Patriquin L., et al.: Postmortem whole-body magnetic resonance
imaging as an adjunct to autopsy: Preliminary clinical experience. J
Magn Reson Imaging 13:277–287, Feb. 2001. Erratum in: J Magn
Reson Imaging 13:818, May 2001.
64. Pierluissi E., et al.: Discussion of medical errors in morbidity and
mortality conferences. JAMA 290:2838–4282, Dec. 3, 2003.
65. Agency for Healthcare Quality and Research: Welcome to AHRQ
Web M&M. http://webmm.ahrq.gov/default.aspx. (last accessed Dec. 8,
2004)
66. Kirch W., Schafii C.: Misdiagnosis at a university hospital
in 4 medical eras. Medicine (Baltimore) 75:29–40, Jan.
1996.
67. Silver N., Wooner K.: QuesTec not yet showing consistency from
umpires. ESPN Baseball. Jun. 5, 2003.
68. Maguire P.: Is an access crisis on the horizon in mammography?
ACP Observer 23:1, Oct. 2003. http://www.acponline.org/journals/
news/oct03/mammo.htm.
Volume 31 Number 2
113
DO PHYSICIANS KNOW WHEN THEIR DIAGNOSES
ARE CORRECT?
Charles P. Friedman, PhD et al.
Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct? Implications for Decision
Support and Error Reduction.
J Gen Intern Med 2005; 20:334-9.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
2941 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 800, Falls Church, VA 22042 T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com
JGIM
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
Implications for Decision Support and Error Reduction
Charles P. Friedman, PhD,1 Guido G. Gatti, MS,1 Timothy M. Franz, PhD,2
Gwendolyn C. Murphy, PhD,3 Fredric M. Wolf, PhD,4 Paul S. Heckerling, MD,5
Paul L. Fine, MD,7 Thomas M. Miller, MD,8 Arthur S. Elstein, PhD6
1
Q1
Center for Biomedical Informatics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; 2Department of Psychology, St. John Fisher College,
Rochester, NY, USA; 3Division of Community Health, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA; 4Department of Medical Education and
Informatics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA; Departments of 5Medicine and 6Medical Education, University of Illinois at
Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA; 7Department of Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; 8Department of Medicine, University of
North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA.
OBJECTIVE: This study explores the alignment between physicians’
confidence in their diagnoses and the ‘‘correctness’’ of these diagnoses,
as a function of clinical experience, and whether subjects were prone to
over-or underconfidence.
DESIGN: Prospective, counterbalanced experimental design.
SETTING: Laboratory study conducted under controlled conditions at
three academic medical centers.
PARTICIPANTS: Seventy-two senior medical students, 72 senior medical residents, and 72 faculty internists.
INTERVENTION: We created highly detailed, 2-to 4-page synopses of
36 diagnostically challenging medical cases, each with a definitive correct diagnosis. Subjects generated a differential diagnosis for each of 9
assigned cases, and indicated their level of confidence in each diagnosis.
MEASUREMENTS AND MAIN RESULTS: A differential was considered
‘‘correct’’ if the clinically true diagnosis was listed in that subject’s hypothesis list. To assess confidence, subjects rated the likelihood that
they would, at the time they generated the differential, seek assistance
in reaching a diagnosis. Subjects’ confidence and correctness were
‘‘mildly’’ aligned (k =.314 for all subjects, .285 for faculty, .227 for residents, and .349 for students). Residents were overconfident in 41% of
cases where their confidence and correctness were not aligned, whereas faculty were overconfident in 36% of such cases and students in
25%.
CONCLUSIONS: Even experienced clinicians may be unaware of the
correctness of their diagnoses at the time they make them. Medical decision support systems, and other interventions designed to reduce
medical errors, cannot rely exclusively on clinicians’ perceptions of
their needs for such support.
KEY WORDS: diagnostic reasoning; clinical decision support; medical
errors; clinical judgment; confidence.
DOI: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.30145.x
J GEN INTERN MED 2005; 20:000–000.
Q2
Accepted for publication October 28, 2004
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.
This report is based on a paper presented at the Medinfo 2001 conference in London, September, 2001.
Address correspondence and requests for reprints to Dr. Friedman:
Center for Biomedical Informatics, University of Pittsburgh, 8084 Forbes
Tower, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (e-mail: cpf@cbmi.pitt.edu).
Address for 2003–2005: Dr. Friedman, Division of Extramural Programs, National Library of Medicine, Suite 301, 6705 Rockledge Drive,
Bethesda, MD 20892 (e-mail: friedmc1@mail.nlm.nih.gov).
W
hen making a diagnosis, clinicians combine what they
personally know and remember with what they can access or look up. While many decisions will be made based on a
clinician’s own personal knowledge, others will be informed by
knowledge that derives from a range of external sources including printed books and journals, communications with
professional colleagues, and, increasingly, a range of computer-based knowledge resources.1 In general, the more routine
or familiar the problem, the more likely it is that an experienced clinician can ‘‘solve it’’ and decide what to do based on
personal knowledge only. This method of decision making uses
a minimum of time, which is a scarce and precious resource in
health care practice.
Every practitioner’s personal knowledge is, however, incomplete in various ways, and decisions based on incorrect,
partial, or outdated personal knowledge can result in errors. A
recent landmark study2 has documented that medical errors
are a significant cause of morbidity and mortality in the United
States. Although these errors have a wide range of origins,3
many are caused by a lack of information or knowledge necessary to appropriately diagnose and treat.4 The exponential
growth of biomedical knowledge and shortening half-life of any
single item of knowledge both suggest that modern medicine
will increasingly depend on external knowledge to support
practice and reduce errors.5
Still, the advent of modern information technology has not
changed the fundamental nature of human problem solving.
Diagnostic and therapeutic decisions, for the foreseeable future, will be made by human clinicians, not machines. What
has changed in recent years is the potential for computerbased decision support systems (DSSs) to provide relevant and
patient-specific external knowledge at the point of care, assembling this knowledge in a way that complements and enhances what the clinician decision maker already knows.6,7
DSSs can function in many ways, ranging from the generation
of alerts and reminders to the critiquing of management
plans.8–14 Some DSSs ‘‘push’’ information and advice to clinicians whether they request it or not; others offer no advice
until it is specifically requested.
The decision support process presupposes the clinician’s
openness to the knowledge or advice being offered. Clinicians
who believe they are correct, or believe they know all they need
to know to reach a decision, will be unmotivated to seek additional knowledge and unreceptive to any knowledge or sug1
2
Friedman et al., Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
gestions a DSS presents to them. The literatures of psychology
and medical decision making15–18 address the relationship
between these subjective beliefs and objective reality. The
well-established psychological bias of ‘‘anchoring’’15 stipulates
that all human decision makers are more loyal to their current
ideas, and resistant to changing them, than they objectively
should be in light of compelling external evidence.
This study addresses a question central to the potential
utility and success of clinical decision support. If clinicians’
openness to external advice hinges on their confidence in their
assessments based on personal knowledge, how valid are
these perceptions? Conceptually, there are 4 possible combinations of objective ‘‘correctness’’ of a diagnosis and subjective
confidence in it: 2 in which confidence and correctness are
aligned and 2 in which they are not. The ideal condition is an
alignment of high confidence in a correct diagnosis. Confidence and correctness can also be aligned in the opposing
sense: low confidence in a diagnosis that is incorrect. In this
state, clinicians are likely to be open to advice and disposed to
consult an external knowledge resource. In the ‘‘underconfident’’ state of nonalignment, a clinician with low confidence in
a correct diagnosis will be motivated to seek information that
will likely confirm an intent to act correctly. However, it is also
possible that a consultation with an external resource can talk
a clinician out of a correct assessment.19 The other nonaligned
state, of greater concern for quality of care, is high confidence
in an incorrect diagnosis. In this ‘‘overconfident’’ state, clinicians may not be open or motivated to seek information that
could point to a correct assessment.
This work addresses the following specific questions:
1. In internal medicine, what is the relationship between clinicians’ confidence in their diagnoses and the correctness
of these diagnoses?
2. Does the relationship between confidence and correctness
depend on clinicians’ levels of experience ranging from
medical student to attending physician?
3. To the extent that confidence and correctness are mismatched, do clinicians tend toward overconfidence or underconfidence, and does this tendency depend on level of
clinical experience?
One study similar to this one in design and intent,20 but
limited to medical students as subjects, found that students
were frequently unconfident about correct diagnostic judgments when classifying abnormal heart rhythms. Our preliminary study of this question has found the relationship
between correctness and confidence, across a range of training levels, to be modest at best.21
METHODS
Experimental Design and Dataset
To address these questions, we employed a large dataset originally collected for a study of the impact of decision support
systems on the accuracy of clinician diagnoses.19 We developed for this study detailed written synopses of 36 diagnostically challenging cases from patient records at the University
of Illinois at Chicago, the University of Michigan, and the University of North Carolina. Each institution contributed 12 cases, each with a firmly established final diagnosis. The 2-to 4page case synopses were created by three coauthors who are
JGIM
experienced academic internists (PSH, PSF, TMM). The synopses contained comprehensive historical, examination, and
diagnostic test information. They did not, however, contain results of definitive tests that would have made the correct diagnosis obvious to most or all clinicians. The cases were
divided into 4 approximately equivalent sets balanced by institution, pathophysiology, organ systems, and rated difficulty. Each set, with all patient-and institution-identifying
information removed, therefore contained 9 cases, with 3 from
each institution.
We then recruited to the study 216 volunteer subjects
from these same institutions: 72 fourth-year medical students,
72 second-and third-year internal medicine residents, and 72
general internists with faculty appointments and at least 2
years of postresidency experience (mean, 11 years). Recruitment was balanced so that each institution contributed 24
subjects at each experience level. Each subject was randomly
assigned to work the 9 cases comprising 1 of the 4 case sets.
Each subject then worked through each of the assigned cases
first without, and then with, assistance from an assigned computer-based decision support system. On the first pass
through each case, subjects generated a diagnostic hypothesis set with up to 6 items. After generating their diagnostic hypotheses, subjects indicated their perceived confidence in their
diagnosis in a manner described below. On the second pass
through the case, subjects employed a decision support system to generate diagnostic advice, and again offered a differential diagnosis and confidence ratings. After deleting cases
with missing data, the final dataset for this work consisted of
1,911 cases completed by 215 subjects.
Results reported elsewhere19 indicated that the computer-based decision support systems engendered modest but
statistically significant improvements in the accuracy of diagnostic hypotheses (overall effect size of .32). The questions addressed by this study, emphasizing the concordance between
confidence and correctness under conditions of uncertainty,
focus on the first pass through each case where the subjects
applied only their personal knowledge to the diagnostic task.
Measures
To assess the correctness of each clinician’s diagnostic hypothesis set for each case, we employed a binary score (correct
or incorrect). We scored a case as correct if the established diagnosis for that case, or a very closely related disease, appeared anywhere in the subject’s hypothesis set. Final scoring
decisions, to determine whether a closely related disease
should be counted as correct, were made by a panel comprised
of coauthors PSF, PSH, and TMM. The measure of clinician
confidence was the response to the specific question: ‘‘How
likely is it that you would seek assistance in establishing a diagnosis for this case?’’ ‘‘Assistance’’ was not limited to that
which might be provided by a computer. After generating their
diagnostic hypotheses for each case, subjects responded to
this question using an ordinal 1 to 4 response scale with anchor points of 1 representing ‘‘unlikely’’ (indicative of high confidence in their diagnosis) and 4 representing ‘‘likely’’
(indicative of low confidence). Because subjects did not receive
feedback, they offered their confidence judgments for each
case without any definitive knowledge of whether their diagnoses were, in fact, correct. Because they reflect only the subjects’ first pass through each case, these confidence judgments
JGIM
3
Friedman et al., Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
were not confounded by any advice subjects might later have
received from the decision support systems.
Analysis
In this study, each data point pairs a subjective confidence
assessment on a 4-level ordinal scale with a binary objective
correctness score. The structure of this experiment and the
resulting data suggested two approaches to analyzing the results. Given that each subject in this study worked 9 cases,
and offered confidence ratings on a 1 to 4 scale for each case,
interpretations of the meanings of these scale points might be
highly consistent for each subject but highly variable across
subjects. Our first analytic approach therefore sought to identify an optimal threshold for each subject to distinguish subjective states of ‘‘confident’’ and ‘‘unconfident.’’ This approach
addresses the ‘‘pooling’’ problem, identified by Swets and Pickett,22 that would tend to underestimate the magnitude of the
relationship between confidence and correctness. Our second
analytical approach took the assumption that all subjects
made the same subjective interpretation of the confidence
scale. This second approach entails a direct analysis of the
2-level by 4-level data with no within-subject thresholding.
Qualitatively, the first approach approximates the upper
bound on the relationship between confidence and correctness, while the second approach approximates the lower
bound.
To implement the first approach, we identified, for each
subject, the threshold value along the 1 to 4 scale that maximized the proportion of cases where confidence and correctness were aligned. With reference to Table 1, we sought to find
the threshold value that maximized the numbers of cases in
the on-diagonal cells. For 58 subjects (27%), we found that
maximum alignment was achieved by classifying only ratings
of 1 as confident and all other ratings as unconfident; for 105
subjects (49%), maximum alignment was achieved by classifying ratings of 1 or 2 as confident; and for the remaining 52
subjects (24%), maximum alignment was achieved by classifying ratings of 1, 2, or 3 as confident. This finding validated
our assumption that subjects varied in their interpretations of
the scale points. We then created a dataset for further analysis
that consisted, for each case worked by each subject, of a binary correctness score and a binary confidence score calculated using each subject’s optimal threshold.
To address the first research question with the first approach, we computed Kendall’s tb and k coefficients to characterize the relationship between subjects’ correctness and
confidence levels. We then modeled statistically the proportions of cases correctly diagnosed, as a function of confidence
(computed as a binary variable as described above), subjects’
level of training (faculty, resident, student), and the interaction
of confidence and training level. To address the second question, we modeled the proportions of cases in which confidence
and correctness were aligned, as a function of training level. To
address the third research question, we focused only on those
cases in which confidence and correctness were not aligned.
We modeled the proportions of cases in which subjects were
overconfident (high confidence in an incorrect diagnosis) as a
function of training level.
All statistical models used the Generalized Linear Model
(GzdLM) procedure23 assuming diagnostic correctness, alignment, and overconfidence to be distributed as Bernoulli variables with a logit link and used naive empirical covariance
estimates24 for the model effects to account for the clustering
of cases within subjects. Wald statistics were employed to test
the observed results against the null condition. Ninety-five
percent confidence intervals were calculated by transforming
logit scale Wald intervals using naive empirical standard error
estimates into percent scale intervals.25 The SPSS for Windows
(SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and SAS26 Proc GENMOD (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) software were employed for statistical
modeling and data analyses.
Our second approach offers a contrasting strategy to address the first and second research questions. To this end, we
computed nonparametric correlation coefficients (Kendall’s tb)
between the 2-level variable of correctness and the 4 levels of
confidence from the original data, without thresholding. We
computed separate tb coefficients for subjects at each experience level, and for the sample as a whole. Correlations were
computed with case as the unit of analysis after exploratory
analyses correcting for the nesting of cases within subjects led
to negligible changes in the results.
The power of the inferential statistics employed in this
analysis was based on the two-tailed t test, as the tests we
performed are analogous to testing differences in means on a
logit scale. Because our tests are based on a priori unknown
marginal cell counts, we halved the sample size to estimate
power. For the analyses addressing research question 1, which
use all cases, power is greater than .96 to detect a small to
Table 1. Crosstabulation of Correctness and Confidence for Each Clinical Experience Level and for All Subjects, with Optimal Thresholding
for Each Subject
Experience Level
Students
Residents
Faculty
All subjects
Correctness of Diagnosis
Correct
Incorrect
Total
Correct
Incorrect
Total
Correct
Incorrect
Total
Correct
Incorrect
Total
High
63
35
98
140
98
238
167
80
247
370
213
583
(55 to 71)
(27 to 43)
(72 to 132)
(129 to 150)
(88 to 109)
(193 to 287)
(155 to 178)
(69 to 92)
(205 to 300)
(352 to 388)
(195 to 231)
(527 to 667)
Cells contain counts of cases, with 95% confidence intervals in parentheses.
Confidence Low
105
442
547
141
259
400
144
237
381
390
938
1,328
(88 to 125)
(422 to 459)
(513 to 573)
(124 to 159)
(241 to 276)
(351 to 445)
(128 to 160)
(221 to 253)
(338 to 433)
(357 to 425)
(903 to 971)
(1,246 to 1,405)
Total
168 (146 to 192)
477 (453 to 499)
645
281 (256 to 306)
357 (332 to 382)
638
311 (293 to 339)
317 (299 to 346)
628
760 (713 to 808)
1,151 (1,103 to 1,198)
1,911
4
JGIM
Friedman et al., Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
moderate effect of .3 standard deviations at an a level of .05.
For analyses addressing research Qquestions 2 and 3, analyses that are based on subsets of cases, the analogous statistical power estimate is greater than .81.
1.0
Proportion Correct Diagnoses
0.9
RESULTS
Results with Threshold Correction
Table 1 displays the crosstabulation of correctness of diagnosis and binary levels of confidence (with 95% confidence interval) for all subjects and separately for each clinical
experience level, using each subject’s optimal threshold to dichotomize the confidence scale. The difficulty of these cases is
evident from Table 1, as 760 of 1,911 (40%) were correctly diagnosed by the full set of subjects. Diagnostic accuracy increased monotonically with subjects’ clinical experience. The
difficulty of the cases is also reflected in the distribution of the
confidence ratings, with subjects classified as confident for
583 (31%) of 1,911 cases, after adjustment for varying interpretations of the scale. These confidence levels revealed the
same general monotonic relationship with clinical experience.
Across the entire sample of subjects, confidence and correctness were aligned for 1,308 of 1,911 cases (68%), corresponding to Kendall’s tb =.321 (Po.0001) and a k value of .314.
Alignment was seen in 64% of cases for faculty (tb =.291
[Po.0001]; k =.285), 63% for residents (tb =.230 [Po.0001];
k =.227), and 78% for students (tb =.369 [Po.0001]; k =.349).
Figure 1 offers a graphical portrayal, for each experience
level, of the proportions of correct diagnoses as a function of
confidence, with 95% confidence intervals. The relationship
between correctness and confidence, at each level, is seen in
the differences between these proportions.
Wald statistics generated by the statistical model reveal a
significant alignment between diagnostic correctness and confidence across all subjects (w2 =199.64, df =1, Po.0001). Significant relationships are also seen between correctness and
training level (w2 =20.40, df =2, Po.0001) and in the interaction between confidence and training level (w2 =17.00, df =2,
Po.0002). Alignment levels for faculty and residents differ
from those of the students (Po.05); and from inspection of
Figure 1 it is evident that students’ alignment levels are higher
than those of faculty or residents.
With reference to the third research question, Table 2
summarizes the case frequencies for which clinicians at each
level were correctly confident—where confidence was aligned
with correctness—as well as frequencies for the ‘‘nonaligned’’
cases where they were overconfident and underconfident. Students were overconfident in 25% of nonaligned cases, corresponding to 5% of cases they completed. Residents were
overconfident in 41% of nonaligned cases, and 15% of cases
overall. Faculty physicians were overconfident in 36% of nonaligned cases, and 13% of cases overall.
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
Faculty
Residents
0.2
0.1
Students
0.0
Low
High
Low
Low
High
High
Diagnostic Confidence - Maximum Subject Calibration
FIGURE 1. Proportions of cases correctly diagnosed at each confidence level, with thresholding to achieve maximum calibration of
each subject. Brackets indicate 95% confidence intervals for each
proportion.
All subjects were more likely to be underconfident than
overconfident (w2 =29.05, Po.0001). Students were found to
be more underconfident than residents (Wald statistics:
w2 =6.19, df =2, Po.05). All other differences between subjects’ experience levels were not significant.
Results Without Threshold Correction
The second approach to analysis yielded Kendall tb measures
of association between the binary measure of correctness and
the 4-level measure of confidence, computed directly from the
study data, without any corrections. For all subjects and cases, we observed tb =.106 (N =1,911 cases; Po.0001). Separately for each level of training, Kendall coefficients are: faculty
tb =.103 (n =628; Po.005), residents tb =.041 (n =638; NS),
and students tb =.121 (n =645 cases; Po.001). The polarity
of the relationship is as would be expected, associating
correctness of diagnosis with higher confidence levels. The tb
values reported here can be compared with their counterparts, reported above, for the analyses that included threshold
correction.
DISCUSSION
The assumption built into the first analytic strategy, that subjects make internally consistent but personally idiosyncratic
interpretations of confidence, generates what may be termed
upper-bound estimates of alignment between confidence and
Table 2. Breakdown of Under- and Overconfidence for Cases in Which Clinician Confidence and Correctness Are Nonaligned
Q6
Experience
Level
Proportion of Cases that
Were Nonaligned
Students
Residents
Faculty
All subjects
140/645
239/638
224/628
603/1911
Parentheses contain 95% confidence intervals.
Percentage of Nonaligned Cases
Reflecting Underconfidence
75.0
59.0
64.3
64.7
(65.2
(50.7
(55.4
(59.5
to
to
to
to
82.8)
66.8)
72.3)
69.5)
Percentage of Nonaligned Cases
Reflecting Overconfidence
25.0
41.0
35.7
35.3
(17.2
(33.2
(27.7
(30.5
to
to
to
to
34.8)
49.3)
44.6)
40.5)
JGIM
Friedman et al., Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
correctness. Under the assumptions embedded in this analysis, the results of this study indicate that the correctness of
clinicians’ diagnoses and their perceptions of the correctness
of these diagnoses are, at most, moderately aligned. The correctness and confidence of faculty physicians and senior medical residents were aligned about two thirds of the time—and in
cases where correctness and confidence were not aligned,
these subjects were more likely to be underconfident than
overconfident. While faculty subjects demonstrated tendencies toward greater alignment and less frequent overconfidence than residents, these differences were not statistically
significant. Students’ results were substantially different from
those of their more experienced colleagues, as their confidence
and correctness were aligned about four fifths of the time and
more highly skewed, when nonaligned, toward underconfidence. The alignment between ‘‘being correct’’ and ‘‘being confident’’—within groups and for all subjects—would be
qualitatively characterized as ‘‘fair,’’ as seen by k coefficients
in the range .2 to .4.27
The more conservative second mode of analysis yielded
smaller relationships between correctness and confidence, as
seen in the tb coefficient for all subjects, which is smaller by a
factor of three. For the residents, the relationship between correctness and confidence does not exceed chance expectations
when computed without thresholding. Comparison across experience levels reveals the same trend seen in the primary
analysis, with students displaying the highest level of alignment.
The greater apparent alignment for the students, under
both analytic approaches, may be explained by the difficulty of
the cases. The students were probably overmatched by many
of these cases, perhaps guessing at diagnoses, and were almost certainly aware that they were overmatched. This is seen
in the low proportions of correct diagnoses for students and
the low levels of expressed confidence. These skewed distributions would generate alignment between correctness and confidence of 67% by chance alone. While students’ alignments
exceeded these chance expectations, a better estimate of their
concordance between confidence and correctness might be obtained by challenging the students with less difficult cases,
making the diagnostic task as difficult for them as it was for
the faculty and residents with the cases employed in this
study. We do not believe it is valid to conclude from these results that the students are ‘‘more aware’’ than experienced clinicians of when they are right and wrong.
By contrast, residents and faculty correctly diagnosed
44% and 50% of these difficult cases, respectively, and generated distributions of confidence ratings that were less skewed
than those of the students. In cases for which these clinicians’
correctness and confidence were not aligned, both faculty and
residents showed an overall tendency toward underconfidence
in their diagnoses. Despite the general tendency toward underconfidence, residents and faculty in this study were overconfident, placing credence in a diagnosis that was in fact
incorrect, in 15% (98/938) and 12% (80/928) of cases, respectively. Because these two more experienced groups are directly
responsible for patient care, and offered much more accurate
diagnoses for these difficult cases, findings for these groups
take on a different interpretation and perhaps greater potential
significance.
In designing the study, we approached the measurement
of ‘‘confidence’’ by grounding it in hypothetical clinical be-
5
havior. Rather than asking subjects directly to estimate their
confidence levels in either probabilistic or qualitative terms, we
asked them for the likelihood of their seeking help in reaching
a diagnosis for each case. We considered this measure to be a
proxy for ‘‘confidence.’’ Because our intent was to inform the
design of decision support systems and medical error reduction efforts generally, we believe that this behavioral approach
to assessment of confidence lends validity to our conclusions.
Limitations of this study include restriction of the task to
diagnosis. Differences in results may be seen in clinical tasks
other than diagnosis, such as determination of appropriate
therapy for a problem already diagnosed. The cases, chosen to
be very difficult and with definitive findings excluded, certainly
generated lower rates of accurate diagnoses than are typically
seen in routine clinical practice. Were the cases in this study
more routine, this may have affected the measured levels of
alignment between confidence and correctness. In addition,
this study was conducted in a laboratory setting, using written
case synopses, to provide experimental precision and control.
While the case synopses contained very large amounts of clinical information, the task environment for these subjects was
not the task environment of routine patient care. Clinicians
might have been more, or less, confident in their assessments
had the cases used in the study been real patients for whom
these clinicians were responsible; and in actual practice, physicians may be more likely to consult on difficult cases regardless of their confidence level. While we employed volunteer
subjects in this study, the sample sizes at each institution for
the resident and faculty groups were large relative to the sizes
at each institution of their respective populations, and thus
unlikely to be skewed by sampling bias.
The relationships, of ‘‘fair’’ magnitude, between correctness and confidence were seen only after adjusting each subject’s confidence ratings to reflect differing interpretations of
the confidence scale. The secondary analytic approach, which
does not correct individuals’ judgments against their own optimal thresholds, results in observed relationships between
correctness and confidence that are smaller. Under either set
of assumptions, the relationship between confidence and correctness is such that designers of clinical decision support
systems cannot assume clinicians to be accurate in their own
assessments of when they do and do not require assistance
from external knowledge resources.
This work was supported by grant R01-LM-05630 from the
National Library of Medicine.
REFERENCES
1. Hersh WR. ‘‘A world of knowledge at your fingertips’’: the promise, reality, and future directions of online information retrieval. Acad Med.
1999;74:240–3.
2. Kohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS, eds. To Err Is Human: Building
a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.
3. Bates DW, Gawande AA. Error in medicine: what have we learned? Ann
Intern Med. 2000;132:763–7.
4. Leape LL, Bates DW, Cullen DJ, et al. Systems analysis of adverse drug
events. ADE Prevention Study Group. JAMA. 1995;274:35–43.
5. Wyatt JC. Clinical data systems, part 3: development and evaluation.
Lancet. 1994;344:1682–7.
6. Norman DA. Melding mind and machine. Technol Rev. 1997;100:29–31.
7. Chueh H, Barnett GO. ‘‘Just in time’’ clinical information. Acad Med.
1997;72:512–7.
6
Friedman et al., Do Physicians Know When Their Diagnoses Are Correct?
8. Hunt DL, Haynes RB, Hanna SE, Smith K. Effects of computer-based
clinical decision support systems on physician performance and patient
outcomes. JAMA. 1998;280:1339–46.
9. Miller RA. Medical diagnostic decision support systems—past, present,
and future. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 1994;1:8–27.
10. Evans RS, Pestotnik SL, Classen DC, et al. A computer-assisted management program for antibiotics and other antiinfective agents. N Eng J
Med. 1998;338:232–8.
11. McDonald CJ, Overhage JM, Tierney WM, et al. The Regenstrief Medical Record System: a quarter century experience. Int J Med Inform.
1999;54:225–53.
12. Wagner MM, Pankaskie M, Hogan W, et al. Clinical event monitoring at
the University of Pittsburgh. Proc AMIA Annu Fall Symp. 1997;188–92.
13. Cimino JJ, Elhanan G, Zeng Q. Supporting infobuttons with terminological knowledge. Proc AMIA Annu Fall Symp. 1997;528–32.
14. Miller PL. Building an expert critiquing system: ESSENTIAL-ATTENDING. Methods Inf Med. 1986;25:71–8.
15. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and
biases. Science. 1974;185:1124–31.
16. Lichtenstein S, Fischhoff B. Do those who know more also know more
about how much they know? Organ Behav Hum Perform. 1977;20:
159–83.
17. Christensen-Szalanski JJ, Bushyhead JB. Physicians’ use of probabilistic information in a real clinical setting. J Exp Psychol. 1981;7:
928–35.
JGIM
18. Tierney WM, Fitzgerald J, McHenry R, et al. Physicians’ estimates of
the probability of myocardial infarction in emergency room patients with
chest pain. Med Decis Making. 1986;6:12–7.
19. Friedman CP, Elstein AS, Wolf FM, et al. Enhancement of clinicians’
diagnostic reasoning by computer-based consultation: a multisite study
of 2 systems. JAMA. 1999;282:1851–6.
20. Mann D. The relationship between diagnostic accuracy and confidence
in medical students. Presented at the annual meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Atlanta, 1993.
21. Friedman C, Gatti G, Elstein A, Franz T, Murphy G, Wolf F. Are Clinicians Correct When They Believe They Are Correct? Implications for
Medical Decision Support. Proceedings of the Tenth World Congress on
Medical Informatics. London; 2000. Medinfo. 2001;10(PP. 1): 454–8.
22. Swets JA, Pickett RM. Evaluation of Diagnostic Systems: Methods from
Signal Detection Theory. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1982.
23. McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed. New York,
NY: Chapman and Hall; 1991.
24. Liang K, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear
models. Biometrika. 1986;73:13–22.
25. Neter J, Kutner MH, Nachstsheim CJ, Wasserman W. Applied Linear
Regression Models. Chicago, IL: Irwin; 1996.
26. SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT User’s Guide, Version 8. Cary, NC: SAS
Institute Inc.; 1999.
27. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for
categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33:159–74.
Isabel Healthcare Inc
3120 Fairview Park Drive, Ste 200, Falls Church, VA 22042
T: 703 289 8855 E: info@isabelhealthcare.com