Men who have taken parental leave
Transcription
Men who have taken parental leave
Running head: MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE Reactions Toward Men who Have Taken Parental Leave: Does the Length of Parental Leave Matter? The final publication is available at Springer via http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0469-x MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 2 Abstract There is some evidence from research in the US that the work-related characteristics of men who take parental leave may be evaluated negatively. In 2007, Germany introduced a new law that encourages men to take parental leave by granting 2 extra months for the second parent to stay home. Since the introduction of this law, the proportion of fathers taking parental leave has increased substantially, but the majority of men take only the minimum of 2 months. We investigated how taking a parental leave affected perceptions of men who applied for a job that required high qualifications and whether a long parental leave of 12 months would lead to backlash effects. In two experimental studies, 203 students in business-related subjects (105 women, 98 men) in South-Western Germany rated vignettes of male applicants on gender role attributes (agency, communion) and work-related characteristics. The applicant took either 0, 2, or 12 months of parental leave. A parental leave resulted in higher communion and likeability ratings but did not make a difference for ratings of agency, respect, competence, or hiring probability. Contrary to our hypothesis, a long parental leave (12 months) compared with a short parental leave (2 months) did not lead to backlash toward the men. The results indicate that in Germany, a country where parental leave for fathers is encouraged, gender role attitudes have changed, and men thereby have more gender role options. Keywords: parental leave, fathers, gender role attributes, backlash, work-related characteristics MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 3 Reactions Toward Men who Have Taken Parental Leave: Does the Length of Parental Leave Matter? Introduction Today, men are still less likely than women to take parental leave, but they are becoming more involved. In Great Britain, a survey of 1,000 fathers revealed that more men were interested in taking parental leave than previously estimated (47% for 1 to 5 months and 21% for 6 months; Burgess, 2011). Interviews with 83 working fathers showed that fathers from both Great Britain and the US expressed a wish for more (paid) parental leave (Kaufman, Lyonette, & Crompton, 2010). In Germany, a representative poll of 1,012 randomly chosen fathers found that nearly half of them wanted more time to spend with their families (forsa Gesellschaft für Sozialforschung und statistische Analysen mbH, 2013). Another representative German study of 1,000 fathers revealed that the vast majority expressed the wish to help with their children’s development from the beginning and rated time with their families as very important (Väter gGmbH, 2012). Women also expect their partners to participate in childcare. A representative study of 1,000 participants in Austria showed that the overwhelming majority of women and men believe a good father contributes as much to child care as his partner (Kapella & Rille-Pfeiffer, 2007). However, there are still large differences between countries in the amount of parental leave taken by fathers. In several Nordic countries, leave rights for fathers have been developed in order to encourage more men to take parental leave (Lammi-Taskula, 2008). In Sweden, for example, since 2002, the law has provided 60 days specifically for men (Statistics Sweden, 2012). According to recent statistics, Swedish men comprise approximately 44% of parents who take parental leave, and they take around 24% of all parental leave days (Haas & Rostgaard, 2011; Statistics Sweden, 2012). In the US, the MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 4 situation is different because the Family and Medical Leave Act of 1993 applies only to a subset of all parents and ensures only an unpaid parental leave of 12 weeks for parents. In a study based on a representative sample in 2001, the majority of 4,638 fathers took only 1 week or even less parental leave (Nepomnyaschy & Waldfogel, 2007). In a nationally representative U.S. sample covering the period from 1988 to 2004, merely 3% of U.S. men had taken a parental leave of 1 month or more (Han, Ruhm, & Waldfogel, 2009). This paper explores the situation in Germany, where policy has changed from encouraging women to stay home with their child to encouraging parents to practice a more equal sharing of parental leave (a paradigm-shift according to Reich, 2010). On the basis of Social Role Theory and Backlash Theory, we investigated how men who take parental leaves of different lengths are evaluated according to gender- and work-related characteristics and whether they should expect negative psychological consequences. In two experimental studies, we asked German business students to rate scenarios in which an applicant who took or did not take parental leave was applying for a job. The main goal of our research was to determine how men who took parental leave would be judged in a country with a new father-friendly parental leave policy. In Germany, the Federal Child-Raising Allowance Act, which was in place until 2006, allowed men and women to take parental leave for up to 3 years after the birth of a child. However, the child-raising allowance, which was paid as compensation for lost wages, was not more than 300 Euros a month, thereby promoting a family with a working father and a child-rearing mother (Reich, 2010). Men constituted only a small minority of parents who took parental leave (3.5% in 2006, Ehlert & Rüling, 2008). In 2007, a new law — the Federal Parental Allowance and Parental Leave Act — was introduced. This law aimed to involve more fathers in early childcare by including two main incentives to encourage men to take MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 5 parental leave: a higher parental allowance to compensate for lost wages (two thirds of a person’s regular wage with an upper limit of 1800 Euros) and the introduction of a regulation that rewards parents who share parental leave (Bundesministerium der Justiz und für Verbraucherschutz, 2006). Since then, after the birth of a child, a parent (the mother or the father) can receive a parental allowance for a minimum of 2 and a maximum of 12 months. If both parents use the parental benefits for at least 2 months, 2 further months of paid parental leave are added, which are called partner months (or "Partnermonate" in German, BMFSFJ, 2013, p. 1). This means that the maximum financial support of 14 months can be achieved only if the father takes at least 2 months of parental leave. These incentives have worked very well, and after the new law was introduced, the number of fathers taking parental leave increased enormously; for children born in 2012, men comprised 23.3%. However, the majority of German fathers (78.2%) take only the minimum of 2 partner months (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2014). What are the reasons that fathers either do not take parental leave at all or take only a short parental leave? Both economic factors (e.g., compensation) and psychological factors (e.g., attitudes toward men who take parental leave) are likely to play a role. The influence of income has been demonstrated in several countries. For example, in a review, Seward, Yeatts, and Zottarelli (2002) stated that for men in the US and Sweden, an immediate (but also long-term) loss of income was one of the main reasons for not taking parental leave. In Germany, where parental leave covers 67% of regular pay up to 1800 Euros, men were more likely to take parental leave when they earned less than their partner and when their partner worked half or full time (Reich, 2010, based on the representative German Microcensus). MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 6 However, psychological factors are also likely to be involved. In the working world, women experience a penalty for motherhood (e.g. Heilman & Okimoto, 2008, U.S. student and working adult samples). Does a penalty also exist for fatherhood, and might this prevent men from taking parental leave? Several U.S. studies have investigated this possibility: Using samples of both students and employees, two studies found that mothers were perceived as less competent and suffered from hiring penalties, whereas fathers were rated as equally competent and at least as successful at getting hired (in comparison to childless women or men, Correll, Benard, & Paik, 2007; Heilman & Okimoto, 2008). Furthermore, women—but not men—were viewed as less competent when they were described as parents (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004, U.S. student sample). In comparison, fathers were sometimes even rated as more competent (Peplau & Fingerhut, 2004, U.S. student sample) and warmer than childless men (Cuddy et al., 2004, U.S. student sample). However, stay-at-home fathers were viewed more negatively than working fathers in adult samples (Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2005, U.S. adult samples). If a fatherhood penalty does not exist, why are fathers not taking more parental leave? The important factor of influence seems to be whether men who think about taking parental leave anticipate support versus barriers from their coworkers and job supervisors. In a study of 317 Swedish fathers working at six different companies, Haas, Allard, and Hwang (2002) found that perceived support from top managers and especially coworkers predicted whether and how long men took parental leave. An online survey of 1,290 fathers in Germany revealed that expectations of career barriers were negatively associated with parental leave (Vogt & Pull, 2010). Is there reason to assume that men who take parental leave will be judged negatively, especially in the working world? MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 7 Social Role Theory and Backlash Theory in the Context of Men’s Parental Leave Social Role Theory and Backlash Theory can be applied to predict possible consequences for men who take parental leave. Social Role Theory (Eagly, 1987; Eagly & Wood, 2012) postulates that social roles affect our behaviors, our characteristics, and our beliefs about the behaviors and characteristics of others. If people fulfil a role, this role information is used to estimate traits that match the role (Deaux & Lewis, 1984, U.S. student samples). In general, women are regarded as more communal than men because they more often have a domestic role and care for (young) children; men are regarded as more agentic because they have higher probabilities of working outside the home and for being in leadership positions (Eagly & Steffen, 1984, U.S. student and adult samples). There is broad empirical evidence that role information significantly influences attributions of a person’s agency and communion, and this is true irrespective of the gender of the person fulfilling the role, as was shown in samples of U.S. adults (Bosak, Sczesny, & Eagly, 2008; Eagly & Wood, 2012). Similarly, in German student samples, participants perceive employees who have jobs that require high qualifications to be masculine and to have agentic characteristics (Sczesny, 2003; Sczesny, Spreemann, & Stahlberg, 2006). Social Role Theory predicts that men who deliberately take parental leave will be evaluated as less agentic and more communal than men who continue to work after the birth of a child. On the one hand, fathers who take parental leave are expected to take on a domestic role and care for young children. To fulfil this role, communal characteristics are needed, so men who take parental leave should be rated higher on these characteristics. On the other hand, fathers who do not take parental leave are expected to place a higher value on their work role, so these men should be evaluated as more agentic (Bosak et al., 2008; Eagly & Wood, 2012). The evaluation of men who take different lengths of parental leave MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 8 should also vary. Men who take a long parental leave (12 months) will be depicted as embracing the domestic role for a longer period of time, and men who take a short parental leave (2 months) will be depicted as embracing the working role for a longer period. Therefore, men who take a short leave should be described as more agentic and less communal than men who take a long parental leave. The term backlash was coined by Rudman (1998) to refer to social and economic sanctions for counterstereotypical behavior and was primarily studied with respect to reactions to women’s behavior. Recently, some studies have also demonstrated backlash in U.S. student samples toward men who “break the gender rules” (Moss-Racusin, Phelan, & Rudman, 2010, p. 1): Supporting the status-incongruity hypothesis, one experimental study revealed that modest men encountered prejudice (Moss-Racusin et al., 2010); another experimental study found that men who were successful in a counterstereotypical job were respected less and rated as ineffectual (Heilman & Wallen, 2010). Taking parental leave might be considered counterstereotypical behavior for men in Germany as it is still mostly done by women (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2014). Therefore, taking parental leave should result in backlash. As backlash for men and women differs, typical backlash for men in a working setting should include less respect and being seen as less competent. Furthermore, both men and women who act in gender-counterstereotypical ways are preferred less as bosses, and this should lower their probability of being hired (Heilman & Wallen, 2010). However, these effects should be found only when stereotypes are violated, and this should occur only for a long parental leave (e.g., 12 months). Increasing numbers of men in Germany currently take short parental leaves of 2 months (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2012a). As stereotypes are dynamic (Diekman & Eagly, 2000, U.S. student and MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 9 working adult samples), a short parental leave cannot be viewed as violating the male stereotype anymore and should not elicit backlash. How are men who take parental leave evaluated in the working world? In the US, several experimental studies, all with (undergraduate) students as participants, have investigated the effects of parental leave on evaluations of men. A case vignette study found that a short parental leave of 3 months taken by a high-performing man did not lead to negative effects on such a man’s perceived organizational commitment or organizational rewards (Allen, Russell, & Rush, 1994). However, the same authors stated that men who took a rather long parental leave (6 months) were perceived as less committed to work and to the organization; they also received fewer recommendations for rewards (Allen & Russell, 1999). In a study examining organizational citizenship behavior, fathers (but not mothers) who had taken 12 weeks of parental leave were assessed as less altruistic at work. They were also rated as generally less compliant, but only by men (Wayne & Cordeiro, 2003). Another study discovered that men who took a 12-week parental leave were expected to be less successful but warmer than men who continued working (Coleman & Franiuk, 2011). A recent study that investigated perceptions of men who had taken a 12-week leave to care for a sick child indicated that they were rated higher on feminine traits and lower on masculine traits (Rudman & Mescher, 2013). Altogether, the results of these studies indicate that there might be backlash effects and altered judgments of agency and communion toward men who have taken parental leave, especially when the length of the leave was quite long. Furthermore, men are usually evaluated as less warm (and less likeable) than women in Germany (Asbrock, 2010). However, men who took parental leave were rated as warmer than men who did not in a MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 10 U.S. study (Coleman & Franiuk, 2011). Therefore, men who take parental leave may be assessed as more likeable than men who do not. There has been some research on the effects of men’s parental leave. However, prior studies did not compare the effects of different lengths of parental leave but usually contrasted taking a parental leave against working or staying at home (Coleman & Franiuk, 2011, U.S. student sample). When different lengths of parental leave were used, it was not within one study, and the results were contradictory (Allen & Russell, 1999; Allen et al., 1994, U.S. student samples). Another limitation of existing research is the fact that all of the studies cited above were conducted in the US, a country in which no paid parental leave is offered at all. It is likely that evaluations of men in other countries that offer and promote opportunities for men to take parental leave would be different. Gender Roles and Gendered Division of Labor in Germany In Germany, there has been a traditional division of labor up to and even after the end of the 20th century. Women are still mostly responsible for raising children, and men are still mostly responsible for working (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2003, 2010, 2014). For example, whereas the majority of men worked full time (80.4%) in 2006, the situation was completely different for women (43.7% full time, 32.6% part time). This can be explained by the fact that many—even highly qualified—women interrupt or reduce their work to part time after they start a family and is backed up by the large numbers of women who take parental leave (96% for 2007 to 2010, Statistisches Bundesamt, 2010, 2012a). In 2010, Germany’s ratio of women who worked part time was considerably above the EU average of 30.8% (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2012b). Time budget studies, which analyze the allocation of time on a daily basis, revealed that men spent more time on paid work, whereas women were more involved in domestic work and child care (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2003). MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 11 In Germany, men also have a much lower probability of interrupting their careers for family reasons. In a 15-year longitudinal study of German physicians, Evers and Sieverding (2014) found that men interrupted their careers for much shorter lengths of time than women, and career interruptions were negatively related to career success in both genders. Furthermore, they discovered that children predicted career interruptions differently for women and men. For women, more children led to longer career interruptions, but interestingly, for men, more children resulted in shorter interruptions. In accordance with Social Role Theory (Eagly, 1987), men are perceived as more agentic than women, and women are perceived as more communal than men in Germany (Steinmetz, Bosak, Sczesny, & Eagly, 2014, German and Japanese student samples). Therefore, as the division of labor between parents and gender stereotypes are still quite traditional in Germany, we expected that a man who deliberately takes parental leave will be judged as more communal and less agentic and will be faced with backlash effects. Goals of the Present Research Our research applied Social Role Theory and Backlash Theory to the explanation of reactions toward men who have taken parental leave. Two experimental studies were conducted in Germany, where a new law was introduced in 2007 with the explicit goal of increasing the number of fathers taking parental leave. The current research makes several novel contributions to previous research. First, most studies that investigated psychological reactions toward men who take parental leave were conducted in the US, a country whose laws do not support men’s parental leave. Our research was conducted in Germany, which has a new law that explicitly aims to increase the number of men taking parental leave. At the same time, it is a country in which a quite traditional division of labor between parents by gender is still dominant. Second, our research applied Social Role Theory and Backlash MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 12 Theory to make theory-driven predictions about the consequences of parental leave on evaluations of men. Third, it systematically varied the length of parental leave and therefore made it possible for us to investigate whether it is the parental leave per se that influences the evaluation of men or whether the length of the parental leave matters. Hypotheses. Our first hypothesis addressed the consequences of (the length of) parental leave on the attribution of communal and agentic characteristics. When fathers take parental leave and care for a child, Social Role Theory predicts that they will be regarded as higher in communion and lower in agency than fathers who continue working. The length of parental leave (and therefore the fulfilment of the role of the principal person caring for the child) was expected to have an impact on the attribution of communal and agentic attributes as well. Fathers who take a long parental leave should be regarded as more communal and less agentic than fathers who take only the 2 partner months because people might interpret such a long and uncommon parental leave as a deliberate and voluntary decision that is driven by underlying traits. Hypothesis 1 (Effects of parental leave on the ascription of communal and agentic traits): Men who take parental leave will be viewed as less agentic and more communal than men who do not take parental leave (1a). Men who take a long parental leave (12 months) will be viewed as less agentic and more communal than men who take a short parental leave (2 months; 1b). The second hypothesis addressed the likeability of men who have taken a parental leave. In Germany, men are generally viewed as less warm (and hence, less likeable) than women (Asbrock, 2010, student sample). However, using U.S. student and adult samples, both Cuddy et al. (2004) and Peplau and Fingerhut (2004) found that fathers were evaluated as warmer than men without children; furthermore, fathers who took parental leave were MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 13 perceived as warmer than working fathers who did not take parental leave (Coleman & Franiuk, 2011, U.S. student samples). Therefore, we expected that men who took a parental leave would be liked more than men who did not take a parental leave. Hypothesis 2 (Effects of parental leave on likeability ratings): Men who take parental leave (regardless of its length) will be liked more than men who do not take parental leave. Our third hypothesis addressed possible backlash effects. Taking a parental leave can still be regarded as counterstereotypical behavior for men and can therefore lead to backlash effects. Men could be rated as deserving less respect, as less competent, and as less likely to be hired. In Germany, as the 2 partner months were introduced by the new law, an increasing number of fathers take advantage of these partner months, but only a minority of less than 2% take a longer parental leave of up to 12 months. As backlash depends on the violation of gender stereotypes (Rudman & Glick, 1999, U.S. student sample), and gender stereotypes are dynamic (Diekman & Eagly, 2000, U.S. student and working adult samples), we expected to find that backlash effects would be directed only toward men who took a long parental leave. Hypothesis 3 (Backlash effects of parental leave): Backlash effects will be directed toward men who take a long parental leave (12 months) but not toward men who take a short parental leave (2 months) or no parental leave. Hypothesis 3a: Men who take a long parental leave (12 months) will be respected less and rated as less competent than men who take no leave or who take a short parental leave (2 months). Hypothesis 3b: Men who take a long parental leave (12 months) will receive lower probability ratings for being hired than men who take no leave or who take a short parental leave (2 months). MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 14 Gender effects. We had no basis for making a prediction about potential gender differences in the evaluation of men who take parental leave. Several studies found gender differences (e.g, Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992, meta-analysis; Rudman, 1998, German and U.S. student samples; Sczesny & Kühnen, 2004) but not always in the same direction; whereas others did not (Rudman & Glick, 1999; Sczesny et al., 2006, German and U.S. student samples). Therefore, we included participants’ gender in an exploratory analysis to determine whether there were any gender effects. Study 1 Study 1 investigated how people evaluate a man who takes parental leave. First, students in business-related subjects read a job advertisement for a leadership position. Then they examined a vignette describing an applicant who provided a good fit to the job; only the length of parental leave differed across conditions. Finally, participants evaluated the applicant’s communion, agency, and competence. They also rated how much the applicant would be liked and respected and how likely the applicant would be to get the job. On the one hand, we hypothesized that parental leave would have a positive effect on the evaluations of communion and likeability. On the other hand, we predicted that parental leave would lead to lower agency, respect, competence, and hiring probability ratings when the parental leave was long (12 months). Method In front of a business-studies library, students studying business-related subjects (e.g., business studies, economics, business law) were approached by a female advanced student (A.F.). The business students were asked whether they would like to participate in a psychological study and were told a cover story that it was about the evaluation and selection of people under time pressure. Students were offered sweets as compensation. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 15 After agreeing to participate, students were led to a table in the room next to the library where they were presented with the vignette about the applicant and the questionnaire. Participants. Ninety students (50 women, 40 men) from a large German university and a Duale Hochschule (a part-time university that cooperates with companies in training students) participated. Students’ mean age was M = 22.27 years (SD = 2.47, Range: 18 – 28). All participants studied business-related subjects (business studies 57.8%, business and economics education 11.2%, economics 6.7%, other business related subjects 23.6%) and had been attending the university for a mean number of semesters of M = 5.46 (SD = 2.82, Range: 2 – 12). Each group of 30 participants received different parental leave information. Demographic characteristics, separated by gender and condition, can be found in Table 1. Material. The material consisted of a job advertisement, a fictitious application, and a questionnaire that measured the dependent variables. A real job advertisement for a manager position with excellent career opportunities was shortened and made anonymous. A cover letter and curriculum vitae constituted the job application, which was constructed to provide a good fit with the job requirements. The applicant was described as having a diploma in business studies with a very good final grade from a prestigious university, several years of work experience as a recruitment consultant, and as speaking three languages. All in all, the applicant was described as very qualified for the advertised job. Length of parental leave was manipulated in the curriculum vitae and in the cover letter (see the Appendix for the original German wording of the scenarios). Procedure. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the conditions. First, they read the job advertisement and the fictitious application. They then filled out the questionnaire, which included the dependent variables. The instructions were as follows: “Please imagine that in your company, you are responsible for hiring a person for the MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 16 advertised job.” The whole procedure took about 10 min, after which the participants were thanked and debriefed. Manipulation check. Two questions “Did the applicant take a parental leave?” (yes vs. no) and “If yes, how long was the parental leave?” were used as a manipulation check. Dependent variables. The dependent variables included ratings of communion, agency, likeability, respect, competence, and hiring probability. The original German wording of the scales or the respective publications can be found in the Appendix. Communion and agency. The femininity and masculinity scales from an adapted German version of the Short Form of the Bem Sex-Role Inventory (Zimmermann, Sieverding, & Müller, 2011) were used to assess communion and agency. The femininity scale (Cronbach’s α = .91) included eight items such as “gentle” and “compassionate”; the masculinity scale (Cronbach’s α = .83) included eight items such as “independent” and “dominant.” Participants assessed how well each attribute described the applicant on a 7point scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 7 (applies very much). All femininity scale items were averaged to form a communion rating (Cronbach’s α = .91 in Study 1, Cronbach’s α = .89 in Study 2); all masculinity scale items were averaged to form an agency rating (Cronbach’s α = .82 in Study 1; Cronbach’s α = .84 in Study 2). Competence. The competence scale (Cronbach’s α = .88), which was composed of achievement motivation, leadership motivation, and general competence, was rated on a 7point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very strongly). Questions were adapted from the scale for describing others from the Business-Focused Inventory of Personality (BIP, Hossiep & Paschen, 2003), for example, “The applicant wants to have a higher performance than others” and “The applicant conveys authority.” General competence was assessed with the MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 17 question “How competent is the applicant?” All items were averaged to form a competence rating (Cronbach’s α = .89 in Study 1; Cronbach’s α = .90 in Study 2). Likeability and respect. The likeability and respect scales were each comprised of two questions that were similar to Heilman and Wallen (2010). To assess likeability, the participants were asked: “How likeable do you consider the applicant to be?” and “How much would the company’s employees like the applicant?”. The questions for respect were “How much would you/the company’s employees respect the applicant?” The participants rated each question on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). The two likeability items were averaged to form a likeability rating (Cronbach’s α = .80 in Study 1; Cronbach’s α = .84 in Study 2), and the two respect items were averaged to form a respect rating (Cronbach’s α = .88 in Study 1; Cronbach’s α = .85 in Study 2). Hiring probability. The four questions used to assess the applicant’s probability of being hired were similar to questions used by Bosak and Sczesny (2011) and Moss-Racusin et al. (2010). They included “Should the applicant be short-listed/interviewed?”, “Would you hire the applicant for the advertised position?”, and “How likely is the applicant to get the job?” Answers were given on 7-point scales ranging from 1 (absolutely not) to 7 (absolutely). The four questions were averaged to form a hiring probability rating (Cronbach’s α = .87 in Study 1; Cronbach’s α = .92 in Study 2). Results The number of female and male participants in conditions with different lengths of parental leave did not differ from an equal distribution, χ²(2) = 0.63, p = .730. The means for all dependent variables and the cell sizes for each condition can be found in Table 2. A manipulation check revealed that 92% of the participants correctly remembered whether MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 18 the applicant had taken a parental leave, and 74% could specify the exact length. Therefore, we concluded that the manipulation was successful. Analysis of data. To test for group differences, we first analyzed the dependent variables using a 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) MANOVA, which showed highly significant differences for parental leave condition, F(12, 160) = 6.62, p < .001, η² = .33, and gender, F(6, 79) = 3.71, p = .003, η ² = .22. We then computed 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVAs for all dependent variables. Ascription of gender role characteristics. The communion and agency ratings are shown in Table 2. In Hypothesis 1, we expected applicants who took a parental leave to be rated as more communal and less agentic than applicants who did not (1a) and applicants who took 12 months as more communal and less agentic than applicants who took 2 months (1b). We tested these predictions with a 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVA for communion and agency, respectively. As expected in Hypothesis 1a, parental leave had a significant effect on communion ratings, F(5, 84) = 13.39, p < .001, partial η² = .41. Participants rated applicants who took no parental leave as less communal than those who took 2 or 12 months of parental leave. Contrary to Hypothesis 1b, communion ratings did not differ as a function of length of parental leave (2 vs. 12 months; see Table 2). Hypothesis 1 was not supported for agency ratings. Parental leave did not affect agency ratings, F(2, 84) = 0.70, p = .501. Participants’ gender did not affect the ratings of communion, F(5, 84) = 3.91, p = .051, or agency, F (1, 84) = 0.92, p = .341. Likeability ratings. Hypothesis 2 predicted that applicants who took a parental leave would be liked more than applicants who did not, and we tested this with a 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVA. In accordance with Hypothesis 2, MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 19 parental leave influenced the likeability ratings, F(5, 84) = 9.75, p < .001, partial η² = .19. Participants thought applicants who took parental leave would be liked more than applicants who did not take parental leave; participants did not differentiate between 2 and 12 months of parental leave (see Table 2). Participants’ gender did not affect the likeability ratings, F(1, 84) = 2.57, p = .113. Work-oriented ratings. Hypothesis 3a predicted that applicants who took no or 2 months of parental leave would be respected more and evaluated as more competent than applicants who took 12 months of parental leave. We tested this prediction with 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVAs on respect and competence, respectively. Parental leave influenced the respect ratings, F(5, 84) = 5.21, p = .007, partial η² = .11, but not always in the hypothesized direction (H3a). Applicants who took 12 months of leave received lower respect ratings than those who took 2 months of parental leave but did not differ from applicants who did not take any parental leave. Unexpectedly, applicants who took 2 months of parental leave received higher respect ratings than applicants who took no parental leave (see Table 2). Regarding competence, Hypothesis 3a was not supported, as participants rated all applicants as equally competent regardless of their parental leave, F(2, 84) = 0.06, p = .943. Hypothesis 3b predicted that applicants who took 12 months of parental leave would be rated as less likely to be hired than applicants who took 2 months or no parental leave. Contrary to Hypothesis 3b, parental leave did not affect the hiring probability ratings, F (2, 84) = 0.56, p = .571. Main effects of participants’ gender on the ratings of work-related characteristics were found. In comparison with male participants, female participants expected applicants to be respected more, F(5, 84) = 17.76, p < .001, partial η² = .18, rated the applicants as more MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 20 competent, F(5, 84) = 5.97, p = .017, partial η² = .07, and rated the applicant’s probability of being hired as higher, F(5, 84) = 6.67, p = .012, partial η² = .07, regardless of (length of) parental leave. We repeated all analyses including only participants who could specify whether the applicant took a parental leave. The results did not differ except for two effects of participants’ gender: In this subsample, men rated the applicant as more communal than women, and women thought the applicant would be liked more than men did. Because we had no hypotheses regarding gender differences, all participants were included in the analyses. Discussion In accordance with Social Role Theory (Eagly, 1987), men’s parental leave was found to influence communion ratings; however, it did not affect agency ratings. Parental leave also led to higher likeability ratings. Contrary to Backlash Theory, no backlash effects were found for the ratings of respect, competence, and hiring probability, even for men who had taken a long parental leave (12 months). Furthermore, we found differences in how men and women evaluated the applicants on respect, competence, and hiring probability. Is it possible that participants were too aware of the socially desirable answers and therefore did not reveal their true beliefs? In addition, students with the same subjects as in Study 1 rated the attractiveness and gender typicality of the advertised job in a post-hoc test (N = 40). The advertised job turned out to be a stereotypically female job and was rather unattractive to this particular group of students. Hence, it is possible that participants viewed the job as appropriate for men who would take parental leave and therefore did not exhibit backlash. Study 2 was conducted to explore these possibilities. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 21 Study 2 Study 2 was a replication of Study 1 except that the job advertisement described a stereotypically male and very attractive job. We consulted experts on employment decisionmaking from a leading university to preselect two job advertisements that are regarded as attractive options for graduates of business-related subjects. We then administered a pretest (N = 22) to the target group to choose the more attractive one. The job advertisement they chose was for a leadership position in finance. To ensure that the ratings of the applicants would not be confounded by social desirability, we applied a short six-item version of the Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17, Stöber, 1999). The hypotheses were the same as in Study 1. Method Students were again approached in front of the business-studies library. They were offered sweets for compensation and were debriefed after the study. Procedure. The procedure for Study 2 was the same as described in Study 1. Participants. The participants were 113 students (55 women, 58 men) from the same German universities as in Study 1. Their mean age was M = 23.34 years (SD = 2.59, Range: 19 – 29). They were all studying business-related subjects (business studies 56.6%, management 23.0%, economics 3.5%, other business-related subjects 16.9%) with a mean number of semesters of M = 5.62 (SD = 3.03, Range: 1 – 12). Material. The material in Study 2 was the same as in Study 1 with the following exceptions. The job advertisement described a leadership position in finance and offered performance-based pay. Hence, the job application was modified to match this job. Also, the questionnaire included a short version of the SDS-17. We used six items that showed the best internal reliability on a pretest (Cronbach’s α = .67), for example, “I accept all opinions, MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 22 even if they do not agree with my opinion” and “In an argument, I always remain factual and objective.” Cronbach’s α for all other measures from Study 2 are reported in Study 1. The original German scales (or the respective publications) and the original German wording of the scenarios can be found in the Appendix. Results The number of female and male participants in conditions with different lengths of parental leave did not differ from an equal distribution, χ²(2) = 0.16, p = .921. As the average score on the SDS-17 was not correlated with any of the dependent variables (all rs < .15, ps > .26), we concluded that social desirability did not confound the evaluations of the applicants, and hence, we did not include the SDS-17 in further analyses. The means for all dependent variables and the cell sizes for each condition can be found in Table 3. A manipulation check revealed that 81% of the participants correctly indicated whether the applicant had taken a parental leave, and 73% could specify the length. Therefore, we concluded that the manipulation was successful. Analysis of data. To test for group differences, we first analyzed the dependent variables by computing a 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) MANOVA, which showed significant differences for condition, F(12, 204) = 1.84, p = .044, η² = .10, but not for gender, F(6, 101) = 0.51, p = .80. We then computed 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVAs on all dependent variables. Gender role characteristics. The communion and agency ratings are shown in Table 3. Hypothesis 1a predicted that applicants who took a parental leave would be rated as more communal and less agentic than applicants who did not. A 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVA on communion showed that, as predicted in Hypothesis 1a, parental leave significantly influenced the communion ratings, F(2, 107) = MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 23 3.46, p = .035, partial η² = .06. Participants rated applicants who did not take parental leave as less communal than those who took 12 months of parental leave (see Table 3). Hypothesis 1b predicted that men who took 12 months of parental leave would be rated as more communal and less agentic than men who took 2 months. However, Tukey’s HSD test showed that participants did not differentiate between 2 months and no parental leave or 2 months and 12 months. Contrary to Hypothesis 1, a 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVA on agency showed that parental leave did not affect the agency ratings, F(2, 107) = 1.55, p = .216. There were no main effects of participants’ gender on the ratings of communion, F(1, 107) = 0.42, p = .514, or agency, F(1, 107) = 0.72, p = .399. Likeability ratings. Hypothesis 2 predicted that applicants who took a parental leave would be liked more than applicants who did not. A 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVA on liking found that participants expected applicants to be liked differently according to length of parental leave, F(2, 106) = 4.66, p = .012, partial η² = .08. Participants rated applicants who took parental leave higher on likeability than applicants who did not, regardless of the length of parental leave (see Table 3). Participants’ gender did not play a role in the likeability ratings, F(1, 106) = 0.59, p = .443. Work-related characteristics. Hypothesis 3 predicted that men who took 12 months of parental leave would be respected less, rated as less competent, and would be less likely to be hired than men who took 2 months or no parental leave. We computed 3 (parental leave: 0, 2, 12 months) x 2 (participants’ gender) ANOVAs on respect, competence, and hiring probability, respectively. The results revealed that contrary to Hypothesis 3, parental leave did not affect the respect, F(2, 106) < 0.01, p = .996, competence, F(2, 107) = 1.21, p = .302, or hiring probability, F (2, 106) = 0.78, p = .462, ratings. Participants’ gender also did MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 24 not influence the respect, F(1, 106) = 1.78, p = .185, competence, F(2, 107) = 0.55, p = .460, or hiring probability, F (1, 106) = 2.01, p = .159, ratings. We repeated all analyses including only participants who specified correctly whether the applicant had taken a parental leave. The only differences were that, in this subsample, parental leave influenced communion only marginally significantly and participants liked applicants who took 2 months of parental leave more than they liked applicants who did not take parental leave. Therefore, we kept all participants in our analysis. Discussion Study 2 replicated most of the findings from Study 1. It used a very attractive job description to ensure that participants would be interested in the advertised job. It also controlled for social desirability, which did not influence the ratings. With regard to gender role characteristics, the predictions made by Social Role Theory (Eagly, 1987) were partly supported. Participants judged applicants who took a longer parental leave as more communal, but they did not evaluate them as less agentic. They also gave applicants who took a parental leave higher likeability ratings than applicants who did not, regardless of length of parental leave. Regarding work-related characteristics, backlash effects were not found. Irrespective of parental leave (length), participants respected applicants equally, found them similarly competent, and predicted the same hiring probability. General Discussion In two experimental studies, we investigated people’s perceptions of a male applicant as a function of an alleged (length of) parental leave. Overall, we did not find negative reactions toward men who took parental leave. By contrast, we found a very positive evaluation of such men. Participants rated men who took a parental leave as more communal and also rated them as more likeable. All applicants were rated as equally MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 25 agentic and competent, were respected similarly, and received similar hiring probabilities. The only exception was that applicants who took 2 months of parental leave were given higher respect ratings than applicants who took no or 12 months of parental leave in Study 1. We will first discuss the results, then consider the limitations and strengths of our studies, and finally discuss the implications. Evaluations of Male Applicants as a Function of Their Parental Leave As predicted by Social Role Theory (Eagly, 1987), men who took a (longer) parental leave were seen as significantly more communal. This finding is in line with findings in U.S. student samples by Rudman and Mescher (2013) who discovered that men who had taken a leave to care for a sick child were rated higher on communal traits. The effect sizes for communion varied between Studies 1 and 2 (η² = .41 in Study 1 vs. η² = .07 in Study 2). It is possible that in Study 1, the effect size was larger because not only had the applicant taken a parental leave, but he had also applied for a stereotypically female job. Both pieces of information together may have reinforced the idea that the communal characteristics of the applicant were due to a stable trait instead of the situation. What is interesting is that applicants who took parental leave were not seen as significantly less agentic. Here, our results differ from Rudman and Mescher (2013): Whereas they found that sick-care leave led to lower ratings of agentic traits, we found that taking parental leave (regardless of length) did not affect ratings of agency. It is important to consider that in our studies, all men were applying for a job that required high qualifications and were described as highly qualified in their curriculum vitae. Describing a person as an employee has been found to induce higher agency judgments (Bosak et al., 2008, U.S. adult sample); hence, it seems that the fact that the men were applying for a job that required MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 26 high qualifications influenced participants’ agency ratings more than whether the applicant had taken parental leave. Participants expected applicants who took a parental leave to be liked more than applicants who did not take a parental leave, regardless of length. These results are in agreement with several studies on student samples in the US that showed that fathers are perceived as warmer than childless men (Cuddy et al., 2004; Peplau & Fingerhut, 2004) and fathers who take a parental leave are perceived as warmer than fathers who do not take a leave (Coleman & Franiuk, 2011). Participants thought all applicants would be respected equally. Maybe participants did not differ in their respect for applicants because they rated all applicants as equally agentic, and agentic characteristics are linked with being respected (Wojciszke, Abele, & Baryla, 2009, diverse Polish samples). In our research, we found correlations between agency and respect of r = .46 and r = .48 (both ps < .001) in Studies 1 and 2, respectively. An exception was that in Study 1, applicants who took 2 months of parental leave received the highest respect ratings. This may be explained by the fact that taking 2 months of parental leave is now (since the introduction of the new law) a clever thing to do in Germany, given that the months for which the parental allowance is paid are lost if not taken by fathers. However, this result was not replicated in Study 2. What is striking is that backlash against men who had taken 12 months of parental leave was not found with respect to the ratings of competence and hiring probability. This is partly in line with results from U.S. student samples by Coleman and Franiuk (2011) who found that competence ratings did not differ between fathers who did not take and fathers who took a parental leave and that such ratings were lower only for fathers who planned to stay home until their child went to school. However, they also found that fathers who took MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 27 parental leave were considered less successful, which is in contrast to our finding that they were not given lower hiring probability ratings. Our results are thus more similar to studies that have compared men and women with and without children. Several studies in the US found negative effects for mothers but not for fathers (e.g. Correll et al., 2007; Cuddy et al., 2004; Heilman & Okimoto, 2008; Peplau & Fingerhut, 2004, U.S. working adults and student samples). Other studies conducted in the US found results that suggested backlash for men who took parental leave (Allen & Russell, 1999, U.S. student samples; Coleman & Franiuk, 2011; Wayne & Cordeiro, 2003), but our studies, which were conducted in Germany, did not identify any indication of this. It is probable that one cause lies in Germany’s completely different legislation concerning parental leave (see Introduction). The new parental leave law in Germany has been in place since 2007, and gender roles and stereotypes are dynamic aspects of culture (Diekman & Eagly, 2000, U.S. working adult and student samples). The gender roles of men may have already changed—at least slightly—to incorporate taking a parental leave. This would make it a normal and not a counterstereotypical act for men, and hence would not elicit backlash. Furthermore, our fictitious applicant was highly qualified, which may have been more important than whether or not he took parental leave. Allen et al. (1994) also did not find negative reactions to high-performing employees who took parental leave in a U.S. student sample. The lack of backlash for hiring can also be discussed with respect to the Lack of Fit Model (Heilman, 1983). The Lack of Fit Model postulates that expectations about the success of an individual in a certain job depend on the fit between the perceived attributes of the individual and the perceived job requirements. A good fit leads to expectations of success and a high probability of being hired, whereas a poor fit results in expectations of failure and MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 28 a low probability of being hired. Our job advertisements, especially the male-typed job in Study 2, required applicants to be agentic and competent. Our results indicate that applicants who took a parental leave were not perceived as less agentic or less competent (similar to Cuddy et al., 2004; Heilman & Okimoto, 2008; and Peplau & Fingerhut, 2004, who found that fathers were perceived as equally or more competent than childless men in U.S. working adults and students samples); instead, they gained communion and likeability. Therefore, applicants who took parental leave still had a good fit with the advertised job and were accordingly rated as having a high probability of being hired. Limitations and Strengths Using a student sample clearly needs to be named as a limitation as we cannot generalize from such a specific sample (young, high education, on average higher socioeconomic background than an average German, e.g. Middendorff, Apolinarski, Poskowsky, Kandulla, & Netz, 2013) to the whole population. It is probable that results would have been different in a real-world situation. For example, we used only an application with a cover letter and curriculum vitae. Hence, we cannot guarantee that the results would have been the same with a real application, which consists of more details, or even in a job interview. Even though our participants were all students in business-related subjects, they still lack experience in hiring situations. Also, we asked participants about a fictitious situation to which they could respond freely on the basis of their views instead of having to take responsibility for the consequences. Furthermore, it might be the case that students are more open to new developments than people who have been on the job for several years. It is also possible that cohort effects exist here, as stereotypes are dynamic and may have changed in line with changing roles (Diekman & Eagly, 2000; in U.S. student and working MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 29 adult samples). For example, using student samples in the US and student and working adult samples in Germany, Wilde and Diekman (2005) found that participants perceived a trend in which women and men were becoming more similar from the past to the present and into the future. However, as the study was conducted before men began to take parental leave in higher numbers in Germany, the development was mainly based on women acquiring more masculine characteristics. Prior studies that have investigated the perceptions of men who take parental leave have also used student participants (Allen & Russell, 1999; Allen et al., 1994; Coleman & Franiuk, 2011; Rudman & Mescher, 2013; Wayne & Cordeiro, 2003, U.S. student samples); therefore, our results can be compared to their findings. However, it would be more interesting to study what people in leadership positions would do in a similar situation. All our applicants had already taken their parental leave so that participants could reason that the applicant would not burden the firm with further absences. However, we did not expect that a parental leave would lead to a rational decision to give the applicant a bad rating. Rather, we were searching for backlash effects, that is, whether the applicant would be sanctioned for implementing actions that were inconsistent with gender stereotypes. Moss-Racusin et al. (2010) explain that backlash occurs when people violate gender stereotypes that justify the gender hierarchy, thus endangering the status quo. Therefore, backlash effects should be independent of whether the action had occurred in the past or would affect the present. However, men taking a parental leave could also encounter rational negative responses. Previous studies have compared parents who took a parental leave with parents who continued to work or with stay-at-home parents (e.g., Coleman & Franiuk, 2011; Wayne & Cordeiro, 2003, U.S. student samples). Because we had three conditions, two of them with MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 30 different lengths of parental leave (2 vs. 12 months), we were able to examine whether taking any kind of parental leave caused an effect or whether the leave had to be of a certain length. In contrast to findings by Allen et al. (1994) and Allen and Russell (1999) with U.S. student samples, it was the act of taking a parental leave that affected the ratings of both communion and likeability, whereas length did not influence the ratings. We also used two different job advertisements from different areas of work (recruitment and finance), and the second advertisement was chosen specifically to ensure it was a male-typed job that was attractive to our target group. Nevertheless, Study 2 replicated the results from Study 1 except for a lack of gender differences. Furthermore, we used a short version of the SDS-17 (Stöber, 1999) to ensure that evaluations were not driven by social desirability and were able to establish that participants’ answers were independent of their desire to provide socially desirable answers. Implications The proportion of men in Germany taking parental leave has increased substantially since the Federal Parental Allowance and Parental Leave Act was introduced in 2007 (see Introduction). Our studies indicate that in Germany today, men might not need to worry when taking a parental leave—especially when it is short—because we found some evidence for positive attitudes toward men’s parental leave. By reserving 2 months only for fathers, the new law has already drastically increased the number of fathers who take a parental leave. This trend parallels the one in Scandinavia, where legislation reserved parental leave time for fathers as early as 1993 in Norway and 1995 in Sweden. In the year after the law was introduced in these countries, the number of fathers taking a parental leave began to rise and has not stopped since (Haas & Rostgaard, 2011). This rise, in turn, may have altered MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 31 the perception of men taking a parental leave because we did not find any negative effects of parental leave. Altogether, our research could not replicate any of the backlash effects that were found in U.S. studies. In Germany, legislation encourages men to take parental leave, and thus, many more men have begun to take advantage of this opportunity since the respective law was introduced (see Introduction). Our research shows some evidence that men who take parental leave are viewed more positively in Germany than in countries that do not have such legislation such as the US—at least among students. Therefore, when trying to get fathers more involved with their children, it is important to consider legislative and economic factors as well as psychological ones. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 32 References Allen, T. D., & Russell, J. E. A. (1999). Parental leave of absence: Some not so family-friendly implications. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 166-191. doi: 10.1111/j.15591816.1999.tb01380.x Allen, T. D., Russell, J. E. A., & Rush, M. C. (1994). The effects of gender and leave of absence on attributions for high performance, perceived organizational commitment, and allocation of organizational rewards. Sex Roles, 31, 443-464. doi: 10.1007/BF01544200 Asbrock, F. (2010). Stereotypes of social groups in Germany in terms of warmth and competence. Social Psychology, 41, 76-81. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000011 BMFSFJ. (2013). Bundeselterngeld- und Elternzeitgesetz [Federal Parental Allowance and Parental Leave Act]. Retrieved from http://www.bmfsfj.de/BMFSFJ/gesetze,did=93110.html. Bosak, J., & Sczesny, S. (2011). Gender bias in leader selection? Evidence from a hiring simulation study. Sex Roles, 65, 234-242. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0012-7 Bosak, J., Sczesny, S., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Communion and agency judgments of women and men as a function of role information and response format. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1148-1155. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.538 Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2005). Attitudes toward traditional and nontraditional parents. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 436-445. doi: 10.1111/j.14716402.2005.00244.x Bundesministerium der Justiz und für Verbraucherschutz. (2006). Bundeselterngeld- und Elternzeitgesetz [Federal Parental Allowance and Parental Leave Act]. Retrieved from http://www.gesetze-im-internet.de/bundesrecht/beeg/gesamt.pdf. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 33 Burgess, A. (2011). ‘Family man’: British fathers' journey to the centre of the kitchen. Retrieved from http://www.fatherhoodinstitute.org/2011/fathers-attitudes-towardsand-experiences-of-parental-leave/ Coleman, J. M., & Franiuk, R. (2011). Perceptions of mothers and fathers who take temporary work leave. Sex Roles, 64, 311-323. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9918-8 Correll, S. J., Benard, S., & Paik, I. (2007). Getting a job: Is there a motherhood penalty? American Journal of Sociology, 112, 1297-1338. doi: 10.1086/511799 Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn't cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 701-718. doi: 10.1111/j.00224537.2004.00381.x Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 9911004. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.991 Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 11711188. doi: 10.1177/0146167200262001 Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: a social-role interpretation (Vol. 1985). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3-22. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.3 Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735-754. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735 MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 34 Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2012). Social role theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 458-476). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Ltd. Ehlert, N., & Rüling, A. (2008). Dossier Elterngeld als Teil einer nachhaltigen Familienpolitik [Dossier parental allowance as part of a sustainable family policy]. Berlin: Bundesministerium für Familie, Senioren, Frauen und Jugend. Retrieved from http://www.bmfsfj.de/RedaktionBMFSFJ/Broschuerenstelle/Pdf-Anlagen/DossierElterngeld,property=pdf,bereich=bmfsfj,sprache=de,rwb=true.pdf. Evers, A., & Sieverding, M. (2014). Why do highly qualified women (still) earn less? Gender differences in long-term predictors of career success. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 38, 93-106. doi: 10.1177/0361684313498071 Family and Medical Leave Act of 1993, Public Law 103-3 (1993). Retrieved from http://www.dol.gov/whd/regs/statutes/fmla.htm. forsa Gesellschaft für Sozialforschung und statistische Analysen mbH. (2013). Meinungen und Einstellungen der Väter in Deutschland [Opinions and attitudes of fathers in Germany]. Berlin: Gruner und Jahr AG & Co KG Retrieved from http://www.eltern.de/c/images/pdf/Ergebnisbericht_Vaeterumfrage_2013.pdf. Haas, L., Allard, K., & Hwang, P. (2002). The impact of organizational culture on men's use of parental leave in Sweden. Community, Work & Family, 5, 319-342. doi: 10.1080/1366880022000041801 Haas, L., & Rostgaard, T. (2011). Fathers' rights to paid parental leave in the Nordic countries: Consequences for the gendered division of leave. Community, Work & Family, 14, 177-195. doi: 10.1080/13668803.2011.571398 MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 35 Han, W.-J., Ruhm, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2009). Parental leave policies and parents' employment and leave-taking. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 28, 29-54. doi: 10.1002/pam.20398 Heilman, M. E. (1983). Sex bias in work settings: The Lack of Fit model. Research in Organizational Behavior, 5, 269-298. Heilman, M. E., & Okimoto, T. G. (2008). Motherhood: A potential source of bias in employment decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 189-198. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.189 Heilman, M. E., & Wallen, A. S. (2010). Wimpy and undeserving of respect: Penalties for men’s gender-inconsistent success. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 664-667. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.01.008 Hossiep, R., & Paschen, M. (2003). Das Bochumer Inventar zur berufsbezogenen Persönlichkeitsbeschreibung [Business-Focused Inventory of Personality (BIP)]. (2. ed.). Göttingen; Bern: Hogrefe, Verl. für Psychologie. Kapella, O., & Rille-Pfeiffer, C. (2007). Einstellungen und Werthaltungen zu Themen der Vereinbarkeit von Familie und Erwerb [Attitudes and value systems about issues of reconcilability of family and earning]. ÖIF Paper Nr. 66, 1-64. Kaufman, G., Lyonette, C., & Crompton, R. (2010). Post-birth employment leave among fathers in Britain and the United States. Fathering, 8, 321-340. doi: 10.3149/fth.0803.321 Lammi-Taskula, J. (2008). Doing fatherhood: Understanding the gendered use of parental leave in Finland. Fathering: A Journal of Theory, Research, & Practice about Men as Fathers, 6, 133-148. doi: 10.3149/fth.0602.133 MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 36 Middendorff, E., Apolinarski, B., Poskowsky, J., Kandulla, M., & Netz, N. (2013). Die wirtschaftliche und soziale Lage der Studierenden in Deutschland 2012. [The economic and social situation of students in Germany 2012]. Berlin: Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung Retrieved from http://www.sozialerhebung.de/download/20/soz20_hauptbericht_gesamt.pdf. Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). When men break the gender rules: Status incongruity and backlash against modest men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 11, 140-151. doi: 10.1037/a0018093 Nepomnyaschy, L., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). Paternity leave and fathers' involvement with their young children: Evidence from the American ECLS-B. Community, Work & Family, 10, 427-453. doi: 10.1080/13668800701575077 Peplau, L. A., & Fingerhut, A. (2004). The paradox of the lesbian worker. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 719-735. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00382.x Reich, N. (2010). Who cares? Determinants of the fathers‘ use of parental leave in Germany. HWWI Research Paper, 1-31. Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629-645. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629 Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004-1010. doi: 10.1037/00223514.77.5.1004 MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 37 Rudman, L. A., & Mescher, K. (2013). Penalizing men who request a family leave: Is flexibility stigma a femininity stigma? Journal of Social Issues, 69, 322-340. doi: 10.1111/josi.12017 Sczesny, S. (2003). A closer look beneath the surface: Various facets of the think-managerthink-male stereotype. Sex Roles, 49, 353-363. doi: 10.1023/a:1025112204526 Sczesny, S., & Kühnen, U. (2004). Meta-cognition about biological sex and gender-stereotypic physical appearance: Consequences for the assessment of leadership competence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 13-21. doi: 10.1177/0146167203258831 Sczesny, S., Spreemann, S., & Stahlberg, D. (2006). Masculine = competent? Physical appearance and sex as sources of gender-stereotypic attributions. Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie, 65, 15-23. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.65.1.15 Seward, R. R., Yeatts, D. E., & Zottarelli, L. K. (2002). Parental leave and father involvement in child care: Sweden and the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 33, 387-399. Statistics Sweden. (2012). Women and men in Sweden - facts and figures 2012. Örebro: Statistics Sweden Retrieved from http://www.scb.se/statistik/_publikationer/LE0201_2012A01_BR_X10BR1201ENG.pd f. Statistisches Bundesamt. (2003). Wo bleibt die Zeit? Die Zeitverwendung der Bevölkerung in Deutschland 2001/02 [Where has the time gone? The time use of the population in Germany 2001/02]. Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt. Retrieved from MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 38 http://www.bmfsfj.de/RedaktionBMFSFJ/Abteilung2/Pdf-Anlagen/wo-bleibtzeit,property=pdf.pdf. Statistisches Bundesamt. (2010). Verdienstunterschiede zwischen Männern und Frauen 2006 [Income differences between men and women in 2006]. Wiesbaden: Retrieved from https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Thematisch/VerdiensteArbeitskosten/Ve rdienstunterschiede/VerdienstunterschiedeMannFrau5621001069004.pdf?__blob=p ublicationFile. Statistisches Bundesamt. (2012a). Pressekonferenz „Elterngeld – wer, wie lange und wie viel?“ [Press conference "Parental allowance - who, how long and how much?"]. Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt. Retrieved from https://www.destatis.de/DE/ZahlenFakten/GesellschaftStaat/Soziales/Sozialleistunge n/Elterngeld/Tabellen/GeburtenGeschlechtJahr2010.html. Statistisches Bundesamt. (2012b). Teilzeitquote von Frauen in Deutschland deutlich über EUDurchschnitt [press release] [Women's ratio of part-time work considerably above EUaverage]. Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt Retrieved from https://www.destatis.de/DE/PresseService/Presse/Pressemitteilungen/2012/03/PD1 2_078_132pdf.pdf?__blob=publicationFile. Statistisches Bundesamt. (2014). Statistik zum Elterngeld: Beendete Leistungsbezüge für im Jahr 2012 geborene Kinder [Statistics about parental allowance: Terminated benefits for children born in 2012]. Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt. Retrieved from https://www.destatis.de/DE/ZahlenFakten/GesellschaftStaat/Soziales/Sozialleistunge n/Elterngeld/Tabellen/GeburtenGeschlechtJahr2010.html. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 39 Steinmetz, J., Bosak, J., Sczesny, S., & Eagly, A. H. (2014). Social role effects on gender stereotyping in Germany and Japan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 52-60. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12044 Stöber, J. (1999). Die Soziale-Erwünschtheits-Skala-17 (SES-17): Entwicklung und erste Befunde zu Reliabilität und Validität [The Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17): Development and first results on reliability and validity]. Diagnostica, 45, 173-177. doi: 10.1026//0012-1924.45.4.173 Väter gGmbH. (2012). Trendstudie „Moderne Väter“ [Trend study "modern fathers"]. Hamburg: Behörde für Arbeit, Soziales, Familie und Integration Hamburg Retrieved from http://vaeter-ggmbh.de/wpcontent/uploads/2012/12/Trendstudie_Vaeter_gGmbH_Download.pdf. Vogt, A.-C., & Pull, K. (2010). Warum Väter ihre Erwerbstätigkeit (nicht) unterbrechen: Mikroökonomische versus in der Persönlichkeit des Vaters begründete Determinanten der Inanspruchnahme von Elternzeit durch Väter [Why fathers (don’t) take parental leave. Microeconomic vs. personality based determinants]. Zeitschrift für Personalforschung, 24, 48-68. Wayne, J. H., & Cordeiro, B. L. (2003). Who is a good organizational citizen? Social perception of male and female employees who use family leave. Sex Roles, 49, 233-246. doi: 10.1023/a:1024600323316 Wilde, A., & Diekman, A. B. (2005). Cross-Cultural Similarities And Differences In Dynamic Stereotypes: A Comparison Between Germany And The United States. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 188-196. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 40 Wojciszke, B., Abele, A. E., & Baryla, W. (2009). Two dimensions of interpersonal attitudes: Liking depends on communion, respect depends on agency. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 973-990. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.595 Zimmermann, F., Sieverding, M., & Müller, S. M. (2011). Gender-related traits as predictors of alcohol use in male German and Spanish university students. Sex Roles, 64, 394404. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9897-9 Running head: MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations (in Parentheses) of Participants’ Demographics in Studies 1 and 2 as a Function of Condition (0, 2, or 12 months of leave) and Gender 0 Variables Women 2 Men Women 12 Men Women Men Study 1 na 17 13 18 12 15 15 21.71 (2.42) 23.46 (2.47) 22.83 (3.03) 22.33 (2.64) 21.47 (1.73) 21.93 (2.05) Semester 3.94 (2.25) 6.77 (2.74) 5.56 (3.20) 5.17 (2.88) 4.80 (2.24) 5.83 (3.04) Subjectb 1: 61.5% 2: 0.0% 3: 7.7% 5: 30.8% 1: 50.0% 2: 11.1% 3: 0.0% 5: 38.9% 1: 58.3% 2: 33.3% 3: 8.3% 5: 0.0% 1: 60.0% 2: 0.0% 3: 20.0% 5: 20.0% 1: 66.7% 2: 0.0% 3: 0.0% 5: 26.7% Age 1: 52.9% 2: 23.5% 3: 5.9% 5: 17.6% Study 2 na 19 19 19 19 17 20 22.95 (2.95) 24.63 (2.52) 22.58 (2.67) 22.95 (2.32) 23.47 (2.15) 23.45 (2.65) Semester 5.47 (2.99) 6.79 (3.31) 5.00 (2.94) 4.47 (2.41) 5.65 (3.14) 6.30 (3.10) Age Subjectb 1: 57.9% 1: 52.6% 1: 42.1% 1: 68.4% 1: 64.7% 1: 55.0% 3: 0.0% 3: 5.3% 3: 5.3% 3: 0.0% 3: 0.0% 3: 10.0% 4: 31.6% 4: 31.6% 4: 26.3% 4: 21.1% 4: 23.5% 4: 15.0% 5: 10.5% 5: 10.5% 5: 26.3% 5: 10.5% 5: 11.8% 5: 20.0% Note. Participants’ age did not differ as a function of gender, F(1, 84) = 1.22, p = .272, in Study 1; F(1, 107) = 1.97, p = .164, in Study 2; or condition, F(2, 84) = 1.30, p =.279, in Study 1, F(2, 107) = 1.58, p = .210, in Study 2. Participants’ semester also did not differ as a function of gender, F(1, 81) = 3.78, p = .056, in Study 1; F(1, 107) = 0.73, p = .396, in Study 2; or condition, F(2, 81) < 0.01, p = .998, in Study 1; F(2, 107) = 2.47, p = .090, in Study 2. Study subject did not differ by gender, Fisher’s exact test p = .843 in Study 1; Fisher’s exact test p = .774 in Study 2; or condition, Fisher’s exact test p = .253 in Study 1; Fisher’s exact test p = .860 in Study 2. Subjects: 1 = business studies, 2 = business and economics education, 3 = economics, 4 = management, 5 = other. aAbsolute numbers. bPercentages. MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 42 Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations (in Parentheses) for all Dependent Variables in Study 1 as a Function of Condition (0, 2, or 12 Months of Leave) and Gender 0 2 12 Variables Women n 17 13 18 12 15 15 Communion 3.18a (0.56) 3.82a (0.97) 4.66 (0.97) 4.90 (0.75) 4.84 (0.59) 4.97 (0.79) Agency 4.51b (0.87) 3.79b (0.88) 4.23 (0.88) 4.58 (1.06) 4.27 (0.94) 4.06 (0.99) Likeability 4.27 (1.09) 4.06 (0.86) 5.28 (0.65) 4.71 (1.05) 5.33 (0.65) 4.80 (0.94) Respect 5.09c (0.81) 3.65c (1.31) 5.33 (0.89) 5.04 (0.99) 5.07d (1.05) 4.10d (1.21) Competence 5.12e (0.97) 4.25e (0.85) 4.77 (1.09) 4.43 (1.24) 4.79 (0.66) 4.46 (1.03) Men Women Men Women Men 5.49f 4.65f 5.23 4.73 5.01 4.52 (0.88) (1.13) (1.10) (1.52) (0.94) (1.14) Note. Possible values ranged from 1 to 7; higher values indicate higher ratings. Main effects for condition were significant for communion, F(5, 84) = 13.39, p < .001, partial η² = .41; liking, F(5, 84) = 9.75, p < .001, partial η² = .19; and respect, F(5, 84) = 5.21, p = .007, partial η² = .11. Main effects for gender were significant for respect, F(5, 84) = 17.76, p < .001, partial η² = .18; competence, F(5, 84) = 5.97, p = .017, partial η² = .07; and hiring, F(5, 84) = 6.67, p = .012, partial η² = .07.Means that share a subscript are significantly different from each other (simple main effects, tests for differences for a variable only within condition). Hiring MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 43 Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations (in Parentheses) for all Dependent Variables in Study 2 as a Function of Condition (0, 2, or 12 Months of leave) and Gender 0 2 12 Variables Women n 19 19 19 19 17 20 Communion 3.48 (1.00) 3.89 (1.04) 4.33 (1.01) 3.79 (1.19) 4.42 (0.86) 4.17 (0.97) Agency 4.98a (0.81) 4.30a (0.92) 4.09 (1.14) 4.44 (1.04) 4.54 (0.75) 4.44 (0.83) Likeability 4.24 (1.25) 4.34 (1.05) 5.00 (1.34) 4.61 (1.12) 5.15 (0.77) 4.95 (0.94) Respect 5.11 (0.77) 4.63 (1.03) 4.89 (1.29) 4.81 (1.23) 4.97 (0.96) 4.72 (1.04) Competence 5.50 (0.61) 4.87 (0.89) 4.71 (1.31) 4.97 (1.22) 5.12 (0.79) 5.06 (0.99) Men Women Men Women Men Hiring 5.00 4.45 4.76 4.60 5.19 4.86 (1.07) (1.55) (1.49) (1.45) (0.81) (1.24) Note. Possible rating values ranged from 1 to 7; higher values indicate higher ratings. Main effects for condition were significant for communion, F(2, 107) = 3.46, p = .035, partial η² = .06; and likeability, F(2, 106) = 4.66, p = .012, partial η² = .08. Means that share a subscript are significantly different from each other (simple main effects, tests for differences for a variable only within condition). MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 44 Appendix The original German scales used in Studies 1 and 2 can be found in Table A1. The original German wording of the scenarios can be found in Table A2. Table A1 Original German Scales Used in Studies 1 and 2 Scale Items Agency See Zimmermann, Sieverding, & Müller, 2011 Communion See Zimmermann, Sieverding, & Müller, 2011 Liking Für wie sympathisch halten Sie den Bewerber? Wie sehr würden die Kollegen in der Firma den Bewerber mögen? Respect Wie sehr würden Sie den Bewerber respektieren? Wie sehr würde der Bewerber von den Kollegen in der Firma respektiert werden? Competence See Hossiep & Paschen, 2003, for achievement motivation and leadership motivation Der Bewerber ist kompetent. Hiring probability Inwieweit soll der Bewerber für die ausgeschriebene Stelle in die Vorwahl einbezogen werden? Soll der Bewerber zu einem Vorstellungsgespräch eingeladen werden? Würden Sie selbst den Bewerber für die ausgeschriebene Stelle einstellen? Wie wahrscheinlich ist es, dass der Bewerber die Stelle bekommt? See Stöber, 1999 SDS-17 Manipulation check War der Bewerber in Elternzeit? Wenn ja: Wie lange war der Bewerber in Elternzeit?“ MEN AND PARENTAL LEAVE 45 Table A2 Parental Leave Manipulation in the Curriculum Vitae (CV) and the Cover Letter German original English translation - - CV Study 1 0 months 2 months 03.2010-05.2010 Elternzeit 03.2010-05.2010 Parental leave 12 months 03.2010-03.2011 Elternzeit 03.2010-03.2011 Parental leave Study 2 0 months - - 2 months 03.2011-05.2011 Elternzeit 03.2011-05.2011 Parental leave 12 months 03.2011-03.2012 Elternzeit 03.2011-03.2012 Parental leave 0 months Momentan möchte ich mich beruflich weiterentwickeln. At the moment, I would like to advance professionally. 2 months Nach der Geburt meines zweiten Kindes und der darauf folgenden Elternzeit von zwei Monaten möchte ich mich beruflich weiterentwickeln. After the birth of my second child and the corresponding parental leave of 2 months, I would like to advance professionally. Cover letter 12 months Nach der Geburt meines zweiten After the birth of my second child Kindes und der darauf folgenden and the corresponding parental Elternzeit von zwölf Monaten leave of 12 months, I would like to möchte ich mich beruflich advance professionally. weiterentwickeln. Note. In Study 1, we used a position in human relations, in Study 2, a position in finance. The parental leave manipulation was always the same in the cover letter. In the Curriculum Vitae, the manipulation varied by the date of parental leave (1 year later in Study 2) because the data in Study 2 were collected 1 year later.