An Analysis of Instructor Extraversion and Student Learning Style
Transcription
An Analysis of Instructor Extraversion and Student Learning Style
Walden University ScholarWorks Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies 2015 An Analysis of Instructor Extraversion and Student Learning Style Celeste Christine Bazier Walden University Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations Part of the Educational Psychology Commons This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact ScholarWorks@waldenu.edu. Walden University College of Social and Behavioral Sciences This is to certify that the doctoral dissertation by Celeste Bazier has been found to be complete and satisfactory in all respects, and that any and all revisions required by the review committee have been made. Review Committee Dr. Peggy Gallaher, Committee Chairperson, Psychology Faculty Dr. Carl Valdez, Committee Member, Psychology Faculty Dr. Virginia Salzer, University Reviewer, Psychology Faculty Chief Academic Officer Eric Riedel, Ph.D. Walden University 2015 Abstract An Analysis of Instructor Extraversion and Student Learning Style by Celeste C. Bazier MA, University of Texas San Antonio, 2002 BA, Purdue University, 1981 Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy General Educational Psychology Walden University February 2015 Abstract An instructor’s personality may influence his or her teaching strategies and instructional style. Correspondingly, a student with a particular learning style may respond more readily to one teacher personality type as opposed to another. This quantitative research, guided by theories of personality and learning, examined the relationship between instructor level of extraversion and student visual, auditory, or kinesthetic learning modalities in a community college setting. A cross-sectional correlation design was implemented. Three hundred and two students from a community college in the southwestern United States were asked to select an instructor (past or present) they thought taught effectively and complete an observer-rated extraversion scale from the Big Five Inventory on the selected instructor. The students also self-reported their learning style using the Barsch Learning Style Inventory along with a demographic questionnaire. Upon establishing the dominant learning style of each student, a one-way ANOVA was conducted to analyze instructor’s extraversion level with student’s dominant style of learning. Pearson correlations were examined to determine relationships between instructor extraversion and auditory, visual, and kinesthetic learning style scores. While findings did not indicate a positive correlation between instructors’ degree of extraversion and students’ visual learning style scores, it did show that visual learners rated effective instructors higher on the trait of extraversion than did auditory or kinesthetic learners. In addition, further analyses indicated that auditory and kinesthetic learning style scores negatively correlated to an instructor’s level of extraversion. This study’s results emphasize the importance of considering both instructors’ personality traits and students’ learning styles in fostering an advantageous learning environment. An Analysis of Instructor Extraversion and Student Learning Style by Celeste C. Bazier MA, University of Texas San Antonio, 2002 BA, Purdue University, 1981 Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy General Educational Psychology Walden University February 2015 Dedication To my daughters, Tiana and Cheyenne, you are my reason for continuing to seek knowledge. You are my strength, my reassurance and my hope for the future. To my husband, Derrick, who has both willingly and unwittingly inspired me in this endeavor. I love you all. To my Mom and Dad, who have passed through this life, it was you who taught me the value of education, through your words and your deeds. I thank you. Acknowledgments To Dr. Gallaher, my advisor, my advocator, my critic, and mentor, I am grateful for your instruction and patience throughout this process. Your guidance has been invaluable to me. Thank you Dr. Valdez, your advice and encouragement has been deeply appreciated and vital to my understanding of this process. My thanks to Dr. Billimek and Dr. Wood who assisted me with the approval process. Also my thanks to a Ms. Linda Bates who was instrumental in encouraging instructors and their students to take part in this research. Table of Contents List of Tables................................................................................. ......................................v Chapter1: Introduction to the Study .....................................................................................1 Preface1 Overview ........................................................................................................................1 Background ....................................................................................................................5 Statement of the Problem .............................................................................................10 Purpose of the Study ............................................................................................. 11 Research Hypotheses ...................................................................................................12 Definitions....................................................................................................................13 Theoretical Constructs .......................................................................................... 13 Definition of Terms............................................................................................... 15 Significance..................................................................................................................15 Nature of the Study ......................................................................................................17 Assumptions and Limitations ......................................................................................17 Summary ......................................................................................................................18 Chapter 2: Literature Review .............................................................................................21 Introduction ..................................................................................................................21 Instructional Models ....................................................................................................24 Jung and Extraversion ........................................................................................... 26 Eysenck and Extraversion ..................................................................................... 30 i The Evolutionary Basis for Extraversion .....................................................................32 The Heritability of Extraversion ..................................................................................35 Myers-Briggs Assessment ...........................................................................................36 Dimensions of Extraversion .........................................................................................37 The Big Five Personality Theory .......................................................................... 37 Eysenck Personality Inventory/Questionnaire ...................................................... 40 Research on Personality and Learning .........................................................................41 Extraversion: A Closer Look .......................................................................................43 Research on Teacher Personality and Student Outcomes ..................................... 44 Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences ......................................................... 47 Learning Styles ............................................................................................................50 VAK model. .......................................................................................................... 51 VARK Model. ....................................................................................................... 54 Genetic Component of Learning Styles .......................................................................55 Learning Style Preferences ..........................................................................................56 Learning Styles Assessments .......................................................................................59 Limitations ...................................................................................................................60 Implications of Past Research on Present Research ....................................................61 Summary ......................................................................................................................62 Chapter 3: Methodology ....................................................................................................64 Introduction ..................................................................................................................64 Purpose of the Study ....................................................................................................64 ii Research Design and Approach ...................................................................................65 Setting and Sample ......................................................................................................66 Participants............................................................................................................ 66 Administering Procedures ..................................................................................... 68 Instrumentation ............................................................................................................69 BFI ................................................................................................................... 69 BLSI ................................................................................................................... 70 Demographics ....................................................................................................... 72 The Research Hypotheses ............................................................................................72 Data Analysis ...............................................................................................................73 Ethical Considerations .................................................................................................75 Summary ......................................................................................................................75 Chapter 4: Results ..............................................................................................................77 Introduction ..................................................................................................................77 Data Collection ............................................................................................................77 Research Questions and Variables Used......................................................................82 Descriptive Statistics....................................................................................................85 Cronbach Alpha Reliability Estimation .......................................................................86 Hypothesis #1: Statistical Results ................................................................................89 Hypothesis# 2 through #4: Statistical Results .............................................................90 Summary ......................................................................................................................90 Chapter 5: Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations ............................................92 iii Introduction ..................................................................................................................92 Interpretation of Findings ............................................................................................93 Research Question 1 ............................................................................................. 93 Research Question 2 ............................................................................................. 95 Research Question 3 ............................................................................................. 96 Research Question 4 ............................................................................................. 97 Methodological, Theoretical and Empirical Implications .........................................100 Implications for Social Change ..................................................................................101 Recommendations for Practice ..................................................................................103 Recommendations for Further Study .........................................................................104 Conclusion .................................................................................................................106 References ..................................................................................................................111 Appendix A: Consent Form .......................................................................................141 Appendix B: Big Five Inventory-Observer rating Scale............................................142 Appendix C: Barsch learning Style Inventory ...........................................................144 Appendix D: Demographic ........................................................................................146 iv List of Tables Table 1. Demographic Variables……………………………………………….……..…80 Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations……………………………………….……….85 Table 3. Internal Consistency Values (Cronbach α)…………………………….……….85 Table 4. Spearman Correlations: Age/Learning Style……………………….……..........87 Table 5. Independent Samples t Test: Gender/ Learning Style……………….................88 Table 6. One-Way Analysis of Variance: Race/Ethnicity/ Learning Style……….......... 88 Table 7. One-Way Analysis of Variance: Hypothesis 1……………………………........89 Table 8. Pearson Correlations: Extraversion/Learning style…………………….…......90 v 1 Chapter 1: Introduction to the Study Preface Acquiring, dispensing, and digesting knowledge has been a lifelong journey of mankind. Great thinkers in past times have pondered the question of how thought processes occur, why people think the way they do, and how people arrive at conclusions and absolutes. Socrates proclaimed, “I cannot teach anybody anything. I can only make them think” (Socrates, n.d.). It is the process of thinking, doubting, accepting truisms, and rejecting ideas that has challenged pedagogical practices throughout history (Marin & Halpern, 2011). According to Trudeau and Barnes (2011), the commonalities and differences among learners and instructors is instrumental in the delivery and absorption of knowledge, the coaxing of creativity, and the development of the human race. Overview This dissertation’s significance is established within the personality trait of extraversion and its effect on different modalities of learning. The assertion is that certain parts of personality, being relatively unchanging after age 30 (Terracciano, McCrae, & Costa, 2010), will produce automatic and natural actions, reactions, and responses that capture the attention of a particular learning style. In this chapter, the purpose of this research is recognized through an understanding of the community college population and its instructors. Detailed descriptions of the problem along with hypotheses are offered in an effort to identify and modify the subtle nuances contributing to the perplexing teacher/learner relationship. There is much controversy concerning the trait of extraversion. Goldberg (1993) and Wiggins (1992) both attributed the characteristic of 2 dominance to be associated with the extraversion trait, while McCrae and Costa (1994) described a medium between dominance and warmth, with the dominance trait being slightly more significant. Extraversion’s identity spans across a variety of descriptions. McCrae and John (1992) proposed six dimensions of extraversion as warmth, gregariousness, confidence, action oriented, excitement seeking, and positive sensations. Wilt and Revelle (2008) suggested adjectives such as boastful, arrogant, garrulous, talker, and chatty. On a continuum, those traits, at their optimal, would represent one end, while the traits of quiet, reserved, shy, and silent would represent the other end (Wilt and Revelle, 2008). A continuing debate within academia is the extent to which students should acclimate to their environment and the extent to which the environment should acclimate to them. Apart from the students’ personality traits, which have shown to be correlated to different learning styles (Komarraju, Karau, Schmeck, & Avdic, 2011), it is the ability of the instructor to penetrate the intellectual psyche that is in question (Bean, 2005). Some have advocated for matching learning styles to teaching methods (Zhang, Sternberg, & Fan, 2013). Charkins, O’Toole, and Wetzel (1985) contended that various methods of teaching have minimum effect on student learning and controlling for the differences in student learning styles is crucial. They emphasized student learning may be aided or hindered by a teacher’s instructional technique and therefore some students’ gains may be counterbalanced by other students’ losses. In contrast, Pashler, McDaniel, Rohrer, and Bjork (2008) recognized several studies that disputed the premise of matching learning styles to teaching methods and 3 concluded that the evidence for this method of instruction was lacking in merit. Instead, emphasis should be placed on examining evidenced-based strategies that improve learning and memory in general. Moreover, the researchers acknowledged the probability that not all learning style modalities have been tested. Controlling for the differences in learning styles is approachable by various methods. I propose one method is by adjusting classes to accommodate particular learning styles and appointing instructors to those classes who are a best fit through their level of extraversion and other variables. Two factors that determine effective instruction are the knowledge of content and the ability to convey this knowledge (Shulman, 1986). According to Terregrossa, Englander, Zhaobo, and Wielkopolski (2012), academic achievement is derived from the teaching effectiveness of the instructor. Their study consisted of first year economic and accounting students with a relatively low number of participants (N = 61). Findings supported the assertion that learning styles may differ according to subject area, all other variables remaining constant. The kinesthetic variable in the economic class was positively correlated while the visual and auditory variables were negatively correlated to academic achievement. In contrast, the auditory component for accounting students had a positive correlation to academic achievement while the tactile/kinesthetic learner was negatively correlated, implying students may be partial to a particular learning style if he or she is inclined to study in a specific subject area. Furthermore, Dunn et al. (2009) contended instruction should match the learning style of the student in order to optimize learning. Terregrossa et al. (2012) identified extraverted instructors as performing better than introverts when teaching takes place in 4 the presence of students, and yet, Baker and Bishel (2006) found extraverts deficient in processing ability, noting that extraverts are less motivated to deliberate over concepts. Instructors who are introverts, preferring abstract ideas (Capretz, 2003), may assume that students need a great amount of autonomy when working. Extraverts tend to enjoy being around other people and delight in lively conversations. According to Terregrossa et al. (2012), extraverted instructors are talkative and may monopolize conversations. Instructors with an ample amount of the extraversion trait are sociable and outgoing (Hills & Argyle, 2001). These are the instructors who encourage conversations in the classroom (Terregrossa et al., 2012). They invite constructive debate and may deviate from typical conversations (Terregrossa et al., 2012). They thrive in the art of persuasion and entertainment (Terregrossa et al., 2012). This tendency may be desirable for an auditory learner who may fixate on the instructor’s every word. Extraverted instructors are also risk takers (Nettle, 2004). It is not inconceivable that they may present unproven theories or erroneous proclamations in order to awaken or challenge an action or reaction from their students. They exercise, play sports, and are generally more active than introverts (Nettle, 2004). According to Capretz, (2003), they are attentive to the external world. This type of behavior may be appealing to the kinesthetic learner who gravitates toward learning activities involving movement and hands-on training (Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1985). The instructor lacking in the extraversion trait is serious and reserved (Hills & Argyle, 2001), preferring solitude, a few close friends, and tranquility. This temperament implicates an intensely thoughtful instructor: expressing opinions sparingly, choosing 5 words carefully, and delivering content purposefully. According to Hills and Argyle, (2001), conversing is not an introvert’s strength; therefore visual learners could be partial to an introverted instructor’s demonstrations and graphic explanations (Dunn et al., 1985). Students may be captivated by slide shows and guest speakers, leaning toward an introverted instructor who would rather sit back and facilitate through other channels. An introverted instructor’s focus is on thoughts and ideas (Capretz, 2003), indicating guidance through self-discovery, peer learning, and activities that provoke thought and consideration. Introverted instructors are comfortable with a silent classroom (Capretz, 2003) and may be inclined to let students guide themselves with unobtrusive assistance. Background Cattell (1943) and Eysenck (1964) first identified the introversion-extraversion traits as being a consistent and dependable aspect of personality. Both researchers developed personality tests that measured levels of extraversion. Cattell established the 16 Personality Factor questionnaire as being reliable and valid utilizing various methods. Cattell’s findings suggested there were high correlations between self-reports and ratings. Fashioned by Eysenck and Eysenck (1964), the Eysenck Personality Inventory’s (EPI) focus was on two components of personality: extraversion/introversion and neuroticism/stability. It comprised 57 yes–no items without replication of test questions. The two dimensions of extraversion and stability were divided into four subsets: (a) stable extraverts were those who were talkative, responsive, and carefree; (b) unstable extraverts described individuals who were high-strung, impulsive, and impatient; (c) stable introverts displayed passive, even-tempered, and dependable characteristics; and 6 (d) unstable introverts could be moody, anxious, and inflexible individuals (Eysenck, 1968). It has been established that both the trait of extraversion (Costa & McCrae, 1994) and learning style (Richardson, 2011) remain constant over long periods of time, indicating an inherent preference for type and degree of human interaction. Terracciano, Costa, and McCrae (2006) substantiated findings that personality traits are primarily consistent after the age of 30 and plateau about age 50. Variables of gender, ethnicity, and education had no influence on personality stability (Terracciano, McCrae, and Costa, 2010). According to the National Center for Education ( as cited in the American Association of Community Colleges, 2013), 45% of community college faculty are over 49 years of age, with 37% between the ages of 40 and 49, indicating an assurance in personality stability of the instructors referred to in this study. Additionally, it has been suggested that instructors teach in the way they themselves learn best (Stitt-Gohdes, 2003). Therefore, those instructors possessing an ample amount of extraversion may prefer lively conversations, different points of view and group work in their classroom (Shah and Meisenberg, 2012) while instructors low in the extraversion trait may assign reading material and give written assignments frequently (Offir, Bezalel, and Barth, 2007). This premise does not mean that teachers should avoid adopting various strategies in accommodating their students, but that modifications can be minimalized if students are predetermined to respond to a certain instructor personality type. Champagne (1991) stated that learning styles could change and adapt over time, and concessions should be 7 made accordingly. However, those concessions might involve a different instructor with an established method of delivering information. These methods could involve PowerPoint presentations, peer presentations, group work, or assigned readings. If students’ learning styles change, then choice of instructor, according to degree of extraversion, may also deviate. Community college students comprise people from many facets of life. Many are parents (married or single) with financial and family responsibilities. Some are first generation college students unfamiliar with collegiate academia and lacking in guidance from parents and relatives who never attended college. Unlike the traditional college student, many of these individuals view themselves as employees who choose to register for college (20%), who select a part-time schedule (44%), and who live with their parents (61%; Mullin, 2011). According to Mullin (2012), the student population in community colleges has become younger, with a growing number registering for courses before graduating from high school. This shift is due to the fact that secondary schools are providing opportunities for college credit. In addition, the female population has increased over the last 30 years in community colleges, standing at 58% in 2008, while male enrollment has remained steady with only a slight increase in recent years (Mullin, 2012). The format in which community college education takes place provides opportunity, accessibility, and affordability. Baum, Little, and Payea (2011) described the conveniences of community colleges by identifying the lower admission 8 requirements, geographical proximity, and flexibility in scheduling compared to 4-year universities. Mullin (2012) stated that 84% of community college students work, with 60% of those students employed more than 20 hours per week, making it more difficult to earn a degree. Community colleges service slightly less than half of all minority college students, with 58% of all African American undergraduates and 66% of all Hispanic undergraduates enrolled in community colleges (Mullin, 2012). As a result, community college students encountered more risk factors than students from other institutions of higher learning. Nevertheless, 71% of the U.S. population was in favor of an individual beginning an educational pursuit at a community college (Associated Press, 2010). There have been few studies conducted on achievement level with part-time community college students (Williams & Kane, 2010). McKenzie and Gow (2004) established that learning strategies predicted grades twice as often in older adult community college students in relation to the customary-age college student. Furthermore, data indicated that 61% of all community college students take at least one remedial course, while 25% take two or more (Goldrick-Rab, 2010), implying the need to meet students at a lower academic level than was originally designed for a course and also indicating a slow progression toward degree or certificate completion. Consequently, diversion in both student population and institutional goals presented a variety of impediments to the community college students’ endeavors. Community college instructors have unique work environments and challenges that influence the way they teach. They cater to an eclectic community of unemployed, underemployed, and displaced workers seeking to make a new start. They provide 9 instruction to the young as well as the old, the middle class and the underprivileged, the single parent, and the recent high school graduate. They may teach students seeking to continue their education at a 4-year university or someone who just wants to brush up on a specific area of interest. Regardless of the student, community college instructors must enter the classroom accustomed to the complexity of the student body and ready to adjust instruction accordingly. Dissimilar to 4-year universities, where the instructor is required to present curriculum and the students are expected to acquire it, community college instructors must take into consideration the complex needs of their students, including their social and psychological backgrounds. However, while enrollment and challenges increase, salaries are shrinking for the community college instructor (Alexander, Karvonen, Ulrich, Davis, & Wade, 2012). Delayed retirements and slow faculty turnover rates may be an explanation for this trend. In addition, higher paying sectors and heavier course loads may promote the lack of qualified instructors in the community college arena. Unlike other higher learning institutions, a community college instructor’s focus is on teaching, as opposed to research and publication demands (Grubb, 1999). Grubb (1999) asserted that many instructors in a community college felt isolated due to the focus on teaching and the absence of academic camaraderie among their colleagues, who at a 4-year university may serve as mentors or give support and advice. According to Goldrick-Rab (2010), community college administrators tended to overlook effective teaching strategies in favor of content knowledge. The instructors might choose to develop teaching methodologies by trial and error or to mimic their own learning 10 experiences by teaching in the way they themselves were taught. Goldrick-Rab (2010) described a typical community college instructor as having a master’s degree in his or her subject area and working for several community colleges as an adjunct professor. The approximate percentage of part-time faculty nationwide has increased from 20% in 1970 to 49% in 2007 (Ehrenberg, 2012). Community college instructors, more so than 4-year college instructors, must attend to the needs of various students by providing an appreciation and excitement for learning that might not be innately ingrained. They must furnish avenues for students to acquire the subject matter, recognize progress, and apply it in real world scenarios. According to Fugate and Amey (2000), all instructors should acquire those competencies, but for community college instructors, it is especially important to have worked in the field in which they were now teaching. Statement of the Problem Many studies have recognized the importance of instructor/student cohesion (Frisby & Martin, 2010; Sierra, 2010). Personality constructs, learning styles, and their relationships have been examined extensively, and yet the investigation into instructor extraversion level and student learning style has no finite conclusions. Community college instructors and students are a unique and definable group in academic society. Community colleges have minimal admission standards that contribute to a low completion rate (Mullin, 2012). In many cases, these students’ investment in education can be ambivalent due to the complications of being minorities, parents, and full- or part- 11 time workers (Mullin, 2012). Consequently, community college instructors are encouraged to expend a considerable amount of energy providing necessary instruction that accommodates students who are not prepared or have little time for the rigors of higher academia. By matching student learning style to instructor degree of extraversion, some of these barriers could be lessened. However, community college instructors have their own set of frustrations. Many community colleges employ adjunct professors, offering low salaries and large class sizes (Modern Language Association Committee on Community Colleges, 2006). As a result, instructors are overwhelmed at the prospect of meeting the needs of all students and may become disillusioned with the students’ inability to grasp concepts and ideas (Modern Language Association Committee on Community Colleges, 2006). Alleviating problematic conditions should consist of an attempt to create teaching environments that are conducive to both instructors and students by attending to inborn predispositions of teaching and learning. Undoubtedly, this affiliation of instructor extraversion and student learning style could not be accomplished in every instance. A limited selection is foreseeable in some cases due to student last minute enrollment, retiring faculty, and area of expertise. However, academic institutions can delegate faculty, who have tenure or are retained from year to year, to classes that are grouped according to the instructor’s propensity to connect with a particular type of learner. Purpose of the Study I analyzed the relationship between the instructor’s level of extraversion and the students’ learning styles of auditory, visual, and kinesthetic modalities. I compared the 12 extraversion trait in community college instructors with their students’ dominant style of learning (auditory, visual, or kinesthetic) to determine any correlational significance. I reviewed mean differences in age, race/ethnicity, and gender for noteworthy preferences for teacher degree of extraversion or student learning style. The intent was to provide academic institutions with methods of grouping students according to the way in which they learn and to provide instructors whose inherent extraversion trait enhances the learning experience, thereby eliminating obstacles to learning. The purpose of this study was to heighten awareness and attend to natural inclinations toward gaining knowledge. Research Hypotheses In this study, I sought to determine if a correlation existed between the processes by which an individual learns (auditory, visual, or kinesthetic) and the degree of extraversion permeated in the instructor. The research questions are as follows: Is there a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s dominant learning style? Is there a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s visual learning style? Is there a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s auditory learning style? Is there a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s kinesthetic learning style? I describe, in Chapter 3, the research hypotheses and the methodology used to ascertain the answers in detail. The variable of teacher extraversion would be measured using the extraversion portion of the Big Five Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991), while the three learning styles of visual, auditory and kinesthetic modalities were measured utilizing the Barsch Learning Style Inventory (Barsch & Creson,1980). The hypotheses are stated as follows: 13 1. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and student’s dominant learning style. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and student’s dominant learning style. 2. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. 3. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. 4. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. Definitions Theoretical Constructs The term learning style is used to describe various constructs of learning or instructional preferences. According to Pritchard (2009), learning style is an individual’s favored approach to thinking and processing information. It entails inherent or learned 14 strategies and habits. It conceptualized individual choice as well as practices and procedures. The many modules of learning styles included, but were not limited to, (a) sensors: those types tended to be detailed-oriented and practical, concentrating on facts and procedures; (b) intuitors: imaginative and meaning- or concept-oriented; (c) thinkers: very logical and rule-oriented individuals; (d) feelers: those who made decisions on a more personal basis, and (e) judgers: people who often followed agendas, seeking closure. Moreover, included in the list of learning styles were reflectors, perceivers, theorists, activists, and pragmatists. In this research, I considered the three learning modalities of auditory (those who are susceptible to the spoken word), visual (those identifying with graphic cues), and kinesthetic/tactile (those individuals who desire motion) learners (Dunn, Griggs, Olson, Beasley, & Gorman, 1995). As detailed in Chapter 2, this research considered personality theories described by Jung (1959) and Eysenck (1964) and the pedagogical practices of instructors possessing or lacking extraverted personality characteristics. Sun (2012) described a mismatch of student learning style and teaching presentation as contributing to problematic conditions of anxiety and negative attitudes among students and instructors alike. Wilt and Revelle (2008) suggested one objective should be an examination into extraversion and student academic functioning. According to Fang (2012), the incompatibility between instruction styles and learning styles is evident and therefore deserves attention. Data will be collected in a community college located in the southwestern part of the United States. These three constructs of auditory, visual and 15 kinesthetic learning styles have met established, reliable, and valid psychometric standards, and they are also easily understood by the general public (Dunn, Beaudry, & Klavas, 2002). Definition of Terms Auditory learning style: Includes learners who have good listening skills. Having a respectable auditory memory, they benefit from audio tapes, interviewing techniques, lectures, and discussions (Dunn & Honigsfeld, 2009). Extraversion: One of the Big Five personality traits. The true ultimate extraverted personality is uninhibited, gregarious, and fun loving. This type of personality loves crowds and group activities and smiles a lot. Extraverts are spontaneous and optimistic and like to entertain. They easily express their emotions and are confident and popular (Dunn & Honigsfeld, 2009). Kinesthetic/tactile learning style: Includes people who learn by doing. They like to manipulate objects and enjoy physical activities. These types of students may find it hard to sit still and need frequent breaks from the classroom (Dunn & Honigsfeld, 2009). Visual learning style: Includes learners who prefer to learn through seeing. They have visual recall and prefer information in the form of graphs, maps, displays, and diagrams (Dunn & Honigsfeld, 2009). Significance Individuals choose what they learn (Fraser & Greenhalgh, 2001). Champagne (1991) asserted that the attention given to information and the value the students placed on that information was essential in the learning process. If students could digest 16 knowledge efficiently and with minimum effort, instructors could move through material more rapidly, therefore covering more material; students would gain more knowledge, skill, and interest; course enrollment would increase; and classes would flow more smoothly than before. In many cases, people attend to that which is understood and absorbed readily, and that which is understood thoroughly can be applied effortlessly and with much success. Accomplishments impact the perception of what the mind, body, and soul can do, affecting the lives of one or of many (Zepke and Leach, 2010). Everyone has a learning style (Dunn et al., 2002). Dependent on the combination of learning modalities—presentation style preferences, instructor characteristics, or classroom environments—an individual will adjust accordingly to the best of his or her ability. It is convenient to have cooperation and cohesion when trying to adjust. Stress levels are reduced, a comfort level is established, and learning becomes potentially promising. For community college students who already have unique obstacles to overcome, the small but significant gesture of deliberately placing them in an environment conducive to their learning style could enhance their educational experience. Erton (2010) contended that teachers should cultivate their teaching styles to purposefully meet the needs of learners. That suggestion, while valid, is incomplete by not proposing an adjustment to student groupings that complements an instructor’s intrinsic personality type. While research on instructor personality type and student achievement has not reached any consensus on an all-inclusive model, it has identified potential projects for 17 examination. Sun (2012) proposed an investigation into the incompatibility of student/teacher relationships; Salehi (2010) suggested that future studies should concentrate on personality factors of students and teachers; while Harris and Sass (2010) asserted, “Teachers significantly influence student achievement, the variation in teacher productivity is still largely unexplained by commonly measured characteristics” (p. 1). I attempted to analyze a small component of teacher/student compatibility with the intention of contributing to the literature of pedagogical practices. Nature of the Study A quantitative approach to this study includes gathering scaled data from students at a community college in the southwestern United States. A cross sectional design of instructor’s degree of extraversion with the variables of an auditory, visual, or kinesthetic learning style will be examined for any significant correlations. According to Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2013), a cross sectional study may be used to define a fraction of the population using multiple regression. This study is not concerned with a cause and effect relationship, nor can it describe variations over time. However, the quantitative approach is appropriate for testing hypotheses in this research involving the correlation of independent variables. In addition, demographic information of race/ethnicity, gender, and age are taken into consideration as covariates. Assumptions and Limitations An assumption in this study consists of the participants’ willingness to take part in completing the inventories, knowing there is no compensation or penalty involved. I expected that participants would answer truthfully and to the best of their capability, 18 realizing that recall may not be easily forthcoming at the moment the inventory is completed, leaving the participants with an “I forgot about that instructor” and a hindsight disposition. Consequently, memory difficulties could result in inaccurate and biased responses. However, it is assumed that the instruments involved in this study are appropriate measures of the constructs to be examined and can be applied to a comparable population. As the researcher, I recognize the omission of factors that could influence findings. The study is administered in the southwest region of the United States and may not be applicable to other regions or nationalities. Additionally, it is not this study’s purpose to defend a three-sizes-fits-all approach to teaching, knowing that different blends of learning styles can complicate meeting the needs of every student. A causal relationship is not assumed between any of the variables under investigation, and therefore, results can only indicate significant associations between student learning styles and instructor level of extraversion. Plausible reasons for connections (if any) will be offered. While the study undertaken will not result in definitive answers, it is designed to encourage speculation about academic practices and its results. Summary The origin of this research began with observations of interactions between various teachers and students in numerous classes over a number of years. The perception of various teaching techniques intertwined with instructor personality sparked an interest in differing responses from students and their implications. Students have various strengths and weaknesses, motivational levels, cultural influences, ambitions, and 19 interests. Likewise, instructors have varying levels of these same categories. Felder and Brent (2005) designated teaching styles as a conduit to the acceptance of education and all that it entailed. They reported, “How much a given student learns in a class is governed in part by that student’s native ability and prior preparation but also by the compatibility of the student’s attributes as a learner and the instructor’s teaching style” (p. 57). While some instructors focus on principles or applications, some opt for understanding over memory (Bain, 2011). Other instructors lecture in lieu of demonstrations. It is feasible that these inclinations are derived from personality constructs that encourage a particular approach to methods of presentation. There are instances of definite mismatches between student and instructor, whether it is from a bad first impression, a disagreement, or an incompatibility of another sort. These personality obstacles are sometimes inevitable and can prevent effective teaching practices from occurring and inhibit learning. It is also possible that the instructor/learner relationship has no bearing on learning at all, with each seeing the other as strictly a learner or a teacher. However, when at all possible, this interaction should be conducive to the giving and receiving of information, the guidance and acceptance of knowledge, and the encouragement and motivation to create and imagine. Instructor/learner communication must rely on both parties’ ability to excuse minor irritations and incongruences, and yet, optimizing the match between personality and learning components proves beneficial to all. In Chapter 2, I addressed the history and discovery of the Big Five personality traits, including the heritability, evolutionary, and biological components of the 20 extraversion trait. Learning style characteristics and their definitions are included in this discussion in conjunction with implications for detecting individual modalities of learning. The development of assessments applied in the research of personality and learning styles is described, along with different models for teaching, learning, and implications for future research. In Chapter 3, I described the process by which this study would be conducted. The purpose of the study, participant characteristics, and instruments applied are highlighted in this section. The research design and approach are explained in detail, with a description of the type of analysis selected. Administering and scoring procedures, as well as ethical considerations, are discussed. Appendices of the inventories are attached. In Chapter4, I will describe data collection procedures, and report significant findings of the statistical results. In Chapter 5 I interpret findings and address conclusions and recommendations. 21 Chapter 2: Literature Review Introduction An educator’s ability to connect with students on various levels is determined by the attributes and detriments of both teacher and student. An ideal teachable moment is when students grasp an idea, concept, or fact that entices them to ponder further, make connections, and originate new ideas. Although the educator’s role in facilitating these student epiphanies develops through training and skill, innate characteristics may enhance or detract from the learning experience. This literature review is focused on the personality trait of extraversion in community college instructors and its relationship to the different learning style categories of visual, auditory, and kinesthetic (used interchangeably with tactile) learners. Community colleges serve about 30% of all students entering higher education, yet only one in four students graduate from a community college (Chen, 2011). Cutting the dropout rate by half would produce income of $30 billion for these individuals and create additional revenue of $5.3 billion for the economy (Chen, 2011). In 2009, approximately 400 community colleges had graduation rates less than 15% (Schneider & Yin, 2012). Undoubtedly, increasing graduation rates among higher education students would benefit an industrial society’s substantial demand for an educated, technical, and skilled workforce. According to Chen (2011) and Schneider and Yin (2012), only a small number of scholars have attempted to address the issue of community college students’ dropout rates. 22 According to Murray, Rushton, and Paunonen (1990), few researchers have examined faculty characteristics that might contribute to engaging community college students. A professor who predominantly lectures throughout a course may find student disinterest in abundance. Students must see the immediate, as well as long term, benefits to completing a higher education. Notwithstanding many other variables (Anders, Frazier, & Shallcross, 2012), classes must be engaging enough to hold the students’ attention, influencing the belief that course enrollment is worthwhile, despite outside obstacles that typically interfere with the progress of community college students. An abundance of literature has suggested that students learn differently (Murray & Moore, 2012; Sadeghi, Kasim, Tan, & Abdullah, 2012). Understanding differences and attending to them could be one of the factors that may increase student retention, achievement, and ultimate success. The purpose of this study was to determine whether there is a relationship between an instructor’s personality traits (specifically, the trait of extraversion) and a student’s learning style. If so, recognition of established personality dispositions among and between instructors may divulge instructing styles that attend to auditory, visual, and kinesthetic learning styles. An examination of the theoretical foundation of personality offered by Carl Jung (1959, 1971a), including the review of Jungian theory of archetypes and the development of the extraversion/introversion component, is included in this chapter, along with the development of the Myers-Briggs Personality Profile Inventory (Myers, McCaulley, & Most, 1985). A description of Eysenck’s (1975) contribution to a conceptual framework is a crucial element in the examination of extraversion. Likewise, an investigation of the 23 five-factor personality theory (McCrae & John, 1992) with an emphasis on the extraversion component is merited. In addition, this literature review will probe into the genetic/biological research of extraversion and its ramifications. Instructional models and teacher presentation styles are duly noted components of this literature review, as well as the need to revisit contemporary methods of instruction. Although research on instructor extraversion and student outcomes is limited (Wilt & Revelle, 2008), the research on teacher personality and student effects is referenced, highlighting an assortment of techniques and models (Jaskyte, Taylor, & Smariga, 2009; Neff, Wang, Abbott, & Walker, 2005; Patterson & Purkey, 1993; Simpson, Gangestad, & Biek, 1993). Turning the focus toward learning styles, the consideration of Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences (Gardner & Hatch, 1987) is warranted, along with the review of biological associations to styles of learning. The learning theories of Dunn and Dunn (Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1985) and the VAK model, which stands for visual, audio, and kinesthetic theories of learning, are also included in this chapter. The VARK model of Fleming and Baume (2006) is an extension of the VAK, adding to its model a reading/writing component. Assessed are deliberations on the individual aspects of each learning style and the implications of corresponding teaching methods, accompanied by the implications of prior research findings involving community college instructors, students, and pedagogical procedures. Moreover, Kolb’s (2005) experiential learning theory is taken into account as an added dimension of the learning schema. Finally, included in this chapter is an inspection of the implications and limitations of 24 past research, present research, and this literature review, with a summation of all entities involved. The search engines used for this literature review consisted of the Walden University Library Portal, Ebsco Host, and Google Scholar. Terms used for those search engines were extraversion, introversion, learning styles, visual, kinesthetic, auditory personality traits, Carl Jung archetypes, Myers-Briggs personality traits, Eysenck, teaching styles, teacher personality, university/community college students, university/community college instructors, history of extraversion, extraversion and instructors/teachers, genetic components of extraversion, genetic components of learning styles, the Big Five personality theory, instructional models/university, instructional models/community college, and student/teacher personality mismatches. Materials were gathered from peer reviewed journal articles, books, trusted websites, and state and federal statistics, with most sources dated within the last 20 years. Databases were Academic Search Complete, Education Research Complete, ERIC, PsycARTICLES, PsycBOOKS, PsycCRITIQUES, PsycEXTRA, PsycINFO, Research Starters, and the Education Teacher Reference Center. Instructional Models A long-standing contention is that many teachers teach the way in which they best learn (Hammerness et al., 2005; Wirz, 2004). Shah and Meisenberg (2012) reported on the reception of various teaching modalities among first-year medical students and instructors. Participants included 30 faculty members and 327 students. They were asked questions about handouts, lectures, media-based learning, textbooks, problem- 25 based learning, team-based learning, and practicums. Students scored highest on mediabased learning instruction, trailed by simulation, handouts, and practicums. Students gave the lowest score to textbooks, problem-based learning, and team-based learning, while instructors gave the highest scores to lectures, followed by practicums and textbooks, and the lowest scores to team-based learning. Lectures, viewed as unpopular by students in a physical classroom environment, were valued more than other methods when provided through computer technology. Recorded lectures afforded students the opportunity to study any time and to repeat important parts, as well as to speed through the mundane parts of a lecture. Overwhelmingly, extraverts (who were not lovers of reading) did report enjoying small group activities and problem-based and team-based learning environments. Polk (2006) proposed that it was the student who could determine clarity in information delivered and was therefore the deciding entity in the degree of successfulness of the provided delivery. “When students cannot learn the way we teach them, we must teach them the way they learn” (Dunn, 1990, p. 15). A pedagogical theory that has been given much attention in recent years is an eclectic method to instruction (Nurmi, Viljaranta, Tolvanen, & Aunola, 2012). Teachers adjust their instruction according to learners’ educational performance. The undeniable awareness of individuality beckons this approach with vigor and persistency, reinforcing the uniqueness of mankind. Mankind is mindful of its differences and attempts to appease them. 26 The numerous models for instruction consist of various and cumulative degrees of cooperative learning, direct instruction, self-directed learning, visual stimuli (Power Points, charts, maps, graphs, and computer-simulated scenarios), guest or expert speakers, hands-on investigations, role playing, writing, audience response systems (clickers that promote interactive engagement), conversing, reading, internships, residencies, and interactive conferencing. Of course, instruction must always maintain a purpose. Goals and objectives containing the three elements of planning the instruction, delivering the instruction, and finally assessing the instruction delivered are essential components. The focus of a portion of this chapter will be on instructional delivery in a conventional classroom setting and how it may differ according to the degree of extraversion. The instructional objective is for students to memorize, analyze, problem solve, think critically, reason, apply, and interpret. The instructor, using skill, expertise, and personality characteristics, must deliver instruction in a meaningful way. Jung and Extraversion Jung (1959, 1971a) developed the concepts of introversion and extraversion as elements of deep-rooted subconscious patterns of reactions to the world. Extraverts were seen as those who react to outward forces and rely on others as an energy source, while introverts choose solitude, seclusion, and reflection to regain their energy. The extravert’s attention is geared toward objects. “If a man thinks, feels, acts and actually lives in a way that is directly correlated with the objective conditions and their demands, he is extraverted” (Jung, 1959, p. 333). The conscious mind of the extravert was prone to an 27 immediate environment, responding both to people and items. The extravert’s value for an object was forever increasing, relying on positive connections to fuel satisfaction with life (Jung, 1959, 1971a). According to Jung (1959), the introvert had an aloof attitude toward the object or person and a need for space and privacy. Although the introvert was aware of external forces, the choice of responding to subjective determinants was evident. Orientation arose from perception and cognition, not necessarily concrete reality. Jung (1959, 1971b) considered personality types as dimensions of the trait of extraversion. Those dimensions were thinking, feeling, sensing, and intuiting functions (Jung, 1959). Jung (1959) further stated that a degree of extraversion (or lack thereof) dictated the results of a combination of those four functions. Thinking was the act of perceiving cause and effect relationships and the way (distant in regard to introversion or forthright in regard to extraversion) in which questioning was pursued. The feeling element attended to the characteristics of warmth and intimacy and how the degree of extraversion dictated those outward tendencies (Jung, 1959). The sensing function was the extent a person relied on present and immediate physical or biological details (Jung, 1959). Digesting circumstances as they seemed to be, no more, no less, implied an extraverted personality type. The intuitiveness aspect described features of leaning toward hunches and impatience with concrete details, which might indicate a lack of extraversion tendencies. Jung (1959) perceived thinking and feeling factors as rational types, while sensing and intuitive factors were seen as irrational. A discussion of the pairing of each type with the extraverted or introverted personality trait follows. 28 According to Jung (1959), normalcy for the extravert depended on the ability to acclimate to the present, while dispatching the unconscious (interchangeably used with the subjective mind) into a submissive ineffective state. Even though the extravert’s thinking was based on concrete objects and concepts, idealistic thinking could also arise in the extravert if extracted from external sources. Thinking was positive and productive, leading to facts and truths based on empirical evidence. For the introverted thinker, intensity was preferred over extensity (Jung, 1959). Jung stated the complexity of thought might oftentimes produce contradictory and inconsistent patterns in introverted thinkers, allowing for the entanglement of conceptions. Ideas were formulated inwardly as opposed to outwardly, and displays of indifference, if not intolerable attitudes, were geared toward people and objects. The introvert’s thoughts might appear arrogant and egotistical to some due to a stubborn headstrong demeanor that insisted what was inwardly clear and coherent was accordingly comprehensible to everyone else. The feeling component of an extravert was always in accord with society’s objective standards. A sentiment might be acknowledged by the extravert just because it was politically correct to do so. Jung (1959) described how a famous painting would be labeled as beautiful to the extravert, because it was created by a famous artist or as not to offend the host exhibiting the picture. The extravert’s aim was to correspond to objects that enhance social stature or credibility among others. The attention sought from the object became what was most valued. Contrastingly, the introvert’s inclination was to devalue the object in preference for subjective thought. Their aim was to scrutinize the object in conjunction with internal truths, interpreting essential elements that might be 29 negative judgments. The feeling patterns of the introvert were not dependent on approval by or persuasion of others, but rather introverted feeling rested with the perception of oneself. Jung portrayed most introverted feeling types as female. He stated they were quiet, difficult to understand, and possessed a quality of sadness. True motives for their feelings were usually hidden, especially when the object was overwhelming. Jung (1959) contended that the extraversion sensation personality typology experienced true enjoyment. Tangible involvements with concrete objects were at the crux of that personality type. Sensing actual life circumstances to its fullest, the extravert sensation type loved reality with little room for reflection. Living in the moment was of great importance, therefore that personality type was usually jovial and excellent company at social events. Sensing life deeply, the sensing extravert desired strong ambiences whether pleasurable or not. The introverted attitude toward sensations was one of subjective dominance. It was a self-interpretation of an object, which in reality might be of little use to the outside world but had meaning and significance for the individual. According to Jung (1959), intuition was an unconscious process that was difficult to grasp. It was an expectancy or prediction of the foreseeable future dependent on concrete objects for the extravert and inner stirrings for the introvert. Jung proposed that the characteristic of intuition attempted to capture a wide range of possibilities, revealing itself through tangible factors in the case of extraverts or physical symptoms and inert yearnings in the case of introverts. According to Jung, the extraverted intuitive type would gravitate toward novelty experiences. Unchanging circumstances were irritants and the urgency of change apparent. Jung suggested many people who fell into that 30 category chose professions of entrepreneurs, politicians, and stockbrokers; they had the ability to spot potential in people and to champion rights for minority groups. Introverted intuitive types were found in artists and the like. They had the propensity to let the mind wander into whimsical or grotesque images. The mind, being limitless, speculated upon the unimaginable. Jung (1959) viewed the unconscious as a compensation device that provided an equalizer to the conscious mind, but was a recessive factor with little surfacing to the conscious. However, an exaggerated version of the extraverted psychological type was dangerous to the subjective mind in that it suppressed natural tendencies, intentions, desires, and needs—elements with a history of acknowledgement. Jung stated the less those tendencies were recognized by the extravert, the more primitive and infantile they became, until they were reduced to primordial instincts. Those instincts could only be eliminated through a slow transformation of genetic makeup. At that point, the unconscious went beyond childish selfishness to a vicious and brutal place where extraversion was at its peak and the subjective mind was manifested through a nervous breakdown. Eysenck and Extraversion Jung (1959) did not see extraversion as dimensions on a continuum, but rather he perceived extraverts and introverts as separate types of people. It was not until Eysenck (1964) examined Jung’s proposals that extraversion was seen in dimensions of stronger to weaker degrees of functioning. In the 1940s and 1950s, it was Eysenck who performed studies on the importance of extraversion as a personality trait. Pioneering the 31 investigation of the core features of extraversion, Eysenck (1975) described individuals with a high degree of extraversion as impulsive and social. In addition, they were assertive enthusiasts who gravitated toward change, warmth, and gregariousness. Eysenck (1975) described those low on the trait of extraversion (introverts) as having reflective behavior: reserved, quiet, and shy. They were those silent types who seemed to be taking in conversations rather than contributing to them. Eysenck hypothesized that extraverted people had a strong sense of inhibition. The extravert’s brain, because it was accustomed to and gravitated toward outward stimuli, could absorb devastating circumstances with less shock, sheltering itself from the full memory of a catastrophe and enabling the brain psychologically to recover rapidly. On the other hand, an introvert’s brain, possessing weak inhibition and unaccustomed to embracing outward stimuli, might be unable to shield itself psychologically from a horrific event. The brain’s protection mechanism did not come to the rescue fast enough, exposing introverts to posttraumatic symptoms. Eysenck (1964) proposed that extraverts committed most violent criminal acts. People with that personality trait were able mentally to dismiss a violent act and emotionally to recover quickly to repeat more violent acts. Contrastingly, extraverts required less positive stimuli to produce an effect (Wilt & Revelle, 2008). Wundt and Judd (as cited in Matthews, Warm, Reinerman, Langheim, & Saxby, 2010) maintained that extraverts should perform better on tasks requiring speed and accuracy, because arousal level would peak, which was what was sought. That also 32 implied that the use of stimulants, social interaction, and sexual activity was higher and more prevalent among extraverts. Gray’s (1970) development of an alternative theory to extraversion, the reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST), involved animal data and was much harder to generalize than Eysenck’s. However, his theory did make a forthright prediction that extraverts were more conducive to rewarding stimuli and therefore produced a more positive effect in learning endeavors when offered rewards. The Evolutionary Basis for Extraversion Psychological mechanisms involve evolutionary processes, and extraversion is no different. Buss (1995) suggested that the evolution of extraversion arose from communal tasks demanded from the social environment. Those tasks could be summarized through behavioral approach and behavioral avoidance structures. Wilt and Revelle (2008) postulated that it was the behavioral approach system that was associated with the extraversion continuum. However, Eysenck (1975) warned that although there was solid evidence to suggest the relevance of genetic factors, relegating gene dominance in preference for introversion or extraversion was a mistake. There were probably instances where the survival of society was dependent on spontaneous and social attributes in certain situations and reclusive or preplanned qualities in others. Nettle (2004) stated that although in the evolution of extraversion, an extraverted individual might have more mates and procreation opportunities, their risky behavior could also cause them to die 33 earlier, thus discontinuing the passing on of their gene pool. The Biological Basis of Extraversion The suggestion that extraversion has biological factors implies that these factors should appear in an individual’s early development. Rothbart, Ahadi, and Evans (2000) found that to be the case. Studying temperament, a positive affect that coincided with the degree of extraversion trait had been found in infants as young as 3 months old. Temperament is related to variations in self-control and reactivity, and it was discovered that those characteristics overlapped with both Eysenck’s (1975) and Gray’s (1970) findings on impulsivity and reinforcement sensitivity. In addition, support for a genetic component of extraversion can be found in research conducted on animals. Luo, Kranzler, Zuo, Wang, and Gelernter (2007) determined genes identified with extraversion as ADH4. Through MRI testing, it was discovered that extraversion was linked to the amount of gray matter in the left amygdala and that lower amounts of gray matter could predict depression. Similarly, extraversion was connected to the thickness of the orbitofrontal lope, and research findings had concluded that low thickness in the prefrontal cortex implied impulsiveness and uninhibited behavior (Omura, Constable, & Canli, 2005). Many had postulated that the trait of extraversion had several determinants and that independent variables might skew results. In addition, Eaves and Eysenck (1975) asserted that the two major components of extraversion were impulsivity and sociability, and that those characteristics could be linked to environmental and genetic factors. It had been asserted that differences in extraversion behavior could be attributed to numerous 34 variations in cortical arousal, and those individuals with fewer extraversion tendencies were aroused to a greater extent than those with more extraversion tendencies from the same stimuli (Schaefer, Heinze, & Rotte, 2012). That research implied that introverts had a lower threshold for excitement than extraverts, and that the extraverted individuals might be inclined to seek out stimulating events. Other research had linked the personality dimension of extraversion to regions in the prefrontal brain (DeYoung et al., 2010), as well as the amygdala and the ventral striatum. Results from a study conducted by Beukeboom, Tanis, and Vermeulen (2012) suggested extraverts were linguistically abstract, whereas introverts were more concrete in their language delivery, and therefore, had a higher degree of credibility when speaking. Verification of that notion could be observed in social situations where an extraverted personality type might dominate conversations, but because introverts spoke so sparingly, when they did speak, everybody listened. Its most common label was extraversion, and its proximate basis was thought to include deviation in dopaminefacilitated reward circuits in the brain (Depue & Collins, 1999). Introverts innately had a tendency to compensate for high arousal stimuli by withdrawing. Contrastingly, extraverts welcomed arousing stimuli, because it took more to excite them. Those who felt the need to be aroused might possess a higher degree of neuroticism than those who were stable (introverts) and had lower thresholds in the intuitive brain (Depue & Collins, 1999). 35 The Heritability of Extraversion According to Nettle (2006), it was uncommon for a single gene to vary in its influence over an inheritable trait; however, those traits affected by several genes were more prone to producing mutations. In fact, the likelihood of mutations was in direct proportion to the number of genes involved. The genes, ADH4 (Luo, Kranzler, Zuo, Wang, & Gelernter, 2007), were identified as polymorphic contributors to the trait of extraversion. Several ADH4 markers contributed to variation in degrees of extraversion, and therefore, various phenotypes and genotypes of extraversion were inevitable. Positive and negative emotions, social ability, and reactivity are dimensions included in all personality constructs. Considering Eysenck’s (1975) proposal that extraversion was made up of the two components of sociability and impulsivity, there was evidence to suggest that sociability, and therefore an element of extraversion, was inherited (Plomin, 1974). Conversely, the evidence on the inheritability of impulsivity was mixed (Buss & Plomin, 1975); nevertheless those two factors were highly correlated to each other (Plomin, 1976). Nettle (2006) contended the trait of extraversion was strongly connected to the number of sexual mates, which could increase physical prowess and sustainability. Those who scored high on the extraversion trait were also prone to terminating relationships in preference for other sexual partners, resulting in homes that were step-parented, a known risk for children (Nettle, 2006). Extraverts were more physically active and gravitated toward explorations of their environment, cultivating social behavior and securing higher quality mates both physically and intellectually. For 36 that reason, more communal support was given to extraverted individuals than to introverted individuals, providing an assortment of engaging activities to satisfy their sensation-seeking tendencies. Extraverts were risk takers and had a higher rate of hospitalization due to accidents or illnesses than those who were not (Nettle, 2006). In addition, extraverts had higher probabilities of migration, criminal and antisocial behavior, and arrests records (Nettle, 2006). Myers-Briggs Assessment The Myers-Briggs Personality Profile scores personality based on four attributes: a preference for outgoingness (extraversion) or seclusion (introversion); whether a situation is assessed based primarily on external (sensing) or internal (intuitive) cues; whether an individual is more prone to make decisions using logic (thinking) or emotions (feeling); and whether an individual is susceptible to judging or perceiving (Sears & Kennedy, 1997). The test was originally designed during World War II to help women self-assess before entering the workforce for the first time while their husbands or male relatives were away fighting for their country. It is now used in colleges and career centers, as well as by psychologists to help their patients. Perception implicates all the ways of becoming cognizant of the environment: people, circumstances, or ideas. Judgment encompasses the various ways of coming to deductions about what has been perceived. If individuals differ systematically in what 37 they observe and in how they reach decisions, then it is realistic for them to differ in their securities, interests, responses, principles, incentives, and abilities. In developing the instrument of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; MyersBriggs, 1962), the aim of Briggs Myers, and her mother, Briggs, was to enlighten and make accessible types of that personality theory to average individuals and groups (Myers et al., 1985). They addressed the two related aims in the development and application of the MBTI instrument, which were the documentation of rudimentary preferences of each of the four dichotomies specified or imbedded in Jung’s (1959) theory, and the identification and explanation of the 16 distinctive personality types that resulted from the interactions among the preferences. The MBTI categorizes people into four personality types: extraversion or introversion, sense or intuition, thinking or feeling, and judging or perceiving (MyersBriggs, 1962). It is a multiple-choice test that produces a personality profile with a combination of four paired facets of personality. Its aim is to simplify Jung’s theory of psychological archetypes into meaningful applicable principles of personality types. As previously stated, this chapter will concentrate on the extraversion/introversion components. Dimensions of Extraversion The Big Five Personality Theory A starting point into the history of the Big Five personality traits is at societal language. Klages (1926), Baumgartner (1933), and Allport and Odbert (1936) all relied on the natural language in defining personality characteristics. Allport and Odbert (1936) 38 set out to identify all the words in the English dictionary connected to human behavior. Their list consisted of 18,000 terms, represented an extensive yet all-inclusive set of words used by the English-speaking population, and reflected societal perception of importance in describing human actions, thought, and temperament. The combining and organizing of those words resulted in four major categories, comprised of (a) personality traits, such as sociable, fearful, or aggressive; (b) temporary states, such as rejoicing, elated, or afraid; (c) personal conduct, such as excellent, irritating, or average; and (d) physical characteristics, talents, and abilities—miscellaneous terms not appropriate for the other categories. Cattell (1943) condensed Allport and Odbert’s (1936) list to 4,500 trait terms when he began his work on personality groupings. Through factor analyses, Cattell documented 12 personality factors common among English-speaking Homo sapiens. Those factors eventually became part of the 16 Personality Factor questionnaire (Cattell, 1943). Cattell contended the reliability and validity across various methods, such as selfreports and evaluations by others, had high correlational findings. Fiske (1949) replicated but was unable to substantiate Cattell’s findings, detecting only five factors with recurring tendencies in three separate factor analysis trials. It was Tupes and Christal (1961) who repeatedly found five recurrent factors when rating personality. Their study consisted of eight separate sample ratings on 35 personality traits first introduced by Cattell (1943). Participants in each sample varied in education, length of acquaintanceship (3 days to over 1 year), and experience in personality rating (novice to years of experience). Other variables included situational 39 factors of fraternity, dormitory, or military training environments. Sample groups were gender homogeneous with seven out of the eight samples exclusively male. Tupes and Christal labeled five recurrent personality traits emerging from this study as (a) surgency, (b) agreeableness, (c) reliability, (d) emotional steadiness, and (e) culture. Surgency, a synonym to extraversion, was described as talkativeness, assertiveness, sociability, cheerfulness, and adventurousness. The development of the Big Five theory emerged almost simultaneously by several researchers, with McCrae and Costa (1985) and Goldberg (1990) among the most prominent. Those researchers approached personality through different methods, but all arrived at the same conclusion that regardless of culture or language, there were five personality traits common to all humans. Those five dimensions were a result of analyzing thousands of questionnaires through factor analysis. It is important to acknowledge that examiners of those five personality traits did not presume which traits would emerge, however repeated findings dictated the consensus (Digman, 1990). Today, the Big Five-personality theory is widely accepted by most researchers of personality. These five basic dimensions of personality—neuroticism, openness, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and extraversion—have been examined, verified, and dissected by numerous researchers for their validity and reliability and have fared very well. The five personality traits have been scrutinized by various instruments across cultural, ethnic, and racial lines. Although there is considerable debate on the adjectives and components that define these traits, most five-factor theorists agree that some description of these five traits is a necessity when describing personality. 40 Eysenck Personality Inventory/Questionnaire Developed by Eysenck and Eysenck (1964), the Eysenck Personality Inventory’s (EPI) focus is on two components of personality: extraversion/introversion and neuroticism/stability. It comprised 57 yes–no items without replication of test questions. The two dimensions of extraversion and stability are divided into four subsets: (a) stable extraverts are those who are talkative, responsive, and carefree; (b) unstable extraverts describe individuals who are high-strung, impulsive, and impatient; (c) stable introverts display passive, even-tempered, and dependable; and (d) unstable introverts can be moody, anxious, and inflexible individuals (Eysenck, 1968). Like the EPI, the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck, 1975) was generated as a counterpart to Eysenck’s personality theory (Eysenck, 1964). It has been used extensively in personality, social, and cognitive assessments and has plainly established usefulness in the area of personality assessments. Adding a third personality dimension to the EPI, the EPQ examined psychoticism, along with degree of extraversion and neuroticism, presenting 100 items in a self-report questionnaire. Rocklin and Revelle (1981) asserted the two assessments (EPI, EPQ) are correlated through the sociability factor rather than the component of impulsivity. In an effort to refine the extraversion scale impulsivity questions were removed from the EPI in lieu of sociability queries that determined degree of empathy and hostility. However, critics have demonstrated a lower consistency in the psychoticism scale than in the extraversion/introversion and neuroticism/stability scales 41 (Rocklin & Revelle, 1981), implying the validity and reliability of this subset may be in question. Research on Personality and Learning Kneipp, Kelly, Biscoe, and Richard (2010) studied student perception of quality of instruction and the Big Five personality types in instructors. Their findings suggested a significant correlation between the characteristic of agreeableness and student perceived instructional quality, and while it was predicted that the trait of extraversion would result in high teacher evaluations, that was not the case. Pishghadam and Sahebjam (2012) examined the relationship between the Big Five and teacher burnout. Results suggested that emotional exhaustion were consistent with the trait of extraversion and neuroticism. The personal qualities of any instructor would manifest themselves through teaching practices, regardless of common materials and syllabi (Carr, 2007). Blank (1970) performed a study determining a positive correlation between instructor involvement and student satisfaction. Therefore, the suggestion that instructor presentation was dependent on personality was apparent; the precise connection has yet to be realized. Patrick (2011) proposed an association between the Big Five personality traits, student grades, course assessment, and student evaluations of teachers at the university level. Findings supported the personality traits of extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness as positive factors, determining a favorable evaluation, while neuroticism did not. A relationship between students’ expected grades and course satisfaction was described by Patrick, but no significant relationship to satisfaction with 42 the instructor was noted. Discrepancies between course and teacher evaluations were attributed to instructor personality more so than grades or perceived learning, which indicated that a student might have liked the course, but not the instructor. In adding to the literature of personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement, Komarraju, Karau, Schmeck, and Avdic (2011) enlisted 308 college students to complete the Inventory of Learning Processes and the five-factor inventory model. Also collected were demographic information and GPA. Participants represented all undergraduate classes enrolled in a variety of subject areas. Correlation and regression analysis indicated various relationships. One relationship significant to this literature review is that the trait of extraversion, along with openness, related positively to elaborative processing, which consisted of connecting and applying new ideas to present knowledge and personal experiences. Understanding the social aspirations of extraverts might suggest that instructors with a greater extraversion dimension had the ability to convey real-life scenarios and practical applications to their students. The aforementioned study reiterated the contention that both learning style and personality traits influenced academic achievements (Komarraju et al., 2011). This study’s examination of instructor degree of extraversion and student learning style attempts to rationalize a diligence toward grouping students according to learning style by identifying levels of instructor extraversion that are advantageous for specific learners. When the instructor's teaching style and personality match the student's learning style, the chance for the student to learn more quickly and easily can lead to an increased rate of degree completion. 43 Extraversion: A Closer Look According to McMillan, Groth, Lane-Getaz, Dittmann, and Bailey-Seiler (2012), extraverts responded readily to positive stimuli. They announced findings on a study conducted with 212 undergraduates (69 men and 143 women) with a mean age of 19.52 years. Participants were asked to complete the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992), then choose among various hypothetical scenarios of positive, negative, and neutral circumstances; self-reporting data on personal characteristics; word preferences describing the intensity or extremity of a word (stone verses pebble, for example); and positive, neutral, or negative photographs. Results indicated that extraversion was robustly significant to positive stimuli, while the components of warmth and positive emotions also had a high correlation. Those results implied that instructors’ positive responses to a class could be affected by the perception of a class being collectively positive and pleasing to the instructor. The argument was then made that a class was pleasing when attentive and engaged by learning in the style that was most comfortable. Likewise, extraversion related positively to neutral stimuli, although not as much as positive stimuli, and because there was no correlation between extraversion and negative stimuli, the study did not confirm a relationship between extraversion and responding to intense negative stimuli. However, Friedman, Förster, and Denzler (2007) suggested that although positive affective states led to freedom and creativity, people with negative affective states might perform better on a task considered important or serious. That 44 implied that instructors who were serious in their teaching endeavors had introverted personality types. Extraverts have a positive well-being and satisfaction with life. The research indicates a substantial correlation between extraversion and happiness (Wilt & Revelle, 2008). Falkenstern and Kwon (2012) examined that assertion, claiming the quality of resilience was a significant variable mediating the outcome of happiness. Ninety-seven participants were administered the extraversion subset scale of the Big Five, an egoresiliency scale, and a satisfaction with life scale. The participants were 76% female and 81% Caucasian. Findings, at a significant level, indicated that degree of extraversion forecast the amount of resiliency and resiliency predicted satisfaction with life. Ripski, LoCasale-Crouch, and Decker (2011) examined trait stability in preservice teachers and found a significant reduction in the trait of extraversion during training. Explanations for that result might be found in the increased confidence students experienced as training continued, maturation developed, and the need for social interaction declined as more responsibilities were acquired. Although the implications that seasoned teachers experienced a reduction in extraversion was plausible, the stability of the Big Five personality constructs had proven to be consistent in individuals over time (McCrae & John, 1992). Research on Teacher Personality and Student Outcomes Patterson and Purkey (1993) defined good teaching as not a matter of teaching method, but rather the personality of the teacher. They posited that teachers must be genuine, open, and honest, and that such behavior—even when teachers become 45 impatient, angry, or irritated—actually reduced educational difficulties. When teachers were genuine, students knew who the teachers were and where they stood in areas that affected them academically. Neff, Wang, Abbott, and Walker (2005) investigated the influence that gestures and language variation had on students’ perception of instructor extraversion, determining if a system of preplanned facial expressions, gestures, tone, and mood of voice could predict the perception of the trait of extraversion. Findings agree with the prediction of a statistically significant link between those paradigms of extraversion, gestures, and language. Simpson, Gangestad, and Biek (1993) investigated the relationship between personality and nonverbal behavior. They videotaped 109 female and 101 male undergraduates answering questions by an interviewer of the opposite sex. Considered attractive by societal norms, those interviewers recorded each participant’s nonverbal behavior on 11 behavioral actions and 34 global attributes. Students considered to be extraverts laughed, smiled, and looked downward less often than those considered nonextraverts; extraverts exhibited more flirtatious glances than those deemed as introverted. In addition, extraverts rated high in self- monitoring, pretentious, socially engaging, and dominating behavior than their less extraverted counterparts. Students and instructors in yet another study were asked to rate the qualities of an innovative teacher (Jaskyte et al., 2009). Fifty-two students along with 48 instructors participated from 20 different academic departments. Students rated flexibility, attending to different learning styles, humor, enthusiasm, and providing a relaxed atmosphere as preferable qualities in an instructor. Students and instructors had similar results in the 46 areas of open-mindedness, contemporary examples, local and global events, differential testing, and novelty presentation methods. Where those two groups differed was in teaching methods and materials used. Student ratings were much lower than instructor ratings in those areas, indicating that other factors such as teacher personality, classroom culture, and student/instructor communication were valued in the learning environment. Jaskyte, Taylor, and Smariga (2009) proposed that teacher personality played an integral part in student ratings of innovational techniques. Regardless of materials used or presentation methods, students yearned for an instructor who would entertain their questions, address their concerns, and challenge their intellect. Technology ranked low in both groups, implying a personal interaction was desired and the way in which material was presented was more important to the student than the material itself. Some students prefer strict teachers, while others like teachers who are flexible, socially inclined to communicate, and humorous. Characteristics like respect, honesty, sincerity, kindness, and confidence are also valued. Akbar (2009) conducted a survey asking 346 students to describe the characteristics of an effective teacher. The aim was to determine if students’ selection of a good teacher was dependent upon their own personality trait of extraversion. Differences among introverts and extraverts were noted by the following: Extraverted students preferred teachers of witty nature to other teachers, while introverted students preferred teachers who accepted the ideas of students and of other teachers. Moreover, students who were introverted preferred strict teachers over other teachers. Ironically, introverted students preferred teachers who had flexible behaviors over other teachers (Akbar). 47 Maximizing an instructor’s ability to convey information is essential. A factor in this maximization is the students’ desire for honesty and genuineness (Jaskyte et al., 2009). In addition, students’ preferences for instructors varied from humor and affability to acceptance and tolerance of others (Akbar, 2009). Determining if student preferences are encouraged by linking learning modalities to instructor level of extraversion is one consideration of this dissertation. Other considerations for future research entail the investigation of learning modality groups in contrast to controlled groups; an examination of the other Big Five personality characteristics of openness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and agreeableness in regard to learning style modalities; and the exploration of learning style modalities in connection with the degree of self-extraversion. It should be the practice of every learning institution to place students in the best possible environment, with the most favorable instructor, and the appropriate materials/instruments for their style of learning. Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences (TMI) (Gardner, 1985) is intriguing, and yet, many have criticized his findings. Gardner presented elements of intelligence as linguistic, logical/mathematical, spatial, musical, body kinesthetic, interpersonal, intrapersonal, and naturalistic skills, and admitted that TMI might have to be expanded to incorporate new intelligences. Many scholars had separated intelligence into different categories, contending that an individual might be competent in one area, but not in others. Moreover, society demonstrated that people who were considered very intelligent 48 in one area were inept in another. That notion could be recognized in individuals with specific talents, from analytical thinking to the mastery of social interactions. Still, others were exceptional in creative endeavors or observational skills (Sattler, 2008). According to Gardner (1985), an individual with pronounced interpersonal intelligence understood the objectives, needs, desires, and motivations of others. They had the ability to connect with others by recognizing the similarities and differences among people. “Interpersonal individuals are usually extraverts” (Furnham & Mansi, 2011, p. 3). Extraverts were empathizers, which promoted caring and supportive interaction. They were also sensitive to facial expressions, voice intonations, and physical gestures, maintaining close relationships with friends and family members. On the other hand, intrapersonal people were primarily introverts ((Furnham & Mansi, 2011). Gardner (2000) described intrapersonal intelligence as understanding one’s own beliefs, attitudes, moods, and ideas. Those individuals had a real understanding of their own strengths and weaknesses. Unlike the extraverts who had a sense of other’s emotions and motivations, the introverts had a sense of themselves; their likes, dislikes, thoughts, feelings, and goals were all at the forefront of their being. They were self-motivated and could often concentrate on a concept, subject, or topic by themselves. Introverts shied away from groups and learned best through observing and listening. One criticism of Gardner’s TMI was the inability to test some intelligence in the same way as others (Gardner & Moran, 2006). Sensory-perceptual aptitudes, such as verbal, number, word articulacy, perceptual promptness, space/visualization, and mechanical intelligence could be influenced by standard cognitive psychological and 49 social psychological research mediation prototypes. They could be easily observed experimentally. Contrastingly, the testing for other non-concrete categories like interpersonal and intrapersonal abilities were viewed as sketchy at best (Hunt, 2001). Although Gardner had never prescribed an exact educational formula derived from TMI, his suggestion that individualized instruction and approaches to topics from different perspectives had generated the interest of many in how individuals learn and what makes us intelligent. His theory was useful to broad and applied audiences, and it was positively welcomed in school settings, where an abundance of kids already fail. Critics argued that Gardner’s contentions were too broad. Gardner countered back, stating that behaviors studied were simply too narrowly defined to qualify as human intelligence (Gardner, 1985). Barrouillet, Bernardin, Portrat, Vergauwe, and Camos’s (2007) speculation of numerous possible cognitive processes lent itself to the validity that human beings were indeed complex and unique. Instruction should be individualized whenever possible, however the feasibility of individual instruction was improbable. Gardner’s (2000) theory was vital in promoting a continuous search for various intelligences in all animal species. Modern psychological assessment of learning had embraced science with flexibility and acceptance, knowing that intelligence could not be fully or even meaningfully understood outside its cultural setting. Research on intelligence within a distinct culture might be unsuccessful at its attempt justify the array of awareness and skills that might constitute intelligence for that particular culture. 50 Learning Styles When examiners first investigated with learning-style prescriptions for teaching college students, significantly greater attainment resulted (Hein & Budny, 1999; Kamuche, 2005). Those improvements were evidenced in several subjects—marketing, mathematics, anatomy, physiology—and across subjects (Taylor & Elias, 2012). According to Dunn (1990), there were dominant preferred learning models in every individual. Those learning models were on a bi-polar continuum, where each modality (auditory, visual, and kinesthetic) was evident to some degree and would mesh with the other models in producing a learning style that was unique to the individual. Brooks and Khandker (2012) reiterated that assertion by pointing out that everyone had a dominant learning strength. Although other modes of learning were evident, research confirmed that accommodating that strength produced a student who could grasp information quicker, retain it longer, and apply it more effectively than learning styles less dominant when placed in an instructional environment conducive to his/her learning style. Honigsfeld and Dunn (2006) put several justifications forth in identifying learning style among groups. Those were as follows: (a) Adult males and females differed significantly in learning style paradigms. On a global scale, females were found to be more auditory, persistent, driven, and accountable than their male counterparts. Women also required more diverseness in instruction. (b) Students with higher grade-point averages had considerably dissimilar styles from those with grade-point averages below 51 normal. While high achievers were described as gravitating toward a traditional classroom environment—a quiet place, bright lights, minimum distractions, studying in solitude, or with a facilitator present—low achievers favored learning environments with conversation in the background or music softly playing. Their preference for studying also included a couch, easy chair, frequent breaks, and the presence of food or drinks. (c) Learning styles also varied by age, with older adults who returned to school after having life experiences demonstrating a significant learning style preference from younger adults. (d) No two adult students would approach the same task with identical strategies. Some might read and highlight everything considered important, while others spent their time searching the Internet, and still others would listen to taped lectures while performing other tasks. Combinations of learning processes might be intermittently interwoven in a student’s repertoire of appealing ways to learn. People consistently utilized the strategies that worked best for them. When various methods were not provided, many students became overwhelmed, discouraged, disheartened, and unable to cope with higher education challenges. They settled for less than their capabilities dictated, becoming less confident, less motivated, and more compliant with the status quo (Honigsfeld and Dunn, 2006) VAK model. Dunn, Dunn, and Price (1985) described learning style in the way a person was able to digest new and difficult knowledge. The VAK model originated from 30 years of work, involving almost a century of research on how people learn differently, by 52 professors Rita and Kenneth Dunn in the 1970s. The controlling premise was that there were distinct differences among learners with implications that academic achievement was attained through relatively fixed categories and that certain factors added to or detracted from learning successes. Those categories were as follows: 1. Environmental—sound verses quiet, temperature of the classroom setting, and formal verses informal seating arrangements. 2. Emotional—being motivationally consistent verses inconsistency, and conformity verses nonconformity. 3. Sociologically—in cooperative pair or group activities, as compared to set routines. 4. Physiologically—the student connected by auditory, visual, or by kinesthetic means. There was a time of the day when a student would display low or high energy levels, or could sit for long periods of time verses the need to move and be active. According to the VAK theory, one or two of our sensory receivers were prevailing, suggesting that learners had a natural partiality for the way they learn. As such, there should be a compulsion for matching teaching styles with learning styles to enhance the learning progression, enabling teachers to address the requirements of learners in a more efficient manner. Yet, one style of learning might not always be the same for some tasks. Moreover, as the concept evolved, research came to support the application of an assortment of modalities in the course of instruction, instead of a single learning style. A learner might prefer one style of learning for one task, and a mixture of styles for a different task. 53 The core value of the VAK learning theory was to enable people to think in terms of various representational arrangements. By combining different instruction techniques and rationalizing about the diverse ways in which we process material, instructors could start to develop a multisensory learning environment. Rather than adapting instructional techniques to each individual, a combination of coinciding stimuli would enable a learning group to improve their storage, retrieval, and clarification of information. Centered on three main sensory receivers (audio, visual, and kinesthetic), that model was considered imperative in determining an individual’s learning style. Visual. The visual learner preferred to observe, rather than talk. Naturally quiet by nature, reading might be appealing to them. They memorized by transforming mental images into visual ones. Charts, graphs, films, and demonstrations were all conducive to that type of learner. Consequently they could easily lose focus by visual distractions. They noticed detail, enjoyed the use of color, might have good handwriting skills, and likewise, draw or doodle. They remembered first impressions and faces, and might sometimes stray away from verbal conversations. Auditory. The auditory learner had an outgoing personality. They loved conversing and social situations. They might even talk aloud to themselves in clarifying events or circumstances. They were easily distracted by noise. They might like to be read to and might pay particular attention to an individual’s choice of words in conversation. Their memory usually followed sequential steps and might find singing or humming appealing while completing tasks. Written instructions could be problematic. 54 Rather than faces, the auditory learner was more prone to remembering names and could many times recognize people by voice. Being aware of rhythm, they enjoyed music and the sounds of others’ voices and could oftentimes mimic another. The auditory learner might absorb more upon verbal repetition of an explanation than the visual or kinesthetic learner. Kinesthetic. Kinesthetic learners could be fidgety and liked to be in motion most of the time. They might tap a pencil or their foot when contemplating an idea. They were naturally outgoing and expressed themselves in physical ways. Reading was not a favorite past time, and therefore, they might find spelling difficult. They thrived in remembering what they had done, rather than what they had seen, heard, or said. The kinesthetic learner solved problems by working them through physically, if possible. They learned and memorized by doing, having good reflexes and timing. They enjoyed activities and the physical handling of objects. The kinesthetic learner liked to touch and use gestures while talking and was usually open for new ideas. VARK Model. The VARK (visual, auditory, reading/writing, and kinesthetic) model was similar to the VAK model, but included the learning modality of reading and writing (Fleming & Baume, 2006). The development of that model rested on the premise that some learners had a preference for writing material, while others preferred symbolic representations of material such as maps, graphs, and charts. Online questionnaires of 13 proposed questions were administered to over 180,000 people during a 6-month period in 2006. Participants were encouraged to reflect on their VARK results with their own 55 perceptions of how they learn. Outcomes reflected a 33% match to student perception of their learning modalities. Although that percentage was not high, positive feedback was substantial. One criticism of the VARK model was that it was hard to statistically validate, which meant it could be problematic in research. Rather, its value lay in helping individuals to think about personal degrees of learning modalities and give options for learning not previously considered. Fleming and Baume conceded to that discrepancy, but pointed out there were currently no instruments that gave reliable readings because the constructs of learning styles were numerous and complex. Genetic Component of Learning Styles Binoy (2012) conducted a study involving monozygotic twins using the VARK model. Usual factors that determine learning preferences—heredity, gender, heredity, age, and culture—were constant because of the nature of the study, however a significant difference of learning styles was found to hold true regardless of genetic sameness. Although many more studies of this kind are called for, it seems that environment overrides genetics in learner preference. Dunn, Beaudry, and Klavas (1989) emphasized a biological component in connection with learning styles and contended those who ignored that presupposition were naïve. Offered was the suggestion that left brain individuals (those who were analytical and used deductive reasoning) learned in small steps, and that right brain individuals (those who perceived concepts on a global scale and used inductive reasoning) learned in broader phases before considering details. After an examination of studies, they concluded that right brainers in grades five through twelve preferred 56 working with music, in groups, informal seating arrangements, and with kinesthetic rather than auditory or visual resources. Left brainers preferred a formal, structured, classroom, and visual, rather than kinesthetic, methods of instruction. In considering adult community college students, those with more right hemisphere brain activities preferred sound, intake, mobility, and kinesthetic procedures more often than their left brain counterparts, who gravitated toward formality and bright lights. Consequently, differences of learning style preferences were identified between age groups and those with similar interests and talents. Interestingly, when inquiring into culturally diverse groups and family dynamics, researchers found more variances and dissimilarities than resemblances. Learning Style Preferences Kazu (2009) wanted to determine the effect of learning styles on education and the process of teaching. He examined various learning styles and their merit in the educational system. The consensus was that learning styles were a particular way in which an individual acquired, processed, and maintained data, mostly stable and unchangeable but could be developed by experiences over time. Kazu reiterated the importance of learning styles by proclaiming its positive effect on the learner: instilling confidence and promoting autonomous learning throughout the educational environment. Conclusions to that study revealed that individuals learned differently, and those differences should be followed in the teaching environment. Eysenck (1998) reviewed literature suggesting that children with a large dose of the trait of extraversion would benefit from discovery learning, where the learning 57 environment was flexible and uninhibited. Those who were more introverted would benefit from reception learning, where the learning environment was controlled and free from distractions. According to a study by Fang (2012), the mismatch between teaching styles and learning styles was evident and therefore deserved attention. The problem arose when that mismatch created an obstruction to learning. The aim was to reduce possible incompatibilities between teacher and student by making the teacher cognizant in designing various teaching strategies to accommodate all learners. Participants included 157 second-year college students in the humanities and science fields, with a mean age of 19.5 years (81 females and 76 males), and four English professors ranging in age from 26 to 42 years old. The survey consisted of 65 statements, measured on a Likert scale (1 = almost never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = almost always). Thirteen dimensions of learning styles were measured. A survey was also administered to evaluate the instructor’s teaching style preferences. Results indicated the common learning style preferences among students were visual, independent, global, random, and reflective, in order of highest percentages, while English instructors were prone to the visual, independent, group-oriented, sequential, and reflective styles of teaching. An evaluation of 36 studies between 1980 and 1990 conducted on the Dunn and Dunn model by Dunn, Griggs, Olson, Beasley and Gorman (1995) evaluated learning styles preferences of 3,181 participants. Results indicated that accommodating students’ learning style would yield a 75% increase in the standard deviation from those who had not been provided with a learning style accommodation. Implications of that meta- 58 analysis divulged that when students’ learning style was considered and matched with educational interventions equipped to satisfy those learning styles, academic achievement was bolstered. In a study conducted by Terregrossa et al. (2012), an examination of the influence of learning styles on introductory accounting and economics courses was conducted using the Building Excellence (BE) learning survey. That survey, similar to the Dunn and Dunn model (Dunn et al., 1990), was comprised of 6 categories and 26 learning style elements. Results proposed evidence that college students’ academic achievement was significantly correlated to their dominant learning style. Furthermore, that study substantiated the notion that learning styles appropriate for a particular discipline (accounting students) might not satisfy the majority of another discipline (economic students). In other words, the type of subject taught might influence the type of leaners taking the course, and inevitably, the way in which the course should be taught. Regardless, it was obvious that a unilateral approach to teaching was inefficient and insufficient; therefore various pedagogical techniques were crucial in reaching all students. The suggestion of corresponding student learning style to instructor teaching style was prevalent in that study’s findings throughout the academic community for any academic discipline. Kamuche (2005) investigated the link between learning styles in statistics students and the teaching style of instructors, developing a hypothesis that students whose learning style was similar to an instructor’s teaching style would perform better on assessments than those who were dissimilar. They would earn higher grades and would 59 have a better understanding of the subject matter, and test performance would correlate with the grade earned. The study lasted for 3 years, from 2002 to 2005, and involved a range total from 105 students the first year to 1,265 in the third year. Results demonstrated a high correlation between student learning styles and instructor teaching styles, along with grade correlation. Simply put, the evidence clearly showed that students performed better when their learning style was paired with a teacher of similar teaching style. Conducting this research on instructor degree of extraversion and student learning style may imply that an instructor’s level of extraversion produces a particular teaching style, resulting in the suggestion that grouping students according to learning style improves academic achievement. Learning Styles Assessments The Kolb Learning Style Inventory (Kolb, 2005) was based on the experiential learning theory. Its premise relied on learning as a process of thinking, feeling, behaving, and perceiving in relation to everyday experiences. It was a holistic process that involved creating knowledge through the assimilation of new experiences with prior ones. It integrated concrete practices with abstract conceptualizations and reflective thought with active experimentation in producing a fully engaged learner. Individuals might gravitate toward one component of experiential learning, as opposed to another, or might use all four components in different orders. An example would be learning how to ride a bike. Some might want to read about how bike riding was performed, the placement of the hands, and the speed at which one should go, and the tilt of the handlebars, while others might want to watch a demonstration of someone riding a bike. While still others might 60 just want to get on and go, knowing that every fall or mishap would teach them what to do or not to do. The experiential learning theory took into consideration the VAK and VARK models, but went further in suggesting that the capabilities of a learner in the auditory, visual, and kinesthetic fields were promoted and influenced by direct personal experience with the task at hand. Kolb identified four types of learners within that realm of experiential theory: assimilators (who adhered to logical principles), convergers (who thrived on practical applications and concepts), accommodators (who gravitated toward hands-on experiences), and divergers (who liked to absorb a wide range of information). Experience had always been a practical and efficient way of learning. Internships, apprenticeships, and residencies all pointed to the concept of replicating best practices. Kolb (2005) contended the usefulness of experience in the reinforcement of ideas was better served in a curriculum than creating novel ones. The complex notion of Kolb’s learning theory is far beyond the scope of this dissertation. However, its connection to the rudimentary learning components of visual, auditory, and kinesthetic learning styles should not be ignored. Limitations Brooks and Khandker (2012) asserted that a comprehensive approach to pedagogical processes was merited due to the complex structure of learners and the overabundance of teaching models. Their research involved matching the right and the left brain hemisphere thinking preferences of the student to the right and the left brain hemisphere thinking preferences of the instructor. Results indicated no significant 61 preference according to final grades received. “…[T]he case for matching students to instructors in order to improve student learning is tentative at best, and at worst it is a large drain on resources” (p. 14). The debate about the existence of learning styles, what types, and how many continues forward. I propose that the auditory, kinesthetic, and visual styles of learning are a given, and that these styles of learning can be enhanced through the right instructor on an extraversion continuum. The variables in testing the hypothesis of instructor extraversion and student learning styles are countless. Some teaching methods may be better in some subject areas than in others. A student’s unique mixture of learning style combinations varies widely. Professors, regardless of personality, can be more skilled in pedagogical methods than other instructors. Student interests must also be taken into account. In this literature review, I do not attempt to dispute or concur with past and present literature on teacher personality and student learning styles, but my intention is to increase and supplement prevailing opinions and assumptions. Implications of Past Research on Present Research Past research entailed primary personality characteristics that all individuals possess to one degree. Jung’s (1959) work with archetypes and Eysenck’s (1975) insight into the trait of extraversion grounded other studies in personality. The question of ingratiating character into the academic realm was a complicated task. Future research should focus on understanding teacher characteristics and what their relationship is to academic success in the learning environment. Eysenck (1998) proposed an examination of student personality and its role in the teacher/learner process. Polk (2006) contended 62 investigating into various teachers’ personality traits might provide insight into the ultimate possibilities of achievements for learners. Sun (2012) described a mismatch of student learning style and teaching presentation as contributing to problematic conditions of anxiety and negative attitudes among students and instructors alike. Wilt and Revelle (2008) suggested one objective should be an examination into extraversion and student academic functioning. Summary The connection between instructor extraversion and learning styles can be envisioned by various means. Some conjectures are that extraverted teachers are prone to verbal communication, and therefore, are better for auditory learners; introverted instructors connect with visual learners because they encourage them to do cooperative learning, leaving the instructors as observers more than participators, or that introverted teachers are better with kinesthetic and visual learners because of the lack of social communication desired. Jung (1959) developed the notion of the self and its entities, while Eysenck (1975) fine-tuned Jung’s work into a more concrete definition. Biological factors concerning the trait of extraversion were corroborated (DeYoung et al., 2010; Luo et al., 2007; Omura et al., 2005), implying the trait was inherent, an integral part of personality and usually established and ingrained in a persona from birth. Garner’s theory of multiple intelligences (Gardner & Moran, 2006) dictated consideration of similar established human traits, where skill and proficiency were a mixture of specific intelligences, a combination of innate and learned factors producing a distinctive individual. The 63 matchup of those traits during the teaching and learning process was a feasible solution to increased productivity and less confusion. The VAK (Dunn et al., 1985) and the VARK (Fleming & Baume, 2006) learning style models simplified and categorized learning methodologies into practical and all-inclusive dimensions of cognitive absorptions. Although everyone had degrees of each learning style, a dominant preference should appear in most. This particular study contributes to the literature by conceptualizing alternatives to teacher/student dynamics. In Chapter 3, the research methodology is presented in its entirety. The purpose of this study is considered, instruments are examined for validity and reliability, participants are described and procedures are reviewed. Data analysis techniques are presented and ethical considerations are discussed. 64 Chapter 3: Methodology Introduction Chapter 3 is an overview of the practices and procedures utilized to conduct this research. The design, as well as a justification, for choosing this particular type of strategy, are detailed throughout this section. Participants’ demographics and certain characteristics are discussed, along with setting and context of the research environment. A detailed description of the administration process are covered, as well as the ethical and moral considerations given to this project. The procedures and instruments used in measuring the results are examined, accompanied by a rationale for the chosen statistical method. Validity and reliability of these instruments are inspected in an attempt to substantiate findings. Purpose of the Study The examination of teacher instructional methods and student learning has been approached from unlimited perspectives. Investigations by researchers involved the skill and training of instructors, in addition to the constructs of personality, setting, demographics, age, culture, and race of both instructor and student. The purpose of this study was to observe correlations between the instructor’s degree of extraversion and student learning style. The purpose of this study is to contribute to pedagogical practices by presenting an approach to embracing innate traits that produce a particular type of teaching strategy geared toward a certain type of learner. Zhang (2006) proposed the 65 appropriate match between teacher and student could yield tremendous results for both parties, and although this suggestion has provided a variety of literature for many years, unchartered territory abounds. Research Design and Approach This research study may indicate a relationship between degree of instructor extraversion and the dominant learning styles of visual, auditory, or kinesthetic learners. A cross-sectional correlation design was implemented to determine the strength of that relationship. A cause and effect relationship is not assumed, but results of data may imply an association between instructor extraversion and student learning style. The population in question in this research was community college instructors and students. My intention was to examine associations between instructors’ degree of extraversion and student learning styles with the aim of generalizing to the larger population. By using a cross-sectional approach, I attempted to reflect a true representation of community college students and instructors in the learning and teaching environment. Findings may be applicable to other community college environments. Although a qualitative analysis could result in detailed information on particular cases, it would be subjective in nature and not generalizable to the community college population, and therefore, qualitative analysis was not appropriate for this study. Empirical data are necessary in order to show significance, validity, and reliability. Data gathered during the school term on demographics, learning style preferences, observer-rated instructor extraversion, and instructor preference were analyzed. These multiple factors were examined for any significant correlations. 66 Setting and Sample Participants Students volunteering for this study were recruited from a community college district in a southwestern region of the United States. This community college district has an enrollment of approximately 10,000 students seeking associate degrees and certificates in various areas of the Arts and Sciences. Permission was sought from the chair of the Psychology Department at a community college and the dean of Arts and Sciences at yet another community college. Both colleges are affiliated with the community college district in a southwest region of the United States. The students participating in this study were enrolled in one of these two colleges as part- or full-time students. Participants were selected on the following criteria: (a) availability, (b) proximity, (c) willingness to participate, (d) 18 years or older, (e) part- or full-time student, and (f) ability to read and comprehend the English language. The community college district designated for this study comprises 61.3% Hispanic, 28.9% Caucasian, 6.4% African American, and 3.4% other. Specifically, the two community colleges utilized in this study have similar racial/ethnic demographics, with mean percentages of 54.5% Hispanic, 31% Caucasian, 9.7% African American, and 4.9% other (Texas Higher Education Coordinating Board, 2012). A sample size of approximately 300 students was taken from this population. 67 In order to examine these hypotheses, I distributed observer extraversion inventories, Barsch Learning Style Inventories (Barsch, 1996), and a demographic questionnaire to community college participants in the southwestern United States. I presented the project in the dialogue that follows: I am conducting a study in pursuit of completing my Ph.D. in educational psychology. Your help would be greatly appreciated by completing a short (approximately 15 minutes in length) inventory. My aim is to examine relationships between a student’s learning style of auditory, visual, or kinesthetic modalities and the level of instructor personality. The question is whether an instructor’s level of extraversion is conducive to or detracts from a student’s natural inclination of acquiring information. In other words, is it possible that the instructor from whom you learned best presented material in an instinctive way that naturally fit with the way you learn? Could this presentation style be influenced by the instructor’s personality? I would like to enlist the help of all students 18 years of age (or older) by completing 3 surveys. One survey asks for demographic information, another asks for information about a past teacher and the third survey asks about your learning styles. This could be an instructor who you like, but not necessarily. Rather, think of an instructor who you consider a good teacher, someone you thought explained well or made you think. Next, there is a learning inventory determining your dominant learning style. In an attempt to find the answer to this question, I am asking that you 68 complete the questionnaire in its entirety so as to eliminate “missing data” barriers and to obtain valid and reliable data. Please DO NOT put your name on the packet I will be handing out. All individual information is anonymous and only group results will be published. However, if you would like to know your individual score, you may do so by contacting me at my e-mail address (keeping in mind that your e-mail address may reveal your identity).You must have your inventory number (shown on the front of the packet) to request your score. Participation is strictly voluntary, and there is no benefits or consequences attached to your participation. Nevertheless, if you would take a few minutes at this time to complete the inventory packet, I would be forever appreciative. Thank all of you in advance for your contribution to what I hope will shed some insight into student/instructor compatibility. Administering Procedures Permission was sought to distribute inventories at the beginning of a class session. Given instructor approval, a brief introduction of the purpose and significance of this research was addressed along with an open invitation for anyone over 18 to participate. A packet containing informed consent (see Appendix A), the Barsch Learning Style Inventory (Barsch, 1996), demographic information, and a short observer rating scale of an instructor’s degree of extraversion was distributed. This instructor could have been from the students’ past or present, but one who the students felt had taught exceptionally well. An e-mail address was provided to answer any questions posed by participants. With this approach, a sufficient amount of data collection was obtainable in 69 a quick and efficient fashion. Those willing to partake did so strictly by choice. If an individual chose to complete the survey, a stamped self-addressed envelope was provided for mailing. Participants are identified by number and not by name on the inventories and may be informed of the results on their learning style, if requested. The number notated on the students’ inventory packet would have to be identified to the researcher before any information could be released. Therefore the participants were asked to write down the inventory number if they would like to know their learning style score at a later date. Instrumentation BFI The Big Five Inventory (BFI) was introduced as an alternative to multi-item personality instruments in the late 1980s (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991). Consisting of 44 items, the BFI measures personality traits of openness, extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Although these personality constructs are broad measures of personality, they have proven to be universally consistent (Heine & Buchtel, 2009). For this study, only items defining the extraversion trait was employed, which are eight adjectives or adjective phrases that can be answered on an observer rating scale (see Appendix B). The domain scales of the BFI have demonstrated consistent reliability, high convergence with other personality scales, and a high self-peer correlation (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). According to Rammstedt and John (2007), the extraversion trait is a Big Five trait that is effortlessly identifiable in others even when encounters are brief. The BFI assessment has proven to be psychometrically sound by having internal consistency, reliability, and validity (Young & Schinka, 2001). Rammstedt and John 70 (2007) affirmed that the reliability and validity of the BFI-44, and the BFI-10 scales are comparable to the NEO-PI-R, where convergent and discriminant validity correlations were considerable. Instructor level of extraversion was ascertained through students’ rating on the BFI inventory, where students rated their chosen instructor. The BFI extraversion items are 1, 6R, 11, 16, 21R, 26, 31R, and 36. Points were credited according to the scale of strongly disagree—1 point, disagree—2 points, neither agree or disagree—3 points, agree—4 points, and strongly agree—5 points. Items 6, 21, and 31 were negatively scored items, and points were deducted from the number 6. An example would be if Item 21 were scored as strongly agree—5 points, then these 5 points would be deducted from the number 6, resulting in a score of 1 for that particular item. Scores from the 8 questions on instructor extraversion, reported by the students, were entered into SPSS with syntax instructions to obtain a total score for extraversion. Permission was granted by the Berkeley Personality Lab to utilize its version of the BFI in conducting this research. The Berkeley Personality Lab is an institution whose purpose is to examine personality constructs, individual differences, and self-perception as it relates to societal and environmental factors (John et al., 1991). The BFI is one measurement among different scales that the Berkeley Personality Lab provides to researchers free of charge and with minimum restrictions. BLSI The Barsch Learning Style Inventory (BLSI) has an item list of 24 questions designed to identify visual, auditory, or tactile modalities in the secondary or college 71 level learner (Barsch & Creson, 1980). The BLSI is an informal, self-report instrument that detects relative strengths and weaknesses along different sensory channels (Appendix C). Barsch and Creson credited the BFI with identifying learning modalities in specially challenged students. A scale of often, sometimes, and seldom was presented as options to the learner in avoiding either/or alternatives. The 24 items were scored according to the type of item and the degree of response, and then computed to reveal a visual, auditory, or tactile/kinesthetic strength. The BLSI is designed for ages 14 to adult and usually takes between 5 to 10 minutes to complete. Based on the popularity of the BLSI, the research contained in the Handbook of Mental Measures (Barsch & Creson, 1980), and the recommendation of the International Personality Item Pool website (n.d.), the Barsch Learning Style Inventory (Barsch, 1996) was chosen to assist in this research. The BLSI identified pre-existing learning style variables of community college student participants as auditory, visual, or kinesthetic/tactile. Scoring of the BLSI consisted of attributing point values to the 24 questions pre-identified as having either auditory, visual, or kinesthetic significance. Questions 2, 3, 7, 10, 14, 16, 20, and 22 were scored as visual learning preferences; Questions 1, 5, 8, 11, 13, 18, 21, and 24 as auditory learning preferences; and Questions 4, 6, 9, 12, 15, 17, 19, and 23 as kinesthetic learning preferences. Points were credited according to participant answers of often—5 points, sometimes—3 points, and seldom—1 point. Points were calculated under each learning style category, with the higher score representing the dominant learning style. Although a minimal amount of data on the reliability and validity of the BSLI is available, it is extensively used by universities and colleges on a global scale 72 (Heaton‐Shrestha, Gipps, Edirisingha, & Linsey, 2007; Sizemore & Schultz, 2005). Kratzig and Arbuthnott (2006) reported ample reliability of the BSLI, with Cronbach’s alpha 0.54 for visual and 0.56 for auditory. Demographics A demographic survey taken from the U. S. Census Bureau (2010) was administered to students with 3 basic questions of age, race/ethnicity, and gender (Appendix D). Five age categories were given values of 1 through 5 as follows: 18–20 group 1, 21–25 group 2, 26–35 group 3, 35–54 group 4, over 55 group 5. With the exception of group 5, there is a 7-year span in each age category. Age groups were sparse in numbers as age increased due to a relatively smaller number of older adults attending community colleges than younger adults. According to the American Association of Community Colleges (2013), the average age of a community college student is 29 and approximately 82% of the faculty is over the age of 40. Terracciano, Costa, and McCrae (2006), described personality traits as relatively stable at about age 30 and plateauing around age 50. Instructors designated by students as skilled in their teaching abilities should have relatively stable personality traits, as well as some student participants. Race/ethnicity was divided into 5 categories of Caucasian, African American, Hispanic/Latino, Asian, and Other. The Research Hypotheses Is there a relationship between instructor level of extraversion and a student’s capacity to learn with a visual, auditory, or kinesthetic learning style? The hypothesis asserted is that student preference for an instructor is influenced by the student’s learning 73 style and the teacher’s level of extraversion, therefore the following hypotheses are proposed: 1. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and student’s dominant learning style. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and student’s dominant learning style. 2. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. 3. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. 4. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. Data Analysis As stated in Chapter 1, the research hypotheses for this study are described in detail to determine if there exists a correlation between the processes in which an individual learns (auditory, visual, or kinesthetic) and the degree of extraversion in the 74 instructor. A one-way ANOVA was performed on the extraversion scores and the scores for visual, auditory, and kinesthetic learning styles. Mean values of teacher level of extraversion were correlated to dominant learning styles. A Pearson correlation was performed on each learning style category (auditory, visual, and kinesthetic) and instructor extraversion scores to determine any significant relationships among those variables. The mean and standard deviation of instructor extraversion and each of the three learning styles (auditory, visual, and kinesthetic) were examined for any significance. Data were keyed manually into the SPSS program under the variables of gender (male = 1, female = 2), and race (Caucasian = 1, African-American = 2, Hispanic = 3, Asian = 4, and other = 5). Correlations were examined with regard to age, race/ethnicity, gender, and learning styles. A sample size of 302 students was calculated using Cohen’s D, with anywhere from 49 to 161 subjects in each learning style category, an alpha of .05, a medium effect size of .30, and a power level of .80. Cohen (1988) described medium effect size as one “large enough to be visible to the naked eye” (p. 26). In addition, Chaun (2006) described an alpha =.05 as standard practice in academic research. In considering external validity, it is recognized that the sample population was recruited from the southwestern region of the United States and may not be an exact representation of community college students in other regions of the country. Likewise, this study attended to the dynamics of Western culture and may not be universally applicable. In addition, internal validity threats may arise from participants’ inaccurate responses due to poor recall of past instructors and their teaching methodologies. 75 However, given the instruments chosen for this research, statistical validity was not compromised. Ethical Considerations Consent was obtained from all subjects before administering inventories. Ethical considerations were conveyed by the researcher and addressed in the consent form (Appendix A). Participants were informed of the nature of this study prior to seeking their consent. At no time did I attempt to conceal information or deceive participants. Participation was strictly voluntary, and subjects’ consent was sought without repercussions or incentives. Participants were also informed that they may discontinue participation at any time without consequences. Paper copies of raw individual data will be kept confidential and will be warehoused with and privy solely to me. Each survey was assigned a number whereby the participant may refer to this number when inquiring about results. Information will not be released to third parties without participant consent, although group data results may be made public. Participants were 18 years or older. Neither I nor this study are affiliated with the educational entity that participated in this study. Subjects were not manipulated in any way or form, eliminating many ethical concerns or interferences. Summary Chapter 3 outlined the process of examining the research question. Participants, variables, instrumentation, recruitment process and data analysis were addressed in detail. A correlational design approach was applied to the variables of extraversion, learning styles and demographics. Data was collected and keyed into SPSS. Chapter 4 is comprised of 76 the data collection process, statistics, and the findings of this research. Verification and analysis of the data will be addressed and correlations identified as appropriate. Chapter 5 is a discussion of findings and conclusions with recommendations for further research. 77 Chapter 4: Results Introduction This study’s objective was to examine correlational relationships between an instructor’s level of extraversion and a student’s dominant style of learning. The instructor, chosen by the student, is one considered to be effective in his or her teaching techniques. Specifically, in this study I investigated whether or not an instructor’s extraversion level is more advantageous in teaching an individual with a dominant auditory, visual or kinesthetic learning modality. The purpose of this chapter is to present the results of this study by discussing the data collection process, data organization and results. Analyses consist of conducting descriptive statistics, Spearman correlations, independent sample t tests, a one way ANOVA and Pearson correlations. Data Collection Data collection began upon receiving approval from the community college’s IRB. At that point, several faculty members were contacted via e-mail asking permission to present this study to their classes. Responses came randomly over a 3-month period giving options of times and dates that would be convenient for the instructor. With confirmed schedules, I proceeded to acquaint these classes with my research at the beginning of each class period, explaining the purpose, and procedure of data collection. The presentation took approximately 15 minutes depending on the number of questions posed by the students. Potential participants expressing interest in the survey were given 78 a consent form, detailing participation requirements; an objective extraversion survey, where they rated an instructor thought of as skillful; a Barsch Learning Style Inventory, where they answered questions about their learning preferences; and a demographic questionnaire with the understanding that participation was strictly voluntarily and there would be no incentives or negative consequences for their involvement. A self – addressed stamped envelope was also included so that the completed inventories could be mailed to a specified post office box. A total of 600 inventories were given out to students with a return rate of 56%, totaling 327 questionnaires over the span of approximately 4 months from 18 classes. Subject areas for these classes consisted of Psychology, Sociology, and Education. Although permission was eventually granted by a second community college’s IRB to collect data, the timeliness of the research was considered and an ample amount of inventories had already been received from the first community college I approached, therefore inventories were never distributed to the second college. Data were manually keyed into the SPSS system for analysis. The sample data was reviewed for completeness with the initial number of study participants totaling 327students.Out of the 327 inventories, 23 had missing demographic information bringing the number of inventories down to 304 from the initial number of 327. Two individuals (Code ID #112 and Code ID #217) noted that they were transgendered. These individuals were deleted from the dataset so that the variable Gender could be constructed as a dichotomous indicator. This elimination reduced the sample size of the dataset from 304 respondents to 302 respondents. 79 Two respondents had missing data. Respondent Number 6 had two questions with missing responses and Respondent Number 14 had one response that was missing. In order to ensure that sample size remain above the critical threshold of 300, it was determined that mean values would be calculated minus these missing values for these two inventories. SPSS calculated the scale score (as an average item response) without the missing item(s). Using this procedure allowed the dataset to remain at a sample size of 302 respondents. As can be seen in Table 1, the majority of respondents (71.2%) are between the ages of 18 and 25. Less than 1% of respondents are over the age of 50. Table 2 also reveals that the sample is roughly split between men (42.1%) and women (57.9%). Six out of every 10 respondents (60.3%) are Hispanic, whereas one out of every four respondents (23.8%) are White. Blacks comprise 13.6% of the sample, and the remaining 2.3% of the sample is Asian. 80 Table 1 Demographic Variables Used in the Statistical Analyses Frequency Distribution of Age Gender and Race Variable Frequency % 18-25 215 71.2 26-33 41 13.6 34-41 36 11.9 42-49 8 2.6 Over 50 2 0.7 Male 127 42.1 Female 175 57.9 White/Caucasian 72 23.8 Black/African American 41 13.6 Hispanic/Latino(a) 182 60.3 7 2.3 Age of respondent Gender of respondent Race of respondent Asian American Demographic statistics for San Antonio Community College sample reflect the general population in the city of San Antonio. According to the United States Census Bureau (2010) the Hispanic population in San Antonio represents a majority of residents at 63.2% while those reporting as White with no Hispanic lineage is at 26.6%. Blacks 81 represent 6.9% of the population, which is considerably less than found in this study while Asians are at 2.4%. Similarly, the racial/ethnic percentage in this research described participants as 60.3 % Hispanic, 23.8 % White, 13.6 % Black and 2.3% Asian. The United States Census reports approximately 51.2% of San Antonio residents as female, while 57.9% are female in the study. However, the variable of age was dissimilar in this study to city and state statistics due to the fact that younger people are more apt to attend community colleges than older adults. At San Antonio Community College, where this study was conducted 71.2% of respondents were between the ages of 18 and 25, 13.6% between the ages of 26 and 33, 11.9 % between the ages of 42 and 49 while only 2% of participants were over 50. According to the American Association of Community Colleges, (2014) the age distribution of this sample does reflect community colleges throughout the United States. It has been reported that the mean age of a community college student in the United States is 24, while 57% of the community college population are between the ages of 2239 and only 14% are over the age of 40. In addition, gender for the community college student nationwide is at 57% female, which is the same gender percentage as in this study. It is concluded that demographic results are in agreement with local, state and federal statistics for this region of the country. Therefore the sample in this study is a true representation of the San Antonio population regarding race/ethnicity and a true representation of community colleges regarding age and gender. 82 Research Questions and Variables Used The study seeks to determine if a relationship exists between the processes in which an individual learns (auditory, visual, or kinesthetic) and the degree of extraversion permeated in the instructor. In order to answer this question, the following hypotheses were created: 1. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s dominant learning style. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s dominant learning style. 2. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores. 3. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores. 4. H0: There is no relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. H1: There is a relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores. 83 For all of the four null and alternative research hypothesis sets above, four of the five variables (BFI – Extraversion; BLSI – Visual; BLSI – Auditory; BLSI – Kinesthetic) were measured as a continuous variable. Dominant Learning Style was measured as a categorical variable. A respondent was placed into one of the three learning style categories according to the highest score received from the three learning style categories. Even if scores were close, the dominant learning style was identified from the highest value. Consequently, all of the research hypotheses above sought to investigate whether or not a relationship existed between the variable “instructor extraversion” and a respondent’s visual, auditory, kinesthetic, and dominant learning style. A One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to see if an instructor’s extraversion score varied as a function of a respondent’s dominant learning style. According to Lesser and Melgoza (2007), the ANOVA is used to determine whether there are any significant variations between the means of three or more groups. An ANOVA was used to investigate Hypothesis 1. Hypotheses 2 through 4 were investigated via a series of Pearson correlations. Pearson’s correlation coefficient technique is the optimal approach for investigating hypotheses 2 through 4. As Ritchey (2008) noted, correlation is the correct method to use when one wishes to see if a relationship exists between two variables. Ritchey further noted that correlation analysis requires that both variables be measured at either an interval or ratio level (i.e., a continuous level), a condition that is satisfied in the current analysis scenario. As such, a Pearson’s correlation was used as the analysis technique to investigate the tenets of research hypotheses 2 through 4. 84 Three additional analyses were conducted to see if there were differences in any of the four key variables (BFI – Extraversion; BLSI – Visual; BLSI – Auditory; BLSI – Kinesthetic) as a function of the three demographic variables of gender, age and race. In order to determine if there was a difference in the four key variables as a function of a respondent’s age, a Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient technique was used. Because the three key variables are continuous in nature, and because the variable Age was modeled on continuous data but was operationalized as an ordinal variable, a Spearman’s rho correlation is the appropriate technique (Ritchey, 2008). In order to determine if there was a difference in the four key variables as a function of a respondent’s gender, an independent sample t test was used. Given the continuous nature of the three key variables and the dichotomous nature of the variable Gender, the use of the independent samples t-test is appropriate (Ritchey, 2008). Finally, because the variable Race is a multiple category nominal-level variable, and because each of the three key variables are continuous, ANOVA is the appropriate analysis technique in this situation. As Ritchey (2008) notes, ANOVA demands a continuous outcome variable and a factor variable that is discrete in nature and has more than two categories. 85 Descriptive Statistics Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations, Focal Variables Variable Mean Std. Dev. BFI – Extraversion 28.11 4.31 BLSI – Visual 3.65 0.60 BLSI – Auditory 3.30 0.69 BLSI – Kinesthetic 3.15 0.63 Table 2 presents information on the four focal variables that were used in the analysis. The first variable in the table, BFI – Extraversion, represents a respondent’s observations about a particular instructor’s level of extraversion. It poses the question of whether or not the instructor, chosen by the respondent, was viewed as an extravert. The mean score for the extraversion scale is 28.11. Possible values for mean scores are between 8 (true introverts) and 40 (true extraverts), which implies that students view instructors as having a proclivity to the extraversion characteristic. The next three variables in Table 2 indicate the average level of visual, auditory and kinesthetic learning style among respondents. Among the 302 respondents, it appears that levels of visual learning (M = 3.65, SD = .60) are higher than are levels of auditory learning (M = 3.30, SD = .69) and kinesthetic learning (M = 3.15, SD =.63). Indeed, it appears that among respondents, a visual learning style is the preferred method through which to learn. 86 Cronbach Alpha Reliability Estimation Table 3 Internal Consistency Values (Cronbach α) Scale BFI – Extraversion BLSI – Visual BLSI – Auditory BLSI – Kinesthetic α 0.73 0.40 0.54 0.36 Table 3 presents the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for each of the four scales that were used in the current investigation. As Tavakol and Dennick (2011) noted, the alpha statistic was developed by Cronbach to provide a measure of the internal consistency reliability of a scale. The measure of alpha ranges between a value of 0 and 1, with higher scores indicating better reliability. Scores of .70 or higher suggest that a scale has an acceptable level of reliability (Cronbach, 1970), although lower levels of alpha are also seen as reliable when a scale has only a few items (Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). Of the four scales presented above, only the BFI – Extraversion scale demonstrates acceptable reliability, although the BLSI – Auditory scale does approach the level of acceptability. Both the BLSI – Visual and BLSI – Kinesthetic scales have low reliability scores. Cronbach’s scores were examined after removing some questions presenting low item total correlations. The auditory scale, comprised of eight items as examined after removing L5 and L8; variable L9 was removed from the kinesthetic scales, and variables 87 L16 and L20 were removed from the visual scales to achieve an optimal Cronbach’s alpha reliability. However, the increase in Cronbach’s alpha were minimal and consequently, this procedure was abandoned. In analyzing the variable of age and its relation to learning modalities and preference of extraversion, a Spearman rho correlation was conducted and results are examined in Table 4. Table 4 Spearman Correlations: Age and Learning Style Variable BLSI - Visual BLSI - Auditory BLSI - Kinesthetic Age r p 0.11 0.06 0.17 0.00 -0.07 0.24 Table 4 contains the statistical results for the correlation of the variable Age with the four key variables or BFI – Extraversion, BLSI – Visual, BLSI – Auditory, and BLSI – Kinesthetic. As can be seen in Table 4, age is positively correlated with extraversion and positively correlated with an auditory learning style. The Spearman rho correlation results contained in Table 4 suggest that as students get older, they prefer extraverted instructors and are more likely to report an auditory learning style. Table 5 contains the results of the independent sample t-test results to determine if the average scores of the three key variables will differ as a function of a respondent’s gender. As can be seen in Table 5, there are no statistically significant differences in any of the three variables as a function of a respondent’s gender. 88 Table 5 Independent Samples t-test for Gender Difference in Learning Style Variable BLSI - Visual BLSI - Auditory BLSI - Kinesthetic Male Mean SD 3.65 0.58 3.38 0.62 3.15 0.71 Female Mean SD 3.64 0.62 3.25 0.74 3.14 0.57 t 0.23 1.61 0.14 p 0.11 0.89 0.81 Note: n=302 Table 6 One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) for Race/Ethnicity Difference in Learning Style Variable BLSI - Visual BLSI - Auditory BLSI - Kinesthetic White Mean SD 3.59 0.64 3.30 0.64 3.25 0.69 Black Mean SD 3.55 0.52 3.13 0.68 2.80 0.70 Hispanic Mean SD 3.70 0.61 3.36 0.72 3.20 0.57 Asian Mean SD 3.32 0.35 3.00 0.29 2.68 0.28 F 1.699 1.735 6.705 Note: White n=72; Black n=41; Hispanic n=182; Asian n=7. Table 6 above presents the results of the one-way ANOVA to determine if the three key variables will differ as a function of a respondent’s race. As can be seen in Table 6, a respondent’s kinesthetic learning style will indeed vary as a function of race. Post-hoc analyses reveal that with respect to a kinesthetic learning style, Asians (M = 2.68) are less likely to use this learning style than Whites (M = 3.25), Blacks (M = 2.80) or Hispanics (M = 3.20). p 0.16 0.00 0.66 89 Hypothesis 1: Statistical Results Table 7 One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) Results, Hypothesis 1 Test Variable Visual Mean SD Auditory Mean SD Kinesthetic Mean SD BFI - Extraversion 28.74 27.60 27.00 4.15 4.49 4.18 F p 4.079 0.02 Note. Visual n = 161; Auditory n = 92; Kinesthetic n = 49. Table 7 shows the results of the one-way ANOVA that was used to investigate the first hypothesis. As can be seen in Table 4, a respondent’s perceived level of instructor extraversion will indeed vary as a function of the student’s dominant learning style (F (2,299)= 4.079, p = .02). Post-hoc analyses via a Tukey’s HSD test reveal that with respect to learning style, visual learners (M = 28.74; p = .03) are more likely to prefer an extraverted instructor as compared to kinesthetic learners (M = 27.00). The results of the ANOVA provide support for the tenets of Hypothesis 1 that there is a relationship between teacher extraversion and a student’s dominant learning style. For hypothesis 1, we can reject the null hypothesis and conclude that extraversion will vary as a function of dominant learning style. 90 Hypothesis 2 through 4: Statistical Results Table 8 Pearson Correlations between teacher extraversion and student learning style r p BLSI – Visual -0.03 .557 BLSI – Auditory -0.19 .001 BLSI - Kinesthetic -0.12 .038 Note: n=302 Table 8 presents Pearson correlations for all four of the focal variables used in this investigation. As can be seen in Table 8, there is no correlation between extraversion and visual variables. However, there are statistically significant negative correlations between the trait of extraversion and an auditory or kinesthetic learning style. The negative nature of the relationships suggests that students who have relatively high scores on the auditory or kinesthetic scales, tended to rate their favorite teacher as relatively low on the extraversion scale. Summary In summary, the statistical results in Table 7 concludes support for hypothesis 1. An instructor’s level of extraversion does influence a student’s propensity to learn according to a dominant learning style. Table 8 provides only partial support for hypotheses 2 through 4. There is no support for Hypothesis 2 (that there would be a relationship between teacher extraversion and visual learning style scores) from the data. For Hypothesis 2, I would fail to reject the null hypothesis and find no support for the alternative hypothesis. However, both Hypothesis 3 (that there would be a relationship 91 between teacher extraversion and auditory learning style scores) and Hypothesis 4 (that there would be a relationship between teacher extraversion and kinesthetic learning style scores) are supported by the data. For these hypotheses, I can reject the null hypothesis and conclude that as an instructor’s extraversion increases, a student with an auditory or kinesthetic learning style is less likely to appreciate the teaching style/mannerisms of the instructor. In Chapter 5 I expound on the ramifications and associations of these results. 92 Chapter 5: Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations Introduction This chapter addresses conclusions, implications, and recommendations that have resulted from this study’s research questions. A quantitative correlational study was conducted to examine relationships between an instructor’s degree of extraversion and a student’s learning modality of visual, auditory, or kinesthetic learning. Instructor extraversion was measured using the extraversion portion of the BFI (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991). Students in psychology, sociology, and educational courses at a southwest community college were asked to rate an instructor. This instructor, from the student’s viewpoint, was one who conveyed information successfully and basically taught well. They were also asked to complete a BLSI (Barsch, 1996) and a demographic questionnaire (U.S Census, 2010). The BLSI, consisting of 24 questions, rated students on three learning modalities widely used to determine learning strengths and preferences in auditory, visual and kinesthetic sensory receivers. The demographic questionnaire distributed to participants contained gender, age and racial origin queries to examine correlations between these entities and learning style modalities. A total of 600 inventories were distributed through community college classrooms with 327 inventories collected; 25 were deemed unusable, resulting in a final count of 302 inventories involved in this study. In this chapter, a summary of the research is presented; findings on demographic data and Research Questions 1 through 4 are examined and interpreted, relating results to 93 Chapters 1 and 2 respectively. I re-emphasize postulates stated previously in corroborating and supporting conclusions derived from the data. Discussed in depth are implications and recommendations for social change, actions proposed, and future research. Finally, the significance of this research is recognized along with a concluding synopsis. Interpretation of Findings Out of a total of 302 inventories, 161 individuals were identified as visual learners, 92 identified as auditory learners and 49 participants were categorized as kinesthetic learners. The questions interpreting learning styles were from the Barsch Learning Style Inventory (Barsch, 1996). The queries for observer rating extraversion were taken from the Big Five Inventory which resulted in an alpha level of .730, acceptable range. Cronbach’s alpha values were 0.399 for visual, 0.536 for auditory, and 0.359 for kinesthetic. Kratzig and Arbuthnott (2006) stated that the BLSI had modest reliability, reporting Cronbach’s alpha as 0.54 for visual and 0.56 for auditory in their analysis. Reliability of a study conducted by Hansen and Cottrell (2012) of the BSLI was also comparable to this study, noting values for Cronbach’s alpha at .56 for auditory and .27 for visual learning styles. A total of eight items per scale was administered. Research Question 1 Research question 1 asked if there is a relationship between an instructor’s level of extraversion and a student’s dominant learning style. The results indicated a correlation between instructor extraversion and a visual learning style (p < .05), which is to say that students with a propensity to learn visually prefer extraverted instructors, more so than 94 auditory or kinesthetic learners. Therefore, there is support for the alternative hypothesis that a student’s dominant learning style is interrelated with an instructor’s level of extraversion. The null hypothesis is rejected and the alternative hypothesis is supported. Although I speculated that the auditory learner, with characteristics of listening skills, would be the learning style most benefiting from an instructor’s high degree of extraversion, my conjecture was not the case. Mean differences are significant, however slight (n2 = 0.03), and the evidence suggests that a learner who likes to learn visually will be inclined to prefer an instructor with a higher degree of extraversion over an auditory or kinesthetic learner. Participants were asked to rate an instructor they thought taught well, not one they necessarily liked. Visual learners expressed a preference to learn from an instructor that was more of an extravert than not. Several studies have indicated that range of motion and expressive gestures are positively correlated with extraversion (Argyle, 1988; Brebner, 1985; Gallaher, 1992). The visual effect that an extraverted instructor might have on a visual student’s attentiveness is noteworthy. Explanations for these results could originate from the demographics of the San Antonio area, with a large population of second language learners (United States Census Bureau, n.d.). According to Gilakjani (2011), second language learners are prone to visual cues and instructions. The large Hispanic population (53%) at the community college might have assisted in the evidence pointing towards visual learners as the primary correlational variable to extraverted instructors. As stated in Chapter 1, instructors who were less serious in their teaching and research were more likely to be extraverts (Friedman, Förster, and Denzler, 2007). For many community college 95 instructors, teaching courses is a secondary vocation after retirement or in conjunction with another occupation (Modern Language Association Committee on Community Colleges, 2006), implying that a large percentage of these instructors are extraverts, resulting in a disproportionate extraverted faculty. This assertion is supported through a normative sample by Srivastava, John, Gosling, and Potter (2003) where they reported extraversion mean scores of 3.12 to 3.31 and standard deviations of .85 to .92 depending on age. This study’s report of mean scores of 4.02 and standard deviations of .62 suggests that student choices have been limited to a preponderance of extraverted instructors. Research Question 2 Is there a relationship between instructor level of extraversion and a visual learning style? Results indicate that there is not support for the alternative hypothesis and the null hypothesis should not be rejected. Failure to accept the alternative hypothesis presents various conjectures. Dobson (2010) pointed out that most college undergraduates perceive themselves as visual learners. In the current study, 161 or 53.3% of the 302 participants identified as visual learners. However, while hypothesis 1 supports a closer relationship with instructor extraversion to the visual learner than the auditory or kinesthetic learner, it has failed to confirm that visual learners profit from having an extraverted instructor. Horton, Wiederman, and Saint, (2012), state that while attending lectures is weakly correlated to academic performance, it is unclear if attending lectures is beneficial to any particular learning style. However, they do provide evidence that furnishing materials such as power points, lecture notes and video recordings in lieu of attending lectures 96 proves to be more beneficial to all learners. Perhaps the extraverted instructors, chosen as effective teachers, did include various visuals in their teaching practices or were intensely dramatic in the use of descriptive words and examples, attending to the visual learners needs. Contrastingly, this technique was possibly neutralized by a continuous droning of words and long winded sentences. Or perhaps visual learners are taking notes, drawing pictures or performing activities to visually imprint concepts while the instructor is speaking, being able to focus visually and grasp onto certain ideas, while missing parts of the over stimulating verbal lecture/discussion. Reddy (2013) states that visual learners gravitate towards the written word and will diligently write down every word. Although there is no support between the visual learner and the extraverted instructor for hypothesis 2, hypothesis 1 does show more of a positive connection than either the auditory or kinesthetic learner. Research Question 3 Research question 3 inquired whether there is support for a relationship between teacher extraversion and an auditory learning style. The evidence suggest there is a negative relationship. Confidently, I speculated that a positive correlation would be apparent. Reiterating from chapter 2, auditory learners have astute listening skills (Dunn & Honigsfeld, 2009) and would therefore seem to be attentive to the extraverted instructor’s every word. Additionally, the consensus is that auditory learners have outgoing personalities and enjoy talking (Vincent, A., & Ross, D. (2001). They learn best by listening, but also by repeating what they have learned and verbally internalizing what they have learned. Dialogue between instructor and student, or student and student, may 97 be essential for the auditory learner to process and understand information (Vincent and Ross, 2001). If an extraverted instructor’s lecture does not permit verbal interaction then the auditory learner may lose interest in the learning process. Granted, extraverted instructors should welcome questions and lively debates, but if prone to lecture for long periods without interruption, monopolizing verbal communication, relishing in the sound of their own voice, the auditory student may become disengaged. This negative relationship implies that an introverted instructor’s mannerisms may be agreeable to the auditory learner, where the introverted instructor is a facilitator who guides interactions but is willing, if not offering, to limit unessential comments and verbal guidance. This leaves the auditory student with ample opportunity to ingrain information by restating the instructor’s words, asking questions, making comments and partaking in dialogue about the concept or topic. Without the auditory student’s active involvement, optimal learning may not occur. They learn by participating in conversations and expressing their own ideas (Vincent and Ross, 2001). Activities that engage the auditory learner, like group conversations, could take precedence over listening to an extraverted instructor, who monopolizes the conversation. Research Question 4 Regarding hypothesis 4 of whether a correlation exists between instructor degree of extraversion and a kinesthetic learning style, there is a negative correlation. Kinesthetic learners are poor listeners (Vincent and Ross, 2001) and thereby may become bored or non-responsive to an instructor with a high degree of extraversion and talkativeness. They learn by doing more so than listening. According to Beagley (2011), kinesthetic 98 learners process tactile information effortlessly. They may become restless during lectures and have trouble focusing. The negative relationship between extraverted instructors and kinesthetic learners may relegate itself to the same difficulties as the auditory learner, where the instructor lectures continuously and does not provide opportunities for group or individual presentations, experimentation or role playing. As stated in chapter 1, dominance is an indicator of extraversion (Goldberg, 1993; Wiggins, 1992) and in conjunction with the expected leadership role of the instructor, a kinesthetic learner may be unresponsive to an extravert’s techniques. Moreover, opportunities for enhancing the kinesthetic student’s learning style may not be presented in a community college setting. Mobley and Fisher, (2014) contended college classes are geared towards lectures and note taking. They have offered alternatives to instruction practices in the form of students physically moving around the classroom in favor of a particular viewpoint, and partial ownership of the classroom blackboard. The assertion is that instructors should use kinesthetic learning principles on a continual basis. It would seem that instructor’s possessing the characteristic of extraversion would gladly fulfill this need if allowed to do so by university administration. Eddy, (2010) addressed the imperative need for change among community college administration by recognizing the importance of culture, collaboration and the implementation of contemporary methods of teaching. Age A Pearson correlation on age and the three learning style variables implied that older participants were more likely to rate their preferred instructor high in extraversion 99 and that they had a tendency towards higher scores on the auditory learning subscale than their younger counterparts (p <.01). A study conducted by Kishon-Rabin, Avivi-Reich and Ari-Even Roth (2013) support the results of this study that some characteristics of auditory learning may be maintained in older adults. History suggests that as age increases, various sensory channels may decrease such as sight, hearing and physical movement. These results are for consideration, at best, since the number of participants over the age of 41 was negligible (3%). Gender As in many studies (Slater, Lujan, and DiCarlo, 2013; Shah, Ahmed, Shenoy, and Srikant, 2013), gender in this study had no significant effect on learning style preferences. In fact, both of the above mentioned studies determined that a multi-modal learning style was preferred by both male and female students. Findings in this examination indicate there is no specific preference of learning style by gender. This is substantiated by research conducted by Jones, Reichard and Mokhtari (2003), where findings indicate there are no statistically significant differences in any of the three variables of learning styles as a function of a respondent’s gender. Race With regards to this study, data analysis established that kinesthetic learning style does differ with regards to race (p < .01). Hispanics are more prone to a visual learning style than other races/ethnicities. Blacks are less likely to use a kinesthetic learning style than Hispanics or Whites. Asians are less likely to be kinesthetic learners than Whites, 100 Hispanics or Blacks. Results are significant although sample size of Asian participants was low (2.3%). Previous studies have indicated that English Language Learner (EFL) students have a tendency towards kinesthetic learning styles (Peacock, 2001; Dunn, Griggs, and Price 1993). While Boyking and Cunningham (2001) published results indicating that incorporating music and movement into teaching strategies of Black students proved to be beneficial in learning. Contrastingly, many more studies advocate the variability of learning styles among cultural groups taking into account the experiences, skills and interests of an individual student from a particular culture (Gutiérrez and Rogoff, 2003). Methodological, Theoretical and Empirical Implications Implications of these findings suggest there is no universal technique of good teaching. However, there is some support for substantiating a system of instructor level of extraversion to a particular learning style of a student. Conceivably, it could be beneficial to focus our attentions elsewhere in the world of pedagogical relationships. Perhaps it is the technique and not the personality of the teacher that drives student learning. There are an abundance of variables that effect student learning and instructor teaching. Whether it is auditory, visual, or tactile, an instructor may be able to accommodate one learning style over another, partly due to personable attributes and/or detriments. An educator’s ability to connect with students on various levels is determined by the qualities and impairments of both teacher and student. An ideal teachable moment is when a student grasps an idea, concept, or fact that entices him/her to ponder further, make connections, and originate new ideas. Although the educator’s role in facilitating these student epiphanies consists 101 of training and skill, innate characteristics may enhance or detract from the learning experience. Surely concessions should be attempted by both parties to produce the best possible outcome, and yet, to compromise suggests a relinquishment of valuable components that enhance the learning environment for both teacher and student. The learner may abandon a successfully proven method of learning, opting for a method influenced by an instructor’s natural inclinations of teaching. The instructor, on the other hand, may be taken out of his/her comfort zone by having to present in a way that feels unnatural or goes against his/her instinctive style of teaching. Either way, energy is needlessly expended on necessary adjustments to instruction, instead of grasping and dispensing information through inherent and familiar systems. Admittedly, there are instances where instructor/learner differences can be altered, but oftentimes, these differences cannot be modified. Binoy’s (2012) study conducted with monozygotic twins using the VARK model revealed that a significant difference of learning styles was found to hold true regardless of genetic sameness. The reality of multiple determinants leads to an interpretation that an exact match between instructor and student is unlikely. However, the results of this study encourages further assessment of key variables effecting student instruction. Implications for Social Change Some studies have indicated that students are flexible in their learning styles and can adjust to the teaching style of the instructor (Uğur, Akkoyunlu and Kurbanoğlu, 102 2011). Other studies proposed that students recognize different learning strategies are vital for various learning environments and students are able to adapt to instructor, subject matter, or presentation style in which a topic is taught (Jones, Reichard and Mokhtari, 2003). The implications of these assertions recognize the need for different learning environments and that a cookie cutter approach to learning is obsolete. Therefore, a society’s willingness to oblige learners and instructors must be flexible. The necessity for unconventional learning environments is apparent. Accepting and respecting alternative ways of teaching and learning should be at the forefront of society’s agenda. An unbiased approach to pedagogical methods, the place where learning occurs, and the instructor’s degrees or knowledge should not take precedence over the information and skill that the learner has acquired. A prestigious university should not be thought of as producing better students. When they are thought of in this way, opportunities are given and doors are suddenly open for a chosen few, disregarding those who attended less prestigious universities, but have attained great skill and knowledge through an exceptional pairing between instructor and student. Instructors may be well qualified on the subject matter and yet unable to convey knowledge due to the instructor’s skill at teaching, or types of learners in the class. Rather, a university’s merit should arise from the number of competent, well trained graduates it produces, whether it is through on line-classes or brick and mortar institutions. The distinction should be placed in a university’s ability to provide different types of learning experiences. In this way, learners will truly feel they got their money’s worth and that learning is a thorough and valuable experience. 103 Recommendations for Practice Institutions must do a better job of keeping students in school, however addressing every student’s learning style becomes a time-consuming challenge. Precious time may be lost, that could have been used in developing subject matter as opposed to making sure everyone grasps an idea. Furthermore, the material, concept, or idea may go astray when justifying individualized learning styles. The efficiency and clarity in which an instructor can teach students, if preferences and inclinations are acknowledged, should be apparent. If the instructor would teach the way in which his/her personality dictates, the student who is inclined to excel with the right presentation style could flourish, and therefore, the learning experience, instructor experience, and the dynamics of the institution should reveal positive results. The capability of the instructor would be enhanced, because he/she is in his/her comfort zone of teaching. Likewise, students thrive in the best possible environment. This proposal could fit many different classroom settings, from large lecture halls to small classes of 15 or 20, where students are aware that classes would be administered visually, with films and power points, or by auditory means, with lecturers and speakers, or kinesthetically, with hands-on scenarios. Accommodating students by appealing to their learning style is crucial toward societal gains. The results can produce a more intelligent and informed workforce, while ensuring the survival of academic progress. To implement a match between instructor and student, countless studies must be conducted. 104 When these studies are eventually completed and a consensus formed, correct prescriptions for learning will be available for the majority of the student body. Recommendations for Further Study Additional analysis of correlations between instructor extraversion and learning style modalities are recommended, along with a suggestion to perform correlational studies of the other Big 4 personality traits (openness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and agreeableness) with the three learning style modalities. There are a plethora of variables that effect student learning and instructor teaching. These variables must be considered together, separately, and in conjunction with other factors. If there is a formula or algorithm for correct teaching to a specific population, it is a complex abundance of external and internal factors. It could be prescriptive per individual learner or groups of learners and may also change over time. Implications of these findings suggest there is no universal technique of good teaching. Consequently, exploring all possible factors is necessary to gain insight into instructor/student paradigms. In addition, examining relationships between students’ learning styles and their own level of extraversion may prove to be significant, as well as the investigation into instructors’ level of extraversion and style/skill in teaching are taken under consideration. Other questions concerning the relationships between extraverted instructors/students and introverted instructors/students deserves exploration. Sun’s (2012) suggestion of an investigation into the incompatibility of student/teacher relationships has weight. Salehi (2010) advised that future studies should deliberate on personality factors of students and teachers; while Harris and Sass (2010) 105 asserted that more than anything else, teachers significantly influence student achievement, but the difference in teacher productivity is baffling. Combinations of variables need investigation to compare with studies previously conducted. Terregrossa, Englander, Zhaobo, and Wielkopolski (2012) contended that learning styles may differ according to subject area, all other variables remaining constant. Studies have also concluded that extraverted students preferred teachers of a witty nature to other teachers, while introverted students preferred teachers who accepted the ideas of students and of other teachers, however introverted students preferred teachers who had flexible behaviors over other teachers (Akbar, 2009). Investigating the compatibility of introverts and extraverts concerning teachers and students is warranted. Research should inquire into whether there are any correlations between student extraversion and learning modality preferences. Exploring whether a learning style preference points to a student’s comfort while learning or his ability to absorb information is another direction for study. Other studies could query as to the extent learning style preferences are effected by cultural, ethnic or environmental backgrounds and what is the nature of these dependences. More research is needed on the extent that teaching to a particular learning style brings satisfaction to the learner and is effective teaching, or is lacking in some way. Investigations into an instructor’s skill at teaching to one learning style over another warrants attention. In addition, exploring the extent that teaching practices contribute to academic performance or the drop-out rate of community college students has merit. What happens when students are given the choice of ways in which to learn? 106 Can instructional technology be tailored to fit a particular style of learning? What are some interventions that take learning styles under consideration and how effective are these interventions? Does a mixture of learning styles in group work result in more knowledgeable learners or can it hinder learning, and what combinations of learners should be incorporated into groups? Is there an increase in conflict when learning styles are mixed in these groups? And if there is conflict, then the question of whether or not productivity increases in spite of this conflict arises. Perhaps making team members aware of individual learning styles in a group reduces this conflict. Conclusion In summarizing the results, I have concluded that instructor extraversion is conducive to a particular dominant learning style: visual. While there is a negative correlation between auditory and kinesthetic learning style and level of instructor extraversion, there is no significant correlation between the visual learning style and degree of instructor extraversion. Thus, while visual learners scored their favorite teachers higher on extraversion, an examination of the correlations between learning style scores and teacher extraversion suggests this is due to the negative correlations with auditory and kinesthetic styles rather than a positive correlation with visual style. While visual learners are more adherent to an extraverted instructor than the auditory and kinesthetic learner, there is no evidence suggesting that visual learners benefit from an extraverted instructor. This study’s results also identifies particular racial/ethnic groups who are prone to be one type learning style over another. Hispanics are considered more visual than other races/ethnicities while Asians are less likely to be 107 kinesthetic learners than other races/ethnicities. There is no significant difference of learning styles with regards to gender, while older students tend to gravitate towards auditory learning. There exists an enormous amount of literature concerning learning styles. While the Dunn and Dunn modal (Dunn, Griggs, Olson, Beasley, and Gorman, 1995) has been well popularized among the pedagogical community, some say that it is the teacher’s own learning style that drives the type of instruction incorporated into their classroom (StittGohdes, 2003). Others proclaim that if teachers change instruction to accommodate a particular learning style, other students may suffer because of the deviation in instruction (Harris and Sass, 2010). Producing a best fit between instructor and student may alleviate the problem, however the variables involved in this prognosis are dubious and inconclusive. The question of whether race, age, gender, environment, culture, biological make-up, a particular style of learning, or a combination of these categories play a major role in predicting the type of instruction that is best suited for a student is overwhelmingly complicated. This study has determined that instructor level of extraversion can influence the type of learner that will be served best, but results can be problematic. One reason is due to the other variables mentioned above. Padhye (2013) research contended that university teachers who display a high rate of extraversion attain higher levels of effectiveness with their students. This study substantiates Padhye’s claim if the dominant learning style is visual, being the opposite case if learners have an auditory or kinesthetic 108 learning style. However, like this study, Padhye failed to incorporate all variables that could manipulate the outcome. Currently, students’ learning styles are intermingled throughout community colleges and not taken under consideration when scheduling student courses. Therefore, participants are not assigned to courses by learning styles and instructors are not assigned to any particular group of students other than subject matter or student choice at the time of registration. Brooks and Khandker, (2013) contended that students are self-sorting. That is to say that a student will automatically pick a class or instructor they feel will best serve them, but there are limitations in this process as well. Students may not be acquainted with the subject matter or the professor that is teaching the required class. There may be scheduling conflicts prohibiting the student from making the desired choice. This study’s results are not conclusive by any means. The conclusions found in this study lead to more questions and the determination that many more studies should be conducted. Obtaining the ultimate skill, training, and knowledge in exchange for time, money, and anticipated success is a problematic endeavor. Educational institutions are given tasks of producing qualified candidates, participating in research and innovative practices, while maintaining financial solvency. Reasoning dictates that the organization and corroboration of administrations, instructors, and students can result in an institution’s success or decline. Therefore, steps taken in finding solutions to seemingly minor annoyances may appear inconsequential to the larger goals of the university, and yet inconveniences (why students are not responding to teaching methods), may prevent 109 an instructor from pursuing research or prevent students from continuing with their studies (they are not understanding concepts or processes). These hindrances can prevent an educational system from thriving in the most essential way of producing a qualified workforce. This research’s findings are supported by a study conducted by Katsioloudis and Fantz, (2012) where although there was some dissimilarity within majors, the general dominant learning style for engineering, industrial, and technology students was the visual style. Furthermore research suggests that 60% of people believe themselves to be visual learners, being one of the easier styles to accommodate on a larger scale (Johnson, 2011). This is in contradiction to Neuhauser’s study concerning the face to face learning environment verses online learning. It was found that there was no evidence supporting learning preference or type as a good predictor of achievement in a face-to-face or on-line teaching environment (2002). Consequently, visual stimuli may be demonstrated in person or with technology to attract the visual learner. The supposition that expertise, experience, training and skill can outweigh a teacher’s personality characteristics must be considered. While this study has concluded that there is a connection between instructor extraversion and student learning style, Kneipp, Kelly, Biscoe, and Richard (2010) found evidence that agreeableness was the only personality trait that correlated significantly with student ratings of instructional quality. In addition, Patricka (2010) contended that instructor personality was essential in determining positive student evaluations above 110 grades and perceived learning. This implies that if an instructor is likeable, they will receive a positive evaluation whether or not the student received a good grade or has learned anything from the class. Recent research (Harris and Sass, 2010) consistently found that teacher productivity is the most important factor in student learning and that it is the components of intelligence, teaching skills and subject knowledge that increases teacher productivity. Yet, there are those who assert that teaching is an innate skill and can neither be taught or learned (Peirce and Martinez, 2012). This conclusion was reached after surveying teachers who reported that they acquired teaching skills through training and practice. Also included was reading books on pedagogy, observing classes, taking classes and attending workshops. Korte and Lavin (2013) found the best teaching traits in instructors were content/subject matter expertise, strong communication skills, approachability, work (industry) experience and class preparedness. Out of these traits, approachability and strong communication factors are most in line with an extraverted instructor, the others being inconsequential to personality characteristics. While it is evident that educators must stay current in their field of expertise, developing new methods of teaching and evaluating learning, it is also evident that a teacher’s personality plays a role in pedagogy practices even though the trait of extraversion may be of minor significance. 111 References Akbar, S. (2009). The study of relationship among personality factors (introversion, extroversion) of students and their effects of selecting a good teacher. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.633 Alexander, A., Karvonen, M., Ulrich, J., Davis, T., & Wade, A. (2012). Community college faculty competencies. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 36(11), 849–862. doi:10.1080/10668926.2010.515511 Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47(1), i–171. doi:10.1037/h0093360 American Association of Community Colleges. (2013). Characteristics of a community college student. Retrieved from http://www.aacc.nche.edu/Resources/aaccprograms/pastprojects/Pages/publicccfa culty.aspx American Association of Community Colleges. (2014). Community college fact sheet. Retrieved from http://www.aacc.nche.edu/AboutCC/Pages/fastfactsfactsheet.aspx Anders, S. L., Frazier, P. A., & Shallcross, S. L. (2012). Prevalence and effects of life event exposure among undergraduate and community college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59(3), 449–457. doi:10.1037/a0027753 112 Associated Press (AP). (2010, October 5). AP–Stanford University Education Poll. New York: Associated Press. Retrieved from http://surveys.ap.org/data%5CSRBI% 5CAPNational%20Education%20Poll%20Topline%20100410_1st%20release.pdf Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication. New York, NY: Routledge Publishing. Bain, K. (2011). What the best college teachers do. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Baker, R. E., Simon, J. R., & Bazeli, F. P. (1986). An assessment of the learning style preferences of accounting majors. Issues in Accounting Education, 1(1), 112. Retrieved from Business Source Complete database (Accession No. 5321424) Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., & Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(3), 570–585. doi:10.1037/02787393.33.3.570 Barsch, J. (1996). Barsch Learning Style Inventory. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. Barsch, J., & Creson, B. (1980). Barsch Learning Style Inventory Yearbook: 9 Subjects: Education Database: Mental Measurements Yearbook. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. Baum, S., Little, K., & Payea, K. (2011). Trends in community college education: Enrollment, prices, student aid, and debt levels. New York: The College Board. doi:10.1080/00779954.2012.746558 113 Baumgartner, F. (1933). Die Charktereigenschaften. [The character traits]. In Beitraege zur charakter und persoenlichkeitsforschung (Whole No. 1). Bern, Switzerland: A. Francke. Beagley, L. (2011). Educating patients: Understanding barriers, learning styles, and teaching techniques. Journal of PeriAnesthesia Nursing, 26(5), 331-337.doi: 10.1016/j.jopan.2011.06.002 Bean, J. P. (2005). Nine themes of college student retention. College student retention: Formula for student success, 215-243. Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=ckk5B_ADM_YC&oi=fnd&pg=P A215&dq=+the+student+psyche&ots=n0zVe0dGqU&sig=OwOdSa3oBPGKInW MsbUu0vrFj_s#v=onepage&q=the%20student%20psyche&f=false Beukeboom, C. J., Tanis, M., & Vermeulen, I. E. (2012). Journal of Language and Social Psychology, published online 5 October 2012. doi:10.1177/0261927X12460844 Binoy, M. K. V. J. (2012). Learning preference among monozygotic twins: An exploratory study. Journal of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, 4(4), 106–109. Retrieved from http://journals.sfu.ca/ijmbs/index.php/ijmbs/article/view/250 Blank, L. (1970) Relationship between student instructional ratings and student-faculty psychological types. Retrieved January 17, 2013, from http://www.eric.ed.gov/ ERICWebPortal/contentdelivery/servlet/ERICServlet?accno=ED040422 Boykin, A. W., & Cunningham, R. T. (2001). The effects of movement expressiveness in story content and learning context on the analogical reasoning performance of African American children. Journal of Negro Education, 72-83. 114 Brebner, J. (1985). Personality theory and movement. In Individual differences in movement (pp. 27-41). Springer Netherlands. Brooks, T. J., & Khandker, A. W. (2012). Returns to different ‘learning styles’: Evidence from a course in microeconomics. New Zealand Economic Papers, (Advance), pp. 1–18. doi:10.1080/00779954.2012.746558 Buss, A., & Plomin, R. A. (1975). A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley Interscience. Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6(1), 1–30. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 7396669) Capretz, L. F. (2003). Teachers are from heaven, students are from hell true or false? Industry and Higher Education, 17(6), 417-422. Retrieved from http://www.eng.uwo.ca/people/lcapretz/mbti-Industry-v2.pdf Carr, D. (2007). Character in teaching. British Journal of Educational Studies. 55(4). 369–389. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8527.2007.00386.x Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38(4), 369–389, 476–506. doi: 10.1037/h0054116 Champagne, D. W. (1991). Improving your teaching: How do students learn. Law Library Journal [0023-9283] 83(85). Retrieved from http://sfxhosted. exlibrisgroup.com/waldenu?sid=google&auinit=DW&aulast= 115 Champagne&atitle=Improving+Your+Teaching:+How+Do+Students+Learn&titl e=Law+library+journal&volume=83&date=1991&spage=85&issn=0023-9283 Charkins, R. J., O’Toole, D. M., & Wetzel, J. N. (1985). Linking teacher and student learning styles with student achievement and attitudes. Journal of Economic Education, 16(2): 111–120. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/discover/ 10.2307/1182513?uid=3739920&uid=2134&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&uid=373925 6&sid=21102525755697 Chen, R. (2011) Institutional characteristics and college student dropout risks: A multilevel event history analysis. Research in Higher Education. 53(5), 487–505. doi:10.1007/s11162-011-9241-4 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Retrieved from http://www.lrdc.pitt.edu/schneider/P2465/Readings/Cohen,%201988%20(Statistic al%20Power,%20273-406).pdf Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Rutledge. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Neo PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 396, 653–655. Costa, Jr., P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T. Heatherton & J. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Cronbach, L. J. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing, 3rd Edition. New York: 116 Harper. Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 491–517. Retrieved from http://www.philipcorr.net/ uploads/downloads/101.pdf DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience brain structure and the Big Five. Psychological Science, 21(6), 820–828. doi: 10.1177/0956797610370159 Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(1), 417. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No 9102251403). Dobson, J. L. (2010). A comparison between learning style preferences and sex, status, and course performance. Advances in Physiology Education, 34(4), 197-204. doi: 10.1152/advan.00078.2010 Dunn, R. (1990). Rita Dunn answers questions on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 15–19. Retrieved from http://ezp.waldenulibrary.org/ login?url=http://search.proquest.com.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/docview/224854779 ?accountid=14872 Dunn, R., Beaudry, J. S., & Klavas, A. (1989). Survey of research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 46(6), 50–58. Retrieved from http://marric.us/files/ CSTA_learnjournal.pdf#page=76 117 Dunn, R., Beaudry, J. S., & Klavas, A. (2002). Survey of research on learning styles. California Journal of Science Education, 2(2), 75–98. Retrieved from http://marric.us/files/CSTA_learnjournal.pdf#page=76 Dunn, R., Dunn, K. J., & Price, G. E. (1985). Learning styles inventory. London: Lawrence Price Systems. (1985). Dunn, R., Giannitti, M. C., Murray, J. B., Rossi, I., Geisert, G., & Quinn, P. (1990). Grouping students for instruction: Effects of learning style on achievement and attitudes. The Journal of Social Psychology, 130(4), 485–494. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No 9707104034). Dunn, R., Griggs, S. A., Olson, J., Beasley, M., & Gorman, B. S. (1995). A meta-analytic validation of the Dunn and Dunn model of learning-style preferences. The Journal of Educational Research, 88(6), 353–362. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 9510112847). Dunn, R, Griggs, S.A., & Price, G.E. (1993). Learning styles of Mexican American and Anglo-American elementary school students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development, 21(4), 237-247. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 9312093617). Dunn, R., & Honigsfeld, A. (2009). Differentiating instruction for at-risk students: What to do and how to do it. pp. 51–60, 89–118. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishing Group, Inc. Dunn, R., Honigsfeld, A., Doolan, L. S., Bostrom, L., Russo, K., Schiering, M. S., & Tenedero, H. (2009). Impact of learning-style instructional strategies on students’ 118 achievement and attitudes: Perceptions of educators in diverse institutions. The Clearing House: A Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas, 82(3), 135–140. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 35608370). Eaves, L., & Eysenck, H. (1975). The nature of extraversion: A genetical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 32(1), 102–112. doi: 10.1111/j.146727.2007.00386.x Eddy, P. L. (2010). Community College Leadership: A Multidimensional Model for Leading Change. Stylus Publishing, Herndon, VA. Ehrenberg, R. G. (2012). American higher education in transition. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 26(1), 193216.https://www.ilr.cornell.edu/cheri/upload/cheri_wp141.pdf Erton, I. (2010). Relations between personality traits, language learning styles and success in foreign language achievement. Journal of Education, 38, 115–126. Retrieved from http://www.efdergi.hacettepe.edu.tr/english/abstracts/ 38/pdf/%C4%B0SMA%C4%B0L%20ERTON.pdf Eysenck, H. J. (1964). Crime and personality. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. Eysenck, H. J. (1968). Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Eysenck, H. J. (1975). Eysenck on extraversion. New York: Halsted Press. 119 Eysenck, H. J. (1998). Personality and the experimental study of education. European Journal of Personality, 10(5), 427–439. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 13608697). Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. London: University of London Press. Falkenstern, M., & Kwon, P. (2012). Extraversion, resilience and satisfaction with life. Convention presentation at the American Psychological Association. Topic 56 personality. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/ 10.1037/e641512012-001 Fang, S. (2012). A survey of teacher-student style mismatches. Higher Education of Social Science. 3(1) 5–12. doi:10.3968/j.hess.1927024020120301.1036 Felder, R. M., & Brent, R. (2005). Understanding student differences. Journal of Engineering Education, 94(1), 57–72. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/ docview/217960231?accountid=14872 Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from different sources. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44(3), 329– 344. doi: 10.1037/h0057198 Fleming, N., & Baume, D. (2006). Learning styles again: VARKing up the right tree! Educational Developments, 7(4), 4. Retrieved from http://www.johnsilverio.com/ EDUI6702/Fleming_VARK_learningstyles.pdf 120 Fraser, S. W., & Greenhalgh, T. (2001). Coping with complexity: Educating for capability. British Medical Journal, International edition 323.7316, 799–803. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/204004238?accountid=14872 Friedman, R. S., Förster, J., & Denzler, M. (2007). Interactive effects of mood and task framing on creative generation. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 141–162. doi:10.1080/10400410701397206 Frisby, B. N., & Martin, M. M. (2010). Instructor–student and student–student rapport in the classroom. Communication Education, 59(2), 146–164. doi: 10.1080/03634520903564362 Fugate, A. L., & Amey, M. J. (2000). Career stages of community college faculty: A qualitative analysis of their career paths, roles, and development. Community College Review, 28(1), 1–22. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/ 213207207?accountid=14872 Furnham, A., & Mansi, A. (2011). Correlates of self-rated business competencies. International Journal of Business and Management, 6(9), 3. doi: 10.5539/ijbm.v6n9p3 Gallaher, P. (1992). Individual differences in nonverbal behavior: Dimensions of style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 133-145. doi:10.1037/00223514.63.1.133 Gardner, H. E. (1985). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books. 121 Gardner, H. E. (2000). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books. Gardner, H. E., & Hatch, T. (1987). Multiple intelligences go to school: Educational implications of the theory of multiple intelligences. American Educational Research Association Stable, 18(8), 4–10. Retrieved on December 14, 2012 from http://cct2.edc.org/ccthome/reports/tr4.html Gardner, H. E., & Moran, S. (2006). The Science of multiple intelligences theory: A response to Lynn Waterhouse. Educational Psychologist, 41(4), 227–232. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4104_2 Gilakjani, A. P. (2011). Visual, auditory, kinaesthetic learning styles and their impacts on English language teaching. Journal of Studies in Education, 2(1), 104-113. doi:10.5296/jse.v2i1.1007 Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1216–1229. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216 Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48(1), 26. doi10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26 Goldrick-Rab, S. (2010). Challenges and opportunities for improving community college student success. Review of Educational Research, 80(3), 437–469. doi: 10.3102/0034654310370163 Graham, E. K., & Lachman, M. E. (2012). Personality stability is associated with better cognitive performance in adulthood: Are the stable more able? The Journals of 122 Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(5), 545– 554. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr149 Gray, J. A. (1970). The physiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8(3), 249–266. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90069-0 Grubb, W. N. (Ed.). (1999). Honored but invisible: An inside look at teaching in community colleges. New York: Routledge. Retrieved May 20, 2013, from http://www.amazon.com/ dp/0415921651/ref%3Dasc_df_04159216512439792/ %3Ftag%3Daskcom05el20%26creative%3D394997%26creativeASIN% 3D0415921651%26linkCode%3 Dasn#reader_0415921651 Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educational researcher, 32(5), 19-25. http://search.proquest.com.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/docview/216901337?accountid =14872 Hammerness, K., Darling-Hammond, L., Bransford, J., Berliner, D., Cochran-Smith, M., McDonald, M., & Zeichner, K. (2005). How teachers learn and develop. Preparing teachers for a changing world: What teachers should learn and be able to do. 358–389. Hoboken, NJ: Jossey-Bass. Hansen, l., & Cottrell, D. (2012). An evaluation of modality preference using a “Morse Code” recall task. Journal of Experimental Education, 8(1), 123-137. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2012.678408 123 Harris, D., & Sass, T. (2010). What makes for a good teacher and who can tell? Washington, DC: CALDER, The Urban Institute. Retrieved July 16, 2013, from http://www.urban.org/UploadedPDF/1001431-what-makes-for-a-goodteacher.pdf Heaton‐Shrestha, C., Gipps, C., Edirisingha, P., & Linsey, T. (2007). Learning and e‐learning in HE: The relationship between student learning style and VLE use. Research Papers in Education, 22(4), 443–464. doi: 10.1080/02671520701651797 Hein, T. L., & Budny, D. D. (1999). Teaching to students’ learning styles: Approaches that work. In Frontiers in Education Conference, 1999. FIE’99. 29th Annual (Vol. 2, pp. 12C1–7). IEEE. doi:10.1109/FIE.1999.841622 Heine, S. J., & Buchtel, E. E. (2009). Personality: The universal and the culturally specific. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 369–394. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163655 Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2001). Happiness, introversion–extraversion and happy introverts. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 595–608. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00058-1 Honigsfeld, A., & Dunn, R. (Winter 2006). Learning-style characteristics of adult learners. Delta Kappa Gamma Bulletin, 72(2), 14–31. Retrieved from https://www.dkg.org/category/library/publications/bulletin 124 Horton, D. M., Wiederman, S. D., & Saint, D. A. (2012). Assessment outcome is weakly correlated with lecture attendance: influence of learning style and use of alternative materials. Advances in physiology education, 36(2), 108-115.doi: 10.1152/advan.00111.2011 Hunt, E. (2001). Multiple Views of multiple intelligence. PsycCRITIQUES, 46(1), 5–7. doi:10.1037/002513 International Personality Item Pool. (n.d.). A scientific collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of personality traits and other individual differences. Retrieved May 5, 2013, from http://ipip.ori.org/ Jaskyte, K., Taylor, H., & Smariga, R. (2009). Student and faculty perceptions of innovative teaching. Creativity Research Journal, 21(1), 111–116. doi: 10.1080/10400410802633673 John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five inventory—versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research. John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.). Handbook of personality: Theory and research New York, NY: Guilford Press. 125 Jones, C, Reichard, C. & Mokhtari, K. (2003) Are Students’ learning styles discipline specific? Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 27:5, 363-375, doi:10.1080/713838162 Jung, C. G. (1959). The Archetypes and the collective unconscious. Translated by Rfc Hull. New York: Bollingen Foundation Inc. Retrieved Jan 2013 from http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=Yc5PlU9MyDwC&oi=fnd&pg=P R5&dq=carl+jung+and+archetypes&ots=tWaI3PvVs1&sig=hbHWVPk77cBztN BU8dFIQ05D68w#v=onepage&q=carl%20jung%20and%20archetypes&f=false Jung, C. G. (1971a). General description of the types. In Psychological types (Rev. Ed., pp. 330–407). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Jung, C. G. (1971b). The type problem in human character. In Psychological types (Rev. Ed., pp. 147–165). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Kamuche, F. U. (2005). Do learning & teaching styles affect students’ performance? An empirical study. Journal of Business & Economics Research, 3(9). 35-40. Retrieved from http://www.journals.cluteonline.com/index.php/JBER/ article/view/2806 Kazu, I. Y. (2009). The effect of learning styles on education and the teaching process. Journal of Social Sciences, 5(2), 85–94. Retrieved from http://thescipub.com/journals/jss Kishon-Rabin, L., Avivi-reich, M., &Ari-Even Roth, d., (2013) Improved gap detection thresholds following auditory training: Evidence of auditory plasticity in older 126 adults. American Journal of Audiology 22(2), 343-346. doi:10.1044/10590889(2013/12-0084 Klages, L. (1926). The science of character. (Translated 1932). London: Allen and Unwin. Kneipp, L. B., Kelly, K. E., Biscoe, J. D., & Richard, B. (2010). The impact of instructor’s personality characteristics on quality of instruction. College Student Journal, 44(4), 1–5. Retrieved from http://www.freepatentsonline.com/ article/College-Student-Journal/248733623.html Kolb, A. Y. (2005). The Kolb learning style inventory—version 3.1 2005 technical specifications. Boston, MA: Hay Resource Direct. Komarraju, M., Karau, S. J., Schmeck, R. R., & Avdic, A. (2011). The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 472–477. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp. waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019 Korte, L., & Lavin, A. (2013). An Investigation into Good Teaching Traits. Learning in Higher Education, 141. Retrieved from http://jwpress.com/JLHE/Issues/JLHE2013-Spring.pdf#page=153 Kratzig, G., & Arbuthnott, K. (2006). Perceptual learning style and learning proficiency: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1, (238–246). doi: 10.1037/0022–0663.98.1.238 Lesser, L. & Melgoza, L. (2007). Simple Numbers: ANOVA example of facilitating Student learning in statistics. Teaching Statistics. 29 (3), p102-105. doi: 127 10.1111/j.1467-9639.2007.00272. x. Luo, X., Kranzler, H. R., Zuo, L., Wang, S., & Gelernter, J. (2007). Personality traits of agreeableness and extraversion are associated with<i> ADH4</i> variation. Biological Psychiatry, 61(5), 599–608. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.017 Modern Language Association Committee on Community Colleges (2006) A community college teaching career. Retrieved from: http://www.mla.org/commcollege_teachcar Marin, L. M., & Halpern, D. F. (2011). Pedagogy for developing critical thinking in adolescents: Explicit instruction produces greatest gains. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(1), 1-13. Retrieved from http://www4.ncsu.edu/~jlnietfe/Creativity_%26_Critical_Thinking_Articles_files/ Marin%20%26%20Halpern%20(2011).pdf Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Reinerman, L. E., Langheim, L. K., & Saxby, D. J. (2010). Task engagement, attention, and executive control. In Handbook of individual differences in cognition. 205–230. New York: Springer-Verlag New York, LLC. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1210-7_13 McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 173–175. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770693 McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr., P. T. (1985). Comparison of EPI and psychoticism scales with measures of the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual 128 Differences, 6, 587–597. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/01918869(85)90008-X McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175–215. Retrieved from. http://sfxhosted.exlibrisgroup.com/waldenu?sid=google&auinit=RR&aulast=McC rae&atitle=An+introduction+to+the+five%E2%80%90factor+model+and+its+ap plications&id=doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x&title=Journal+of+ personality&volume=60&issue=2&date=1992&spage=175&issn=0022-3506 McKenzie, K., & Gow, K. (2004). Exploring the first year academic achievement of school leavers and mature-age students through structural equation modelling. Learning and Individual Differences, 14(2), 107–123. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp. waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2003.10.002 McMillan, D., Groth, C., Lane-Getaz, S., Dittmann, A., & Bailey-Seiler, S. (2012). Extraversion and extreme responses to positive and neutral stimuli. Convention presentation at the American Psychological Association, Topic 56.2 Personality Assessment. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/ 10.1037/e643972012-001 Mobley, K., & Fisher, S. (2014). Ditching the Desks: Kinesthetic Learning in College Classrooms. The Social Studies, (ahead-of-print), 1-9. doi:10.1080/00377996.2014.951471 Mullin, C. M. (2011, October). The road ahead: A look at trends in the educational attainment of community college students (Policy Brief 2011-04PBL). 129 Washington, DC: American Association of Community Colleges. Retrieved from http://www.aacc.nche.edu/Publications/Briefs/Documents/PB_RoadAhead.pdf Mullin, C. M. (2012, February). Why access matters: The community college student body (Policy Brief 2012-01PBL). Washington, DC: American Association of Community Colleges. Murray, H. G., Rushton, J. P., & Paunonen, S. V. (1990). Teacher personality traits and student instructional ratings in six types of university courses. Journal of Educational Psychology 82(20), 250–261. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.250 Murray, S., & Moore, K. (2012). Inclusion through multiple intelligences. Journal of Student Engagement; Education Matters 2(1) 42–48. Retrieved from http://ro.uow.edu.au/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1014&context=jseem&seiredir=1&referer=http%3A%2F%2Fscholar.google.com%2Fscholar%3Fq%3D%2 BInclusion%2Bthrough%2Bmultiple%2Bintelligences.%2B%26btnG%3D%26hl %3Den%26as_sdt%3D0%252C44#search=%22Inclusion%20through%20multipl e%20intelligences.%22 Myers, I. B., McCaulley, M. H., & Most, R. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Myers-Briggs, I. (1962). Manual for the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Neff, M., Wang, Y., Abbott, R., & Walker, M. (2005). Evaluating the effect of gesture and language on personality perception in conversational. Evolution and Human 130 Behavior 26 (4), 363–373. Retrieved from http://classes.soe.ucsc.edu/cmps140/ Winter11/papers/extraversion_final_PRE_PRINTpdf Nettle, D. (2004). An evolutionary approach to the extraversion continuum. In Psychology, brain and behaviour. Newcastle, United Kingdom: University of Newcastle. Retrieved from UK.http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/ j.evolhumbehav.2004.12.004 Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61(6), 622–631. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.6.622 Neuhauser, C (2002) Learning Style and Effectiveness of Online and Face-to-Face Instruction, American Journal of Distance Education, 16:2, 99-113, doi: 10.1207/S15389286AJDE1602_4 Nurmi, J. E., Viljaranta, J., Tolvanen, A., & Aunola, K. (2012). Teachers adapt their instruction according to students’ academic performance. Educational Psychology, 32(5), 571–588. doi:10.1080/01443410.2012.675645 Offir, B., Bezalel, R. & Barth, I. (2007). Introverts, extroverts, and achievement in a distance learning environment. American Journal of Distance Education 21, (1), doi:10.1080/08923640701298613. Omura, K., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2005). Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism. Neuroreport, 16(17), 1905–1908. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/neuroreport/Abstract/2005/ 11280/Amygdala_gray_matter_concentration_is_associated.7.aspx 131 Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., & Bjork, R. (2008). Learning styles concepts and evidence. Psychological science in the public interest, 9(3), 105-119. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x Patrick, C. L. (2011). Student evaluations of teaching: Effects of the Big Five personality traits, grades and the validity hypothesis. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 36(2), 239–249. doi:10.1080/02602930903308258. Patricka, C. L. (2009). Student evaluations of teaching: effects of the Big Five personality traits, grades and the validity hypothesis doi: 10.1080/02602930903308258 Patterson, C. H., & Purkey, W. W. (1993). The preparation of humanistic teachers for schools of the next century. The Journal of Humanistic Education and Development, 31(4), 147–155. Retrieved from http://www.sageofasheville.com/ pub_downloads/THE_PREPARATION_OF_HUMANISTIC_TEACHERS.pdf Peacock, M. (2001). Match or mismatch? Learning styles and teaching styles in EFL. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11(1), 1. Retrieved from http://sfxhosted.exlibrisgroup.com/waldenu?sid=google&auinit=M&aulast=Peaco ck&atitle=Match+or+mismatch%3F+Learning+styles+and+teaching+styles+in+E FL&id=doi:10.1111/14734192.00001&title=International+journal+of+applied+linguistics&volume=11&iss ue=1&date=2001&spage=1&issn=0802-6106 Peirce, K. L., & Martinez, G. D. (2012). How we learn to teach: Trial by fire, by the seat of our pants, and other (more scientific) methods. Journalism & Mass 132 Communication Educator, 67(2), 134-144. Retrieved from ttp://search.proquest.com/docview/1026948451?accountid=14872 Pishghadam, R., & Sahebjam, S. (2012). Personality and emotional intelligence in teacher burnout. The Spanish Journal of Psychology. 15(1), 227–236. Retrieved from ISSN11://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2012.v15.n1.37314 Plomin, R. A. (1974). A temperament of personality development: Parent-child interactions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/ pqdtft/docview/302740036/previewPDF/13FB19E2A87724536AD/1?accountid= 14872 Plomin, R. A. (1976). Extraversion: Sociability and impulsivity? Journal of Personality Assessment, 40(1), 24. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier database (Accession No. 6387992). Polk, J. A. (2006). Traits of effective teachers. Arts Education Policy Review, 107(4) 23– 29. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/211016802?accountid =14872 Pritchard, A. (2009). Ways of learning: Learning theories and learning styles in the classroom. Abingdon: NY: Routledge. Retrieved from http://www.amazon.com/dp/0415466083/ ref%3Dasc_df_04154660832439747/ %3Ftag%3Daskcom05el-20%26creative%3D394997%26creativeASIN% 3D0415466083%26linkCode%3Dasn#reader_0415466083 133 Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 203–212. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 Reddy, R. S. (2013). Supplementary Materials to Enhance Language Skills of Learners. International Journal of Social Sciences & Interdisciplinary Research, 2(9).Retrieved from http://indianresearchjournals.com/pdf/IJSSIR/2013/September/16.pdf Richardson, J. T. (2011). Approaches to studying, conceptions of learning and learning styles in higher education. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(3), 288–293. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.lindif. 2010.11.015 Ripski, M. B., LoCasale-Crouch, J., & Decker, L. (2011). Pre-service teachers: Dispositional traits, emotional states, and quality of teacher-student interactions. Teacher Education Quarterly, 38(2), 77–96. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/863831085?accountid=14872 Ritchey, F. (2008). The Statistical Imagination: Elementary Statistics for the Social Sciences, 2nd Edition. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. Rocklin, T., & Revelle, W. (1981). The measurement of extroversion: A comparison of the Eysenck Personality Inventory and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 20(4), 279–284. Retrieved from http://personality-project.org/revelle/publications/rr81.small.pdf 134 Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 122. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122 Sadeghi, N., Kasim, Z. M., Tan, B. H., & Abdullah, F. S. (2012). Learning styles, personality types and reading comprehension performance. English Language Teaching, 5(4), 116. doi:10.5539/elt.v5n4p116 Salehi, A. (2010). The study of relationship among personality factors (introversion, extroversion) of students and their effects to selecting a good teacher. ProcediaSocial and Behavioral Sciences, 2(2), 4016–4020. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.633 Sattler, J. M. (2008). Assessment of children: Cognitive foundations (5th ed.). La Mesa CA: Jerome M. Sattler Publisher, Inc. Schaefer, M., Heinze, H. J., & Rotte, M. (2012). Touch and personality: Extraversion predicts somatosensory brain response. NeuroImage, 62, 432–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage. 2012.05.004 Schneider, M., & Yin, L. M. (2012). Completion matters: The high cost of low community college graduation rates. American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy. Retrieved on January 8, 2013, from http://www.aei.org/files/2012/04/02/completion-matters-the-high-cost-of-low-community-college-graduationrates_173407573640.pdf 135 Sears, S. J., & Kennedy, J. J. (1997). Myers-Briggs personality profiles of prospective educators. Journal of Educational Research, 90(4), 195–202. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/204193676?accountid=14872 Sendziuk, P. (2010). Sink or swim? Improving student learning through feedback and self-assessment. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 22(3) 320–330. Retrieved from http://www.compact.org/resources/service-learning-resources/publishing-outletsfor-service-learning-and-community-based-research/international-journal-ofteaching-and-learning-in-higher-education/945/ Shah, K., Ahmed, J., Shenoy, N., & Srikanth, N. (2013). How different are students and their learning styles. International Journal of Research in Medical Sciences. (pp.212-215) doi: 10.5455/2320-6012.ijrms20130808 Shah, S., & Meisenberg, G. (2012). Opinions about teaching modalities: A comparison between faculty and students. Hindawi Publishing Corporation Education Research International, Volume 2012, Article ID 604052, 7 pages. doi: 10.1155/2012/604052 Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational researcher, 15(2), 4-14. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1175860?origin=JSTOR-pdf Sierra, J. J. (2010). Shared responsibility and student learning ensuring a favorable educational experience. Journal of Marketing Education, 32(1), 104–111. doi: 10.1177/0273475309344802 136 Simpson, J. A., Gangestad, S. W., & Biek, M. (1993). Personality and nonverbal social behavior: An ethological perspective of relationship initiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(5), 434–461. doi:10.1006/jesp.1993.1020 Sizemore, M. H., & Schultz, P. N. (2005). Ethnicity and gender influences on learning styles in nursing students from an Hispanic-serving institution. Journal of Hispanic Higher Education, 4(4), 343–353. doi:10.1177/1538192705279590 Slater, J. A., Lujan, H. L., & DiCarlo, S. E. (2013). Does gender influence learning style preferences of. Journal of Dental Education, 77(10), 1371-1378. doi:10.1152/advan.00010.2007 Socrates. (n.d.). Retrieved July 26, 2013, from http://thinkexist.com/quotation/i_cannot_ teach_anybody_ anything-i_can_only_make/149592.html Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2003). Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1041-1053. doi:10.1037/00223514.84.5.1041 Stitt-Gohdes, W. L. (2003, April). Student teachers and their students: Do their instructional and learning preferences match? Business Education Forum 57(4), 22–27. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?q=Student+teachers+and+ their+students%3a+Do+their+instructional+and+learning+preferences+match%3f +&id=EJ664042 Sun, F. (2012). A survey of teacher-student mismatches. Higher Education of Social Science. 3(1), 5–12. doi:10.3968/j.hess.1927024020120301.1036 137 Svanum, S., & Aigner, C. (2011). The influences of course effort, mastery and performance goals, grade expectancies, and earned course grades on student ratings of course satisfaction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(4), 667–679. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02011.x Taylor, K., & Elias, D. (2012). Transformative learning: A developmental perspective. The handbook of transformative learning: Theory, research, and practice (p. 47). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Tavakol, M., & Dennick, R. (2011). Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. International Journal of Medical Education, 2, 53-55. doi: 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 999-1009. doi:10.1177/0146167206288599 Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2010). Intra-individual change in personality stability and age. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(1), 31-37. doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.006 Terregrossa, R. A., Englander, F., Zhaobo, W., & Wielkopolski, T. (2012). How college instructors can enhance student achievement: Testing a learning styles theory. International Journal of Education, 7(1), 1–5. Retrieved from http://www.iabpad.com/IJER/how_college_instructors.pdf Texas Higher Education Coordinating Board. (2012). Retrieved February 20, 2013, from http://www.thecb.state.tx.us/apps/resumes/ 138 Trudeau, G. P., & Barnes, K. J. (2011). Shared Expectations: Identifying Similarities And Differences Between Student And Faculty Teaching Values Based On Student Evaluation Of Faculty Classroom Performance. International Business & Economics Research Journal , 1(7), 67-79. Retrieved from http://www.journals.cluteonline.com/index.php/IBER/article/viewFile/3956/4009 Tupes, E., & Christal, R. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 225–251. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1037/e531292008-001 Uğur, B., Akkoyunlu, B., & Kurbanoğlu, S. (2011). Students’ opinions on blended learning and its implementation in terms of their learning styles. Education and Information Technologies, 16(1), 5-23. doi:10.1007/s10639-009-9109-9 United States Census Bureau. (n.d.). State and County Quick Facts Retrieved from http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/states/48/4865000.html US Census (2010). Overview of Race and Hispanic Origin: Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-02.pdf Vincent, A., & Ross, D. (2001). Learning Style Awareness. Journal Of Research On Computing In Education, 33(5), 1-10.Retrieved from http://www.editlib.org/j/ISSN-0888-6504/ Wiggins, J. S. (1992). Have model will travel. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 527–532. Retrieved from Academic Search Complete database (Accession No. 9208170756). 139 Williams, J., & Kane, D. (2010). The part-time students experience 1996–2007: An issue of identity and marginalization. Tertiary Education and Management, 16, 183– 209. doi:10.1080/13583883.2010.497342 Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2008). Extraversion. In M. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 1–28). New York: Guilford Publishing. Wirz, D. (2004). Students’ learning styles vs professors’ teaching styles. Inquiry, 9(1), 1– 5. Retrieved on May 2, 2013, from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ876847 Wright, G., (2011) Student-centered learning in higher education. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education 23(1), 92-97. Retrieved from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ938583.pdf Young, M. S., & Schinka, J. A. (2001). Research validity scales for the NEO-PI-R: Additional evidence for reliability and validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76(3), 412–420. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7603_04 Zepke, N., & Leach, L. (2010). Improving student engagement: Ten proposals for action. Active Learning in Higher Education, 11(3), 167-177. Retrieved from http://alh.sagepub.com.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/content/11/3/167.full.pdf+html Zhang, L. F. (2006). Does student–teacher thinking style match/mismatch matter in students’ achievement? Educational Psychology, 26(3), 395–409. doi: 10.1080/01443410500341262 140 Zhang, L. F., Sternberg, R. J., & Fan, J. (2013). Revisiting the concept of ‘style match’. British Journal of Educational Psychology. doi:10.1111/bjep.12011 141 Appendix A: Consent Form CONSENT FORM You are invited to take part in a research study about the relationship between instructor degree of extraversion and student learning style. The researcher is inviting all enrolled students at St. Phillips College or San Antonio College over the age of 18 years of age to be in the study. This form is part of a process called “informed consent” to allow you to understand this study before deciding whether to take part. This study is being conducted by a researcher named Celeste Bazier, who is a doctoral student at Walden University. Background Information: Corresponding teacher degree of extraversion to student preferred learning style may be imperative in producing the best results for students and teachers alike. The potential implications address the placement of teachers in a learning environment that is best suited to a particular type of student with a particular learning style and conducive to the needs of a specific group in optimizing achievement. Procedures: If you are 18 years of age (or older) and agree to be in this study, you will be asked to: Complete the extraversion portion of the Big Five Extraversion Inventory on a previous instructor and… Complete a Barsch Learning Style Inventory Complete a general questionnaire about myself- age, race/ethnicity, gender. Voluntary Nature of the Study: This study is voluntary. Everyone will respect your decision of whether or not you choose to be in the study. No one at your community college will treat you differently if you decide not to be in the study. If you decide to join the study now, you can still change your mind later. You may stop at any time. 142 Risks and Benefits of Being in the Study: Being in this type of study involves some risk of the minor discomforts that can be encountered in daily life, such as minor fatigue, stress or lack of memory recall. Being in this study would not pose risk to your safety or wellbeing. The ability of the student to recognize instructor personality traits that are best suited for their particular style of learning may be crucial to academic success. This match may reduce dropout rate and encourage the pursuit of educational accomplishments by making learning a more enjoyable and valuable experience. Payment: There is no compensation, monetary or otherwise, for participation in this study. Privacy: Any information you provide will be kept anonymous. The researcher would only know the identity of the participant if the participant elected to know inventory scores through email. The researcher will not use your personal information for any purposes outside of this research project. Also, the researcher will not include your name or anything else that could identify you in the study reports. Data will be kept secure in a locked file cabinet at an undisclosed location. Data will be kept for a period of at least 5 years, as required by the university. Contacts and Questions: You may ask any questions you have now. Or if you have questions later, you may contact the researcher via celeste.bazier@waldenu.edu. If you want to talk privately about your rights as a participant, you can call Dr. Leilani Endicott. She is the Walden University representative who can discuss this with you. Her phone number is 612-3121210. Walden University’s approval number for this study is IRB will enter approval number here and it expires on IRB will enter expiration date. The researcher will give you a copy of this form to keep. Statement of Consent: I have read the above information and I feel I understand the study well enough to make a decision about my involvement. I confirm that I am at least 18 years of age, and by completing these surveys and inventories, I agree to participate in the study as described in this consent form. 143 Appendix B Big Five Inventory- Observer Rating Scale Think of a recent instructor or high school teacher who you enjoyed learning from, thought you learned a lot from them, and can still remember many ideas and concepts that were grasped in their classroom environment. This instructor does not necessarily have to be likable but rather, were they a good teacher, easily relaying information. Rate this professor according to the scale below. 1 Disagree Strongly Talkative Reserved Full of energy Enthusiastic Quiet temperament Assertive Shy or inhibited Outgoing or sociable 2 Disagree a little 3 Neither agree nor disagree 4 Agree a little 5 Agree strongly 144 Appendix C Barsch Learning Style Inventory Often Sometimes Seldom 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Can remember more about a subject through listening than reading. Follow written directions better than oral directions. Like to write things down or take notes for a visual review. Bear down extremely hard with a pen or pencil when writing. Require explanations of diagrams, graphs or visual directions. Enjoy working with tools. Are skillful with and enjoy developing and making graphs and charts. 8. Can tell if sounds match when presented with pairs of sounds. 9. Remember best by writing things down several times. 10. Can understand and follow directions on maps. 7. 11. Do better at academic subjects by listening to lectures and tapes. 12. Play with coins or keys in pocket. 13. Learn to spell better by repeating the letters out loud than by writing the word on paper. 14. Can better understand a news article by reading about it in the paper than by listening to radio. 15. Chew gum, smoke or snack during studies. 145 16. Feel the best way to remember is to picture it in your head. 17. Learning spelling by “finger spelling” the words 18. Would rather listen to a good lecture or speech than read about the same material in a book. 19. Are good at solving and working on jigsaw puzzles and mazes. 20. Grip objects in hands during learning period. 21. Prefer listening to the news on the radio rather than reading about it in a newspaper. 22. Obtain information on an interesting subject by reading relevant materials. 23. Feel very comfortable touching others, hugging, handshaking, etc. 24. Follow oral directions better than written ones. Jeffrey Barsch developed this short questionnaire to determine an individual’s preferred method of learning. Please answer the following questions by checking the appropriate line after each statement about auditory, visual and tactile learners. Inquiries about results may be addressed to celeste.bazier@waldenu.edu. 146 Appendix D Demographic information- Please answer BOTH Question 2 about Hispanic origin and Question 3 about race. For this study, Hispanic origins are not races. 1. Your Age__ Gender ____M__F 2. Are you Hispanic, Latino, or of Hispanic origin? ___No, not of Hispanic, Latino, or of Hispanic origin ___Yes, Mexican, Mexican American or Chicano ___Yes, Puerto Rican ___Yes, Cuban ___Yes, another Hispanic, Latino, or Spanish origin----Print origin below, for example, Argentinian, Columbian, Dominican, Nicaraguan, Spaniard and so on. ________________________________________________ 3. What is your race? Check one or more. ___White_ ___Black, African American, or Negro ___American Indian or Alaskan Native- Print name or enrolled in principal tribe below ___Asian Indian ___ Japanese ___Native Hawaiian ___Chinese ___ Korean ___ Guamanian or Chamorro ___Filipino ___Vietnamese ___ Samoan ___Other Asian—Print race for example ___Some ____Other Pacific Islander- Print race Hmong, Laotian, Thai for example, Fijian, Tongan, Pakistani, Cambodian, and so on. and so on. other race ---Print race ____________________________________