Differential saccadic adaptation controlled by target shape and/or color
Transcription
Differential saccadic adaptation controlled by target shape and/or color
Differential saccadic adaptation controlled by target shape and/or color Laurent Madelain, Jérémie Jozefowiez & Sohir Rahmouni Laboratoire SCALab, UMR CNRS 9193,Université de Lille INTRODUCTION BLOCKING PARADIGM Procedure: Same as in the shape and color experiment but shape and color are decorrelated. Visual context cue Motor context cue Contextual saccadic adaptation has been found using motor cues but not with visual cues. Visual Hemifield Alahyane & Pelisson, 2004 Orbital eccentricity or vergence Chaturvedi & Van Gisbergen, 1997 Head position(tilt +/- 45°; supine/upright) Shelhamer & al, 2002 Gravity (parabolic flight) Shelhamer & al, 2003 Target + distractor (1) Circle goes UP Triangle goes DOWN Target + distractor (2) Single subject - target and distractor Target motion (speed or direction) Azadi & Harwood, 2014 Target shape Bahcall & Kowler, 2000 Target color Deubel, 1995 Target color & shape Azadi & Harwood, 2014 Circle goes DOWN Triangle goes UP Single subject - target only Contextual effect is blocked when two possible contexts are conflicting Target color and shape can be used as contextual cues Sorted by color Sorted by shape (1) (2) Madelain and colleagues (2010) showed that saccadic adaptation is unhampered by target selection. In this study, we ask whether target selection would facilitate context dependent saccadic adaptation. Adding a visual distractor could elicit distinct saccade gain states. P3 METHODS -20% Y P1 ° te +20% P2 Fi rs ts X -20% 90 p +20% P0 Four amplitudes were used for the first step: 7.3; 9.5; 11.7 and 13.9 45° Fixation 850 ms (+/-150) n=6 Target step until saccade (350 ms max) Trial duration 1500 ms Tim e All subjects experienced the same trial structure. Target luminance was fixed for all targets (5cd/m≤). CONCLUSION Post saccadic position Target + Distractor 500 ms Shape Color Without distractor With distractor Target and distractor (1) TRIAL TYPE A TRIAL TYPE B TRIAL TYPE A N= 18 n=18 Shape and color TRIAL TYPE A OR Target and distractor (2) OR Target only (1) OR Target only (2) OR Shape and color can be used to evoke differential gain states. Target selection makes visual context cues relevant. When two cues are conflicting, contextual effects are blocked. REFERENCES Baseline Adaptation mini-blocks Random Recovery n=18 0 100 200 300 400 500 Trial number 600 700 800 n=14 Alahyane, N., & Pelisson, D. (2004). Eye Position Specificity of Saccadic Adaptation. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 45(1), 123130. Azadi, R., & Harwood, M. R. (2014). Visual cues that are effective for contextual saccade adaptation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 111, 2307-2319. Bahcall, D.O., & Kowler, E. (2000).The control of saccadic adaptation: implications for the scanning of natural visual scenes. Vision Research, 40: 27792796. Chaturvedi, V., & Van Gisbergen, J. A. (1997). Specificity of saccadic adaptation in three-dimensional space. Vision Research, 37(10), 13671382. Deubel, H. (1995). Is saccadic adaptation context-specific? In J. M Findlay, R. Walker, & R. W. Kentridge (Eds.), Eye movement research: Mechanisms, processes and applications (pp. 177- 187). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. Madelain L., Harwood M.R., Herman J.P, Wallman J. (2010). Saccade adaptation isunhampered by distractors. J Vision 10: 29. Shelhamer, M., & Clendaniel, R. (2002). Sensory, motor, and combined contexts for context-specific adaptation of saccade gain in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 332(3), 200204. Shelhamer, M., Clendaniel,R.A., Roberts, D.C. (2003). Context-specific adaptation of saccade gain in parabolic flight. J. Vestib. Res., 12, pp. 211–221 PhD grant from local government (Région Nord Pas de calais) and grant ANR-13-APPR-008 (Agence Nationale pour la Recherche)