Yodels - Youngstown State University
Transcription
Yodels - Youngstown State University
Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” Given by Male Common Loons (Gavia immer) in Northern Wisconsin John N. Mager, III Department of Biological and Allied Health Sciences Ohio Northern University Ada, Ohio 45810 419. 772. 2333 j-mager@onu.edu Charles Walcott Department of Neurobiology and Behavior Cornell University Ithaca, New York 14853 ABSTRACT Although Common Loons (Gavia immer) are recognized for their dynamic vocal repertoire, we still know very little about the information that is communicated by these signals. Male loons give a vocalization called the yodel. We assessed the acoustic structural characteristics of the yodel and the ecological context in which males within a population of individuallybanded loons in northcentral Wisconsin yodeled. From behavioral observations during the course of the 2002 breeding season, we found that males yodeled mostly in response to other loons that had either flown over or landed upon their territories. Males also yodeled more frequently early in the breeding season when territories are being reestablished, and at the time when the chicks are hatching—a critical period with respect to annual reproductive success. These results confirm previous hypotheses that the yodel functions as a territorial threat signal. INTRODUCTION Common Loons (Gavia immer) are long-lived, seasonally-monogamous, migratory waterbirds that defend allpurpose breeding territories from both conspecific (Rummel and Goetzinger 1975, 1978, McIntyre 1988) and interspecific (Kennedy 1981, Sperrey 1987, Kirkham and Johnson 1988) intruders. One of the most charismatic features of this species is its dynamic vocal repertoire that consists of three long-range vocalizations: 327 328 Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” Figure 1. Sound spectrogram (above) showing the change in frequency (in kilohertz), and waveform (below) showing the change in energy (in micropascals) of a typical yodel over time (in seconds). Structurally, the yodel consists of two features: an introductory phrase of 3-4 notes that rise in frequency, and a motif of 2-syllable repeat phrases that follows the introductory phrase. the wail, the tremolo, and the yodel (see Barklow 1979, McIntyre 1988, Vogel 1995, and Lindsay 2002). Of these three, the yodel can be considered the “song” of the Common Loon in that it is (following Catchpole and Slater 1995) a long, complex vocalization given only by males during the breeding season primarily in response to conspecific intrusions. Structurally, the yodel is composed of two distinguishing features: an introductory phrase of 3–4 notes that rise in frequency, and a series of 2-syllable repeat phrases that follows the introductory phrase (Fig. 1). Hypotheses of the function of the yodel have ranged from those associated with mate attraction (Sim 1923, Sjølander and Årgen 1972) to those that currently consider it a territorial threat signal (Munro 1945, Rummel and Goetzinger 1975, 1978, Barklow 1979, Young 1983, McIntyre 1988, Wentz 1990, Vogel 1995, Lindsay 2002). Hypotheses regarding the information communicated by the yodel include individual or class (e.g., neighbor/non-neighbor) identity (Barklow 1979, 1979b, Miller 1988, Vogel 1995, Walcott et al. 1999, Walcott and Evers 2000, Lindsay 2002), internal “motivation” or willingness to escalate an aggressive interaction (Barklow 1979), and/or the physical condition (Lindsay 2002) of an individual. Although all of these hypotheses regarding the function of the yodel seem reasonable, they have yet to be tested with much rigor in the field. In the present study, we begin an investigation of these hypotheses by assessing the characteristics of yodels as well as the behavioral context in which males within a population of individually-banded loons in northern Wisconsin yodeled during the breeding season. 329 The Passenger Pigeon, Vol. 69, No. 3, 2007 METHODS Study Area We conducted this study during the 2002 breeding season within a group of roughly 200 lakes located within Oneida County, Wisconsin, northwest of the city of Rhinelander (latitude N 45° 42′, longitude W 89° 37′; see Meyer et al. (1998) for ecological characteristics). Since 1991, biologists have individually-banded, and have monitored the productivity, behavior, and environmental threats to this population (see Piper et al. 1997a, 1997b; 2000; 2002; Meyer et al. 1995; 1998), making it one of the longest continuously studied populations of banded loons in the country (Evers et al. 2000). Common Loons within this population are seasonally monogamous (Piper et al. 1997a), exhibit high territory and mate fidelity (Piper et al. 2000), and aggressively defend all-purpose breeding territories from conspecifics. Soon after the winter ice has melted, males return to the area and establish territories and form pair bonds with returning females. Breeding pairs begin nesting usually within a month after returning to their territories. Females lay 1–2 eggs per nesting attempt, and both parents share in incubating duties for 25–28 d (Strong and Hunsicker 1987; McIntyre 1988; Evers 1994; Mager 1995; Paruk 2000). Subprecocial chicks hatch asynchronously within a 24-h period, establish dominance, and are fed and protected from threats (including conspecifics) by both parents until they fledge 8–10 weeks after hatching (McIntyre 1988; McIntyre and Barr 1997; Mager 2000). Both male and fe- male loons frequently intrude on the territories of other loons during the breeding season. Such intrusions probably function as a way for intruders to assess the quality of a breeding territory (Piper et al. 2000). At times, intrusions escalate into dangerously aggressive interactions between individuals, and can lead to displacement or even death of individuals (Paruk 1999; Piper et al. 2000). Protocol for eliciting, recording, and measuring yodels. To acquire an adequate sample of yodels from each individual while minimizing disturbance, we gathered at least ten yodels from each male within the population each year. We obtained yodels from individuals by: (1) recording them in situ during 1-h time-activity budget behavioral samples in the field, and/or (2) by recording them as males responded to a series of broadcasted recordings of loon vocalizations (wails, tremolos, flight tremolos, and yodels) over no more than four 20-min sampling periods. To elicit yodels, we broadcast compact disk recordings of loon vocalizations through a RadioShack PowerHorn megaphone at ~70–80 dB from a distance of ~100 m from the focal subject, but adjusted the intensity of recordings as loons approached or retreated from the speaker. We recorded yodels onto digital audio tapes (DAT) using a Sennheiser MKH70 shotgun microphone connected to HHB PortaDAT recorder at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz, and then downloaded and converted these recordings into digital audio (AIFF) files using the Canary sound analysis software package (v. 1.5, Cornell Uni- Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” 330 Table 1. Measured and calculated parameters of yodels analyzed. Parameter Definition Frequency Parameters FINTRO1Uw Frequency (in kHz) with highest intensity at end of second harmonic of first note of introductory phrase (from Walcott, et al. 1999) FINTRO3w Frequency (in kHz) with highest intensity at end of third note of introductory phrase (from Walcott, et al. 1999) PFINTRO3 Peak frequency (in kHz) of entire third note of introductory phrase PFALLRPT Peak frequency (in kHz) of entire motif of repeat phrases PFRPTx Peak frequency (in kHz) of repeat phrase x PFS1RPTx Peak frequency (in kHz) of first syllable of repeat phrase x PFS2RPTx Peak frequency (in kHz) of second syllable of repeat phrase x Duration/Latency Parameters #R Number of repeat phrases in entire yodel DENT Duration (in seconds) of entire yodel DINTRO Duration (in seconds) of introductory phrase DINTROw Duration (in seconds) of second and third notes of introductory phrase (from Walcott, et al. 1999) DGAPx Latency (in seconds) of time before repeat phrase x DRPTx Duration (in seconds) of entire repeat phrase x versity Bioacoustics Research program). With this program, we used a fast Fourier transformation (using a 349.7 Hz bandwidth, 4096 points per frame, 50% overlap of frames in successive transforms, and a Hamming sampling window) to generate sound spectrograms from which we measured a number of frequency, latency, and duration parameters of each yodel (Table 1). Following previous work by Barklow (1979) and Vogel (1995), we also calculated a “ratio of coefficient of variation” (CV, or the standard deviation expressed as a percentage of the mean, calculated as (s × 100)/ Y, where s is the standard deviation and Y is the arithmetic mean of the data set—see Sokal and Rohlf 1997) between individuals (CVb)/ average CV within individuals (CVi) to determine those acoustic parameters that exhibit a low within-individual variability yet high between-individual variability. These, in turn, could help determine which parameters could be used by loons to discriminate individuals. Behavioral Observations We conducted behavioral observations of territorial pairs of loons during the entire 2002 breeding season (between 15 April, when winter ice melted, and 12 October, when all males had left their breeding territories and which was roughly 3 weeks before winter ice returned). Behavioral observations consisted of 1-hour continuous samples of focal pairs roughly 3-4 times each week and between 0430 and 1400h each day, where we recorded information regarding the type of yodel (crouch- or vultureposture, see McIntyre, 1988 for description), the intended recipient of the signal, and the number of repeat phrases given per yodel. We assessed weekly changes in yodeling rate (# yodels/hour of observation) both with Julian calendar date as well as the 331 The Passenger Pigeon, Vol. 69, No. 3, 2007 Table 2. Means and ranges of yodel frequency, duration, and latency measurements recorded from individual male Common Loons caught in Oneida County, Wisconsin, in 2002. Measurementa Mean ± 1 S.E. Frequency Parameter FINTRO1Uw FINTRO3w PFINTRO3 PFALLRPT PFRPT1 PFS1RPT1 PFS2RPT1 Range CVi CVb CVb CVi 1.480 1.803 1.787 1.737 1.741 1.795 1.705 ± 0.007 kHz ± 0.056 kHz ± 0.013 kHz ± 0.028 kHz ± 0.031 kHz ± 0.019 kHz ± 0.031 kHz 1.277–1.602 kHz 1.508–2.242 kHz 1.300–2.138 kHz 1.045–2.127 kHz 1.045–2.245 kHz 1.079–2.245 kHz 1.045–2.214 kHz 0.019 0.017 0.041 0.049 0.054 0.045 0.065 0.028 0.172 0.041 0.090 0.099 0.060 0.101 1.474 10.118 1.000 1.837 1.833 1.333 1.554 Duration/Latency Parameter #R 3.342 DENTYODEL 6.477 DENTINTRO 1.741 DINTROw 0.954 DGAP 0.305 DRPT1 1.131 ± 0.216 kHz ± 0.291 kHz ± 0.031 kHz ± 0.032 kHz ± 0.015 kHz ± 0.025 kHz 0–15 repeat phrases 1.672–17.083 sec 0.370–2.645 sec 0.213–1.936 sec 0.147–1.888 sec 0.270–1.832 sec 0.372 0.277 0.095 0.104 0.156 0.044 0.494 0.250 0.118 0.184 0.269 0.125 1.328 0.903 1.242 1.769 1.724 2.841 aRefer to Table 1 for definitions of yodel parameters. period within the breeding season relative to the hatching date. We assessed not only changes in seasonal rates of yodeling, but also whether these rates corresponded with conspecific intrusion by looking at the number of flyovers and intrusions upon the breeding territories. RESULTS Structural Characteristics of Yodels of Oneida County Males There was considerable variability in the frequency and timing characteristics of their yodels (Table 2). Some frequency parameters, such as FINTRO1Uw and FINTRO3w, showed very little within-individual variability (CVi, Table 2). Other parameters, such as the latency parameter DGAP, exhibited high between-individual variation (CVb, Table 2). Only one parameter, FINTRO3w, exhibited a CVb/CVi score significantly larger than the other parameters suggesting it might be used by loons for individual recognition. From a qualitative examination of spectrograms generated from recording the same yodel at various distances, those features of the sound which best resist attenuation with distance (i.e., 300–400 m) were the peak frequency and duration of the yodel that includes the third note of the introductory phrase and the motif of repeat phrases (Fig. 2). Behavioral Context in which Yodels Are Given Seasonally, males yodeled mainly between May and July, and rarely yodeled after their chicks were roughly nine weeks old, when presumably chicks are fledging from parental care (Fig. 3). The rate at which males yodeled peaked twice during 2002: (1) right after winter ice had melted from 332 Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” Figure 2. Acoustic spectrograms (plotting changes in frequency [in Kilohertz] over time [in seconds]) of a yodel recorded at various distances (from top to bottom, 50, 100, 200, 300, and 400 m.) over an open lake. Qualitatively, individual timing and latency elements are lost with the distance while dominant frequencies (those of the final note of the introduction and of the repeat phrases) and the entire duration of these frequencies are maintained. The Passenger Pigeon, Vol. 69, No. 3, 2007 lakes, and (2) at the time of year chicks were hatching (ANOVA F1,521 = 16.061, P < 0.0001, N = 24 males, Fig. 3). Most of the yodels given were in response to conspecifics (Fig. 4), and corresponded with times when conspecifics either flew over or intruded upon the resident male’s territory (coefficient of correlation, r = 0.183, F1,429 = 14.905, P < 0.0001, N = 24 males). DISCUSSION Empirical evidence from this study supports the notion that the yodel is a territorial signal that is given in response to territorial intrusions primarily by conspecifics. Seasonally, yodels were given most frequently when males returned to their breeding territories just after winter ice had melted. This is believed to be a period when males are not only establishing/reestablishing territories, but also establishing/ reestablishing pair bonds with mates. As such, in addition to a signal that declares ownership, there is the possibility that the signal also functions to reestablish pair bonds between males and females. This, in turn, would require a need for acoustic parameters within the territorial signal that would facilitate individual recognition. Like the studies by Barklow (1979) and Vogel (1995), analysis of individual frequency, duration, and latency elements of the yodels recorded in 2002 reflects frequency features of the yodel (particularly FINTRO3w, see Table 2) that exhibit low intra-individual variation and high inter-individual variation. As such, these features may be ideal candidates for communicating individual identity. However, other studies have shown 333 that there are individually-distinguishing suites, rather than individual acoustic features of the yodel, that might facilitate individual recognition (Walcott et al. 1999, Lindsay 2002, Mager 2005). Although the ability to distinguish individuals using these mathematical tools to analyze their yodels may lead to potential features of the yodel that might permit individual recognition among loons, it does not necessarily mean that the loons actually use these characteristics to recognize individuals. One way to determine if loons pay attention to these parameters is to use an acoustic playback study that compares the responses of individuals to yodels that vary in these potential characteristics. Data from two preliminary studies indicate that loons are able to discriminate between the yodels from territorial neighbors and non-neighbors (Lindsay 2002, Mager 2005) as well as the possibility that females are able to distinguish between the yodels of their mates and the yodels of non-mates (Vogel 1995). However, because no study has manipulated specifically the individual, or suite of potential acoustic individually-distinguishing features of the signal that have been identified in previous studies, it still is uncertain as to what feature loons are paying attention in order to discriminate between individual males. In addition to a peak in yodeling rate with the return and reestablishment of territories/mates, we detected a peak in the rate at which males yodeled during the breeding season at a point when chicks were about to hatch. This should not be surprising, as this period is one of the most critical of the breeding season to a breeding pair in terms of the risk of 334 Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” Figure 3. Bar graphs representing mean weekly yodeling rates of 24 male loons that fledged chicks during the 2002 breeding season. The top figure represents the weekly change in yodeling rate during the Julian calendar year beginning at the point of ice-out and ending 2-3 weeks prior to iceon, when a majority of males had already left for the winter. The bottom figure represents the mean weekly yodeling rates of the same individuals during the breeding season relative to the hatching date, with negative values on the x-axis representing the weeks prior to the day of hatching and positive values representing weeks after the day of hatching. Rates were determined from 580 1-hour behavioral sampling periods. reproductive failure, and might necessitate a period of greater awareness of potential intruders and vigilance in their removal. The nesting period is the most critical period of the breed- ing season, as the failure to successfully hatch eggs (not the failure of parents to nest, nor the failure of parents to successfully fledge hatched chicks) is the biggest reason why resi- 335 The Passenger Pigeon, Vol. 69, No. 3, 2007 Figure 4. Pie-chart of the perceived targets of loon yodels. Males yodeled primarily at other Common Loons, but also in response to human intrusions/disturbances. dent pairs experience reproductive failure during the breeding season (Mager 2005). As such, in terms of reproductive fitness, it is the period when loons have the most to lose. Therefore, loons should be the most attentive to territorial intrusions and protective of their nests/offspring during this period of the breeding season. This, in turn, can explain the peak in the number of yodels given by males during this period. The two acoustic parameters transmitted over the greatest distances were: (1) the peak frequency and duration of the yodel that includes the third note of the introductory phrase and (2) the number of repeat phrases. Acoustically, because the frequencies that are incorporated in these notes characteristically exhibit minimal attenuation (McIntyre 1994), it is not surprising that these parameters hold up better than others over a substantial distance. However, it makes one wonder if these two parameters of the yodel are important to long-distance territorial communication; and if so, it begs the question as to what information is communicated by the frequency and duration of this aspect of the yodel. In preliminary work, we have found that the pitch of the third note and the number of repeat syllables may communicate information regarding the relative fighting ability and aggressive “motivation” of each male, respectively. The peak frequency of this part of the yodel is correlated strongly with loon body weight and condition—larger males tend to produce lower-frequency yodels and loons appear to respond with more alarm to lower-frequency yodels (Mager et al. 2007). Since weight and condition are related to the ability of a male to acquire and hold onto a territory (Evers 2001), this frequency parameter may indeed communicate something about male fighting ability and/or condition. Additionally, each loon can increase the number of repeat syllables and thus vary the duration of the yodel. Loons produce 336 Structural and Contextual Characteristics of Territorial “Yodels” longer yodels when the value of a resource is enhanced or when the risk of retaliation increases (see Mager 2005). This finding supports Barklow’s hypothesis (1979) that yodel duration communicates information regarding the “motivation” or willingness to escalate a contest. Therefore, we believe that, with this part of the yodel, loons may be communicate both how strong a competitor they are and how “motivated” or willing they are to escalate a contest. In conclusion, it appears that the yodel is a territorial threat signal that is given both when males are reestablishing territories and pair bonds as well as during the most critical period of the breeding season in terms of reproductive fitness. An analysis of the variability in acoustic features of the yodel, as well as an analysis as to how the yodel attenuates over distances, suggests there are many characteristics of the yodel that would facilitate long-distance communication of identity, as well as the communication of individual fighting ability and motivation. Only with further analyses and playback studies can we elucidate the importance of these features in the signaling strategies of male loons. LITERATURE CITED Barklow, W. E. 1979. The function of variations in the vocalizations of the Common Loon (Gavia immer) (Ph.D. dissertation). Medford, Massachusetts: Tufts University. Bradbury, J. W. and S. L. Vehrencamp. 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates, Inc. Catchpole, C. K. and P. J. B. Slater. 1995. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press. Evers, D. C. 1994. Activity budgets of a marked Common Loon (Gavia immer) nesting population. Hydrobiologia 279/280: 415–420. Evers, D. C. 2001. Common Loon population studies: continental mercury patterns and breeding territory philopatry (Ph.D. dissertation). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota. Evers, D. C. 2006. Status assessment and conservation plan for the Common Loon (Gavia immer) Denver, Colorado: US Fish and Wildlife Service Technical Report Series. Evers, D. C., J. D. Kaplan, P. S. Reaman, J. D. Paruk, and P. Phifer. 2000. Demographic characteristics of the common loon in the Upper Great Lakes. Pages 78–90 in Loons: old history and new findings (J. McIntyre and D. C. Evers, editors). Holderness, New Hampshire: North American Loon Fund. Kennedy, A. J. 1981. Interspecific aggressive display by a Common Loon. The Murrelet 62: 20–21. Kirkham, I. R. and S. R. Johnson. 1988. Interspecific aggression in loons. Journal of Field Ornithology 59: 3–6. Lindsay, A. R. 2002. Molecular and vocal evolution in Loons (Aves: Gaviiformes) (Ph. D. dissertation). Ann Arbor, Michigan: The University of Michigan. Mager, J. N. 1995. A comparison of the timeactivity budgets of breeding male and female Common Loons (Gavia immer) (MS thesis). Oxford, Ohio: Miami University. Mager J. N. 2000. The effect of chick age and brood size upon parental effort within breeding Common Loons (Gavia immer). Pages 55–65 in Loons: old history and new findings (J. McIntyre and D. C. Evers, editors). Holderness, New Hampshire: North American Loon Fund. Mager J. N. 2005. What information is communicated by the territorial “yodel” of male Common Loons (Gavia immer) (Ph. D. dissertation). Ithaca, New York: Cornell University. Mager, J. N., C. Walcott, and W. H. Piper. 2007. Male Common Loons, Gavia immer, communicate body mass and condition through dominant frequencies of territorial yodels. Animal Behaviour 73: 683–690. McIntyre, J. W. 1988. The Common Loon: spirit of northern lakes. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. McIntyre, J. W. 1994. Loons in freshwater lakes. Hydrobiologia 279/280: 393–413. McIntyre, J. W. and J. F. Barr. 1997. Common Loon (Gavia immer). In The Birds of North America, No. 313 (A. Poole and F. Gill, editors). Philadelphia, Pennsylvania and Washington, DC: The Academy of Natural Sciences, and The American Ornithologists’ Union. Meyer, M. W., D. C. Evers, T. Daulton, and W. E. Braselton. 1995. Common Loons (Gavia The Passenger Pigeon, Vol. 69, No. 3, 2007 immer) nesting on low pH lakes in northern Wisconsin have elevated blood mercury content. Water, Air, and Soil Pollution 80: 871–880. Meyer, M. W., D. C. Evers, J. J. Hartigan, and P. S. Rasmussen. 1998. Patterns of Common Loon (Gavia immer) mercury exposure, reproduction, and survival in Wisconsin, USA. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 17: 184–190. Miller, E. 1988. Collection of yodel calls for individual identification of male Common Loons. Pages 44–52 in Papers from the 1987 Conference on Loon Research and Management (P. I. V. Strong, editor ). Meredith, New Hampshire: North American Loon Fund. Munro, J. A. 1945. Observations of the loon in the Cariboo Parklands, British Columbia. The Auk 62: 38–49. Paruk, J. D. 2000. Incubating roles and patterns in Common Loons (Gavia immer). Pages 50–54 in Loons: old history and new findings (J. W. McIntyre and D. Evers, editors). Holderness, New Hampshire: North American Loon Fund. Piper, W. H., D. C. Evers, M. W. Meyer, K. B. Tischler, J. D. Kaplan, and R. C. Fleischer. 1997a. Genetic monogamy in the Common Loon (Gavia immer). Behaviorial Ecology and Sociobiology 41: 25–31. Piper, W. H., J. D. Paruk, D. C. Evers, M. W. Meyer, K. B. Tischler, M. Klich, and J. J. Hartigan. 1997b. Local movements of colormarked Common Loons. Journal of Wildlife Management 61: 1253–1261. Piper, W. H., K. B. Tischler, M. Klich. 2000. Territory acquisition in loons: the importance of takeover. Animal Behavior 59: 385–394. Piper, W. H., M. W. Meyer, M. Klich, K. B. Tischler, and A. Dolsen. 2002. Floating platforms increase reproductive success in Common Loons. Biological Conservation 104: 199–203. Rummel, L. and C. Goetzinger. 1975. Communication of intraspecific aggression in common loons. Auk 92: 333–346. Rummel, L. and C. Goetzinger. 1978. Aggressive display in the common loon. Auk 95: 183–196. Sim, R. J. 1923. The Common Loon. Bird Lore 25: 167–175. Sjølander, S. and G. Årgen. 1972. Reproductive behavior of the common loon. The Auk 84: 296–308. Sokal, R. R. and F. J. Rohlf. 1997. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research, 3rd ed. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company. Sperrey, M. L. 1987. Common Loon attacks waterfowl. Journal of Field Ornithology 58: 201–205. 337 Strong, P. I. V. and L. Hunsicker. 1987. Sibling rivalry in common loon chicks. Passenger Pigeon 49: 136–137. Vogel, H. S. 1995. Individuality in, and discrimination through, the two-note wail and yodel calls of the common loon (Gavia immer) (MS thesis). Guelph, Ontario: University of Guelph. Walcott, C., D. C. Evers, M. Froehler, and A. Krakauer. 1999. Individuality in “yodel” calls recorded from a banded population of Common Loons, Gavia immer. Bioacoustics 10: 101–114. Walcott, C. and D. C. Evers. 2000. Loon vocal tagging and evaluation of its feasibility using a banded population of loons. Pages 35–42, in Loons: old history and new findings (J. W. McIntyre and D. C. Evers, editors). Holderness, New Hampshire: North American Loon Fund. Wentz, L. E. 1990, Aspects of the nocturnal vocal behavior of the Common Loon (Aves: Gavia immer) (Ph. D. dissertation). Columbus, Ohio: The Ohio State University. Wiley, R. H. and D. G. Richards. 1982. Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound propagation and signal detection. Pages 147–208 in Acoustic communication in birds, Volume 1 (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, editors). New York: Academic Press. Young, K. E. 1983. Seasonal and temporal patterning of Common Loon (Gavia immer) vocalizations (MS thesis). Syracuse: Syracuse University. Jay Mager teaches ecology and ornithology as an assistant professor of Biological & Allied Health Sciences at Ohio Northern University. He was formerly in the Department of Neurobiology and Behavior at Cornell University and has spent the past 16 years researching the life history and behavioral ecology of common loons, focusing on parental care strategies as well as the role acoustic communication plays when loons select breeding territories. Charlie Walcott is a professor of Neurobiology and Behavior at Cornell University and former director of the Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology. When not studying the ecology and acoustic behavior of loons, he serves at the Dean of the University Faculty at Cornell. Red-bellied Woodpecker at the suet was caught by John Krerowicz. 338