Nominal categories
Transcription
Nominal categories
Armin Buch Overview Case Nominal categories Armin W. Buch1 2012/11/07 1 Relying heavily on material by Gerhard Jäger and David Erschler Overview Armin Buch Overview Case I Nominal categories: I I I I I I Number Possession Gender Definiteness Case Case will lead to case systems (next week) Number • Possibilities for number: no overt marking on nouns (rare, but possible) • Plural vs Singular • Plural : Dual : Singular • Plural: Paucal : Dual : Singular Dual • Hebrew (Semitic, Afro-Asiatic): dual for natural pairs: ajin ejn-ajim regelragl-ajim ‘eye’ ‘eyes’ ‘leg’ ‘legs’ but ecba ecba-ot xatul xatul-im ‘finger, toe’ ‘fingers, toes’ ‘cat’ ‘cats’ • Dual was present in early Indo-European languages • Hierarchy (Greenberg 1966) Sing > Plur > Dual > Trial/Paucal • Sanskrit noun inflection frequencies (Greenberg): Singular = 70.3% Plural = 25.1% Dual = 4.6% Paucal: Manam Oceanic, Austronesian, New Guinea (Lichtenberk 1983) Marking of the Plural (Haspelmath 2011) Map (Haspelmath 2011) Other Nominal Grammatical Categories • • • • Number Possession Gender Definiteness Possession • Alienable and inalienable possession • John's mother and John's bike belong to John in different senses. • Inalienably possessed nouns: body parts, kin terms • Western European languages usually don't distinguish these categories. • In some Slavic languages, they are distinguished to some extenct. Alienable vs Inalienable • Russian, body parts No copula: inalienable possession U Maši golubyje glaza at Masha blue eyes 'Masha has blue eyes.' Copula: alienable possession #U Maši jestʲ golubyje at Masha is blue 'Masha owns blue eyes.' glaza eyes Alienable vs Inalienable • Digor Ossetic a. Inalienable aslan-ɐn suʁzɐrijnɐ k’oχ-tɐ jes Aslan-DAT gold hand-PL exists ‘Aslan has golden hands = He is very skillful at handwork.’ b. Alienable aslan-mɐ suʁzɐrijnɐ k’oχ-tɐ jes aslan-ALL gold hand-PL exists ‘Aslan owns golden hands (i.e. a piece of jewellery).’ Alienable vs Inalienable • Adyghe (West Caucasian, Gorbunova 2009) a. Inalienable wə-šə 2SG-brother ‘your brother’ b. Alienable w-jə-wəne 2SG-AL-house ‘your house’ Possessive classes • There might be more classes of possession: • An extreme example (via Nichols & Bickel 2011): Anêm (isolate; Papua New Guinea) "has at least 20 possessive classes based on the allomorphy of the stem-final suffix and of the possessive suffix." Four of these classes are shown on the next slide Possessive Classes in Anem 1sg 2sg 3sg.m 3sg.f ‘water’ ‘child’ ‘leg’ ‘mat’ kom-i kom-î kom-u kom-îm gi-ng-e gi-ng-ê gi-ng-o gi-ng-êm ti-g-a mîk-d-at ti-g-îr mîk-d-ir ti-g-î mîk-d-it ti-g-î mîk-d-it Possessive Classification (Nichols & Bickel 2011) Possessive Classification (Nichols & Bickel 2011) Other Nominal Grammatical Categories • • • • Number Possession Gender Definiteness Grammatical Gender • Number of genders may vary from zero (e.g. in Iron Ossetic wəj 'he, she, it' or in Modern Persian) to about 10. • Grammatical gender systems often have something to do with the natural sex (i.e. males usually belong to the masculine gender and females to the feminine gender), but systems may be very non-transparent, e.g. German nouns with -chen are always neutral. Corbett (2011) calls that 'formal assignment' Semantically Transparent Gender Systems, Corbett (2011) • In Kannada, nouns denoting male humans are masculine, those denoting female humans are feminine. There are also deities, demons and heavenly bodies in these genders. All remaining nouns, including those denoting infants and animals, are neuter. Thus appa ‘father’, and candra ‘moon’ are masculine, amma ‘mother’ is feminine, and na:yi ‘dog’ is neuter. Bininj Gun-Wok Grammatical Gender • Example: Hinuq (East Caucasian, Daghestan), Isakov & Khalilov 2001 Singular • Gender I: males, God, prophet, angels • Gender II: females, witch, houri, etc. • Gender III: 'child', animals, devils, dragons • inanimates are distributed across these 3 genders or belong to the 4th one: • Gender IV: 'flower', 'grass', 'barley', 'water', 'road', 'cave' Grammatical Gender • Example: Hinuq cont. • Plural • Rational beings (humans and such like) vs irrational beings • Gender shows in the verb agreement. Grammatical Gender: Swahili noun classes Grammatical Gender: Swahili noun classes • This ki-/vi- alteration even applies to foreign words where the ki- was originally part of the root, so vitabu "books" from kitabu "book" Number of Genders (Corbett 2011) Number of Genders Sex-based vs non-sex-based gender systems Principles of Gender Assignment, Corbett (2011) Other Nominal Grammatical Categories • • • • Number Possession Gender Definiteness Definiteness • Definite/indefinite • Dryer 2011: "There are, broadly speaking, two functions associated with definite articles. • One of these is an anaphoric function, to refer back to something mentioned in the preceding discourse. • The other is a nonanaphoric function, to refer to something not mentioned in the preceding discourse but whose existence is something that the speaker assumes is known to the hearer. Definiteness • Definite article may be a separate word distinct from a demonstrative: (1) Lakhota (Ingham 2001: 16) wic‘aṡa ki he man the that ‘that man’ Definiteness • Definiteness may be marked with a demonstrative: • (4) Eastern Ojibwa (Nichols 1988: 46) “mii maanpii wii-bkeyaanh” but here intend-turn.off.1sg kido giiwenh wa mko say.3sg it.is.said that bear ‘“Well, this is where I turn off,” the bear said.’ Definiteness • Definiteness may be marked by an affix • Hebrew ha-bayit ha-gadol DEF-house DEF-big 'the big house' Definiteness vs Specificity • An article may actually mark specific indefinites, and then it may be hard to classify it. • English I've seen a girl today. • She might be unknown to me and to the hearer, so indefinite, but specific. Definiteness • There might be a special article for this • (5) Korowai (Awju-Dumut, Trans-New Guinea; Papua, Indonesia) uma-té-do abül-fekha khomilo-bo tell-3pl.real-ds man-indef die.3sg.real-prf 'They told that a certain man had died.’ Definiteness Definiteness Definiteness • Indefinite articles (Dryer 2011) • A morpheme is considered here to be an indefinite article if it accompanies a noun and signals that the noun phrase is pragmatically indefinite in the sense that it denotes something not known to the hearer, like the English word a in a dog. • Often such a morpheme is cognate or identical to the word 'one'. Definiteness • In some languages, like English, articles belong to a category of determiner with a particular position in the noun phrase. • Other languages lack such a category in the sense that definite articles and demonstratives occur in different positions in the noun phrase and can co-occur. Definiteness • For example, the indefinite article in Dongolese Nubian is a clitic that attaches to the last word in the noun phrase, (i); however, the demonstrative precedes the noun, as in (ii). (i) hánu=wɛ̄r donkey=indef ‘a donkey’ (ii) máŋ kā́ that house ‘that house’ Definiteness • there are languages with both a definite article and an indefinite article in which the two articles appear in different positions in the noun phrase, thus apparently not forming a category in the language. • For example, in Ju|'hoan (Northern Khoisan; Namibia), the indefinite article follows the noun, while the definite article precedes the noun Definiteness Ju|'hoan (Dickens 1992: 41) a. mí hoa jù n|úí ko !aòh 1sg see person indef obl yard ‘I saw a (certain) person in the yard.’ b. ||'à jù-à kú !aàh def person-rel.sg impf run ‘The person was running.’ Semantic roles Armin Buch Overview Case Semantic roles ● ● ● Verb describe event types Arguments and adjuncts denote participants of the event described by the verb Semantic role ● type of relationship between event and participant ● invariant across verbs and languages Semantic roles ● Agent ● Elvis lived at Graceland. The cook has diced the carrots. ● Benefactive ● ● Comitative I always go to the store with my dog. ● ● ● ● ● Source Whitney entered from the rear of the stage. ● Temporal Father John left the church at noon. Instrumental Blake is walking with a cane. Recipient Ivana will donate her diamonds to the Church. Goal Ann arrived at the store to buy shoes. Purposive The pauper washed dishes for his supper. Experiencer Freud feared his mother. Patient George chopped down the cherry tree. The chapter has been written for Lindsay ● Locative ● Theme The Unabomber threatened to send a package to LAX. Grammatical relations ● ● ● ● functional roles in clauses, such as subject or object characterized by a cluster of morphological and syntactic features each subsumes several semantic roles much cross-linguistic variation regarding marking and semantic content Grammatical relations ● ● universally, languages have three grammatical roles that are realized as arguments English: ● ● ● subject The man gave Bill a ticket. direct object The man gave Bill a ticket. indirect object The man gave Bill a ticket. Grammatical relations ● inventory differs between languages German Der Junge ist dreckig. the boy.NOM is dirty 'The boy is dirty.' Der Junge wirft einen Stein. DEF boy.NOM throw a.ACC stone 'The boy is throwing a stone.' Kalkatungu (Australia) Kaun muu-yan-ati dress.ABS dirt-PROP-INCH 'The dress is dirty.' Kuntu wampa-ngku kaun not girl-ERG dress.ABS 'The girl will not dirty the dress.' muu-yan-puni-mi. dirty-PROP-CAUS-FUT Marking of grammatical relations ● Three ways to code grammatical relations: ● ● ● word order The stone hits subject the bucket. object case Der Stein trifft NOM->subject den Eimer. ACC->object agreement: Kinyarwanda umugabo y-ataaye umwaana mu maazi C1.man C1-threw child in water 'The man has thrown the child into the water.' Case Armin Buch Overview Case I Case marks argument roles I While a lot of languages don’t mark case at all, most do to some extent I The number of cases is quite arbitrary: WALS 49A I What is not expressed by case is usually marked by adpositions I Therefore, fused adpositions are a source of case inflection Cases in Hungarian (Iggesen 2011) Armin Buch Overview Case Cases in Hungarian (Iggesen 2011) Armin Buch Overview Case I 21 cases (although for large case systems, analyses differ) I Typical: agglutinative morphology Fusional case systems better be small I I I telic: understandability causal: Fusion reduced the effective number of case morphemes Case hierarchy (Blake 1994) Armin Buch Overview Case I If a case on the hierarchy is present in the language, then all the higher ones are present as well (implicational universal) I Nom > Acc/Erg > Gen > Dat > Loc > Abl Icelandic has the dative. Genitive and accusative expected. This is indeed so: I I I I I Nom: hattur (‘hat’) Acc: hatt Gen: hatts Dat: hatti Case Marking • Most common: suffixes or postpositions Turkish ev-de house-LOC 'in the/a house' • Prepositions for a girl, to a girl, with a hammer, etc. Case Marking • Prefixes Krongo (Reh 1985: 151) àpá-ŋ ìʔìŋ á-kùufì M.PERF.hit-TR 3SG.M INSTR-baton ‘He hit him with the baton.’ Gurr-goni (Burarran; Australia; Green 1995: 93) burr-wupunj awurr-bogi-ni INSTR-canoe 3AUG.SUBJ-goPRECONTEMP ‘They all went by canoe.’ Case Marking • Tonal cases (Africa) • Nandi (Creider and Creider 1989: 124) a. kè:r-éy kípe:t la:kwé:t look.at-IMPF Kipet.SUB child.NSUB ‘Kibet is looking at the child.’ b. kè:r-éy kipe:t kípro:no look.at-IMPF Kibet.NSUB Kiprono.SUB ‘Kiprono is looking at Kibet.’ Case Marking • Stem alternations • Nuer (Nilotic, Southern Sudan) Frank 1999