Asymmetrical visual fields distribution of attention in dyslexic
Transcription
Asymmetrical visual fields distribution of attention in dyslexic
Neuroscience Letters 290 (2000) 216±218 www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Asymmetrical visual ®elds distribution of attention in dyslexic children: a neuropsychological study Andrea Facoetti a,b,*, Massimo Turatto b a Neuropsychology Unit, Scienti®c Institute `E. Medea', Bosisio Parini, Lecco, Italy b University of Padova, Padova, Italy Received 10 April 2000; received in revised form 29 May 2000; accepted 12 July 2000 Abstract Visual spatial attention was evaluated in dyslexic and normally reading children by using a ¯anker task. When an irrelevant distractor is presented adjacent to a target stimulus, interference is observed when the two stimuli are associated with con¯icting responses. In the present study the distractor ¯anked the target either to the right or to the left. Results showed an asymmetric ¯anker effect in dyslexics, whereas it was symmetrical in normal readers. Dyslexics exhibited a reduced ¯anker effect in the left visual ®eld, concomitant with a strong ¯anker effect in the right visual ®eld. These results give further support to the hypothesized left-side minineglect in dyslexics. Data also provide evidence for a reduced ability in suppressing distracting information in the right visual ®eld. Such visual ®eld asymmetry is thought to play a crucial role in reading disorders. Right over-distractibility and left inattention suggest an impairment of the right parietal functions as supported by the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. q 2000 Published by Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. Keywords: Dyslexia; Spatial attention; Flanker effect; Visual ®eld; Asymmetry; Right parietal lobe Developmental dyslexia is de®ned as a speci®c reading disorder despite normal intelligence and teaching, regardless of any manifest sensory de®cit. Recently, the hypothesis of an `auditory temporal processing de®cit' has become of central interest in the study of dyslexia etiology [4]. However, a temporal processing de®cit appears to affect information processing in different sensory modalities [15]. Accordingly, it has been suggested that the major problem of dyslexic children would be a general impairment in the processing of rapid streams of information, regardless of the stimulated modalities. The magnocellular-transient (M) system, which would also include the auditory system, is thought to be the neural basis underlying such de®cit [10]. The information processed by the M system ends in the parietal posterior cortex (PPC), which is an important supramodal selective spatial attention area [17]. An impairment of this area would lead to a reduced ability to focus attention in the stimulated modality. As far as the visual domain is concerned, there is evidence of both an impaired focusing of attention in dyslexic children [3] and a prolonged dwell time * Corresponding author. Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy. Tel.: 139-049-8276929, fax: 139-049-8276600. E-mail address: andreaf@psico.unipd.it (A. Facoetti). in dyslexic adults [8]. In addition, much converging evidence indicated an asymmetric distribution of attention between the two visual ®elds in dyslexics. Hari and Koivikko [7] suggested that, as compared to the right visual ®eld (RVF), dyslexics suffer from `mini-neglect' in the left visual ®eld (LVF). Fowler et al. [5], and Eden et al. [1] showed that in visual search tasks dyslexic children omitted a greater number of targets when presented in the LVF than in the RVF. This left-side de®cit appears to be linked to a right-side enhancement in the processing of visual information, as demonstrated by an increased ability of dyslexics in letter recognition in the RVF [6]. Therefore, the present study was aimed at further exploring a possible attentional visual ®eld asymmetry in dyslexia. To this purpose, we used the response competition procedure [2]. Typically participants are required to react to a central target ¯anked by either a response-compatible or response-incompatible distractor. What has been observed is that compatible distractors produce faster reaction times (RTs) than the incompatible ones. The RT difference between the compatible and the incompatible condition is de®ned as `¯anker effect'. Participants consisted of 14 dyslexic children (mean and SD, age 12.1 ^ 2.3 years, ten males, four females) and 11 normally reading children 0304-3940/00/$ - see front matter q 2000 Published by Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. PII: S0 30 4- 39 40 ( 00) 0 13 54- 9 A. Facoetti, M. Turatto / Neuroscience Letters 290 (2000) 216±218 (mean and SD, age 11.4 ^ 2.7 years, eight males, three females). All children were selected by: (1) a full scale IQ . 85, (2) normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing, (3) normal visual ®eld, (4) absence of attention de®cit disorder with hyperactivity (ADHD), (5) no known gross behavioral or emotional problems, (6) absence of drug therapy, (7) normal teaching opportunities, and (8) right manual preference. The children were classi®ed as dyslexic as their performances in oral reading of text, words and non-words were 2 SD below the norm on age-standardized Italian tests. Variables considered are speed and accuracy. The dyslexic group had signi®cantly lower reading test scores in comparison to control children (reading accuracy, one-sided t-test, P , 0:0001, and reading speed, P , 0:0001). The two groups did not differ with regard to IQs (mean IQ of dyslexics was 99 ^ 14, mean IQ of normal readers was 104 ^ 12). Tests were carried out in a dimly lit room with a ambient luminance of 1.5 cd/m 2. Participants sat in the front of a monitor (15 inches and with a luminance of 0.5 cd/m 2) with their head positioned on a headrest so that the eyes-screen distance was 40 cm. Fig. 1 shows the experimental set-up. A small white dot (0.58 of visual angle) presented in the center of the screen served as ®xation point. The target stimulus was either a left-pointing arrow or right-pointing arrow (28) presented at the center of the screen. A smaller arrow (18) ¯anking the target either to the right to the left side was used as ¯anker. Target-¯anker distance covered 18 of visual angle. In the compatible condition both the target and the ¯anker pointed to the same direction, whereas in the incompatible condition the target and the ¯anker pointed to opposite directions. Stimuli luminance was set at 24 cd/m 2. Each trial began with the onset of the ®xation point (accompanied by a 1000-Hz warning tone) which lasted for 201 ms, followed by a 50-ms blank screen. After that the target and the ¯anker appeared for 201 ms (see Fig. 1). Eye movements were monitored by means of a system composed of Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the display used in the experiment. 217 infrared-ray spectacles. Any eye movements larger than 18 was detected by the system and the correspondent trial was discarded and replaced. Participants were instructed to press a key on the keyboard as quickly as possible at the onset of the target (the `Y' key for the left-pointing arrow, and the `B' key for the right-pointing arrow). RTs were recorded by the computer with millisecond accuracy, and the maximum time allowed for response was 1500 ms. The experimental session consisted of 240 trials divided into two blocks of 120 trials distributed as follows: compatible and incompatible conditions consisted of 60 trials each. In each condition there were 30 trials in which the target and the ¯anker pointed to the left (15 trials with the ¯anker in the LVF and 15 trials with the ¯anker in the RVF), and 30 trials in which the target and the ¯anker pointed to the right (15 trials with the ¯anker in the LVF and 15 trials with the ¯anker in the RVF). Before the experiment began participants made some practice trials until they felt con®dent with the task. Fig. 2 shows participants' RTs for target detection as a function of ¯anker compatibility and location (LVF and RVF). Mean correct RTs were entered into a three-way analysis of variance in which the within-subjects factors were type of ¯anker (compatible and incompatible) and location (LVF and RVF), whereas the between-subjects factor was group (normal readers and dyslexics). The main ¯anker-type effect was signi®cant F 1; 23 66:26; P , 0:0001, the ¯anker effect was 82 ms. Also, the type of ¯anker £ location £ group interaction was signi®cant F 1; 23 6:23, P , 0:02, indicating that the two groups processed the ¯anker in the two visual ®elds differentially. Planned comparisons showed that in normal readers the ¯anker effect was present in both the LVF (M 70 ms, P , 0:02) and the RVF (M 73 ms, P , 0:02), but the difference between the two visual ®elds was not signi®cant (3 ms, P . 0:05). By contrast, dyslexics exhibited an asymmetrical ¯anker effect, with the ¯anker effect virtually absent in the LVF (M 36 ms, P . 0:05), but deeply present in the RVF (M 144 ms, P , 0:0001). On the basis of the present results it may be hypothesized that dyslexics display a reduced processing of the ¯anker in Fig. 2. Mean reaction times as a function of compatible and incompatible ¯anker condition, visual ®eld and group. Data show an asymmetrical effect of the ¯anker in dyslexics but not in normal readers. 218 A. Facoetti, M. Turatto / Neuroscience Letters 290 (2000) 216±218 the LVF, concomitant with an exaggerated ¯anker processing in the RVF. In line with this interpretation we found that in compatible trials dyslexics showed an increased ¯anker effect in the RVF vs. the LVF (P , 0:03). Likewise, in incompatible trials a decreased ¯anker effect was observed in the LVF vs. the RVF (P , 0:02). The RT pattern emerged from this study can be interpreted in light of recent ®ndings showing an asymmetrical distribution of spatial attention in dyslexics. Such an asymmetry is re¯ected by a left-side minineglect as suggested by Hari and Koivikko [7] and by Stein and Fowler [16], and by an increased ability in letter recognition in the RVF [6]. Thus, the left de®cit exhibited by dyslexic children can potentially indicate an increased inhibition for information presented in the LVF, as shown by a reduced ¯anker effect. By contrast, the profound ¯anker effect in the right side might be due to an impairment in the ability to suppress distracting information in the RVF. How can these de®cits be related to the reading problem manifested by dyslexic children? Basically, the idea is that during reading visual information presented in the RVF is not ®ltered out ef®ciently in order to obtain an ef®cient letter and/or word processing. As suggested by LaBerge and Brown [9], the ability to suppress information ¯anking the attended area would crucial especially when unfamiliar words have to be identi®ed. It can be suggested that this suppressive mechanism could be particularly defective in the RVF in dyslexics, who would be distracted by letters or words to the right from ®xation. What is more dif®cult to explain is how the left inattention de®cit might affect the reading process in dyslexic children. It can be speculated that this de®cit might play a role during regressive saccades, which are rapid backward eye movements involved in reading [11]. A right parietal cortex dysfunction is thought to be the neurological basis underlying dyslexics' attentional de®cits [7,15]. This interpretation is supported by the fact that a visual spatial de®cit similar to that emerged in the present study and in those from Hari and Koivikko [7] has also been reported by Ro et al. [12] in subjects suffering from a temporoparietal junction lesion. These patients, like dyslexics in our study, did not show the interference ¯anker effect in the contralesional visual ®eld (left side), but displayed a larger interference effect in the ipsilesional visual ®eld (right side). This suggests not only that there was no interference when incongruent distractors were presented to the controlesional ®eld but also that there was a `hyperorienting' response towards the ipsilesional ®eld [13]. In addition, a Smania et al. [14] study revealed that left-hemineglect and extinction patients with a right parietal cortex damage showed a paradoxical RT decrement in the RVF as target eccentricity increased. To conclude, the present study as well as other recent works provide converging indications that dyslexia is associated with an asymmetrical distribution of spatial attention in the visual ®eld. However, on the basis of our ®ndings, we suggest that the major problem of dyslexic children might be an inability to suppress distracting information in the RVF. [1] Eden, G.F., Stein, J.F. and Wood, F.B., Visuospatial ability and language processing in reading disabled and normal children, In S.F. Wright and R. Groner (Eds.), Studies in visual information processing: facets of dyslexia and its remediation, North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1993, pp. 321± 335. [2] Eriksen, B.A. and Eriksen, C.W., Effects of noise letters upon the identi®cation of a target letter in a non-search task, Percept. Psychophys., 16 (1974) 143±149. [3] Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M.L. and Paganoni, P., The spatial distribution of visual attention in developmental dyslexia, Exp. Brain Res., 4 (2000) 531±538. [4] Farmer, M.E. and Klein, R.M., The evidence for a temporal processing de®cit linked to dyslexia: a review, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2 (1995) 460±493. [5] Fowler, M.S., Riddell, P.M. and Stein, J.F., Vergence eye movement control and spatial discrimination in normal and dyslexic children, In G. Pavlidis (Ed.), Perspectives on Dyslexia, Wiley, London, 1990. [6] Geiger, G. and Lettvin, J.Y., Peripheral vision in persons with dyslexia, New Engl. J. Med., 316 (1987) 1238±1243. [7] Hari, R. and Koivikko, H., Left-side mini-neglect and attentional sluggishness in dyslexic adult, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 25 (1999) 1634. [8] Hari, R., Valta, M. and Uutela, K., Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adult, Neurosci. Lett., 271 (1999) 202±204. [9] LaBerge, D. and Brown, V., Theory of attentional operations in shape identi®cation, Psychol. Rev., 96 (1989) 101±124. [10] McAnally, K.I. and Stein, J.F., Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci., 263 (1996) 961± 965. [11] Morris, R.K. and Rayner, K., Eye movements in skilled reading: implications for developmental dyslexia, In J.F. Stein (Ed.), Vision and Visual Dyslexia, MacMillan Press, London, 1991, pp. 233±242. [12] Ro, T., Cohen, A., Ivry, R. and Rafal, R.D., Response channel activation and the tempoparietal junction, Brain Cogn., 37 (1998) 461±476. [13] Seyel, M., Ro, T. and Rafal, R.D., Increased sensitivity to ipsilateral cutaneous stimuli following transcranical magnetic stimulation of the parietal lobe, Ann. Neurol., 38 (1995) 264±267. [14] Smania, N., Martini, M.C., Gambina, G., Tomelleri, G., Palamara, A., Natale, E. and Marzi, C.A., The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients, Brain, 121 (1998) 1759±1770. [15] Stein, J. and Walsh, V., To see but not to read, the magnocellular theory of dyslexia, Trends Neurosci., 20 (1997) 147± 152. [16] Stein, J. and Fowler, M.S., Visual dyslexia, Trends Neurosci., 4 (1981) 77±80. [17] Vidyasagar, T.R., A neural model of attentional spotlight: parietal guiding the temporal, Brain Res. Rev., 30 (1999) 66±76.