Prospective Memory: A Neuropsychological Study
Transcription
Prospective Memory: A Neuropsychological Study
Neuropsychology 1999, Vol. 13, No. 1, 103-110 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0894-4105/99/S3.00 Prospective Memory: A Neuropsychological Study Mark A. McDaniel Elizabeth L. Glisky and Susan R. Rubin University of New Mexico University of Arizona Melissa J. Guynn Barbara C. Routhieaux University of New Mexico University of Arizona To examine the neuropsychology of prospective remembering, older adults were divided preexperimentally into 4 groups on the basis of their scores on 2 composite measures: one assessing frontal lobe function and the other assessing medial temporal lobe function. The groups reflected the factorial combination of high and low functioning for each neuropsychological system, and they were tested on an event-based laboratory prospective memory task. High-functioning frontal participants showed better prospective remembering than lowfunctioning frontal participants. There was no significant difference in prospective memory performance attributable to medial temporal functioning. The results support the theoretical notion that frontal lobe processes play a key role in prospective remembering. Discussion focuses on the particular components of prospective memory performance that frontal lobes might mediate. A kind of memory task that is pervasive in daily activities is remembering to perform some intended action at a particular point in the future (in keeping with the literature, we label this prospective memory). Examples of this kind of task are remembering to give a colleague a message when you next see her or remembering to buy bread when you pass the store on the way home. Despite the pervasiveness of prospective memory in everyday life, there has been little experimental research on this topic (see Brandimonte, Einstein, & McDaniel, 1996), especially in contrast to the plethora of work published on retrospective memory. In terms of theoretical treatments of memory in general, currently popular theoretical approaches incorporate considerations of neuropsychological systems that subserve various kinds of memory performance (e.g., see Schacter & Tulving, 1994). Neuropsychological models of memory, however, also fail to consider the processes and systems that underlie prospective memory. The purpose of this article is to sketch some preliminary theoretical approaches concerning the neuropsychology of prospective memory, and to present an experiment to inform these approaches. In this article we focus on event-based prospective remembering. Event-based prospective memory tasks are those for which some environmental event signals the appropriateness of the intended action (e.g., remembering to give a friend a message when you encounter the friend; Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). By contrast, in timebased prospective memory tasks, a particular time or a particular amount of elapsed time signals the appropriateness of the intended action (a 2:00 PM dentist appointment; take cookies out of oven in 10 min). Time-based prospective memory tasks have been associated with significant agerelated deficits and generally lower performance than observed in event-based tasks (Einstein, McDaniel, Richardson, Guynn, & Cunfer, 1995; Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell, & Mayhorn, 1997). Because the present approach required exclusive testing of older adults, low performances with a time-based prospective memory task could have diminished the experimental sensitivity of influences of the neuropsychological systems discussed next (cf. McDaniel, Glisky, Routhieaux, & Remers, 1994). Accordingly, this initial study used event-based prospective memory as the experimental task. When speculations concerning the neuropsychology of prospective memory are offered, albeit infrequently, the typical notion is that prospective remembering depends largely on prefrontal systems (cf. Bisiacchi, 1996; Burgess & Shallice, 1997; Glisky, 1996; West, 1996). There are many formal aspects to a prospective memory task that appear to articulate with putative frontal lobe functions. For example, in prospective memory tasks there is no external agent prompting the person to initiate a search of memory. Therefore, one hypothesis is that prospective memory requires monitoring the environment for the event or marker (or cue) that signals the appropriateness of the intended activity (Bisiacchi, 1996; Burgess & Shallice, 1997). In one neuropsychological model of cognition, these kinds of processes are mediated by a "supervisory attentional sys- Mark A. McDaniel and Melissa J. Guynn, Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico; Elizabeth L. Glisky, Susan R. Rubin, and Barbara C. Routhieaux, Department of Psychology, University of Arizona. This project was supported in part by National Institute on Aging Grants AG05627 and AG09195. We thank Alysha Teed and Laurel Remers for their assistance in testing participants, and we appreciate helpful comments from Art Shimamura on an earlier version of this article. A subset of the data reported herein was presented at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, Georgia, April 1994. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mark A. McDaniel, Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico 87131. Electronic mail may be sent to mcdaniel@unm.edu. 103 104 McDANIEL, GLISKY, RUBIN, GUYNN, AND ROUTHIEAUX tern," a system intimately linked to frontal functioning (Burgess & Shallice, 1997; Shallice & Burgess, 1991; see also Cohen & O'Reilly, 1996). Further, some theorists have suggested that once the general significance of the environmental event (cue) has been noticed, retrospective memory is then probed to attempt retrieval of the intended action (McDaniel, 1995; Einstein & McDaniel, 1996b; McDaniel & Einstein, 1993). To the extent that such retrieval processes are strategic and voluntary, they may involve prefrontal systems (Moscovitch, 1994; Shimamura, Janowsky, & Squire, 1991). Retrieval of the intended action may also depend on the activation level of the memory representation of the intended action, with activation level related to planning initiated at the time of intention formation (Mantyla, 1996). Such planning processes have been linked to frontal lobe structures (cf. Burgess & Shallice, 1997). Finally, having activated the intended activity at the appropriate time, the person must then interrupt or inhibit ongoing actions, and organize and execute a sequence of several responses (including the prospective memory response; Guynn, McDaniel, & Einstein, in press; McDaniel, Robinson-Riegler, & Einstein, 1998). These kinds of processes are also presumed to be supported by frontal structures (Petrides & Milner, 1982; Shimamura, 1994). In sum, there is a general theoretical basis on which to link prospective remembering with prefrontal functioning. Up to this point, however, there have been very few attempts to empirically establish such linkages. To our knowledge the only published work suggesting a relationship between frontal functioning and prospective memory consists of several single case studies. Cockburn (1995) described a patient with bilateral frontal infarcts who displayed prospective memory failures. Shallice and Burgess (1991) tested 3 patients with evidence of focal frontal lobe damage on planning tasks. The patients performed poorly relative to normal participants on the planning tasks. To the extent that the tasks involved some prospective memory components (as argued by Burgess & Shallice, 1997), then these results may implicate frontal involvement in prospective remembering. In a pilot study reported by Bisiacchi (1996; conducted by Sgaramella, Zattin, Bisacchi, Verne, & Rago, 1993) 3 closed-head-injury patients with frontal lobe involvement were tested on laboratory prospective memory tasks. The findings were mixed, with 2 patients completely failing to remember to perform the prospective memory activity and the other patient remembering on 100% of the trials. Moreover, when considered as a group, it is unclear whether the average performance level obtained for the frontal patients would be impaired relative to various control patient groups with unimpaired frontal function. Thus, the idea that prefrontal neuropsychological systems play a role in prospective memory remains virtually untested. The current study was conducted to provide an initial experimental test of that idea. It should be noted that it is not a foregone conclusion that prospective memory, especially the kind of event-based task studied herein, would be primarily frontally based. Another neuropsychological system that could play a primary role in event-based prospective memory is the medial-temporal system, including the hippocampus (for purposes of exposition we subsequently refer to this system as the hippocampal system). For example, in Moscovitch's (1994) neuropsychological model of memory, the hippocampal system supports conscious recollection through an external cue that automatically interacts with information that has been previously associated with that cue. If the cue is good enough, then the hippocampal system rapidly, obligatorily, and with few cognitive resources delivers to consciousness the information associated with the cue. That is, the system responds reflexively to cues—it cannot conduct a memory search if cues are initially ineffective. One hypothesis, then, is that prospective remembering is a reflexive process that is either successfully stimulated by the presence of the event or fails if the event is an ineffective cue (McDaniel et al., 1998). This hypothesis is consistent with participants' reports concerning their introspections (for event-based laboratory prospective memory tasks like those used in the present experiment) that the intended action appears to "pop into mind" on presentation of the event signaling that the action should be performed (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990, 1996a). Thus, the idea here is that an involuntary associative memory system (hippocampal) may be more intimately involved in event-based prospective memory than a system responsible for self-initiated retrieval activity (frontal). To inform the alternative theoretical views introduced above, we selected older adults that varied in their performance on neuropsychological tests presumed to assess hippocampal functioning and frontal functioning. Specifically, we sampled older adults whose performance on the neuropsychological tests placed them in one of four cells representing the factorial combination of high and low hippocampal functioning crossed with high and low frontal functioning. The participants were then given an eventbased laboratory prospective memory task patterned on that used by Einstein et al. (1995). We reasoned that if this kind of prospective memory task involved planful, strategic, frontally associated retrieval processes (e.g., Bisiacchi, 1996), then high-frontal-functioning adults should perform better than low-frontal-functioning adults and there should be no modulating influence of hippocampal functioning. That is, for both low- and high-hippocampal participants, there should be significant enhancement of prospective memory associated with high frontal functioning. In contrast, on the view that event-based prospective remembering is predominantly a hippocampally (associative system) mediated process (Guynn et al., in press; McDaniel et al., 1998), there should be main effects of the hippocampal variable, with the high-hippocampal-functioning participants performing better than the low-hippocampal-functioning participants, but no effect of the frontal variable. Of course, it is possible that the hippocampal and frontal systems cooperate to support the processes involved in prospective memory. If so, then an interaction between the hippocampal and frontal variables could emerge, with a more complex framework than those sketched above being implicated. One other feature of our experiment requires comment. PROSPECTIVE MEMORY As stated at the outset, the proposal that prefrontal systems are involved in prospective memory is based on the perspective that there are many component processes in prospective memory that are potentially related to frontal functioning (cf. Bisiacchi, 1996). These include monitoring for the event and identifying it as a significant signal, probing memory for the intended action once the event has been identified, and coordinating the interruption of the ongoing task along with the execution of the prospective memory activity. As an initial attempt to more precisely identify the particular processes in prospective remembering that are associated with prefrontal systems (if any), we manipulated the saliency of the target event. Past work has shown that making the target event more salient significantly enhances prospective memory performance, presumably because a more salient target event (hereafter termed the cue) would not be as dependent on self-initiated monitoring and more directly signals the significance of the event (Brandimonte & Passolunghi, 1994; Einstein & McDaniel, 1990; McDaniel & Einstein, 1993). We reasoned that if frontal processes were primarily involved in monitoring, then decrements in prospective memory for the low-frontal group relative to the high-frontal group would be especially pronounced in the low-salient cue condition (a Frontal X Cue Salience interaction). If, on the other hand, frontal-related decrements in prospective memory were as robust for the high-salient as for the low-salient cue condition, then the frontal effects might more fruitfully be interpreted as reflecting problems in probing memory once the cue has been identified as such, or coordinating the execution of various activities at time of retrieval, or both. Method Participants and Design Forty-one older adults between the ages of 66 and 85 years (M = 74 years) were selected from a larger pool of elderly research participants at the University of Arizona's Amnesia and Cognition Unit. All members of the pool are healthy, community-dwelling volunteers without dementia, depression, or any history of neurological, psychiatric, or substance abuse problems. Those who participated in this study were paid $5/hr. All of the individuals in the pool have been categorized along two dimensions on the basis of a factor analytic study of neuropsychological test performance (Glisky, Polster, & Routhieaux, 1995). The analysis revealed two independent factors: one based on tests of memory function, which we have referred to as a Hippocampal factor, and the other based on tests associated with frontal function, which we have labeled a Frontal factor. Although we cannot be sure that the factors reflect actual functioning of these particular brain regions, previous studies have shown that they are predictive of performance on tasks presumed to be associated with these structures (Glisky et al., 1995). The Hippocampal factor is based on a composite measure derived from Logical Memory I, Verbal Paired Associates I, and Visual Paired Associates II from the Wechsler Memory Scale— Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) and Long-Delay Cued Recall from the California Verbal Learning Test (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987). The Frontal factor is based on a composite measure derived from the number of categories achieved on the modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Hart, Kwentus, Wade, & Taylor, 1988), the total number of words generated on the Controlled Oral 105 Word Association Test (Spreen & Benton, 1977), Mental Arithmetic from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981), Mental Control and Backward Digit Span from the WMS-R. The composite scores represent average z scores relative to a normative population of 100 older adults from a previous factor analytic study. Variance attributable to age was removed prior to the factor analysis. Of the 41 participants in the present study, 21 had z scores above the mean and 20 had z scores below the mean on the frontal composite measure. All z scores were greater than the absolute value of .08, so that no participant was at or nearly at the mean for either composite measure. Eleven of the participants in the high-frontal group and 10 in the low-frontal group had hippocampal scores above the mean. The remainder of the participants in each frontal group (10 from each group) had hippocampal scores below the mean. Thus, there were 10 participants in three of the cells formed by the combination of the two composite measures of frontal lobe and hippocampal function and 11 participants in the fourth cell. A separate two-factor (frontal, hippocampal) betweensubjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each composite measure confirmed that the high- and low-functioning frontal groups differed significantly from each other with respect to the frontal composite score, F( 1,37) = 117.71, MSB = 0.10, p< .05,but the hippocampal groups did not differ on the frontal score (F < 1). Similarly, the high- and low-functioning hippocampal groups differed significantly on the hippocampal score, F(l, 37) = 87.39, MSB = 0.16, p < .05, but the frontal groups did not differ on that score (F < 1). Further, there were no significant interactions (see Table 1). Other characteristics of the four groups are also shown in Table 1. Between-subjects ANOVAs (2 X 2) on the demographic variables revealed no group differences in education or the MiniMental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). Age varied significantly across the cells, with the high-frontal-highhippocampal and low-frontal-low-hippocampal groups being slightly older on average than the remaining two groups, F( 1, 37) = 5.96, MSB = 23.52, p < .05, for the interaction. No significant group differences were observed in intelligence as assessed by Performance IQ on the WAIS-R. The Performance IQ measure was chosen because none of its subtests were included in the frontal or hippocampal composite scores. In addition, because several of the older adults were over the age of 74, the Mayo Older Americans Normative Studies norms (Ivnik, Malec, & Smith, 1992) were used to calculate the IQ scores for all of the sample. For Verbal IQ, the Vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-R indicated that scores were significantly lower for low-frontal than for high-frontal participants, F(l, 36) = 11.4, MSB = 56.51, p < .05, and for low-hippocampal relative to high-hippocampal participants, F(l, 36) = 9.54, MSB = 56.5\,p < .05 (for 1 participant there was no vocabulary score). These main effects were qualified by an interaction showing that the low-frontal-low-hippocampal group showed lower vocabulary scores than the other three groups, F( 1, 36) = 4.18, MSB = 56.51, p< .05. Materials and Procedure The ongoing task in this study was a multiple choice test of general knowledge, facts, and trivia. Two different sets of 172 questions were selected from materials used in Einstein et al. (1995, Experiment 3) and from popular trivia games. Half of the participants received Set 1 and half received Set 2. Each question consisted of one or two sentences followed by four alternative choices. Participants indicated their answers by pressing one of four keys on the computer keyboard (the / g, h, or j keys, which 106 McDANIEL, GLISKY, RUBIN, GUYNN, AND ROUTHIEAUX Table 1 Mean Characteristics of Experimental Groups Frontal functioning Low High Participant characteristics High hippocampal Age PIQ Educ Vocab MMSE Frontal" score Hippocampala score 73.0 120.6 16.1 62.1 29.0 .45 .59 Low hippocampal High hippocampal Low hippocampal 69.9 116.0 15.8 59.6 28.7 .63 -.78 69.1 113.8 15.0 58.9 28.3 -.47 .44 73.4 110.0 14.4 46.7 28.0 -.59 -.53 Note. PIQ = Performance IQ on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (Wechsler, 1981); Educ = years of education; Vocab = vocabulary; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). "See text for explanation of this score. were labeled a, b, c, d to correspond to the multiple choices). Embedded in this task were eight target questions about presidents in which the word president appeared (Set 1) or eight target questions about states in which the word state appeared (Set 2). The target questions appeared approximately every 5 min in the course of the general knowledge task. Salience of the target words was also manipulated: Targets were either highlighted by using bold-faced type (i.e., made salient), or they appeared normally (i.e., indistinguishable from other words in terms of display format). Highlighting was blocked such that either the first four or the last four targets were highlighted in counterbalanced order across question sets. Participants received instructions concerning the general knowledge task and were told that from time to time they would see questions about presidents (Set 1) or states (Set 2) and at that time they should respond by pressing the F8 key on the computer. This constituted the prospective memory task. After these instructions were given and before the start of the general knowledge task, participants were engaged in one of two retrospective memory tasks (the tasks were varied for purposes not central for present concerns). These retrospective memory tasks were included to introduce a delay between the prospective memory instructions and the general knowledge task (following Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). Half of the participants received a computerized version (i.e., presented visually) of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey, 1964). This test consisted of the presentation of 15 unrelated concrete nouns presented at the rate of 1 every 2 s, followed by an immediate test of free recall. This study-test sequence was repeated five times. Words were presented by computer and recall was written. The test took approximately 12 min to complete. The other half of the participants received a recognition memory test in which 56 unrelated words were presented on the computer screen at the rate of 1 every 2 s. Immediately after presentation, participants were given a paper and pencil recognition test on which the 56 studied words were randomly mixed with 56 new words. Participants were instructed to circle all of the old items that they could remember and to rate their degree of confidence in their decision (1 = very confident, 2 = fairly confident, 3 = unsure, but I think I've seen it before). This task required approximately 10 min. After the recall or recognition task, participants began the general knowledge multiple choice task. No additional instructions concerning the prospective memory task were provided at this time. General knowledge questions were presented on the computer one at a time for 12s, during which the participants were required to read the question and make a keypress indicating their choice of answer. Feedback was provided. It took approximately 40 min to complete this task. Results The rejection level for all statistical tests was set at .05. We first report the analyses associated with the question answering task and the other retrospective memory tasks, and these data are displayed in Table 2. The analyses of the prospective memory data follow. Retrospective Memory Tasks The proportions of correct responses on the question answering task were tabulated and submitted to a 2 X 2 between-subjects ANOVA, with frontal functioning (high, low) and hippocampal functioning (high, low) as variables (see Table 2 for the means). This analysis indicated that high frontals answered significantly more questions correctly Table 2 Question Answering, Recall, and Recognition Performance Dependent measure and hippocampal functioning Frontal functioning High Low .50 .47 .44 .36 .75 .68 .70 .62 .64 .24 .59 .47 .70 .62 .64 .54 .16 .31 .12 .14 3 Question answering High Low 2 Recall High (n = 4) Low (n = 6) Recognition scoreb High (n = 6) Low (n — 4) Hits High Low FAs High Low Note. A participant in the high frontal-high hippocampal group was excluded from the recognition means because of 100% hits coupled with 100% FAs. FAs = false alarms. "Proportion correct. b(Hits - FAs)/l - FAs. PROSPECTIVE MEMORY than low frontals, F(l, 37) = 7.34, MSB = 112.56 (percentage scores were used in the ANOVA), and that high hippocampals tended to answer more questions than low hippocampals, F(l, 37) = 3.24, p < .08. There was no significant interaction between the frontal and the hippocampal factors. The recognition and recall tests were also scored. Recall scores represent the proportion of correct responses. To obtain a measure of recognition that considers both the hit and false alarm (FA) rates we computed the following recognition scores: (Hits - FAs)/l - FAs (Table 2 gives the means for recall, recognition scores, hits, and FAs). Because each test was administered to half of the participants, the number of observations is small. Even so, a 2 X 2 between-subjects ANOVA of the recognition scores found that high hippocampals performed significantly better than low hippocampals, F(l, 16) = 4.67, MSB = .07, whereas there was no significant effect of frontal functioning (F < 1) nor an interaction. A 2 X 2 between-subjects ANOVA of the recall data revealed no significant effects. Prospective Memory Table 3 Prospective Memory Performance as a Function of Neuropsychological Condition and Cue Salience High Nonsalient Salient M Low Nonsalient Salient M MSE = .17. There were no other significant effects in the overall ANOVA. Because frontal status significantly affected performance on the task in which the prospective memory task was embedded (question answering), it could be the case that the effect of frontal status on prospective memory was due in part to differential difficulty across groups in the ongoing activity (cf. Einstein, Smith, McDaniel, & Shaw, 1997). To statistically factor out any influence on prospective memory of the question answering task per se, we computed an analysis of covariance on the prospective memory scores with question answering performance as the covariate and frontal and hippocampal status as independent variables. The adjusted means still showed diminished performance for the low-frontal (.44) relative to the high-frontal group (.66), a difference that was marginally significant, F(l, 36) = 3.16, MSE = .13, p < .09. In contrast, there was little variation in prospective memory as a function of the hippocampal variable (F < 1; low-hippocampal = .51 and high-hippocampal = .59). Discussion For the high-salient cue and the low-salient cue conditions, we computed the proportion of times that each participant remembered to press the response key out of four possible tries (yielding two scores per participant). Table 3 provides the means. These scores were included in a 2 X 2 X 2 mixed ANOVA, with the frontal and hippocampal status as between-subjects variables and cue salience as the within-subject variable. There was a significant main effect of frontal status, with the high-frontal group (M = .72) performing significantly better than the low-frontal group (M = .38), F(l, 37) = 8.20, MSB = .29. Though the high-hippocampal group showed a nominal advantage over the low-hippocampal group (.63 vs. 46, respectively), this difference was not significant, F(l, 37) = 1.94,p < .20. A significant main effect of cue salience emerged, such that high-salient cues produced better prospective memory than low-salient cues, F(l, 37) = 12.12, MSE = .05. Cue salience did not interact with frontal status (F < 1), and comparisons confirmed that frontal status significantly affected prospective memory for highly salient cues, F(\, 37) = 7.78, MSE = .18, and less salient cues, F(l, 37) = 6.13, Hippocampal functioning and cue salience 107 Frontal functioning High Low .70 .91 .81 .38 .52 .45 .52 .72 .62 .22 .38 .30 This experiment provides initial evidence regarding the neuropsychological systems that are involved in prospective remembering. Older adults with lower than average scores on neuropsychological tests associated with frontal functioning showed diminished prospective memory performance relative to older adults with higher than average scores on frontal tests. This pattern is in line with theoretical speculation that prefrontal systems subserve significant processes in prospective memory. The findings are not as clear-cut with regard to hippocampal involvement in prospective memory. Older adults with low scores on neuropsychological tests implicating hippocampal functioning showed decreased prospective memory performance compared to older adults with high scores on the hippocampal tests, but this difference was not statistically significant. We first consider the implications of the differences between the low- and high-frontal groups, and then discuss the outcome associated with the hippocampal variable. The present support for prefrontal involvement in prospective memory is indirect, as it rests on the assumption that the factor scores are reflective of the functioning of the corresponding brain regions. Though the tests that comprised the frontal factor have been found to be sensitive to frontal dysfunction (cf. Glisky et al., 1995), it is possible that their common variance is associated with cognitive processes that are partly or wholly dependent on other unspecified brain regions. Though this possibility cannot be categorically rejected, there is cause to consider the neuropsychologically defined frontal variable to be a reasonable first approximation to illuminating frontal function. In this regard, most of the present participants (78%) were included in two studies that examined item and source memory for sentential material (Glisky et al., 1995, and an unpublished study). The results consistently showed a dissociation such that the participants with the low-frontal factor scores demonstrated significant decline in source memory but not item memory 108 McDANIEL, GLISKY, RUBIN, GUYNN, AND ROUTHIEAUX relative to the participants with high-frontal factor scores. Because source memory but not item memory is implicated in frontal functioning (Janowsky, Shimamura, & Squire, 1989; Shimamura & Squire, 1987), this pattern converges with the present assumption that the Frontal factor as presently denned was reflective of frontal lobe function. Given the theoretical consensus that frontal systems play a role in prospective remembering (Bisiacchi, 1996; Glisky, 1996; Guynn, McDaniel, & Einstein, in press) and in light of the present evidence converging on that consensus, some speculation concerning the role of frontal systems in prospective memory seems warranted. One theoretical view of prospective memory is that it involves processes that monitor the environment for "markers" that signal the appropriateness of performing the intended action (cf. Bisiacchi, 1996; Burgess & Shallice, 1997). Frontal lobes might be involved in these putative monitoring processes. If so, then one might expect a greater degree of prospective memory decrements for low-frontal-functioning individuals with less salient cues (markers) than for salient cues. The reasoning here is that less salient cues would demand more monitoring and so should be more likely to show decrements if monitoring were compromised. This logic is supported by findings of Einstein, McDaniel, Manzi, and Cochran (reported by McDaniel, Guynn, & Einstein, 1997), in which a divided attention task (which should hamper monitoring) produced declines in prospective memory for nonsalient cues but not for salient cues (with saliency manipulated by means of perceptual features, as in the present experiment). In opposition to the above expectation, the deficits in prospective memory for low-frontal participants were as robust for the salient cues as for the nonsalient cues. Note that the absence of an interaction between frontal functioning and cue salience is not due to a weak manipulation of cue salience; high-salient cues increased prospective memory 39% over low-salient cues. Thus, it may be that frontal lobes are not closely tied to the kinds of monitoring processes required for prospective memory (if such processes are indeed involved), at least for the present range of frontal functioning examined or for the particular locus of dysfunction reflected by the present neuropsychological assessment. Probing memory for cue significance, the intended action, or both may require strategic retrieval processes, processes that have been linked to frontal lobes (Moscovitch, 1994; Shimamura, 1994). The idea here is that low-frontal participants in the present experiment may have displayed reduced prospective memory performance because they were compromised in their ability to retrieve the requisite information concerning the cue. This interpretation is consonant with the finding that low-frontal participants showed a significant decline in retrieving the correct information to the general knowledge questions as well. Countervening the above interpretation is the observation that the retrieval of the intended activity (once the cue is noticed) should not be particularly difficult. In line with this claim, in another study with a similar kind of intended (prospective memory) action (press an F# key on the keyboard) paired with a highly salient cue, prospective memory performance was nearly perfect (over 90%) for older adult participants (McDaniel et al., 1997). This suggests that given that the cue is noticed, the intended action is readily retrieved. Further support for this assertion comes from a divided attention condition in which a secondary task was added to the ongoing activity (reading text passages) during the prospective memory retrieval phase. There was no decline in performance for older adults in the high salient cue condition, again suggesting that once the cue is noticed very little strategic processing is needed for retrieving the intended action. These considerations suggest another formulation. Retrieval of the intended action could depend in part on the level of resting activation of the intended action (Mantyla, 1996; see also Marsh, Hicks, & Bink, 1998) that would influence the success of an associative retrieval process triggered by the prospective memory cue (Guynn et al., in press; McDaniel & Einstein, 1993; McDaniel et al., 1998). The level of resting activation would be a function of the quality of the initial encoding or planning processes (Mantyla, 1996), and this encoding process has been posited to be supported by frontal subsystems (Burgess & Shallice, 1997). The present finding of improved prospective remembering with increased frontal functioning (as assessed by the neuropsychological tests) for both low and high-salient cue conditions is consistent with this idea that frontal processes might be involved in forming a coherent, highly activated encoding of the anticipated prospective memory cue and the intended action. Frontal processes may be implicated in one final component of performing a prospective memory task. Once the intended action has been retrieved, successful prospective memory performance requires that participants maintain ongoing goals in working memory (associated with the ongoing activity as well as with the intended action), and interrupt and initiate execution of the intended prospective memory action (cf. Guynn et al., in press; McDaniel et al., 1998). Neuropsychological studies using tasks that require participants to organize a sequence of responses to be executed show a severe and across the board (e.g., using different stimulus materials) deficit for patients with frontal lobe excisions (see Petrides & Milner, 1982). Accordingly, the present decline in prospective memory performance due to low frontal functioning may reflect deficits in the executive (or working memory) processes that are required for interrupting ongoing activity and scheduling the execution of the prospective memory task. More specifically, for low-frontal participants the demands of processing and attempting to answer the general knowledge-trivia questions could completely capture their attention such that even if the intended action were retrieved, these participants would not have the available inhibitory mechanisms to interrupt the question answering activity and switch resources to execute the competing activity. This interpretation of frontal involvement in prospective memory coincides with Shallice and Burgess's (1991) finding that patients with frontal lesions are significantly impaired in inhibiting performance on one activity in order to initiate another intended activity within a certain time frame. The impact of the question answering task is potentially PROSPECTIVE MEMORY more complicated. Because low-frontal participants did not perform as well on this task as the high-frontal participants, it is possible that question answering posed more difficulty or processing demands to low-frontal participants. If so, then the effects of frontal functioning on prospective memory could be due to differential difficulty of the ongoing activity across the low- and high-frontal groups (cf. Einstein et al., 1997). Although this possibility cannot be completely ruled out, when performance on the question answering task was statistically factored out there remained an advantage in prospective memory for high- versus low-frontal participants. Another feature of the experiment raises an additional potential explanation of the frontal effect on prospective remembering. Because the event-based cues that signalled the appropriateness of the intended action occurred regularly at 5-min intervals, participants could have used time cues to perform the prospective memory task. If so, then the reduced prospective remembering for the low-frontal participants might be related to difficulties in time monitoring associated with low frontal functioning.1 Clearly, more evidence would be needed to compel this interpretation that participants relied on time cues rather than the target cues and that time perception is mediated by frontal systems. Finally, it is worth noting that there was a 17% advantage in prospective remembering for high-hippocampal relative to low-hippocampal participants. Though this difference was not statistically significant, the magnitude of the difference favoring the high-hippocampal participants approximated that reflected in the significant cue-salience effect (a withinsubjects manipulation). The absence of statistical significance for the hippocampal effect may represent some lack of precision associated with the neuropsychological groupings. Thus, it remains possible that the nominal prospective memory advantage in the high hippocampal group reflects some hippocampal involvement in prospective remembering. For instance, the hippocampal system could be involved in retrieval of the intended action upon encounter of the prospective memory cue (Guynn et al., in press; McDaniel et al., 1998). This possibility is consistent with general views that assume that hippocampal systems play a role in associative memory retrieval (Moscovitch, 1994), and suggests that even more robust hippocampal effects might be observed for prospective memory tasks in which the retrieval component is more challenging (i.e., having to remember different responses to different cues; e.g., Robinson-Riegler, 1994). In sum, the present experiment represents the first largescale study (using more than several participants) of which we are aware to examine the influence of frontal systems and hippocampal systems on prospective remembering. The findings support the hypothesis proposed by several researchers that frontal functioning should play a key role in prospective memory performance (e.g., Bisiacchi, 1996; Burgess & Shallice, 1997; Glisky, 1996; Guynn et al., in press; West, 1996). The particular components of prospective memory that are supported by frontal mechanisms still need to be identified, as well as the particular frontal subsystems that articulate with prospective memory performance. The current results seem most consistent with the 109 idea that executive functions served by frontal processes associated with the current neuropsychological assessment procedures are involved in prospective memory performance. These executive functions might include encodingplanning the prospective memory task and inhibiting ongoing activity and organizing the execution of the intended activity. Convergent with this idea, planning and organizational activities have been related to mid-dorsal lateral frontal activity (Petrides, 1995), a region also thought to be reflected in the neuropsychological frontal tests used here (Milner, 1963). The data also suggest, though less strongly, that hippocampal functioning influences prospective memory, perhaps in the service of retrieving the intended activity during presentation of the target event. 1 We thank a reviewer for raising this possibility. References Bisiacchi, P. S. (1996). The neuropsychological approach in the study of prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 297-318). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (Eds.). (1996). Prospective memory: Theory and applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brandimonte, M. A., & Passolunghi, M. C. (1994). The effect of cue-familiarity, cue-distinctiveness, and retention interval on prospective remembering. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 47(A), 656-687. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The relationship between prospective and retrospective memory: Neuropsychological evidence. In M. A. Conway (Ed.), Cognitive models of memory (pp. 247-272). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cockburn, J. (1995). Task interruption in prospective memory: A frontal lobe function? Cortex, 31, 87-97. Cohen, J. D., & O'Reilly, R. C. (1996). A preliminary theory of the interactions between prefrontal cortex and hippocampus that contribute to planning and prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 267-295). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717-726. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996a). Prospective memory: Remembering a forgotten topic. In D. Hermann, C. McEvoy, C. Hertzog, P. Hertel, & M. Johnson (Eds.), Basic and applied memory research: Vol. 2. Practical applications (pp. 79—94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996b). Retrieval processes in prospective memory: Theoretical approaches and some new empirical findings. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 115-142). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Richardson, S. L., Guynn, M. J., & Cunfer, A. R. (1995). Aging and prospective memory: Examining the influence of self-initiated retrieval processes. 110 McDANIEL, GLISKY, RUBIN, GUYNN, AND ROUTHIEAUX Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 996-1007. Einstein, G. O., Smith, R. E., McDaniel, M. A., & Shaw, P. (1997). Aging and prospective memory: The influence of increased task demands at encoding and retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 12, 479^88. Folstein, M. E, Folstein, S. E, & McHugh, P. R. (1975). MiniMental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Glisky, E. (1996). Prospective memory and frontal lobes. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 249-266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Glisky, E., Polster, M. R., & Routhieaux, B. C. (1995). Double dissociation between item and source memory. Neuropsychology, 9, 229-235. Guynn, M. J., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (in press). Remembering to perform actions: A different type of memory? In H. D. Zimmer & R. L. Cohen (Eds.), Memory for action: A distinct form of episodic memory. Hart, R. P., Kwentus, J. A., Wade, J. B., & Taylor, J. R. (1988). Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in elderly normal, depressed and demented patients. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2, 49-56. Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. E, & Smith, G. E. (1992). Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies. WAIS-R norms for ages 56-97. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 1-30. Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1043-1056. Mantyla, T. (1996). Activating actions and interrupting intentions: Mechanisms of retrieval sensitization in prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 93-113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bink, M. L. (1998). The activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 350-361. McDaniel, M. A. (1995). Prospective memory: Progress and processes. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 33, pp. 191-221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1993). The importance of cue familiarity and cue distinctiveness in prospective memory. Memory, 1, 23^1. McDaniel, M. A., Glisky, E. L., Routhieaux, B. C., & Remers, L. A. (1994, April). The role of frontal lobe functioning in temporallybased and cue-based prospective remembering in older adults. Paper presented at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA. McDaniel, M. A., Guynn, M. J., & Einstein, G. O. (1997, November). Prospective memory: Two views and some new data. Paper presented at the 38th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia. McDaniel, M. A., Robinson-Riegler, B., & Einstein, G. O. (1998). Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptuallydriven processes? Memory & Cognition, 26, 121-134. Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90-100. Moscovitch, M. (1994). Memory and working with memory: Evaluation of a component process model and comparisons with other models. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 94 (pp. 269-310). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Kidder, D. P., Morrell, R. W, & Mayhorn, C. B. (1997). Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 12, 314-327. Petrides, M. (1995). Functional organization of the human frontal cortex for mnemonic processing: Evidence from neuroimaging studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (pp. 85-96). Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249-262. Rey, A. (1964). L'Examen Clinique en Psychologic [The Clinical Examination in Psychology]. Paris: Press Universitaire de France. Robinson-Riegler, M. B. (1994). The recognition-recall hypothesis of prospective memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University. Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory systems 94. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Shallice, T, & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727-741. Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Memory and frontal lobe function. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. SOSSIS). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R. (1991). What is the role of frontal lobe damage in memory disorders? In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe functioning and dysfunction (pp. 173-195). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1987). A neuropsychological study of fact memory and source amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 464—473. Spreen, O., & Benton, A. L. (1977). Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia (NCCEA) (Revised edition). Victoria, British Columbia, Canada: University of Victoria Neuropsychology Laboratory. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychology Bulletin, 120, 272-292. Received January 14, 1998 Revision received April 27, 1998 Accepted April 28, 1998